Binocular Function
Binocular Function
6,700
Open access books available
180,000
International authors and editors
195M
Downloads
154
Countries delivered to
TOP 1%
most cited scientists
12.2%
Contributors from top 500 universities
Binocular Functions
Arvind Kumar Morya, Kanchan Solanki, Sahil Bhandari
and Anushree Naidu
Abstract
Binocular single vision is the ability to use both eyes simultaneously so that each
eye contributes to a common single perception. Normal binocular single vision
occurs with bifoveal fixation and normal retinal correspondence in everyday sight.
There are various anatomical and physiological factors concerned in the develop-
ment of Binocular vision. The development of binocular function starts at 6 weeks
and is completed by 6 months. Any obstacles, sensory, motor, or central, in the flex
pathway is likely to hamper the development of binocular vision. The presence of
these obstacles gives rise to various sensory adaptations to binocular dysfunction.
Clinically the tests used can be based on either of the two principles: (A) assessment
of relationship between the fovea of the fixing eye and the retinal area stimulated in
the squinting eye, viz. Bagolini striated glasses test, red filter test, synoptophore
using SMP slides for measuring the objective and subjective angles, and Worth
4-dot test; and (B) Assessment of the visual directions of the two foveae, viz. after
image test (Hering Bielschowsky); and Cuppers binocular visuoscopy test (foveo-
foveal test of Cuppers). Anomalies of binocular vision results in confusion, diplopia,
which leads to suppression, eccentric fixation, anomalous retinal correspondence,
and amblyopia.
1. Introduction
1
Eye Motility
During the initial few years of life certain normal anatomical and physiological
conditions are required for the development of binocular vision [2]. The factors
required for the development of Binocular vision and which enable the eyes to
function in a coordinated manner are as follows [3, 4]
A. Anatomical factors: The eyes are spruced up in the orbit in such a way that
the visual axis of both eyes is aligned. This occurs as a result of multiple
anatomical factors viz.
i. Architecture of the orbit
ii. Ligaments, muscles and connective tissues, i.e., adjacent ocular structures.
The extra-ocular muscles play an important role as they provide motor
correspondence because of the reciprocal innervation of the extra-ocular muscles [4].
Following are the aims of motor correspondence:
i. To convert field of vision into field of fixation thereby widening the view.
ii. To ensure that the object of attention is sustained and maintained on fovea.
iii. To ensure that both the eyes are aligned at all times.
2
Binocular Functions
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5772/intechopen.84162
i. Fixation reflexes
ii. Accommodation convergence reflex: The aim of this reflex to align the
eyes in such a way that the fixation on the object of interest is
maintained. It includes three reflexes, viz., (a) vergence fixation reflex,
(b) accommodation reflex, and (c) fusional vergence reflex.
iii. The refixation reflex: The aim of this reflex is to bring the eye back to
the original orientation point or to the new orientation point.
Cerebral activity maintains the fusional reflex which are either conditioned or
acquired. They develop as a result of experience gained from environmental stimu-
lus. Once these reflexes are formed as a result of continuous reinforcement they
transform into unconditioned reflexes. Aimed to form binocular single vision, it
consists of all the activities generated from the retina through the brain to maintain
the images received on the fovea of both the eyes.
The elements of fusion mechanism are:
i. Fixation reflex
iii. Conjugate fusional reflexes: the alignment of the two eyes in all positions of
gaze is maintained by this reflex.
At birth, the child has random, nonconjugate and aimless ocular movements and
the fixation reflex is very poorly developed. During the first few weeks of life there
are no pursuit movements. The optomotor reflex is essentially a postnatal event,
and it follows the following time schedule:
3
Eye Motility
1. Central obstacles
2. Sensory obstacles
• Proprioceptive obstacle
3. Motor obstacles
The presence of any of these obstacles gives rise to various sensory adaptations
to binocular dysfunction disruptive factor is present in the sensitive period. This can
be in the form of:
2. Suppression
3. Amblyopia
4
Binocular Functions
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5772/intechopen.84162
This is an obsolete theory. According to this theory Visual stimuli are exterior-
ized along the lines of direction. If a person fixates binocularly, a “bicentric” pro-
jection is supposed to occur that places the impression of each eye at the point of
intersection of the lines of projection [5, 6].
This theory fails to explain certain fundamental observations such as
1. Physiologic diplopia
The basic reason for the failure of the projection theory is that the distinction
between physical and subjective space is disregarded and it does not explain the
localization to a dioptic-geometric scheme.
5
Eye Motility
perception, if horizontal disparity remains within limits of Panum’s area. With the
increasing disparity the perceived depth increases. However, quality of stereopsis
decreases with increasing disparity which may eventually lead to diplopia.
Approximately [9] 80% of the neurons in the striate cortex can be stimulated
from either eye in response to a visual stimulus, assuming there is a precise and
orderly arrangement of connections along the retino-geniculate striate pathway. Of
these 75% represented graded response from either left or right eye while 25% are
binocularly driven cells and are equally stimulated from each eye. These 75% cells
that could be driven by stimulation of either eye had receptive fields of nearly equal
size and in corresponding positions of visual field. In normal binocular single
vision, optical stimulus will excite a cortical cell only. Only one object feature is
detected by each cortical cell and assigned by it to a single locus in space although
two receptive fields are involved. Anatomically identical regions in the two retinas
are not always occupied by the two receptive fields. There are cells whose fields
have exactly corresponding points in the two retina and cells whose fields have
slightly different position in the two eyes is seen at a given locus in the retino-optic
cortical map. This retinal field disparity is detected by sensitive binocular neurons
giving rise to binocular vision and stereopsis which occurs as a result of the differ-
ence in direction or distance of the fields in each retina forms the basis of Panum’s
fusion area.
5. Review of literature
3. Veith used the same Euclidean theorems to prove that theoretically the locus of
equal angles of binocular subtense is the locus of fused image and both are a
circle passing through the center of eyes.
4.In seventeenth and eighteenth century, clearly stated that binocular vision
contributes to impression of visual depth.
6
Binocular Functions
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5772/intechopen.84162
8. Inputs from the corresponding regions from two retinas converged on what he
called isodynamic cells and that his mechanism forms the basis of unified
binocular vision
• Motor: It is the ability of both the eyes to maintain sensory fusion through a
range vergence movements.
Binocular single vision can be classified into three stages according to Worth’s
classification (Figure 1)
7
Eye Motility
Figure 1.
The classical model of binocular visual function is composed of three hierarchical degrees.
2. Fusion
3. Stereoscopic vision
The ability of both the eyes to perceive simultaneously two images, one formed
on each retina is defined as simultaneous perception. Simultaneous perception of
the two images formed on corresponding areas, with the projection of these images
to the same position in space is superimposition. This occurs based on the corre-
spondence whether it is normal or abnormal. If fusion does not occur then two
similar images are seen as separate but superimposed and no fusion range can be
demonstrated.
Image 1.
Simultaneous perception—image for the first eye.
8
Binocular Functions
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5772/intechopen.84162
Image 2.
Simultaneous perception—image for second eye.
Image 3.
Simultaneous perception—binocular vision image.
Exemplary on Image 1 there is element visible for one eye and on Image 2
visible for second eye. Patient with ability to simultaneous perception should
perceive image similar to Image 3.
7.2 Fusion
9
Eye Motility
located on corresponding retinal areas but also should be sufficiently similar with
respect to size, brightness and sharpness to permit sensory are the prerequisites for
sensory fusion. A severe obstacle to fusion are unequal images.
The ability to align both the eyes in such a way that sensory fusion can be
maintained is termed as Motor fusion. Retinal disparity formed outside Panum’s
area and the eyes moving in opposite direction which may be horizontal, vertical or
cyclovergence is the stimulus for these fusional eye movements. Unlike sensory
fusion, motor fusion is a function of the extrafoveal retinal periphery. Fusion,
whether sensory or motor, is always a central process, i.e., it takes place in the
visual cortex.
7.3 Stereopsis
The fused image will be perceived in vivid depth nearer or farther to the point
of fixation within some range of limiting conditions, when two similar images are
presented to both the eyes with a binocular disparity that has a horizontal com-
ponent. The objects give rise to the stereoscopic depth from Horizontal binocular
disparities, e.g., arrow at different distances and it gives rise to stereoscopic
depth perception. Here the arrowhead has a lesser eccentricity on the nasal retina
of the right eye than on the temporal retina of the left eye. The fovea is the site
of fixation. The observer is aware alternately of the image to one eye and the
image to the other if such dichoptic image formed is of high contrast, due to
binocular rivalry that forms between the two monocular images. As a result of
interocular suppression if one eye is strongly dominant as a result of either
stimulus characteristics or organismic variables, perception of the image in the
other eye may be entirely absent. Prolonged periods of dichoptic summation may
be obtained, during which the different stimuli in the two eyes appear to be
summed together as if their contrasts were added linearly throughout the
dichoptic field. If however, the stimulus contrast is low for dichoptic stimuli.
When the presentation time is brief (150 ms) dichoptic summation also is
obtained for high contrast stimuli.
Where the image appears doubled but clearly at a different depth from zero-
disparity targets stereoscopic depth from horizontal disparities is perceived both in
the region of binocular fusion of the monocular targets into a single image and also
in the region of diplopia, the smallest disparity interval that produces reliable depth
discrimination under particular conditions is stereo acuity.
8.1 Suppression
10
Binocular Functions
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5772/intechopen.84162
2. Constant deviation
Motor adaptation is in the form of abnormal head posture and occurs primarily
in children with congenitally abnormal eye movements who use the abnormal head
posture to maintain the binocular single vision.
9. Retinal correspondence
Retinal correspondence occurs when the retinal points of both the eyes share a
common visual direction. Non corresponding retinal points will never have a com-
mon visual direction.
11
Eye Motility
points located nasal to the fovea in one eye correspond to the points located tem-
poral to the fovea of the other eye.
2. Constant deviation
Figure 2.
Empirical horopter. F, fixation point; FL and FR, left and right foveae, respectively. Point 2, falling within
Panum‘s area, is seen singly and stereoscopically. Point 3 falls outside Panum‘s area and is therefore seen doubly.
12
Binocular Functions
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5772/intechopen.84162
It is the ability of both the eyes to fuse images that lie within Panum’s fusional
area resulting in three dimensional perception of the object. Diplopia elicited by an
object point off the horopter but within Panum’s fusional area is known as physio-
logical diplopia. Images of a single object that do not stimulate corresponding retinal
points in both eyes are said to be disparate; binocular disparity is defined as the
difference in position of corresponding points between images in the two eyes.
Figure 3.
Crossed and uncrossed disparities result when objects produce images that are formed on closely separated
retinal points. Any point within Panum’s area yields a percept of a single image, while points outside Panum’s
area produces diplopia.
13
Eye Motility
Apparent size: It depends on the size of the object as well as the distance of the
object from the retina. The objects that are closer to the retina appear larger in
size and those farther away appear smaller. Similarly as the object move
towards the retina it appears to be increased in size.
Interposition: The objects that are relatively near conceal the objects that are
far.
Aerial perspective: Environmental factors like water vapor, dust and smoke
cause scattering of the light and hence cause decrease in the color saturation as
well as visibility of the distance object.
Shading: Whenever light falls on a solid object it casts shadow and when it falls
on the concave surface the shadow is cast in a graded manner.
Geometric perspective: The line that is parallel pragmatically appears to join
together near the horizon, e.g., railroad tracks.
Relative velocity: The velocity of image of a moving target that is at a distance
is slower that the velocity of image of a moving target that is near.
Motion parallax: If the fixation point is at an intermediate distance the objects
that are nearer to it move in the opposite direction when the head is moved and
those that are farther away from the fixation point move along with the head.
13. Fusion
14
Binocular Functions
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5772/intechopen.84162
When two similar images are formed on the corresponding areas of each eye, the
ability to interpret them as one is termed as sensory fusion. Retinal correspondence
can be certified from the fact that a single image is formed. Size, brightness and
sharpness of similar degree are equally essential components required for sensory
fusion to occur as is the retinal correspondence images of unequal size are a severe
obstacle to fusion.
For the sensory fusion to be maintained it is essential that the eyes are aligned
and the ability to do so is termed as motor fusion. Retinal disparity outside Panum’s
area and the eyes moving in opposite direction (vergence) are the stimuli for the
fusional movements. Motor fusion is the exclusive function of the extrafoveal
retinal periphery, unlike sensory fusion which is dependent on fovea. However both
sensory and motor fusion are central processes the control of which lies in the visual
cortex.
13.3 Diplopia
15
Eye Motility
2 Suppression
3 Simultaneous perception
4 Fusion
5 Stereopsis
It is essential to assess the visual acuity, fixation in the deviating eye and
direction and amount of deviation in every case.
Test for retinal correspondence:
Clinically the tests used can be based on either of the two principles:
1. Bagolini striated glasses test: The patient is asked to fixate a small light, after
being provided
With plano lenses with narrow fine striations across one meridian (micro Maddox
cylinders) a source of light is seen as a line at right angles to the striations. The axis
of striations of the eyes is kept at right angles to each other. The interpretation of
this test is as follows:
b.Asymmetrical cross response or two lines cutting each other at some other
point than midline, indicates an incomitant squint with normal retinal
correspondence (diplopia response).
16
Binocular Functions
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5772/intechopen.84162
c. Single line seen: If only one line is seen it indicates suppression of the
other eye (suppression response).
d.Cross response with central gap in one line indicates a central suppression
scotoma in that eye.
2. Red filter test: It is a characteristic test used to check dissociation between two
eye. It is done by placing a red filter over the preferential fixating eye while it
fixates over a small light source.
Results can be interpreted as:
a. Normal retinal correspondence: two lights are reported by the patient; one
red and other white.
17
Eye Motility
3. Worth 4-dot test: This is a simple test using the principle of red-green color
dissociation. It is more dissociating than the Bagolini’s glasses and hence is less
physiological. The apparatus for this test consists of a box containing four
panes of glass, arranged in diamond formation, which are illuminated
internally from a light source. The two internal panes are green, the upper one
is red and lower one is white. The patient wears a green lens in front of the
right eye, and a red lens in front of the left eye. The test can be performed
separately for distance and near vision. The interpretation of this test is as
follows:
b.If the green and red lights alternate, alternating suppression is present.
a. If central fixation is present, the gaps correspond to the visual direction of each
fovea.
18
Binocular Functions
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5772/intechopen.84162
15. Suppression
Suppression involves active inhibition at the visual cortex level when the blurred
image from one eye is inhibited under binocular condition. Pre requisite for sup-
pression is large angle deviation, constant deviation and deviation that occurs in
early childhood.
The extent or the area of suppression can be charted under binocular conditions
(fixating with one eye while the field of other eye is charted). This may be done by
different methods:
19
Eye Motility
This test is used for diagnosing a small facultative scotoma in a patient with
monofixation syndrome and no manifest small deviation. In this test, a 4▲ Base out
Prism is placed before one eye and then other under binocular viewing condition
Patient with bifixation show a bilateral version movement away from the eye
covered by the Prism followed by unilateral fusional convergence movement of the
eye not under the Prism.
In Monofixation no movement is seen when Prism is placed over the nonfixating
eye. A refixation version is seen when Prism is placed over fixating eye but then
fusional convergence does not occur.
The term simultaneous perception does not necessarily mean bifoveal fixation as
it can also occur in Anamolous retinal correspondence. It just indicates the presence
or absence of suppression.
Normal range of amplitudes of fusion:
Divergence 5–7 Δ
Verticgence Supravergence 3Δ
Infravergence 3Δ
Cyclovergence — 2–3 Δ
20
Binocular Functions
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5772/intechopen.84162
It is necessary to assess fusion for both viz. determining the prognosis and
outlining the management of the patients of strabismus.
To restore binocular single vision fusion is essential.
Following are the tests used to determine the presence of fusion are:
• Synaptophore
Following tests can be used to determine stereopsis: Tests to determine stereopsis
are based on two principles viz.
1. The targets used are such that they lie in two planes, but are constructed in
such a way that they cause stimulation of disparate retinal elements which
gives a three-dimensional effect, for example:
• TNO test
• Polaroid test
2. Using targets that are three dimensional (e.g., Lang’s two pencil test)
Tests used to determine stereopsis can be qualitative or quantitative. The unit for
measurement of stereopsis is seconds of an arc.
• Synaptophore
• TNO Test
21
Eye Motility
• Polaroid test
• TNO test
16.1 Stereograms
1. Both eyes should be used together to see the one with three concentric circles
and a check dot for each eye.
2. One with three eccentric circles has to be seen with each eye separately.
Interpretation:
b.Patient sees eccentric circles: Should be asked whether the inner circles
are towards right or left. This will help us determine whether the disparate
elements are suppressed in right or left eye.
B. Polaroid test: There are two types of test that are commonly used
b.With circles: It consists of nine sets of four circles arranged in the form of a
diamond. In this test there are nine sets of four circles which are arranged
to form a diamond. One of these circles is imaged dispared in each set
randomly. The threshold ranges from 800 to 40 s of arc. The child is
asked to push down the circle that stands out. If the stereopsis is limited
the child makes mistakes.
22
Binocular Functions
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5772/intechopen.84162
C. E-random dot test: In this test, there are two cards, one with E printed and
the other is blank. The child is explained about the procedure using a test card.
After explaining the procedure the child is provided with a polaroid glass and
then child is shown the test cards. The child is asked to answer which card has
E printed on it.
D.Random dot stereogram of Julesz: In this test when the child sees
uniocularly, the stereogram reveals random dots scattered everywhere. When
viewed binocularly, there appears a square that lies at various depth
perception above or below the paper. This exposes child to the requirements
similar to that in daily life.
16.2.1 Classification
Divergence insufficiency
Basic esophoria
Basic exophoria
Divergence excess
Ill-sustained accommodation
Accommodative excess
Accommodative infacility
Table 1.
Summary of the classification system for binocular, accommodative and ocular motor anomalies.
23
Eye Motility
Author details
Arvind Kumar Morya1*, Kanchan Solanki2, Sahil Bhandari2 and Anushree Naidu2
© 2019 The Author(s). Licensee IntechOpen. This chapter is distributed under the terms
of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/
by/3.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium,
provided the original work is properly cited.
24
Binocular Functions
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5772/intechopen.84162
References
25