Environmental Psychology Matters
Environmental Psychology Matters
net/publication/256835685
CITATIONS READS
586 11,224
1 author:
Robert Gifford
University of Victoria
211 PUBLICATIONS 20,132 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Robert Gifford on 12 November 2017.
ANNUAL
REVIEWS Further
Environmental Psychology
Click here for quick links to
Annual Reviews content online, Matters
including:
• Other articles in this volume
• Top cited articles Robert Gifford
• Top downloaded articles
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
• Our comprehensive search Department of Psychology, University of Victoria, Victoria V8W 3P5, Canada;
email: [email protected]
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
541
PS65CH20-Gifford ARI 9 November 2013 16:15
Contents
THE BEHAVIORAL SCIENCE OF HUMAN TRANSACTIONS WITH
AND WITHIN BUILT AND NATURAL ENVIRONMENTS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 543
PROENVIRONMENTAL CONCERN AND BEHAVIOR . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 544
Childhood Experience . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 544
Knowledge and Education . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 544
Personality . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 545
Values and Worldviews . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 545
Felt Responsibility, Moral Concerns, and Commitment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 547
Frugality, Diversity, and Empowerment Attitudes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 547
Place Attachment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 547
Norms, Habits, and Defaults: Behavioral Momentum. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 547
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Affect . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 548
Demographic Factors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 548
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
Measures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 549
MAJOR AND MACRO THEORETICAL APPROACHES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 550
MESO THEORIES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 550
PROENVIRONMENTAL BEHAVIOR IS STILL
INSUFFICIENT: WHY? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 551
The Main Theories Are Too Exclusive or Too Inclusive . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 552
Psychological Barriers to Proenvironmental Behavior. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 552
Self-Reports Do Not Change Behavior . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 552
Four Ways Forward . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 553
CLIMATE CHANGE AND HUMAN BEHAVIOR . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 553
Mitigative and Adaptive Responses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 554
High- Versus Low-Impact Mitigation Behaviors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 555
Impact-Oriented Versus Intent-Oriented Mitigation Behaviors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 555
Curtailment Versus Efficiency Behaviors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 555
INTERVENTION SCIENCE . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 556
Informational and Communication Strategies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 556
Antecedent Versus Consequence Strategies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 556
Informational Versus Structural Strategies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 557
NATURE: THE CAPRICIOUS RESTORATIVE AGENT . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 558
Measuring the Connection . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 558
Causes and Consequences of the Connection. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 558
Nature Restores . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 559
Restoration Is Not Limited to Nature . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 559
Nature Is Not Always Nice . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 559
PLACE ATTACHMENT AND IDENTITY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 559
Person, Place, and Process . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 560
The Functions of Place Attachment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 561
How Place Matters . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 562
SOCIAL DESIGN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 562
HOME AND NEIGHBORHOOD . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 564
VIRTUAL ENVIRONMENTS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 565
542 Gifford
PS65CH20-Gifford ARI 9 November 2013 16:15
Wherever you go, there you are—and it matters. This is the fundamental premise of environmental
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
psychology: We are always embedded in a place. In fact, we are always nested within layers of
place, from a room, to a building, to a street, to a community, to a region, to a nation, and to the
world. If, instead, we happen to be in a vehicle, an urban park, on the water, or in a wilderness,
we are still somewhere. Person-place influences are both mutual and crucial. We shape not only
buildings but also the land, the waters, the air, and other life forms—and they shape us.
Environmental psychology includes theory, research, and practice aimed at improving human
relations with the natural environment and making the built environment more humane. Consid-
ering the enormous investment society makes in developing and shaping the physical environment,
and the huge current and potential costs of misusing nature and natural resources, environmental
psychology is a key component of human, animal, and environmental welfare. It is essential for
policy-making (Vlek 2000). Environmental psychology matters (Gifford 2002).
Environmental psychologists continue to investigate fundamental psychological processes such
as environmental perception, spatial cognition, social space, human development, and personality
as they filter and structure interactions with the environment. The traditional emphasis of the field
on the built environment has remained stable (Giuliani & Scopelliti 2009), and the recent growth
of the field stems from investigations of proenvironmental behavior, climate change, interactions
with nature, and attachment to place.
Environmental psychology began half a century ago because psychology had rarely extended
its concern to the physical setting of behavior; for the most part, the discipline proceeded in its
investigations as if people acted and interacted nowhere, in a black void. By implication, the physical
locus of existence did not matter, but in reality, of course it does. Obvious as this fundamental
premise may be, and even though the first Annual Review of Psychology survey of the field appeared
41 years ago (Craik 1973), in operational terms the discipline as a whole still does not fully accept
it: Most departments of psychology in 2014 do not include even one environmental psychologist.
Those with two or more faculty members who are primarily dedicated to the field probably can
be counted on one’s fingers.
Despite this severe shortage of landed scientists, research in environmental psychology
has somehow managed to flourish. Two comprehensive handbooks have appeared (Bechtel &
Churchman 2002, Clayton 2012), and a third is in preparation. The number of submissions to
the Journal of Environmental Psychology ( JEP) quadrupled from 2002 to 2012. This explosion of
interest is partially rooted in the global development in the field; JEP received submissions from
over 40 countries in 2012. The 2008 American Psychological Association Presidential Address
was devoted to psychology’s contribution to a sustainable environment (Kazdin 2009).
Perhaps this is because the wider society gradually has awakened to the importance of the
natural environment over approximately the same half-century as the lifespan of environmental
psychology, including its fragility and vulnerability to human actions and its potential for enhanc-
ing human life. Over the same period, the ability of environmental psychology to contribute ideas
and solutions to the design of the built environment has been realized. One gets the feeling that,
perhaps more than in some other areas of psychology, environmental psychologists are driven by
personal conviction to a cause.
This article focuses on key developments in environmental psychology over the past decade or
so, but it does not refrain from reaching further back where necessary. The last broadband ARP
survey of the field was 18 years ago (Sundstrom et al. 1996), which forces the present attempt to
update readers to be much more selective than its author would wish.
Many environmental problems are rooted in human behavior and can thus be solved by under-
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
Childhood Experience
Children who spend time in nature are more likely to engage in proenvironmental behavior as
adults (Cheng & Monroe 2012); this is more true when the time is spent in “wilder” than in
“domesticated” nature (Wells & Lekies 2006). They are also more likely to spend time in nature
as adults (Thompson et al. 2008).
1
Of course, influences at other levels of analysis also influence proenvironmental choices, such as structural barriers, economic
and political factors, and technological advances (Gifford 2008b).
2
Finally, but importantly, we must note that environment-friendly behavior is often undertaken for nonenvironmental reasons,
such as to save money or to improve one’s health (cf. Whitmarsh 2009). In engaging in behavior choices like these, individuals
have been called “honeybees” because, like those insects pollinating fruit trees in the pursuit of a nonenvironmental, self-
interested goal, they inadvertently provide important environmental benefits (Gifford 2011).
544 Gifford
PS65CH20-Gifford ARI 9 November 2013 16:15
what causes habitat destruction) but less knowledgeable about others (e.g., climate change, energy
production, and water quality) (Robelia & Murphy 2012). Making informed proenvironmental
choices obviously depends on having correct knowledge. Even self-reported knowledge, fallible
as it may be, seems to predict proenvironmental behavior reasonably well (Fielding & Head
2012).
Education is also important. Individuals with more education in general are more concerned
about the environment. Education alone often does not lead to more proenvironmental behavior,
but it serves as a priming agent. For example, reading classic environmental books such as Rachel
Carson’s Silent Spring has been associated with more frequent environmental behavior (Mobley
et al. 2010). Some forms of education, such as by peers in a workplace (Carrico & Riemer 2011),
in a classroom where the desired behavior is proximate (Werner et al. 2012), or in teaching
people how to reduce the smoke from their woodstoves (Hine et al. 2011), have been shown to be
effective.
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Personality
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
The Big Five personality factors represent much of the normal personality domain. Openness has
been related to more proenvironmental activities (Fraj & Martinez 2006) and to more frequent
proenvironmental behaviors, but this relation was fully mediated by environmental attitudes and
connection to nature (Markowitz et al. 2012). In a German study, greater openness and greater
agreeableness and, to a lesser extent, more conscientiousness and less emotional stability were
associated with greater environmental concern (Hirsh 2010). Openness, agreeableness, and con-
scientiousness were strongly linked to environmental engagement across both persons and nations
(Milfont & Sibley 2012). Agreeableness, conscientiousness, and less Machiavellianism were related
to more recycling (Swami et al. 2011).
Consideration of future consequences—the tendency to establish and achieve goals and to plan
strategies for meeting long-term obligations—was positively related to engaging in sustainable
behaviors (Corral-Verdugo & Pinheiro 2006, Milfont & Gouveia 2006), including choosing public
transport more often ( Joireman et al. 2004).
Internal locus of control and self-efficacy have been associated with stronger proenvironmental
intentions and behavior, including less use of cars for commuting (Abrahamse et al. 2009), more
recycling in mainland China (Tang et al. 2011) and in Spain (Tabernero & Hernández 2011),
and less electricity use among Danish consumers (Thøgersen & Grønhøj 2010). Locus of control
also seems to moderate the link between one’s values and proenvironmental behavior (Engqvist
Jonsson & Nilsson 2013). In order for values to be expressed in proenvironmental behavior, people
apparently must believe they have some control over events.
However, their relations with proenvironmental behavior typically are weak, so moderating
and mediating variables such as personal norms and beliefs are needed to satisfactorily predict
behavior from values (Nordlund & Garvill 2003).
These environmental values are related to the ways that individuals construe themselves. Peo-
ple with independent self-construal (i.e., individuals who differentiate themselves from others)
tend to have egoistic values and to report being competitive about managing resources; those with
interdependent self-construal (i.e., people who relate to others) tend toward sharing resources; and
those with meta-personal self-construal (i.e., individuals who feel fundamentally interconnected
with all living things) tend to have biospheric values and to report they would cooperate more
in a commons dilemma (Arnocky et al. 2007). Despite these self-reports, in a commons dilemma
microworld, participants with prosocial and proself orientations made similar resource manage-
ment choices (Hine et al. 2009). However, their motives may differ: Proselfs may view harvesting
restraint by others as a chance to maximize their own profit, whereas prosocials may be trying to
maximize the group’s outcome by compensating for what they think might be too much restraint
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by others. This was supported in that proselfs responded to overharvesting by others by increasing
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
their own harvests, whereas prosocials’ harvesting did not increase in response to others’ lack of
restraint.
In terms of political, economic, and technological values, individuals who value free-market
principles, view technology as the solution to environmental problems, and believe that eco-
nomics is the best measure of progress tend to have less environmental concern (Heath & Gifford
2006).
Postmaterialist values typically are held by more affluent citizens who have fewer worries
about the necessities of life; they tend to be concerned with higher-level goals and actions such
as self-improvement, personal freedom, and providing direct input to government. Holding post-
materialist values and political competence is related to an increased interest in environmental
political action (e.g., Oreg & Katz-Gerro 2006), probably because these values are associated with
environmental concern and perceived threat, which when combined with these individuals’ sense
of control, leads to a willingness to sacrifice and thus causes the adoption of proenvironmental
behaviors.
Beliefs about the nature of nature are related to one’s environmental concern. Those who
believe that nature is ephemeral (that it is delicate and fragile and even small disturbances will
have drastic consequences) are most concerned; those who hold nature-benign worldviews (that
nature is very capable of adapting) are least concerned (Poortinga et al. 2003). Another widespread
worldview held by individuals is that threats to the environment are weaker in their own area than
in distant places (Gifford et al. 2009). Egalitarians believe this more strongly, and individualists
believe it less strongly (Lima & Castro 2005).
However, relations between values and environmental attitudes may not be as simple as some
of these findings imply. People have multiple values, and the relations between values must be
considered. For example, appeals to environmental values are more effective in increasing proen-
vironmental behavior than are appeals to self-interest (financial) values (Bolderdijk et al. 2013) or
even appeals to the combination of environmental and financial values (Evans et al. 2013). When
two values conflict, the difference between the pre-existing level of endorsement of the two values
predicts one’s environmental attitudes better than the endorsement level of either single value
(Howes & Gifford 2009). Moreover, values may combine with motivational style to more strongly
predict proenvironmental intentions; for example, the more people hold altruistic and biospheric
values and are self-determined as a motivational style, the more they act proenvironmentally (de
Groot & Steg 2010).
546 Gifford
PS65CH20-Gifford ARI 9 November 2013 16:15
Three other attitudes appear to assist in the understanding of proenvironmental concern and be-
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
havioral intentions. Positive attitudes toward frugality predict intentions to reduce energy choices
(Fujii 2006). Positive attitudes toward sociocultural diversity and biodiversity also predict proen-
vironmental behavior (Corral-Verdugo et al. 2009). If one expects to feel empowered—that is, to
develop a sense of self-efficacy and solidarity—one is more likely to participate in the development
of a proenvironmental program (Maeda & Hirose 2009).
Place Attachment
If individuals have a strong attachment to a place, they probably want to protect it (Scannell &
Gifford 2013). In one study, adding place attachment to the standard values-beliefs-norms (VBN)
model doubled the predictability of whether people would conserve native plants (Raymond et al.
2011). However, place attachment comes in multiple varieties, and not all are equally related to
proenvironmental behavior: Natural place attachment but not civic place attachment appears to
have that connection (Scannell & Gifford 2010a).
group feedback, social comparison feedback, and social norms) across 29 studies found that norms
were the least potent influence on behavior (W. Abrahamse & L. Steg, manuscript under review).
Perhaps this is because the norm-behavior link is not as straightforward as it appears. For
example, what happens if a person is faced with multiple norms, and the norms conflict? When an
injunctive norm conflicts with a descriptive norm, behavior intentions weaken (Smith et al. 2012).
However, other work suggests that norm conflict can actually strengthen perceived environmen-
tal effectiveness if the person has a positive attitude toward the issue (McDonald et al. 2013).
Nevertheless, when people see evidence of counter-norm behavior (e.g., litter) in the presence of
information proscribing that behavior, such as a “Do Not Litter” sign, they often will engage in
antinorm behavior, that is, add to the litter (Keizer et al. 2008).
Norms can become habits, a second form of behavioral momentum. Unfortunately, the obvious
approach to measuring habit—asking about a person’s past behavior—may not be an adequate
way of measuring it (Knussen & Yule 2008). Assessing habits may not be as straightforward as it
appears. A third form of behavioral momentum is choosing the default. Often, people will say they
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
prefer the green alternative but in fact they often tend to (passively) “choose” whichever default
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
is offered to them (Pichert & Katsikopoulos 2008). Clearly, the policy strategy implied by this
finding is to make the green option the default.
Affect
Emotions play a role in proenvironmental concern and behavior. For example, a Swedish study
reports that worry, hope, and joy play a role in recycling (Ojala 2008). Having an affective con-
nection to nature significantly predicts the intention to engage with it (Hinds & Sparks 2008).
Anticipating unpleasant emotions predicts the desire to engage in proenvironmental actions (Car-
rus et al. 2008). On the other hand, positive affect toward one’s pollution-emitting device weakens
one’s support for policy measures that would restrict its use and for the willingness to switch to a
less-polluting device (Hine et al. 2007).
Demographic Factors
Age, gender, wealth, religion, urban-rural residence, and identification with a group have been
related to environmental concern. Older people generally report more proenvironmental concern
or behavior than younger people (e.g., Grønhøj & Thøgersen 2009), but not always (e.g.,
Sardianou 2007).
Gender differences are inconsistent. In some studies, women report stronger environmental
attitudes, concerns, and behaviors than men (e.g., Scannell & Gifford 2013). However, in China,
women are more engaged than men in domestic environmental behaviors (e.g., recycling), but
outside the home (e.g., environmental organization donations) no gender differences are exhibited,
and women express lower levels of concern than men (Xiao & Hong 2010). A possible reason for
this pattern is that health and safety, which are threatened by problematic environments are more
important to women, particularly women with children at home (see, e.g., Dietz et al. 2002).
One generalization is that environmentalists tend to be middle-class or upper-middle-class
individuals. Environmental concern also appears to be related to wealth on the global scale; it
has a clear positive relation with national gross domestic product per capita (Franzen 2003). Of
course, not everyone in wealthier places is environmentally concerned; for example, conservative
white males in the United States clearly are less concerned, on average, than are other US demo-
graphic groups (McCright & Dunlap 2012). Meanwhile, some research concludes that citizens
548 Gifford
PS65CH20-Gifford ARI 9 November 2013 16:15
Empirical research on this issue remains divided. One recent study found that no differences
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
exist between Christians and non-Christians in the perception of general environmental threats
and that Christians judged the threat of genetically modified crops to be more serious than did
non-Christians (Biel & Nilsson 2005). Another study found that Islamic religious teachings are
associated with proenvironmental behavior, thus lending support to the theory that an Islamic
environmental ethic exists (Rice 2006).
People who live in rural areas experience the environment in very different ways from their
urban counterparts; doubtless most inhabitants of rural areas are more in touch with nature. Does
that result in greater or lesser environmental concern or behavior? Again, the results are mixed. In
China, residents of larger cities are more likely to engage in proenvironmental behaviors than are
residents of smaller cities (Chen et al. 2011). However, students in the United Kingdom who had
grown up in rural areas report more positive orientations toward the natural environment than
do urban-raised students (Hinds & Sparks 2008). The anthropocentric beliefs of rural residents
seem consistent with their more direct use of natural resources for human ends.
Finally, and perhaps less intuitive than some of the results discussed above, is the finding
that individuals with a stronger sense of identification with a group report engaging in more
proenvironmental behavior (Dono et al. 2010). However, this depends on which sort of group
one identifies with.
Measures
Many tools for measuring environmental attitudes have been proposed. One compilation includes
14 such measures (Gifford 2007a, chapter 3). Perhaps the most comprehensive recent measure,
built from numerous existing measures, is the Environmental Attitudes Inventory (Milfont &
Duckitt 2010). The Environmental Attitudes Inventory is a multidimensional instrument that
captures the hierarchical nature of environmental attitudes; it is composed of 12 scales that appear
to represent most or all of the main constructs tapped by earlier measures. These include enjoyment
of nature, support for interventionist conservation policies, intentions of personal environmental
activism, support for conservationism if it provides human benefits, confidence that science and
technology can solve environmental problems, fear of ecological collapse, support for using nature
for development, self-report of personal conservation behaviors, beliefs that humans are meant to
dominate nature, beliefs that humans should use nature, beliefs that nature is valuable for its own
sake, and support for population control policies.
Another instrument recently created with the specific purpose of combining and building
upon earlier measures is the New Human Interdependence Paradigm scale (Corral-Verdugo et al.
2008). However, the most widely used measure so far is the revised New Ecological Paradigm
scale (Dunlap et al. 2000).
that attempt to describe the full, complex interrelationship of persons and settings; (e) operant
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
approaches, which adopt a direct problem-solving approach that employs behavior modifica-
tion techniques; ( f ) environment-centered theories, such as the spiritual-instrumental model and
ecopsychology, which emphasize the environment’s own welfare; and ( g) theories that include
such elements as goals, norms, intentions, values, and attitudes. Most of the above might be called
macro approaches; the last are more specific meso-scale approaches.
Within the zone of proenvironmental attitudes and behavior, recent theories vary in scope,
from the meso to the macro. One such macro-scale proposal is the general model of so-
cial dilemmas (Gifford 2006, 2008b). It posits that impactful behavior choices made by in-
dividuals (including those who head organizations as well as the average citizen) often have
a geophysical influence (e.g., weather, extent and accessibility of the resource), occur within
a regulatory context (e.g., policies and pricing), and are influenced by technological devel-
opments (e.g., new drilling methods, factory fishing boats) as well as psychological elements
(motivations, cognitions, norms, interpersonal influences, and decision-making strategies). The
general model of social dilemmas includes downstream consequences of these decisions for
(a) the decision maker (and significant others), (b) the community, and (c) the environment. Finally,
the model recognizes that these outcomes feed their consequences back upstream, influencing the
regulatory context (in particular) but also sometimes influencing the other upstream factors—
geophysical (such as climate impacts), technological, and social factors—in a continuing dynamic
cycle of influence. However inclusive it might be, such a theory is difficult to test, at least as a
whole (although its individual links can and should be tested).
MESO THEORIES
With fewer constructs, meso-scale theories are far easier to investigate. Among these are the
theory of planned behavior (TPB) (Ajzen 2005), VBN theory (Stern 2000), and goal-framing
theory (Lindenberg & Steg 2007). Other, less-specified, notions include the reasonable person
model (Kaplan & Kaplan 2009) and the human interdependence paradigm (Gärling et al. 2002).
These meso-scale theories propose attractively parsimonious accounts of behavior and there-
fore are eminently investigable (e.g., Kaiser & Gutscher 2003). The TPB, for example, proposes
that attitudes toward a behavior, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control predict be-
havioral intention, which predicts actual behavior. However, many studies have identified personal
and social factors that enhance the TPB’s predictive validity, including habit, descriptive norms,
self-identity, and place attachment (e.g., Bamberg & Schmidt 2003, Chen & Tung 2010, Fielding
550 Gifford
PS65CH20-Gifford ARI 9 November 2013 16:15
et al. 2008, Heath & Gifford 2002, Hinds & Sparks 2008, Raymond et al. 2011, Whitmarsh &
O’Neill 2010). Structural inadequacies, such as a lack of availability of recycling facilities3 or
public transport, also constrain proenvironmental behavior (e.g., Heath & Gifford 2002, Steg &
Gifford 2005). Assuming these factors are useful additions to the understanding and prediction of
proenvironmental behavior, they tend to push theoretical thinking back toward the macro scale.
The VBN theory (Stern 2000) predicts, in a chain-like sequence, that one’s values (altruistic,
biocentric, and not egocentric) cause one to espouse an ecological worldview, which leads one to
believe that adverse consequences to the environment can occur, which increases one’s perceived
ability to reduce threats to the environment, which leads to a sense of obligation to act in a proen-
vironmental manner, which culminates in four kinds of proenvironmental behavior (activism such
as participating in public demonstrations, nonactivist behaviors in the public sphere such as letter
writing, private-sphere behaviors such as recycling at home, and behaviors within organizations
such as lobbying for double-sided printing). This sequence has been supported for climate-relevant
behaviors such as the acceptability of household energy-saving policies (Steg et al. 2005), and parts
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
of it have been verified in terms of values and awareness of consequences (Hansla et al. 2008).
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
That is, researchers have shown that each link in VBN’s chain of hypothesized constructs is indeed
predicted by its predecessor.
Goal-framing theory (Lindenberg & Steg 2007) posits that three types of goals influence the
way people process information and act upon it: hedonic (pleasure oriented), gain (self-interest),
and normative (what others are thought to be doing). At any given time, one goal is presumed to
be focal while the others are in the background and might increase or decrease the strength of the
focal goal.
The reasonable person model (Kaplan & Kaplan 2009) proposes that understanding people’s
informational needs in particular settings has the potential to make it easier for people to help
themselves. People are unreasonable when a place does not support their needs for information
and are more likely to be reasonable in environments that do.
The human interdependence paradigm (Gärling et al. 2002) emphasizes the tension between
the drive for human development, which tends to require unsustainable use of resources, and
concern for the environment, which usually implies preservationist or at least sustainable use of
resources. This paradigm reflects the central conflict in commons dilemmas between self-interest
and that of a common resource pool, which has been the subject of dozens of studies (cf. Biel
et al. 2008, Kopelman et al. 2002) that often have used microworld simulations (e.g., Gifford &
Gifford 2000) to investigate the 30 or more influences on cooperation in the commons (Gifford
2007a, chapter 14).
Each of these meso theories certainly includes a part of the truth about proenvironmental
behavior, and their relatively few components allow for convenient testing. However, the price of
simplicity is incompleteness, and excluded influences undoubtedly play a role.4
3
One disturbing recent finding deserves further research attention. When participants in two different contexts had structural
facilities available for recycling, they used more paper than when facilities were not available (Catlin & Wang 2013).
4
See Footnote 2.
species is “defecting,” that is, extracting resources faster than they can replenish. The responsi-
bility for this can be placed with the individuals, households, and organizations whose actions are
contributing to the problem, but environmental psychology as a science can support those whose
job is to create more effective policy by developing improved theory, focusing on the barriers
experienced by people, and conducting studies that emphasize behavior change over self-reports.
statements and overt acts both stem from one root behavioral disposition (Kaiser et al. 2010). If
the attitude-behavior gap does exist, as most believe it does, it probably occurs in part from the
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
dampening influence of structural barriers (one cannot take public transport if it does not exist in
one’s community) and psychological barriers.
A problem with macro-scale theories is that although they are inclusive about the drivers of
human behavior, they are difficult to investigate as a whole through conventional empirical study.
They do, however, serve as big-picture reminders of the human and nonhuman antecedents
and consequences of environment-impactful behavior and therefore can assist policy makers in
understanding more about the antecedents and consequences of their policies.
In sum, one reason that proenvironmental behavior remains a challenge may be that meso
theories are too narrow for policy makers to effectively utilize and macro theories are too broad
for scientists to test. If so, one goal for environmental psychology should be to develop an
intermediate-sized theory that incorporates most of the impactful drivers of behavior but remains
manageable in size and parsimonious.
552 Gifford
PS65CH20-Gifford ARI 9 November 2013 16:15
share only about 20% of their variance. This gap can be caused in part by imperfect memory,
social desirability bias, and the lack of opportunity to observe others’ behavior (e.g., in reporting
household energy use, the reporter will not always have observed the actions of others in the
household). Attempts have been made to overcome this problem by using Rasch models (Kaiser
et al. 2007), although the proposed solution itself depends on self-reports of one’s past behavior,
which may not escape the problem.
be made. Fourth, more meta-analyses that examine the relative potency of the many drivers of
proenvironmental behavior are needed; each individual study is useful but almost always is limited
at least in generalizability resulting from relatively small or localized samples. At least in principle,
meta-analyses reveal something closer to the truth of relations between constructs.
Fortunately, four pertinent meta-analyses have been conducted. The first considered 315 rel-
evant studies and concluded that proenvironmental behavior was most strongly predicted by
knowledge of the issues, knowledge of action strategies, locus of control, attitudes, verbal com-
mitment, and sense of responsibility (Hines et al. 1986–1987). A second meta-analysis, performed
20 years later, confirmed those results for the most part but also concluded that the intention to
engage in proenvironmental behavior mediates the impact of the other personal and social influ-
ences, that personal norms influence this intention, and that problem awareness is a significant
indirect influence on proenvironmental intention; the impact of problem awareness appears to be
mediated by moral and social norms, guilt, and attribution processes (Bamberg & Möser 2007).
A third meta-analysis focused on individuals’ commitment to action; the findings confirmed
that commitment can be effective but called for further examination of the reasons that it works
(Lokhorst et al. 2013). A fourth reported on the relative value of different treatments or interven-
tions for promoting proenvironmental behavior; it concluded that the most effective approaches
employed cognitive dissonance, goal setting, social modeling, and prompts, although as might be
expected, different treatments are more effective for different proenvironmental behaviors, and
combined approaches often work better than single approaches (Osbaldiston & Schott 2012).
In sum, an important goal for environmental psychology is to find a theoretical framework that
is more parsimonious than the macro approaches but more inclusive and therefore more predictive
of environmental behavior than the meso approaches. This model should include more contextual
(extrapsychological) factors, attend to the psychological barriers between concern and action, and
make greater use of objective measures of behavior. Given the bewildering plethora of influences
on environmental concern, intentions, and behavior, more cooperation with other experts is called
for, and more meta-analyses are needed to clarify connections among the constructs and thereby
to contribute to a more viable and powerful account of proenvironmental behavior.
have caused the Earth’s temperature to rise higher than it has been since civilization developed
10,000 years ago (Intergov. Panel Climate Change 2007). Worldwide GHG emissions continue to
rise despite official efforts to raise awareness and many citizens’ efforts to change. Further climate
change probably will result not only in higher maximum temperatures (and therefore more heat-
related deaths), but also in more frequent extreme weather events, a rise in sea levels, an increase
in widespread infectious diseases, and decreases in crop yields and water quality. Climate change
matters.
Current climate change is primarily driven by GHG-emitting human behaviors and therefore
may be largely mitigated by changes to human behavior. However, human behavior is the least
understood aspect of the climate change system (Intergov. Panel Climate Change 2007). Thus,
unfortunately, the main cause of the problem is its least understood element. Understanding be-
havior at the psychological level of analysis therefore is essential, given that the cumulative impact
of individuals’ decisions and behaviors is the key factor driving climate change (Gifford et al. 2011a).
Fortunately, some environmental psychologists have been working on the problem for more
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
than 30 years (Fischhoff & Furby 1983); they have learned much about different behaviors with
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
different impacts and are actively developing interventions (Gifford 2008a, Spence et al. 2008). The
pace of research has accelerated recently, including the American Psychological Association (2010)
task force report and subsequent special issue (Swim et al. 2011). At the same time, recognition
that the wider political and social context must be considered when interpreting the meaning of
attitudes in places with different dominant political ideologies (Räthzel & Uzzell 2009) can and
should be integrated with the climate change models and efforts by other disciplines.
554 Gifford
PS65CH20-Gifford ARI 9 November 2013 16:15
and the latter through its expertise in ameliorating stress and facilitating coping (Swim et al.
2011).
One hope is that people will gravitate from low- to high-impact behaviors, a progression called
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
the spillover effect, via low-impact catalyst behaviors such as recycling. Some evidence for spillover
exists (e.g., Thøgersen & Ölander 2003, Whitmarsh & O’Neill 2010), but sometimes action in
one behavioral domain actually leads to less action in others so that no net positive effect occurs
(Herring & Sorrell 2008).
because they do not require consistent, long-term maintenance of the target behavior (Lehman
& Geller 2004): Once the change is made, the savings are automatic or behavior free. However,
efficiency behaviors may be more subject to losses caused by the rebound effect, the tendency to
overspend energy as a psychological compensation for making a climate-virtuous choice.
INTERVENTION SCIENCE
All the knowledge so far gained can be used in another of environmental psychology’s key roles:
the development, evaluation, and implementation of interventions that target direct and indirect
sustainability and climate-impactful behaviors. Intervention science is not as simple as turning
a switch; in order to accomplish its ultimate goal of behavior change, it must take the social,
political, economic, and cultural context into account; help to design climate-related policies and
regulations; create effective public messages; predict public reactions to proposed policies; provide
explanations for the public acceptance or rejection of new technologies, and comprehend how risks
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
In order to be effective, intervention scientists should make several careful decisions in their
work plans (e.g., Steg & Vlek 2009). They should carefully consider the behavior in question.
Targeted behaviors should have large and negative demonstrated impacts but be amenable to
change. The demographics of the key group for that behavior must be taken into account. The
type of intervention (see below) must be carefully selected. Resources must be used efficiently.
Psychologically important aspects of the targeted behaviors should be considered, such as perceived
costs and benefits, norms and habits, and emotional and moral dimensions. The expected outcomes
also need to be considered: These can include change in the behavior itself, improvements to the
environment, and changes to the person’s own quality of life.
556 Gifford
PS65CH20-Gifford ARI 9 November 2013 16:15
need to become more aware or possess more facts) has been widely criticized (e.g., Kolmuss &
Agyeman 2002). A second antecedent strategy is modeling, in which a key player enacts the de-
sired behavior so as to influence proximate others to follow suit (e.g., Sussman et al. 2013). A third
such strategy is to obtain behavioral commitments from individuals or organizations (e.g., Baca-
Motes et al. 2013). A fourth antecedent strategy uses prompts to change behavior, for example,
by posting a sign at an exit door directing individuals to turn off unused lights (e.g., Sussman &
Gifford 2012).
Consequence strategies aim to change behavior after it has occurred in order to influence its
future occurrence. This family of strategies, including giving people feedback (e.g., new desktop
wireless devices tell householders how much energy they are using at the moment), rewards (e.g.,
rebates for saving energy), and punishment (e.g., fines or even jail for overfishing or poaching),
aims to influence the selected behavior after its performance.
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
The goal of informational strategies is to change the (internal) psychological precursors (e.g., at-
titudes, knowledge, and motivation) of proenvironmental behavior. The goal of structural strate-
gies is to change the (external) physical, technical, legal, or pricing circumstances surrounding the
proenvironmental behavior.
Informational approaches appear to be best suited for easier (i.e., low cost in terms of effort,
money, or social disapproval) behaviors with few barriers (Steg & Vlek 2009). They are somewhat
effective for (a) prompting and eliciting proenvironmental behavioral commitments (Abrahamse
et al. 2005); (b) social marketing, in which an intervention is carefully tailored to the needs and
barriers of a particular group (Abrahamse et al. 2007, McKenzie-Mohr 2000, Thøgersen 2007);
(c) implementation intention strategies, which ask people not only to commit to some behavior
change but also how they plan to do so (see, e.g., Bamberg 2002); and (d ) the provision of descriptive
norm information, that is, telling people what other proximate individuals, such as neighbors or
other hotel guests, are doing for the environment (e.g., Schultz et al. 2007). They may also
be effective in campaigns to increase public acceptance of structural strategies, such as policies
designed to reduce car use (Gärling & Schuitema 2007).
Structural strategies seem to be more effective for changing less-convenient and higher-cost
behaviors (Steg & Vlek 2009), often so that the incentives (or disincentives) render the behav-
ior more (or less) attractive (Thøgersen 2005). Among the main structural strategies (physical
design changes that make the desired behavior easier or more obvious, pricing, rewards, and
punishments), rewards often are most effective (Geller 2002). However, reward-based structural
strategies sometimes last only as long as the rewards are offered, and often they are only effective
in conjunction with a person’s existing behavior-change goals (Gärling & Loukopoulos 2007).
Like efficiency behaviors, to which they are similar, physical forms of structural changes, such as
putting a recycling bin in every office instead of down the hall, have the advantage of changing
behavior in a more permanent way.
Policy versions of structural strategies for increasing proenvironmental behavior (especially
price increases or restrictive changes such as a reduced speed limit) are more acceptable when they
are perceived to be fair, when they are effective, and when they do not seriously infringe on individ-
ual freedom. They are more acceptable to individuals with strong environmental values, those who
are more aware of the problem, and those who feel morally obligated to ease the problem. Policies
that can make proenvironmental behavior seem more attractive are likely to be evaluated as more
effective and acceptable. Finally, policies that promote the adoption of energy-efficient equipment
are preferred to those that seek to reduce the use of existing equipment (Gifford et al. 2011b).
The most effective interventions are tailored to the individual (or household) and to the specific
behavior, take into account the particular barriers, and employ mixed strategies. Combinations
of strategies, such as implementing information, feedback, and social interaction in a group often
are the most effective and durable. In implementing one such combination, 19 of 38 household
behaviors in the Netherlands were changed, and the changes were maintained or increased two
years later (e.g., Staats et al. 2004). In another Dutch study, a combination of tailored information,
goal setting, and tailored feedback was used to encourage households to reduce their gas, electricity,
and fuel use (Abrahamse et al. 2007). After five months, intervention households used 5.1% less
energy, whereas control households used 0.7% more.
capacities (e.g., Kaplan 1995, Ulrich 1984), but its destructive power also compels the study of
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
558 Gifford
PS65CH20-Gifford ARI 9 November 2013 16:15
Nature Restores
Abundant evidence favors the straightforward proposition that nature is restorative (Kaplan 1995).
Nature improves cognitive functioning, productivity, mood, vitality, connectivity with nature,
and speed of recovery in hospital, and it reduces stress and anger. These trends hold for actually
being in nature (e.g., Berman et al. 2008), for merely having some nature (e.g., plants) in a room
(e.g., Raanaas et al. 2011), for seeing a poster image of nature in one’s room or office—at least
for males (Kweon et al. 2008), or even for seeing nature through one’s window (Ulrich 1984).
More-fatigued people report greater restoration than less-fatigued people from walking in a forest
(Hartig & Staats 2006). Green spaces improve the functioning of children with attention deficit
disorder (Taylor et al. 2001).
In some ways, people seem to realize these effects and to expect even more of them. When
asked why they engage in nature activities, individuals report doing so for 10 reasons: They
believe that nature activities will facilitate a sense of cognitive freedom, allow them to simply
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
experience nature, enhance their ecosystem connectedness, escape from stress, offer a physical
challenge, foster personal growth, provide an opportunity to guide others, heighten their sense of
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
self-control, renew social connections, and improve their health (Gifford 2007a).
People rate murals of nature scenes as more restorative than murals of indoor scenes,
particularly nature scenes that include water (Felsten 2009). At the same time, even as they report
being happier after a walk in nature than after a walk indoors, they underestimate the hedonic
benefit they received (Nisbet & Zelenski 2011). This suggests that support for the conservation
of nature, and for spending time in it, would be greater if people realized that they benefit from
it more than they think.
change. The psychology of place, in a variety of guises, has been a very active topic recently in
environmental psychology. Some efforts have focused on defining it, some on discovering its
correlates, some on its antecedents, and some on its consequences. Phenomenologist approaches
emphasize the meaning of place (e.g., Seamon 2012). Theory in the area is developing but remains
uncrystalized (Lewicka 2011).
Place attachment has most often been described as an emotional connection to a place (e.g.,
Brown et al. 2003). For the most part, it is portrayed as a multifaceted concept that characterizes
the bonding between individuals and their important places (e.g., Giuliani 2003). Obviously, place
attachment is rarely attained instantly; residents need to spend time in a place, to hear stories, or
to be part of a spiritual quest centered there (Hay 1998). One grows attached to settings where
memorable or important events occurred (Manzo 2005).
Place identity is most often defined in terms of an overlap with one’s sense of self. It develops
when individuals experience similarities between self and place and incorporate cognitions about
the physical environment (memories, thoughts, values, preferences, and categorizations) into their
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
self-definitions. It has been viewed as stemming from the development of three processes: con-
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
gruity between self and place, fit with the environment, and self-extension (Droseltis & Vignoles
2010).
One view of the relation between place attachment and place identity is that the former evolves,
with time, into the latter (Clayton 2003). Consistent with this idea, natives of a place tend to have
both place attachment and place identity, but people who move to a new place tend to report more
place attachment than place identity (Hernandez et al. 2007), which suggests that attachment
precedes identity.
A few other constructs bear some similarity to place attachment and place identity: sense of
community, place dependence, and environmental identity. Some suggest that sense of place
encompasses the subconcepts of place identity, place attachment, and place dependence (e.g.,
Jorgensen & Stedman 2001) or that it includes ancestral ties, feeling like an insider, and a desire
to stay in the place (Hay 1998).
560 Gifford
PS65CH20-Gifford ARI 9 November 2013 16:15
(Hidalgo & Hernández 2001, Lewicka 2010). The social dimension of place attachment (feeling
attached to the people in the place) may be stronger than the physical dimension (feeling attached
to the built and natural elements of the place), although physical and social attachments both
influenced the overall bond (Lewicka 2010).
Place attachment may be civic or natural. Civic place attachment is focused on one’s community
(e.g., Vorkinn & Riese 2001); natural place attachment is focused on nature (Scannell & Gifford
2010a). Civic and natural place attachments can, and do, predict outcomes differently, which
reinforces the notion that they are distinct constructs (Scannell & Gifford 2010a). The identity
analog of the relation to nature is environmental identity, the inclusion of nature in one’s self-
concept (Clayton 2003).
Other researchers focus on the meaning that a place has for a person. The meaning-mediated
model of place attachment (Stedman 2003) proposes that individuals do not become directly
attached to the physical features of a place but rather to the meaning that those features represent.
In this view, a developed area may symbolize community or an underdeveloped area may symbolize
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
wilderness. The physical aspects are said to constrain the possible meanings a place may adopt
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
and, therefore, physically based place attachment rests in these symbolic meanings.
Places also become meaningful from personally important experiences, such as place-based
revelations (e.g., religious epiphanies in a sacred place) or secular realizations of connectedness
to nature in a wilderness, milestones (e.g., where I first met my lover), or experiences of personal
growth: “[I]t is not simply the places themselves that are significant, but rather what can be
called ‘experience-in-place’ that creates meaning” (Manzo 2005, p. 74). Place attachment seems
to develop from experiences and emotional bonds first established with a place in childhood
(Morgan 2010).
Place attachment can be faith based. Through religion, the meanings of certain places become
elevated to the status of sacred (Mazumdar & Mazumdar 2004). Revered places such as Mecca
or Jerusalem or, on a smaller scale, churches, temples, shrines, burial sites, or sacred places, are
central to many religions, and their spiritual meanings are shared among worshippers.
Third, place attachment is about the processes it involves. Five processes can be distinguished:
Place-related distinctiveness is about knowing one is from A, not B; place-referent continuity
is about perceived similarity between one’s current place and an earlier place to which one was
attached, often one’s childhood home; place-congruent continuity refers to the similarity of the
current place’s climate with that of one’s childhood; place-related self-esteem is about feeling good
in a place, or proud to be living there; and place-related self-efficacy occurs if the place supplies
all or most of one’s needs (Knez 2005).
Kobrin 2001). Perhaps the resolution of this lies in the recent finding that natural place attachment
appears to foster proenvironmental behavior, but civic place attachment does not (Scannell &
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
Gifford 2013).
The physical aspects of a place may suffer if people cling to place meanings that are incompatible
with place preservation or in cases where NIMBY (not in my backyard)-ism is retrogressive, that
is, when it hinders desirable social goals or the normal evolution of architectural style, or positive
forms of development that would accomplish these things. However, when planning incorporates
or enhances elements that are central to the meaning of the place, it will be better received (e.g.,
Manzo & Perkins 2006).
Different forms of place relationships have different outcomes. Hikers with a greater sense of
place identity viewed problems (such as crowding, litter, or noise) along a trail to be more impor-
tant, but those with a greater sense of place dependence perceived problems to be less important
(Kyle et al. 2004a), perhaps because these problems tended to be by-products of their own place use.
Emotional relationships with place usually are positive, but they can include fear, hatred, and
ambivalence (Manzo 2005). For example, unhappy or traumatic experiences in a childhood home
may well create what might be called negative place attachment. If place attachment did not
exist, neither would homesickness—but it does; it causes problems for people who are forced by
circumstance to leave a place to which they are attached, such as students who leave home for
their education (e.g., Scopelliti & Tiberio 2010).
Finally, the loss of a place to which one is attached can result in grief and distress. Individuals
who have been absent from their homes for an extended period of time often express a great desire
to return to or visit the place, and at times, the return can involve much effort or cost.
SOCIAL DESIGN
In many ways, social design is where environmental psychology began (e.g., Osmond 1957;
Sommer 1969, 1983). Social design matters. According to a famous dictum, architectural form is
supposed to follow function, but for too many buildings, it does not seem to (Nasar et al. 2005).
Public participation in design is an important part of the design process (e.g., Churchman 2012).
However, now that public participation has become a routine part of architectural practice—at
least nominally—less effort has been expended on it in recent years by academic or science-oriented
environmental psychologists.
Nevertheless, environmental psychology–oriented design research and thinking continues
(e.g., Peponis et al. 2007). For example, which office design features foster creativity? Green
562 Gifford
PS65CH20-Gifford ARI 9 November 2013 16:15
hues seem to facilitate creativity (Lichtenfeld et al. 2012). In one study, a positive social climate
and a lack of environmental distractions predicted perceived creativity in offices (Stokols et al.
2002), but in another, perceived creativity was associated with more complexity and less use of
cool colors, as well as views of natural environments and less use of manufactured or composite
surface materials (McCoy & Evans 2002). Importantly, only the second of these studies compared
perceptions of creativity with objective creative performance; greater creativity was found in an
office with more of the attributes examined (views and less manufactured materials) than in an
office with fewer of those features.
More broadly, which workplace features help to support the growing cadre of knowledge-
oriented employees? The work engagement of employees is influenced by their aesthetic judg-
ments and mood, which in turn are influenced by how they appraise the lighting in their workspace
(Veitch et al. 2011). Lighting that employees are able to adjust for themselves at their own work-
station is better than overhead lighting: Employees find their workstation more satisfying and
pleasurable, the environment as a whole more satisfying, and their job more satisfying. They re-
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
port being more committed to their organization, express less intention to change jobs, and report
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
fewer and less intense symptoms of physical illness (Veitch et al. 2010).
Employees believe a workplace offers more support for collaboration when the distance from
their workstation to a meeting space is shorter, the distance to a kitchen or coffee area is longer,
and the percentage of floor space that is dedicated to shared services and amenities is larger
(Hua et al. 2011). Research contradicts the common belief that open-plan designs produce more
collaboration, even when the occupants judge the space favorably (Lansdale et al. 2011).
Others have examined which office forms are more satisfying or are associated with better
health and job satisfaction (e.g., Danielsson & Bodin 2008). The lowest health status was found
among employees in medium-sized and small open-plan offices; the best in cell offices (four-walls
and window) and flex offices (shared open space with machines, no assigned workspaces). Workers
in these office forms and in shared-room offices also had the highest job satisfaction. Lowest job
satisfaction was in “combi” offices (in which employees have their own workstations, but more
than 20% of their time is spent in teamwork away from one’s own desk), followed by medium-
sized open-plan offices. Not surprisingly, open-plan office workers are more satisfied next to a
window, especially if they also have reasonably tall partitions around them (Yildirim et al. 2007),
but windows apparently do not guarantee better work performance (Wang & Boubekri 2010).
The effects of building elements on their users are rarely simple; they can and do interact with
influences external to the building. For example, school buildings in poor condition matter for
student achievement, but this interacts with student mobility (the frequency with which a school’s
students change schools), so that poor condition plus mobility hinder achievement apart from
either influence alone (Evans et al. 2010).
A challenge for social design is that the aesthetic preferences of architects and laypersons
often diverge (e.g., Douglas & Gifford 2001). Some buildings are admired by laypersons but not
architects, some by architects but not laypersons, some by both groups, and some by neither
(Gifford et al. 2000). Researchers have begun to isolate the specific building features that result
in these differences (e.g., Brown & Gifford 2001, Gifford et al. 2002). Fortunately, strategies are
now being developed to find ways to reconcile the preferences of the two groups (e.g., Fawcett
et al. 2008).
Social design as a construct might be enlarged to include broader societal benefits, such as
constructing green buildings. To the extent that such buildings have technical benefits such as re-
duced energy or water use, they are valuable. A further reasonable assumption might be that green
buildings are beneficial for their occupants (e.g., Joye 2007). However, what seems reasonable
is not always the case: In one study of 15 buildings that varied in objective greenness, employee
engagement and attitudes did not increase with increasing greenness, and, in fact, employees’
impressions of their offices were negatively correlated with increasing greenness (McCunn &
Gifford 2012).
Therefore, home can be a behavioral wedge in the effort to improve overall sustainability (Stern
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
564 Gifford
PS65CH20-Gifford ARI 9 November 2013 16:15
in adding amenities such as kiosks, benches, trellises for hanging gardens, and artwork, and it
resulted in many residents reporting that the neighborhood was better and a good place to live,
with more social interaction and participation, and that they had an increased sense of place
(Semenza & March 2009).
VIRTUAL ENVIRONMENTS
The very world in which people in industrialized societies live has profoundly changed. Distance
has disappeared. We experience far-off places and people through screens. For video gamers, reality
is animated. Although the “screen revolution” has been going on since the advent of television,
the difference now is that individuals have much more power to decide where to go and with
whom to interact, rather than being limited to the offerings of a few major networks and receiving
little opportunity to interact with the content. At the same time, physically proximate people and
environments are allocated less time because of that spent on screens. The pace of technological
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
change has greatly increased, and many people have what I call change overload disorder.
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
Environmental psychologists have been examining what these changes might mean (e.g.,
Stokols & Montero 2002) and how they might affect human functioning, behavior settings (i.e.,
theoretical entities that capture the essence of the relation between a standing pattern of behavior—
such as the usual actions of football players, referees, and the audience—and the milieu within
which it occurs, the stadium), and transactions between humans and the natural environment.
Putting screens between ourselves and the actual physical and social worlds changes our connec-
tions with them in fundamental ways (e.g., Levi & Kocher 1999). The new reality will require a
very different understanding of environmental transactions and new ways of thinking about how
people perceive and represent the world (e.g., Heft 2001).
One challenge for an environmental psychology that matters, therefore, is to understand the
import of dividing our time between the proximate and the distant and virtual worlds. For example,
spatial discounting means that although individuals may be more aware of problems in distant
environments through watching news stories on television or online, they may be less concerned
about local problems (e.g., Gifford et al. 2009, Uzzell 2000). Will this blending of the near, the
far, and the unreal lead to increased assistance for victims when disaster strikes far away, or will it
cause numbness to their plight resulting from awareness of too many far-flung disasters?
Environmental psychologists are still struggling with the boundaries and definitions of the
physical and social environments (Heft 1998, Kaplan & Kaplan 2009), but understanding them
better, including new ways of conceptualizing the intertwined nature of the real and the virtual
worlds we live in, is crucial (Stokols et al. 2009).
of perception must include its embeddedness in a sociocultural and historical web, remains an
important perspective (e.g., Heft 2012).
Recent research clarifies how people experience aspects of their world. For example, one’s level
of awareness, degree of adaptation, and necessary selectiveness in attending to environmental cues
within complex real scenes mean that people sometimes miss important elements of a scene,
resulting in negative consequences for their health or safety (e.g., Stamps 2005). For example,
perceived safety in outdoor places depends in part on (greater) perceived enclosure, that is, whether
the scene is experienced as having wall-like elements, which in turn depends in part on atmospheric
permeability, that is, the distance that one can see (Stamps 2010). Fog—and presumably thick
smog—reduces atmospheric permeability, which makes places appear more open, which implies
to perceivers that they are less safe (Stamps 2012).
In interiors, warm colors are not only more attractive, but they seem to be better remembered
(Hidayetoglu et al. 2012). Putting more credentials on the wall makes a therapist appear more
qualified (Devlin et al. 2009), and dim lighting also seems to elicit more favorable impressions from
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
their clients (Miwa & Hanyu 2006). Other work suggests that certain long-held assumptions may
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
not be tenable. For example, older research reported that humans prefer savannah-like landscapes,
but we may prefer forest views instead (Han 2007).
566 Gifford
PS65CH20-Gifford ARI 9 November 2013 16:15
(a) should not be monotonously uniform, (b) should have a series of distinct local cues and as much
visual access to the destination as possible, and (c) should have good signage so that navigation can
proceed from point to point with fewer errors (Passini et al. 2000).
Responses to Noise
The global sound level is rising, and more people are exposed to greater sound levels. For ex-
ample, the European Union estimates that 30% of its citizens are exposed to road traffic sound
levels greater than those recommended by the World Health Organization. Although the normal
assumption is that loud sounds are broadly detrimental, the history of noise research includes
examples of null effects, and a few examples of increased performance exist (see Gifford 2007b,
chapter 12). For example, somewhat loud sound may increase physiological arousal, which can
speed the performance of easy, routine, or well-learned tasks.
Some results confirm the more expected outcomes. For example, louder noise is more annoying
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
(Pierrette et al. 2012). Road traffic noise impairs children’s reading speed and basic mathematics
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
(Ljung et al. 2009). Irrelevant speech affects cognitive tasks and increases mental workload levels
of open-plan office workers (Smith-Jackson & Klein 2009). Louder sound affects the memory,
motivation to work, and tiredness of open-plan office workers ( Jahncke et al. 2011).
Some researchers are moving toward finer-grained analyses to identify (a) which kinds of noise
result in annoyance or detrimental effects on (b) which kinds of human responses in (c) which
kinds of settings for (d ) which sorts of persons. In one example of this approach, aircraft noise was
associated with elevated hyperactivity scores in children, and road noise was associated with lower
scores on a behavior problems scale, but neither type of noise was associated with a broadband
measure of the children’s mental health (Stansfeld et al. 2009).
Among other examples of finer-grained results, speech noise was more harmful than aircraft
noise to the prose memory of adolescent students (Sörqvist 2010). Nature sounds appear to not
harm memory, but when they are combined with voices or ground traffic, memory does suffer
(Benfield et al. 2010). Street noise seems to harm the executive functioning of urban boys but not
girls (Belojevic et al. 2012). Clearly, noise matters (Stewart et al. 2011), but much more research
is needed to determine just how and when it does—and does not—have negative impacts.
Social Space
Personal space, crowding, territoriality, and privacy clearly are important parts of everyday func-
tioning; social space matters. However, some might argue that enough is already known about
these topics or that they do not have the societal-level importance of some topics covered previ-
ously. Research on social space has slowed to a trickle as efforts on topics deemed more important
have increased.
Recent work confirms some expected outcomes, such as that being forced to sit close to others
on public transport results in adverse reactions (Evans & Wener 2007). People who prefer seats
at the end of long tables have a greater need to define their own territory (Kaya & Burgess 2007).
Children who have been abused need more personal space around them (Vranic 2003). High
density in a room alters preschoolers’ choice of activities and time spent on off-task activities.
However, some less-predictable outcomes have also been reported, suggesting that despite the
slowdown in social space research, not everything is known about how people use social space after
all. For example, one might expect that territorial intrusions would result in faster responses by
men than women, but at least in one context (a very large religious gathering) the reverse was true
(Ruback & Kohli 2005). Technology is changing social space usage: Headphone wearers choose
larger interpersonal distances (Lloyd et al. 2009). Finally, who would guess that spatially confined
shoppers react by making more varied and unique product choices (Levav & Zhu 2009)? Social
space matters.
Physical Activity
Around the world, whether waistlines a few years ago were relatively small or relatively large,
they are now expanding. Getting people to move at all, and without the aid of GHG-producing
machines, is a challenge both for human health and climate change mitigation. Physical activity
matters.
Efforts have been made to make neighborhoods more walkable, schoolyards more encouraging
of physical activity, and stairs more enticing. Walkable neighborhoods obviously include sidewalks,
shops, and amenities, natural features such as trees and parks, and high residential density (e.g.,
Brown et al. 2007). Less obviously, walkable neighborhoods include slopes and stairs, moderate
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
lobby, helps encourage their use. For example, when text encouraging the use of stairs was placed
directly on four successive stair risers at eye level, stair use increased (Eves et al. 2009).
CONCLUSION
Scientific psychology began in the nineteenth century, but not until the middle of the twentieth
century was psychology’s range extended in any serious way to the physical environment. From
the vantage point of the early twenty-first century, the psychology that attempted to understand
persons in a physical vacuum is now revealed as woefully inadequate. Environmental psychology
completes the picture by including the built and natural settings within which all humans exist. It
is therefore essential not only to a complete understanding of human thought and behavior, but
also for a full account of every other psychological process and for every application of psychology
to the improvement of everyday life. Environmental psychology matters.
DISCLOSURE STATEMENT
The author is not aware of any affiliations, memberships, funding, or financial holdings that might
be perceived as affecting the objectivity of this review.
LITERATURE CITED
Abrahamse W, Steg L, Gifford R, Vlek C. 2009. Factors influencing car use for commuting and the intention
to reduce it: a question of self-interest or morality? Transport. Res. Part F: Psychol. Behav. 12:317–24
Abrahamse W, Steg L, Vlek C, Rothengatter T. 2005. A review of intervention studies aimed at household
energy conservation. J. Environ. Psychol. 25:273–91
Abrahamse W, Steg L, Vlek C, Rothengatter T. 2007. The effect of tailored information, goal setting,
and tailored feedback on household energy use, energy-related behaviors, and behavioral antecedents.
J. Environ. Psychol. 27:265–76
Adeola F. 2009. Katrina cataclysm: Does duration of residency and prior experience affect impacts, evacuation,
and adaptation behavior among survivors? Environ. Behav. 41:459–89
Ajzen I, ed. 2005. Attitudes, Personality and Behavior. Milton-Keynes, UK: McGraw-Hill
Am. Psychol. Assoc. 2010. Psychology and Global Climate Change: Addressing a Multifaceted Phenomenon and set of
Challenges. Report of the American Psychological Association Task Force on the Interface Between Psychology and
Global Climate Change. http://www.apa.org/science/about/publications/climate-change.aspx
568 Gifford
PS65CH20-Gifford ARI 9 November 2013 16:15
Arnocky S, Stroink M, DeCicco T. 2007. Self-construal predicts environmental concern, cooperation, and
conservation. J. Environ. Psychol. 27:255–64
Baca-Motes K, Brown A, Gneezy A, Keenan EA, Nelson LD. 2013. Commitment and behavior change:
evidence from the field. J. Consum. Res. 39:1070–84
Bamberg S. 2002. Effects of implementation intentions on the actual performance of new environmentally
friendly behaviors—results of two field experiments. J. Environ. Psychol. 22:399–411
Bamberg S, Möser G. 2007. Twenty years after Hines, Hungerford, and Tomera: a new meta-analysis of
psycho-social determinants of pro-environmental behaviour. J. Environ. Psychol. 27:14–25
Bamberg S, Schmidt P. 2003. Incentives, morality, or habit? Predicting students’ car use for university routes
with the models of Ajzen, Schwartz and Triandis. Environ. Behav. 35:264–85
Bechtel RB, Churchman A, eds. 2002. Handbook of Environmental Psychology. New York: Wiley
Belojevic G, Evans GW, Paunovic K, Jakovljevic B. 2012. Traffic noise and executive functioning in urban
primary school children: the moderating role of gender. J. Environ. Psychol. 32:337–41
Benfield JA, Bell PB, Troup LJ, Soderstrom N. 2010. Does anthropogenic noise in national parks impair
memory? Environ. Behav. 42:693–706
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Berman MG, Jonides J, Kaplan S. 2008. The cognitive benefits of interacting with nature. Psychol. Sci. 19:1207–
12
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
Biel A, Eek D, Gärling T, Gustafsson M, eds. 2008. New Issues and Paradigms in Research on Social Dilemmas.
New York: Springer
Biel A, Nilsson A. 2005. Religious values and environmental concern: harmony and detachment. Soc. Sci. Q.
86:178–91
Bolderdijk JW, Steg L, Geller ES, Lehman PK, Postmes T. 2013. Comparing the effectiveness of monetary
versus moral motives in environmental campaigning. Nat. Clim. Change 3:413–16
Borst HC, Miedema HME, de Vries SI, Graham JMA, van Dongen JEF. 2008. Relationships between street
characteristics and perceived attractiveness for walking reported by elderly people. J. Environ. Psychol.
28:353–61
Brown BB, Altman I, Werner CM. 2012. Place attachment. In The International Encyclopedia of Housing and
Home, ed. M Pareja-Eastaway, pp. 183–88. Oxford, UK: Elsevier
Brown BB, Perkins DD, Brown G. 2003. Place attachment in a revitalizing neighborhood: individual and
block levels of analysis. J. Environ. Psychol. 23:259–71
Brown BB, Werner CM, Amburgey JW, Szalay C. 2007. Walkable route perceptions and physical features:
converging evidence for en route walking experiences. Environ. Behav. 39:34–61
Brown G, Gifford R. 2001. Architects predict lay evaluations of large contemporary buildings: whose concep-
tual properties? J. Environ. Psychol. 21:93–99
Caia G, Ventimiglia F, Maass, A. 2010. Container vs. dacha: the psychological effects of temporary housing
characteristics on earthquake survivors. J. Environ. Psychol. 30:60–66
Campbell DT. 1963. Social attitudes and other acquired behavioral dispositions. In Psychology: A Study of a
Science, Vol. 6, ed. S Koch, pp. 94–172. New York: McGraw-Hill
Carrico AR, Riemer M. 2011. Motivating energy conservation in the workplace: an evaluation of the use of
group-level feedback and peer education. J. Environ. Psychol. 31:1–13
Carrus G, Passafaro P, Bonnes M. 2008. Emotions, habits and rational choices in ecological behaviours: the
case of recycling and use of public transportation. J. Environ. Psychol. 28:51–62
Catlin JR, Wang Y. 2013. Recycling gone bad: when the option to recycle increases resource consumption.
J. Consum. Psychol. 23:122–27
Chen M-F, Tung P-J. 2010. The moderating effect of perceived lack of facilities on consumers’ recycling
intentions. Environ. Behav. 42:824–44
Chen X, Peterson MN, Hull V, Lu C, Lee GD, et al. 2011. Effects of attitudinal and sociodemographic factors
on pro-environmental behaviour in urban China. Environ. Conserv. 38:5–52
Cheng JCH, Monroe MC. 2012. Connection to nature: children’s affective attitude toward nature. Environ.
Behav. 44:31–49
Churchman A. 2012. Public participation around the world: introduction to the special theme issue. J. Archit.
Plan. Res. 29:1–4
Clayton S. 2003. Environmental identity: conceptual and operational definition. In Identity and the Natural
Environment: The Psychological Significance of Nature, ed. S Clayton, S Opotow, pp. 45–65. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press
Clayton S, ed. 2012. The Oxford Handbook of Environmental and Conservation Psychology. New York: Oxford
Univ. Press
Coluccia E, Iosue G, Brandimonte MA. 2007. The relationship between map drawing and spatial orientation
abilities: a study of gender differences. J. Environ. Psychol. 27:135–44
Corral-Verdugo V, Bonnes M, Tapia-Fonllem C, Fraijo-Sing B, Frı́as-Armenta M, Carrus G. 2009. Correlates
of pro-sustainability orientation: the affinity towards diversity. J. Environ. Psychol. 29:34–43
Corral-Verdugo V, Carrus G, Bonnes M, Moser G, Sinha JBP. 2008. Environmental beliefs and endorsement
of sustainable development principles in water conservation: towards a New Human Interdependence
Paradigm Scale. Environ. Behav. 40:703–25
Corral-Verdugo V, Frı́as-Armenta M. 2006. Personal normative beliefs, antisocial behavior, and residential
water conservation. Environ. Behav. 38:406–21
Corral-Verdugo V, Pinheiro JQ. 2006. Sustainability, future orientation and water conservation. Rev. Eur.
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Danielsson CB, Bodin L. 2008. Office type in relation to health, well-being, and job satisfaction among
employees. Environ. Behav. 40:636–68
Davis JL, Green JD, Reed A. 2009. Interdependence with the environment: commitment, interconnectedness,
and environmental behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 29:173–80
de Groot JIM, Steg L. 2007. Value orientations and environmental beliefs in five countries. J. Cross-Cult.
Psychol. 38:318–32
de Groot JIM, Steg L. 2010. Relationship between value orientations, self-determined motivational types and
pro-environmental behavioural intentions. J. Environ. Psychol. 30:368–78
Devine-Wright P, Howes Y. 2010. Disruption to place attachment and the protection of restorative environ-
ments: a wind energy case study. J. Environ. Psychol. 30:271–80
Devlin AS, Donovan S, Nicolov A, Nold O, Packard A, Zandan G. 2009. “Impressive?” Credentials, family
photographs, and the perception of therapist qualities. J. Environ. Psychol. 29:503–12
Dietz T, Kalof L, Stern PC. 2002. Gender, values, and environmentalism. Soc. Sci. Q. 83:353–64
Dono J, Webb J, Richardson B. 2010. The relationship between environmental activism, pro-environmental
behavior and social identity. J. Environ. Psychol. 30:178–86
Douglas D, Gifford R. 2001. Evaluation of the physical classroom by students and professors: a lens model
approach. Educ. Res. 43:295–309
Droseltis O, Vignoles VL. 2010. Towards an integrative model of place identification: dimensionality and
predictors of intrapersonal-level place preferences. J. Environ. Psychol. 30:23–34
Dunlap RE, Van Liere KD, Mertig AG, Jones ER. 2000. Measuring endorsement of the new ecological
paradigm: a revised NEP scale.J. Soc. Issues 56:425–42
Dutcher DD, Finley JC, Luloff AE. 2007. Connectivity with nature as a measure of environmental values.
Environ. Behav. 39:474–93
Engqvist Jonsson A-K, Nilsson A. 2013. Exploring the relationship between values and pro-environmental
behavior: the influence of locus of control. Environ. Values. In press
Evans GW, Saegert S. 2000. Residential crowding in the context of inner city poverty. In Theoretical Per-
spectives in Environment-Behavior Research: Underlying Assumptions, Research Problems, and Relationships, ed.
S Wapner, J Demick, H Minamik, T Yamamoto, pp. 247–68. New York: Plenum
Evans GW, Wells NM, Chan HE, Saltzman H. 2000. Housing quality and mental health. J. Consult. Clin.
Psychol. 68:526–30
Evans GW, Wener RE. 2007. Crowding and personal space invasion on the train: Please don’t make me sit
in the middle. J. Environ. Psychol. 27:90–94
Evans GW, Yoo MJ, Sipple J. 2010. The ecological context of student achievement: School building quality
effects are exacerbated by high levels of student mobility. J. Environ. Psychol. 30:239–44
Evans L, Maio GR, Corner A, Hodgetts CJ, Ahmed S, Hahn U. 2013. Self-interest and pro-environmental
behavior. Nat. Clim. Change 3:122–25
570 Gifford
PS65CH20-Gifford ARI 9 November 2013 16:15
Eves FF, Olander EK, Nicoll G, Puig-Ribera A, Griffin C. 2009. Increasing stair climbing in a train station:
the effects of contextual variables and visibility. J. Environ. Psychol. 29:300–3
Fawcett W, Ellingham I, Platt S. 2008. Reconciling the architectural preferences of architects and the public:
the ordered preference model. Environ. Behav. 40:599–618
Feinberg M, Willer R. 2013. The moral roots of environmental attitudes. Psychol. Sci. 24:56–62
Felsten G. 2009. Where to take a study break on the college campus: an attention restoration theory prospec-
tive. J. Environ. Psychol. 29:160–67
Ferguson MA, Branscombe NR. 2010. Collective guilt mediates the effect of beliefs about global warming on
willingness to engage in mitigation behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 30:135–42
Fielding KS, Head BW. 2012. Determinants of young Australians’ environmental actions: the role of respon-
sibility attributions, locus of control, knowledge and attitudes. Environ. Educ. Res. 18:171–86
Fielding KS, McDonald R, Louis WR. 2008. Theory of planned behaviour, identity and intentions to engage
in environmental activism. J. Environ. Psychol. 28:318–26
Fischhoff B, Furby L. 1983. Psychological dimensions of climatic change. In Social Science Research and Climate
Change, ed. RS Chen, E Boulding, SH Schneider, pp. 183–203. Dordrecht, Neth.: Reidel
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Fornara F, Carrus G, Passafaro P, Bonnes M. 2011. Distinguishing the sources of normative influence on pro-
environmental behaviours: the role of local norms in household waste recycling. Group Proc. Intergroup
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
Relat. 5:623–35
Fraj E, Martinez E. 2006. Influence of personality on ecological consumer behaviour. J. Consum. Behav.
5:167–81
Frankenstein J, Mohler BJ, Bülthoff HH, Meilinger T. 2012. Is the map in our head pointed north? Psychol.
Sci. 23:120–25
Frantz CM, Mayer FS, Norton C, Rock M. 2005. There is no “I” in nature: the influence of self-awareness
on connectedness to nature. J. Environ. Psychol. 25:427–36
Franzen A. 2003. Environmental attitudes in international comparison: an analysis of the ISSP surveys 1993
and 2000. Soc. Sci. Q. 84:297–308
Fujii S. 2006. Environmental concern, attitude toward frugality, and ease of behavior as determinants of
pro-environmental behavior intentions. J. Environ. Psychol. 26:262–68
Garcı́a Mira R, Real JE, Uzzell D, San Juan C, Pol E. 2006. Coping with a threat to quality of life: the case of
the “Prestige” disaster. Eur. Rev. Appl. Psychol. 56:53–60
Gardner GT, Stern PC. 2002. Environmental Problems and Human Behavior. Boston, MA: Pearson. 2nd ed.
Gardner GT, Stern PC. 2008. The short list: the most effective actions U.S. households can take to curb
climate change. Environment 50:12–25
Gärling T, Biel A, Gustafsson M. 2002. The new environmental psychology: the human interdependence
paradigm. See Bechtel & Churchman 2002, pp. 85–94
Gärling T, Loukopoulos P. 2007. Effectiveness, public acceptance, and political feasibility of coercive measures
for reducing car traffic. See Gärling & Steg 2007, pp. 313–24
Gärling T, Schuitema G. 2007. Travel demand management targeting reduced private car use: effectiveness,
public acceptability and political feasibility. J. Soc. Issues 63:139–53
Gärling T, Steg L, eds. 2007. Threats to the Quality of Urban Life from Car Traffic: Problems, Causes, and Solutions.
Amsterdam: Elsevier
Gatersleben B, Steg L, Vlek C. 2002. Measurement and determinants of environmentally significant consumer
behavior. Environ. Behav. 34:335–62
Geller ES. 2002. The challenge of increasing proenvironmental behavior. See Bechtel & Churchman 2002,
pp. 525–40
Gifford R. 2002. Making a difference: some ways that environmental psychology has made a difference. See
Bechtel & Churchman 2002, pp. 323–34
Gifford R. 2006. A general model of social dilemmas. Int. J. Ecol. Econ. Stat. 5:23–40
Gifford R. 2007a. Environmental Psychology: Principles and Practice. Colville, WA: Optimal Books. 4th ed.
Gifford R. 2007b. Environmental psychology and sustainable development: expansion, maturation, and chal-
lenges. J. Soc. Issues 63:199–212
Gifford R. 2007c. The consequences of living in high-rise buildings. Arch. Sci. Rev. 50:2–17
Gifford R. 2008a. Psychology’s essential role in alleviating the impacts of climate change. Can. Psychol. 49:273–
80
Gifford R. 2008b. Toward a comprehensive model of social dilemmas. In New Issues and Paradigms in Research
on Social Dilemmas, ed. A Biel, D Eek, T Gärling, M Gustafsson, pp. 265–79. New York: Springer
Gifford R. 2011. The dragons of inaction: psychological barriers that limit climate change mitigation. Am.
Psychol. 66:290–302
Gifford R, Comeau L. 2011. Message framing influences perceived climate change competence, engagement,
and behavioral intentions. Global Environ. Change 21:1301–7
Gifford J, Gifford R. 2000. FISH 3: A microworld for studying social dilemmas and resource management.
Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 32:417–22
Gifford R, Hine DW, Muller-Clemm W, Reynolds DJ, Shaw KT. 2000. Decoding modern architecture:
understanding the aesthetic differences of architects and laypersons. Environ. Behav. 32:163–87
Gifford R, Hine DW, Muller-Clemm W, Shaw KT. 2002. Why architects and laypersons judge buildings
differently: cognitive and physical bases. J. Archit. Plan. Res. 19:131–48
Gifford R, Kormos C, McIntyre A. 2011a. Behavioral dimensions of climate change: drivers, responses, bar-
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
21:177–89
Gifford R, Scannell L, Kormos C, Smolova L, Biel A, et al. 2009. Temporal pessimism and spatial optimism
in environmental assessments: an 18-nation study. J. Environ. Psychol. 29:1–12
Gifford R, Steg L. 2007. The impact of automobile traffic on quality of life. See Gärling & Steg 2007,
pp. 33–51
Gifford R, Steg L, Reser JP. 2011b. Environmental psychology. In IAAP Handbook of Applied Psychology, ed.
P Martin, F Cheung, M Kyrios, L Littlefield, M Knowles, B Overmier, JP Prieto, pp. 440–70. Chichester,
UK: Wiley Blackwell
Giuliani MV. 2003. Theory of attachment and place attachment. In Psychological Theories for Environmental
Issues, ed. M Bonnes, T Lee, M Bonaiuto, pp. 137–70. Aldershot, UK: Ashgate
Giuliani MV, Scopelliti M. 2009. Empirical research in environmental psychology: past, present, and future.
J. Environ. Psychol. 29:375–86
Graumann CF. 2002. The phenomenological approach to people-environment studies. See Bechtel &
Churchman 2002, pp. 95–113
Grønhøj A, Thøgersen J. 2009. Like father, like son? Intergenerational transmission of values, attitudes, and
behaviours in the environmental domain. J. Environ. Behav. 29:414–21
Han K-T. 2007. Responses to six major terrestrial biomes in terms of scenic beauty, preference, and restora-
tiveness. Environ. Behav. 39:529–56
Hansla A, Gamble A, Juliusson A, Gärling T. 2008. The relationships between awareness of consequences,
environmental concern, and value orientations. J. Environ. Psychol. 28:1–9
Hartig T, Staats H. 2003. Guest editors’ introduction: restorative environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 23:103–7
Hartig T, Staats H. 2006. The need for psychological restoration as a determinant of environmental prefer-
ences. J. Environ. Psychol. 26:215–26
Hay R. 1998. Sense of place in developmental context. J. Environ. Psychol. 18:5–29
Heath Y, Gifford R. 2002. Extending the theory of planned behavior: predicting the use of public transporta-
tion. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 32:2154–85
Heath Y, Gifford R. 2006. Free-market ideology and environmental degradation. Environ. Behav. 38:48–71
Heft H. 1998. Essay review: the elusive environment in environmental psychology. Br. J. Psychol. 89: 519–23
Heft H. 2001. Ecological Psychology in Context: James Gibson, Roger Barker, and the Legacy of William James’s
Radical Empiricism. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Heft H. 2012. The foundations of ecological psychology. In The Oxford Handbook of Environmental and Con-
servation Psychology, ed. S Clayton, pp. 1–40. New York: Oxford
Heft H. 2013. Environment, cognition, and culture: reconsidering the cognitive map. J. Environ. Psychol.
33:14–25
Hernandez D, Hidalgo MC, Salazar-Laplace E, Hess S. 2007. Place attachment and place identity in natives
and non-natives. J. Environ. Psychol. 27:310–19
572 Gifford
PS65CH20-Gifford ARI 9 November 2013 16:15
Herring H, Sorrell S, eds. 2008. Energy Efficiency and Sustainable Consumption: Dealing with the Rebound Effect.
Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave Macmillan
Herzog TR, Hayes LJ, Applin RC, Weatherly AM. 2011. Incompatibility and mental fatigue. Environ. Behav.
43:827–47
Herzog TR, Ouellette P, Rolens JR, Koenigs AM. 2010. Houses of worship as restorative environments.
Environ. Behav. 42:395–419
Herzog TR, Rector AE. 2009. Perceived danger and judged likelihood of restoration. Environ. Behav. 41:387–
401
Hidalgo MC, Hernández B. 2001. Place attachment: conceptual and empirical questions. J. Environ. Psychol.
21:273–81
Hidayetoglu ML, Yildirim K, Akalin A. 2012. The effects of color and light on indoor wayfinding and the
evaluation of the perceived environment. J. Environ. Psychol. 32:50–58
Hinds J, Sparks P. 2008. Engaging with the natural environment: the role of affective connection and identity.
J. Environ. Psychol. 28:109–20
Hine DW, Bhullar N, Marks ADG, Kelly P, Scott JG. 2011. Comparing the effectiveness of education and
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
technology in reducing wood smoke pollution: a field experiment. J. Environ. Psychol. 31:282–88
Hine DW, Gifford R, Heath Y, Cooksey R, Quain P. 2009. A cue utilization approach for investigating harvest
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
Kaiser FG, Gutscher H. 2003. The proposition of a general version of the theory of planned behavior:
predicting ecological behavior. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 33:586–603
Kaiser FG, Oerke B, Bogner FX. 2007. Behavior-based environmental attitude: development of an instrument
for adolescents. J. Environ. Psychol. 27:242–51
Kaplan S. 1995. The restorative benefits of nature: towards an integrative framework. J. Environ. Psychol.
15:169–82
Kaplan S, Kaplan R. 2009. Creating a larger role for environmental psychology: the reasonable person model
as an integrative framework. J. Environ. Psychol. 29:329–39
Karpiak CP, Baril GL. 2008. Moral reasoning and concern for the environment. J. Environ. Psychol. 28:203–8
Kaya N, Burgess B. 2007. Territoriality: seat preferences in different types of classroom arrangements. Environ.
Behav. 36:859–76
Kazdin AE. 2009. Psychological science’s contributions to a sustainable environment: extending our reach to
a grand challenge of society. Am. Psychol. 64:339–56
Keizer K, Lindenberg S, Steg L. 2008. The spreading of disorder. Science 322:1681–85
Kim J, Kaplan R. 2004. Physical and psychological factors in sense of community: new urbanist Kentlands
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
574 Gifford
PS65CH20-Gifford ARI 9 November 2013 16:15
Lima ML, Castro P. 2005. Cultural theory meets the community: worldviews and local issues. J. Environ.
Psychol. 25:23–35
Lindenberg S, Steg L. 2007. Normative, gain and hedonic goal frames guiding environmental behavior. J. Soc.
Issues 63:117–37
Ljung R, Sörqvist P, Hygge S. 2009. Effects of road traffic noise and irrelevant speech on children s reading
and mathematical performance. Noise Health 11:1 94–98
Lloyd DM, Coates A, Knopp J, Oram S, Rowbotham S. 2009. Don’t stand so close to me: the effect of auditory
input on interpersonal space. Perception 38:617–20
Lokhorst AM, Werner C, Staats H, van Dijk E, Gale JL. 2013. Commitment and behavior change: a meta-
analysis and critical review of commitment-making strategies in environmental research. Environ. Behav.
45:3–34
Lorenzoni I, Nicholson-Cole S, Whitmarsh L. 2007. Barriers perceived to engaging with climate change
among the UK public and their policy implications. Glob. Environ. Change 17:445–59
Maeda H, Hirose Y. 2009. Expectation of empowerment as a determinant of citizen participation in waste
management planning. Jap. Psychol. Res. 51:24–34
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Manzo LC. 2003. Beyond house and haven: toward a revisioning of emotional relationships with places.
J. Environ. Psychol. 23:47–61
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
Manzo LC. 2005. For better or worse: exploring multiple dimensions of place meaning. J. Environ. Psychol.
25:67–86
Manzo LC, Perkins DD. 2006. Neighborhoods as common ground: the importance of place attachment to
community participation and development. J. Plan. Lit. 20:335–50
Marans RW, Stimson R, eds. 2011. Investigating Quality of Urban Life: Theory, Methods, and Empirical Research.
Dordrecht, Neth.: Springer
Markowitz EM, Goldberg LR, Ashton MC, Lee K. 2012. Profiling the “pro-environmental individual”: a
personality perspective. J. Personal. 80:81–111
Marques S, Lima ML. 2011. Living in grey areas: industrial activity and psychological health. J. Environ.
Psychol. 31:314–22
Marzbali MH, Abdullah A, Razak NA, Tilaki MJM. 2012. The influence of crime prevention through envi-
ronmental design on victimisation and fear of crime. J. Environ. Psychol. 32:79–88
Matthies E, Selge S, Klöckner CA. 2012. The role of parental behaviour for the development of behaviour
specific environmental norms—the example of recycling and re-use behaviour. J. Environ. Psychol. 32:277–
84
Mayer FS, Frantz CM. 2004. The connectedness to nature scale: a measure of individuals’ feeling in
community with nature. J. Environ. Psychol. 24:503–15
Mazumdar S, Mazumdar S. 2004. Religion and place attachment: a study of sacred places. J. Environ. Psychol.
24:385–97
McCoy JM, Evans GW. 2002. The potential role of the physical environment in fostering creativity. Creat.
Res. J. 14:409–26
McCright AM, Dunlap RE. 2012. Bringing ideology in: the conservative white male effect on worry about
environmental problems in the USA. J. Risk Res. 16:211–26
McCunn LJ, Gifford R. 2012. Do green offices affect employee engagement and environmental motivation?
Archit. Sci. Rev. 55:128–34
McDonald RI, Fielding KS, Louis WR. 2013. Energizing and de-motivating effects of norm conflict.
Personal. Soc. Psychol. 39:57–72
McKenzie-Mohr D. 2000. Fostering sustainable behaviour through community-based social marketing. Am.
Psychol. 55:531–37
Milfont TL, Duckitt J. 2010. The Environmental Attitudes Inventory: a valid and reliable measure to assess
the structure of environmental attitudes. J. Environ. Psychol. 30:80–94
Milfont TL, Gouveia VV. 2006. Time perspective and values: an exploratory study of their relations to
environmental attitudes. J. Environ. Psychol. 26:72–82
Milfont TL, Sibley CG. 2012. The big five personality traits and environmental engagement: associations at
the individual and societal level. J. Environ. Psychol. 32:187–95
Mishra S, Mazumdar S, Suar D. 2010. Place attachment and flood preparedness. J. Environ. Psychol. 20:187–97
Miwa Y, Hanyu K. 2006. The effects of interior design on communication and impressions of a counselor in
a counseling room. Environ. Behav. 38:484–502
Mobley C, Vagias WM, DeWard SL. 2010. Exploring additional determinants of environmental behavior:
the role of environmental literature and environmental attitudes. Environ. Behav. 42:420–47
Morgan P. 2010. Towards a developmental theory of place attachment. J. Environ. Psychol. 30:11–22
Moser SC. 2010. Communicating climate change: history, challenges, process and future directions. WIRES
Clim. Change 1:31–53
Mostafa MM. 2012. Does globalisation affect consumers’ pro-environmental intentions? A multilevel analysis
across 25 countries. Int. J. Sustain. Dev. World Ecol. 19:1–9
Nasar JL, Stamps AE, Hanyu K. 2005. Form and function in public buildings. J. Environ. Psychol. 25:159–65
Ng CF. 2003. Satisfying shoppers’ psychological needs: from public market to cyber-mall. J. Environ. Psychol.
23:439–55
Nilsson A, von Borgstede C, Biel A. 2004. Willingness to accept climate change strategies: the effect of values
and norms. J. Environ. Psychol. 24:267–77
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Nisbet EK, Zelenski JM. 2011. Underestimating nearby nature: affective forecasting errors obscure the
happy path to sustainability. Psychol. Sci. 22:1101–6
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
Nisbet EK, Zelenski JM, Murphy SA. 2009. The nature relatedness scale: linking individuals’ connection with
nature to environmental concern and behavior. Environ. Behav. 41:715–40
Nolan JM, Schultz PW, Cialdini RB, Goldstein NJ, Griskevicius V. 2008. Normative social influence is
underdetected. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 34:913–23
Nordlund AM, Garvill J. 2002. Value structures behind proenvironmental behavior. Environ. Behav. 34:740–56
Nordlund AM, Garvill J. 2003. Effects of values, problem awareness and personal norm on willingness to
reduce personal car use. J. Environ. Psychol. 23:339–47
Oishi S. 2010. The psychology of residential mobility: implications for the self, social relationships, and
well-being. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 5:5–21
Ojala M. 2008. Recycling and ambivalence: quantitative and qualitative analyses of household recycling among
young adults. Environ. Behav. 40:777–97
Oreg S, Katz-Gerro T. 2006. Predicting proenvironmental behavior cross-nationally. Environ. Behav. 38:462–
83
Ornstein R, Ehrlich P. 2000. New World New Mind. Malor Books
Osbaldiston R, Schott JP. 2012. Environmental sustainability and behavioral science: meta-analysis of proen-
vironmental behavior experiments. Environ. Behav. 44:257–99
Osmond H. 1957. Function as the basis of psychiatric ward design. Ment. Hosp. (Archit. Suppl.) 8:23–30
Passini R, Pigot H, Rainville C, Tétreault M-H. 2000. Wayfinding in a nursing home for advanced dementia
of the Alzheimer’s type. Environ. Behav. 32:684–710
Pendola R, Gen S. 2008. Does “Main Street” promote sense of community? A comparison of San Francisco
neighbourhoods. Environ. Behav. 40:535–74
Peponis J, Bafna S, Bajaj R, Bromberg J, Congdon C, et al. 2007. Designing space to support knowledge work.
Environ. Behav. 39:815–40
Pichert D, Katsikopoulos KV. 2008. Green defaults: information presentation and pro-environmental be-
haviour. J. Environ. Psychol. 28:63–73
Pierrette M, Marquis-Favre C, Morel J, Rioux L, Vallet M, et al. 2012. Noise annoyance from industrial
and road traffic combined noises: a survey and a total annoyance model comparison. J. Environ. Psychol.
32:178–86
Poortinga W, Steg L, Vlek C. 2003. Myths of nature and environmental management strategies. A field study
on energy reductions in traffic and transport. In People, Places, and Sustainability, ed. G Moser, E Pol,
Y Bernard, M Bonnes, J Corraliza, pp. 280–90. Ashland, OH: Hogrefe & Huber
Raanaas RK, Evensen KH, Rich D, Sjostrom G, Patil C. 2011. Benefits of indoor plants on attention capacity
in an office setting. J. Environ. Psychol. 31:99–105
Rabinovich A, Morton M, Birney ME. 2012. Communicating climate science: the role of perceived commu-
nicator’s motives. J. Environ. Psychol. 32:11–18
576 Gifford
PS65CH20-Gifford ARI 9 November 2013 16:15
Räthzel N, Uzzell, D. 2009. Changing relations in global environmental change. Glob. Environ. Change 19:326–
35
Raymond CM, Brown G, Robinson GM. 2011. The influence of place attachment, and moral and normative
concerns on the conservation of native vegetation: a test of two behavioural models. J. Environ. Psychol.
31:323–35
Reser JP, Swim JK. 2011. Adapting and coping with the threat and impacts of climate change. Am. Psychol.
66:277–89
Rice G. 2006. Pro-environmental behavior in Egypt: Is there a role for Islamic environmental ethics? J. Bus.
Ethics 65:373–90
Robelia B, Murphy T. 2012. What do people know about key environmental issues? A review of environmental
knowledge surveys. Environ. Educ. Res. 18:299–321
Ruback RB, Kohli N. 2005. Territoriality at the Magh Mela: the effects of organizational factors and intruder
characteristics. Environ. Behav. 37:178–200
Sardianou E. 2007. Estimating energy conservation patterns of Greek households. Energy Policy 35:3778–91
Scannell L, Gifford R. 2010a. The relations between natural and civic place attachment and pro-environmental
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
30:1–10
Scannell L. Gifford R. 2013. The role of place attachment in receptivity to local and global climate change
messages. Environ. Behav. 45:60–85
Schmuck P, Schultz WP, eds. 2002. Psychology of Sustainable Development. Dordrecht, Neth.: Kluwer Acad.
Schultz PW, Gouveia VV, Cameron LD, Tankha G, Schmuck P, Franek M. 2005. Values and their
relationship to environmental concern and conservation behavior. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 36:457–75
Schultz PW, Nolan JM, Cialdini RB, Goldstein NJ, Griskevicius V. 2007. The constructive, destructive, and
reconstructive power of social norms. Psychol. Sci. 18:429–34
Schultz PW, Shriver C, Tabanico JL, Khazian AM. 2004. Implicit connections with nature. J. Environ. Psychol.
24:31–42
Schwartz SH. 1992. Universals in the content and structures of values: theoretical advances and empirical
tests in 20 countries. In Advances in Experimental Psychology, Vol. 25, ed. M Zanna, pp. 1–65. Orlando,
FL: Academic
Scopelliti M, Giuliani MV. 2004. Choosing restorative environments across the lifespan: a matter of place
experience. J. Environ. Psychol. 24:423–37
Scopelliti M, Tiberio L. 2010. Homesickness in university students: the role of multiple place attachment.
Environ. Behav. 42:335–50
Seamon D. 2012. Place, place identity, and phenomenology. In The Role of Place Identity in the Perception,
Understanding, and Design of the Built Environment, ed. H Casakin, F Bernardo, pp. 3–21. London: Bentham
Semenza JC, March TL. 2009. An urban community-based intervention to advance social interactions. Environ.
Behav. 41:22–42
Smit B, Wandel J. 2006. Adaptation, adaptive capacity and vulnerability. Glob. Environ. Change 16:282–92
Smith JR, Louis WR, Terry DJ, Greenaway KH, Clarke MR, Cheng X. 2012. Congruent or conflicted? The
impact of injunctive and descriptive norms on environmental intentions. J. Environ. Psychol. 32:353–61
Smith-Jackson TL, Klein KW. 2009. Open-plan offices: task performance and mental workload. J. Environ.
Psychol. 29:279–89
Smolders KCHJ, de Kort YAW, Tenner AD, Kaiser FG. 2012. Need for recovery in offices: behaviour-based
assessment. J. Environ. Psychol. 32:126–34
Sommer R. 1969. Personal Space: The Behavioral Basis of Design. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall
Sommer R. 1983. Social Design: Creating Buildings With People in Mind. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall
Sörqvist P. 2010. Effects of aircraft noise and speech on prose memory: what role for working memory capacity?
J. Environ. Psychol. 30:112–18
Spence A, Pidgeon N, Uzzell D. 2008. Climate change: psychology’s contribution. Psychologist 22:108–11
Staats H, Harland P, Wilke HAM. 2004. Effecting durable change: a team approach to improve environmental
behavior in the household. Environ. Behav. 36:341–67
Stamps AE. 2005. Enclosure and safety in urbanscapes. Environ. Behav. 37:102–33
Stamps AE. 2010. Effects of permeability on perceived enclosure and spaciousness. Environ. Behav. 42:864–86
Stamps AE. 2012. Atmospheric permeability and perceived enclosure. Environ. Behav. 44:427–46
Stansfeld SA, Clark C, Cameron RM, Alfred T, Head J, et al. 2009. Aircraft and road traffic noise exposure
and children’s mental health. J. Environ. Psychol. 29:203–7
Stedman RC. 2003. Is it really just a social construction? The contribution of the physical environment to
sense of place. Soc. Natural Resour. 16:671–85
Steg L, Dreijerink L, Abrahamse W. 2005. Factors influencing the acceptability of energy policies: a test of
VBN theory. J. Environ. Psychol. 25:415–25
Steg L, Gifford R. 2005. Sustainable transportation and quality of life. J. Transp. Geogr. 13:59–69
Steg L, Vlek C. 2009. Encouraging pro-environmental behaviour: an integrative review and research agenda.
J. Environ. Psychol. 29:309–17
Stern PC. 2000. Toward a coherent theory of environmentally significant behavior. J. Soc. Issues 56:407–24
Stern PC. 2008. Environmentally significant behavior in the home. In The Cambridge Handbook of Psychology
and Economic Behaviour, ed. A. Lewis, pp. 363–82. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Stern PC, Dietz T, Kalof L. 1993. Value orientations, gender, and environmental concern. Environ. Behav.
25:322–48
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
Stewart J, McManus F, Rodgers N, Weedon V, Bronzaft A. 2011. Why Noise Matters: A Worldwide Perspective
on the Problems, Policies and Solutions. London: Routledge
Stokols D, Clitheroe C, Zmuidzinas M. 2002. Qualities of work environments that promote perceived support
for creativity. Creativ. Res. J. 14:137–47
Stokols D, Misra S, Runnerstrom MG, Hipp JA. 2009. Psychology in an age of ecological crisis: from personal
angst to collective action. Am. Psychol. 64:181–93
Stokols D, Montero M. 2002. Toward an environmental psychology of the Internet. See Bechtel & Churchman
2002, pp. 661–75
Sullivan WC, Kuo FE, DePooter SF. 2004. The fruit of urban nature: vital neighborhood spaces. Environ.
Behav. 36:678–700
Sundstrom E, Bell PA, Busby PL, Asmus C. 1996. Environmental psychology 1989–1994. Annu. Rev. Psychol.
47:485–512
Sussman R, Gifford R. 2012. Please turn off the lights: the effectiveness of visual prompts. Appl. Ergon. 43:596–
603
Sussman R, Greeno M, Gifford R, Scannell L. 2013. The effectiveness of models and prompts on waste
diversion: a field experiment on composting by cafeteria patrons. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 43:24–34
Swami V, Chamorro-Premuzic T, Snelgar R, Furnham A. 2011. Personality, individual differences, and de-
mographic antecedents of self-reported household waste management behaviours. J. Environ. Psychol.
31:21–26
Swim JK, Stern PC, Doherty TJ, Clayton S, Reser JP, et al. 2011. Psychology’s contributions to understanding
and addressing global climate change. Am. Psychol. 66:241–50
Tabernero C, Hernández B. 2011. Self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation guiding environmental behavior.
Environ. Behav. 43:658–75
Tang Z, Chen X, Luo J. 2011. Determining socio-psychological drivers for rural household recycling behavior
in developing countries. Environ. Behav. 43:848–77
Taylor AF, Kuo F, Sullivan WC. 2001. Coping with ADD: the surprising connection to green play settings.
Environ. Behav. 33:54–77
Thøgersen J. 2005. How may consumer policy empower consumers for sustainable lifestyles? J. Consum. Policy
28:143–78
Thøgersen J. 2007. Social marketing of alternative transportation modes. See Gärling & Steg 2007, pp. 367–81
Thøgersen J, Grønhøj A. 2010. Electricity saving in households—a social cognitive approach. Energy Policy
38:7732–43
Thøgersen J, Ölander F. 2003. Spillover of environment-friendly consumer behaviour. J. Environ. Psychol.
23:225–36
Thompson CW, Aspinall P, Montarzino A. 2008. The childhood factor: adult visits to green places and the
significance of childhood experience. Environ. Behav. 40:111–43
578 Gifford
PS65CH20-Gifford ARI 9 November 2013 16:15
Ulrich RS. 1984. View through a window may influence recovery from surgery. Science 224:420–21
Uzzell D, Pol E, Badenas D. 2002. Place identification, social cohesion, and environmental sustainability.
Environ. Behav. 34:26–53
Uzzell D, Räthzel N. 2009. Transforming environmental psychology. J. Environ. Psychol. 29:340–50
Uzzell DL. 2000. The psycho-spatial dimensions of global environmental problems. J. Environ. Psychol.
20:307–18
van den Berg AE, ter Heijne M. 2005. Fear versus fascination: an exploration of emotional responses to natural
threats. J. Environ. Psychol. 25:261–72
Vaske JJ, Kobrin KC. 2001. Place attachment and environmentally responsible behavior. J. Environ. Educ.
32:16–21
Veitch JA, Newsham GR, Mancini S, Arsenault CD. 2010. Lighting and office renovation effects on employee
and organizational well-being. Rep. IRC-RR-306. Ottawa, Can.: Natl. Res. Counc. Inst. Res. Constr.
Veitch JA, Stokkermans MGM, Newsham GR. 2011. Linking lighting appraisals to work behaviors. Environ.
Behav. 45:198–214
Venables D, Pidgeon NF, Parkhill KA, Henwood KL, Simmons P. 2012. Living with nuclear power: sense of
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
place, proximity, and risk perceptions in local host communities. J. Environ. Psychol. 32:371–83
Vlek C. 2000. Essential psychology for environmental policy making. Int. J. Psychol. 35:153–67
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
Vorkinn M, Riese H. 2001. Environmental concern in a local context: the significance of place attachment.
Environ. Behav. 33:249–63
Vranic A. 2003. Personal space in physically abused children. Environ. Behav. 35:550–65
Wang N, Boubekri M. 2010. Investigation of declared seating preference and measured cognitive performance
in a sunlit room. J. Environ. Psychol. 30:226–38
Wells JC, Baldwin ED. 2012. Historic preservation, significance, and age value: a comparative phenomenology
of historic Charleston and the nearby new-urbanist community of I’On. J. Environ. Psychol. 32:384–400
Wells NM, Harris JD. 2007. Housing quality, psychological distress, and the mediating role of social with-
drawal: a longitudinal study of low-income women. J. Environ. Psychol. 27:69–78
Wells NM, Lekies KS. 2006. Nature and the life course: pathways from childhood nature experiences to
adult environmentalism. Child. Youth Environ. 16:1–24
Werner CM, Cook S, Colby J, Lim HJ. 2012. “Lights out” in university classrooms: Brief group discussion
can change behaviour. J. Environ. Psychol. 32:418–26
White M, Smith A, Humphryes K, Pahl S, Snelling D, Depledge M. 2010. Blue space: the importance of
water for preference, affect, and restorativeness ratings of natural and built scenes. J. Environ. Psychol.
30:482–93
Whitmarsh L. 2009. Behavioural responses to climate change: asymmetry of intentions and impacts. J. Environ.
Psychol. 29:13–23
Whitmarsh L, O’Neill S. 2010. Green identity, green living? The role of pro-environmental self-identity in
determining consistency across diverse pro-environmental behaviours. J. Environ. Psychol. 30:305–14
Williams K, Harvey D. 2001. Transcendent experience in forest environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 21:249–60
Xiao C, Hong D. 2010. Gender differences in environmental behaviors in China. Popul. Environ. 32:88–104
Yildirim K, Akalin-Baskaya A, Celebi M. 2007. The effects of window proximity, partition height, and gender
on perceptions of open-plan offices. J. Environ. Psychol. 27:154–65
Annual Review of
Contents
Psychology
Genetics of Behavior
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
Gene-Environment Interaction
Stephen B. Manuck and Jeanne M. McCaffery p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p41
Cognitive Neuroscience
The Cognitive Neuroscience of Insight
John Kounios and Mark Beeman p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p71
Color Perception
Color Psychology: Effects of Perceiving Color on Psychological
Functioning in Humans
Andrew J. Elliot and Markus A. Maier p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p95
Infancy
Human Infancy. . . and the Rest of the Lifespan
Marc H. Bornstein p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 121
vi
PS65-FrontMatter ARI 13 November 2013 20:27
Individual Treatment
Combination Psychotherapy and Antidepressant Medication Treatment
for Depression: For Whom, When, and How
W. Edward Craighead and Boadie W. Dunlop p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 267
Psychological Intervention
Geoffrey L. Cohen and David K. Sherman p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 333
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
Gender
Gender Similarities and Differences
Janet Shibley Hyde p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 373
Small Groups
Deviance and Dissent in Groups
Jolanda Jetten and Matthew J. Hornsey p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 461
Social Neuroscience
Cultural Neuroscience: Biology of the Mind in Cultural Contexts
Heejung S. Kim and Joni Y. Sasaki p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 487
Environmental Psychology
Environmental Psychology Matters
Robert Gifford p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 541
Contents vii
PS65-FrontMatter ARI 13 November 2013 20:27
Community Psychology
Socioecological Psychology
Shigehiro Oishi p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 581
Organizational Climate/Culture
(Un)Ethical Behavior in Organizations
Linda Klebe Treviño, Niki A. den Nieuwenboer, and Jennifer J. Kish-Gephart p p p p p p p 635
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:541-579. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Job/Work Design
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
Timely Topics
Properties of the Internal Clock: First- and Second-Order Principles of
Subjective Time
Melissa J. Allman, Sundeep Teki, Timothy D. Griffiths, and Warren H. Meck p p p p p p p p 743
Indexes
Errata
An online log of corrections to Annual Review of Psychology articles may be found at
http://psych.AnnualReviews.org/errata.shtml
viii Contents
ANNUAL REVIEWS
Connect With Our Experts
development, vision models, computer vision, and the mechanisms of visual disease, dysfunction, and sight restoration. The
study of vision is central to progress in many areas of science, and this new journal will explore and expose the connections that
Access provided by 24.108.22.200 on 04/25/16. For personal use only.
TABLE OF CONTENTS FOR VOLUME 1: • Organization of the Central Visual Pathways Following Field
• Adaptive Optics Ophthalmoscopy, Austin Roorda, Defects Arising from Congenital, Inherited, and Acquired
Jacque L. Duncan Eye Disease, Antony B. Morland
• Angiogenesis in Eye Disease, Yoshihiko Usui, • Contributions of Retinal Ganglion Cells to Subcortical
Peter D. Westenskow, Salome Murinello, Michael I. Dorrell, Visual Processing and Behaviors, Onkar S. Dhande,
Leah Scheppke, Felicitas Bucher, Susumu Sakimoto, Benjamin K. Stafford, Jung-Hwan A. Lim,
Liliana P Paris, Edith Aguilar, Martin Friedlander Andrew D. Huberman
• Color and the Cone Mosaic, David H. Brainard • Ribbon Synapses and Visual Processing in the Retina,
• Control and Functions of Fixational Eye Movements, Leon Lagnado, Frank Schmitz
Michele Rucci, Martina Poletti • The Determination of Rod and Cone Photoreceptor Fate,
• Deep Neural Networks A New Framework for Modeling Constance L. Cepko
Biological Vision and Brain Information Processing, • A Revised Neural Framework for Face Processing,
Nikolaus Kriegeskorte Brad Duchaine, Galit Yovel
• Development of Three-Dimensional Perception in Human • Visual Adaptation, Michael A. Webster
Infants, Anthony M. Norcia, Holly E. Gerhard • Visual Functions of the Thalamus, W. Martin Usrey,
• Functional Circuitry of the Retina, Jonathan B. Demb, Henry J. Alitto
Joshua H. Singer • Visual Guidance of Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements,
• Image Formation in the Living Human Eye, Pablo Artal Stephen Lisberger
• Imaging Glaucoma, Donald C. Hood • Visuomotor Functions in the Frontal Lobe, Jeffrey D. Schall
• Mitochondria and Optic Neuropathy, Janey L. Wiggs • What Does Genetics Tell Us About Age-Related
• Neuronal Mechanisms of Visual Attention, John Maunsell Macular Degeneration? Felix Grassmann, Thomas Ach,
Caroline Brandl, Iris M. Heid, Bernhard H.F. Weber
• Optogenetic Approaches to Restoring Vision, Zhuo-Hua
Pan, Qi Lu, Anding Bi, Alexander M. Dizhoor, Gary W. Abrams • Zebrafish Models of Retinal Disease, Brian A. Link,
Ross F. Collery