Further Development of A Measure of Perceived Envi
Further Development of A Measure of Perceived Envi
net/publication/237398120
CITATIONS READS
209 4,296
3 authors:
Peter Bowler
University of California, Irvine
175 PUBLICATIONS 1,700 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Florian G Kaiser on 07 January 2014.
Further Development of a
Measure of Perceived
Environmental Restorativeness
Terry Hartig
Florian G. Kaiser
Peter A Bowler
May 1997
tel: 026-14 77 00
fax: 026-14 78 02 Gävle, 1997
Further development of a measure of
perceived environmental restorativeness
Terry Hartig
Institute for Housing Research
Uppsala University, Sweden
Florian G. Kaiser
Department of Psychology
University of Trier, Germany
Peter A. Bowler
Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology
University of California, Irvine, USA
May, 1997
2
ABSTRACT
Restorative environments enable the renewal of cognitive resources needed for effective
functioning. Conceivably, any environment can aid this process for someone at sometime,
though a moment’s reflection tells us that some environments are much more likely than
others to be sources of restorative experiences. Indeed, some environments are stereotyped
through reference to their high restorative potential. For some, the stereotypical restorative
environment is the home, the place one retreats to at the end of a long work day. For others, it
is a campsite beside a lake in the mountains, a place where one is far away from the traffic,
noise, and crowds of the city. Yet such places are not universally regarded as places for
restoration. For some, the residence is just another workplace or the setting for tense
interpersonal relations, and the lakeside campsite is associated with coldness, biting insects,
bad food, and poor sleep. Even for those who regularly seek them out for restoration, the
restorativeness of such places may well diminish over periods of time spent in them; after two
weeks in the mountains, the city may regain some appeal. So it is: The restorative potential
seen in a place varies from one person to another, and over time for any one person.
No one environment, then, is restorative for all people at all times. In fact, no one
environment can be restorative for any one person all of the time. Restoration is a process
tied to some preceding deficit or deficits, to some antecedent condition from which one
3
recovers. As such, it is a process that can eventually be completed; the relevant psychological
resources can eventually be renewed, something like equilibrium can be regained. When that
has happened, the person-environment transaction by definition can no longer serve
restoration. An on-going experience in the given environment may still be psychologically
beneficial, but the benefits are other than restorative. Or the on-going experience there may
lead to the depletion of psychological resources.
Rather than focusing on any one environment, theories concerned with restorative
environments emphasize certain characteristics or qualities of the transactions between a
person and the environment (Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989; Ulrich, Simons, Losito, Fiorito, Miles,
& Zelson, 1991). These qualities are to be found to some greater or lesser extent in all person-
environment transactions, restorative and otherwise. There are reasons to expect that these
restorative qualities will be more characteristic of experiences in some environments than of
experiences in others, reasons which conform to some extent with stereotypic notions
regarding the overall restorative quality of those places. Most notably, evolutionary
assumptions common to two well-known restorative environments theories (e.g., Kaplan &
Kaplan, 1989; Ulrich et al., 1991) provide a basis for the hypothesis that natural
environments will, in general, be disproportionately restorative in comparison to built
environments. Still, theories about restorative environments do not make absolute statements
regarding the restorativeness of any one environment or type of environment over time.
The present study is part of an on-going effort to develop a valid measure of the restorative
potential in given environments. The measure is based on attention restoration theory, an
approach to understanding restorative environments that has been advanced primarily by
Stephen and Rachel Kaplan (Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989; Kaplan, 1983, 1995; Kaplan & Talbot,
1983).1 Attention restoration theory sets out theoretical constructs that are useful for
characterizing a broad range of possible person-environment transactions that may proceed
over extended periods of time. The theory builds on assumptions about the evolution of
human cognitive capabilities in natural environments and the significance of the resultant
constraints in those capabilities for functioning in contemporary environments. It starts from
James’ (1892) distinction between two forms of attention, which the Kaplans refer to as
4
directed attention and fascination. Most significant in this distinction is the assumption that,
whereas directed attention requires effort, fascination is effortless. Functioning in
contemporary environments typically makes demands on the capacity to inhibit distractions, a
capacity that is basic to directed attention. Prolonged use of directed attention in functioning
diminishes this capacity, and the ability to further direct attention consequently weakens. The
resultant directed attention fatigue may become manifest not only in performance decrements
such as decreased helping behavior and increments in accidents, but also in negative
emotions, irritability and decreased sensitivity to interpersonal cues. Restoration of the
directed attention capability requires entering a situation in which functioning does not
involve demands on directed attention but can instead rely on fascination.
Being away implies movement to some other situation. That situation is less likely to be
restorative if it lacks certain qualities. One of these, extent, is treated by the Kaplans (1989) as
a function of connectedness and scope. Connectedness refers to the relatedness of the
immediately perceived environmental elements or features, both to one another and, as a
whole, to some larger organizational structure, such as a mental representation of the area.2
Scope refers to the scale of the domain in which the perceptual and organizational activity
takes place. It can encompass the immediate surroundings and areas that are out of sight but
imagined. Conceptual domains such as intellectual problems and imaginary worlds can also
be described in terms of their scope. Extent thus relies on the structuring of perceptual and
conceptual elements and the scale of the frame of reference to which the organized elements
might in turn be related. As the relevant frame of reference may exist in a conceptual or
imaginary domain, extent can be experienced through intellectual activities, and not only in
physical environments.
If one is away from everyday routines and demands on directed attention, fascination can
more readily come into function. Fascination, or effortless attention, can go toward particular
contents and events and can also be engaged in processes of exploring and making sense of
an environment, processes that are essential to building a sense of extent. However, the
relationship between fascination and restoration has some nuances. People can be fascinated
by events that may have negative implications for their mental and emotional states, such as
violence. Also, fascination can sometimes preclude a focus on that which the particular
situation demands one attend to. The term ”soft fascination” has been used to represent an
5
experience of moderate fascination with aesthetically pleasing stimuli (Kaplan & Kaplan,
1989). Thus, fascination may have pleasantness and intensity dimensions as well as a
functionality dimension. Although soft fascination is assumed to be most conducive to
restoration, restoration may be advanced by more intense fascinations, particularly those that
fit in or contribute to a sense of extent.
A fourth construct in attention restoration theory is compatibility. The term refers to the
match between the person's goals and inclinations, environmental demands, and the
information available in the environment for the support of intended and required activities
(Kaplan, 1983). This formulation holds that a person's activity in an environment is a function
of personal intentions as well as environmental dictates. The potential for engaging in an
activity is seen as a function of the information available to support that activity. When the
supporting patterns of information are interesting and contribute to a sense of extent,
compatibility will be high. In short, compatibility exists in situations in which what the
person wants to do matches with what the environment demands and supports.
Attention restoration theory offers a set of constructs that can help us understand the
restorative experiences of purposive individuals. These constructs have been in the focus of
efforts to develop a measure of restorative qualities perceived in environments, the Perceived
Restorativeness scale. The validity of the Perceived Restorativeness scale (PRS) has been
assessed in a series of four previous studies (Hartig, Korpela et al., 1996). Those studies gave
preliminary indications of adequate reliability and validity for the PRS, but raised new
questions or left key questions unanswered. The primary question concerns the factor
structure of the instrument. In those studies, the same four sites were evaluated by American,
Swedish, and Finnish students. The sites were located at different positions on two
theoretically meaningful dimensions (natural-built; outdoor-indoor), and various means were
used to present the sites to subjects (on-site, video, photographic slides). Analyses of
evaluations were then completed for each of the sites in each study. Looking across sites and
studies, the pattern of results showed generally adequate to strong internal consistencies for
four subscales composed of items intended to represent the theoretical constructs in the
attention restoration framework. However, instead of a steady correspondence between a
priori subscales and empirical factors, factor analyses showed the intended being away,
fascination, and compatibility items aligning in various combinations with three of the
empirical factors, which tended to have moderate to strong correlations. In contrast, the
remaining empirical factor consistently was defined by the items intended to represent the
extent construct. Following up on these results, the evaluations were subjected to an
additional analysis in which two rather than four factors were specified for extraction prior to
oblique rotation. Looking across sites and studies, the intended being away, fascination, and
compatibility items were consistently aligned with one empirical factor and the intended
extent items with the other. These two factors tended to be weakly correlated (see also
Korpela & Hartig, 1996). In short, then, the results raised the question of whether evaluations
obtained with that version of the PRS are best represented in terms of (a) two orthogonal
factors (2-factor model), with evaluations of being away, fascination, and compatibility
loading on a General Restorativeness factor; (b) a second-order factor model with loading of
6
first-order being away, fascination, and compatibility factors on a second-order general factor
(second-order factor model); or (c) four factors (4-factor model), which would be largely
consistent with attention restoration theory but for the obliqueness of some factors.
An operational explanation for the obliqueness in the four-factor solution referred to the use
of words that would have confounded preference with the different restorative factors or that
simultaneously tapped into the whole set of restorative factors (S. Kaplan, personal
communication, October 30, 1995). For example, the word ”good” in one of the intended
being away items - ”Spending time here gives me a good break from my day-to-day routine” -
may have forced subjects to also make evaluations of the potential for fascination, extent,
and/or compatibility. Another explanation of this type was offered for the consistency with
which the intended extent items had defined a separate factor, whether in the 4- or 2-factor
solution. The relative independence of this factor, it was suggested, owed to the negative
wording of all four of those items (T. Herzog, personal communication, October 12, 1995; S.
Kaplan, personal communication, October 30, 1995). Since the content of those items was
most in keeping with a hypothesized coherence aspect of extent (and the subscale accordingly
referred to as the Coherence subscale), there was in any case inadequate sampling of items
from the domain of the extent construct.
Although the factor analytic results prompted the detection of problems with the PRS, other
results from the four studies were more encouraging with respect to reliability and validity.
Composite scores based on the 2-factor solution consistently correlated in predicted ways
with subscale scores from another environmental evaluation measure and an emotional state
measure. Also, the environments that had been evaluated could be reliably differentiated on
the basis of the composite scores, and in ways that had been predicted on theoretical grounds.
That the pattern of between-site differences in Coherence ratings was not the same as that
obtained with the General Restorativeness scores, but differed in reasonable and interpretable
ways, suggested that the distinctiveness of the Coherence factor in the factor analytic results
did not owe only to the negative wording of its constituent items.
Results obtained by Korpela and Hartig (1996) gave another view on the psychometric
properties of the preliminary PRS. In this study subjects evaluated favorite and unpleasant
places of their own designation, and also the central square of the town in which they lived.
These subjects were the same as those in the fourth study reported by Hartig, Korpela et al.
(1996), and they evaluated the central square and the favorite and unpleasant places before
evaluating the four sites of interest in the studies reported by Hartig, Korpela et al. (1996).
The central square and the favorite and unpleasant places were evaluated on the basis of
memory or imagination; thus, with the exception of the central square, the sites were the same
across subjects only in terms of their presumed personal significance. Multivariate analyses
paralleled those of the validation studies. Internal consistency among items in the a priori
being away, fascination, coherence, and compatibility subscales was again generally adequate
to strong. In contrast to the factor analytic results of Hartig, Korpela et al. (1996), a more
consistent 4-factor solution was obtained in which there was closer correspondence between
theoretical and empirical factors. Given this, and in light of the exploratory character of the
7
study, further analyses were based on composite scores for the four a priori subscales. These
analyses, like those of Hartig, Korpela et al. (1996), spoke to the sensitivity of the PRS to
meaningful differences between environments, and indicated criterion validity with respect to
the self-reported emotional states that subjects associated with the given environments.
In sum, previous studies have given some indication of reliability and validity for the PRS,
but have raised questions about its factor structure and have prompted critical comments on
the construction of some items. In an effort to further develop the PRS, the present study
takes up these issues in several ways.3 First, some items were revised. Second, negatively
worded items were included for constructs other than coherence. Third, in an attempt to better
represent the extent construct, items were included to represent legibility. A construct often
used in related environmental evaluation research, legibility refers to the possibilities one sees
in an environment for staying oriented and making sense of the surroundings as one proceeds
further (S. Kaplan & R. Kaplan, 1982; Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989). It was a point of discussion
whether legibility plays a larger role in extent than it does in compatibility (T. Herzog,
personal communication, October 12, 1995; S. Kaplan, personal communication, October 30,
1995). Finally, confirmatory factor analyses enabled an empirical comparison of the three
candidate models: 2-factor, 4-factor, and second-order.
METHOD
Subjects. The total sample consisted of 488 students, mostly either social ecology or biology
majors, recruited from one lower-division and two upper-division courses at the University of
California at Irvine. The mean age of participants (44.4% male) was 21.4 years (range: 17-
50). For reasons to be explained below, the analyses of primary interest are based on a
subsample (n = 313).
Study site. In contrast to the previous PRS development studies, each of which involved
evaluation of several sites expected to differ in perceived restorative potential, the present
study involved evaluation of only one site. The San Joaquin Marsh Reserve is a 202 acre
remnant of the freshwater marsh system that once covered much of what is now Orange
County, California. It is bounded on three sides by freeways, surface streets, and other
development, and on the fourth by a flood control dike which contains a stream that runs
through agricultural and residential areas before emptying into a coastal estuary. Because
seasonal flooding of this stream no longer passes through the marsh, vegetation that formerly
had been flushed out on a regular basis has been allowed to collect, and areas that had been
open water are now filled with cattails and reeds. The marsh is also bisected by a berm on top
of which a major traffic artery is situated.
Given its particular characteristics, it was expected that this site would elicit evaluations in
the middle of the range of restorative potential. The density and uniformity of the vegetation,
the extent to which the vegetation limits views, the extent to which commercial buildings are
present in and enclose the longer views that are available, and the audibility of automobile
traffic and overflights into a nearby airport were all reasons to expect that the marsh would
8
receive middle-range evaluations. Such evaluations would be relatively low for natural
environments to which the subjects would ordinarily have had access, which include some
exceptional beach and mountain areas. These characteristics of the marsh also gave reason to
expect considerable variability in evaluations.
Measures. The Perceived Restorativeness Scale (PRS) described by Hartig, Korpela et al.
(1996) consisted of 16 items designed to tap the factors being away, fascination, coherence,
and compatibility. In the present study, those 16 items, some slightly revised, were joined
with 15 new items designed for the scale development purposes described earlier; that is, new
items were added so that some of the theoretical constructs might be better represented, and
some of those items were worded negatively. Those items ultimately retained for the present
version of the PRS are given in the Appendix. Responses were made using a 7-point scale to
indicate the extent to which the given statement described their experience in the given
setting (0 = Not at all; 6 = Completely).
Procedure. All participants filled out a questionnaire containing the PRS and other items
during a single class period, either on location in the marsh or in a lecture hall. For those
students who completed the questionnaire in the lecture hall, evaluation of the marsh with the
PRS was aided by projecting a color slide of the marsh onto a large screen.4 The
questionnaire required about 35 minutes to complete.
RESULTS
The PRS measures used in the present study were derived with a series of exploratory
principal factor analyses (PFA). The first included only the 16 items (some slightly revised)
from that version of the PRS used in the previous studies. As in the previous studies, the
extraction of four factors was specified and communality estimates were derived iteratively,
using the highest correlation of each variable with any other variable as a starting value.
However, in contrast to the oblimin rotation used in the previous studies, the final solution
was varimax rotated to maximize the operational distinctiveness of the concepts; the possible
obliqueness of the theoretical constructs was a question for the confirmatory factor analyses.
The loadings and item communalities are given in Table 1; the numbering of the items
corresponds to that given in the Appendix. The pattern of factor loadings from this analysis
shows close correspondence between a priori subscale membership of the items and their
loadings on the four empirical factors. Consistent with the results of Hartig, Korpela et al.
(1996), the intended being away, fascination, and compatibility items had cross loadings
which pointed to the empirical relatedness of the factors defined by those items. The solution
accounted for 57.6% of the variance in evaluations.
The next step in the derivation of PRS measures involved a series of three PFA. These were
meant to strengthen existing subscales through the addition of new items. All three analyses
were carried out as the analysis above with respect to the extraction of factors, derivation of
communality estimates, and rotation of the final solution. The first analysis included the 16
initial PRS items plus the 15 new items. It marked two of the new items for exclusion from
9
Factor
A priori Subscale Item # I II III IV h2
Table 1. Factor loadings and item communalities for the 16 original Perceived
Restorativeness Scale (PRS) items.
Note: The item numbers correspond to those given in the Appendix. An item´s
highest loading is given in bold faced type.
further analysis; both had loadings of less than .30 in the unrotated matrix. The second
analysis excluded those items and marked three more new items for elimination due to
mismatch between theoretical intent and empirical result. The third analysis then included the
26 remaining items; the four factor solution accounted for 52.6% of the variability in
evaluations. The loadings and item communalities are given in Table 2; the numbering of the
items corresponds to that given in the Appendix. Note that the results show the intended
legibility items aligning with Compatibility rather than with Coherence. Thus, rather than
combining them with the Coherence items to represent extent, those items were joined with
the Compatibility items for this study. Similarly, three of the intended compatibility items had
their strongest loadings on the Fascination factor. Because those items more frequently
10
Factor
A priori Subscale Item # I II III IV h2
loaded on a Compatibility factor in previous studies, and in the present study had substantial
secondary loadings on the Compatibility factor, they also were used to represent
Compatibility.
Two composite scores apiece were calculated for the Being Away (BA), Fascination (FA),
Coherence (CO), and Compatibility (CM) subscales, for use in the confirmatory factor
analyses to follow.5 Each composite score was the average of two or more of the items from
the given subscale. The division in terms of the number of items contributing to each
composite score was as even as possible.6 For some subjects the composite score could not be
calculated because of a missing value for one or more of the constituent items; for these
cases, the mean based on the remaining subjects’ scores was imputed. The number of such
cases never exceeded 1.2% of the sample.
The next step in analysis involved a hierarchical set of three confirmatory factor analyses
(CFA) of the data from the revised scale. These directly tested the fit of the data to the
different models suggested by theory and the results of Hartig, Korpela et al. (1996). The
models were as follows: (1) a 2-factor model in which being away, fascination, and
compatibility indicators load on one factor and coherence indicators on the second; (2) a
second-order factor model in which the relatedness of Being Away, Fascination, and
Compatibility factors is driven by a second-order factor; and (3) a 4-factor model which
consists of separate but oblique Being Away, Fascination, Coherence, and Compatibility
factors.
The data used in the CFA were from a subset (n = 313) of the initial sample. The particular
reduction in the number of cases was made to bring the set of CFA into agreement with the
structural equation modeling to be done as part of the larger study (see Note 3); poor
estimates of a key variable, General Ecological Behavior (GEB), were obtained for some
subjects, so the structural equation modelling was carried out only with those subjects for
whom an adequate estimate was obtained (for further details, see Kaiser & Wilson, 1997). It
is not certain whether the factors that might have affected the reliability of the GEB measure
(e.g., hasty responding, homogeneity of the sample) also would have affected evaluations
provided with the PRS items; use of the subsample is assumed by us to be a conservative
approach to analysis.7 The covariance matrix used in the confirmatory factor analyses is given
in Table 3, along with descriptive statistics for the various composite scores.
The CFA testing the 2-factor model (Model 1) produced the following fit statistics: χ2 =
258.97, df = 19, p = .0001, non-normed fit index (NNFI) = 0.77, root mean square error of
approximation (RMSEA) = 0.20. The CFA testing the second-order model (Model 2)
produced the following fit statistics: χ2 = 40.38, df = 17, p = .001, NNFI = 0.98, RMSEA =
0.07. The CFA testing the 4-factor model (Model 3) produced the following fit statistics: χ2
= 21.20, df = 14, p = .097, NNFI = 0.99, RMSEA = 0.04. Model 2 provides a better fit to the
data than Model 1 (∆ df = 2, ∆ χ2 = 218.59, p < .001), but Model 3 provides a better fit than
Model 2 (∆ df = 3, ∆ χ2 = 19.18, p < .001). Model 3 is also more satisfying from a theoretical
perspective. The reliability parameters and correlations between the factors for this model are
12
given in Figure 1. It appears that although a model with four separate factors is more in
keeping with the data than the 2-factor or second-order models, the Being Away, Fascination,
and Compatibility factors are nonetheless highly correlated.8
Covariance Matrix
CO2 313 3.88 1.39 0.26 0.22 0.19 0.15 1.03 1.94
CM1 313 3.69 1.12 0.94 0.91 0.85 0.67 0.05 0.00 1.26
CM2 313 3.77 1.01 0.76 0.72 0.69 0.61 0.07 0.12 0.93 1.03
Table 3. Descriptive statistics for the Perceived Restorativeness Scale (PRS) variables and the
covariance matrix used as input for the confirmatory factor analyses.
DISCUSSION
Studies reported by Hartig, Korpela et al. (1996) gave some indications of reliability and
validity for an initial version of the Perceived Restorativeness scale (PRS), but raised
interrelated questions about its underlying factor structure, the construction of some items,
and the adequacy with which the given items represented their target theoretical constructs.
Such questions need to be resolved in order to maximize the value of the PRS as a tool for the
development as well as application of restorative environments theory.
To help address these questions, evaluations of a freshwater marsh were obtained from
university students using a revised version of the PRS. The current version of the PRS
included previously used items that had been revised to eliminate problematic word choices
and some additional items for better representation of target constructs. Also, confirmatory
factor analyses were used to assess the suitability of different models relating the measured
14
variables to the target constructs specified in attention restoration theory. These analyses
indicated that a 4-factor model consistent with attention restoration theory fit the data
obtained with the revised PRS better than the 2-factor model and 2-factor higher-order model
that emerged as alternatives from the analyses reported by Hartig, Korpela et al. (1996).
Exploratory factor analyses used to establish indicators for use in the confirmatory factor
analyses shed some light on two additional issues. The first concerns both the representation
of the extent construct and the consistency with which the items used to represent extent had
defined a separate factor in the analyses of Hartig, Korpela et al. (1996). The extent items
used in those studies were in fact more accurately considered as representative only of the
coherence aspect of extent. Moreover, they represented coherence in negative terms. That
they were the only negatively worded items in the earlier version of the PRS suggested an
operational explanation for the consistency with which those items defined a separate factor,
no matter whether the extraction of two or four factors had been specified. Those studies had
also provided evidence that the operational explanation was not sufficient, but inclusion of
negatively worded items for other constructs was viewed as a step that could be taken to
ameliorate lingering doubts. The use of negatively worded items to represent Fascination in
the present version of the PRS offers additional evidence that the operational explanation is
insufficient. Specifically, as shown in Table 2, Items 11 and 13 have loadings greater than .35
on the Coherence factor, the only non-Coherence items with loadings of that magnitude. Yet
their highest loadings still are with the Fascination factor. This suggests that negative wording
alone cannot explain the distinctiveness of the Coherence factor as seen in this and the
previous studies.
The second issue also starts from concern for the representation of extent. In the effort to
more adequately sample items from the domain of the extent construct, and so to represent
more than its hypothesized coherence/connectedness aspect, items were included to represent
legibility, a concept drawn from Kaplan and Kaplan’s (1982) theoretical framework for the
prediction of environmental preferences. Since it had been a matter of discussion as to
whether legibility plays a larger role in extent than it does in compatibility (T. Herzog,
personal communication, October 12, 1995; S. Kaplan, personal communication, October 30,
1995), it was of interest whether these items would align with the proposed coherence items
or with the proposed compatibility items. In the present study they aligned with a
Compatibility factor. This is hardly conclusive evidence that legibility is more an aspect of
compatibility than extent, given lingering uncertainties about the adequacy of the PRS
measures for those constructs. Still, it serves as a reminder of the fact that each of the factors
in attention restoration theory is described with respect to more than one key aspect (e.g.,
content and process fascination; connectedness and scope), and a measure that is theoretically
satisfying and complete should represent these various aspects. A related question is how the
various preference predictors might map onto the restorative factors and contribute to such
representation. In any case, researchers planning to use the present version of the PRS should
not combine the present legibility items with the present compatibility (or coherence) items
unless such a combination is indicated by the results of appropriate factor analyses.
15
As with the previous PRS development studies, the present study is limited by narrow
sampling of subjects and environments. Only one environment was evaluated, and by a rather
small and homogenous sample. Future research with the PRS could benefit from the inclusion
of larger numbers of environments and subjects, not to mention the use of non-student subject
samples. The present study simply constitutes another step toward the goal of a valid measure
with both theoretical and practical utility.
NOTES
1. Roger Ulrich (1983; Ulrich et al., 1991) provides another view on restorative
environments that places more emphasis on emotional and psychophysiological aspects of
restoration. For discussions of the similarities and differences between the attention
restoration approach and the psychophysiological stress reduction approach, see Hartig,
Mang, and Evans (1991; Hartig & Evans, 1993; Hartig, Böök, Garvill, Olsson, & Gärling,
1996).
3. The present study was part of a larger study concerned with relations between
environmental attitudes, ecological behavior, and perceptions of environmental
restorativeness within the context of university-level environmental education. This report
only concerns the PRS development issues referred to above, and to the extent possible
excludes details that do not bear on those issues.
4. The fact that only one group of students completed their forms in the classroom
should not be an issue, since they had visited the study site and had most likely driven
through portions of the site at some time in travelling to and from the campus. Furthermore,
Hartig, Korpela et al. (1996) found that mean PRS evaluations of other environments did not
significantly differ when made on-site or with the aid of slide or video simulations.
5. Composite scores based on all of the items for a given PRS subscale are strongly
correlated with their respective factor scores. For the whole sample, minus cases with a
missing value on any item in the factor analysis (n = 458), the correlations are as follows: BA,
r = .91; FA, r = .92; CH, r = .94; CM, r = .82.
7. Composite scores based on all items for a given PRS subscale are strongly correlated
with their respective factor scores in the subsample with reliable GEB estimates, minus cases
with a missing value on any item in the factor analysis (n = 292): BA, r = .98; FA, r = .99;
CH, r = .93; CM, r = .92.
8. A test of this model based on the full sample (n = 488) also yields an acceptable fit to
the data (χ2 = 25.50, df = 14, p = .03, NNFI = 0.99, RMSEA = 0.04). Note that the χ2 statistic
is affected by sample size, which is in this analysis increased by 175 participants over that
involved in the analysis reported in the main body of the results. This increase can be held
partly responsible for the decrease in model fit as represented by the χ2 statistic. Note also
that the NNFI is independent of sample size and does not change in this analysis. This
suggests that the model still fits well. Thus, the model fit for the whole sample concurs with
the findings from the subset of 313.
AUTHOR NOTES
Primary support for the study came through Grant #DUE9554965 from the U.S. National
Science Foundation (P.A.B). For assistance with conduct of the study we are grateful to the
staff of the University of California’s San Joaquin Marsh Reserve and to the Department of
Ecology and Evolutionary Biology at the University of California, Irvine. Preparation of this
article was partially supported by Training Grant #T32 HL07365-14 from the U.S. National
Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (T.H.) and Fellowship #8210-040207 from the Swiss
National Science Foundation (F.G.K.). We thank Gabriel Magassy and Samantha Boltax for
assistance with data preparation and entry.
17
REFERENCES
Hartig, T., Böök, A., Garvill, J., Olsson, T. & Gärling, T. (1996). Environmental influences
on psychological restoration. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 37, 378-393.
Hartig, T., & Evans, G.W. (1993). Psychological foundations of nature experience. In T.
Gärling & R.G. Golledge (Eds.), Behavior and environment: Psychological and
geographical approaches. Amsterdam: North-Holland, pp. 427-457.
Hartig, T., Korpela, K.M., Evans, G.W., & Gärling, T. (1996). Validation of a measure of
perceived environmental restorativeness (Göteborg Psychological Reports, 26:7).
Göteborg, Sweden: Department of Psychology, Göteborg University.
Hartig, T., Mang, M., & Evans, G.W. (1991). Restorative effects of natural environment
experiences. Environment & Behavior, 23, 3-26.
James, W. (1892). Psychology: The briefer course. New York: Holt.
Kaiser, F.G., & Wilson, M. (1997). Assessing people’s ecological behavior: A cross-cultural
measure. Manuscript under review.
Kaplan, R., & Kaplan, S. (1989). The experience of nature: A psychological perspective.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Kaplan, S. (1983). A model of person-environment compatibility. Environment & Behavior,
15, 311-332.
Kaplan, S. (1995). The restorative benefits of nature: Toward an integrative framework.
Journal of Environmental Psychology, 15, 169-182.
Kaplan, S., & Kaplan, R. (1982). Cognition and environment: Functioning in an uncertain
world. New York: Praeger.
Kaplan, S., & Talbot, J.F. (1983). Psychological benefits of a wilderness experience. In I.
Altman & J.F. Wohlwill (Eds.), Behavior and the natural environment (pp. 163-203).
New York: Plenum Press.
Korpela, K., & Hartig, T. (1996). Restorative qualities of favorite places. Journal of
Environmental Psychology, 16, 221-233.
Ulrich, R.S. (1983). Aesthetic and affective response to natural environment. In I. Altman &
J.F. Wohlwill (Eds.), Behavior and the natural environment (pp. 85-125). New York:
Plenum Press.
Ulrich, R.S. (1992). Effects of interior design on wellness: Theory and recent scientific
research. Journal of Health Care Interior Design, 3, 97-109.
Ulrich, R.S., Simons, R.F., Losito, B.D., Fiorito, E., Miles, M.A., & Zelson, M. (1991). Stress
recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. Journal of
Environmental Psychology, 11, 201-230.
18
APPENDIX
The results are based on a version of the Perceived Restorativeness Scale (PRS) comprised of
the 26 items below. Participants made their responses using a 7-point scale to indicate the
extent to which the given statement described their experience in the given setting (0 = Not at
all; 6 = Completely).
The items are grouped by subscale membership (Being Away, Fascination, Coherence, and
Compatibility, respectively):
Notes: (-) an item for which the value must be reversed in coding; (*) the item is from the
earlier PRS and is in its original form; (†) the item is from the earlier PRS but is in a revised
form; (‡) the item was designed to represent legibility.
19
. 74
. 24 . 79
. 75 . 20 . 04 n .s .
B e in g F a sc in a t i o n C o h e r e nc e C o m p a t i b i li t y
A w ay
. 92 . 86 . 93 . 83 . 87 . 67 . 95 . 86
S
1
c
a
le
c
a
S
S
1
2
S
c
a
le
c
a
le
l
1
2
S
c
a
le
c
a
le
1
2
c
a
le
c
a
le
.0
9
.1
3
.5
6
.1
6
.3
0
.2
6
n
.2
6
.s
.
.2
5