COGS1000
COGS1000
Lectures Page 1
1 - Basic Principles of Brain Organisation and Neuroanatomy
Tuesday, 15 February 2022 2:25 AM
Chapter 1 Reading
Focus on these sections (pp. 2-16):
• Cellular components of the nervous system
• Neurons
• Glial cells
• Cellular diversity in the nervous system
• Neural circuits
• Other ways to study neural circuits
• Organisation of the human nervous system
• But a little over than a century ago, there was a vigorous debate about this question.
• Many early theories of brain organisation posited a system of neighbouring elements fused in
a giant, continuous, net-like arrangement or reticulum. This is known as reticular theory.
• One of the most famous proponents was Italian neuroanatomist Camillo Golgi (1843-1926),
who developed a ground-breaking staining method that selectively stains neurons and their
dendritic trees and axons so they can be observed under a microscope.
• Using Golgi's staining method, the Spanish neuroanatomist Santiago Ramon y Cajal
(1852-1934), produced detail images of neurons and their processes. He arrived at a very
different conclusion from that of Golgi. According to Ramon y Cajal, neurons were individual,
discrete elements that could make contact with one another, but are not continuously fused.
This is known as neuron theory.
• At the time of Golgi and Ramon y Cajal, synapses couldn't be observed directly, so decisive
evidence in favour of the reticular or neuron theory was unavailable.
• It was only with the invention of the electron microscope (1930s) that neuroscientists
discovered tiny gaps in the synapses - now known as the synaptic cleft - that connect neurons.
• Figure: Electron microscope image of an excitatory synapse. The white arrows indicate the
Lectures Page 2
• Figure: Electron microscope image of an excitatory synapse. The white arrows indicate the
synaptic cleft - the gap between the pre- and post-synaptic neurons. The scale bar represents
400 nm.
What is a neuron?
• Neurons or cells that are specialised for long-distance electrical signalling and intercellular
communication.
• Dendrites are branched projections of neurons that receive signals or electrical impulses
(known as action potentials or spikes) from other neurons and propagate these signals to the
cell body, or soma, of the neuron.
• Axons are long, slender projections of neurons that conducts electrical impulses away from
the cell body or soma.
---> Right: myelinated axon, which is a sheath, a lipid layer that coats or surrounds the axon and
facilitates faster transmission of electrical signals, like rubber plastic coating on electrical wires.
• An insulating kind of cable for electrical signals to be more efficiently conducted along the
length of an axon.
Lectures Page 3
Neural Circuits
Lectures Page 4
Organisation of the Human Nervous System
Lectures Page 5
Surface Anatomy of the Cerebral Hemisphere
Lectures Page 6
Dorsal View of the Human Brain
Lectures Page 7
Coronal View of the Human Brain
Lectures Page 8
2 - Electrical Signals of Nerve Cells
Sunday, 13 March 2022 5:06 PM
• Cell body with a dendritic tree in different shapes. These particular trees reach out to the
upper layers of the cortex and every neurons has axons, with particular axons going out of the
cortex to other places in the brain, e.g. basal ganglia, brain stem etc. This is a projection
neuron as it is projecting out of cortex.
• Dendrite tree attached to the cell body, with axons which extend away from the cell body.
There are little sheaths of fat which are called Myelin sheaths that improve its electrical
Lectures Page 9
There are little sheaths of fat which are called Myelin sheaths that improve its electrical
characteristics.
• The inside of the neuron had overall quite a negative charge which is an electricity concept
and negative electrical charge, with the outside of the neuron having quite a positive charge.
• Cell wall which is made up of a lipid bilayer, which is a biochemical figure on how to heat
things that are inside the cell inside the cell, and things outside the cell, outside the cell.
• Brackets ( [K+] ) indicate concentration; ( -> ) indicates force, e.g. electric force or diffusion
force
Lectures Page 10
• Diffusion is the force on molecules to move from areas of high concentration to areas of low
concentration and fill the empty space.
Lectures Page 11
• How does the flow differ when you have a large and wide open channel as compared to the
narrow channel on the left?
• Diffusion force depends on the concentration gradients and channel permeability. The
diffusion force tends to push things from high concentrations to lower concentrations, and the
greater the concentration disparity, the more diffusion that will occur, the quicker the
diffusion will occur.
Lectures Page 12
• Asking basically what is the charge of the inside relative to the outside? In this regard, the
voltage will be less than zero, as it will be negative. The inside is less positive than the outside,
therefore it's a negative number.
• V ~ (what's the overall charge on the inside and what's the overall charge on the outside and
what's the different between those overall charges and the answer to that question will be
voltage)
Lectures Page 13
• Diffusion force likes to have an equal number of things on the left as there are on the right.
But every time a positive charge moves across the membrane, it likes it because it's got more
space to roam and is more spread out.
• However, it's further away from these negative charges and we know that positive things like
negative things.
• So, where the diffusion forces are trying to pull the positive charges out of the cell, the electric
force is trying to pull positive charges into the cell and that's characteristic of real cells.
• At t = infinity, at a certain point, the electric force is equal and opposite to the diffusion force.
This means that you are in a state of equilibrium with no net flow.
Lectures Page 14
• A person came up with an equation that asked at what point did the flow of a particular ion
stop, and what is the value of E that causes that flow to stop. What is the potential where all
these things stopped flowing from one side to the other?
• This person said that the resulting membrane potential is proportional to 1/Z, where Z is the
charge of the thing you're looking at flowing across a particular membrane, which in this case
is a plus charge.
• If you assume in this example that Z > 0, and if you further assume the concentration of the
thing that's carrying those positive chargers, X, is greater than the concentration of X on the
outside, it means that the fraction Xin is divided by Xout, it means that this is greater than 1,
which leads to a voltage greater than 0.
Lectures Page 15
• Equilibrium is dominated by the ion with the most permeability. In this example, the ion with
the greatest permeability is Blue, as it has a larger channel than Yellow. When t = infinity, due
to the Blue channel being larger, more Blue ions will move from the inside to the outside,
whereas a small amount of Yellow ions will move outside, with most staying on the inside.
• Diffusion forces equals electric forces is about the entire system, and not about any particular
ion species alone.
• Diffusion does not know that the Blue ions and Yellow ions are different, and want to get to
where there is space. Similarly, the electric force does not know about identity either. It wants
to move away from charges that are similar and wants to move towards charges that are
different.
• In this example, because Blue has much more ability to travel across this membrane, it ends up
filling the empty space more than Yellow.
Lectures Page 16
• Due to the permeability of K+ being greater than the permeability of Na+ - which is true in the
actual cell, at least at rest - we ought to have a situation where more K+ ends up on the
outside than Na+.
• This is what we should observe in theory, however this does not occur in reality.
Lectures Page 17
• An active transporter does biochemical work against the forces that may be.
• In this particular case, it will take three Na+ ions in the inside of the cell when they get close to
it, it's going to bundle them all up and it's going to move them from the inside of the cell to the
outside of the cell. In doing so, it's going to trade two K+ cells and move them in the other
direction.
• This is not something that happens in accordance with the electric forces or with diffusion
forces. This is something that happens by biomechanical work being done to make this
happen.
• Even though this K+ channel is much better than this Na+, as more K+ gets out from the inside
to the outside, this pump pushes it back in and pulls Na+ out, and that's how you end with the
situation we see in the real world.
Lectures Page 18
• Diffusion pulls K+ from the inside to the outside, but the electric force pulls it back in the other
direction
• Diffusion pulls Na+ from the outside to the inside, as does the electric force
• Diffusion pulls Cl- into the cell, electric forces pushed it out because it's negative and it doesn't
like negative inside the cell
• Diffusion would - if it could - pull these negative proteins (A-) out and the electric force would
as well, but they're stuck and cannot move because they're too big to fit through any of the
channels in the membrane and there's no selective channels for them or are very few.
• The Goldman equation tells us that the equilibrium potential of neuron will depend on all of
Lectures Page 19
• The Goldman equation tells us that the equilibrium potential of neuron will depend on all of
the ion species and their conductivities for their permeability.
• The bigger the concentration gradient, the more diffusion will tend to push things apart and
the more similar those concentrations, the less those things will tend to come apart.
• At rest, PK is doing most of the work to decide what the membrane potentials look like, what
the resting membrane potential is. But if a cell is strongly stimulated by the current injection
from us using the battery from another cell injecting its neurotransmitters.
Lectures Page 20
• From injection from the battery of another cell injecting its neurotransmitters, it starts at cell,
becomes excited and increases, comes down and returns to baseline.
• The permeability of Na+ starts small, becomes exponential, and falls really quickly, not lasting
long. It's pretty sharp and that corresponds to the P of the action potential.
• If you look at the permeability of K+, it starts out a little higher, with more conductivity or
permeability in general.
• This action potential is driven by an opening by an increase of Na+ transaction or flow and then
a relatively slower increase in K+ flow. And then that Na+ flow shuts down really quick and goes
back up to what it was and the K+ flow takes a little longer to shut down since K+ is a bit
sluggish.
• Baseline phase ---> rising phase ---> overshoot phase ---> declining phase ---> undershoot
phase ---> baseline phase
• The baseline phase is characterised by both permeability of Na+ and K+ being the default
baseline level, that the rising phase is characterised by a very small increase in K+ transduction
and a much greater increase in Na+.
• Na+ does most of the work and the rising phase becomes very dominant, with K+ doing
relatively little compared to what Na+ is doing.
• In the declining phase Na+ is reversing, but it's also that K+ has reached a point where it's
contributing a little more to the game here and balancing out that Na+ is doing.
• The undershoot phase is where Na+ is going back to its normal level, which doesn't explain
Lectures Page 21
• The undershoot phase is where Na+ is going back to its normal level, which doesn't explain
why it would make the cell membrane go even more negative than it used to be. What's
happening is that it's taken a while for K+ to return to baseline. K+ is still pushing things out
with a positive charge out of the cell which makes the cell negative.
Lectures Page 22
3 - Voltage-Dependent Membrane Permeability
Sunday, 13 March 2022 5:11 PM
Lectures Page 23
Lectures Page 24
Lectures Page 25
• Hyperpolarisation - going from a polarized negative state to an even more negative state.
• To hyperpolarizing inputs, nothing interesting happens. Whatever state the cell is in at its
resting potential of -65 mV, it's satisfied being there. You can go further negative and nothing
happens.
• If you go from negative to positive, that is where the interesting behaviour occurs. Most of the
action here occurs for depolarizing inputs.
Lectures Page 26
• As you progress further along by increasing the membrane potential, the transient inward
current gradually becomes smaller, and ultimately disappears.
• If you take the increasing peak values and plot them as a function of the clamped membrane
potential, what you observe is that for very negative clamp values, there's not much of a dip
and not as much of a delayed outward current.
• As you get to appreciably less negative and eventually positive values, you see that the
delayed late component keeps growing with no sign of slowing down, whereas the early
component grows and then tapers off.
Lectures Page 27
• Apparently, a membrane potential of 52 mV kills that component of the current, 52 mV is the
reversal potential of Na+. Another way of saying that this is the equilibrium potential of Na+.
It's the potential at which it would stop flowing across the membrane, or it's very close to the
potential which it would stop flowing across the membrane given its concentration on the
inside and the outside of the cell.
• If you have a normal amount of Na+ on the outside then you get a normal response. If you take
that Na+ away you do not get much of a response. You can change the behaviour of this early
response by manipulating the Na+ concentration. This provides stronger evidence that the
early component is Na+ oriented.
• Another way we can reason that the component is K+ related is by using clever chemistry.
Tetrodotoxin will block the flow of Na+ which disable the Na+ channels from working. Or you
could add tetraethylammonium (TEA).
• If you block Na+ channels, you end up with no early component. If you block K+ channels, you
get the early component but no late component. Now we can clearly associated Na+ with the
early and K+ with the late.
• Manipulating the levels of Na+ manipulates the amplitude and behaviour of the early
component and blocking Na+ blocks the early component and blocking K+ blocks the late
component.
Lectures Page 28
• g = conductance
• The question now is how do we express these conductivity ease selective for Na+ or selective
for K+ as a function of the membrane potential and time.
• The conductivity of Na+ rises quickly. The rise starts at the very beginning of the clamp, and it
happens very fast, and it keeps growing but it doesn't sustain so it reaches higher and higher
peak values but always decays. This means that there is an activation component and an
inactivation component. We're increasing conductivity but it's a transient increase and it shuts
down quickly.
• Whereas for K+ you get more of a sluggish response, and it's much less peaky than the Na+ but
it sustains itself for much longer and lasts longer. In these plots they show no evidence of
decaying.
Lectures Page 29
• We're examining how the conduction of Na+ and K+ behave at membrane potential regimes
experienced during an action potential. If you take all that together, what you observe is that
both Na+ on the left and K+ on the right, you find that they both have a sigmoidal shape.
• This means that at very negative membrane potentials, the conduction is incredibly small. This
goes a long way towards understanding equilibrium because at negative potentials which the
cell is at equilibrium, K+ is much more permeable than Na+, but both of those things are very,
very small in the grand scheme.
• That is, you do not have very permeable ions or very permeable channels near equilibrium
potentials. But as you start to excite that neuron and give it sufficient input, then you get this
regime here.
• The first thing to do is understand conceptually what an action potential is from the
perspective of Na+ permeability and K+ permeability.
Lectures Page 30
perspective of Na+ permeability and K+ permeability.
• That initial fast rise is driven primarily by this Na+ conductivity. At the peak, the Na+
conductivity turns back off and Na+ stops rushing into the cell pretty quickly.
• You have an initial activation of Na+ and that drives the upshoot of this action potential and
then a few things drive this downshoot and the undershoot, and that's going to be the
deactivation of Na+ and an activation K+.
• You give your neuron some current, and what happens is that causes Na+ conductivity to
increase, i.e. opening Na+ channels. Na+ current increases, which is a depolarizing effect. This is
going to drive your neuron into the action potential, and as that's happening you are opening
K+ channels at a slower time scale and that's going to increase K+ current outside of the cell.
• You have a fast depolarizing effect and a slow hyperpolarizing effect, which is going to give you
an undershoot that is going to take a while to get back to its normal state.
• Hodgkin and Huxley built a model that said that an action potential is coming completely from
Na+ and K+ conductance changes.
• After Na+ enters and K+ exits, how do these concentrations return to equilibrium? There's
going to be some flow naturally back through the ion channels, but then you have the Na+ and
K+ pump which has a significant role in returning to equilibrium.
Lectures Page 31
• When Na+ rushes into the cell to depolarize it, there some bleed over. Some passive current is
going to flow to the right and it's going to activate the next sodium channel and then repeats.
It sort of activates its neighbours by virtue of passive flow.
Lectures Page 32
• Oligodendrocytes reach out with some fatty sheath and they wrap up the axons of other
neurons.
• If you have an action potential at Point A, that local bleed over gets all the way to Point B
because there is no leak. The action potential starts there and does not go to its neighbour
because this neighbour has this Myelin sheath without any Na+ channels, so it immediately
gets pushed to the next possible node.
• With this structure, it lets action potentials propagate more quickly when you have a
myelinated axon compared to when you do not.
Lectures Page 33
• Myelinated axons are going to have, on average, higher conduction velocities, which means
how fast does the action potential get from Point A to Point B than their unmyelinated
counterparts.
• There are two ways you can make action potentials faster. You can make the axons big, or you
can myelinate them.
Lectures Page 34
• Action potential is sufficient input to a neuron to make these concentrations of ions change
very quickly due to a very quick change in the conductivity of the ion channels that dictate
their flow. The key players here are the Na+ channel and the K+ channel.
• An intrinsic property of an ion channel is its permeability to a particular ion, or its conductivity
to a particular ion.
• An action potential is associated with a fast rise in sodium conductivity, and the decline in
membrane potential is associated with a decline in Na+ conductivity and an increase in K+
conductivity.
• The basic idea is that when you get current that's injected into a cell, you're going to generate
an action potential. It opens Na+ channels as it happens. As that occurs, more Na+ rushes
inside the cell. This is what generates this depolarizing effect. Eventually, this change in the
Na+ conductance catches up, and Na+ begins rushing out of the cell. So, in one case, you have
positive charge in the form of Na+ going into the cell, and then eventually you have positive
charge in the form of K+ going out of the cell.
• This balances the effect an in fact overwhelms the effect of Na+ eventually and pulls back
down, and the fact that you get pulled down further than you initially started is just the way it
works.
Lectures Page 35
works.
• What we have here is first the demonstration that there's always a transient aspect to the
response that goes negative. This would be an inward current and you have a later aspect
that's less transient and more stable, and that would be an outward current.
• The early dip in the graphs is all about Na+. We know this because the early dip disappears at
the +52mV mark, which is very close to the reversal potential for Na+.
• The rise and fall means that there is an activation aspect to Na+ activation and a deactivation
aspect to it.
Lectures Page 36
• Voltage clamp - a constant electrical potential applied to a cell membrane, typically in order to
measure ionic currents.
• A refractory period is this period immediately after an action potential where the cell
membrane potential is lower than it's resting potential. That means the cell is going to be
more difficult to make that cell fire an action potential than it normally would because it's
hyperpolarized.
Lectures Page 37
4 - Ion Channels and Transporters
Thursday, 17 March 2022 3:29 PM
• Can expose different faces of the transport to the pipette and control and measure it in
different ways. Look into the textbook for patch clamp and write them here
Lectures Page 38
• Saw a transient activation and then a subsequent inactivation of Na+ currents in response to a
sustained input.
• If you look at the Na+ channels, you get the same one under the different conditions a few
times.
• (C) represents the micro whereas (D) represents the macro. (C) is at the individual level, but
once you average them you got (D).
• (E) says that if you average all of these things together and that corresponds to the
macroscopic level and then you look at the peak value of that current as a function of the
amplitude of your voltage, you get this shape in terms of how open is your channel or what's
the probability of getting some current flow as a function of how negative or positive your
memory potential is.
Lectures Page 39
• Comparison of Na+ channel (left) and K+ channel (right). You have the clamp, the microscopic
different ions being measured or one ion channel being measured repeatedly in different
channels, the sum or average of those behaviours and then you've got the macroscopic thing
that you would get from a voltage clamp and then you've got the response the likelihood of
ion permeability or channel being open as a function of member potential on the bottom.
Lectures Page 40
• For the middle diagram: when at rest or when you're sufficiently depolarized, the channel is
closed. But once you turn on that clamp voltage, the channel opens and is permeable or
conductive to Na+. As it stays on for a bit longer, the paddle flips over and blocks the channel.
The channel is open but inactive.
• Underneath the middle diagram: a depiction for how channels initially allow Na+ to cross in
and then gradually close Na+ off. The K+ version, it stays closed, and then the current comes on
and it's slow and stays closed. But eventually the current is on long enough, it begins to open
and then it stays open until the current turns off. When the current turns off, then the paddle
closes.
• Another way of reinforcing that Na+ is a quick response but then inactivates. Whereas K+ does
not inactivate, just has a slower activation response.
Lectures Page 41
• These ion specific channels are made up of these protein ribbons that are all coiled up and
different protein coils are embedded in different parts of the cell membrane.
• The wires are called pore loops which create a little cavity inside the cell wall. You would
expect that because with a cavity, things can get through that cavity.
• The way that these structures become ion specific or selective for particular ions, it's largely
structural in nature. It's largely controlling how narrow or wide. But, there are optimal channel
size for specific ions.
• This diagram is voltage dependent, i.e. how do these channels display voltage dependence?
Lectures Page 42
• You do have things out here in the cell wall that have charged and therefore will move in
response to potential differences between the inside or the outside. That's how you can get
the geometry of these complicated proteins to change. And it's the geometry of these proteins
that allow things into or out of them.
• This figures shows how do the various ion species selective channels compare to each other in
terms of their biochemical organic chemistry makeup.
• They all have pore loops that allow specific ions to be in the middle of them, and whether this
pore is Na+ specific, Ca+ specific or K+ specific is going to have to do with the structural
properties of that pore. So how big is the pore and therefore what's the optimal size ion that
can survive in it, that can not only pass through it but it's stable without being attached to a
bunch of water molecules.
• Overall the structure is quite similar but they're pointing to the heterogeneity and saying that
you can get a lot of different specificities out of this heterogeneity.
Lectures Page 43
• This figure demonstrates the K+ is by far the most diverse. With a hyperpolarizing clamp and a
depolarizing clamp, what you'll see is that each of these is a different type of K+ channel, and
some of them behave like we've seen before, which they are opened in response to a positive
clamp and they don't do anything in response to a negative clamp.
• Getting you to notice that not all K+ ion channels look like the standard model. This is how they
operate in a giant squid axon, but these ion channels are expressed in many areas and
locations within cells and neural tissue and they have a great variety of behaviours and
something similar is true for the other ion channels.
Lectures Page 44
• These diagrams show that there's many more types of sensitivities in these ion channels than
just voltage. They can be sensitive to other molecules, so neurotransmitters for instance, so
things that come from other axons and are released after an action potential. Molecules enter
this nest of subunits stuck onto your channel, in this case a glutamate molecule, which binds
to this complex and changes the confirmations of the transmembrane pore and allow Na+ or K+
ions to go through it.
• The point is that there are ways to make a molecule across the membrane and have the shape
about those molecules and the pores that they define be sensitive to temperature just as you
can with mechanical force.
• Active transporters. Reinforce the idea that they are energy dependent. Energy is usually
exchanged or has to do with phosphoryl or dephosphoryl relation or particular molecules.
Lectures Page 45
exchanged or has to do with phosphoryl or dephosphoryl relation or particular molecules.
• To get an ion transfer against its concentration gradient you need to actively transport it, and
that just means it costs energy, you have to find some way of dealing with this energy cost.
• This figure is explaining to you is that these active transports depend on energy (trough).
They're saying how much Na+ is going through this active transport, how much Na+ is being
transported and if you disrupt ATP synthesis (taking energy away from the system), less Na+
gets through, costing you energy. You need ATP to do this (2) and also K+, because without K+
you lose Na+ transport as well.
• Geometry = function
• When we see a confirmation of change, what we're doing is that we're changing the geometry
and therefore changing its function or functional properties.
• Here you bound a Na+ and then there's a conformational change and you can release the Na+
that you had bound up, but in releasing the Na+ that's bound up, you have to bind up K+ for
that to happen. Once K+ gets wrapped up, it comes back through the other way and will
eventually get you back to this fissure where you release the K+. You can only release the K+ if
you grab more Na+.
Lectures Page 46
• This is an ion exchanger.
• Here you're grabbing Na+ and K+ together and there's this bundle up and careful exchange
with an ATP being consumed. If permeable to one positive charge going in this direction and a
different positive charge going in the other direction, the identity of those charges can be
different.
• If ions are going in different directions they are called antiporters. If ions are going in the same
direction across the membrane in or out, they're called co-transporters.
Review
• A great variety of mechanistic functions and that great variety comes about by geometric
properties/functional properties of these transmembrane proteins and how they manifest
across the cell. Should deeply understand the patch clamp and should understand the figures
associated with the patch clamp and how the probabilistic average of microscopic currents is
what generates macroscopic observations.
• Geometry = function and how ion specificity comes from pore width. There's an optimal width,
an optimal channel size that conspires with the properties of the proteins that are forming
that channel to dictate whether it's going to permit Na+, K+, Ca+, Cl- or whatever.
• How voltage dependence occurs (nothing beyond what is discussed in the chapter)
Lectures Page 47
6 - Vision: The Eye
Saturday, 2 April 2022 2:04 AM
• 3 layers of tissue
• Iris: Coloured region that contains two sets of muscles to adjust pupil size
• Cornea: Transparent tissue that permits light rays to enter the eye
Lectures Page 48
Myopia and Other Refractive Errors
• Emmetropia (Normal): relaxed ciliary muscles result in focused image in the distance
Lectures Page 49
• Fovea - has the photoreceptors that detect different wavelengths of light, called cones and
that's what results in our perception of colour.
• The retina is at the back of the eyeball, and light has to travel through all of these layers before
it arrives at the photoreceptors that detect it. The signals then travel backwards from the
photoreceptors to the bipolar cells to the ganglion cells, all still within the retina.
Lectures Page 50
Structure of the Retina II
Lectures Page 51
---> The physiological scotoma is a blind region where the ganglion cell axons leave the retina in the
optic nerve
Lectures Page 52
• Light activation causes a graded change in membrane potential
• In the dark you have a lot of cyclic GMP and therefore these channels that depend on it are
open, which allows these positive ions to come in which depolarizes the outer segment. At the
same time, you've got some flow of K+ out which hyperpolarizes. But overall, you get a
depolarized cell which releases lots of neurotransmitters in the dark.
• In the light, all of a sudden the photon has decreased your cyclic GMP. These channels that
depend on it can't do anything anymore, and so the positive ions can't get into the cell. But
you've still got this flow out because that's not affected by the amount of cyclic GMP because
these channels don't depend on it. The net effect is a hyperpolarized cell that released less
neurotransmitters.
Lectures Page 53
• Photon ---> absorbed by retina ---> changes the configuration ---> causes change to opsin --->
activates transducin (intracellular messenger) ---> activates phosphodiesterase (PDE) --->
hydrolyses cGMP ---> less cGMP
• When you've adapted to low light, your vision gradually improves because the settings
become more optimal for the particular illumination. Then someone turns on a torch, briefly
saturates your rods, and you have to go through adaptation again.
Lectures Page 54
○ Each photon makes a relatively small change in membrane potential, so you need about
100 photons to make a cone change as much as one photon for a rod. Cones can cope
with a lot more light before they saturate.
○ About rods and cones, cones do not saturate at high levels of steady illumination, so
adaptation is more efficient in cones than rods. Cones actually recover about four times
faster than rods, that's why they don't saturate in high, steady illumination.
• Cones:
○ One cone to one cone bipolar cell - and then straight to ganglion cells: less sensitive but
high spatial acuity
Lectures Page 55
○ You do not know where that photon of light was, because with this distribution, all these
cells have different receptor fields. Because you're pulling all of that information, you
lose some of that acuity. Whereas for a cone, if this cone gets stimulated, you exactly
where it is.
• Rods:
○ Throughout periphery, none in the centre
○ Low spatial acuity
Lectures Page 56
○ This distribution of different cell types explains that you don't have colour vision in the
periphery because you don't have anything that can tell you information about colour
vision. Your brain makes it up.
○ Eccentricities are distance from a centre point. When you're fixating centrally, that's the
central point. And then as you move out, you get greater and greater eccentricity. It's in
degrees of visual angle.
---> The fovea is a region of the retina packed densely with cones that supports high acuity colour
vision
Lectures Page 57
Cones and Colour Vision
• Rods detect a single photopigment (rhodopsin)
• Opsin is bound to the retina inside that disc membrane of the photoreceptor, and it's crucial in
determining what that photoreceptor is sensitive to.
• Colour is always a perception based on the relative reflection of wavelengths of light, not the
absolute values.
Lectures Page 58
• The reason that you most often like a lack of one of these is because the genes that encode for
the opsins that absorb medium and long wavelength are quite similar and are next to each
other on the X chromosome.
• Anomalous trichromats have three different opsins, but they have a different distribution or a
different numbers of the different opsins.
• The different wavelengths of light are absorbed by opsins in your cones at different
frequencies, and it's the relative resulting response that drives your perception.
• We have really complex context effects that demonstrate that the colours we experience can
be either a different colour. When the wavelength of light being reflected are identical or they
can be the same experience even though they're different wavelengths of light.
Lectures Page 59
can be the same experience even though they're different wavelengths of light.
• Basically, what colour we perceive depends on the assumptions that your brain is making
about the illumination, combined with the wavelengths of light that are being reflected from
surrounding areas.
A reminder…
• These ganglion cells responds to changes in illumination in specific locations of space. It's the
change, not the absolute value of light.
Lectures Page 60
• Remember: Photoreceptors reduce release of transmitter when photons are absorbed (i.e. in
light)
• When some bipolar cells have lots of glutamate, they hyperpolarize, so they are less likely to
release glutamate themselves. And then you have some bipolar cells that, like glutamate,
activates them, they depolarize and so they release more glutamate.
• If you are an ON-centre ganglion cell, bipolar cell sends you more glutamate, then you fire off
centre ganglion cells. If you're OFF-centre ganglion cells and your OFF-centre bipolar cell
hyperpolarizes, it's not going to release much glutamate, so you're not going to release much
glutamate and you're not going to fire off any action potentials.
Lectures Page 61
The effect of illumination - how our ganglion cells adapt
• Our ganglion cells have to be able to detect changes over a broad range of general
illumination. They cannot work in isolation and have to work together.
• For Image 2: within your retina, there are circuits that are essentially adjust the gain of
ganglion cells depending on the illumination to make sure they're still sensitive. So if you get
an increase in background luminescence (e.g. the sun comes out on a cloudy day), you get an
adaptive shift in the ganglion cells so that you need more photons to result in the same
number of action potentials heading to the brain.
• Ganglion cell firing therefore reflects the general luminous conditions as well as the specific
stimulation within that receptive field.
Ganglion cells signal luminance contrast - they like edges & boundaries!
Lectures Page 62
t1: Cone with an ON-centre ganglion cell: Light in centre hyperpolarises cell
---> less transmitter released on bipolar cell
---> ON-centre bipolar cell depolarizes (more activity)
---> releases more transmitter
---> ganglion cell fires action potentials
Lectures Page 63
---> hyperpolarizes horizontal cells
---> reduces activity on synapses with central photoreceptor
---> depolarizes centre cone
---> increases transmitter release
---> ON-centre bipolar cell hyperpolarizes (less activity)
---> ganglion cell fires less action potentials
Net effect: stimulation of the surround counteracts the stimulation of the centre = overall less
ganglion response to the light
• The amount of transmitter released by that centre cone cell is not just determined by its own
photoreceptor, but also what's happening in the surrounding photoreceptors through the
modulation by the horizontal cells.
• Not only do our ganglion cells have to modulate depending on kind of illumination, but our
brain is interpreting that information based on the context.
Lectures Page 64
Summary
• Anatomy of the eye
• Accommodation & refractive errors
• The structure of the retina
• The cells types in the retina
• Photoreceptors - rods & cones
• Phototransduction & the biochemical interactions
• Colour (a start anyway…)
• Receptive fields & the centre-surround of ganglion cells
• Adaptation to different levels of illumination
Lectures Page 65
7 - Central Visual Pathways
Wednesday, 6 April 2022 2:33 AM
Lectures Page 66
---> R periphery -> nasal R retina (D) -> L occipital
• There's a monocular portion of visual field on both sides, because your nose blocks part of the
input from the outer periphery as part of space. Then there's the inner region that's seen by
both eyes
What matters most is where the stimulus is in space (not which eye it's being registered by)
---> Need to keep signals from the same location together
Lectures Page 67
Primary Visual Pathway
Lectures Page 68
Visuotopic organisation
Let's review what you now know…by thinking about what happens with different lesions
Lectures Page 69
The other targets of retinal ganglion cells
Lectures Page 70
Active learning platform #4
---> You shine a light in the left eye, which constricts the pupil, but the right eye doesn't change. This
suggests that there is damage:
• Between the left pretectum and the right Edinger-Westphal nucleus or the right ciliary
ganglion or the right pupillary constrictor muscle
Lectures Page 71
Orienting responses
Interim Summary
• Your two eyes have slightly different views
• Information partially crosses at optic chiasm to keep information about a side of space
together, regardless of eye
• We can work out location of damage by looking at the pattern of visual deficits
Lectures Page 72
The layers of the cortex
Neural Responses in V1
Lectures Page 73
Functional maps: neurons are clustered by receptive field and selectivity
Functional imaging reveals orderly mapping of orientation preference in the primary visual cortex
Lectures Page 74
Neurons responding to stimulation of both eyes first occur in primary visual cortex
Lectures Page 75
Magno-, parvo-, and koniocellular pathways
• Parvocellular pathway
○ P ganglion cells = smaller receptive fields, slower conduction, sustained responses,
sensitive to differences in wavelengths
▪ Colour, not motion
• Koniocellular pathway
○ Some role in colour vision (short-wavelength-sensitive cones particularly)
Lectures Page 76
"Seeing" what your magno and parvocellular pathways are showing you…
Lectures Page 77
Let's have a look at that in 'real life'
Lectures Page 78
Example of specialisation in the 'what' (ventral) pathway
Lectures Page 79
Summary
• What does where from the retina to primary visual cortex
○ The decussation at the optic chiasm
○ Retinotopic map of space
Lectures Page 80
8 - Lower Motor Neuron Circuits and Motor Control
Tuesday, 26 April 2022 9:28 PM
Preview
• Chapter 16 - LMN
Lectures Page 81
Electrical Foundations of Movement
Lectures Page 82
Head Transplant
Action Potential
Lectures Page 83
Action Potential
Neuromuscular Junction
Lectures Page 84
• Lower motor neurons are those motor neurons that connect with other motor neurons so
they're either coming out of the brain stem and projecting out towards muscle. These α motor
neurons axons that connect to muscle fibres are what cause movements to occur, and it's this
connection that we call the neuromuscular junction.
Contraction
Lectures Page 85
The Motor Unit
Motor Units
Lectures Page 86
Force and Fatigability
Lectures Page 87
Motor Unit Plasticity
Lectures Page 88
MU Summary
Turning it on
Reflex
Lectures Page 89
Stretch Reflex
Lectures Page 90
γ Motor Neurons
Lectures Page 91
Muscle Spindles and Golgi Tendon Organs
Lectures Page 92
Negative Feedback from Golgi Tendon Organs
Summary
Lectures Page 93
Lectures Page 94
9 - Upper Motor Neuron Control of the Brainstem and Spinal
Cord
Sunday, 15 May 2022 1:56 PM
Preview
• Review: Basic brain terminology and anatomy
• Premotor cortex
Brain Axes
Lectures Page 95
Brain Landmarks
Lectures Page 96
Sensorimotor Control Loop
Spinal Cord
Lectures Page 97
Descending Tracts
Lectures Page 98
Primary Motor Cortex
Lectures Page 99
Lectures Page 100
What do motor maps represent?
Spike-triggered averaging
Premotor Cortex
Broca's Area
Preview
• Overview of sensorimotor integration and the functional role of eye movements in vision
Sensorimotor Integration
Extraocular Muscles
Vestibulo-ocular reflex
What is Cognition?
Neo cortex
Brodmann Areas
Unique Features
Contralateral Neglect
What does the asymmetry of neglect tell us about the neuroanatomy of attention?
---> The right parietal lobe is more important for attention than the left
Contralateral Neglect
"Attention Neurons"
What would you need to check before concluding a patient has a specific deficit for recognising
faces?
•
"Recognition Neurons"
Synaesthesia
Synaesthesia
Summary
We Make Noises
Characteristics of Aphasia
Sign Language
1. Define α motor neurons, γ motor neurons, muscle spindles, and Golgi tendon organs.
• Larger motor neurons are called α motor neurons, whereas smaller motor neurons are called γ
motor neurons. Muscle spindles are specialised muscle fibres are sensory receptors arranged
in parallel with the force-generating striated muscle fibres. Golgi tendon organs are
encapsulated afferent nerve endings located at the junction of a muscle and a tendon. The
Golgi tendon circuit is this a negative feedback system that regulates muscle tension; it
decreases the activation of a muscle when exceptionally large forces are generated and, in this
way, protects the muscle.
2. What are the two types of lower motor neurons and how do they differ?
• Two types of lower motor neurons are found in the motor neuron pools of the ventral horn:
○ α motor neurons: relatively large motor neurons that innervate the striated muscle
fibres that actually generate the forces required for posture and movement (force-
producing)
○ γ motor neurons: relatively small motor neurons that innervate specialised (intrafusal)
muscle fibres and for part of the sensory receptors called muscle spindles. Muscle
spindles provide information about limb and body position, and are essential for
proprioception.
• Most extrafusal skeletal muscle fibres are innervated by only a single motor neuron.
• Because there are many more muscle fibres than α motor neurons, individual axons branch
within muscles to synapse
2. Name the different extraocular muscles and describe/draw how each is responsible for
different eye movements
a. Superior rectus
b. Lateral rectus
c. Inferior rectus
d. Inferior oblique
e. Superior oblique
4. What is the latency of saccades and why are saccades described as ballistic?
a. After the onset of a target for a saccade it takes about 200 ms for eye movement to
begin. During this delay, the position of the target with respect to the fovea (i.e., how far
the eye has to move) is computed, and the difference between the initial and intended
position is converted into a motor command that activates the extraocular muscles to
move the eyes the correct distance in the appropriate direction.
b. A ballistic movement cannot be modified by new information once it is initiated. The
saccade-generating system doesn't respond to subsequent changes in target position
during a saccade. If the target moves again during this time (which is on the order of 15
to 100 ms), the saccade will miss the target, and a second saccade must be made to
correct the error.
6. How does the alignment of visual and motor maps in the superior colliculus support sensor-
motor integration?
a. When an object appears at a particular location in the visual field, neurons are activated
in the corresponding part of the visual map
b. This triggers activity in subjacent neurons in the motor layer that activate upper motor
neurons that move the eye just the right amount to centre the fovea on the region of
visual space that provided the stimulation
1. Describe the 4 main similarities in structure across neocortex areas, and note the major difference
between the association cortex and visual cortex.
• The circuitry of all cortical regions has some common features. First, each cortical layer has a
primary source of inputs and a primary output target. Second, each area has connections in
the vertical axis (called columnar or radial connections) and connections in the horizontal axis
(called lateral or horizontal connections). Third, cells with similar functions tend to be arrayed
in radially aligned groups that span all the cortical layers and receive inputs that are often
segregated into radial bands or columns. Finally, interneurons within specific cortical layers
give rise to extensive local axons that extend horizontally in the cortex, often linking
functionally similar groups of cells.
• The signals coming into the association cortices via the thalamus reflect sensory and motor
information that has already been processed in the primary sensory and motor areas of the
cerebral cortex and is being fed back to the association regions. The primary sensory cortices,
in contrast, receive thalamic information that is more directly related to peripheral sense
organs.
3. What is contralateral neglect syndrome and where are you likely to find the damage responsible
for it?
• The hallmark of contralateral neglect syndrome is an ability to attend to objects, or even one's
own body, in a portion of space, despite the fact that visual acuity, somatic sensation, and
motor ability remain intact. Affected individuals fail to report, respond to, or even orient to
stimuli presented to the side of the body (or visual space) opposite the lesion.
• Importantly, contralateral neglect syndrome is typically associated with damage to the right
parietal cortex (see Chapter 29). The unequal distribution of this particular cognitive function
between the hemisphere is thought to arise because the right parietal cortex mediates
attention to both left and right halves of the body and extra personal space, whereas the left
hemisphere mediates attention primarily to the right; this hemispheric bias is thought to arise
from specialisation of the left hemisphere for language, thereby driving attentive functions
into the right hemisphere. This, left parietal lesions tend to be compensated by the intact right
hemisphere. In contrast, when the right parietal cortex is damaged, there is little or no
compensatory capacity in the left hemisphere to mediate attention to the left side of the body
or extrapersonal space.
4. What is prosopagnosia and where are you likely to find the damage responsible for it?
• One of the most thoroughly studied agnosia following damage to the temporal association
cortex in humans is the inability to recognise and identify faces. After damage to the inferior
temporal cortex, typically on the right, patients are often unable to identify familiar individuals
by their facial characteristics, and in some cases cannot recognise a face at all. Nonetheless,
such individuals are perfectly aware that some sort of visual stimulus is present and can
describe particular aspects or elements of it without difficulty.
5. What is the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task and where are you likely to find the damage in a patient
who struggles with this task?
• In this test, the examiner places four cards with symbols that differ in number, shape, or
colour before the subject, who is given a set of response cards with similar symbols on them.
• On the basis of the synaesthetic colours they see, they can segregate targets from
backgrounds, they can group targets in apparent motion displays, and they show the Stroop
effect - the slowed reaction time that everyone exhibits when the printed ink and the spelling
of a colour word are at odds.
3. The cells that supports the activity of neurons but do not themselves participate in electrical
signalling.
a. Glial cell
5. A channel that becomes inactive during the overshoot phase of the action potential
a. Na+ voltage-gated channel
8. The slow opening of K+ voltage-gates channels during depolarisation allows for this type of
current.
a. A late outward current
12. A type of bipolar cell that hyperpolarises when light is shone on the centre of its receptive
field.
a. An OFF-centre bipolar cell
15. A multisensory midbrain structure that coordinates head & eye movements towards targets.
a. The superior colliculus
18. The recruitment of motor units in size order based on the amount of force needed.
a. The size principle
20. Percentage of pyramidal tract fibres that decussate to form the lateral corticospinal tract.
a. 90%
22. Comprised of an alpha motor neuron and the muscle fibres it innervates.
a. A motor unit
23. Motor maps over-represent body parts responsible for finer movements (relative to larger
body parts responsible for coarser movements)
a. Cortical magnification
25. When the eye is adducted, this muscle is primarily responsible for elevating the eye
a. The inferior oblique
28. A spinal injury on this area could cause paralysis in the hands
a. Lateral ventral horn
30. A region of the frontal lobe that mediates decision making and planning
a. The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
34. The emotional valence of speech, made using intensity, pitch, rhythm etc.
a. Prosody
36. Tactile objects recognition in split-brain patients confirmed this feature of language control.
a. Language lateralisation
b. Na+ conductance increases because the voltage-gated sodium channels have opened,
causing an influx of Na+.
e. The cell returns to its usual resting membrane potential (because the K + in and out of
the cell find their electrochemical equilibrium again).
2. Explain the structural organisation of the retina. Then list some of the main
structural/functional differences between rods and cones.
○ Rods:
▪ There is only one type for shades from light to dark.
▪ Optimal in darker light conditions.
▪ Narrower than cones
▪ Majority of them are in the periphery
▪ Many rods connect to one bipolar cell
○ Cones:
▪ There are 3 different types that respond to different light frequencies, resulting in
colour vision.
▪ Optimal in brighter light conditions
▪ Wider than rods
▪ Majority in the foveal region
▪ They have a one-to-one connection with bipolar cells.