4 - Published Research
4 - Published Research
Author manuscript
Psychol Men Masc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 January 19.
Author Manuscript
Abstract
Social media/social networks (SM/SNs), while ubiquitous in their use, have not been well
integrated into psychological theory or practice. Most research addressing SM/SNs has examined
frequency and modality of SM/SN use, rather than the valence of online interactions or potential
mental health consequences of use. Further, SM/SN use has also not been well integrated with
relevant paradigms from the psychology of men and masculinities paradigms. The present study
contributes to both of these research need areas by testing the associations among SM/SN use,
toxic masculinity, positive or negative SM/SN interactions, and depression among a sample of
402 men. Results of a structural equation modeling analysis indicated that SM/SN use and
toxic masculinity were associated with depression. Positive and negative SM/SN interactions
mediated the relationship between SM/SN use and depression indicators, and negative SM/SN
interactions mediated the relationship between toxic masculinity and depression. Implications for
Author Manuscript
future research directions and for working with men who use SM/SNs are discussed.
Keywords
toxic masculinity; social media; depression
(Donchev, 2016). Reddit, a forum site with subforums devoted to thousands of topics,
has an estimated 16 million users in the United States (Barthel, Stocking, Holcomb, &
Mitchell, 2010). Instagram, used to post photos and videos, is used by 32% of online adults
in the United States (Greenwood et al., 2016). The ubiquity of SM/SNs to modern life
and their integration with an enormous range of human behaviors speaks to the need to
better integrate SM/SN use with theories of human behavior. Specific domains of SM/SN
use may be especially relevant to men. Online interactions may be a fertile ground for
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Mike C. Parent, Department of Educational Psychology, University of
Texas at Austin, 1912 Speedway, Stop D5000, Austin, TX 78712. [email protected].
Parent et al. Page 2
the enactment of toxic masculinity due to the anonymity, asynchronous interaction, and
Author Manuscript
impersonal interactions that characterize SM/SN use. The present study sought to explore
relations among SM/SN use, toxic masculinity, and depression.
nonheavy users (Morrison & Gore, 2010). At the same time, other studies have found
no relationship between Internet use and depression symptoms (Sanders, Field, Diego, &
Kaplan, 2000).
Regarding use of SM/SNs in particular, several studies have reported a positive association
between SM/SN use and depression. One investigation of SM/SN use and depression among
a sample of 1,787 U.S. adults compared those within the highest and lowest quartiles of
SM/SN use (defined as self-reported minutes on SM/SN sites per day). Participants in
the highest quartile had increased odds of having depression (adjusted odds ratio = 1.66)
compared with those in the lowest quartile of SM/SN use (Lin et al., 2016). However, as
with general Internet use, findings of a link between SM/SN use and depression are not
consistently supported. A study on SM/SN use and depression among 200 Filipino young
Author Manuscript
adults found no link between SM/SN use and depression (r = .04; Datu, Valdez, & Datu,
2012).
The mixed findings on the association between Internet use, or SM/SN use, and depression
have prompted the undertaking of more nuanced examinations of depression and SM/SN
use. One study of U.S. college students examined depression and SM/SN use by assessing
whether individuals who experience a feeling of subordination in response to viewing
SM/SN posts from others (called “Facebook envy”) had higher rates of depression.
Facebook envy was hypothesized to arise from viewing others’ posts and photos and
comparing idealized portrayals of friends’ lives to one’s own, resulting in a sense of
inferiority. This study found no direct relationship between SM/SN use frequency and
depression (r = .01). However, Facebook envy did mediate the relationship between SM/SN
use frequency and depression. In addition, both the relationships from Facebook use to
Author Manuscript
Facebook envy, and from Facebook envy to depression, were positive (Tandoc, Ferrucci, &
Duffy, 2015).
Another study assessed the sense of loss of control over SM/SN use, dubbed “Facebook
intrusion,” among a sample of 672 Polish Facebook users. Facebook intrusion was
operationalized as problematic engagement in Facebook that disrupted daily activities and
relationships. There was a positive association between self-reported time spent using the
Psychol Men Masc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 January 19.
Parent et al. Page 3
Internet and Facebook intrusion (r = .24), and Facebook intrusion was positively associated
Author Manuscript
with symptoms of depression (r = .24). Men were more likely than women to experience
Facebook intrusion (effect sizes not reported). However, there was no direct relationship
between time on the Internet in general and depression (r = .08), suggesting that the affective
valance of SM/SN behaviors influences the relationship between frequency of daily Internet
use and depression (Błachnio, Przepiórka, & Pantic, 2015).
Based on these findings, we hypothesized that the manner in which one engages with
SM/SN would mediate the relationship between SM/SN use frequency and depression. Two
paradigms that may be relevant to understanding the relationship between SM/SN use and
depression are toxic masculinity and affect-biased attention.
Toxic Masculinity
Author Manuscript
Online interactions are a potential fertile ground for the proliferation of toxic masculinity.
Many online environments are anonymous, with user names taking the place of actual names
and users themselves being generally unidentifiable (Christopherson, 2007; Santana, 2014).
Even when online interactions are not anonymous, such as on Facebook, asynchronous
and non-face-to-face interactions may be more disinhibited, more volatile, and more prone
Psychol Men Masc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 January 19.
Parent et al. Page 4
to toxicity than live, in-person communication (Lapidot-Lefler & Barak, 2012). Such
Author Manuscript
Online, groups with which one disagrees are readily available. Some persons may easily
seek out such material to engage with, or “troll,” an individual for expressing opinions
contrary to their own (Buckels, Trapnell, & Paulhus, 2014; Herring, Job-Sluder, Scheckler,
& Barab, 2002). Even if one does not seek out material with which one disagrees, such
material could easily be encountered online. Adherence to toxic masculinity may promote
engagement with, and dwelling upon, such negative interactions, as one component of
toxic masculinity is a need to dominate interactions. Such negative interactions constitute
Author Manuscript
a form of affect-based attention, and may promote the occurrence of depressive symptoms
(Robinson & Alloy, 2003).
Psychol Men Masc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 January 19.
Parent et al. Page 5
Pursuant to the present study, affect-biased cognitions may mediate the relationship between
Author Manuscript
SM/SN use and depression. In particular, individuals may encounter both positive and
negative material in various online contexts. Greater attending to negative material (e.g.,
seeking out material that one will find aggravating or ruminating about a negative interaction
online) may be associated with greater levels of distress. At the same time, bias toward
positive interactions (e.g., actively seeking out affirming material or reminding oneself of a
positive interaction online) may help to buffer against negative emotions. Thus, time spent
online may not be an adequate means by which to assess quality of SM/SN interactions.
Rather, engagement in self-enhancing SM/SN use (e.g., seeking out supportive interactions)
may be associated with lower levels of psychological distress. In contrast, engagement in
antagonistic or confrontational SM/SN use (e.g., seeking out confrontation and discord) may
be associated with higher levels of psychological distress.
negative material online. Although no research has explored the relationship between
masculinities and negative interactions online, there is evidence that adherence to masculine
gender roles is associated with more hostile or antagonistic interpersonal attitudes toward
others, particularly women (Gallagher & Parrott, 2011; Murnen, Wright, & Kaluzny, 2002).
Subsequently, it is probable that specific aspects of masculine gender role conformity are
associated with greater proclivity toward interpersonal hostility. One such aspect may be
toxic masculinity.
Leimkühler, 2013; Rice, Fallon, & Bambling, 2011). However, many investigations of
the link between masculinity and depression have focused on examining masculinity
broadly, rather than examining theoretically relevant, specific aspects of masculinity. This
is inconsistent with more nuanced assessments of masculinity as composed of multiple
components. Toxic masculinity is one such form of expression of masculinity, and focuses
on a drive to win, homophobia, and misogyny. Adherence to this constellation of masculine
norms may increase the frequency or intensity of maladaptive or hostile behaviors, and
ultimately men’s experiences of depression. For example, in previous studies, scores on the
CMNI-46 Winning subscale, which assesses a need to succeed and dominate, have been
associated with a variety of negative outcomes, including depression (Wong, Ho, Wang,
& Miller, 2017). Scores on the CMNI-46 Heterosexual Self-Presentations subscale, which
assesses a desire to present oneself as heterosexual, have been associated with homophobia,
Author Manuscript
restricted affect, and restriction of expression of friendship between men (Parent & Moradi,
2011; Rankin, 2013). Scores on the CMNI-46 Power Over Women subscale, which assesses
a desire to maintain patriarchal power structures within one’s own life, have been associated
with perpetrating unwanted sexual advances (Kupers, 2005; Mikorski & Szymanski, 2017).
The relationship between masculinity and depression may be particularly relevant when
applied to masculine variations of depression symptoms.
Psychol Men Masc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 January 19.
Parent et al. Page 6
masculinity may seek out negative interactions online, driven by affect-biased attention.
Such exposure to negative online interactions may, in turn, encourage the development of
both traditional and masculine forms of depression.
levels of toxic masculinity would be associated with greater levels of negative interactions
online, which in turn would be associated with higher levels of depression. Figure 1 presents
the hypothesized and tested direct relationships. Specifically, we anticipated the following
hypotheses:
Hypothesis 5: Positive and negative online behaviors would mediate the relationships
between time spent on SM/SNs, and typical and masculine depression.
Psychol Men Masc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 January 19.
Parent et al. Page 7
Method
Author Manuscript
Participants
Participants were recruited via Mturk, a resource for crowd-sourced task completion. Mturk
contains numerous opportunities for paid task completion and is a commonly used source
of data for research (Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling, 2011). Mturk workers complete
tasks online, including marketing surveys, research, or text or image evaluation, and are
compensated for completed tasks. Although some concerns have been raised about issues
such as participant attentiveness during task completion, these concerns can be minimized
through the use of attention check items, restriction of access to the task to individuals in
the United States, and setting a high threshold for Mturk worker’s previous task completions
(Goodman, Cryder, & Cheema, 2013; Ipeirotis, Provost, & Wang, 2010; Rouse, 2015), all of
which were undertaken in this study.
Author Manuscript
Participants were a sample of 402 men who ranged in age from 18 to 74 (M = 33.36, SD
= 10.98). Participants identified as White (71%), Asian American (10%), Hispanic/Latino
(8%), Black or African American (6%), multiracial (4%), Native American (1%), or another
identity (<1%). Most participants identified as heterosexual (89%), gay (5%), bisexual
(4%), or another identity (<1%). On a 101-point Subjective Socioeconomic Status scale,
participants rated themselves on average 44.90 (SD = 19.44; range = 0–93).
Measures
Toxic masculinity.—Toxic masculinity was assessed using three subscales of the
CMNI-46 (Parent & Moradi, 2011) that reflect the core aspects of toxic masculinity: sexism,
heterosexism, and competitiveness. These subscales were Winning (six items; sample item:
“In general, I will do anything to win”), Heterosexual Self-Presentation (six items; sample
Author Manuscript
item: “I would be furious if someone thought I was gay”), and Power Over Women (four
items; sample item: “In general, I control the women in my life”). Responses were made on
a 4-point scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). Validity of the three
subscales has been demonstrated through correlations with relevant constructs. For example,
in previous research using samples of U.S. college men, the Winning subscale demonstrated
positive correlations with measures of masculinity as agency (Bogaert & McCreary, 2011;
Parent & Moradi, 2011); the Heterosexual Self-Presentation subscale demonstrated positive
correlations with measures of homophobia (Keiller, 2010; Parent et al., 2011); and the
Power Over Women subscale demonstrated positive correlation with measures of sexism
(Levant, Rankin, Williams, Hasan, & Smalley, 2010; Parent et al., 2011). In the present
study, Cronbach’s αs for responses to items on each of the subscales were .78 for Winning,
.90 for Heterosexual Self-Presentation, and .81 for Power Over Women.
Author Manuscript
Psychol Men Masc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 January 19.
Parent et al. Page 8
online?”; “Think about [positive/negative] things people have said to you online, after you
Author Manuscript
go offline?”; and “Watch videos or listen to audio shows about topics you [like and agree
with/don’t like and disagree with]”?). Responses were made on a 5-point scale, ranging from
1 (never) to 5 (very often). We verified the intended factor structure using a principal axis
factor analysis, using direct oblimin rotation. The scree plot suggested two factors, and all
items loaded primarily onto their intended factor (i.e., all positive-valence items onto one
factor, and all negative-valence items onto the other). Cronbach’s αs for responses to the
positive and negative item sets were .78 and .77, respectively.
participants seeking mental health services, the PHQ-9 demonstrated validity via positive
correlations with established depression screening tools and clinical interviews (Gilbody,
Richards, Brealey, & Hewitt, 2007; Kroenke et al., 2001; Martin, Rief, Klaiberg, & Braehler,
2006). In the current study, the Cronbach’s α for responses to items on the PHQ-9 was .91.
masculine gender role conformity (Nadeau et al., 2016). In this study, the Cronbach’s α was
.87 for the MDS-Externalizing scale, and .97 for the MDS-Internalizing scale.
Procedure
This study was approved by the institutional review board at the first author’s institution.
Participants were recruited via Mturk. Mturk participants were eligible to participate if they
identified as male in their Mturk profile, were located in the United States, and had a
previous Mturk task approval rating of at least 95%. All measures were completed online.
The survey contained two validity check items (e.g., “Please check strongly disagree”);
participants who failed the validity check items were removed from the data set before
analyses and are not included in any reporting in the present study. Participants were
compensated with $1.50 to their Mturk accounts.
Author Manuscript
Results
Data Analysis
Data were inspected for univariate and multivariate normality. All variables used in the
structural equation modeling analysis met guidelines for univariate normality (Tabachnick
& Fidell, 2007); highest skew = 1.48, highest kurtosis = 1.84. To assess multivariate
Psychol Men Masc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 January 19.
Parent et al. Page 9
test was significant, and five participants were identified as multivariate outliers with
Mahalanobis distances significant at p < .001. Removing these five participants had no
effect on the results of the analysis. Subsequently, their responses were retained in the data
set, and maximum likelihood estimation with robust standard errors was used to estimate
the model. Missing data were minimal; a total of seven data points of 10,050 were missing,
representing a missing data rate of 0.07%. We used full information maximum likelihood
estimation in Mplus to handle missing data in the structural equation modeling and available
item analysis (Parent, 2013) to handle missing data in the calculation of descriptive statistics
and Cronbach’s α coefficients.
Primary Analyses
Table 1 displays the correlations among the variables, means, and standard deviations.
Author Manuscript
Most correlations fell within the moderate range (i.e., near .30). Higher correlations were
present for the associations between the three depression indices, consistent with those three
measures assessing different aspects of depression. Before conducting the primary analysis
in Mplus (Muthén & Muthén, 2012), we constructed item parcels using methods consistent
with recommended practices (Little, Cunningham, Shahar, & Widaman, 2002). Specifically,
for hegemonic masculinity, we used mean scores on the three CMNI-46 subscales as
indicators of the latent variable. For positive and negative interactions, we used all four
individual items for each of the two latent variables as indicators of the latent variables. For
the PHQ-9 and the two subscales of the MDS, we entered all items into individual principal
axis factoring factor analyses, constraining the solution to one factor. We examined factor
loading values, and assigned items to parcels in countervailing order.
Primary analyses were run using Mplus. We assessed fit using the confirmatory fit index
Author Manuscript
(CFI), root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and standardized root-mean-
square residual (SRMR). Adequate fit would be indicated by CFI > .90, RMSEA < .10, and
SRMR < .10, and good fit by CFI > .95, RMSEA < .06, and SRMR < .08 (Weston & Gore,
2006). First, we assessed the measurement model, which fit the data very well; χ2(170) =
354.73, p < .001; CFI = .96; RMSEA = 0.05, 0.04, 0.06; SRMR = 0.05. All latent variable
indicators loaded onto their intended latent variables at p < .001. Most of the covariances
in the measurement model were significant at p < .001. Exceptions were the covariances
between: positive interactions and depression as assessed with the PHQ-9 (p = .73), toxic
masculinity and depression as assessed by the PHQ-9 (p = .12), and positive interactions and
externalizing masculine depression (p = .62).
Next, we tested the structural model. This model also demonstrated good fit; χ2(173) =
Author Manuscript
380.82, p < .001; CFI = .96; RMSEA = 0.06, 0.05, 0.06; SRMR = 0.05. Direct path
coefficients are displayed in Figure 1. Time spent on SM/SNs was associated positively
with positive and negative online behaviors, supporting Hypothesis 1. Toxic masculinity
was associated positively with negative online behaviors, consistent with Hypothesis 2.
Positive online behaviors were associated negatively with typical and masculine depression,
consistent with Hypothesis 3, and negative online behaviors were associated positively with
typical and masculine depression, consistent with Hypothesis 4. Table 2 displays indirect
Psychol Men Masc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 January 19.
Parent et al. Page 10
paths from hours of SM/SN use to each of the three dependent variables; all indirect paths
Author Manuscript
were significant, indicating that all posited mediation hypotheses were supported, consistent
with Hypothesis 5. R2 values were .14 for masculine externalizing depression, .27 for
masculine internalizing depression, and .09 for depression as assessed with the PHQ-9.
Discussion
The findings of the present study extend research on men’s SM/SN use, toxic masculinity,
and depression. Relationships between SM/SN use and toxic masculinity to depression, via
quality of SM/SN interactions, were consistent with theories of affect-biased attention and
toxic masculinity as applied to models of depression. Overall, the results emphasized the
mediating roles of positive and negative SM/SN interactions in the relations between SM/SN
use/toxic masculinity and depressive symptoms. These findings, outlined in the following
text, can be used to advance research and practice with men in the digital age.
Author Manuscript
First, consistent with some of the past research on SM/SN use, more frequent SM/SN use
was associated with higher levels of depression. In addition, SM/SN use was associated
directly with traditional depression and masculine externalizing depression, and indirectly
associated with both of those forms of depression and masculine internalizing depression
via positive and negative interactions on SM/SN sites. Regarding the indirect relations, more
frequent SM/SN use was indirectly associated with all three indicators of depression via
negative and positive online interactions (i.e., the affect-biased attention measures mediated
the relationship between SM/SN use and depression). The affect-biased attention variables
mediated the SM/SN use/depression relationships in different directions, such that positive
online behaviors appeared to buffer the relationship, whereas negative online behaviors
exacerbated it. These results suggest that simple evaluation of time spent on SM/SN sites,
Author Manuscript
without assessment of the valence of interactions, is likely insufficient to understand the role
SM/SN use has in men’s mood disturbances. Rather, it appears important to understand the
ways in which men interact online.
As anticipated, toxic masculinity was directly associated with affect-biased attention, here
defined as negative online behaviors. That is, men who more strongly endorsed the
dominance–heterosexism–misogyny triad of aspects of conformity to masculine norms
were more likely to report negative online interactions. This finding is consistent with
past research and theoretical work on toxic masculinity that suggests that adherence
to traditional masculinity is associated with maladaptive communication and interaction
styles (Burn & Ward, 2005; Coughlin & Wade, 2012; Jakupcak, Lisak, & Roemer, 2002;
Rochlen, McKelley, Suizzo, & Scaringi, 2008). The relation between toxic masculinity
and affect-biased attention is one of the most notable aspects of this study. In essence,
Author Manuscript
this finding suggests that men who adhere to toxic masculinity may engage in reliably
more negative SM/SN behaviors. For example, toxic masculinity may be associated with
increased propensity to seek out and read content with which one disagrees, ruminate
about disagreements or arguments on SM/SN sites, or make hostile responses to such
disagreements. Toxic masculinity is characterized by a need to dominate, antifemininity,
and homophobia (Kupers, 2005, 2010; Lorde, 1984; Parent & Moradi, 2011), which were
three aspects of masculinity assessed in the present study. It is possible that these three
Psychol Men Masc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 January 19.
Parent et al. Page 11
SM/SN sites are often an outlet for expressing political or ideological positions (Kushin
& Yamamoto, 2010; Rainie, Smith, Schlozman, Brady, & Verba, 2012; Shah et al., 2012),
sometimes anonymously, generating a limitless stream of stimuli for affect-biased attention
to fixate on. A position of antifeminism and homophobia, combined with a hyperfocus on a
need to dominate or win interactions, may promote engagement with SM/SN material with
which one disagrees, leading to negative SM/SN interactions (Herring et al., 2002; Shaw,
2014).
The indirect relationships from toxic masculinity to the three indicators of depression
were also significant, suggesting that negative online interactions mediate the relationship
between toxic masculinity and symptoms of depression. This finding is consistent with
previous research on affect-biased attention and depression, in that attunement to messages
perceived to be negative is associated with mood disturbance (Todd et al., 2012).
Author Manuscript
It is notable that negative online behaviors were associated uniquely and positively
with traditional depression, masculinized internalizing depression, and masculinizing
externalizing depression. This robust association suggests the need to further assess the
role of affect-biased attention in SM/SN involvement as it relates to mood disturbances.
In contrast, positive online behaviors, suggestive of a more adaptive affect-biased attention
attunement, were associated negatively with all three facets of depression assessed here.
This occurred despite the strong positive correlation between negative and positive online
behaviors, and suggests that the cultivation of a more positive affect-biased attention may
help individuals to reduce mood disturbances.
masculinity would be associated with higher levels of depression via affect-biased attention
(i.e., negative online interactions) was supported. That is, men who more strongly endorse
the aspects of toxic masculinity assessed in this study may be more likely to engage with
and ruminate on negative interactions online, and may benefit from examination of the
purpose and implications of such behavior. Efforts have been made to address affect-biased
attention through clinical interventions for a range of topics, including anxiety, pain, obesity,
and substance use (Castellanos et al., 2009; McGeary, Meadows, Amir, & Gibb, 2014; Roy,
Dennis, & Warner, 2015). Such interventions have principally involved direct practice in
shifting attention away from stimuli seen as anxiety provoking, and has shown promise as a
novel behavioral treatment approach.
Yet, endorsement of masculine norms is associated with more negative attitudes toward
Author Manuscript
help seeking (Galdas, Cheater, & Marshall, 2005; Möller-Leimkühler, 2002; Shepherd
& Rickard, 2012), and as such men high in these norms would be unlikely to seek
psychological help for mood disturbances. It may be more effective to engage in outreach
either in person or via SM/SNs to address toxic masculinity in online contexts. On college
campuses, psychologists might work with student groups involved in either online activities
(e.g., gaming student groups) or political groups whose members may be active in online
forums. As well, SM/SNs themselves may be harnessed to conduct broad public health
Psychol Men Masc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 January 19.
Parent et al. Page 12
interventions aimed to raise the level of online discourse and discourage negative rumination
Author Manuscript
over material viewed online, using models of successful online public health or awareness
interventions (Guo & Saxton, 2014; Obar, Zube, & Lampe, 2012).
Research on SM/SN interaction and depression has led to mixed results over numerous
studies. The present study suggests that quality of interactions rather than raw amount of
time on SM/SN sites is associated with depression among men. At the same time, our
findings are more nuanced with the moderate positive correlation that emerged between
positive and negative online interactions. Rather than individuals being divisible into groups
of those who primarily interact positively and those who react negatively online, there may
be underlying individual personality or circumstance characteristics that promote fixation
on online interactions, both positive and negative. Online interactions can be helpful to a
range of presenting concerns, including depression (Griffiths et al., 2012; Houston, Cooper,
& Ford, 2002), and simply recommending reduced SM/SN time may not be beneficial to
Author Manuscript
Given the integration of SM/SN use into everyday life, it may be reasonable to assess the
frequency and valence of SM/SN behaviors at intake with patients. SM/SN use can be
assessed as a source of support, connection, or personal development (e.g., using SM/SN
sites to maintain connections with family and friends, as a bridge to engage in real-world
activities with friends, or to learn about new topics). SM/SN use can also be assessed as a
source of negative interactions (e.g., regularly commenting negatively on others’ postings or
having one’s own postings be negatively commented upon, engaging in anonymous online
behavior that is antagonistic toward others, regularly choosing to view media that one finds
aggravating and frustrating). Modifications to SM/SN use can be implemented in the context
of empirically supported interventions. For example, if patients find that they engage in
rapid, angered responses to SM/SN posts with which they disagree, they might turn on a
Author Manuscript
mobile device in session, seek out such a post, and go over their reactions in real time with
the therapist (akin to an exposure and response prevention paradigm; Abramowitz, 1996).
In another circumstance, a patient may find themselves ruminating over online posts,
or being distracted from daily activities by mentally composing responses in their mind
while away from a computer. Such behaviors may be amenable to thought-stopping from
cognitive–behavioral intervention paradigms (Bakker, 2009), or mindfulness/present-focus
interventions from acceptance and commitment therapy (McCracken & Vowles, 2014). As
Psychol Men Masc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 January 19.
Parent et al. Page 13
well, SM/SNs can be leveraged for social support (Merolli, Gray, & Martin-Sanchez, 2013;
Author Manuscript
Oh, Ozkaya, & LaRose, 2014), particularly among individuals who may have limited access
to real-world networks (e.g., rural men; Stern & Adams, 2010). Anonymity may also be
useful to reducing barriers to help seeking, such as in the use of anonymous support forums
for various issues (Powell, Inglis, Ronnie, & Large, 2011; Tsui, Cheung, & Leung, 2010).
Providers might encourage patients to leverage such positive aspects of SM/SN use to
bolster treatment.
Negative online interactions may also be incorporated into treatment planning. For example,
individuals who regularly engage in negative or antagonistic behaviors online could
explore the motivation for such behaviors from developmental, dynamic, or social learning
paradigms. Such behaviors could also be altered cognitively and behaviorally. For example,
if a patient regularly gets into heated discussions online (sometimes called “flame wars”)
over various issues, these could be addressed in a therapeutic setting. The nature of such
Author Manuscript
postings would allow for the patient to open their SM/SN program and view the material
that prompted their reaction in session, and process through how they reacted to it in the
moment rather than rely completely on recall and reconstruction of events. Individuals prone
to antagonistic interactions online may view stimuli that would provoke such reactions in
session, and use in-session opportunities to engage in response prevention and practice
emotional regulation.
Given the negative association between conformity to traditional masculinity and attitudes
toward psychological help seeking, men high in toxic masculinity are unlikely to present in
therapy (Berger, Levant, McMillan, Kelleher, & Sellers, 2005; Galdas et al., 2005; Hammer,
Vogel, & Heimerdinger-Edwards, 2013; Möller-Leimkühler, 2002; Vogel, Heimerdinger-
Edwards, Hammer, & Hubbard, 2011). However, patients who endorse toxic masculinity
Author Manuscript
and high levels of SM/SN use may also be encouraged to explore their motivations for
maladaptive SM/SN use and how such use may negatively influence them or others.
Consistent with modern approaches to treating affect-biased attention, patients may be
helped to learn to intentionally alter their attentional focus. As well, mental energy and time
devoted to antagonistic or aggressive online behaviors might be worked on in therapy to be
funneled toward more adaptive or growth-oriented outlets (Jennings & Apsche, 2014).
sites. Longitudinal and experimental research could be used to further explore causality
among the variables in the study. Second, we used data collected online from Mturk.
Although this allows us to avoid concerns that may be raised with convenience samples of
undergraduate men, sampling from Mturk does have limitations. Although extant research
on Mturk has suggested that with protocols such as those included in this study (e.g.,
restricting participation to people from the United States, including validity check items)
data integrity can be improved (Rouse, 2015), there is still a risk that participants are paying
Psychol Men Masc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 January 19.
Parent et al. Page 14
minimal attention to items as they complete a study or are engaged in other behaviors (e.g.,
Author Manuscript
watching TV) while completing a study. Third, we assessed SM/SN involvement broadly.
We made this decision consciously, as SM/SN sites are broad interactive mediums and no
validated measures exist of nuanced interactions on SM/SN. Further, assessing the type of
social network used (e.g., Facebook, Reddit, Instagram, etc.) may not itself be meaningful.
For example, some individuals may use Reddit’s active and often volatile political forums,
whereas others may use other forums dedicated to funny pictures, science, world news,
music, video games, or photographs of baby animals (all subforums with more than 10
million subscribers worldwide). Thus, the medium is not the manner in Internet interaction.
The present study would have been augmented by, and speaks to the need for, modern
measures of social interaction that address Internet and SM/SN behaviors. Fourth, we
relied on use of subscales of the CMNI-46 to assess toxic masculinity; the present study
speaks to the need to develop a dedicated measure of toxic masculinity. Finally, our broad
Author Manuscript
assessment of the affective bias in attention in SM/SN use also suggests the need for a
dedicated measure that more clearly delineates the use of SM/SN. For example, reactive
and hostile responding to online material may have different antecedents and consequences
than ruminating carefully for hours over a response. Further, affect-biased attention research
often includes behavioral components, such as eye movement tracking. Extension of the
current research to such methodologies could be informative. For example, research may
assess whether individuals who endorse toxic masculinity also attend visually to stimuli that
is perceived as a threat (e.g., to a fake SM/SN post with which they disagree, compared with
one with which they agree).
The current research suggests a host of potential future research endeavors. Research
on men’s online interactions, and the relationships between those behaviors and mental
and physical health, is lacking. Further research may explore differences among SM/SN
Author Manuscript
References
Abramowitz JS (1996). Variants of exposure and response prevention in the treatment of
obsessive-compulsive disorder: A meta-analysis. Behavior Therapy, 27, 583–600. 10.1016/
S0005-7894(96)80045-1
Aldao A, Nolen-Hoeksema S, & Schweizer S (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across
psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 217–237.
10.1016/)cpr.2009.11.004 [PubMed: 20015584]
Psychol Men Masc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 January 19.
Parent et al. Page 15
Barthel M, Stocking G, Holcomb J, & Mitchell A (2010). Nearly eight-in-ten Reddit users get
news on the site. Pew Research Center. Retrieved from https://researchspace.auckland.ac.nz/handle/
2292/8354
Berger JM, Levant RF, McMillan KK, Kelleher W, & Sellers A (2005). Impact of gender role conflict,
traditional masculinity ideology, alexithymia, and age on men’s attitudes toward psychological help
seeking. Psychology of Men and Masculinity, 6, 73–78. 10.1037/1524-9220.6.1.73
Berking M, Margraf M, Ebert D, Wupperman P, Hofmann SG, & Junghanns K (2011). Deficits
in emotion-regulation skills predict alcohol use during and after cognitive-behavioral therapy
for alcohol dependence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79, 307–318. 10.1037/
a0023421 [PubMed: 21534653]
Błachnio A, Przepiórka A, & Pantic I (2015). Internet use, Facebook intrusion, and depression:
Results of a cross-sectional study. European Psychiatry: The Journal of the Association of European
Psychiatrists, 30, 681–684. 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2015.04.002 [PubMed: 25963476]
Bogaert AF, & McCreary DR (2011). Masculinity and the distortion of self-reported height in men.
Author Manuscript
Connell RW, & Messerschmidt JW (2005). Hegemonic masculinity: Rethinking the concept. Gender
and Society, 19, 829–859. 10.1177/0891243205278639
Coughlin P, & Wade JC (2012). Masculinity ideology, income disparity, and romantic relationship
quality among men with higher earning female partners. Sex Roles, 67, 311–322. 10.1007/
s11199-012-0187-6
Courtenay WH (2000). Constructions of masculinity and their influence on men’s well-being:
A theory of gender and health. Social Science and Medicine, 50, 1385–1401. 10.1016/
S0277-9536(99)00390-1 [PubMed: 10741575]
Coyne SM, Padilla-Walker LM, Day RD, Harper J, & Stockdale L (2014). A friend request from
dear old dad: Associations between parent-child social networking and adolescent outcomes.
Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 17, 8–13. 10.1089/cyber.2012.0623
[PubMed: 23845157]
Datu JAD, Valdez JP, & Datu N (2012). Does Facebooking make us sad? Hunting relationship between
Facebook use and depression among Filipino adolescents. International Journal of Research
Author Manuscript
Psychol Men Masc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 January 19.
Parent et al. Page 16
Donchev D (2016, March 19). Youtube Statistics - 2017. Retrieved July 26, 2017, from https://
fortunelords.com/youtube-statistics/
Author Manuscript
Galdas PM, Cheater F, & Marshall P (2005). Men and health help-seeking behaviour: Literature
review. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 49, 616–623. 10.1111/j1365-2648200403331x [PubMed:
15737222]
Gallagher KE, & Parrott DJ (2011). What accounts for men’s hostile attitudes toward women? The
influence of hegemonic male role norms and masculine gender role stress. Violence Against
Women, 17, 568–583. 10.1177/1077801211407296 [PubMed: 21531691]
Gilbody S, Richards D, Brealey S, & Hewitt C (2007). Screening for depression in medical settings
with the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ): A diagnostic meta-analysis. Journal of General
Internal Medicine, 22, 1596–1602. 10.1007/s11606-007-0333-y [PubMed: 17874169]
Goodman JK, Cryder CE, & Cheema A (2013). Data collection in a flat world: The strengths and
weaknesses of Mechanical Turk samples. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 26, 213–224.
10.1002/bdm.1753
Gottfried J, & Shearer E (2016). News use across social media platforms 2016. Pew
Research Center. Retrieved from http://www.journalism.org/2016/05/26/news-use-across-social-
Author Manuscript
media-platforms-2016/
Greenwood S, Perrin A, & Duggan M (2016). Social Media Update 2016. Retrieved from http://
www.pewinternet.org/2016/11/11/social-media-update-2016/
Griffiths KM, Mackinnon AJ, Crisp DA, Christensen H, Bennett K, & Farrer L (2012). The
effectiveness of an online support group for members of the community with depression: A
randomised controlled trial. PLoS ONE, 7, e53244. 10.1371/journal.pone.0053244 [PubMed:
23285271]
Guo C, & Saxton GD (2014). Tweeting social change: How social media are changing nonprofit
advocacy. Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Quarterly, 43, 57–79. 10.1177/0899764012471585
Hammer JH, Vogel DL, & Heimerdinger-Edwards SR (2013). Men’s help seeking: Examination of
differences across community size, education, and income. Psychology of Men and Masculinity,
14, 65–75. 10.1037/a0026813
Herring S, Job-Sluder K, Scheckler R, & Barab S (2002). Searching for safety online:
Managing “trolling” in a feminist forum. The Information Society, 18, 371–384.
10.1080/01972240290108186
Author Manuscript
Houston TK, Cooper LA, & Ford DE (2002). Internet support groups for depression: A 1-
year prospective cohort study. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 2062–2068. 10.1176/
appiajp15912.2062 [PubMed: 12450957]
Ipeirotis PG, Provost F, & Wang J (2010). Quality management on amazon mechanical turk. In
Proceedings of the ACM SIGKDD workshop on human computation (pp. 64–67). New York, NY:
ACM.
Iwamoto DK, Corbin W, Lejuez C, & MacPherson L (2014). College men and alcohol use: Positive
alcohol expectancies as a mediator between distinct masculine norms and alcohol use. Psychology
of Men and Masculinity, 15, 29–39. 10.1037/a0031594 [PubMed: 25705133]
Jakupcak M, Lisak D, & Roemer L (2002). The role of masculine ideology and masculine gender
role stress in men’s perpetration of relationship violence. Psychology of Men and Masculinity, 3,
97–106. 10.1037/1524-9220.3.2.97
Jennings JL, & Apsche JA (2014). The evolution of a fundamentally mindfulness-based treatment
methodology: From DBT and ACT to MDT and beyond. International Journal of Behavioral and
Author Manuscript
Psychol Men Masc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 January 19.
Parent et al. Page 17
Kupers TA (2010). Role of misogyny and homophobia in prison sexual abuse. UCLA Women’s Law
Journal, 18, 107–130.
Author Manuscript
Kushin MJ, & Yamamoto M (2010). Did social media really matter? College students’ use of online
media and political decision making in the 2008 election. Mass Communication and Society, 13,
608–630. 10.1080/15205436.2010.516863
Lapidot-Lefler N, & Barak A (2012). Effects of anonymity, invisibility, and lack of eye-
contact on toxic online disinhibition. Computers in Human Behavior, 28, 434–443. 10.1016/
j.chb.2011.10.014
Levant RF, Rankin TJ, Williams CM, Hasan NT, & Smalley KB (2010). Evaluation of the factor
structure and construct validity of scores on the Male Role Norms Inventory–Revised (MRNI-R).
Psychology of Men and Masculinity, 11, 25–37. 10.1037/a0017637
Lin LY, Sidani JE, Shensa A, Radovic A, Miller E, Colditz JB, … Primack BA (2016). Association
between social media use and depression among U.S. young adults. Depression and Anxiety, 33,
323–331. 10.1002/da.22466 [PubMed: 26783723]
Little TD, Cunningham WA, Shahar G, & Widaman KF (2002). To parcel or not to parcel:
Exploring the question, weighing the merits. Structural Equation Modeling, 9, 151–173. 10.1207/
Author Manuscript
S15328007SEM0902_1
Lorde A (1984). Sister outsider: Essays and speeches by Audrey Lorde. Berkeley, CA: Crossing Press.
Magovcevic M, & Addis ME (2008). The Masculine Depression Scale: Development
and psychometric evaluation. Psychology of Men and Masculinity, 9, 117–132.
10.1037/1524-9220.9.3.117
Martin A, Rief W, Klaiberg A, & Braehler E (2006). Validity of the Brief Patient Health Questionnaire
Mood Scale (PHQ-9) in the general population. General Hospital Psychiatry, 28, 71–77. 10.1016/
j.genhosppsych.2005.07.003 [PubMed: 16377369]
McCracken LM, & Vowles KE (2014). Acceptance and commitment therapy and mindfulness
for chronic pain: Model, process, and progress. American Psychologist, 69, 178–187. 10.1037/
a0035623 [PubMed: 24547803]
McGeary JE, Meadows SP, Amir N, & Gibb BE (2014). Computer-delivered, home-based, attentional
retraining reduces drinking behavior in heavy drinkers. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 28,
559–562. 10.1037/a0036086 [PubMed: 24955674]
Author Manuscript
Merolli M, Gray K, & Martin-Sanchez F (2013). Health outcomes and related effects of using social
media in chronic disease management: A literature review and analysis of affordances. Journal of
Biomedical Informatics, 46, 957–969. 10.1016/j.jbi.2013.04.010 [PubMed: 23702104]
Mikorski R, & Szymanski DM (2017). Masculine norms, peer group, pornography, Facebook, and
men’s sexual objectification of women. Psychology of Men and Masculinity, 18, 257–267.
10.1037/men0000058
Möller-Leimkühler AM (2002). Barriers to help-seeking by men: A review of sociocultural and
clinical literature with particular reference to depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 71, 1–9.
10.1016/S0165-0327(01)00379-2 [PubMed: 12167495]
Morales S, Fu X, & Pérez-Edgar KE (2016). A developmental neuroscience perspective on affect-
biased attention. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 26–41. 10.1016/j.dcn.2016.08.001
[PubMed: 27606972]
Morrison CM, & Gore H (2010). The relationship between excessive Internet use and depression:
A questionnaire-based study of 1,319 young people and adults. Psychopathology, 43, 121–126.
10.1159/000277001 [PubMed: 20110764]
Author Manuscript
Murnen SK, Wright C, & Kaluzny G (2002). If “boys will be boys,” then girls will be victims? A
meta-analytic review of the research that relates masculine ideology to sexual aggression. Sex
Roles, 46, 359–375. 10.1023/A:1020488928736
Muthén LK, & Muthén BO (2012). Mplus user’s guide. Los Angeles, CA: Author.
Nadeau MM, Balsan MJ, & Rochlen AB (2016). Men’s depression: Endorsed experiences and
expressions. Psychology of Men and Masculinity, 17, 328–335. 10.1037/men0000027
Nadkarni A, & Hofmann SG (2012). Why do people use Facebook? Personality and Individual
Differences, 52, 243–249. 10.1016/j.paid.2011.11.007 [PubMed: 22544987]
Psychol Men Masc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 January 19.
Parent et al. Page 18
Obar JA, Zube P, & Lampe C (2012). Advocacy 2.0: An analysis of how advocacy groups in
the United States perceive and use social media as tools for facilitating civic engagement and
Author Manuscript
for conformity to masculine norms. Psychology of Men and Masculinity, 12, 354–367. 10.1037/
a0023837
Parent MC, Torrey C, & Michaels MS (2012). “HIV testing is so gay”: The role of masculine gender
role conformity in HIV testing among men who have sex with men. Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 59, 465–470. 10.1037/a0028067 [PubMed: 22774868]
Powell J, Inglis N, Ronnie J, & Large S (2011). The characteristics and motivations of online health
information seekers: Cross-sectional survey and qualitative interview study. Journal of Medical
Internet Research, 13, e20. 10.2196/jmir.1600 [PubMed: 21345783]
Rainie L, Smith A, Schlozman KL, Brady H, & Verba S (2012). Social media and
political engagement. Washington DC: Pew Research Center’s Internet & American
Life Project. Retrieved from http://www.pewinternet.org/files/old-media/Files/Reports/2012/
PIP_SocialMediaAndPoliticalEngagement_PDF.pdf
Rankin Z (2013). Conformity to masculine norms, gender role conflict and relationship satisfaction in
inter-male friendships. Arcata, CA: Humboldt State University.
Rice SM, Fallon BJ, Aucote HM, & Möller-Leimkühler AM (2013). Development and preliminary
Author Manuscript
validation of the Male Depression Risk scale: Furthering the assessment of depression in men.
Journal of Affective Disorders, 151, 950–958. 10.1016/j.jad.2013.08.013 [PubMed: 24051100]
Rice SM, Fallon BJ, & Bambling M (2011). Men and depression: The impact of masculine role
norms throughout the lifespan. The Educational and Developmental Psychologist, 28, 133–144.
10.1375/aedp282133
Robinson MS, & Alloy LB (2003). Negative cognitive styles and stress-reactive rumination interact to
predict depression: A prospective study. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 275–291. 10.1023/
A:1023914416469
Rochlen AB, McKelley RA, Suizzo M-A, & Scaringi V (2008). Predictors of relationship satisfaction,
psychological well-being, and life satisfaction among stay-at-home fathers. Psychology of Men
and Masculinity, 9, 17–28. 10.1037/1524-9220.9.1.17
Rouse SV (2015). A reliability analysis of Mechanical Turk data. Computers in Human Behavior, 43,
304–307. 10.1016/j.chb.2014.11.004
Roy AK, Dennis TA, & Warner CM (2015). A critical review of attentional threat bias and its role
Author Manuscript
in the treatment of pediatric anxiety disorders. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 29, 171–184.
10.1891/0889-8391.29.3.171 [PubMed: 32755946]
Sanders CE, Field TM, Diego M, & Kaplan M (2000). The relationship of Internet use to depression
and social isolation among adolescents. Adolescence, 35, 237–242. [PubMed: 11019768]
Santana AD (2014). Virtuous or vitriolic: The effect of anonymity on civility in online newspaper
reader comment boards. Journalism Practice, 8, 18–33. 10.1080/17512786.2013.813194
Shah DV, Friedland LA, Wells C, Kim YM, Rojas H, & Bennett WL (2012). The personalization of
politics: Political identity, social media, and changing patterns of participation. The Annals of the
American Academy of Political and Social Science, 644, 20–39. 10.1177/0002716212451428
Psychol Men Masc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 January 19.
Parent et al. Page 19
Shaw A (2014). The internet is full of jerks, because the world is full of jerks: What feminist
theory teaches us about the internet. Communication and Critical/Cultural Studies, 11, 273–277.
Author Manuscript
10.1080/14791420.2014.926245
Shepherd CB, & Rickard KM (2012). Drive for muscularity and help-seeking: The mediational role of
gender role conflict, self-stigma, and attitudes. Psychology of Men and Masculinity, 13, 379–392.
10.1037/a0025923
Shklovski I, Barkhuus L, Bornoe N, & Kaye J (2015). Friendship maintenance in the digital age:
Applying a relational lens to online social interaction. In Proceedings of the 18th ACM Conference
on Computer Supported Cooperative Work & Social Computing (pp. 1477–1487). New York, NY:
ACM. 10.1145/2675133.2675294
Sosik VS, & Bazarova NN (2014). Relational maintenance on social network sites: How Facebook
communication predicts relational escalation. Computers in Human Behavior, 35, 124–131.
10.1016/j.chb.2014.02.044
Stern MJ, & Adams AE (2010). Do rural residents really use the internet to build
social capital? An empirical investigation. American Behavioral Scientist, 53, 1389–1422.
10.1177/0002764210361692
Author Manuscript
Tabachnick BG, & Fidell LS (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn &
Bacon.
Tandoc EC Jr., Ferrucci P, & Duffy M (2015). Facebook use, envy, and depression among college
students. Computers in Human Behavior, 43, 139–146. 10.1016/j.chb.2014.10.053
Todd RM, Cunningham WA, Anderson AK, & Thompson E (2012). Affect-biased attention as emotion
regulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 365–372. 10.1016/j.tics.2012.06.003 [PubMed:
22717469]
Tsui V, Cheung M, & Leung P (2010). Help-seeking among male victims of partner abuse: Men’s hard
times. Journal of Community Psychology, 38, 769–780. 10.1002/jcop.20394
Vogel DL, Heimerdinger-Edwards SR, Hammer JH, & Hubbard A (2011). “Boys don’t cry”:
Examination of the links between endorsement of masculine norms, self-stigma, and help-seeking
attitudes for men from diverse backgrounds. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 58, 368–382.
10.1037/a0023688 [PubMed: 21639615]
Wade J, & Brittan-Powell C (2001). Men’s attitudes toward race and gender equity: The importance of
masculinity ideology, gender-related traits, and reference group identity dependence. Psychology
Author Manuscript
Psychol Men Masc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 January 19.
Parent et al. Page 20
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Figure 1.
Standardized path coefficients and standard errors.
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Psychol Men Masc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 January 19.
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
Table 1
Construct Intercorrelations
Variable 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 M SD
Parent et al.
1. Hours on social media .06 .11* −.03 .19** .21** .16** .16** .17** 2.16 1.03
2. CMNI—Winning .21** .25** .10 .02 −.01 .09 .00 2.52 0.54
3. CMNI—Power Over Women .42** .11* .34** .10* .36** .19** 1.91 0.63
Note. CMNI = Conformity to Masculine Norms Inventory-46; PHQ-9 = Patient Health Questionniare–9.
*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.
Psychol Men Masc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 January 19.
Page 21
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
Table 2
Indirect Effects From Hours of Social Media and Toxic Masculinity to Depression
Note. PHQ-9 = Patient Health Questionnaire–9; MDS = Masculine Depression Scale; CI = confidence interval. All 95% CIs do not cross 1, and as such all paths are significant at p < .05.
Psychol Men Masc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 January 19.
Page 22