Hooper 2011
Hooper 2011
To cite this article: Lisa M. Hooper , Kirsten Doehler , Scyatta A. Wallace & Natalie J. Hannah
(2011) The Parentification Inventory: Development, Validation, and Cross-Validation, The American
Journal of Family Therapy, 39:3, 226-241
LISA M. HOOPER
Department of Educational Studies in Psychology, Research Methodology, and Counseling,
The University of Alabama, Tuscaloosa, Alabama, USA
KIRSTEN DOEHLER
Department of Mathematics and Statistics, Elon University, Elon, North Carolina, USA
SCYATTA A. WALLACE
Department of Psychology, St. John’s University, Jamaica, New York, USA
NATALIE J. HANNAH
Department of Educational Studies in Psychology, Research Methodology, and Counseling,
The University of Alabama, Tuscaloosa, Alabama, USA
226
Parentification Inventory 227
INTRODUCTION
At least 4 of every 100 children living in the United States, the United King-
dom, or Australia will fulfill a caregiving role in their families (Becker, 2007).
These rates are likely much higher in developing countries such as Africa,
where the AIDS virus is widely prevalent (Becker, 2007). A survey conducted
in the United States in 2005 estimated that there are around 1.3 to 1.4 million
caregiving children just between ages 8 and 18 (Barber & Siskowski, 2008).
Young caregiving is an important clinical and research issue because it can
affect the health and development of young caregivers from cradle to grave
(i.e., during their childhood and adolescence as well as adult years) (Barber
& Siskowski, 2008). Although most children give care to another family mem-
ber in some form during their childhood years (Becker, 2007; Boszormenyi-
Nagy & Spark, 1973; Minuchin, 1974), some children take on adult roles and
responsibilities not normally entrusted to children or to a degree that is con-
sidered developmentally inappropriate and excessive (Boszormenyi-Nagy &
Spark, 1973; Locke & Newcomb, 2004; Winton, 2002). This excessive level
of caregiving in childhood is known as parentification, and it is a common
clinical condition experienced in a range of demographic populations that
can be measured as it is occurring and retrospectively.
Although instruments have been used to measure parentification, these
instruments—we believe—were not developed with three current and rel-
evant issues in mind. More specifically, we contend that absent from the
literature base are parentification measures that consider the implications
of culture and the perceived benefits of parentification and how they may
interact with the roles and processes that undergird the construct of paren-
tification. Additionally, we believe that current measures fail to capture both
to whom the parentified processes are directed (e.g., sibling or parent) and
the multi-factorial nature of the parentification processes. Thus, our primary
rationale for the development of a new measure buttressed by systems the-
ory was to create an instrument that captured the multidimensional roles
and processes (e.g., inverted processes of emotionally focused behaviors
and instrumentally focused behaviors) of parentification that considered the
familial, cultural, and ecological context in which these parentified roles and
behaviors take place.
The primary aim of the current project, the Childhood Roles and Re-
sponsibilities and Adult Functioning study (Project Chores), was to develop
and validate an instrument that assessed for retrospective parentification in
adults. This process was conducted using two non-overlapping samples.
Specifically, the full convenience sample was split into two random sam-
ples (i.e., separate non-overlapping samples) for exploratory factor analysis
228 L. M. Hooper et al.
(Study 1) and confirmatory factor analysis (Study 2). The remainder of this
article discusses the types and prevalence of parentification, the develop-
ment of the PI, and the design and outcome of the two studies that assessed
reliability and validity.
TYPES OF PARENTIFICATION
PREVALENCE
ages 5 to 15 were providing care in some form to at least one family mem-
ber (Doran, Drever, & Whitehead, 2003). The figures revealed that 18,000
of those young caregivers cared for family members 20 or more hours per
week and that almost 9,000 provided care more than 50 hours per week.
Mayseless, Bartholomew, Henderson, and Trinke (2004) found that 13%
of 128 adult Canadian participants (contacted via random-digit dialing) in
their study on parentification reported facing large amounts of role rever-
sal during their childhood years. They found that those who reported role
reversal in their study were more likely to be women than men and were
more likely to have reversed roles with their mothers than with their fathers
(Mayseless et al., 2004).
In summary, parentification is a ubiquitous construct and a common
clinical condition that is often experienced in a range of demographic popu-
lations (Byng-Hall, 2008; Hooper, 2007). Establishing instruments that allow
for the empirical study and clinical measurement of parentification is an im-
portant area of study. Currently, there are only a few instruments that meet
this need.
and behaviors take place. Like those who developed other assessments of
parentification, we chose an initial development and validation sample of
college-aged participants.
Following preliminary item construction, we conferred with several ex-
pert family systems clinicians, researchers, and scholars to garner consensus
on the content and face validity (i.e., the extent to which the instrument
appears to be assessing the desired qualities based on a review of the in-
strument by one or more experts) of the PI. The pool of items was reviewed
for clarity, appropriateness, and representativeness of the parentification
construct. Finally, the PI was piloted with graduate-level students for clar-
ity and comprehensiveness. The resultant newly developed measurement of
parentification consisted of 32 items and asked participants to respond to
questions about various roles and responsibilities of parentification and to
whom (e.g., parent, sibling, etc.) these responsibilities were directed/offered.
Additionally, in an attempt to capture the perceived benefits attached to the
multidimensional nature of parentification and thus the psychological se-
quelae associated with parentification, the PI allowed for perceived benefit
finding that may be associated with childhood parentification. The initial
self-report PI took approximately 10 minutes to complete.
For Study 1, we had two aims: (1) to conduct an exploratory factor
analysis to determine the factor structure of the PI and (2) to evaluate the
internal consistency of the PI scores with Cronbach’s coefficient alpha.
Method
PARTICIPANTS
The convenience sample consisted of 431 student volunteers recruited from
12 undergraduate classes in a southeastern university. Participants’ ages
ranged from 19 to 48 (mean = 20.86, SD = 3.55). The sample primarily
consisted of White Americans (82.13%, n = 354) and females (76.10%, n =
328). Self-reported races of the remaining participants included American
Indian (0.46%, n = 2), Asian American (0.70%, n = 3), African American
(12.30%, n = 53), Hispanic/Latino (0.70%, n = 3), mixed race (3.25%, n =
14), and failed to report (0.46%, n = 2).
MEASURES
Sociodemographic Information Sheet. This questionnaire, created for
the study, asks survey participants to respond to several background ques-
tions. Questions covered the participant’s year in school, academic discipline,
age, gender, racial and ethnic background, and religious affiliation.
Parentification Inventory. The initial PI used in Study 1 was a ret-
rospective, 32-item, self-report measure that captured caregiving roles and
responsibilities and the perceived benefits of performing caregiving roles in
Parentification Inventory 231
PROCEDURE
Following Institutional Review Board approval, we recruited undergraduate
student participants to take part in a study investigating the link between
childhood roles and responsibilities and adult psychological functioning.
With the permission of university professors, we approached participants in
undergraduate-level classrooms and then later by email. We administered the
survey packet online using a web-based survey methodology. Specifically,
we sent participants an electronic invitation to participate in the study, which
included a description of the study, a direct link to the electronic survey, and
an informed consent form.
Results
EXPLORATORY FACTOR ANALYSIS
Preliminary estimates of communalities were set to 1. We established an a
priori criterion for the inclusion of items: only those with a loading of .4 or
higher would be considered in the development of the PI. The participants’
responses from the Project Chores study produced six eigenvalues that were
greater than 1, although a visual interpretation of the Cattel’s (1966) scree
plot showed that a large portion of the variance—a break at the elbow—was
232 L. M. Hooper et al.
RELIABILITY ESTIMATES
We used Cronbach’s alpha to examine the internal consistency estimates of
the empirically constructed factors/subscales (Aim 2). Consistent with the
survey development literature base (Worthington & Whittaker, 2006), we
established an a priori criterion that an alpha equal to or greater than .70
was satisfactory. Results suggested adequate reliability for two of the three
PI subscale scores (.88 for PFP, .83 for SFP, and .37 for PBP) and the PI total
scale score (.87) for Study 1. Table 1 shows the mean, standard deviation,
and reliability of the study factors in the context of the current sample.
TABLE 1 Means, Standard Deviations, and Cronbach’s Alpha Reliability Coefficients for the
Parentification Inventory Factor Scores
No. of Alpha
Factors M SD Items Coefficient
Method
PARTICIPANTS AND PROCEDURE
The convenience sample for Study 2 consisted of 416 student volunteers
recruited from 12 undergraduate classes in a southeastern university. Partici-
pants’ ages ranged from 19 to 48 (mean = 20.89, SD = 3.83). The sample pri-
marily consisted of White Americans (79.57%, n = 331) and females (75.00%,
n = 312). Self-reported races of the remaining participants included Ameri-
can Indian (0.48%, n = 2), Asian American (0.48%, n = 2), African American
(15.14%, n = 63), Hispanic/Latino (1.20%, n = 5), and mixed race (2.64%,
n = 11), and failed to report (0.48%, n = 2). We used the identical procedures
employed in Study 1.
MEASURES
Sociodemographic Information Sheet. This questionnaire, created for
the study, asks survey participants to respond to several background ques-
tions. Questions covered the participant’s year in school, academic discipline,
age, gender, racial and ethnic background, and marital status.
Parentification Inventory. The modified PI in Study 2 was a retro-
spective, 29-item, self-report measure that captured caregiving roles and
responsibilities and the perceived benefits of performing caregiving roles in
the family of origin. Participants responded to 29 items on the PI using a
five-point Likert-type scale, ranging from 1 (never true) to 5 (always true).
Depression Symptomatology. The Beck Depression Inventory (BDI;
Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996) consists of 21 self-report items that capture
depressive symptomatology. Scores for each item range from 0 to 3. The
maximum possible total score is 63, and higher scores reflect greater severity
of depression symptomatology and a greater likelihood of major depres-
sion. The BDI is one of the most widely used instruments that measures
depression, and scores from this instrument have been shown to have good
reliability and validity (Beck et al., 1996). Consistent with other studies, the
234 L. M. Hooper et al.
obtained reliability in the current study was more than adequate; Cronbach’s
alpha was .92 for the current study sample.
General Psychological Symptoms. The Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI)
is a 53-item, self-report inventory designed to reflect the psychological symp-
tom patterns of psychiatric and general community populations. The BSI
reports nine symptom scores and three broad scores measuring distress
(Derogatis, 1993). The three broad scores (or “global indices”) include global
severity index, positive symptom distress index, and positive symptom total.
The psychometric properties of the BSI and subscale scores are excellent
(Derogatis & Spencer, 1982). Cronbach’s alpha coefficient has yielded scores
on the nine symptom categories in the range of .71 to .85. Test-retest relia-
bility yielded .69 to .91 for the nine subscales and .80 to .90 for the global
indices (Derogatis & Spencer, 1982). Cronbach’s alphas for the global indices
were in the range of .87 to .97 for the current study sample.
Parentification Questionnaire. Parentification was also assessed using
the Parentification Questionnaire (PQ; Jurkovic & Thirkield, 1998), which
is a self-report instrument that measures both instrumental and emotional
parentification. Participants rate how true the statements are on a five-point
Likert scale, where 1 is “strongly disagree” and 5 is “strongly agree.” Subscale
scores can fall in the range of 10 to 50, with higher scores indicative of greater
parentification. The PQ has a reported Spearman-Brown split half reliability
of .85 (Burt, 1992). Research has documented convergent validity for the
PQ, indicating that scores on the instrument are related to variables such
as features of depression, anxiety, and somatic symptomatology (Hooper &
Wallace, 2010). Cronbach’s alpha for the PQ was .88 for the current study
sample.
Results
CONFIRMATORY FACTOR ANALYSIS
We examined missing data, confirmatory factor analysis, and reliability as
measured by Cronbach’s alpha. All analysis models included subjects with
nonmissing values for the appropriate scales. Only observed values were
used; no imputation was performed. An initial confirmatory factor analysis
was conducted on the 29 items of the PI inventory using the CALIS pro-
cedure in SAS. The maximum likelihood estimation method was used. We
used several fit indices to evaluate the confirmatory factor analysis models
produced by the data in Study 2.
The goodness-of-fit index and nonnormed fit index were used to evalu-
ate the fit of these models. Stevens (2002) suggested that models with these
indices above .90 were considered a good fit, with values closer to 1 indi-
cating a better fit. In addition, the root mean square error of approximation
(RMSEA) and its 90% confidence interval were used to evaluate the model
Parentification Inventory 235
fit. Values below .08 are considered to indicate moderately good model fit,
while values above .10 indicate poor fit (Browne & Cudeck, 1993).
The results of the confirmatory factor analysis of the 29-item, three-
factor model shows an unacceptable model fit, as reflected in the following
fit statistics. The RMSEA value of .085 was above the upper limit of moderate
model fit for this index. The goodness-of-fit index and nonnormed fit index
results of .76 and .76, respectively, were below the recommend cut-off of .90
for these indices. The resultant X2/df ratio was 2.97. The CAIC was −1343.05,
and the AIC was −356.49, which is reported for the purposes of comparisons
with our attempts to iteratively improve model fit.
Items with large residuals, low communality, and low factor loadings
were then removed iteratively to improve the model fit, resulting in a 22-
item, three-factor model. By iteratively removing items with the poorest fit, as
recommended by Hagger and Orbell (2005), we achieved a slightly better fit,
using only 22 of the 29 items. The goodness-of-fit index (.81), RMSEA (.07),
X2/df ratio (3.37), and nonnormed fit index (.77) reflect this improvement.
The decreased values of the CAIC (−671.22) and AIC (−282.80) also indicate
that this model has a slightly improved fit. Therefore, we concluded that
the three-factor, 22-item model produced the optimal fit in this study. We
also tested other models, but the fit indices revealed findings in the poor
range.
Based on the final confirmatory factor analysis model, 12 items were
assigned to Factor 1 (Parent-Focused Parentification), 7 items were assigned
to Factor 2 (Sibling-Focused Parentification), and 3 items were assigned to
Factor 3 (Perceived Benefits of Parentification) (see Table 2). Therefore, a
total of 22 items were retained for the final, three-factor, theory-based model.
None of the items in the final model loaded on more than one factor. The
factor loadings from the confirmatory factor analysis, presented in Table 2,
were similar to the loadings from the exploratory factor analysis. Therefore,
we retained the labels put forward in the exploratory factor analysis.
TABLE 2 Final Rotated Component Factor Analysis, Factor Loadings, and Communalities for
the 22-Item Parentification Inventory
Factor Loadings
PI Item # Text I II III h2
TABLE 3 Zero-Order Correlations, Means, and Standard Deviations for Study Variables
Study Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1. PI: PFP 1
2. PI: SFP .377∗∗∗ 1
3. PI: PBP −.243∗∗∗ −.168∗∗∗ 1
4. AUDIT .147∗∗∗ .097∗ −.146∗∗∗ 1
∗∗∗
5. BDI .156 .088∗∗ −.364∗∗∗ .235∗∗∗ 1
6. GSI .212∗∗∗ .096∗∗ −.303∗∗∗ .277∗∗∗ .746∗∗∗ 1
∗∗∗
7. PQ: IP .437 .534∗∗∗ −.384∗∗∗ .108∗∗ .210∗∗∗ .242∗∗∗ 1
8. PQ: EP .622∗∗∗ .319∗∗∗ −.479∗∗∗ .099∗∗ .263∗∗∗ .260∗∗∗ .610∗∗∗ 1
Mean 2.02 2.53 4.06 6.83 8.51 2.09 17.87 22.56
SD .52 .38 .87 6.25 8.50 2.55 6.66 7.20
Note. PI = Parentification Inventory; PFP = Parent-Focused Parentification; SFP = Sibling-Focused Par-
entification; PBP = Perceived Benefits of Parentification; AUDIT = Alcohol Use Disorders Identification
Test; BDI = Beck Depression Inventory; GSI = Global Severity Index (of the Brief Symptom Inventory);
PQ = Parentification Questionnaire; IP = Instrumental Parentification; EP = Emotional Parentification.
∗ p < .05; ∗∗ p < .01; ∗∗∗ p < .001
GSI scores (r = .212; p < .001). Similarly, participants with higher SFP scores
also report higher BDI scores (r = .088; p < .01), AUDIT scores (r = .097;
p < .05), and GSI scores (r = .096; p < .01). Finally, as expected, there was
an inverse relationship with regard to the PBP scores and the hypothesized
correlates; participants who reported higher levels of perceived benefits from
engaging in roles and responsibilities of parentification had lower BDI scores
(r = −.364; p < .001), AUDIT scores (r = −.146; p < .001), and GSI scores
(r = −.303; p < .001).
DISCUSSION
REFERENCES
Aldridge, J. (2006). The experiences of children living with and caring for parents
with mental illness. Child Abuse Review, 15, 79–88. doi:10.1002/car.904
Barber, M., & Siskowski, C. (2008). Youth caregivers: Unrecognized providers of
care. Pediatrics, 121, 873–874. doi:10.1542/peds/2008-0093
Bartlett, M. S. (1954). A further note on the multiplying factors for various X 2 approx-
imations in factor analysis. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, 16, 296–298.
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Beck Depression Inventory manual
(2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Becker, S. (2007). Global perspectives on children’s unpaid caregiving in the family:
Research and policy on “young carers” in the UK, Australia, the USA and Sub-
Saharan Africa. Global Social Policy, 7, 23–50. doi:10.1177/1468018107073892
Boszormenyi-Nagy, I., & Spark, G. (1973). Invisible loyalties: Reciprocity in intergen-
erational family therapy. Hagerstown, MD: Harper & Row.
Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A.
Bollen & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136–162).
Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
240 L. M. Hooper et al.