The Science of Wellbeing and Positive Psychology
The Science of Wellbeing and Positive Psychology
Chapter
The Science of Wellbeing and
2 Positive Psychology
Lindsay G. Oades and Lara Mossman
The seemingly innocuous question “What is wellbeing?” easily leads to passionate debates
and much conceptual confusion. This chapter first provides some signposts for a definition
of wellbeing. Sound definition of the construct is essential to the measurement of wellbeing
and the growth of the science of wellbeing. The emergence of the science of positive psychol-
ogy and its application through positive psychology interventions (i.e. strategies to increase
wellbeing) have created new opportunities to develop wellbeing in individuals, organiza-
tions and communities. After positive psychology is defined, key theories of wellbeing and
positive psychology are summarized. An overview of key research evidence from positive
psychology interventions is then provided. This overview is intended not as an exhaustive
review, but rather as a sample to enable readers to explore the fertile interface of the science
of wellbeing and positive psychology with the literature and experience of recovery.
What Is Wellbeing?
Responses to the question What is wellbeing? vary greatly depending on whether it is a lay
person, health practitioner, person with lived experience of illness, economist, philosopher,
psychologist or sociologist answering the question. Lay persons or persons unfamiliar with
defining concepts or constructs commonly give examples of situations or experiences that lead
to wellbeing, such as having good friends or a good job. A philosophical definition is com-
monly brief and coherent, but may lack the precision that a scientist is seeking. One philosoph-
ical example is the following definition: how well someone’s life is going for them (Crisp, 2014).
Philosophers have been arguing over and defining wellbeing for many centuries. Econ-
omists have been doing so for over one hundred years. Psychologists and wellbeing scien-
tists are relatively new to this arena. Although recognizing the contributions to this area
from philosophers (e.g. the distinction between hedonic and eudaemonic wellbeing) and
economists (e.g. the distinction between evaluative and experienced wellbeing), this chapter
focuses primarily on empirical attempts to define, measure and increase wellbeing, with
particular focus on positive psychology and its related interventions.
In attempting to define wellbeing it is useful to consider several questions from the outset:
1. What is the level of analysis? for example: individual, group, organization, society
2. What domain is being discussed? for example: physical, mental, social
3. Is it to be defined subjectively or objectively?
4. Does it necessarily require the presence of positive attributes? in other words, is it more
than the absence of negative attributes such as symptoms of illness?
Wellbeing, Recovery and Mental Health, ed. Mike Slade, Lindsay Oades and Aaron Jarden.
Published by Cambridge University Press. © Mike Slade, Lindsay Oades and Aaron Jarden 2017
Wellbeing
construct Definition Reference
Hedonic “focuses on happiness and defines well- Ryan and Deci (2001b, p. 141)
wellbeing being in terms of pleasure attainment and
pain avoidance” “the predominant view
among hedonic psychologists is that well-
being consists of subjective happiness and
concerns the experience of pleasure versus
displeasure broadly construed to include all
judgments about the good/bad elements
of life. Happiness is thus not reducible to
physical hedonism, for it can be derived from
attainment of goals or valued outcomes in
varied realms”
Eudaemonic “The eudaimonic perspective of wellbeing – Gale et al. (2013, p. 687)
wellbeing based on Aristotle’s view that true happiness
comes from doing what is worth doing –
focuses on meaning and self-realization, and
defines wellbeing largely in terms of ways of
thought and behavior that provide fulfillment”
Wellbeing “refers to optimal psychological experience Deci and Ryan (2008, p. 1)
and functioning” “Ryff and Keyes (1995) Ryan and Deci (2001b, p. 146)
… spoke of psychological well-being
(PWB) as distinct from SWB and presented
a multidimensional approach to the
measurement of PWB that taps six distinct
aspects of human actualization: autonomy,
personal growth, self-acceptance, life purpose,
mastery, and positive relatedness. These six
constructs define PWB both theoretically and
operationally and they specify what promotes
emotional and physical health”
Wellbeing
construct Definition Reference
Psychological “Each dimension of PWB articulates different Keyes et al. (2002, p. 1008)
wellbeing challenges individuals encounter as they
strive to function positively (Ryff, 1989a; Ryff
and Keyes, 1995). That is, people attempt
to feel good about themselves even
while aware of their own limitations (self-
acceptance). They also seek to develop and
maintain warm and trusting interpersonal
relationships (positive relations with others)
and to shape their environment so as to meet
personal needs and desires (environmental
mastery). In sustaining individuality within
a larger social context, people also seek a
sense [of ] self-determination and personal
authority (autonomy). A vital endeavor is to
find meaning in one’s efforts and challenges
(purpose in life). Lastly, making the most of
one’s talents and capacities (personal growth)
is central to PWB.”
Subjective “As an operational definition, SWB is most Deci and Ryan (2008, p. 1)
wellbeing often interpreted to mean experiencing
a high level of positive affect, a low level
of negative affect, and a high degree of
satisfaction with one’s life …The concept of
SWB, assessed in this way, has frequently been
used interchangeably with ‘happiness.’ Thus,
maximizing one’s well-being has been viewed
as maximizing one’s feelings of happiness.”
Wellbeing “In 2 large samples, results supported the Gallagher et al. (2009, p. 1025)
proposed latent structures of hedonic,
eudaimonic and social well-being and indicated
that the various components of well-being
could be represented most parsimoniously with
3 oblique second-order constructs of hedonic,
eudaimonic, and social well-being.”
Social “Keyes (1998) conceived of a five-component Gallagher et al. (2009, p. 1027)
wellbeing model of social well-being: social integration,
social contribution, social coherence, social
actualization, and social acceptance. These five
elements, taken together, indicate whether
and to what degree individuals are overcoming
social challenges and are functioning well
in their social world (alongside neighbors,
coworkers, and fellow world citizens).”
(cont.)
Wellbeing
construct Definition Reference
Mental health “a syndrome of symptoms of positive feelings Keyes (2002, p. 207)
and positive functioning in life”
Flourishing “high levels of wellbeing” Hone et al. (2014, p. 62)
“To be flourishing in life, individuals must Keyes (2002, p. 210)
exhibit a high level (high = upper tertile) on
one of the two measures of emotional well-
being and high levels on six of the 11 scales of
positive functioning”
Life satisfaction “Life satisfaction, according to Campbell et al. Keyes et al. (2002,
(1976), reflects individuals’ perceived distance pp. 1007–1008)
from their aspirations … satisfaction is a
judgmental, long-term assessment of one’s life”
Happiness “according to Bradburn (1969), results from a Keyes et al. (2002, p. 1008)
balance between positive affect and negative
affect … happiness is a reflection of pleasant
and unpleasant affects in one’s immediate
experience”
Optimal “high subjective wellbeing and psychological Keyes et al. (2002, p. 1007)
wellbeing wellbeing”
Happiness “In the first version of his theory, Seligman Forgeard et al. (2011, p. 96)
(2002) claimed that ‘happiness’ was composed
of three subjective facets: positive emotion,
engagement, and meaning. Happiness was
therefore achievable by pursuing one or
more of these facets. As a result, individuals
low in one aspect could still be ‘happy’ if they
nurtured other components.”
Wellbeing “wellbeing consists of the nurturing of one Forgeard et al. (2011, p. 96)
or more of the five following elements:
Positive emotion, Engagement, Relationships,
Meaning, and Accomplishment (abbreviated
as the acronym PERMA). These five elements
are the best approximation of what humans
pursue for their own sake”
Positive “we identified the positive pole of each Huppert and So (2013,
wellbeing symptom dimension. This resulted in ten p. 849)
features representing positive aspects of
mental functioning: competence, emotional
stability, engagement, meaning, optimism,
positive emotion, positive relationship,
resilience, self-esteem, and vitality … it
includes both hedonic and eudaimonic
components; that is, both positive feeling and
positive functioning.”
fresh lens. In addition to the breadth, they also found that the size, reach and impact of the
field had grown substantially since its founding. This growth has generated new sub–fields
focused on the application of the science, such as positive education (Norrish 2015; Nor-
rish et al., 2013) or positive organizational behavior and positive organizational scholarship
(Wright and Quick, 2009).
As the science disseminates, there are some issues that arise by virtue of the immaturity of
the field. While the breadth of research undertaken is substantial (Rusk and Waters, 2013b),
there is tension between the depth of research and the desire for real-world application. A
number of single studies have shown promising findings, but more research that replicates
them across a range of populations and contexts is required: whom do interventions work
for and under what conditions (i.e. effectiveness rather than efficacy: see Hone et al., 2015)?
More longitudinal studies are required, too. As the field matures, longitudinal studies will
be conducted. In the meantime, positive psychology researchers are reminded to adhere to
rigorous scientific standards (Snyder et al., 2011) such as those of meta-analysis and ran-
domized control trials (Vella-Brodrick, 2013). Paradoxically, while positive psychology
offers itself as the antidote to the negativity bias of psychology as usual, it must not fall prey
to focusing solely on the good in the world. Good science looks at the whole picture (Snyder
et al., 2011). As the field works toward this, those working with the application of positive
psychology must ensure that they are conscientious consumers of the science, and as such
new journals focusing specifically on these issues are emerging (e.g. the International Jour-
nal of Applied Positive Psychology, which will launch in 2016). It is with this in mind that we
move to the key theories and evidence.
with higher levels of positive emotion were more successful across many life domains such
as work, relationships and health.
As an example of a causal study, Lyubomirsky et al. (2005) examined the results of experi-
mental studies and found that increasing people’s positive emotions had the effect of making
them better at conflict resolution and more social. They also found in their meta-analysis, which
included longitudinal studies, that happiness does lead to better relationship and work out-
comes. As examples of evidence they cited findings that increases in happiness lead to greater
creativity, productivity and quality of work, and income (Estrada et al., 1994) and to stronger
social support and an increased likelihood of marriage (Harker and Keltner, 2001), that sub-
jective wellbeing impacts mental and physical health (Pressman and Cohen, 2005) and leads
to healthier immune function (Davidson et al., 2003), and that happier people even live longer.
Furthermore, what both this cross-sectional and causal research suggest is that positive
emotions can and do have impacts beyond individuals to their close connections.
treated with a level of caution. They found four factors that heighten the efficacy of PPIs:
high levels of depression; increased age; individual interventions (rather than group inter-
ventions); and longer interventions. There is some debate as to whether participants should
be matched to interventions based on a person–activity fit or not. For example, participant
preference for undertaking a PPI has been positively related to commitment to adhere to
the activity (Schueller, 2010), and matching participants to an intervention that was based
on an orientation to happiness that differed from their dominant orientation was effective at
enhancing wellbeing (Giannopoulos and Vella-Brodrick, 2011).
However, Schueller (2011) found no significant difference between participants assigned
to activities based on preference and those randomly assigned to activities. PPIs have been
used in studies specifically targeting clinical participants, such as positive psychotherapy
(PPT; Seligman et al., 2006; see Chapter 11) and in nonclinical studies. However, regardless
of the population, many study designs include pre- and post-test measures of depression
and/or negative affect as well as measures of positive functioning.
Typically, studies include a broader range of measures, such as stress (Cheng et al., 2015);
state and trait anxiety (Cheavens et al., 2006); ruminative thinking (Odou and Brinker,
2014a); self-esteem (Sergeant and Mongrain, 2011); Big Five personality (Huppert and John-
son, 2010); working memory capacity (Jha et al., 2010); physical symptoms (Emmons and
McCullough, 2003; Sergeant and Mongrain, 2011); and self-compassion (Odou and Brinker,
2014a, 2014b).
Table 2.3 includes examples of the efficacy of PPIs. It should be noted that many con-
structs explored in PPIs have their origins outside positive psychology, such as gratitude,
forgiveness and mindfulness. While positive psychology cannot lay claim to these concepts,
its role does include an understanding of their function in building human strength and
generating high human potential.
Conclusion
In this chapter we introduced key definitions relating to wellbeing and highlighted ways to
approach this contested domain. After a summary of some key theories within the science
of wellbeing and positive psychology, a brief overview of recent empirical evidence for pos-
itive psychology interventions (PPIs) was provided. Key debates continue about evidence
from clinical compared with nonclinical populations, adding emphasis to the usefulness of
cross- [?] fertilization between wellbeing research and research in mental health recovery.
References
Cheavens, J. S., Feldman, D. B., Gum, A., Scott, T. M., & Snyder, C. R. (2006). Hope therapy in a
community sample: A pilot investigation. Social Indicators Research, 77, 61–86.
Cheng, S-T., Tsu, P. K., & Lam, J. H. M. (2015). Improving mental health in health care practitioners:
Randomized controlled trial of a gratitude intervention. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 83(1), 177–186.
Crisp, R. (2014). Well-being. In Zalta, E. N. (Ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Winter
2014 ed.). Available at http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2014/entries/well-being/.
Crompton, W. C., & Hoffman, E. (2013). Positive psychology: The science of happiness and flourishing.
Belmont, CA: Wadsworth, Cengage Learning.
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Nakamura, J. (2011). Positive psychology: Where did it come from, where
is it going? In Sheldon, K. M., Kashdan, T. B., & Steger, M. F. (ch. 1). Designing positive psychology:
Taking stock and moving forward. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Davidson, R. J., Kabat-Zinn, J., Schumacker, J., Rosenkranz, M., Muller, D., Sontorelli, S. F., et
al. (2003). Alterations in brain and immune function produced by mindfulness meditation.
Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 564–570.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2008). Hedonia, eudaimonia and well-being: An introduction.
doi:10.1007/s10902–006–9018–1.
Diener, E., & Chan, M. Y. (2011). Happy people live longer: Subjective well-being contributes to
health and longevity. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being, 3(1), 1–43, doi:10.1111/j.1758–
0854.2010.01045.x.
Emmons, R. A., & McCullough, M. E. (2003). Counting blessings versus burdens: An experimental
investigation of gratitude and SWB in daily life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84,
377–389.
Estrada, C., Isen, A. M., & Young, M. J. (1994). Positive affect influences creative problem solving and
reported source of practice satisfaction in physicians. Motivation and Emotion, 18, 285–299.
Forgeard, M. J. C., Jayawickreme, E., Kern, M., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2011). Doing the right
thing: Measuring wellbeing for public policy. International Journal of Wellbeing, 1(1), 79–106,
doi:10.5502/ijw.v1i1.15.
Fredrickson, B. L., & Levenson, R. W. (1998). Positive emotions speed recovery from the
cardiovascular sequelae of negative emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 191–220.
Froh, J. J., Kashdan, T. B., Ozimkowsk, K. M., & Miller, N. (2009). Who benefits the most from a
gratitude intervention in children and adolescents? Examining positive affect as a moderator.
Journal of Positive Psychology, 4, 408–422.
Gale, C. R., Booth, T., Mõttus, R., Kuh, D., & Deary, I. J. (2009) Neuroticism and extraversion in
youth predict mental wellbeing and life satisfaction 40 years later: The hierarchical structure of
well-being. Journal of Personality 77(4), 1025–1050, doi:10.1111/j.1467–6494.2009.00573.x.
Giannopoulos, V. L., & Vella-Brodrick, D. A. (2011). Effects of positive interventions and orientations
to happiness on subjective well-being. Journal of Positive Psychology, 6, 95–105.
Harker, L., & Keltner, D. (2001). Expressions of positive emotions in women’s college yearbook
pictures and their relationship to personality and life outcomes across adulthood. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 112–124.
Hone, L., Jarden, A., & Schofield, G. (2015). An evaluation of positive psychology intervention
effectiveness trials using the re-aim framework: A practice-friendly review. Journal of Positive
Psychology, 10(4), 303–322.
Hone, L. C., Jarden, A., Schofield, G. M., & Duncan, S. (2014). Measuring flourishing: The impact
of operational definitions on the prevalence of high levels of wellbeing. International Journal of
Wellbeing, 4(1), 62–90, doi:10.5502/ijw.v4i1.4.
Huppert, F. A., & Johnson, D. M. (2010). A controlled trial of mindfulness training in schools:
The importance of practice for an impact on well-being. Journal of Positive Psychology, 5,
264–274.
Huppert, F. A., & So, T. T. C. (2013). Flourishing across Europe: Application of a new conceptual
framework for defining well-being. Social Indicators Resource, 110(3), 837–861.
International Positive Psychology Association (2015, October 6). About IPPA. Available at http://
www.ippanetwork.org/about/.
Jha, A. P., Stanley, E. A., Kiyonaga, A., Wong, L., & Gelfand, L. (2010). Examining the protective
effects of mindfulness training on working capacity and affective experience. Emotion, 10, 54–64.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past, present, and future. Clinical
Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, 144–156.
Kerr, S. L., O’Donovan, A., & Pepping, C. A. (2015). Can gratitude and kindness interventions
enhance well-being in a clinical sample? Journal of Happiness Studies, 16, 17–36.
Keyes, C. L. M. (2002). The mental health continuum: From languishing to flourishing in life. Journal
of Health and Social Research, 43, 207–222.
Keyes, C. L. M., Shmotkin, D., & Ryff, C. D. (2002). Optimizing well-being: The empirical
encounter of two traditions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(6), 1007–1022,
doi:10.1037//0022–3514.82.6.1007.
Lyubomirsky, S., King, L., & Diener, E. (2005). The benefits of frequent positive affect: Does
happiness lead to success? Psychological Bulletin, 131(6), 803–855, doi:10.1037/0033–
2909.131.6.803.
Maslow, A. H. (1970). Motivation and personality. New York, NY: Harper & Row.
Norrish, J. M. (2015). Positive education: The Geelong Grammar School journey. Oxford, UK: Oxford
University Press.
Norrish, J. M., Williams, P., O’Connor, M., & Robinson, J. (2013). An applied framework for positive
education. International Journal of Wellbeing, 3(2), 147–161, doi:10.5502/ijw.v3i2.2.
Odou, N., & Brinker, J. (2014a). Exploring the relationship between rumination, self-compassion,
and mood. Self and Identity, 13(4), 449–459, doi:10.1080/15298868.2013.840332.
Odou, N., & Brinker, J. (2014b). Self-compassion, a better alternative to rumination than distraction
as a response to negative mood. Journal of Positive Psychology, 1–11, doi:10.1080/17439760.2014.9
67800.
Otake, K., Shimai, S., Tanaka-Matsumi, J., Otsui, K., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2006). Happy people
become happier through kindness: A counting kindness intervention. Journal of Happiness
Studies, 7, 361–375.
Peters, M. L., Flink. I. K., Boersma, K., & Linton, S. J. (2010). Manipulating optimism: Can mental
imagining a best possible self be used to increase positive future expectancies? Journal of Positive
Psychology, 5, 204–211.
Pressman, S. D., & Cohen, S. (2005). Does positive affect influence health? Psychological Bulletin, 131,
925–971.
Ramírez, E., Ortega, A. R., Chamorro, A., & Colmenero, J. M. (2014). A program of positive
intervention in the elderly: Memories, gratitude and forgiveness. Aging & Mental Health, 18(4),
463–470, doi:10.1080/13607863.2013.856858.
Rusk, R. D., & Waters, L. E. (2013). Tracing the size, reach, impact, and breadth of positive
psychology.Journal of Positive Psychology, 8(3), 207–221, doi:10.1080/17439760.2013.777766.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-Determination Theory and the facilitation of intrinsic
motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55(1), 68–78,
doi:10.1037110003–066X.55.1.68.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001a). Hedonia, eudaimonia, and well-being: An introduction. Journal of
Happiness Studies, 9, 1–11, doi:10.1007/s10902–006–9018–1.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001b). On happiness and human potentials: A review of research on
hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 141–166.
Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological
wellbeing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 57(6), 1069–1081.
Ryff, C. D. (1995). Psychological well-being in adult life. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
4(4), 99–104.
Ryff, C. D., & Keyes, C. L. M. (1995). The structure of psychological well-being revisited. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 69(4), 719–727.
Seligman, M. E. P. (1998). Building human strength: Psychology’s forgotten mission. APA Monitor,
29(1), 2.
Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Authentic happiness. New York, NY: Free Press.
Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An introduction. American
Psychologist, 55, 5–14.
Seligman, M. E. P., Rashid, T., & Parks, A. C. (2006). Positive psychotherapy. American Psychologist,
61, 774–788.
Seligman, M. E. P., Steen, T., Park, N., & Peterson, C. (2005). Positive psychology progress:
Empirical validation of interventions. American Psychologist, 60(5), 410–421, doi:10.1037/0003–
066X.60.5.410.
Sergeant, S., & Mongrain, M. (2011). Are positive psychology exercises helpful for people with
depressive personality styles? Journal of Positive Psychology, 6, 260–272.
Sheldon, K. M., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2006). How to increase and sustain positive emotion: The effect
of expressing gratitude and visualising best possible selves. Journal of Positive Psychology, 1, 73–82.
Shueller, S. M. (2010). Preferences for positive psychology exercises. Journal of Positive Psychology, 5,
192–203.
Shueller, S. M. (2011). To each his own well-being boosting intervention: Using preference to guide
selection. Journal of Positive Psychology, 6, 300–313.
Sin, N. L., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2009). Enhancing well-being and alleviating depressive symptoms
with positive psychology interventions: A practice-friendly meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical
Psychology: In Session, 65(5), 467–487, doi:10.1002/jclp.20593.
Snyder, C. R. (2002). Hope theory: Rainbows in the mind. Psychological Inquiry, 13(4), 249–275.
Snyder, C. R., Lopez, S. J., & Pedrotti, J. T. (2011). Positive psychology: The scientific and practical
explorations of human strengths (2nd ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Sage.
Steger, M. F., Shigehiro, O., & Kashdan, T. B. (2009). Meaning in life across the life span: Levels and
correlates of meaning in life from emerging adulthood to older adulthood. Journal of Positive
Psychology, 4, 43–52.
Tennant, R., Hiller, L., Fishwick, R., Platt, S., Joseph, S., Weich, S., et al. (2007). The Warwick–
Edinburgh Mental Well-Being Scale (WEMWBS): Development and UK validation. Health and
Quality of Life Outcomes, 5 (63). doi:10.1186/1477-7525-5-63.
Tugade, M. M., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). Resilient individuals use positive emotions to bounce
back from negative emotional experiences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(2),
320–333.
Vella-Brodrick, D. A. (2013). Positive psychology interventions: Research evidence, practical utility,
and future steps. In Keyes, C. L.M. (ch. 15). Mental Well-Being: International Contributions to the
Study of Positive Mental Health. Dordrecht, the Netherlands, Springer.
Waters, L. (2011). A review of school-based positive psychology interventions. Australian Educational
and Developmental Psychologist, 28(2), 75–90, doi:10.1375/aedp.28.2.75.
Waugh, C. E., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2006). Nice to know you: Positive emotions, self-other overlap,
and complex understanding in the formation of a new relationship. Journal of Positive Psychology,
1, 93–106.
Williams, S., & Shiaw, W. T. (1999). Mood and organizational citizenship behavior: The effects of
positive affect on employee organizational citizenship behavior intentions. Journal of Psychology,
133, 656–668.
Wright, T. A., & Quick, J. C. (2009). The emerging positive agenda in organizations: Greater than a
trickle, but not yet a deluge. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 30, 147–159.