Meier - Reinecke - Postprint - 2020 - CMC, Social Media, and Mental Health - Conceptual and Empirical Meta-Review
Meier - Reinecke - Postprint - 2020 - CMC, Social Media, and Mental Health - Conceptual and Empirical Meta-Review
empirical meta-review
Department of Communication
Acknowledgments
We are grateful for the financial support of this research by the Forschungsschwerpunkt
University Mainz, Germany. We extend our thanks to Catalina Toma and four anonymous
reviewers, who have given invaluable feedback to earlier versions of this manuscript. Our
deep gratitude also goes to Alicia Gilbert, Robin Riemann, Mareike Weiß, Lea Wilke, and
Abstract
Computer-mediated communication (CMC), and specifically social media, may affect the
mental health (MH) and well-being of its users, for better or worse. Research on this topic has
conceptual and operational approaches to CMC and MH and individual review findings is
desperately needed. To this end, we first develop two organizing frameworks that systematize
conceptual and operational approaches to CMC and MH. Based on these frameworks, we
integrate the literature through a meta-review of 34 reviews and a content analysis of 594
social media use and MH. However, effects are complex and depend on the CMC and MH
indicators investigated. Based on our conceptual review and the evidence synthesis, we devise
psychopathology, meta-analysis
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 2
empirical meta-review
Computer-mediated communication (CMC), the Internet, and now social and mobile
media have repeatedly been characterized as a blessing or a curse for users’ mental health
(MH). Widely different claims about the impact of CMC on MH have been reiterated for
decades and across disciplines (e.g., Burke & Kraut, 2016; Chan, 2015; Meier et al., 2020;
Orben & Przybylski, 2019; Twenge et al., 2018). Research on this relationship has
accumulated particularly rapidly in recent years, with a strong focus on social media (Meier et
al., 2020). Yet, the fast-paced, interdisciplinary, and fragmented nature of the field requires
base (for initial meta-reviews, see Appel et al., 2020; Orben, 2020).
A key driver as well as consequence of this state of the field is the conceptual diversity
of researchers’ approaches to CMC and MH. Many studies and reviews seem to work from
narrow, unsystematic approaches to CMC and MH, investigating widely different technology
indicators (e.g., “screen time”, self-presentation on SNS, intensity of Facebook use) and a
satisfaction) (e.g., Huang, 2017; Twomey & O'Reilly, 2017). Recent specification curve
analyses demonstrate that the relationship between CMC and MH can differ drastically,
depending on how researchers operationalize them (e.g., Orben & Przybylski, 2019). Hence,
without a higher-level conceptual and empirical integration, the bigger picture of associations
This study addresses the need for such higher-level integration twofold. We first
develop two organizing frameworks that specify how CMC and MH are conceptualized and
operationalized in the literature. These frameworks allow researchers to navigate the field
more reliably and facilitate systematic identification of patterns, gaps, and conceptual
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 3
conflation. Moreover, they provide the background of our empirical analysis, a meta-review
of systematic reviews and meta-analyses on CMC and MH. This empirical meta-review aims
to (a) synthesize the main findings on the relationships between CMC and MH indicators
from existing reviews. In addition, (b) we seek to apply the two organizing frameworks to the
primary studies included in these reviews, to systematically identify the conceptual foci of
To this end, we first develop the theoretical frameworks of CMC and MH based on
conceptual reviews and relevant empirical literature. Using these frameworks as organizing
principles for the empirical meta-review, we then synthesize the findings from 34
included in these syntheses. By reflecting on the empirical meta-review findings through the
lens of the new organizing frameworks, we conclude with an agenda for future research.
interpersonal message exchange, encompassing everything from mere social attention (e.g.,
browsing through the Instagram feed) to deep communication (e.g., a conversation via
WhatsApp voice call; Hall, 2018). This meta-review also limits ICTs to those whose primary
(e.g., email, mobile texting, instant messenger, social network sites, but not, e.g., games).
These ICTs have been at the center of recent public concern and research regarding MH
effects (e.g., Twenge et al., 2018), thus representing a reasonable focus for this meta-review.
approaches to CMC. A key question guided this conceptual review: How can we organize as
many CMC indicators with as few levels of analysis as possible? Since no such framework
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 4
existed, we used concept mapping (Booth et al., 2012) on all CMC indicators included in
literature reviews on CMC and MH (see Method for the sample of included reviews). That is,
we iteratively mapped out existing CMC measures in a conceptual space to reveal their key
conceptual and operational similarities, hierarchies, and differences. This was done until
theoretical saturation was reached, meaning that no further levels were needed to encompass
all available indicators. Additionally, we grounded the identified levels and approaches in
literature that theorizes CMC (see next section). This approach was advantageous over
adopting, for instance, an affordances approach (Evans et al., 2017), since none of the
reviewed empirical studies on CMC and MH used measures that explicitly operationalized
Instead, by breaking down CMC measures into their basic levels of analysis, we build
a parsimonious taxonomy that applies not just to one single or a few ICTs (e.g., Facebook,
smartphones), but remains useful even in the face of technological change (Ellison & boyd,
2013). This taxonomy should further be exhaustive enough to encompass a wide range of
CMC variables and hence facilitate navigation through the entire research landscape. With
both analytical parsimony and conceptual inclusivity as our guiding principles, we propose
Carr & Hayes, 2015; Ledbetter, 2014). The channel-centered approach aligns with classic
(mass) media uses and effects research that studies the channel as a whole but treats the
communication within the channel largely as a black box. Typical examples for the channel-
centered approach are investigations of “screen time” spent on a device (e.g., the smartphone)
contrary, opens up the channel black box and investigates communication as a complex social
further differentiated into four main levels of analysis: (1) device, (2) type of application, (3)
branded application, and (4) feature. Likewise, the communication-centered approach can be
differentiated into (5) an interaction and (6) a message level. These levels of analysis are
crucial to reflect upon for at least two reasons. First, each level focusses on unique aspects of
CMC. For instance, studies at the device level imply that the presence, absence, or usage of
the device (e.g., the smartphone) itself has implications for MH, irrespective of the specific
applications or features used, or the exact nature of the communication via the device (e.g.,
Gonzales & Wu, 2016). In contrast, studies at the message level may, for instance, assume
that certain message content is the crucial driver of CMC effects on MH (e.g., Holland &
Tiggemann, 2016). Studies differing in the levels at which they operationalize CMC are likely
to differ drastically in how they can explain effects of CMC on MH. They will thus differ in
Second, depending on the level of CMC analysis, studies may differ in the effects they
find. For instance, studies at the interaction level may find that CMC and face-to-face
communication reinforce one another and, thus, CMC can be beneficial for MH. However,
this does not preclude that studies at the device level come to the conclusion that CMC is
negatively related to MH, for instance, because the device can distract from other activities.
Researchers wishing to draw conclusions about the bigger picture of relationships between
CMC and MH need to consider the multiple levels of analysis at which CMC can be studied.
(1) Devices represent the physically palpable ICTs (e.g., laptops, smartphones, or
tablets) that enable CMC. Research at the device level, for instance, investigates how the
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 6
number of devices used to connect to strong and weak ties (i.e., media multiplexity; Chan,
2015), smartphone use during face-to-face interactions (“phubbing”; Gonzales & Wu, 2016),
(2) Devices enable CMC because they allow access to types of applications built
around mediated social interaction and user-generated content. As unique applications often
share a specific set of core characteristics and features, they are studied under a common label
(Ellison & boyd, 2013). For instance, classic types of CMC applications include email, chat
rooms, or discussion boards, later joined by texting and instant messengers. More recently,
applications allowing users to engage in interactions with both broad and narrow audiences
have been defined under the labels of social media, with social network sites (SNS) often
considered a sub-type (see Bayer et al., 2020, for a detailed discussion). Studying such types
of applications is typically more precise than the device level, as it avoids conflating CMC
(3) The branded application level refers to variables that focus only on one or several
branded application(s), such as Facebook or Instagram. While these branded applications can
be subsumed under the broader types outlined above (e.g., SNS), they are frequently studied
individually as key exemplars (e.g., Meier & Schäfer, 2018). It is important to distinguish this
level of analysis from the previous one, as unique applications may have properties and user
cultures that diverge from related applications or their broader types. For instance, while both
Facebook and Twitter are considered SNS, Facebook currently affords more diverse uses
(e.g., closed groups formed around specific interests). Finally, whether research investigates
(4) CMC channels, at their most detailed level of analysis, are constituted by
technical tool […] that enables activity on the part of the user” (Smock et al., 2011, p. 2323).
Facebook users, for instance, may use the site for status updates, comments, private messages,
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 7
groups, the news feed, or any combination of these features, resulting in a unique user
experience, with unique relations to MH (e.g., Burke & Kraut, 2016). Crucially, research
investigating the feature level specifies in more detail the kind of interactions a specific
channel enables. It thus allows researchers to test channel effects even while channels change
specify the interaction level. In contrast to previous levels, this level goes beyond the mere
technological properties of channels and instead clarifies the process of how and with whom
users communicate within a channel. Early on, CMC research conceptualized the
the source and audience size of a communication episode, and distinguished between
synchronous and asynchronous communication (e.g., Morris & Ogan, 1996). Beyond their
configuration, the characteristics of communication partners (e.g., their tie strength) can be
specified and studied in relation to MH (Burke & Kraut, 2016). If either the sender or receiver
network structure of this group (e.g., network size, diversity) can also be considered at this
level. Interactions may further differ in their interaction functions, such as self-disclosure or
self-presentation (Walther, 2010). Another key concept clarifying the how of communication
reaching from two-way truly interactive (e.g., a continuous message exchange), over two-way
through the Instagram feed). Similarly, in research on CMC (specifically, SNS) and MH,
usage is often grouped into “active” and “passive”. While active usage, in its broadest sense,
refers to “activities that facilitate direct exchanges with other(s)” (Verduyn et al., 2017,
p. 281), passive usage refers to the mere consumption of messages from status updates,
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 8
comments, profiles, or stories without any direct response to the sender, akin to classic mass
media usage (e.g., watching TV). Thus, passive usage is entirely non-interactive and instead
can be thought of as one-way communication from the recipient’s perspective, solely entailing
non-directed messages (i.e., messages not sent in reaction to a previous message) (Burke &
Kraut, 2016). Active usage, in contrast, may entail both non-interactive one-way
communication from the sender’s perspective (e.g., posting a status update without getting
any response), as well as two-way reactive, and fully interactive communication (Rafaeli,
1988). In conclusion, the interaction level focusses on social interaction as the process of
message exchange, including instances in which this “exchange” is one-sided (i.e., sending or
(6) While interactions have specific properties, each individual message within an
interaction can be considered as the final level of analysis (Ledbetter, 2014). A first
distinction is made between different modes of messages (e.g., text, image, voice, video, or
one-click reactions such as likes or emojis; Burke & Kraut, 2016; Walther, 2010). While
originally a property of separate (types of) applications (e.g., email vs. video-conferencing),
many modes of communication can now be readily switched within a single application or
even a message exchange (e.g., receiving a text message in WhatsApp and replying with a
short voice recording). The mode of a communication is thus best placed at the message level.
Along with the mode varies bandwidth (i.e., the available cues) and social presence (Walther,
2010). Similarly, the persistence versus ephemerality of a message used to be a fixed channel
characteristic but can now often be modified from message to message (e.g., on Snapchat).
The same applies to the accessibility of a message, varying on a continuum from private to
public (O’Sullivan & Carr, 2018). The content of a message is another key variable at this
Note that the taxonomy organizes the six CMC levels in a hierarchy, emphasizing that
each lower level (e.g., a single message) can be nested in a higher level (e.g., an interaction).
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 9
Thus, necessarily, properties of lower levels (e.g., whether an interaction is active or passive)
can be incorporated at higher levels (e.g., active vs. passive use of Instagram). The six levels
research may often (inadvertently) conflate hierarchical analytical levels, that is, combine
properties of several levels in a single CMC indicator. For instance, “passive usage of the
Facebook news feed” entails information on a unique branded application, a feature, and the
directionality of an interaction process. Finding that such an indicator affects MH raises the
question whether this is caused by Facebook (but not other applications), the news feed (but
not other features), or passive usage (but not other forms of engaging with the Facebook news
through the taxonomy, researchers will be better able to identify at which level(s) of analysis
their explanatory focus is located, hence avoiding conflation and increasing construct validity.
Beyond the two conceptual approaches (channel- vs. communication-centered) and the
six levels of analysis, we supplement our taxonomy with two operational approaches to
separate measurement from level of analysis. Prior research on CMC and MH has used a
staggering number of measures, ranging from time spent with a device, over types of self-
presentation on Facebook, to the content of messages encountered on SNS (e.g., Holland &
Tiggemann, 2016; Twenge et al., 2018; Twomey & O'Reilly, 2017). We contend that the
aspect of technology usage, such as its volume (time spent, frequency) or message content,
which can principally be observed (e.g., digitally tracked), though they are often measured via
component that qualifies how a person processes using a CMC technology or why he or she
uses it, which is often most validly captured by self-reports (e.g., attitudes about technology,
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 10
motivations for usage, perceptions of message content). This distinction is critical, because
the two approaches imply drastically different explanatory foci when relating a CMC variable
to MH. Essentially, the technology-centered approach argues that the mere exposure to some
aspect of a technology itself is related to MH, whereas the user-centered approach explains
any relation between CMC and MH through the user’s psychology in interaction with the
technology. We note that, in principle, both operational approaches can be applied to all six
levels of analysis.
Mental health (MH), according to the World Health Organization, is more than the
absence of mental disorders, but “a state of well-being in which every individual realizes his
or her own potential, can cope with the normal stresses of life, can work productively and
fruitfully, and is able to make a contribution to her or his community” (World Health
recognized and implemented in policy and practice (e.g., Saxena et al., 2013), research on
MH is still mostly divided into two distinct perspectives, psychopathology and psychological
include actions, emotions, motivations, and cognitive and regulatory processes—that causes
personal distress or impairs significant life functions, such as social relationships, education,
work, and health maintenance” (Lahey et al., 2017, p. 143). While well-being, in contrast,
means “how well individuals are doing in life, including social, health, material, and
The present study builds on a two-continua model of mental health that integrates
these two perspectives into a single coherent framework (Greenspoon & Saklofske, 2001;
Keyes, 2007). Several arguments call for such a twofold perspective on MH. First, PTH and
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 11
PWB represent different psychological states. PTH indicates severe disturbance of a person’s
the source(s) of disturbance and inhibits normal functioning until the disturbance has been
mitigated or eliminated (Lahey et al., 2017). PWB, in turn, indicates how well a person is
doing and how much (s)he thrives psychologically. Higher PWB is associated with a variety
of positive outcomes such as longevity and prosocial behavior (Diener et al., 2018). Thus,
PWB is not the absence of PTH, just as PTH is not the absence of PWB. Second, PTH and
PWB are sensitive to different individual and environmental influences (e.g., genes, age, life
events) and their indicators fluctuate in unique patterns and timeframes (Diener et al., 2018;
Lahey et al., 2017). Third, PTH and PWB are sometimes empirically dissociated. That is,
individuals can show high levels on some aspects of PWB while also reporting moderate to
high levels on indicators of PTH, or vice versa (e.g., Greenspoon & Saklofske, 2001; Hides et
al., 2020). In conclusion, researchers should understand and assess MH as two continua, PTH
and PWB, and reflect upon which of these continua is relevant for their research.
Since researchers in the field of CMC and MH employ a variety of so far disconnected
MH indicators (e.g., loneliness, self-esteem, life satisfaction, depression, or anxiety; see, e.g.,
Huang, 2017; Liu et al., 2019), we refine and explicate the classic two-continua model by
integrating main dimensions and manifestations of both PTH and PWB, as well as risk and
Online Appendix I). In doing so, we enable researchers to locate and reflect upon MH
indicators within the broader context of MH research, both clinical and non-clinical. This
should not only facilitate integration of future research on CMC and MH but also lays the
foundation for our empirical meta-review. In the following, we will outline how PTH and
from symptoms (e.g., Lahey et al., 2017). Clusters of symptoms represent the (more or less)
manifest basis for the categorical diagnosis of disorders, which are described in diagnostic
manuals such as the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). A disorder comprises
a set of symptoms relevant for a specific diagnosis (e.g., depressive symptoms for major
depressive disorder). While clinical disorders are categorically diagnosed as either present or
absent (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), symptoms are often measured via self- or
“continuously distributed in the population” (Conway et al., 2019, p. 428) and individuals
healthy from a clinical point of view can show sub-clinical levels of PTH symptomatology.
between PTH disorders are largely artificial, as symptoms across disorders show high
systematic covariation (i.e., comorbidity; Lahey et al., 2017). Specifically, researchers now
underlying latent dimensions (see Conway et al., 2019, for a detailed mapping of disorders
onto PTH dimensions). In the context of CMC research, we focus on the internalizing and
externalizing dimensions of PTH, as these (a) are most widely recognized, especially in
Clinical Psychology research on children and adolescents (e.g., Lahey et al., 2017), and (b)
show the clearest connections to CMC (e.g., Sarmiento et al., 2018). While internalizing PTH
refers to overcontrolled behavior, cognitions, and emotions (e.g., anxiety, social phobia, and
(e.g., hyperactivity, aggression, delinquency, and substance abuse; Conway et al., 2019;
Lahey et al., 2017). We thus extend the dual-factor model of MH by clustering PTH
instead of investigating disconnected PTH indicators, this allows for the recognition of effect
patterns between CMC and higher-level dimensions and manifestations of PTH (see Conway
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 13
et al., 2019, for additional arguments supporting a dimensional approach to PTH). However,
as the research integration of major PTH dimensions is still ongoing (Conway et al., 2019),
future revisions of the MH model may include additional PTH dimensions. Moreover,
categorical diagnoses are expedient for clinical practice and thus remain relevant.
eudaimonic well-being (Huta & Waterman, 2014; Martela & Sheldon, 2019; Ryan & Deci,
2001). According to the hedonic view, happiness and well-being are defined purely by a
approach is Diener et al.’s subjective well-being (Diener et al., 2018; Huta & Waterman,
2014), consisting of the two interrelated components affective well-being (high positive and
low negative affect) and cognitive well-being (satisfaction with life overall and specific life
domains). In contrast, the eudaimonic view understands well-being as more than just pleasure
and satisfaction. Instead, it propagates the realization of a “true self” (i.e., the daímōn), a
concept often associated with striving for meaning and purpose, personal growth, authenticity,
and excellence (Huta & Waterman, 2014). At its core, hedonic well-being is about “feeling
well”, whereas eudaimonic well-being is about “doing well” (Martela & Sheldon, 2019).
While appearing somewhat “elitist” at first glance, eudaimonic well-being is present in the
everyday lives of the general population (for recent reviews, see Huta, 2017; Huta &
Waterman, 2014; Martela & Sheldon, 2019). Individuals may experience eudaimonic well-
hedonic well-being does not have to be associated with increased eudaimonic well-being and
vice versa (Huta, 2017). From this, it follows that investigations into the relationship between
CMC and PWB should consider both sides of well-being, hedonic and eudaimonic.
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 14
distinguished by their manifestations in daily life. Huta (2017; Huta & Waterman, 2014)
proposes that PWB concepts can be differentiated by their (1) category of analysis and (2)
level of measurement (trait vs. state). The category of analysis specifies what exactly the
well-being indicator measures: orientations (i.e., values, motives, and goals), behaviors (i.e.,
overt activities such as socializing or writing a diary), experiences (i.e., subjective cognitive
and affective states), and functioning (i.e., how well a person is doing, e.g., concerning
abilities, accomplishments, or healthy habits; see Huta, 2017, for a detailed description).
Finally, the level of measurement distinguishes between traits that are relatively stable
over time, though not immutable, and states that capture the construct of interest with regard
to a specific timeframe (e.g., in the moment, the last week, or the last month). As these
distinctions crucially specify what exactly researchers are studying when they employ PWB
measures, we incorporate Huta’s distinctions into the MH model (see Fig. A1 in Online
Appendix I). We refer readers interested in the multitude of potential PWB indicators and
their place in this model to Huta (2017), as a detailed mapping of all indicators goes beyond
Saklofske, 2001; Keyes, 2007), we complement it with risk and resilience factors. Adding
these factors appears necessary, as they comprise several variables that have been studied
extensively in relation to CMC and are often interpreted as directly indicative of MH (e.g.,
Huang, 2017; Liu et al., 2019). However, they do not distinctively map onto underlying
the MH literature (see the sections above). Instead, risk factors are here defined as sub-clinical
aspects of psychosocial functioning that are (a) non-specific to PTH or PWB dimensions and
PWB (and vice versa for resilience factors). Risk factors may include perceived loneliness,
actual social isolation, perceived stress, or poor sleep quality, among many others. Resilience
factors include, for instance, social capital, social support, self-esteem, or high sleep quality.
turn our attention to the evidence on the relationship between CMC and MH. Currently,
researchers, practitioners, and members of the general public (e.g., parents, teachers, policy
makers, or entrepreneurs) are left with a disconnected and fast-growing review literature that
lacks higher-level conceptual and empirical integration. We thus aim to move this field
First, we aim to synthesize the main findings on the relationship between CMC and
MH, considering all available evidence that matches the definitions of CMC and MH. Based
on this evidence, we can arrive at (1) more reliable conclusions about the associations
between CMC and MH and (2) the current state of the field as well as (3) discover higher-
level patterns of results. These efforts are guided by the following research question:
RQ1: What are the main findings of research syntheses on the relationship between
Beyond reviewing the findings of research syntheses, we also aim to apply the two
newly developed organizing frameworks to the empirical studies conducted on CMC and MH
so far. Specifically, we seek to explore which levels of CMC analysis and which dimensions
of MH have been primarily investigated so far. In doing so, we will be able to systematically
identify patterns of prior research focus, discuss their implications, and uncover where
research attention may be particularly needed. This is guided by the following question:
RQ2: Which (a) indicators of CMC and (b) indicators of MH have been studied by
prior research and (c) which gaps can be identified based on this assessment?
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 16
The literature proposes multiple theoretical links and boundary conditions for CMC
and MH effects. These include displacement or disruption of activities beneficial for well-
being, such as face-to-face communication or sleep (e.g., Sbarra et al., 2019); social
comparison (Verduyn et al., 2017); or relational maintenance (Burke & Kraut, 2016), among
many others. While these mechanisms currently lack higher-level integration, as well, this is
outside the scope of the present study. Instead, we prioritize conceptual approaches to the key
Method
Montgomery, 2017, p. 92), focusing “on breadth rather than depth of coverage” (Thomson et
al., 2010, p. 198). Therefore, they typically investigate broader constructs (here: CMC and
MH) and include a range of operationalizations. They allow comparisons between research
foci, results, and conclusions from multiple reviews. Thus, meta-reviews help identify
inconsistencies and discord in the literature and point to future directions (Polanin et al.,
2017).
2017), researchers can generally apply the steps undertaken in systematic reviews of primary
research to conduct a meta-review (Polanin et al., 2017). Accordingly, we (1) state pre-
defined eligibility criteria, (2) use a systematic, multi-step literature search, and (3)
systematically synthesize and present the characteristics and findings of included reviews
synthesize information from the primary research included in all reviews to answer RQ2.
Eligibility Criteria
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 17
To be eligible, a review had to meet seven inclusion criteria concerning scope (i.e.,
meet our definitions of (1) CMC and (2) MH, (3) their conceptual independence, and (4)
include investigations of non-clinical samples) and methodology (i.e., synthesis articles had to
be (5) systematic (Booth et al., 2012), (6) synthesize empirical evidence, and be (7) written in
English and published). A more detailed description as well as exclusions resulting from these
criteria can be found in Online Appendix II. Note that we purposefully excluded research on
problematic or addictive ICT usage, as this research, by default, defines and measures CMC
as a pathological behavior that impairs MH. Similarly, we excluded clinical samples since we
were interested in CMC and MH in the general population. In case a review included
participants) next to evidence matching our inclusion criteria, we included it and synthesized
larger effort to review literature on CMC and MH, we conducted standardized academic
database searches, citation searches, and reference searches. This was complemented by a
Google Scholar title search, targeted specifically at finding systematic reviews and meta-
analyses. A detailed description of all steps undertaken in the literature search and selection,
including reliability analysis, can be found in Online Appendix III. The search was first
completed in December 2017 and then updated during peer review in September 2019. The
publications included in more than one review (Pieper et al., 2014). Our sample of reviews
included 1313 unique publications. Based on the formula provided by Pieper et al. (2014),
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 18
overlap can be characterized as “slight”, with a corrected covered area (CCA) of 1.3%. Bias
Methods of Synthesis
Synthesis was conducted in two stages. In stage one, we descriptively synthesized the
information (i.e., narrative conclusions, investigated constructs, effect sizes) from the 34
reviews to answer RQ1. In stage two, we synthesized the CMC and MH indicators
investigated in all relevant primary research publications included in the 34 reviews to answer
RQ2. For this stage, a coding protocol was developed. We first determined whether a
publication was eligible for our meta-review (see eligibility criteria 1-4 and 7) and then coded
all relevant CMC and MH indicators. A description of the coding protocol and results of inter-
Results
To answer RQ1, we first summarize the narrative conclusions about the relationship
between CMC and MH from all 34 reviews. Since the reviews included 14 meta-analyses and
these provide more informative and conclusive evidence synthesis than narrative reviews, we
then summarize the meta-analytic effects, effect heterogeneity, and moderator analyses.
Narrative Conclusions
First results on RQ1 (see Online Appendix IV for details) show that 14 out of 34
reviews concluded the relationship was mixed, finding evidence for positive, negative, and
non-significant associations between CMC and MH. Notably, these were mostly narrative
reviews rather than meta-analyses. While an additional 11 reviews concluded that negative
relationships prevailed, 6 found predominantly positive relationships between CMC and MH.
However, these six reviews exclusively synthesized evidence on social resources (capital or
support) and/or older adults. Notably, 24 of 34 reviews qualified the investigated effects as
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 19
mediators. Finally, 7 reviews qualified the evidence as insufficient for a definitive conclusion.
Meta-Analytic Effects
indicators that matched our conceptual definitions. Almost all meta-analyses focused on
indicators of global SNS use (i.e., time spent, frequency, and/or intensity). We refer to these
simply as SNS use below and summarize all effects of SNS use in Fig. 2. As meta-analyses
mostly assessed the type of or branded application levels, we organize this section along the
SNS use, if they rely on k > 2 effect sizes. If multiple effects for the same relationship are
available, we only report the one relying on the largest number of k effect sizes within the
text. For details on all effect sizes, meta-analyses, and references, see Online Appendix V.
resources (capital and support) showed small to moderate positive associations with SNS use.
While general Internet use, blogs, chat, and email were not significantly associated with
perceived social resources, SNS (r = .30, 95% CI [.14; .46]) and forum use (r = .14, 95% CI
[.09; .20]) were. Notably, user-centered attitudinal measures of “intensity” (e.g., the Facebook
intensity scale) consistently generated larger effect sizes than technology-centered ones (time
spent or frequency). Almost all SNS features and interaction properties of SNS use were
positively associated with increased social resources, albeit at varying strength (see Online
Appendix V for details and references). Only few meta-analyses specifically investigated self-
esteem, and none reported findings on other resilience factors. General time spent online was
unrelated to self-esteem, but SNS use was slightly negatively related to self-esteem in three
meta-analyses finding similar effect sizes (e.g., r = -.05, 95% CI [-.09; -.01]).
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 20
found (see also RQ2 below). General time spent online showed a small negative association
with life satisfaction (r = -.05, 95% CI [-.12; -.01]). SNS use, however, showed no significant
association with life satisfaction in two meta-analyses. One meta-analysis reported an overall
effect size of SNS use (i.e., global use, number of friends, active and passive use) on “positive
indicators of MH”, comprising life satisfaction, well-being, self-esteem, and positive affect (r
= .05, 95% CI [.01; .08]). However, when separated by SNS indicators, only the number of
SNS friends showed a small positive association with “positive MH” (r = .13, 95% CI [.05;
.21]). Three other meta-analyses reported effects on “well-being” that included reverse coded
negative indicators (e.g., depressive symptoms or loneliness) alongside resilience factors (e.g.,
self-esteem) and life satisfaction. Time spent online was found to be slightly negatively
associated with such “overall well-being” (r = -.04, 95% CI [-.07; -.01]), though this
relationship was nonsignificant for social Internet use. SNS use, however, was slightly
negatively associated with overall well-being in two meta-analyses (e.g., r = -.06, 95% CI [-
.09; -.03]). Differentiating between SNS uses revealed that “self-presentational” use (status
searching, monitoring) was negatively (r = -.14, 95% CI [-.20; -.08]), and “interactions”
(replying, commenting, liking) were positively related (r = .14, 95% CI [.08; .20]). While
phone calls showed a small positive association with overall well-being (r = .10, 95% CI [.06;
.15]), texting and instant messenger use were not related to overall well-being.
Risk factors. Findings on risk factors are limited to loneliness and stress. While two
smaller meta-analyses (both k = 23) found a small positive association between SNS use and
loneliness, a considerably larger one (k = 196) found no association (r = .01, 95% CI [-.02;
.05]). Phone calls, texting, and instant messaging showed small negative associations with
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 21
loneliness, though based on only a few studies each (see Online Appendix V for details). SNS
use showed a small positive association with stress (r = .13, 95% CI [.05; .21]).
was depressive symptoms. No meta-analyses of externalizing PTH were found (see also RQ2
below). Five meta-analytic effect sizes for the relationship between SNS use and depressive
symptoms existed, all showing a small positive association (e.g., r = .11, 95% CI [.08; .14]).
In addition, one meta-analysis reported a small positive association between general social
comparison on SNS and depressive symptoms (r = .23, 95% CI [.12; .34]), and a somewhat
higher one for upward comparison (r = .33, 95% CI [.20; .47]). General time spent online was
.07; -.02]), while instant messaging was not associated. SNS use further showed a small
positive relation to social anxiety (r = .10, 95% CI, [.05; .15]) and to anxiety symptoms in
general (r = .10, 95% CI [.03; .18]). Time spent online, instant messaging, texting, or email
use were not related to (social) anxiety. However, social comfort experienced online (r = .34,
95% CI [.25; .41]) and comfort specifically due to reduced non-verbal cues online (r = .27,
95% CI [.23; .31]) showed moderate positive associations with social anxiety.
One meta-analysis focused on SNS and body image disturbance, which can be
considered an indicator of internalizing PTH. Combining all measures of SNS use (general
use and appearance-focused use), there was a small positive association with disturbed body
image (r = .17, 95% CI [.13; .21]). When analyzed separately, similar effects were found for
using multiple SNS or Facebook, but not for Instagram or other SNS (though based on k < 5).
Notably, technology-centered measures of SNS use showed about a third of the effect (r =
.11, 95% CI [.08; .15]) of appearance-focused use (r = .31, 95% CI [.22; .39]), which included
reported an overall effect of SNS use (i.e., global use, number of friends, active and passive
use) on “negative indicators of MH”, comprising depression, anxiety, and loneliness (r = .06,
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 22
95% CI [.03; .09]). However, when separated by indicators, only global SNS use (time spent,
frequency) showed a small association with negative MH (r = .11, 95% CI [.06; .15]).
All meta-analyses tested for effect size heterogeneity based on the Q statistic, and
nearly all concluded that there was “significant heterogeneity”, with I² often exceeding 75%.
We thus synthesized findings from moderator analyses on three key sample characteristics
found that with increasing age the effects of SNS use on MH became less negative
respectively. Two others found that the relationship between several CMC measures and
social anxiety was stronger in older samples. Seven meta-analyses found no effect of age.
Overall, there is little evidence for age effects, but age had a range restricted to young users in
most analyses.
in study samples. Three meta-analyses found some evidence for a moderation by gender,
albeit with no consistent overall trend for who benefited more or less from SNS use. Seven
meta-analyses found no gender effects. Overall, there is little meta-analytic evidence for
gender effects.
country. Only one found no moderation effect. However, the evidence from the remaining six
is incoherent, with two finding more positive effects in Western/individualistic countries, two
in Eastern/collectivistic countries, and two finding mixed results. Overall, culture seems to be
Publication bias. Seven meta-analyses concluded that there was “no bias” at all.
Three meta-analyses concluded there was “little bias” and two found “some bias” for specific
CMC indicators. Accordingly, meta-analysts overall found little evidence of publication bias.
One key source of the high heterogeneity of effects in previous meta-analyses may be
the diversity with which CMC and MH are operationalized in studies. To systematize this
diversity and answer RQ2, we turn to the primary research included in all 34 reviews. Of the
1313 publications coded, 594 (45%) met our eligibility criteria 1-4 and 7. The remaining 719
publications were excluded due to lack of a relevant MH (30%) or CMC variable (15%), the
manuscript being unpublished (16%) or its full text unavailable (10%), or because the
contained only qualitative research, which was unsuitable for this stage of synthesis.
Regarding CMC, most publications included either one (23%) or two (24%)
indicators, followed by three (19%), four (16%), or more (18%) (M = 3, SD = 2.1). Of the
1829 CMC indicators in total, 51% addressed more than just one of the six CMC levels of
analysis. This demonstrates considerable conflation of analytical levels within many CMC
measures. Turning to the four levels of the channel-centered approach, 16% of all indicators
addressed the device level (of which 68% mobile/smartphone, 19% computer, 8% various1,
5% other), 27% the types of application level (43% SNS, 15% various, 13% texting, 12%
social media, 6% email, 4% instant messenger, 7% other), 54% the branded application level
(78% Facebook, 4% Instagram, 9% various, 9% other), and 15% the feature level (37%
various, 24% status update, 15% profile, 8% comment, 16% other). With regard to the two
interaction level (27% network characteristics, 18% sending messages one-to-one or one-to-
1
“Various“ refers to measures that address several manifestations of the same level (e.g., several devices, apps,
interaction characteristics) in a single indicator.
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 24
many, 9% self-disclosure, 8% passive usage, 38% other) and 9% the message level (51%
addressed aspects of both channel and communication, suggesting that lower levels
(interaction or message) were often studied in the context of a specific channel (e.g., a
branded application). Six percent of indicators assessed generalized “Internet use”, neither
most indicators followed the technology-centered (69%) rather than the user-centered
Concerning MH, most publications included only one MH indicator (43%), followed
by two (28%), three (16%) or more (13%) (M = 2, SD = 1.5). Of the 1258 MH indicators in
total, 28% addressed internalizing PTH (of which 39% depressive symptoms, 22% social
anxiety/social phobia, 14% anxiety symptoms, 11% eating disorder symptoms, 14% other),
3% externalizing PTH (e.g., substance abuse, aggression, AD/HD), 18% hedonic PWB (36%
life satisfaction, 25% domain-specific satisfaction, 21% affect, 10% discrete emotions, 8%
other), 2% eudaimonic PWB (e.g., meaning, authenticity, mastery), 17% risk factors (53%
loneliness, 20% poor sleep, 19% stress, 8% other) and 31% resilience factors (38% self-
esteem, 24% social support, 22% social capital, 8% good sleep, 8% other). Thus, the most
studied indicators overall were risk and resilience factors (47%), followed by PTH (31%) and
PWB (20%). A majority of PTH (57%) and PWB (79%) indicators as well as risk (84%) and
resilience factors (91%) were measured at the trait level, without specifying a timeframe.
Discussion
Extending prior work (Appel et al., 2020; Orben, 2020), this study synthesized the
media, and MH through a meta-review. Our contribution to the literature is twofold. First, we
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 25
contribute to theory building by presenting two parsimonious frameworks that offer increased
connecting and comparing review findings (RQ1) as well as units of analysis (RQ2).
In a first step, we synthesized main findings of prior reviews (RQ1). This offers
several key insights. (1) Meta-analyses condensing various CMC and MH measures into one
overall effect size find a (very) small negative association (r ≈ -.05 to -.15). Yet, when
associations are investigated by CMC and MH indicators separately, effect patterns become
more complex. (2) There is consistent evidence that those who use SNS more intensely
perceive moderately (r ≈ .20 to .40) increased social resources (social capital and support).
However, there is little evidence for other positive associations between CMC and MH. (3)
The remaining evidence consistently suggests those who use SNS more intensely experience
slightly (r ≈ .05 to .20) more internalizing PTH (e.g., depressive symptoms), stress, and lower
self-esteem. (3) Meta-analyses show no evidence for an association between SNS use and life
satisfaction, the only meta-analyzed PWB indicator. Thus, SNS use is not associated with the
cognitive side of hedonic well-being. The largest available meta-analysis also revealed no
association between SNS use and loneliness. (4) There was little indication of publication bias
across meta-analyses. Nonetheless, effect sizes should be interpreted in light of evidence that
meta-analyses produce almost three-times larger effects than preregistered replication studies
(5) For applications other than SNS, the evidence base is small and, overall, shows
little to no association with MH. There is narrative review evidence for a negative association
between the device level and MH, specifically for mobile CMC. However, this requires
further meta-analytic synthesis. (6) The meta-analytic evidence for the feature or interaction
level (e.g., active vs. passive use) is scarce and inconsistent (cf. Online Appendix V).
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 26
However, it currently suggests that effects are more nuanced than for higher levels of the
CMC taxonomy (i.e., types of or branded applications). The clearest pattern for the message
disturbance. Overall, findings suggest the need for more systematic research relating the
feature, interaction, and message levels to MH. (7) Across several meta-analyses, there was
consistent evidence that user-centered measures (e.g., attitudes toward Facebook, social
comparison on SNS) resulted in two- to three times larger effect sizes than technology-
centered ones (e.g., time spent, frequency). Whether this suggests that user-centered measures
reveal stronger, potentially more relevant effects or produce artifacts due to, for instance,
(8) Among all 34 reviews, the most common narrative conclusion was that effects
for moderating effects of age and gender—despite popular concerns about more negative
effects particularly among younger and female users (e.g., Twenge et al., 2018). It should be
noted, however, that the age range was quite restricted (participants were mostly adolescents
or young adults) and that narrative reviews on CMC among older adults highlighted mostly
positive effects, specifically on social resources. Thus, future research needs to sample across
the life span (e.g., Chan, 2015). The culture or country a study was conducted in did emerge
should thus compare cultures more systematically. Overall, research needs to test additional
Given the range of average effects across meta-analyses (i.e., r ≈ .00 to |.40|), how
researchers measure CMC and MH seems to matter considerably for the conclusions drawn in
this field (see also Orben & Przybylski, 2019). In a second step, we thus synthesized
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 27
conceptual and operational approaches (RQ2). Based on the detailed analysis of 1829 CMC
(1) Research has largely relied on the channel-centered (e.g., devices, applications)
rather than the communication-centered (e.g., interactions, messages) approach. Notably, the
default approach of the field has been to study individual branded applications, specifically
Facebook. This limits the evidence base severely, as findings on single applications may
demonstrate little generalizability over time (e.g., due to changes in design or popularity).
Instead, identifying key features used for CMC in numerous applications (e.g., status updates,
profiles, private messages) should be a more future-proof way to study channels (Bayer et al.,
2020).
(2) The measures of CMC in this field show considerable conflation of analytical
levels, thus potentially resulting in misattribution of effects to the wrong causes (e.g., to
“screen time” on a device rather than to a certain type of interaction). Research on the
communication-centered approach (i.e., the interaction and message level), specifically, has
conflated most measures with individual channels (e.g., “passive Facebook use”). Given that
users now communicate via a multitude of channels simultaneously (i.e., media multiplexity;
Chan, 2015) and the dynamic design changes of these channels, the low generalizability of the
channel-centered approach also applies to most of the available evidence at the interaction and
message level. Research should thus strive to develop measures that capture interaction and
low generalizability of the current channel-centered approach (Bayer et al., 2020), studying
media as overall “good” or “bad”); (c) helps clarify whether one assumes effects to result
(O’Sullivan & Carr, 2018) rather than unspecific “screen time”; and (d) allows for more
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 28
nuanced conclusions about the causes of any effects, hence facilitating the development of
(3) Beyond illuminating conceptual approaches, our analysis shows that researchers
have largely relied on technology-centered (e.g., time spent, frequency) rather than user-
centered operational approaches (i.e., how technology use was processed). However, both
reports, are notoriously unreliable (Orben, 2020) and risk conflation of distinct phenomena
such as interpersonal and mass communication (O’Sullivan & Carr, 2018). User-centered
measures, in contrast, may artificially inflate the association between outcome (i.e.,
perceptions of MH) and predictor (i.e., perceptions of CMC). Moreover, they may result in
finding upward comparison on Instagram negatively affects well-being cannot inform upon
Meier & Schäfer, 2018). Our recommendation for future research is therefore a combination
of the technology- and user-centered approaches. Studies should strive to measure technology
use descriptively, ideally via digital tracking (e.g., Bayer et al., 2018) and at multiple levels of
the taxonomy, to allow level comparisons. Additionally, studies should assess key
motivations and psychological processes that occur across channels (e.g., social comparison
or social support seeking), and test how these processes are modulated by channel features
far and the measures identified in our conceptual synthesis. Meta-analytic evidence is mostly
limited to “global SNS use”, that is, time spent on, frequency of, or intensity of using a SNS,
while many more CMC measures exist. More research on the other levels (i.e., devices,
features, interactions, messages), and meta-analyses comparing these levels, are needed to
ground conclusions about the role of CMC for MH in a more comprehensive evidence base.
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 29
(1) Existing research focusses largely on internalizing PTH, the cognitive side of
hedonic PWB (i.e., life satisfaction and domain-specific satisfaction), and risk and resilience
factors. Research has paid less attention to eudaimonic and affective PWB as well as
externalizing PTH. Yet, these dimensions capture relevant and unique aspects of MH.
Ignoring them in empirical research on CMC may thus overlook crucial effect patterns.
Recent research, for instance, suggests that conclusions about the effects of social comparison
on SNS partly depend on whether one investigates internalizing PTH (e.g., depression) or
outcomes such as inspiration (eudaimonic PWB) and positive affect (hedonic PWB) (Meier &
Schäfer, 2018). Externalizing PTH (e.g., aggression) could, in turn, be affected by online
incivility and may be a more relevant PTH indicator among men (e.g., Kramer et al., 2008).
The field should thus broaden its empirical approach in order to cover the two continua of
MH more completely.
(2) A second finding is a strong reliance on risk (e.g., loneliness) and resilience factors
(e.g., self-esteem). These factors tap into important aspects of psychosocial functioning,
relevant to MH in multiple ways. They are crucial predictors or boundary conditions for MH
(e.g., social support as a buffer that increases PWB; Burke & Kraut, 2016) or link CMC
indirectly to more central MH indicators (e.g., stress as a risk factor for depressive symptoms;
Aalbers et al., 2019). However, our review of MH literature reveals that none of the
prominent risk and resilience factors (e.g., self-esteem, loneliness, social capital) is integrated
into current conceptual models of PTH or PWB. This remains an important task for MH
research at large. For researchers interested in effects of CMC on MH, this suggests, however,
that to truly measure MH studies should include indicators more central to our current
CMC, which hinders research synthesis. The field should thus agree on a core outcome set of
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 30
MH indicators (Brunton et al., 2020). If studies were to measure a set of the same indicators,
tapping into core aspects of MH, this would greatly enhance evidence accumulation and
research integration (e.g., meta-analyses). Our tentative proposal for such an outcome set
internalizing and externalizing PTH symptoms (Conway et al., 2019); cognitive and affective
well-being (Diener et al., 2018); meaning as the most useful “proxy for eudaimonic
experience” (Huta, 2017, p. 22); and competence, autonomy, and relatedness need satisfaction
as a self-determination theory approach to eudaimonia (Martela & Sheldon, 2019). This set
may, of course, be complemented by key risk and resilience factors (e.g., self-esteem,
(4) Finally, findings show that most evidence on CMC and MH relies on trait level
assessments of MH, that is, measures that do not specify a timeframe. This is problematic for
specific timeframes (e.g., Diener et al., 2018), which the measurement should reflect. Second,
For instance, from a network perspective on PTH, phenomena such as depression are “a
complex, dynamic network of symptoms that cause each other” (Aalbers et al., 2019,
p. 1454). Thus, risk factors such as stress, and depressive symptoms such as sad mood, may
cause other, increasingly more severe symptoms (e.g., suicidal ideation; Aalbers et al., 2019).
Identifying at which points of this temporal symptom network CMC is particularly relevant is
thus a crucial direction for future research. More generally, MH research should theorize and
test the dynamic interplay between PTH and PWB indicators over time. For instance,
individuals suffering from internalizing PTH may lack the energy necessary to pursue
eudaimonic PWB. Finally, a temporal perspective on MH and CMC would also sensitize for
Limitations
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 31
CMC and MH is largely based on small-scale, cross-sectional studies. The findings on the
association, let alone causal order, of CMC and MH should be treated as preliminary (for an
extended discussion, see Orben, 2020). In addition, our review, while relying on
limited. First, we excluded some research areas, particularly on “addictive” usage of CMC
and cyberbullying. These may come to different conclusions about the relationship between
CMC and MH. Second, we excluded evidence from clinical samples, as research on these
populations differs markedly from the evidence reviewed here. Third, we did not review
theoretical mechanisms on the relationship between CMC and MH. Several reviews provide
crucial syntheses of such mechanisms (e.g., Bayer et al., 2020; Liu et al., 2019; Sbarra et al.,
boundary conditions is outside the scope of our work. Fourth, our conceptual framework of
MH by no means reflects and integrates all approaches to, and dimensions of, MH. For
instance, there may be several additional dimensions of PTH beyond the internalizing and
externalizing spectra (see Conway et al., 2019). Rather, our proposed MH model presents a
working model covering the most relevant aspects of PTH and PWB that current theorizing
from Clinical and Positive Psychology can agree on. We call on future researchers to revise
any literature review is a time lag between the available evidence and the evidence included in
the review. Thus, there may be conceptual and empirical approaches to CMC and MH this
meta-review does not include. However, given the scope of our evidence base, spanning
Conclusion
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 32
Public concern and research attention on the impact of CMC, specifically social
media, on the mental health and well-being of (young) users has dramatically increased in
recent years. This study offers a conceptual and empirical review of reviews. Findings suggest
an overall (very) small negative association between using SNS, the most researched CMC
application, and mental health. Findings further show, however, that associations partly
depend on the choice of MH indicators. On both conceptual and empirical grounds, research
thus needs to develop and measure a more comprehensive set of MH outcomes, so as not to
overlook effects. Moreover, associations become more complex when research addresses not
just the channels used for CMC (i.e., “screen time” spent on devices or applications), but the
types of interactions and messages transmitted via those channels. Instead of investigating
“screen time” monolithically, the new decade of research on CMC, social media, and MH
should operationalize channels through their core features, tease apart the types of interactions
users engage in across channels, and consider the characteristics of messages they send and
receive. Ideally, research tests how these interactions and messages are modulated by the core
features and affordances of social media. By reflecting on the CMC taxonomy proposed here,
specifically by avoiding conflation of its levels in measures, future research can more
rigorously test which uses of social media contribute to, impair, or are irrelevant for mental
health.
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 33
References
Aalbers, G., McNally, R. J., Heeren, A., Wit, S. de, & Fried, E. I. (2019). Social media and
Appel, M., Marker, C., & Gnambs, T. (2020). Are social media ruining our lives? A review of
https://doi.org/10.1177/1089268019880891
Ballard, M., & Montgomery, P. (2017). Risk of bias in overviews of reviews: A scoping
Bayer, J. B., Ellison, N., Schoenebeck, S., Brady, E., & Falk, E. B. (2018). Facebook in
context(s): Measuring emotional responses across time and space. New Media & Society,
Bayer, J. B., Triệu, P., & Ellison, N. B. (2020). Social media elements, ecologies and effects.
010419-050944
Booth, A., Papaioannou, D., & Sutton, A. (2012). Systematic approaches to a successful
Brunton, G., Webbe, J., Oliver, S., & Gale, C. (2020). Adding value to core outcome set
248–259. https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.1391
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 34
Burke, M., & Kraut, R. E. (2016). The relationship between Facebook use and well-being
Carr, C. T., & Hayes, R. A. (2015). Social media: Defining, developing, and divining. Atlantic
Chan, M. (2015). Multimodal connectedness and quality of life: Examining the influences of
technology adoption and interpersonal communication on well-being across the life span.
https://doi.org/10.1111/jcc4.12089
Conway, C. C., Forbes, M. K., Forbush, K. T., Fried, E. I., Hallquist, M. N., Kotov, R.,
Mullins-Sweatt, S. N., Shackman, A. J., Skodol, A. E., South, S. C., Sunderland, M.,
Waszczuk, M. A., Zald, D. H., Afzali, M. H., Bornovalova, M. A., Carragher, N.,
Diener, E., Lucas, R. E., & Oishi, S. (2018). Advances and open questions in the science of
https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.115
Ellison, N. B., & boyd, d. m. (2013). Sociality through social network sites. In W. H. Dutton
(Ed.), The Oxford handbook of Internet studies (pp. 151–172). Oxford University Press.
Evans, S. K., Pearce, K. E., Vitak, J., & Treem, J. W. (2017). Explicating affordances: A
Gonzales, A. L., & Wu, Y. (2016). Public cellphone use does not activate negative responses
https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007219227883
Hall, J. A. (2018). When is social media use social interaction? Defining mediated social
https://doi.org/10.1177/1461444816660782
Hides, L., Quinn, C., Stoyanov, S., Cockshaw, W., Kavanagh, D. J., Shochet, I., Deane, F.,
Kelly, P., & Keyes, C. L. M. (2020). Testing the interrelationship between mental well-
being and mental distress in young people. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 15(3),
314–324. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2019.1610478
Holland, G., & Tiggemann, M. (2016). A systematic review of the impact of the use of social
networking sites on body image and disordered eating outcomes. Body Image, 17, 100–
110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.02.008
Huang, C. (2017). Time spent on social network sites and psychological well-being: A meta-
https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2016.0758
Reinecke & M. B. Oliver (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of media use and well-being:
International perspectives on theory and research on positive media effects (pp. 14–33).
Routledge.
Huta, V., & Waterman, A. S. (2014). Eudaimonia and its distinction from hedonia:
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-013-9485-0
Kramer, M. D., Krueger, R. F., & Hicks, B. M. (2008). The role of internalizing and
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291707001572
Kvarven, A., Strømland, E., & Johannesson, M. (2020). Comparing meta-analyses and
434. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0787-z
Lahey, B. B., Krueger, R. F., Rathouz, P. J., Waldman, I. D., & Zald, D. H. (2017). A
https://doi.org/10.1080/10510974.2014.927298
Liu, D., Baumeister, R. F., Yang, C.-C., & Hu, B. (2019). Digital communication media use
Martela, F., & Sheldon, K. M. (2019). Clarifying the concept of well-being: Psychological
need satisfaction as the common core connecting eudaimonic and subjective well-being.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1089268019880886
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 37
Meier, A., Domahidi, E., & Günther, E. (2020). Computer-mediated communication and
R. E. Rice (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of digital technology and society (pp. 79–110).
Meier, A., & Schäfer, S. (2018). The positive side of social comparison on social network
sites: How envy can drive inspiration on Instagram. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and
Morris, M., & Ogan, C. (1996). The Internet as mass medium. Journal of Computer-Mediated
O’Sullivan, P. B., & Carr, C. T. (2018). Masspersonal communication: A model bridging the
https://doi.org/10.1177/1461444816686104
Orben, A. (2020). Teenagers, screens and social media: A narrative review of reviews and key
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-019-01825-4
Orben, A., & Przybylski, A. K. (2019). The association between adolescent well-being and
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0506-1
Pieper, D., Antoine, S.-L., Mathes, T., Neugebauer, E. A. M., & Eikermann, M. (2014).
Systematic review finds overlapping reviews were not mentioned in every other overview.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2013.11.007
Polanin, J. R., Maynard, B. R., & Dell, N. A. (2017). Overviews in education research: A
https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654316631117
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 38
Rafaeli, S. (1988). Interactivity: From new media to communication. Sage Annual Review of
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: A review of research
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.141
Sarmiento, I. G., Olson, C., Yeo, G., Chen, Y. A., Toma, C. L., Brown, B. B., Bellmore, A.,
& Mares, M.-L. (2018). How does social media use relate to adolescents’ internalizing
Saxena, S., Funk, M., & Chisholm, D. (2013). World health assembly adopts comprehensive
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61139-3
Sbarra, D. A., Briskin, J. L., & Slatcher, R. B. (2019). Smartphones and close relationships:
596–618. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691619826535
Smock, A. D., Ellison, N. B., Lampe, C., & Wohn, D. Y. (2011). Facebook as a toolkit: A
uses and gratification approach to unbundling feature use. Computers in Human Behavior,
Thomson, D., Russell, K., Becker, L., Klassen, T., & Hartling, L. (2010). The evolution of a
new publication type: Steps and challenges of producing overviews of reviews. Research
Twenge, J. M., Martin, G. N., & Campbell, W. K. (2018). Decreases in psychological well-
being among American adolescents after 2012 and links to screen time during the rise of
Verduyn, P., Ybarra, O., Résibois, M., Jonides, J., & Kross, E. (2017). Do social network sites
enhance or undermine subjective well-being? A critical review. Social Issues and Policy
D. R. Roskos-Ewoldsen (Eds.), The handbook of communication science (2nd ed., pp. 489–
505). Sage.
World Health Organization. (2005). Promoting mental health: Concepts, emerging evidence,
practice. http://www.who.int/mental_health/evidence/MH_Promotion_Book.pdf
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 40
Figures
Note. Effects within as well as between individual publications are not independent, due to overlap of primary studies. Empty cells are due to missing
information. Larger effect size squares correspond to narrower confidence intervals. Effect sizes are sorted by resilience factors, positive MH
indicators, risk factors, negative MH indicators. *Liu and Baumeister reported 95% credible intervals instead of confidence intervals.
Figure 2. Forest plot of effect sizes for global SNS use (i.e., time spent, frequency, and/or intensity) and mental health
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW – ONLINE APPENDIX 42
empirical meta-review
-- Online Appendix --
Contents
Appendix I: The Extended Two-Continua Model of Mental Health ....................................... 43
Appendix III: Details of the Systematic Literature Search and Coding ................................... 50
Appendix IV: Descriptive Overview of Included Meta-Analyses and Systematic Reviews ... 54
Figure A1
The extended two-continua model of mental health
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW - ONLINE APPENDIX 44
Note. The two-continua model of mental health is based on Greenspoon and Saklofske (2001) and Keyes (2005). The distinction between
internalizing and externalizing PTH dimensions is based on various sources (e.g., Conway et al., 2019; Krueger et al., 2001; Lahey et al., 2017). The
explication of PTH manifestations as disorders and symptoms is based on the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). The distinction
between the hedonic and eudaimonic well-being dimensions is based on various sources (e.g., Diener et al., 2018; Huta & Waterman, 2014; Martela
& Sheldon, 2019; Ryan & Deci, 2001). Manifestations of PWB (i.e., category of analysis and level of measurement) are explicated in Huta (2017)
and Huta and Waterman (2014). The distinction of risk and resilience factors is made by the authors, based on extensive literature on these concepts
and their lacking integration into existing models of PTH and PWB.
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW - ONLINE APPENDIX 45
Below, we note all seven eligibility criteria of the meta-review in detail and explain
1. The synthesis had to investigate CMC as non-pathological usage of ICTs whose primary
a. In line with our CMC definition and previous work in this field (Huang, 2017;
addictions such as SNS addictions (cf., e.g., Çikrıkci, 2016; Elhai et al., 2017;
addictive usage does not provide evidence about the relationship between non-
pathological, everyday usage of CMC and MH, which is the focus of this
meta-review.
b. For this reason, we also excluded research on extreme forms of mediated social
relevant for MH, these forms of CMC are highly specific concerning message
style, interaction context, and user characteristics. Moreover, they have been
extensively reviewed elsewhere and thus lie outside the scope of this review
(Chen et al., 2017; Kosenko et al., 2017; Kowalski et al., 2014; Kwan et al.,
2010; Ferguson et al., 2011; Greitemeyer & Mügge, 2014; Li et al., 2016;
also excluded (e.g., Klaps et al., 2016), as these do not inform research on
2. The synthesis had to investigate MH with at least one construct that is an established
(see Appendix II) and the literature this model is based on (e.g., Conway et al.,
2019; Huta & Waterman, 2014). Thus, if a review assessed only outcomes not
al., 2017; Liu & Yang, 2016; Yang & Shen, 2018). If a review confounded
narcissism or the “big five” were excluded (e.g., Gnambs & Appel, 2018; Liu
not indicative of MH per se, particularly due to its relatively high temporal
al., 2014), were included in this review (e.g., Liu & Baumeister, 2016).
3. The synthesis had to be based on research that assessed CMC and MH as distinct and
association.
al., 2008) were excluded for this reason (see also Gilmour et al., 2020). While
these reviews provide insights into processes crucial to CMC research, they do
not explicitly inform research on the empirical association between CMC and
MH. For instance, the review by Derks et al. (2008) synthesized evidence on
from this research, it is impossible to assess a media effect, that is, whether
b. For the same reason, reviews assessing how CMC content (e.g., Facebook
4. Fourth, the synthesis had to include studies with healthy, non-clinical participants from
excluded for this reason (e.g., Grajales et al., 2014, Laranjo et al., 2015; Rains
b. Research on the effects of CMC among people with special needs (e.g.,
5. Fifth, concerning review methodology, the synthesis had to contain a systematic and, in
principle, replicable literature search (i.e., use databases and search terms), clearly
specified eligibility criteria, and should not fully overlap with a more recent review.
even if they reviewed relevant research literature and provided insights into
their respective subject matter (e.g., Bargh & McKenna, 2004; Verduyn et al.,
all studies from Mingoia et al. (2017) was available (i.e., Saiphoo & Vahedi,
2019).
6. Sixth, the synthesis article had to contain empirical studies (quantitative and/or
a. We are unaware of any articles that would have to be excluded for this reason,
evidence.
7. Seventh, we only included articles written in English and those that were published or
a. We are unaware of any articles that would have to be excluded for this reason,
(1) First, as part of an ongoing effort to identify relevant literature on CMC and MH,
Abstracts, EconLit, LISTA, PSYNDEX; ScienceDirect; and Web of Science) using pretested
search terms. The search string used Boolean operators to combine synonyms of CMC with
OR “quality of life” OR “the full life” OR “life satisfaction” OR “satisfaction with life” OR
OR “SWB”).
A number of search terms (e.g., social support, social capital, psychopathology, mental
health, depression) were considered during string development, but excluded from the final
string. We decided to exclude these terms due to very high rates of false-positive hits
(sometimes in the tens of thousands), which would have decreased search precision and thus
impeded feasibility. This first search was restricted to the timespan from January 1995 to
April 2016.
We retrieved 9.427 abstracts from the database searches, which were then pre-
screened for relevant articles (both primary research studies and reviews) by three trained
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW - ONLINE APPENDIX 51
student coders (two undergraduates, one graduate) based on a coding protocol. Inclusion and
exclusion criteria for the screening were the same as the ones outlined in Appendix III, with
two exceptions. For this first step of the search, we also included research on problematic or
addictive forms of CMC as studies in this field often assess regular CMC as well (i.e., not just
scales of problematic or pathological usage, but also of regular usage) and often rely on non-
clinical samples (see, e.g., Tokunaga & Rains, 2010). Moreover, at this point, we still
agreement = 96% and Krippdendorf’s alpha = .73. Intra-coder reliabilities with a one month
difference between T1 and T2 were Coder 1: Pairwise agreement = 96%, Krippendorf’s alpha
= .68; Coder 2: Pairwise agreement = 96%, Krippendorf’s alpha = .68; Coder 3: Pairwise
agreement = 93%, Krippendorf’s alpha = .62. It should be noted that the comparatively low
alpha coefficients are strongly influenced by the highly skewed distribution of coding
decisions (i.e., zero-inflation, indicating that most of the coding decisions in abstract
screening were exclusions, as is typical for systematic reviews) (Lacy et al., 2015). The
The pre-screening of studies resulted in a reduced sample of 409 records that were
then “forward searched” (Card, 2012) via Google Scholar’s “cited by” function by the same
student coders. For each article, coders screened the first 50 citations, thereby retrieving an
additional 381 articles. All articles were entered into a literature database, which was
subsequently searched for the terms “review” and “meta-analysis” to identify eligible research
(2) Second, to accommodate for any limitations of our previous database search
attempts, we then “forward searched” all citations and “backward searched “ all references of
the identified seven review articles and repeated this procedure for any new review articles
found in the process. This procedure is highly common for meta-reviews as synthesis articles
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW - ONLINE APPENDIX 52
typically cite related syntheses in order to clarify their unique contribution in contrast to
already published syntheses articles (Polanin et al., 2017). These searches resulted in nine
specifically at finding systematic reviews and meta-analyses on CMC and MH. In doing so,
we were able to compare the results of our previous broader systematic literature search with
a more targeted search, testing whether our previous search attempts were exhaustive. This
final search used the search string from the systematic database search and several additional
terms omitted from the first string (e.g., “social support” or “social capital”). The resulting
string was then combined with the terms “systematic review”, “narrative review”, “review”,
and “meta-analysis”. This complementary search resulted in only five additional review
articles in December 2017, underlining the exhaustiveness and validity of our previous search
efforts. This last step of the search was then updated during peer review in September 2019,
resulting in an additional 15 reviews published in 2018 and 2019. The final sample of eligible
research publications from the review articles and including 58 MH and 74 CMC indicators
scaled data, we only report α. For most categories, reliability was sufficient: number of MH
dimension (97%, α = .95), MH manifestation (91%, α = .88), MH trait vs. state measurement
(95%, α = .89), CMC device (100%, α = 1.00), type of application (96%, α = .87), branded
application (97%, α = .94), interaction (93%, α = .86), and message level (92%, α = .66), and
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW - ONLINE APPENDIX 53
the conceptual approach to CMC (91%, α = .84). For the operational approach (80%, α = .55)
and the feature level (88%, α = .46), α values were low. These disagreements were discussed
until consensus was reached and the full dataset was recoded accordingly.
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW - ONLINE APPENDIX 54
Table A1
Descriptive overview of included meta-analyses and systematic reviews
Narrative
Review Population Publications Type of CMC concept(s) MH concept(s) conclusion
Author(s) Year
type investigated included studies synthesized synthesized about overall
relationship
Baker & 2016 SR General 30 QN SNS use (various) Depression Mixed;
Algorta Conditional
Best et al. 2014 SR Adolescents 43 QN & QL SM use (various) Various (e.g., self-esteem, (Mixed;
social support, social Conditional)
capital, social isolation,
depression)
Cheng et al. 2019 MA General 161 QN SNS use (various) Social capital, social Mixed:
anxiety, loneliness Conditional
Dobrean & 2016 SR General 20 QN SNS use (various) Social anxiety (Mixed)
Pasarelu
Domahidi 2018 MA General 63 QN Internet, SM & SNS use Social support, social Positive;
(various) capital Conditional
Erfani & 2018 SR General 22 QN & QL SNS use (various) Various (e.g., life Mixed:
Abedin satisfaction, self-esteem, Conditional
affect)
Forsman & 2015 SR Older 32 QN & QL Internet use (various) Various (e.g., quality of Positive
Nordmyr Adults life, depression,
loneliness)
Frost & 2017 SR General 65 QN FB use (various) Various (e.g., anxiety, Mixed;
Rickwood depression, disordered Conditional
eating, alcohol abuse)
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW - ONLINE APPENDIX 55
Holland & 2016 SR General 20 QN SNS use (various) Eating disorder symptoms (Negative;
Tiggemann Conditional)
Huang 2010 MA General 40 QN Internet use (various) Various (depression, Negative
loneliness, self-esteem,
life satisfaction)
Huang 2017 MA General 61 QN SNS use (time spent) Various (depression, Negative;
loneliness, self-esteem, Conditional
life satisfaction)
Keles et al. 2019 SR Adolescents 13 QN SM use (various) Depression, anxiety, Negative;
distress Conditional
Khosravi et 2016 SR Older 34 QN ICT & SNS use (various) Social isolation, Positive
al. Adults loneliness
Krause et 2019 SR General 49 QN SNS use (various) Self-esteem Mixed;
al. Conditional
Liu, 2016 MA General 58 QN SNS use (various) Social capital Positive;
Ainsworth Conditional
et al.
Liu & 2016 MA General 80 QN SNS use (various) Self-esteem, loneliness Negative;
Baumeister Conditional
Liu, 2019 MA General 124 QN ICT & SNS use (various) Various (e.g., anxiety, Mixed;
Baumeister, depression, happiness, Conditional
et al. loneliness, self-esteem)
Liu, 2018 MA Students 31 QN SNS use (various) Social support Positive;
Wright, et Conditional
al.
McCrae et 2017 MA Children & 11 QN SM use (various) Depression (Negative;
al. Adolescents Conditional)
Meng et al. 2017 SR General 88 QN SNS use (various) Social support Unclear
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW - ONLINE APPENDIX 56
Yin et al. 2019 MA General 63 QN SNS use (various) Various (e.g., depression, Mixed;
loneliness, anxiety, envy, Conditional
affect, life satisfaction,
self-esteem)
Yoon et al. 2019 MA General 45 QN SNS use (various) Depression Negative;
Conditional
Note. Review type: SR: systematic narrative review, MA: meta-analysis. Type of studies: QN: quantitative, QL: qualitative. SNS: social network
sites. SM: social media. FB: Facebook. ICT: information and communication technology. PTH: psychopathology. PWB: psychological well-
being. Conclusion: The conclusion refers to the relationship between CMC and MH as operationalized in the respective review, with higher
levels of MH meaning higher levels of PWB and lower levels of PTH. Negative: negative relationships between CMC and MH prevail. Positive:
positive relationships prevail. Mixed: positive, negative, and/or non-significant relationships were found. Unclear: no explicit conclusion about
the relationship was articulated. Conditional: the strength and/or direction of the relationships depend on moderators (e.g., age, gender, culture,
concepts or measures investigated) and/or mediators. Brackets indicate that author(s) found the evidence insufficient for a definitive conclusion.
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW - ONLINE APPENDIX 58
Table A2
Effect sizes of the relationship between CMC and MH indicators from fourteen meta-analyses
Effect size r
k effect N par-
Publication CMC indicator MH indicator 95% CI [LL;
sizes ticipants
UL]
2019b
et al.,
Global SNS use (total) Bridging social 50 22,290 .32 [.27; .37]
capital
-- intensity 32 14,711 .35 [.34; .36]
-- time 13 5,726 .15 [.12; .17]
Liu, Ainsworth et al., 2016a
(various) symptoms
evaluative
-- behavioral 12 — .21 [.14; .28]
-- cognitive 9 — .23 [.17; .29]
-- Multiple SNS Body image 31 — .16 [.12; .20]
disturbance (total)
-- Facebook 23 — .21 [.14; .29]
-- Instagram 5 — .10 [-.18; .36]
-- Other SNS 4 — .10 [-.06; .25]
-- General use 44 — .11 [.08; .15]
-- Appearance-focused 16 — .31 [.22; .39]
use
symptoms
2019
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW - ONLINE APPENDIX 61
symptoms
Frequency of checking 14 8,041 .10 [.03; .16]
SNS
General social 8 1,715 .23 [.12; .34]
comparison on SNS
Upward social 6 2,298 .33 [.20; .47]
comparison on SNS
Note. Effect sizes within as well as between individual meta-analyses should not be treated as
independent. Effect sizes statistically significant at p < .05 or lower are highlighted in bold.
Empty cells in the N participants column are due to missing information (i.e., (sub-)sample
sizes were not reported in the respective publications). If publications reported information on
indicators that did not match our definitions of CMC (e.g., gaming, entertainment) or MH
(e.g., narcissism), this information was omitted. Effect sizes collapsing indicators that
matched and did not match our definitions were also omitted (e.g., an effect size including
both general and problematic usage). As far as possible, we used the CMC and MH indicator
labels as used by the original author(s) to facilitate reproducibility. However, the labeling was
also slightly extended and harmonized across publications to facilitate interpretability of
findings. “Global SNS use” refers to time spent on the SNS, frequency of, and/or intensity of
use.
a
Author(s) conducted a Hunter & Schmidt correction of effect sizes based on internal
consistency (e.g., Cronbach’s alpha) of the measures.
b
Author(s) conducted a three-level (random effects or mixed effects) meta-analysis. All other
findings are based on random effects models.
c
Author(s) report credible intervals instead of confidence intervals.
d
Author(s) used robust standard errors and confidence intervals.
(r)
Measure was reversed by the author(s).
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 62
Anderson, C. A., Shibuya, A., Ihori, N., Swing, E. L., Bushman, B. J., Sakamoto, A., Rothstein,
H. R., & Saleem, M. (2010). Violent video game effects on aggression, empathy, and
Baker, D. A., & Algorta, G. P. (2016). The relationship between online social networking and
Bargh, J. A., & McKenna, K. Y. A. (2004). The internet and social life. Annual Review of
Best, P., Manktelow, R., & Taylor, B. (2014). Online communication, social media and
adolescent wellbeing: A systematic narrative review. Children and Youth Services Review, 41,
27–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2014.03.001
Card, N. A. (2012). Applied meta-analysis for social science research. Guilford Press.
Cheatham, L. P. (2012). Effects of Internet use on well-being among adults with physical
https://doi.org/10.3109/17483107.2011.625071
Chen, L., Ho, S. S., & Lwin, M. O. (2017). A meta-analysis of factors predicting cyberbullying
perpetration and victimization: From the social cognitive and media effects approach. New
Cheng, C., Wang, H.-Y., Sigerson, L., & Chau, C.-L. (2019). Do the socially rich get richer? A
nuanced perspective on social network site use and online social capital accrual.
Chung, J. M., Robins, R. W., Trzesniewski, K. H., Noftle, E. E., Roberts, B. W., & Widaman, K.
Conway, C. C., Forbes, M. K., Forbush, K. T., Fried, E. I., Hallquist, M. N., Kotov, R., Mullins-
Sweatt, S. N., Shackman, A. J., Skodol, A. E., South, S. C., Sunderland, M., Waszczuk, M.
A., Zald, D. H., Afzali, M. H., Bornovalova, M. A., Carragher, N., Docherty, A. R., Jonas, K.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691618810696
Derks, D., Fischer, A. H., & Bos, A. E.R. (2008). The role of emotion in computer-mediated
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2007.04.004
DeYoung, C. G. (2015). Cybernetic big five theory. Journal of Research in Personality, 56, 33–
58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.07.004
Diener, E., Lucas, R. E., & Oishi, S. (2018). Advances and open questions in the science of
Dobrean, A., & Pasarelu, C.-R. (2016). Impact of social media on social anxiety: A systematic
InTechOpen. https://doi.org/10.5772/65188
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 64
Domahidi, E. (2018). The associations between online media use and users’ perceived social
https://doi.org/10.1093/jcmc/zmy007
Elhai, J. D., Dvorak, R. D., Levine, J. C., & Hall, B. J. (2017). Problematic smartphone use: A
conceptual overview and systematic review of relations with anxiety and depression
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2016.08.030
Erfani, S. S., & Abedin, B. (2018). Impacts of the use of social network sites on users’
Ferguson, C. J., Coulson, M., & Barnett, J. (2011). A meta-analysis of pathological gaming
prevalence and comorbidity with mental health, academic and social problems. Journal of
Forsman, A. K., & Nordmyr, J. (2015). Psychosocial links between Internet use and mental
health in later life: A systematic review of quantitative and qualitative evidence. Journal of
Frost, R. L., & Rickwood, D. J. (2017). A systematic review of the mental health outcomes
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.08.001
Gilmour, J., Machin, T., Brownlow, C., & Jeffries, C. (2020). Facebook-based social support and
https://doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000246
Gnambs, T., & Appel, M. (2018). Narcissism and social networking behavior: A meta-analysis.
Grajales, F. J., Sheps, S., Ho, K., Novak-Lauscher, H., & Eysenbach, G. (2014). Social media: A
review and tutorial of applications in medicine and health care. Journal of Medical Internet
https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007219227883
Greitemeyer, T., & Mügge, D. O. (2014). Video games do affect social outcomes: A meta-
analytic review of the effects of violent and prosocial video game play. Personality and Social
Holland, G., & Tiggemann, M. (2016). A systematic review of the impact of the use of social
networking sites on body image and disordered eating outcomes. Body Image, 17, 100–110.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.02.008
Huang, C. (2017). Time spent on social network sites and psychological well-being: A meta-
https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2016.0758
Huta, V. (2017). An overview of hedonic and eudaimonic well-being concepts. In L. Reinecke &
M. B. Oliver (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of media use and well-being: International
perspectives on theory and research on positive media effects (pp. 14–33). Routledge.
Huta, V., & Waterman, A. S. (2014). Eudaimonia and its distinction from hedonia: Developing a
Keles, B., McCrae, N., & Grealish, A. (2019). A systematic review: The influence of social
Keyes, C. L. M. (2005). Mental illness and/or mental health? Investigating axioms of the
complete state model of health. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73(3), 539–
548. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.73.3.539
Khosravi, P., Rezvani, A., & Wiewiora, A. (2016). The impact of technology on older adults’
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.092
Klaps, A., Ponocny, I., Winker, R., Kundi, M., Auersperg, F., & Barth, A. (2016). Mobile phone
base stations and well-being - A meta-analysis. The Science of the Total Environment, 544,
24–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scitotenv.2015.11.009
Kosenko, K., Luurs, G., & Binder, A. R. (2017). Sexting and sexual behavior, 2011-2015: A
Kowalski, R. M., Giumetti, G. W., Schroeder, A. N., & Lattanner, M. R. (2014). Bullying in the
digital age: A critical review and meta-analysis of cyberbullying research among youth.
Krause, H.-V., Baum, K., Baumann, A., & Krasnova, H. (2019). Unifying the detrimental and
beneficial effects of social network site use on self-esteem: a systematic literature review.
Krueger, R. F., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. G. (2001). The higher-order structure of common
8869(00)00106-9
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 67
Kwan, I., Dickson, K., Richardson, M., MacDowall, W., Burchett, H., Stansfield, C., Brunton,
G., Sutcliffe, K., & Thomas, J. (2020). Cyberbullying and children and young people’s mental
Lacy, S., Watson, B. R., Riffe, D., & Lovejoy, J. (2015). Issues and best practices in content
https://doi.org/10.1177/1077699015607338
Lahey, B. B., Krueger, R. F., Rathouz, P. J., Waldman, I. D., & Zald, D. H. (2017). A
hierarchical causal taxonomy of psychopathology across the life span. Psychological Bulletin,
Laranjo, L., Arguel, A., Neves, A. L., Gallagher, A. M., Kaplan, R., Mortimer, N., Mendes, G.
A., & Lau, A. Y. S. (2015). The influence of social networking sites on health behavior
change: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of the American Medical Informatics
Li, J., Theng, Y.-L., & Foo, S. (2016). Effect of exergames on depression: A systematic review
https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2015.0366
Liu, D., Ainsworth, S. E., & Baumeister, R. F. (2016). A meta-analysis of social networking
https://doi.org/10.1037/gpr0000091
Liu, D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2016). Social networking online and personality of self-worth: A
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2016.06.024
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 68
Liu, D., Baumeister, R. F., Yang, C.-C., & Hu, B. (2019). Digital communication media use and
Liu, D., & Campbell, W. K. (2017). The big five personality traits, big two metatraits and social
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2017.08.004
Liu, D., Kirschner, P. A., & Karpinski, A. C. (2017). A meta-analysis of the relationship of
academic performance and social network site use among adolescents and young adults.
Liu, D., Wright, K. B., & Hu, B. (2018). A meta-analysis of social network site use and social
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2018.08.024
Liu, D., & Yang, C.-C. (2016). Media niche of electronic communication channels in friendship:
https://doi.org/10.1111/jcc4.12175
Martela, F., & Sheldon, K. M. (2019). Clarifying the concept of well-being: Psychological need
satisfaction as the common core connecting eudaimonic and subjective well-being. Review of
McCrae, N., Gettings, S., & Purssell, E. (2017). Social media and depressive symptoms in
childhood and adolescence: A systematic review. Adolescent Research Review, 2(4), 315–
330. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40894-017-0053-4
Meng, J., Martinez, L., Holmstrom, A., Chung, M., & Cox, J. (2017). Research on social
networking sites and social support from 2004 to 2015: A narrative review and directions for
https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2016.0325
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 69
Mingoia, J., Hutchinson, A. D., Wilson, C., & Gleaves, D. H. (2017). The relationship between
social networking site use and the internalization of a thin ideal in females: A meta-analytic
Polanin, J. R., Maynard, B. R., & Dell, N. A. (2017). Overviews in education research: A
https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654316631117
Prizant-Passal, S., Shechner, T., & Aderka, I. M. (2016). Social anxiety and internet use – A
meta-analysis: What do we know? What are we missing? Computers in Human Behavior, 62,
221–229. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.04.003
Rodgers, R. F., & Melioli, T. (2016). The relationship between body image concerns, eating
disorders and Internet use, part I: A review of empirical support. Adolescent Research Review,
Ruppel, E. K., Gross, C., Stoll, A., Peck, B. S., Allen, M., & Kim, S.-Y. (2017). Reflecting on
https://doi.org/10.1111/jcc4.12179
Rus, H. M., & Tiemensma, J. (2017). “It’s complicated.” A systematic review of associations
between social network site use and romantic relationships. Computers in Human Behavior,
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: A review of research on
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.141
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 70
Ryan, T., Chester, A., Reece, J., & Xenos, S. (2014). The uses and abuses of Facebook: A review
https://doi.org/10.1556/JBA.3.2014.016
Saiphoo, A. N., & Vahedi, Z. (2019). A meta-analytic review of the relationship between social
media use and body image disturbance. Computers in Human Behavior, 101, 259–275.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.07.028
Sarmiento, I. G., Olson, C., Yeo, G., Chen, Y. A., Toma, C. L., Brown, B. B., Bellmore, A., &
Mares, M.-L. (2018). How does social media use relate to adolescents’ internalizing
Seabrook, E. M., Kern, M. L., & Rickard, N. S. (2016). Social networking sites, depression, and
https://doi.org/10.2196/mental.5842
Shapiro, L. A. S., & Margolin, G. (2014). Growing up wired: Social networking sites and
adolescent psychosocial development. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 17(1),
1–18. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-013-0135-1
Song, H., Zmyslinski-Seelig, A., Kim, J., Drent, A., Victor, A., Omori, K., & Allen, M. (2014).
Does Facebook make you lonely? A meta analysis. Computers in Human Behavior, 36, 446–
452. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.04.011
Thomée, S. (2018). Mobile phone use and mental health. A review of the research that takes a
Tokunaga, R. S. (2010). Following you home from school: A critical review and synthesis of
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2009.11.014
CMC AND MENTAL HEALTH: A META-REVIEW 71
relationships between problematic Internet use, time spent using the Internet, and
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.2010.01386.x
Vahedi, Z., & Zannella, L. (2019). The association between self-reported depressive symptoms
and the use of social networking sites (SNS): A meta-analysis. Current Psychology, 30(2),
252. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-0150-6
Valkenburg, P. M., Peter, J., & Walther, J. B. (2016). Media effects: Theory and research.
033608
Verduyn, P., Ybarra, O., Résibois, M., Jonides, J., & Kross, E. (2017). Do social network sites
enhance or undermine subjective well-being? A critical review. Social Issues and Policy
Wängqvist, M., & Frisén, A. (2016). Who am I online? Understanding the meaning of online
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40894-016-0025-0
Williams, J. R. (2019). The use of online social networking sites to nurture and cultivate bonding
social capital: A systematic review of the literature from 1997 to 2018. New Media & Society,
Wongkoblap, A., Vadillo, M. A., & Curcin, V. (2017). Researching mental health disorders in
the era of social media: Systematic review. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 19(6), 1-17.
https://doi.org/10.2196/jmir.7215
Wu, Y.-J., Outley, C., Matarrita-Cascante, D., & Murphrey, T. P. (2016). A systematic review of
recent research on adolescent social connectedness and mental health with Internet technology
Yang, F., & Shen, F. (2018). Effects of web interactivity: A meta-analysis. Communication
Yin, X.-Q., de Vries, D. A., Gentile, D. A., & Wang, J.-L. (2019). Cultural background and
measurement of usage moderate the association between social networking sites (SNSs) usage
and mental health: A meta-analysis. Social Science Computer Review, 37(5), 631–648.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0894439318784908
Yoon, S., Kleinman, M., Mertz, J., & Brannick, M. (2019). Is social network site usage related to