(Medical Psychiatry) Buysse, Daniel - Sateia, Michael J - Insomnia - Diagnosis and Treatment-CRC Press (2016)
(Medical Psychiatry) Buysse, Daniel - Sateia, Michael J - Insomnia - Diagnosis and Treatment-CRC Press (2016)
MEDICAL PSYCHIATRY
Advisory Board
Edited by
Michael J. Sateia
Dartmouth Medical School
Lebanon, New Hampshire, U.S.A.
Daniel J. Buysse
University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine
Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, U.S.A.
CRC Press
Taylor & Francis Group
6000 Broken Sound Parkway NW, Suite 300
Boca Raton, FL 33487-2742
This book contains information obtained from authentic and highly regarded sources. While all reasonable
efforts have been made to publish reliable data and information, neither the author[s] nor the publisher can
accept any legal responsibility or liability for any errors or omissions that may be made. The publishers wish to
make clear that any views or opinions expressed in this book by individual editors, authors or contributors are
personal to them and do not necessarily reflect the views/opinions of the publishers. The information or guid-
ance contained in this book is intended for use by medical, scientific or health-care professionals and is provided
strictly as a supplement to the medical or other professional’s own judgement, their knowledge of the patient’s
medical history, relevant manufacturer’s instructions and the appropriate best practice guidelines. Because of
the rapid advances in medical science, any information or advice on dosages, procedures or diagnoses should be
independently verified. The reader is strongly urged to consult the drug companies’ printed instructions, and
their websites, before administering any of the drugs recommended in this book. This book does not indicate
whether a particular treatment is appropriate or suitable for a particular individual. Ultimately it is the sole
responsibility of the medical professional to make his or her own professional judgements, so as to advise and
treat patients appropriately. The authors and publishers have also attempted to trace the copyright holders of all
material reproduced in this publication and apologize to copyright holders if permission to publish in this form
has not been obtained. If any copyright material has not been acknowledged please write and let us know so we
may rectify in any future reprint.
Except as permitted under U.S. Copyright Law, no part of this book may be reprinted, reproduced, transmit-
ted, or utilized in any form by any electronic, mechanical, or other means, now known or hereafter invented,
including photocopying, microfilming, and recording, or in any information storage or retrieval system, without
written permission from the publishers.
For permission to photocopy or use material electronically from this work, please access www.copyright.com
(http://www.copyright.com/) or contact the Copyright Clearance Center, Inc. (CCC), 222 Rosewood Drive, Dan-
vers, MA 01923, 978-750-8400. CCC is a not-for-profit organization that provides licenses and registration for a
variety of users. For organizations that have been granted a photocopy license by the CCC, a separate system of
payment has been arranged.
Trademark Notice: Product or corporate names may be trademarks or registered trademarks, and are used only
for identification and explanation without intent to infringe.
Visit the Taylor & Francis Web site at
http://www.taylorandfrancis.com
As the field of sleep medicine has matured in recent decades, it has become increasingly appar-
ent that disruption or failure of normal sleep function, not unlike failure of other essential
physiologic functions, has sweeping adverse consequences. Chronic insomnia is arguably the
most common form of sleep disruption, although its significance as a serious health problem
has been, and continues to be, largely overlooked. Much of this neglect seems to arise from
the tacit assumption that this condition is, in effect, more a benign existential problem than a
disorder deserving serious medical attention. Yet, investigations of chronic insomnia over the
past 20 years underscore the importance of addressing this issue as a component of the patient’s
overall health care. Not only is it clear that chronic insomnia is associated with impairments
in quality of life and function, comparable to those seen in disorders such as major depression
and congestive heart failure, but emerging data also indicate that insomnia may be a significant
risk factor for development of major psychiatric and possibly medical disorders. The availabil-
ity of effective therapies that can produce clinically meaningful and durable improvement or
resolution of symptoms lends further weight to the importance of identifying insomnia.
Although a small and dedicated group of sleep researchers and clinicians have made sig-
nificant strides in addressing this problem, an enormous knowledge and awareness gap exists
among most health care providers with respect to insomnia. Although we have made sub-
stantial efforts to address this through educational means, clearly the work has only begun. A
necessary foundation of sound educational efforts is a detailed, accessible body of knowledge
that addresses the pathophysiology, evaluation, and treatment of the disorder. Although many
excellent books have been published on insomnia, somewhat surprisingly, there have been only
limited efforts to produce a comprehensive reference text in this area. Our hope is that this
volume will help to meet this need.
Insomnia is relevant to virtually all aspects of medicine. For this reason, researchers and
clinicians from all fields must have access to detailed information. The subject matter of this
book will certainly be of considerable interest and importance to all sleep medicine clinicians.
However, its relevance and utility should extend well beyond this audience. As data increasingly
underscore the important and complex interaction between insomnia and mental illness, it is
incumbent upon psychiatrists, psychologists, and other mental health workers to not only
identify but also actively intervene when chronic insomnia complicates psychiatric disease.
Although the evidence that compels effective management of insomnia in comorbid medical and
neurological disease is not yet as well developed as that for mental illness, there is, nonetheless,
ample basis for internists, family practice physicians, neurologists, and other specialists to
appreciate the significant role that insomnia may play in the pathogenesis and maintenance of
physical illnesses.
The past 20 years have seen enormous progress in our understanding of the nature and
characteristics of chronic insomnia and in our ability to accurately assess and effectively treat
the problem. These advances have profoundly affected our view of chronic insomnia—most
importantly, transitioning its position from that of “secondary” symptom of other disorders
to a condition comorbid with other disorders. This paradigm shift is important because most
chronic insomnia is, in fact, comorbid with other medical or psychiatric disease. This perspective
suggests that chronic insomnia exhibits its own unique and somewhat independent pathophys-
iology, which is not only influenced by comorbid disorders but also, in turn, has major influence
on those disorders.
viii Preface
This book is intended to provide the reader with a comprehensive and detailed overview
of the current research and state-of-the-art practice parameters related to insomnia in three
parts. The first section, Fundamentals, addresses the characteristics and consequences of chronic
insomnia and, in effect, speaks about the nature of the disorder and the question of why
it deserves medical attention. The Evaluation section provides the clinician with a detailed
description of causes and comorbidities and offers clinicians specific guidelines and tools for
evaluating the disorder in its varied and often complex presentations. Finally, the section on
Management represents what we believe to be the most detailed and comprehensive description
of treatment modalities for chronic insomnia that is currently available. Each of the chapters
in this book is authored by recognized experts in the field whose research and writing have
collectively defined this area of sleep medicine.
In the modern era, medicine has moved progressively toward a multideterminant model
of causation of disease and multimodal treatment. It has become increasingly apparent that
sleep is one of the critical determinants of health and well-being. It is no longer possible for
researchers to effectively study disease or clinicians to effectively treat it without considering
the potential role of sleep and circadian factors. We believe that this comprehensive volume
will serve to stimulate interest and further inquiry in this area by scientists and clinicians from
many fields, and will provide practitioners with a guide for the management of this common
malady.
We welcome your thoughts, comments, and suggestions on the text to help us prepare for
the Second Edition.
Michael J. Sateia
Daniel J. Buysse
Contents
Preface . . . . vii
Contributors . . . . xiii
38. Clinical Trials and the Development of New Therapeutics for Insomnia 436
Gary K. Zammit
The worst thing in the world is to try to sleep and not to.
F. Scott Fitzgerald
HISTORY
Fitzgerald succinctly captured the feelings of many chronic insomnia sufferers who labor nightly
to achieve the solace and restoration of a good night’s sleep. History is replete with examples of
the tragedies and anguish wrought as a result of insomnia. In one of the earliest known pieces
of literature, the Epic of Gilgamesh, the epic hero and ruler of the first known civilization is
beset by sleeplessness (1). In the biblical tale of Job, God inflicts insomnia on Job, causing him
to lament: “When I lie down, I say, ‘when shall I arise, and the night be gone?’ and I am full of
tossings to and fro unto the dawning of the day.”
The earliest recorded discussion of insomnia as a medical symptom can be traced to
Hippocrates (2), who recognized perturbations of sleep as a sign of disease. However, it is likely
that insomnia was being treated with opium and herbal preparations well prior to the rise of
Greek civilization (3). During much of the past two millennia, insomnia has been viewed as
a secondary phenomenon, attributable to a variety of causes. Disturbances of sleep have, for
millennia, been associated with an unquiet mind, in the form of guilt, vexation or rumination.
Examples of this are commonly cited in literature, such as in the works of Shakespeare, when
Romeo’s advisor, Friar Lawrence, notes: “Care keeps his watch in every old man’s eye, and
where care lodges, sleep can never lie.” Environmental factors such as noise, light, temperature,
or characteristics of the bed have been blamed. Benjamin Franklin, a noted insomniac, would
remove himself from bed and allow it to air so that the sheets might cool (4). Charles Dickens,
another insomnia sufferer, would sleep only in the exact middle of north-facing beds (5). Dickens
often wandered the streets of London during the night, providing a source of inspiration for
many of his characters and scenes. Insomnia became the subject of increasing medical attention
in Victorian England, when the queen herself was reportedly prescribed cannabis for sleep. An
article from the British Medical Journal of 1894 (6) lamented: “The hurry and excitement of
modern life is held to be responsible for much of the insomnia of which we hear; and most of
the articles and letters are full of good advice to live more quietly and of platitudes concerning
the harmfulness of rush and worry. The pity of it is that so many people are unable to follow
this good advice and are obliged to lead a life of anxiety and high tension.”
As Horne (5) points out in a 2008 essay, it is remarkable that, despite the widely held
notion that sleep disturbance was the product of psychological factors, “cures” for the affliction
were overwhelmingly pharmacological in nature. Opiates, alcohol, and herbals, such as valerian,
were likely used to treat insomnia in Sumerian and Greek civilizations, and continued to occupy
a central role in this respect (e.g., laudanum, a combination of opioid and alcohol) until the late
19th century when they gradually became supplanted by newer compounds such as bromides,
chloral hydrate, and later, barbiturates.
The conceptualization of insomnia in the first half of the 20th century was largely domi-
nated by the psychoanalytic view of insomnia as a psychoneurotic symptom, treatable, in theory,
through analysis. This view perpetuated the long-standing perception of insomnia as a symptom
secondary to psychological distress or other primary conditions. Nevertheless, the most widely
used therapies during this period were barbiturate and like compounds such as glutethimide
c01 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:11 Char Count=
2 SATEIA
DEFINING INSOMNIA
For much of the modern era of sleep medicine, the field has suffered from a lack of a compre-
hensive, widely accepted and utilized definition of insomnia. This absence has resulted in a
considerable degree of heterogeneity in definitions and, hence, difficulties in comparing clinical
or research samples of “insomnia.” There are a number of components that may be considered
in a general definition of insomnia. These include: (1) symptom profile—most definitions have
historically included problems getting to sleep and staying asleep, the latter including early
awakening. There continues to be debate as to whether nonrestorative sleep complaints should
be included in this definition; (2) chronicity—most modern-day definitions have distinguished
between acute and chronic insomnia, although the exact durations that separate these have
varied significantly from as short as two weeks to as long as six months. Historically, some have
suggested that the term insomnia be reserved only for chronic disturbances; (3) subjective ver-
sus objective findings—although it has long been recognized that insomnia is in many respects,
a highly subjective experience, researchers and some clinical approaches have attempted to
define insomnia by means of objective parameters such as sleep latency, number of awaken-
ings, wake time after sleep onset or total sleep time. At present, quantitative, objective criteria
are employed primarily in research settings, while clinical diagnostic criteria rely solely on sub-
jective complaints; (4) frequency—there has been wide variability with respect to application
of frequency criteria. Presently, neither the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disor-
ders, fourth edition (DSM-IV) (8) nor the International Classification of Sleep Disorders, second
edition (ICSD-2) (9) include a frequency criterion; (5) presence of daytime consequences–while
it has long been understood that complaints of daytime symptoms and dysfunction are one of
the major complications of insomnia, inclusion of these consequences has not been a standard
component of many research criteria until recent years. Both DSM-IV and ICSD-2 include a
requirement of daytime dysfunction.
The modern-day definitions of insomnia can be traced to the 1979 publication of the
Diagnostic Classification of Sleep and Arousal Disorders (10). It is of note that this volume does
not, in fact, offer a general definition of insomnia but only describes features specific to each
particular diagnosis. The insomnia diagnoses are clumped within a single category of disorders
of initiating and maintaining sleep (DIMS). There are no specific diagnostic criteria other than
presence of a DIMS and descriptors of key characteristics that are focused largely on presumed
etiologies. The first edition of the International Classification of Sleep Disorders (11) did not
offer a general definition of insomnia, other than to describe degrees of severity in terms of “a
. . . complaint of an insufficient amount of sleep or not feeling rested.” ICSD-1 does include a
requirement of “a complaint of decreased functioning during wakefulness” with some (though,
curiously, not all) insomnia related diagnoses.
The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV)
specifically includes difficulty initiating or maintaining sleep, as well as nonrestorative sleep
in its insomnia criteria. It also includes minimum one-month duration and a requirement that
the disturbance results in “clinically significant distress or impairment in social, occupational,
or other important areas of function.” The 2005 revision of the ICSD (second edition) was the
first to include explicit general criteria for insomnia (Table 1). The criteria include the unique
addition of a requirement of adequate opportunity and circumstances to sleep, drawing a
bright line between insomnia and insufficient sleep syndromes. All insomnia disorders must
be of at least one-month duration, excepting adjustment insomnia, which must be of less than
c01 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:11 Char Count=
A. A complaint of difficulty initiating sleep, difficulty maintaining sleep, or waking up too early, or sleep that is
chronically nonrestorative or poor in quality. In children the sleep difficulty is often reported by the
caretaker and may consist of observed bedtime resistance or inability to sleep independently.
B. The above sleep difficulty occurs despite adequate opportunity and circumstances for sleep.
C. At least one of the following forms of daytime impairment related to the nighttime sleep difficulty is
reported by the patient:
i Fatigue/malaise;
ii Attention, concentration, or memory impairment;
iii Social/vocational dysfunction or poor school performance;
iv Mood disturbance/irritability;
v Daytime sleepiness;
vi Motivation/energy/initiative reduction;
vii Proneness for errors/accidents at work or while driving;
viii Tension, headaches, and/or GI symptoms in response to sleep loss; and
ix Concerns or worries about sleep.
three months duration. These criteria also identify specific daytime consequences, which must
occur as a result of the sleep disturbance. A companion of sorts to the ICSD-2 criteria is the
Research Diagnostic Criteria (RDC) (12) for insomnia. The general criteria of the RDC are
essentially identical to those of the ICSD-2. The extensive review of criteria undertaken as
part of the development of the RDC examined the frequency with which various diagnostic
criteria were utilized for subject selection in the 165 insomnia papers selected. These data
are reproduced in Figure 1. Research studies have typically included objective PSG criteria as
criteria for inclusion in insomnia studies. This begs the question of whether objective criteria
should be applied to clinical populations as well. In research environments, such criteria allow
for establishment of more uniform and highly specific populations, essential to the research.
In the clinical setting, however, effective application of such objective criteria is fraught with
significant problems. These complications are best encapsulated by the oft-cited observation
that not every (objectively) poor sleeper has insomnia and not every insomniac has (objectively)
poor sleep. Indeed, there are many individuals who suffer great distress as a result of perceived
sleep disturbance who would not meet typical objective (e.g., PSG) criteria for “insomnia” (e.g.,
patients with paradoxical insomnia), while many others who would meet such criteria have no
insomnia complaints. Thus, degree of distress, perhaps to a greater extent than any objective
changes, seems to dictate the presence or severity of this condition.
Investigations of insomnia have, for some time, identified this discrepancy between objec-
tive findings and subjective perception of sleep in patients with insomnia as characteristic of
the condition (13,14). Persons with insomnia most often overestimate the degree of sleep dis-
turbance in comparison to objective (polysomnographic or actigraphic) criteria. Thus, sleep
latency, frequency of awakening and amount of wake after sleep onset are overestimated, while
total sleep time is underestimated. This discrepancy is observed at its greatest magnitude in
paradoxical insomnia, a condition in which sleep is objectively normal or near normal (by
standard PSG scoring criteria), while subjective perception suggests very little or no sleep.
While there is no well-established explanation for this discrepancy, emerging data on alter-
ation of biological and cognitive activity in the sleep of chronic insomnia patients (discussed in
later sections) may help explain why PSG-defined sleep may be misperceived as wake in this
population.
Nonrestorative Sleep
Nonrestorative sleep (NRS) represents a little-investigated complaint that has been catego-
rized in both ICSD-2 and DSM-IV with more standard insomnia symptoms of initiation and
maintenance problems. ICSD-2 references sleep that is “chronically nonrestorative and poor in
quality” (9). Thus, the affected individual is required to invoke a causal relationship between
c01 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:11 Char Count=
4 SATEIA
Sleep-related daytime sx
PSG findings
Frequency criteria
Medication exclusions
Presence of DIMS sx
Psychiatric exclusions
Medical exclusions
Duration criteria
Diagnostic criteria
0 10 20 30 40 50
% of samples
Figure 1 Criteria used for Sample Selection. Source: From Ref. 12.
the poorly defined nocturnal disturbance and daytime symptoms. When complaints of non-
restorative sleep are associated with other insomnia complaints (trouble falling or staying
asleep), this etiologic leap may seem relatively straightforward. In the absence of these more
typical insomnia complaints, patients may experience “light” sleep (a vaguely defined sense of
partial/intermittent wakefulness) or no specific nocturnal complaints at all. In these cases, the
etiologic association would seem significantly more tenuous. Much of the available data on NRS
is epidemiologic in nature (15,16). The subject has also been recently reviewed (17). Although
the definitions applied in such studies typically include the criterion that the complaint occurs
“despite normal sleep duration,” this may not adequately account for individuals with longer
sleep requirements who are, in effect, sleep-deprived. Absent objective assessment of sleep, the
possibility of other, unidentified sleep disorders must also be considered. In this regard, it is
noteworthy that one survey of NRS (16) found that this group has a threefold higher rate of
excessive daytime sleepiness complaints, a finding more often associated with sleep disorders
other than insomnia. Many of the questions utilized to define NRS focus on morning symp-
toms (“not feeling rested on awakening,” “ease in getting started” or “waking up exhausted or
fatigued”), perhaps reflecting more of an issue of high sleep inertia in some, as opposed to truly
nonrestorative sleep.
The estimated prevalence of nonrestorative sleep is in the range of approximately 10%
(16). However, when the prevalence of NRS, in the absence of any other insomnia symptoms,
is examined, the prevalence drops to approximately 1% to 2%. Thus, it seems likely that there
is a subgroup of individuals who may manifest psychophysiologic disturbance and daytime
consequences similar to those of the more well-defined insomnia population, absent the usual
insomnia complaints. However, this remains poorly characterized and appears likely to repre-
sent a relatively small percentage of the total population of “insomnia” patients.
c01 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:11 Char Count=
CONCLUSION
It seems clear that work remains in achieving a comprehensive and broadly applied definition
of insomnia. While insomnia definitions have varied considerably in the clinical and clinical
research arenas, even greater variability in definition can be found in the epidemiological
research on insomnia. This is discussed below. Although psychological and behavioral aspects
of insomnia will likely remain important components in defining the disorder, current research
in the biology of insomnia holds the promise that more physiologically based definitions may
hold greater sway as we move forward.
EPIDEMIOLOGY
The earliest identified epidemiologic study of insomnia in the modern era of sleep research was
published by Bixler and colleagues in 1979 (18). This study surveyed around 1000 residents of
the Los Angeles area and found a prevalence of “insomnia” of 42.5%. The survey relied solely
on respondents endorsing “insomnia” as a symptom. Since that time, epidemiologic surveys
have become progressively more sophisticated, including criteria that assess symptom profile,
frequency, severity, consequences and other characteristics (e.g., “dissatisfaction with sleep”)
that are considered germane in the current understanding of the disorder. With this increasing
refinement of definition, prevalence data have diminished from the remarkably high percentage
originally described by Bixler.
Detailed discussions of the epidemiology of insomnia have been published elsewhere
(19,20). The influence of varying criteria on prevalence of insomnia has also been reviewed
at length (21,22). Ohayon categorizes the approaches into four major categories: (1) insomnia
as a symptom, with or without frequency and/or severity criteria; (2) insomnia with daytime
consequences; (3) dissatisfaction with quality of sleep; (4) application of formal insomnia diag-
nostic criteria such as DSM-IV or ICSD (Fig. 2). Broadly speaking, use of a symptom criterion
alone expectedly produces the highest prevalence—roughly in the 30% to 40% range. Addition
of severity and/or frequency criteria reduces this to a range of approximately 15% to 25%.
Requirement of daytime consequences produces prevalence rates in the 10% to 15% range.
Similar, if perhaps, slightly lower estimates are observed when “dissatisfaction with quality or
quantity of sleep” is applied. Formal diagnostic criteria yield the lowest estimates—in the 5%
to 6% range.
As discussed above in the Definition section, identifying those characteristics that accu-
rately define this condition can be elusive. Currently, the clear trend in epidemiologic research
is toward requirement of daytime consequences and application of formal diagnostic criteria.
A survey (22) of nearly 25,000 Europeans found that 16.8% of those sampled reported one or
more symptoms of insomnia (difficulty with sleep initiation or maintenance, or nonrestorative
sleep). Addition of a one-month duration criterion reduces this to 15.8%, while further require-
ment of associated daytime consequences yields 11.1% meeting all three criteria. Quite similar
results were reported for a Canadian population (15), beginning with an initial criterion of
dissatisfaction with sleep (17.8%). Addition of an insomnia symptom requirement (sleep ini-
tiation and/or maintenance disturbance) yielded a prevalence of 11.2%. Addition of duration
and daytime consequences criteria further reduces this to 4% to 5%. The complete criteria in
both of the studies cited above approximate DSM-IV/ICSD criteria for chronic insomnia. While
some variability exists, most recent studies have reported similar prevalence when comparable
criteria are employed.
Risk Factors
Numerous factors are associated with higher rates of chronic insomnia. In almost all surveys,
insomnia is reported to be more frequent in women than in men (23–31). Explanations for
this are not clear, although hypotheses range from reporting bias to endocrine differences to
higher prevalence of certain mental disorders (e.g., depression) in women. Likewise, older age
has been found to be highly associated with chronic insomnia (24,25,32). This is certainly not
surprising in light of the multiple factors that also covary with age, including medical illness,
pain, medication use, and increased prevalence of other sleep disorders. In fact, some data
suggest that it is largely these covariates, rather than age itself, that are responsible for the
increased prevalence observed in later life (33).
c01 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:11 Char Count=
6 SATEIA
Insomnia symptoms
-Presence: 30–48%
-At least 3 nights/week or often
or always: 16–21%
-Moderately to extremely: 10–28%
Insomnia symptoms +
daytime consequences
9–15%
Insomnia diagnosis
6%
Figure 2 Average prevalence of insomnia symptoms and diagnoses. Source: From Ref. 21.
genesis and maintenance of psychiatric disorders. This issue is addressed in greater detail in
Chapter 4.
Numerous medical comorbidities have likewise been associated with insomnia. In most
cases, the characteristics of the relationship have not been well-investigated. Chronic pain is a
common condition frequently associated with insomnia (16). A multinational study of almost
19,000 persons found that 40% of those with insomnia complaints reported at least one chronic
pain problem. In this population, chronic pain was associated with greater difficulty getting
back to sleep and shorter duration of sleep, as well as more prominent daytime consequences.
Other medical conditions frequently associated with insomnia include lung disease, cardiovas-
cular diseases and various neurological disorders, especially degenerative diseases and stroke
(39–42). As with mental disorders, the causal relationship between these conditions is undoubt-
edly complex and is yet to be fully elucidated. Some recent studies (43,44) have explored
potential predisposition to cardiovascular disease as a result of chronic insomnia, but results
are preliminary and a causal link between chronic insomnia and incident hypertension is yet to
be clearly established.
CONCLUSION
Chronic insomnia is a highly prevalent and unremitting condition characterized by complaints
of difficulty initiating or maintaining sleep or sleep that is not restorative. General agreement
exists that the definition must also include the presence of daytime symptoms attributable to
the insomnia. Women, older adults, and persons with psychiatric or medical comorbidities are
at increased risk for chronic insomnia. As discussed elsewhere, chronic insomnia may, in turn,
represent a risk for development of these comorbidities. The duration of chronic insomnia is
generally long, with many individuals experiencing fluctuating levels of sleep disturbance over
decades. Failure of patients to report the problem, and health care providers to identify and
treat it, contributes to persistence of the condition.
REFERENCES
1. Summers-Bremner E. Insomnia: A Cultural History. London: Reaktion Books, 2008.
2. Hippocrates. Aphorisms. eBooks@Adelaide. 2009. http://ebooks.adelaide.edu.au/h/hippocrates/
aphorisms/index.html
c01 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:11 Char Count=
8 SATEIA
3. Schiff P Jr. Opium and its alkaloids. Am J Pharm Educ 2002; 66:186–194.
4. The History of Insomnia. http://www.bbc.co.uk/dna/h2g2/A294031. Accessed 5, 2008.
5. Horne J. Insomnia—Victorian Style. Psychologist 2008; 21(10):910–911.
6. Anonymous. Sleeplessness. Br Med J 1894; 2(1761):719.
7. NIH State of the Science Conference statement on Manifestations and Management of Chronic Insom-
nia in Adults. J Clin Sleep Med 2005; 1(4):412–421.
8. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV).
Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association, 1994.
9. American Academy of Sleep Medicine. International classification of sleep disorders, 2nd ed. Diag-
nostic and coding manual. Westchester, IL: American Academy of Sleep Medicine, 2005.
10. Association of Sleep Disorders Centers and the Association for the Psychophysiological Study of Sleep.
Diagnostic classification of sleep and arousal disorders. 1979 first edition. Sleep 1979; 2(1):1–154.
11. American Sleep Disorders Association, International Classification of Sleep Disorders. ed. Diagnostic
Classification Steering Committee. 1990, Rochester MN.
12. Edinger JD, Bonnet MH, Bootzin RR, et al. Derivation of research diagnostic criteria for insomnia:
Report of an American Academy of Sleep Medicine Work Group. Sleep 2004; 27(8):1567–1596.
13. Carskadon M, Dement W, Mitler M, et al. Self report versus sleep laboratory findings in 122 drug-free
subjects with the complaint of chronic insomnia. Am J Psychiatry 1976; 133:1382–1388.
14. Frankel BL, Coursey RD, Buchbinder R, et al. Recorded and reported sleep in chronic primary insom-
nia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1976; 33(5):615–623.
15. Ohayon MM, Caulet M, Guilleminault C. How a general population perceives its sleep and how this
relates to the complaint of insomnia. Sleep 1997; 20(9):715–723.
16. Ohayon MM. Relationship between chronic painful physical condition and insomnia. J Psychiatr Res
2005; 39(2):151–159.
17. Stone KC, Taylor DJ, McCrae CS, et al. Nonrestorative sleep. Sleep Med Rev 2008; 12(4):275–288.
18. Bixler E, Kales A, Soldatos C, et al. Prevalence of sleep disorders in the Los Angeles metropolitan area.
Am J Psychiatry 1979; 136:1257–1262.
19. Lichstein KL, Riedel BW, Taylor DJ. Epidemiology of sleep: Age, gender, and ethnicity. Mahwah, New
Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 2004.
20. Partinen M, Hublin C. Epidemiology of sleep disorders. In Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement WC, eds.
Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine. Elsevier Saunders: Philadelphia, 2005:626–647.
21. Ohayon MM. Epidemiology of insomnia: What we know and what we still need to learn. Sleep Med
Rev 2002; 6(2):97–111.
22. Ohayon MM, Roth T. What are the contributing factors for insomnia in the general population? J
Psychosom Res 2001; 51(6):745–755.
23. Hajak G. Epidemiology of severe insomnia and its consequences in Germany. Eur Arch Psychiatry
Clin Neurosci 2001; 251(2):49–56.
24. Hohagen F, Rink K, Käppler C, et al. Prevalence and treatment of insomnia in general practice: A
longitudinal study. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1993; 242:329–336.
25. Mellinger G, Balter M, Uhlenhuth E. Insomnia and its treatment: Prevalence and correlates. Arch Gen
Psychiatry 1985; 42:25–232.
26. Weyerer S, Dilling H. Prevalence and treatment of insomnia in the community: Results from the Upper
Bavarian Field Study. Sleep 1991; 14:392–398.
27. Chevalier H, Los F, Boichut D, et al. Evaluation of severe insomnia in the general population: Results
of a European multinational survey. J Psychopharmacol 1999; 13(4 Suppl 1):S21–S24.
28. Hatoum HT, Kania CM, Kong SX, et al. Prevalence of insomnia: A survey of the enrollees at five
managed care organizations. Am J Manag Care 1998; 4(1):79–86.
29. Leger D, Guilleminault C, Dreyfus JP, et al. Prevalence of insomnia in a survey of 12,778 adults in
France. J Sleep Res 2000; 9(1):35–42.
30. Ohayon MM, Caulet M, Priest RG, et al. DSM-IV and ICSD-90 insomnia symptoms and sleep dissat-
isfaction. Br J Psychiatry 1997; 171:382–388.
31. Ohayon MM. Prevalence of DSM-IV diagnostic criteria of insomnia: Distinguishing insomnia related
to mental disorders from sleep disorders. J Psychiatr Res 1997; 31(3):333–346.
32. Ohayon M. Epidemiological study on insomnia in the general population. Sleep 1996; 19(3 Suppl):S7–
S15.
33. Ohayon MM, Zulley J, Guilleminault C, et al. How age and daytime activities are related to insomnia
in the general population: Consequences for older people. J Am Geriatr Soc 2001; 49(4):360–366.
34. Regenstein Q, Dambrosia J, Hallett M, et al. Daytime alertness in patients with primary insomnia. Am
J Psychiatry 1993; 150:1529–1534.
35. Gellis LA, Lichstein KL, Scarinci IC, et al. Socioeconomic status and insomnia. J Abnorm Psychol 2005;
114(1):111–118.
c01 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:11 Char Count=
36. Ford D, Kamerow D. Epidemiologic study of sleep disturbances and psychiatric disorders. JAMA
1989; 262:1479–1484.
37. Breslau N, Roth T, Rosenthal L, et al. Sleep disturbance and psychiatric disorders: A longitudinal
epidemiological study of young adults. Biol Psychiatry 1996; 39(6):411–418.
38. Weissman MM, Greenwald S, Nino-Murcia G, et al. The morbidity of insomnia uncomplicated by
psychiatric disorders. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 1997; 19(4):245–250.
39. Gislason T, Reymisdötter H, Kritbjarnarson H, et al. Sleep habits and sleep disturbances among the
elderly—An epidemiological survey. J Intern Med 1993; 234:31–39.
40. Klink M, Quan S. Prevalence of reported sleep disturbances in general adult population and their
relationship to obstructive airway disease. Chest 1987; 91:540–546.
41. Klink M, Quan S, Kaltenborn W, et al. Risk factors associated with complaints of insomnia in a general
adult population. Arch Intern Med 1992; 152:1634–1637.
42. Elwood P, Hack M, Pickering J, et al. Sleep disturbance, stroke, and heart disease events: Evidence
from the Caerphilly cohort. J Epidemiol Community Health 2006; 60(1):69–73.
43. Phillips B, Buzkova P, Enright P. Insomnia did not predict incident hypertension in older adults in the
cardiovascular health study. Sleep 2009; 32(1):65–72.
44. Lanfranchi PA, Pennestri MH, Fradette L, et al. Nighttime blood pressure in normotensive subjects
with chronic insomnia: Implications for cardiovascular risk. Sleep 2009; 32(6):760–766.
45. Foley DJ, Monjan A, Simonsick EM, et al. Incidence and remission of insomnia among elderly adults:
An epidemiologic study of 6,800 persons over three years. Sleep 1999; 22(Suppl 2):S366–S372.
46. Morgan K, Clarke D. Longitudinal trends in late-life insomnia: Implications for prescribing. Age
Ageing 1997; 26(3):179–184.
47. Morin CM, Belanger L, LeBlanc M, et al. The natural history of insomnia: A population-based 3-year
longitudinal study. Arch Intern Med 2009; 169(5):447–453.
48. Buysse DJ, Angst J, Gamma A, et al. Prevalence, course, and comorbidity of insomnia and depression
in young adults. Sleep 2008; 31(4):473–480.
c02 IHBK059-Sateia March 17, 2010 7:37 Char Count=
The International Classification of Sleep Disorders Second Edition (ICSD-2) (1) requires both
poor sleep and daytime functional compromise for the diagnosis of insomnia. Patients report
numerous consequences of their insomnia, and the ICSD-2 lists the following as diagnostically
sufficient examples:
Fatigue or malaise
Poor attention or concentration
Social or vocational dysfunction
Mood disturbance
Daytime sleepiness
Reduced motivation or energy
Increased errors or accidents
Tension, headache or gastrointestinal symptoms
Continuing worry about sleep
Insomnia produces numerous medical, psychological, and economic deficits in addition to
changes in subjective state and performance, but only the latter two factors, specifically related
to the diagnostic criteria required for a diagnosis of insomnia, will be considered in this chapter.
A section reviewing subjective deficits will be followed by a section examining objective deficits.
Mood
Dysphoria is commonly reported by patients with insomnia. A number of studies have assessed
various mood components in these patients.
Sleepiness/Fatigue
The most commonly reported mood dimensions have been subjective fatigue and sleepi-
ness. In a review of studies prior to 2000 (3), it was reported that 7 of 12 studies using the
Stanford Sleepiness Scale in insomnia patients found significantly greater sleepiness in patients
as compared to controls. However, as the evaluation of insomnia has evolved, investigators
began to differentiate frank sleepiness (by asking patients if they would actually fall asleep in
a given circumstance using a questionnaire such as the Epworth Sleepiness Scale—ESS) from
fatigue (feeling tired or without energy but not likely to fall asleep). In recent years, the Stanford
c02 IHBK059-Sateia March 17, 2010 7:37 Char Count=
Sleepiness Scale has been used less frequently in insomnia patients, and a number of more
fatigue-related mood scales and measures have appeared (4). For example, in a study that
compared sleepiness measured by the ESS and fatigue measured by the Tiredness Symptoms
Scale (TSS), insomnia patients had sleepiness levels on the ESS that were nonsignificantly lower
than controls and significantly lower than sleep apnea patients whereas their TSS scores were
nonsignificantly higher than sleep apnea patients and significantly higher than controls (5).
These findings of lower sleepiness despite increased fatigue are consistent with objective sleepi-
ness data from the Multiple Sleep Latency Test. A recent review found that none of 12 studies
comparing objective daytime sleepiness in insomnia patients with controls found significantly
greater objective sleepiness in the insomnia patients (6). These findings suggest that sleepiness
and fatigue can be diagnostically differentiated if patients are given sufficient descriptive lati-
tude and that insomnia patients view their problem as more of a fatigue problem rather than
excessive sleepiness.
Fatigue has been measured from general mood scales such as the Profile of Mood States
(POMS), which has a specific fatigue scale, in addition to more recently developed specific scales
such as the Fatigue Severity Scale (FSS). Significant increases in fatigue have been reported in
insomnia patients in four (3,7–9) of seven studies (10–12).
Quality of Life
There are several means of assessing quality of life (QOL) in patients. Some studies have used
one or two specific questions whereas others have used QOL scales such as the Quality of
Life in Insomnia Scale (4). One QOL scale that has been widely used in diverse patient pop-
ulations is the Medical Outcomes Study Short Form (SF-36) (33). The SF-36 assesses physical
functioning, limitations due to physical health problems, bodily pain, general health percep-
tion, vitality, social functioning, limitations due to emotional health problems, and mental
health. Patients with insomnia reported decreased QOL compared with normal controls on all
dimensions of the SF-36 (P < 0.0001) (34,35) and SF-12 (36). One study (37) compared SF-36
results in a group of mild and severe insomnia patients with groups of patients diagnosed
with depression or congestive heart failure (CHF). Severe insomnia patients had numerically
greater loss of function than patients with CHF on all of the SF-36 scales except physical func-
tioning. Insomnia patients also reported more physical problems (the first four scales of the
SF-36) than patients with depression (37). Such findings suggest that the subjective dysphoria
and loss of function associated with insomnia is similar to that seen in other significant chronic
illness.
Research studies have also begun to examine changes in QOL with insomnia treatment.
One study found significantly improved ability to function during the day and physical well-
being throughout six months in patients receiving eszopiclone 3 mg compared with placebo in a
double-blind design (15). A study of eszopiclone 1 and 2 mg in elderly insomnia patients found
improved QOL on some subscales at the 2 mg but not the 1 mg dose over two weeks of medica-
tion administration (13). Another study (38) tracked the 3-item subscale of the Insomnia Severity
Index that deals with QOL and functioning in patients receiving indiplon 10 mg, indiplon 20 mg,
or placebo in a double-blind study and found that patients receiving either dose of medication
were significantly improved compared to the placebo group during each of the three months of
the study. The percentage of patients reporting minimal to no daytime impairment was signif-
icantly higher in the active treatment group (73%) versus the placebo group (60%). At the end
of the study approximately 2/3 of the patients in the medication treatment conditions scored
within the normal range on the complete Insomnia Severity Index compared with 38% in the
placebo group, also a significant difference (38). Finally, in a study that evaluated eszopiclone
3 mg or matching placebo under double-blind conditions for six months (39), the complete
SF-36 was administered at baseline and after one, three, and six months. At baseline, patients
had significantly lower values on the SF-36 Vitality, Social Functioning, and Mental Health
scales compared with healthy U.S. population norms. After six months of treatment, patients
taking eszopiclone had significantly improved on these three scales in comparison with the
insomnia patients receiving placebo and were at or above the U.S. population norms. QOL has
also been assessed in a behavioral treatment paradigm (40). In this study, patients with chronic
insomnia who had been treated with pharmacotherapy for 10 years on average were given
cognitive-behavioral therapy or no additional therapy (patients were allowed to either continue
or be withdrawn from their current sleeping medications). At intake, patients were found to
have significantly worse QOL compared with population norms, and this was significant for all
scales on the SF-36 for younger subjects (age 30–49). However, the oldest subjects (age 70–100)
were much closer to population norms and were significantly worse than those norms only
on physical functioning, mental health, vitality, and bodily pain. By the end of the six-month
trial, patients across all ages in the cognitive-behavioral therapy group had significantly better
scores than the control population on physical functioning, mental health, and limitations due
to emotional health problems. Unfortunately, these improvements occurred primarily because
QOL deteriorated in control patients rather than truly improving in the patients with behavioral
therapy.
The studies of QOL are consistent in showing that decrements in insomnia patients can be
reversed with return to normal levels after treatment for as long as six months (the duration of
study trials). There has also been improvement in parallel placebo groups, but improvement in
medication treatment groups has been significantly greater than that seen in the placebo groups.
Improved QOL has implications for decreased health care utilization and improved work per-
formance. One study has actually used QOL improvement as a basis for a determination of cost
effectiveness for long-term treatment of primary insomnia (41).
c02 IHBK059-Sateia March 17, 2010 7:37 Char Count=
Work Performance
Another means of assessing QOL is to assess job performance or career success in patients
compared to controls. One study found that self-reported poor sleepers in the Navy received
significantly fewer promotions and were less likely to be recommended for reenlistment (42).
Questionnaire studies have shown that subjectively identified patients with insomnia felt more
fatigued and irritated with their children and had more health care consequences on a number of
dimensions (43). Insomnia patients also reported consequences at work including significantly
more errors, significantly more accidents, more absenteeism, and poor efficiency (43,44). How-
ever, a recent study that attempted to replicate the finding of increased absenteeism in insomnia
patients found, in a logistic regression model, that absenteeism based on company sick leave
records was determined only by sex, profession, and depression (45). To some extent these
results may reflect a limitation of questionnaire studies of sleep problems (in which patients
with sleep apnea, other sleep pathology, or untreated depression may also be included in insom-
nia groups). Moreover, subjective responses to queries about accidents and sick leave further
confound results.
Fortunately, careful studies have begun to more clearly identify patients with primary
insomnia. One recent study used the Work Limitations Questionnaire (46) to assess time
demands, physical demands, mental demands, output demands, and work productivity loss
in primary insomnia patients given eszopiclone 3 mg or placebo in a double-blind design for
six months. Patients receiving medication were significantly improved compared with placebo
on all scales for the one to six month average score. Scores at the end of the study were in the
range of normal scores on all of the scales except time demands, but there was large variability.
In another study using the same questionnaire (47), patients given zolpidem extended release
12.5 mg versus placebo in a double-blind study for 12 weeks showed significant improvement
on the time demands and output demands scales at the end of the study. The magnitude of
improvement on these scales was similar in both studies.
Memory
Numerous memory tasks have been used to document differences between insomnia patients
and controls. Common short-term memory tasks that have been used include the Digit Symbol
Substitution Task (NS in seven of seven studies), digit span (NS in three of five studies), the
MAST (NS in six of seven reports from two studies), and simple short-term memory (NS in six of
nine studies). The nine studies reporting number of words recalled in an immediate memory task
were reviewed, and data from the four of those that included means plus standard deviations
in comparable patient and control groups were combined to calculate an effect size (58). From
those studies (7–9,53), mean words recalled from normals and insomnia patients were 10.8
and 8.4 words respectively (combined standard deviation of 2.69 with 105 and 185 Ss) giving a
combined t-value of t288 = 7.51 (P < 0.01) and effect size (ES) of 0.91. However, ESs for other types
of memory measures were lower (in the range of 0.28). In studies with long-term recall, insomnia
subjects performed significantly worse than controls in only one of four studies. More recently,
primary insomnia was associated with impairment of procedural memory in comparison to
controls (52). The number of studies showing significant results for various memory variables
c02 IHBK059-Sateia March 17, 2010 7:37 Char Count=
is low, but the combination of studies with largely nonsignificant results to increase the sample
size produced a significant effect that also had a large ES. Therefore, examination of memory
effects in larger studies is indicated.
Balance
Two studies that have examined balance in insomnia patients and controls have both reported
statistically significant decreases in balance in insomnia patients (10,12) compared with controls,
and ES from the one study with data sufficient to calculate the statistic was 3.1 (12). One
implication of these results is a recent finding that insomnia, in elderly nursing home residents,
is associated with a greater risk of falls, and this risk is not based on the use of sleeping
medication (59). These data also imply that further examination of balance could be important.
Math
Computational ability has been infrequently examined in insomnia patients. None of the three
studies that have examined ability to perform additions have found significant decrements in
insomnia patients compared with controls.
Vigilance
True vigilance tasks and simple and choice reaction time tasks were included with vigilance
tasks. Two significant vigilance results were found from six studies; two significant 4-choice
reaction time results were found from four studies, and one significant simple reaction time
result was found from eight studies. Of studies reporting mean and standard deviation for
simple reaction time (10,11), reaction times from normals and insomnia patients were 290 and
364 msec, respectively (combined standard deviation of 256 msec with 56 Ss in both groups),
giving a small ES of 0.288. One study that reported nonsignificant differences for simple reaction
time in large groups (60) did report significantly slower response latencies for insomnia patients
compared with controls for responses to more complex decisions.
Hand/Eye Coordination
Hand/eye coordination tasks included tremor, card sorting, Purdue pegboard, tapping, line
tracing, trail making, and visual resolution. Of 13 total studies in this area, only one statistically
significant result, for the single study of line tracing (17), was found.
Reasoning
Four studies reported logical reasoning or proofreading in insomnia patients versus controls
but no statistically different results were reported.
Sleepiness/Alertness
Objective sleepiness/alertness is commonly measured with the Multiple Sleep Latency Test
(MSLT) or Maintenance of Wakefulness Test (MWT). A number of studies have examined the
ability of insomnia patients to fall asleep using the MSLT. These studies are of particular interest
because, if patients suffer mainly from reduced sleep at night, their daytime nap latencies
should be significantly reduced (61). However, if patients primarily suffer from physiological
hyperarousal, their daytime nap latencies should be increased. Twelve studies were found where
daytime nap latency was compared between insomnia patients and study controls (7,9,17–
19,26,49–51,62–64). Sleep latency was not significantly reduced in insomnia patients as compared
to controls in any study. Sleep latency was significantly increased in insomnia patients as
compared to controls in 6 of the 12 studies. Of the 12 studies, 7 used a relatively standard
protocol and reported both means and standard deviations for the groups. When the data for
these seven studies were combined, the average sleep latency for insomnia patients (N = 192)
was 13.5 (±4.9) minutes and for controls (N = 167) was 12.1 (±5.3) minutes. This difference was
statistically significant (t357 = 2.586, P = 0.01). The ES, based on these group data, was 0.27. The
t-value and ES value are both somewhat lower than in studies for which subjects were required
to meet both a subjective and objective sleep criterion to be included (6). In general, these data
are consistent with subjective data that show insomnia patients report increases in subjective
sleepiness and more specifically, fatigue, without objective sleepiness.
c02 IHBK059-Sateia March 17, 2010 7:37 Char Count=
SUMMARY
Extensive psychomotor performance and sleepiness data recorded from insomnia patients over
the past 25 years have shown that these patients have marginal decreases in performance. Of
functions assessed, decrements in balance accompanied by increased falls in elderly insomnia
patients are the most persuasive. Several individual studies and the pooled analysis of short-
term memory data do suggest the likelihood of some memory deficits in patients. Unfortunately,
objective performance indicators have only been reported in treatment studies in the negative
sense (absence of decrements shortly after awakening rather than improved performance during
the day). The large number of nonsignificant results along with the finding that insomnia
patients are significantly less sleepy than controls on the MSLT suggests that insomnia is not
the same as sleep deprivation. Since many of the tests used to compare insomnia patients with
controls have been selected secondary to their sensitivity to sleep deprivation, it is possible
that the psychomotor tests employed have not been the optimal measures for assessment of
impairment and that different or more specific tests might provide additional information. In
addition, the results from memory tests suggest that future studies of performance in insomnia
patients can benefit from larger sample sizes to more easily demonstrate deficits.
It is also the case that the picture of performance in patients with the insomnia is much
different when one examines objective performance measures than when one examines subjec-
tive measures such as mood or QOL. It will be important to reconcile the differences between
data sets that should reasonably be expected to correlate with one another. One means of doing
this is to compare objective and subjective results from within the same study. For example,
one study (8) both asked patients to subjectively rate their performance ability and employed
several objective performance measures. As expected, insomnia patients subjectively rated their
performance compared with their real capacity as significantly worse than did control subjects
with a large effect size (ES = 1.20). However, of eight objective performance measures including
memory tests, motor tests, reaction time, and executive function, insomnia patients performed
significantly worse than normals on only one of the four memory tests (digit span), and the
median ES for the performance tests was low (around 0.4). In the same study, insomnia patients
had reported a significantly longer subjective sleep latency compared to the controls (ES = 1.32)
while their objective PSG sleep latency was actually nonsignificantly shorter than the controls
(ES = −0.05). Similarly, the insomnia patients reported subjectively that their total sleep time
was significantly shorter than the controls (ES = 2.15) while their actual total sleep time was not
significantly different from the controls (ES = 0.4). These objective and subjective sleep data are
very consistent with the objective and subjective performance data reported above and suggest
that the same mechanism that results in patients subjectively reporting much worse sleep than is
corroborated objectively may also operate similarly when subjective performance is compared
with objective psychomotor performance measures.
patients with longer MSLT values (higher daytime alertness) also had longer subjective sleep
latency at night and higher anxiety scores on the POMS. Such findings link dysphoria and
poor subjective sleep with hyperarousal rather than sleepiness and suggest that performance
deficits in insomnia patients will be more apparent on psychomotor performance tasks that are
sensitive to central activation rather than sleepiness. Because many of the tests used in insomnia
research have evolved from sleep deprivation research, they may simply be inappropriate or
irrelevant for use in insomnia patients. The development of more appropriate and sensitive
tests for insomnia-related impairment may allow us to better define the relationship between
objective and subjective consequences of the condition.
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
This study was supported by the Dayton Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Wright
State University School of Medicine, and the Sleep-Wake Disorders Research Institute.
REFERENCES
1. American Academy of Sleep Medicine. International Classification of Sleep Disorders: Diagnostic
and Coding Manual. 2nd ed. Westchester, IL: American Academy of Sleep Medicine, 2005.
2. Krystal AD. Treating the health, quality of life, and functional impairments in insomnia. J Clin Sleep
Med 2007; 3(1):63–72.
3. Riedel B, Lichstein K. Insomnia and daytime functioning. Sleep Med Rev 2000; 4:277–298.
4. Buysse DJ, Ancoli-Israel S, Edinger JD, et al. Recommendations for a standard research assessment
of insomnia. Sleep 2006; 29(9):1155–1173.
5. Schneider C, Fulda S, Schulz H. Daytime variation in performance and tiredness/sleepiness ratings in
patients with insomnia, narcolepsy, sleep apnea and normal controls. J Sleep Res 2004; 13(4):373–383.
6. Bonnet MH, Arand DL. Consequences of insomnia. Sleep Med Clin 2006; 1:351–358.
7. Rosa R, Bonnet M. Reported chronic insomnia is independent of poor sleep as measured by elec-
troencephalography. Psychosom Med 2000; 62:474–482.
8. Vignola A, Lamoureux C, Bastien CH, et al. Effects of chronic insomnia and use of benzodiazepines
on daytime performance in older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2000; 55(1):P54–P62.
9. Pedrosi B, Roehrs T, Rosenthal L, et al. Daytime function and benzodiazepine effects in insomniacs
compared to normals. Sleep Res 1995; 24:48.
10. Hauri PJ. Cognitive deficits in insomnia patients. Acta Neurol Belg 1997; 97(2):113–117.
11. Crenshaw MC, Edinger JD. Slow-wave sleep and waking cognitive performance among older adults
with and without insomnia complaints. Physiol Behav 1999; 66(3):485–492.
12. Mendelson WB, Garnett D, Linnoila M. Do insomniacs have impaired daytime functioning? Biol
Psychiatry 1984; 19(8):1261–1264.
13. Scharf M, Erman M, Rosenberg R, et al. A 2-week efficacy and safety study of eszopiclone in elderly
patients with primary insomnia. Sleep 2005; 28(6):720–727.
14. Soares CN, Joffe H, Rubens R, et al. Eszopiclone in patients with insomnia during perimenopause
and early postmenopause: a randomized controlled trial. Obstet Gynecol 2006; 108(6):1402–1410.
15. Krystal AD, Walsh JK, Laska E, et al. Sustained efficacy of eszopiclone over 6 months of nightly
treatment: results of a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study in adults with chronic
insomnia. Sleep 2003; 26(7):793–799.
16. Krystal AD, Erman M, Zammit GK, et al. Long-term efficacy and safety of zolpidem extended-release
12.5 mg, administered 3 to 7 nights per week for 24 weeks, in patients with chronic primary insomnia:
a 6-month, randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel-group, multicenter study. Sleep
2008; 31(1):79–90.
17. Schneider-Helmert D. Twenty-four-hour sleep-wake function and personality patterns in chronic
insomniacs and healthy controls. Sleep 1987; 10(5):452–462.
18. Mendelson WB, Garnett D, Gillin CG, et al. The experience of insomnia and daytime and nighttime
functioning. Psychiatry Res 1984; 12:235–250.
19. Bonnet MH, Arand DL. 24-Hour metabolic rate in insomniacs and matched normal sleepers. Sleep
1995; 18:581–588.
20. Beutler L. Psychological variables in the diagnosis of insomnia. In: Williams RL, Karacan I, eds. Sleep
Disorders: Diagnosis and Treatment. New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1978:61–100.
21. Coursey RD, Buchsbaum M, Frankel BL. Personality measures and evoked responses in chronic
insomniacs. J Abnorm Psychol 1975; 84:239–249.
22. Szelenberger W, Niemcewicz S. Severity of insomnia correlates with cognitive impairment. Acta
Neurobiol Exp (Wars) 2000; 60:373.
c02 IHBK059-Sateia March 17, 2010 7:37 Char Count=
23. Kales A, Caldwell AB, Soldatos CR, et al. Biopsychobehavioral correlates of insomnia. II. Pattern
specificity and consistency with the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory. Psychosom Med
1983; 45(4):341–355.
24. Kalogjera-Sackellares D, Cartwright RD. Comparison of MMPI profiles in medically and psycholog-
ically based insomnias. Psychiatry Res 1997; 70:49–56.
25. Bliwise NG, Bliwise DL, Dement WC. Age and psychopathology in insomnia. Clin Gerontol
1985/1986; 4:3–9.
26. Seidel WF, Ball S, Cohen S, et al. Daytime alertness in relation to mood, performance, and nocturnal
sleep in chronic insomniacs and noncomplaining sleepers. Sleep 1984; 7(3):230–238.
27. Hauri P, Fisher J. Persistent psychophysiologic (learned) insomnia. Sleep 1986; 9(1):38–53.
28. Bonnet MH. Recovery of performance during sleep following sleep deprivation in older normal and
insomniac adult males. Percept Mot Skills 1985; 60(1):323–334.
29. Freedman RR, Sattler HL. Physiological and psychological factors in sleep-onset insomnia. J Abnorm
Psychol 1982; 91:380–389.
30. Chambers M, Keller B. Alert insomniacs: are they really sleep deprived? Clin Psychol Rev 1993;
13:649–666.
31. Morin CM, Gramling SE. Sleep patterns and aging: comparison of older adults with and without
insomnia complaints. Psychol Aging 1989; 4(3):290–294.
32. Fava M, McCall WV, Krystal A, et al. Eszopiclone co-administered with fluoxetine in patients with
insomnia coexisting with major depressive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2006; 59:1052–1060.
33. McHorney CA, Ware JE Jr., Lu JF, et al. The MOS 36-item Short-Form Health Survey (SF-36): III. Tests
of data quality, scaling assumptions, and reliability across diverse patient groups. Med Care 1994;
32:40–66.
34. Zammit GK, Weiner J, Damato N, et al. Quality of life in people with insomnia. Sleep 1999; 22
(suppl):S379–S385.
35. Leger D, Scheuermaier K, Philip P, et al. SF-36: evaluation of quality of life in severe and mild
insomniacs compared with good sleepers. Psychosom Med 2001; 63(1):49–55.
36. LeBlanc M, Beaulieu-Bonneau S, Merette C, et al. Psychological and health-related quality of life
factors associated with insomnia in a population-based sample. J Psychosom Res 2007; 63(2):157–
166.
37. Katz D, McHorney C. The relationship between insomnia and health-related quality of life in patients
with chronic illness. J Fam Pract 2002; 51:229–235.
38. Scharf MB, Black J, Hull S, et al. Long-term nightly treatment with indiplon in adults with pri-
mary insomnia: results of a double-blind, placebo-controlled, 3-month study. Sleep 2007; 30(6):
743–752.
39. Walsh JK, Krystal AD, Amato DA, et al. Nightly treatment of primary insomnia with eszopiclone for
six months: effect on sleep, quality of life, and work limitations. Sleep 2007; 30:959–968.
40. Dixon S, Morgan K, Mathers N, et al. Impact of cognitive behavioral therapy on health-related quality
of life among adult hypnotic users with chronic insomnia. Behav Sleep Med 2006; 4:71–84.
41. Botteman MF, Ozminkowski RJ, Wang S, et al. Cost effectiveness of long-term treatment with eszopi-
clone for primary insomnia in adults: a decision analytical model. CNS Drugs 2007; 21(4):319–334.
42. Johnson LJ, Spinweber CL. Good and poor sleepers differ in Navy performance. Mil Med 1983;
148:727–731.
43. Leger D, Guilleminault C, Bader G, et al. Medical and socio-professional impact of insomnia. Sleep
2002; 25:625–629.
44. Leger D, Massuel MA, Metlaine A. Professional correlates of insomnia. Sleep 2006; 29(2):171–178.
45. Philip P, Leger D, Taillard J, et al. Insomniac complaints interfere with quality of life but not with
absenteeism: respective role of depressive and organic comorbidity. Sleep Med 2006; 7(7):585–591.
46. Lerner D, Amick BC, Rogers WH, et al. The Work Limitations Questionnaire. Med Care 2001; 39:72–85.
47. Erman M. Ambien cr work performance. In: US Psychiatric and Mental Health Congress; 2008;
Orlando.
48. Church MW, Johnson LC. Mood and performance of poor sleepers during repeated use of flurazepam.
Psychopharmacology 1979; 61:309–316.
49. Edinger JD, Fins AI, Sullivan RJ, et al. Do our methods lead to insomniacs’ madness? Daytime testing
after laboratory and home-based polysomnographic studies. Sleep 1997; 20(12):1127–1134.
50. Sugerman JL, Stern JA, Walsh JK. Daytime alertness in subjective and objective insomnia: some
preliminary findings. Biol Psychiatry 1985; 20(7):741–750.
51. Stepanski E, Zorick F, Roehrs T, et al. Daytime alertness in patients with chronic insomnia compared
with asymptomatic control subjects. Sleep 1988; 11(1):54–60.
52. Nissen C, Kloepfer C, Nofzinger EA, et al. Impaired sleep-related memory consolidation in primary
insomnia—a pilot study. Sleep 2006; 29(8):1068–1073.
c02 IHBK059-Sateia March 17, 2010 7:37 Char Count=
53. Orff HJ, Drummond SP, Nowakowski S, et al. Discrepancy between subjective symptomatology and
objective neuropsychological performance in insomnia. Sleep 2007; 30(9):1205–1211.
54. Raymann RJ, Van Someren EJ. Time-on-task impairment of psychomotor vigilance is affected by mild
skin warming and changes with aging and insomnia. Sleep 2007; 30(1):96–103.
55. Varkevisser MG, Kerkhof GA. Chronic insomnia and performance in a 24-h constant routine study. J
Sleep Res 2005; 14:49–59.
56. MacMahon K, Broomfield N, Espie C. Attention bias for sleep-related stimuli in primary insomnia
and delayed sleep phase syndrome using the dot-probe task. Sleep 2006; 29:1420–1427.
57. Semler C, Harvey A. Daytime functioning in primary insomnia: does attentional focus contribute to
real or perceived impairment? Behav Sleep Med 2006; 4:85–103.
58. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates, 1988.
59. Avidan AY, Fries BE, James ML, et al. Insomnia and hypnotic use, recorded in the minimum data
set, as predictors of falls and hip fractures in Michigan nursing homes. J Am Geriatr Soc 2005;
53(6):955–962.
60. Edinger JD, Means MK, Carney CE, et al. Psychomotor performance deficits and their relation to
prior nights’ sleep among individuals with primary insomnia. Sleep 2008; 31:599–607.
61. Bonnet MH, Arand DL. The consequences of a week of insomnia. Sleep 1996; 19:453–461.
62. Lichstein KL, Wilson NM, Noe SL, et al. Daytime sleepiness in insomnia: behavioral, biological and
subjective indices. Sleep 1994; 17(8):693–702.
63. Stepanski E, Lamphere J, Badia P, et al. Sleep fragmentation and daytime sleepiness. Sleep 1984;
7(1):18–26.
64. Haynes SN, Fitzgerald SG, Shute GE, et al. The utility and validity of daytime naps in the assessment
of sleep-onset insomnia. J Behav Med 1985; 8(3):237–247.
65. Bonnet MH, Rosa RR. Predictors of objective sleepiness in insomniacs and normal sleepers. Sleep
2001; 24(suppl):A79–A80.
IHBK059-03 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:19 Char Count=
INTRODUCTION
Insomnia is now widely recognized as one of the major complaints associated with numerous
psychological and physical diseases in the general population and in primary care patients
around the world (1–4). Despite insomnia’s high prevalence, it is still frequently unrecognized
as a serious health threat by health professionals. One challenge is the fact that insomnia
is frequently considered as a symptom, rather than as a true disease, and it is not clear to
practitioners whether it is a symptom or a disease. Another challenge is that it is often difficult
for patients and for health professionals to understand when insomnia is severe enough to
require a treatment. In addition, there is still insufficient knowledge about the management of
insomnia. In the last decade, several consensus meetings about insomnia and its recognition,
diagnosis, and treatment have published recommendations (5–10). All these consensus groups
have underlined the effect of insomnia on public health and the need to better encompass the
consequences of insomnia on work, economics, and health-related quality of life (QOL).
The aim of this chapter was to carefully describe the possible links between insomnia and
public health concerns and to point out what are the certitudes and the missing data on the
consequences of insomnia on work, economics, and health-related QOL.
20 LEGER
on responses to questionnaires to explain these differences (14). Soldatos et al. (2), in a survey
that included 35,327 questionnaires and subjects from 10 countries, found that 31.6% of subjects
had “insomnia” while another 17.5% could be considered as having “subthreshold insomnia.”
More recently, Leger et al. (3) in a survey comparing sleep disorders among representative
samples of 3962 North Americans, 5005 Europeans, and 1165 Japanese found that insomnia was
significantly higher in the United States (39%) than in Europe (28%) and Japan (21%).
about sleep, medication use, sleepiness and fatigue, and medical help-seeking (23). Aikens
and Rouse conversely found that a high percentage (52%) of patients with probable insomnia
reported discussing this with a physician. Multivariate logistic regression analyses indicated
that discussing one’s probable insomnia with a physician was independently associated with
having a greater number of medical conditions [OR: 2.19 (95% CI: 1.13–4.22)], being more highly
educated [OR: 1.67 (95% CI: 1.11–2.51)], sleeping less per night [OR: 0.71 (95% CI: 0.52–0.96)],
and greater perceived daytime impairment due to insomnia [OR: 2.07 (95% CI: 1.06–4.03)].
Pires et al. (24) compared two studies carried out in Brazil in 1987 and 1995. They showed
that only 12.5% of the Brazilian insomniacs had sought medical help for their sleep problems
or had informed their physician of sleep problems during evaluation of other problems in
1987; this was even less (10.8%) in 1995. In a study carried out in the United States at five
managed care organizations, Hatoum et al. (25) found that only 0.9% of American patients
were seeing physicians due to sleep problems. Of these, only 11.6% were taking prescription
medications specifically for sleep problems and 21.4% were taking over-the-counter medications
for sleep. Moreover, the diagnosis of insomnia is not always followed by treatment. In Germany,
a Nationwide Insomnia Screening and Awareness Study (NISAS-2000) found that close to 50%
of all patients with insomnia did not receive a prescription for a specific insomnia therapy (26).
Absenteeism
In economic and epidemiological studies, overall measures of the respondent’s health appeared
to be the most important covariate of absenteeism (36,37). In a large, cross- sectional, national
probability sample of 1308 workers in the United States, Leigh (37) demonstrated that complain-
ing of insomnia was the most predictable factor of absenteeism from among 36 variables. In a
study comparing 80 insomniacs at work to 135 good sleepers, it was found that insomniacs had
double the control rate of absenteeism (21). Lavie (38) also found a higher rate of absenteeism
in insomniacs, which is significantly linked to a higher rate of work accidents in insomniacs. He
hypothesized that coworkers of the absentee insomniacs are more exposed to accidents because
of their work overload.
IHBK059-03 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:19 Char Count=
22 LEGER
However, these preliminary studies were based on general population samples: insomnia
was not always clearly defined and the groups of insomniacs were heterogeneous. Moreover,
the absenteeism data were mainly based on the patients’ declaration and not on objective data.
In a recent study (39), we specifically surveyed the absenteeism of a group of insomni-
acs at work compared to a matched group of good sleepers. Insomniacs showed almost twice
as much absenteeism as good sleepers. The difference between insomniacs and good sleepers
was particularly high for managers (OR = 2.29) and women (OR = 2.31). We believe that this
study is of particular interest because (i) we processed objective (rather than subjective) data
on absenteeism, (ii) insomnia was defined according to international classifications, (iii) sub-
jects with depression and anxiety were excluded, (iv) subjects were all full-time workers and
representative of the active population in the area, and (v) subjects with chronic disease (which
may interfere with sleep) and pregnant women were excluded from the study. Hence, in the
group studied here, it seems more probable that significant differences between insomniacs and
good sleepers reflect the impact of insomnia itself, rather than the effects of comorbidities. In
another study also assessing long-term absenteeism (including absence more than six months)
and comparing insomniac subjects (n = 986) with control subjects (n = 584), subjects with insom-
nia complaints (with or without mood or behavioral/physiologic sleep complaints) reported
poorer QOL and had a higher absenteeism rate than did controls (9.6 ± 31 vs. 5.8 ± 19 days;
p < 0.01). Logistic regression analysis of insomniacs with depressive complaints indicated that
absenteeism was more significantly associated with depression than insomnia itself (40).
Accidents
The impact of sleep disorders on automobile accidents is a crucial issue from a public health
point of view. Public authorities and the media are well-informed on the risk of sleepiness at
the wheel during the night and on the effects of sleep debt and sleep pathologies (sleep apnea,
hypersomnia) on accidents. However, there are very few data on the risk of accidents due to
insomnia by itself.
Insomnia may impact the risk of accident in different ways: sleep deprivation, lack of
attention, and side effects of hypnotics. Motor vehicle accidents (MVA) and work accidents
(WA) have been the primary areas of investigation.
IHBK059-03 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:19 Char Count=
In the French study comparing 240 SI with 391 GS (21), WA were found to occur eight
times more commonly over the past 12 months in SI (8%) than in GS (1%) (p = 0.0150), with an
average number of 0.07 (±0.25) accidents per SI versus 0.01 ± 0.11 per GS (p = 0.0550). There
was, however, no statistical difference for MVA over the past 12 months between the groups
(9% vs. 10%). The authors explained the discrepancy between WA and MVA by the fact that SI
may have avoided driving or driven shorter distance: 65.8% of SI versus 72.5% of GS drove a
car (p = 0.012). Lavie also showed a higher rate of WA in insomniacs (in their lifetime) than in
GS (52.1% vs. 35.6%; p < 0.01) (38). The rate of MVA due to fatigue (5% vs. 2%; p = NS) was
found to be slightly, but not significantly, increased in insomniacs.
Similarly, Daley et al., in a group of 930 adults in Quebec, did not find a different rate of
MVA in the last six months between insomniacs and GS. However, 23.5% of drivers reporting
an accident felt that insomnia played an important role in this event (42). Moreover, 39.5%
of participants described a link between their sleep difficulties and other types of accidents
(p < 0.001).
In Japan, in a study collecting data on occupational injuries in 1298 workers of small-
scale manufacturing firms, Nakata et al. (43) found that insomnia symptoms were significantly
associated with occupational injuries in both genders [OR = 1.64 (95%CI: 1.23–2.18)].
Regarding the effects of treatments on driving ability, it is usually generally held that
long-term half-life hypnotics (medium to long-term benzodiazepines and antihistamines) may
induce a risk of accidents, while driving, in the morning and a risk of falls in the night in
older adults. In Europe, the vast majority of hypnotics are labeled with a sign indicating
the possible risk of accidents due to the treatment. There are, however, very few published
studies on the effects of common hypnotics on driving ability. Partinen et al. (44) recently per-
formed a double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled, three-treatment three-period crossover
study investigating the effects of zolpidem (10 mg) and temazepam 20 mg versus placebo in
18 insomniacs in a real-life condition on driving performance. After polysomnography at base-
line and each treatment night, patients underwent, a STISIM driving simulator test at 7:30 a.m.,
5.5 hours after drug intake. There were no differences between treatments for the primary out-
come measure (mean time to collision; baseline: 0.120 s, P: 0.124, T: 0.118, Z: 0.124; p ≥ 0.12
for all pairwise comparisons). No differences were recorded for speed deviation and reaction
time to tasks for the verum treatments; however, lane position deviation was greater after
administration of zolpidem in comparison with both placebo and temazepam (p = 0.025 and
0.05, respectively). They underlined the necessity to strongly advocate against the late intake of
hypnotics if patients intend to drive a car early the next morning. Using a mathematical model,
Menzin et al. calculated the potential effects of sleep medications on MVA and costs, and applied
the model to the French setting. They used the model of standard deviation of a vehicle’s lateral
position (SDLP) and hypothesized that compared with zaleplon, the use of zopiclone over 14
days would be expected to result in 503 excess accidents per 100,000 drivers (45).
24 LEGER
undergone a medical evaluation in the past six months (p = 0.0138), with an average of 2 (±3.0)
evaluations for SI versus 1.2 (±2.2) evaluations for GS (p = 0.0198). SI had had more blood
studies (48% vs. 34%; p = 0.0005) and radiological procedures (17% vs. 10%; p = 0.0142) than
GS. SI also had more outpatient visits and used more medications (particularly cardiovascular,
central nervous system, genitourinary, and gastrointestinal) than GS. However, there was no
difference in the use of analgesic medications, despite the fact that 46% of insomniacs versus 29%
of GS (p < 0.001) said that they were particularly sensitive to pain. This is an important point,
as pain may be an obvious cause of sleep disturbance. Katz and McHorney (48) have reported
that poor mental and physical health were far more prevalent among insomniacs than among
controls. Recently, Katz and McHorney (49) calculated the odds ratio between chronic diseases
and complaints of insomnia. Severe insomnia was strongly linked to current depression (OR =
8.2), as well as to congestive heart failure (OR = 2.5), obstructive airway disease (OR = +1.6),
and prostrate problems (OR = 1.6). Darko et al. (58) showed in a prospective study that fatigue
and sleep disturbance were frequent symptoms of advanced HIV infection and emphasized the
significance of these complaints in assessment of patients’ clinical status.
Finally, the fact that insomnia can be a risk factor for psychiatric diseases and alcoholism
(3–7,33) was also firmly demonstrated by Katz and McHorney (49). These findings have two
implications. First, insomnia seems to be associated with poorer health status; indeed, insom-
niacs should be routinely evaluated for psychiatric and somatic disorders. Second, although
we cannot conclude whether insomnia is the cause of or the result of worsened health status,
insomniacs are clearly at increased risk for certain diseases and a higher use of medical ser-
vices. Many of the findings reported to be consequences of insomnia are actually correlates.
Until a cause–effect relationship is established, correlate or comorbidity may be a more accurate
term to describe the relationship between insomnia and poor medical status. Finally, taking
care of insomnia may significantly reduce the severity of comorbidities as Dirksen and Epstein
(50) demonstrated: Women receiving cognitive-behavioral therapy for insomnia had significant
improvements in fatigue, trait anxiety, depression, and QOL.
COSTS OF INSOMNIA
At this time, there are very little published studies to address the economic consequences of
insomnia. The National Commission of Sleep Disorders Research (NCSDR) in the United States
estimated the direct cost of insomnia in 1990 to be $15.4 billion, extrapolating from available
data (51). However, in the judgment of the Commission, “the absence of hard epidemiological
data makes it impossible to calculate the precise cost of sleep disorders, but some data do exist
to show that the costs are substantial.” In 1988 Leger (52) examined, the cost of accidents related
to sleep disorders in the United States for the NCSDR and estimated its cost between $43.15
billion and $56.02 billion. Stoller (53) made an estimate of the total cost of insomnia in the United
States in 1988, based on a literature review on the economic costs and effects associated with
insomnia. Her cost estimate ranged from $92.5 billion to $107.5 billion (53). All consensus reports
agree on the lack of socioeconomic data to better understand the burden of insomnia on society.
One difficulty is the paucity of information about insomnia-related use of health care services.
Another is the degree of overlap between insomnia and many somatic and psychiatric diseases.
The following sections summarize the work that has been accomplished in the field and define
what could be undertaken to better understand the economic consequences of insomnia.
The economic impact of insomnia can be divided into direct costs, indirect costs, and
related costs. Direct costs of insomnia are charges for medical care or self–treatment that are
borne by patients, government, organized health care providers, or insurance companies. Indi-
rect costs refer to patient– and employer–borne costs that result from insomnia–related morbid-
ity and mortality. Related costs are other costs that can be rationally associated with the illness,
such as the cost of property damage resulting from accidents associated with insomnia.
to be $13.93 billion, which consisted of health care services ($11.96 billion), including nursing
home care ($10.9 billion), and medications/substances used for treatment ($1.97 billion). Leger
et al. (55) also made an estimate of the direct costs of insomnia in France in 1995 (based on 1995
dollars values). They gave a $2.067 billion value divided mainly into $1.75 billion for outpatient
visits and $310,59 million for substances used for insomnia. It was of particular interest to
observe the very limited costs of sleep centers in this estimate, $1.75 million. In both estimates,
the cost of prescriptions was very little compared with other costs. However, the direct costs
related to sleep disorders evaluation by practitioners seem to be a small part of the total cost of
insomnia. Recently, an update of the direct and indirect costs of untreated insomnia in adults
was made in the United States (56). With the help of a self-insurer, employer-sponsored plan,
the authors compared direct costs of insomnia (including inpatient, outpatient, pharmacy, and
emergency department costs for all diseases) for six months before the diagnosis of insomnia
or the first prescription of hypnotics was made. They compared 138,820 younger adults and
75,558 older adults with insomnia to control groups. After logistic regression they found $924
greater direct costs per six months for insomniacs versus controls and $1143 greater estimated
direct costs in older adult insomniacs.
26 LEGER
understand the daily economic impact of diseases in insomniacs around the world. Future
studies might try to adopt economical values such as the national gross product for a better and
more comprehensive implication of the results at each country level.
QUALITY OF LIFE
There were very few studies specifically designed to assess the impact of insomnia on QOL. Most
of these were devoted to the impact of sleep disorders on the QOL of patients suffering from can-
cer. Some of the QOL studies have also been performed to explore the relationship of poor sleep
with diabetes, depression, Parkinson disease, chronic renal diseases with hemodialysis, and
HIV or chronic psychiatric diseases. QOL is also sometimes used to evaluate pharmacological
and nonpharmacological treatments of insomnia.
223 subjects explored for snoring or daytime somnolence. They showed that the SF-36 score is
sensitive to sleep disruption.
Finally, Katz and McHorney (49) demonstrated that insomnia acts by itself on the QOL of
patients suffering from chronic illness. Insomnia was found to be severe in 16% and mild in 34%
of these patients. Differences between patients with mild insomnia versus no insomnia showed
small-to-medium decrements across SF-36 subscales ranging from 4.1 to 9.3 points (on a scale
of 100) and for severe insomnia from 12.0 to 23.9 points. Insomnia appeared in this study as an
independent factor of a worsened QOL to almost the same extent as chronic conditions such as
congestive heart failure and clinical depression.
28 LEGER
better encompass the QOL of patients. This is why HD-16, a QOL scale specifically designed
for insomniacs, has been proposed (78). On the basis of interviews with SI patients, a 43-item
questionnaire on QOL has been built and tested on three groups composed of 240 SI, 422
mild insomniacs, and 391 GS. Ten steps led to the construction of a specific QOL scale. Five
dimensions have been validated as both relevant and independent from each other: physical
role, psychological role, social relationships with the others, cognitive (concentration, attention,
memorization), and energy. Sixteen items out the 43 initially tested were retained and were
found to be significantly different within the groups in each dimension. On the basis of these
16 items selected, we called the scale Hotel Dieu-16 (HD-16). The score’s specificity (correlation
score: 0.36) and reliability (Cronbach coefficient alpha: 0.78) were verified.
CONCLUSION
Insomnia affects the daily lives of millions of people around the world. The economic impact of
insomnia on society seems enormous. There is also increasing evidence linking insomnia to sev-
eral severe public health concerns: obesity, diabetes, depression, and cardiovascular diseases.
Besides the patients themselves, their family and work relatives are also affected by the conse-
quences of poor sleep. Public authorities are becoming increasingly focused on sleep education
and the protection of sleepers’ environment against noise. However, much work has to be done
to convince them that having good nights of sleep may deeply benefit individuals as well as
society.
REFERENCES
1. Ohayon MM. Epidemiology of insomnia: what we know and what we still need to learn. Sleep Med
Rev 2002; 6:97–111.
2. Soldatos CR, Allaert FA, Ohta T, et al. How do individuals sleep around the world? Results from a
single-day survey in ten countries. Sleep Med 2005; 6:5–13.
3. Leger D, Poursain B, Neubauer D, et al. An international survey of sleeping problems in the general
population. Curr Med Res Opin 2008; 24:307–317.
4. Alattar M, Harrington JJ, Mitchell CM, et al. Sleep problems in primary care: a North Carolina Family
Practice Research Network (NC-FP-RN) study. J Am Board Fam Med 2007; 20:365–374.
5. Roth T, Hajak G, Ustun TB. Consensus for the pharmacological management of insomnia in the new
millennium. Int J Clin Pract 2001; 55:42–52.
6. World Health Organization. Insomnia: an international consensus conference report. Versailles, Octo-
ber 13–15, 1996. WHO Publications MSA/MDN/98.2, 1998:55.
7. Buysse DJ. Diagnosis and assessment of sleep and circadian rhythm disorders. J Psychiatr Pract 2005;
11:102–115.
8. Kupfer DJ, Reynolds CF. Management of insomnia. N Eng J Med 1997; 336:341–346.
9. Schenck CH, Mahowald MW, Sack RL. Assessment and management of insomnia. JAMA 2003; 19:
2475.
10. Edinger JD, Bonnet MH, Bootzin RR, et al. Derivation of research diagnostic criteria for insomnia:
report of an American Academy of Sleep Medicine Work Group. Sleep 2004; 27:1567–1596.
11. Ohayon MM, Smirne S. Prevalence and consequences of insomnia disorders in the general population
of Italy. Sleep Med 2002; 3:115–120.
12. Leger D, Guilleminault C, Dreyfus JP, et al. Prevalence of insomnia in a survey of 12,778 adults in
France. J Sleep Res 2000; 9:35–42.
13. Kim K, Uchiyama M, Okawa M, et al. An epidemiological study of insomnia among the Japanese
general population. Sleep 2000; 23:41–47.
14. National Sleep Foundation (NSF). Sleep in America 2004. Gallup Organization.
www.sleepfoundation.org
15. Ohayon MM. Prevalence and correlates of nonrestorative sleep complaints. Arch Intern Med 2005;
165:35–41.
16. Ohayon MM, Lemoine P. Daytime consequences of insomnia complaints in the French general popu-
lation. Encephale 2004; 30:222–227.
17. Nakata A, Haratani T, Takahashi M, et al. Job stress, social support, and prevalence of insomnia in a
population of Japanese daytime workers. Soc Sci Med 2004; 59:1719–1730.
18. Doi Y, Minowa M, Tango T. Impact and correlates of poor sleep quality in Japanese white-collar
employees. Sleep 2003; 26:467–471.
IHBK059-03 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:19 Char Count=
19. Gellis LA, Lichstein KL, Scarinci IC, et al. Socioeconomic status and insomnia. J Abnorm Psychol 2005;
114:111–118.
20. National Sleep Foundation (NSF). Sleep in America. Princeton, NJ: Gallup Organization, 1991.
21. Leger D, Guilleminault C, Bader G, et al. Medical and socio-professional impact of insomnia. Sleep
2002; 25:625–629.
22. Johnson EO, Roehrs T, Roth T, et al. Epidemiology of alcohol and medication as aid sleep in early
adulthood. Sleep 1998; 21:178–186.
23. Aikens JE, Rouse ME. Help-seeking for insomnia among adult patients in primary care. J Am Board
Fam Pract 2005; 18:257–261.
24. Pires ML, Benedito-Silva AA, Mello MT, et al. Sleep habits and complaints of adults in the city of São
Paulo, Brazil, in 1987 and 1995. Braz J Med Biol Res 2007; 40:1505–1515.
25. Hatoum HT, Kania CM, Kong SX, et al. Prevalence of insomnia: a survey of the enrollees at five
managed care organizations. Am J Manag Care 1998; 4:79–86.
26. Wittchen HU, Krause P, Hofler M, et al. NISAS-2000: the “Nationwide Insomnia Screening and Aware-
ness Study”. Prevalence and interventions in primary care. Fortschr Med Orig 2001; 119:9–19.
27. Buysse DJ, Angst J, Gamma A, et al. Prevalence, course, and comorbidity of insomnia and depression
in young adults. Sleep 2008; 31:473–480.
28. Roth T, Jaeger S, Jin R, et al. Sleep problems, comorbid mental disorders, and role functioning in the
national comorbidity survey replication. Biol Psychiatry 2006; 60:1364–1371.
29. Gjesdal S, Ringdal PR, Haug K, et al. Long-term sickness absence and disability pension with psychi-
atric diagnoses: a population-based cohort study. Nord J Psychiatry 2008; 14:1–8.
30. Kessler RC, Heeringa S, Lakoma MD, et al. Individual and societal effects of mental disorders on
earnings in the United States: results from the national comorbidity survey replication. Am J Psychiatry
2008; 165:703–711.
31. Wang PS, Aguilar-Gaxiola S, Alonso J, et al. Use of mental health services for anxiety, mood, and
substance disorders in 17 countries in the WHO world mental health surveys. Lancet 2007; 370:841–
850.
32. Stepanski E, Zorick F, Roehrs T, et al. Daytime alertness in patients with chronic insomnia compared
with asymptomatic control subjects. Sleep 1988; 11:54–60.
33. Riedel BW, Lichstein KL. Insomnia and daytime functioning. Sleep Med Rev 2000; 4:277–298.
34. Schneider-Helmert D. Twenty four hour sleep/wake function and personality patterns in chronic
insomniacs and healthy controls. Sleep 1987; 11:54–60.
35. Manocchia M, Keller S, Ware JE. Sleep problems, health-related quality of life, work functioning and
health care utilization among the chronically ill. Qual Life Res 2001; 10:331–345.
36. Lerner D, Henke RM. What does research tell us about depression, job performance, and work pro-
ductivity? J Occup Environ Med 2008; 50:401–410.
37. Leigh P. Employee and job attributes and predictors of absenteeism in a national sample of workers:
the importance of health and dangerous working conditions. Soc Sci Med 1991; 33:127–131.
38. Lavie P. Sleep habits and sleep disturbances in industrial workers in Israel: main findings and some
characteristics of workers complaining of daytime sleepiness. Sleep 1981; 4:147–158.
39. Leger D, Massuel MA, Comet D; the SISYPHE group. Consequences of insomnia on professional
activity in the Paris Region. Sleep 2004; 27:A270.
40. Philip P, Leger D, Taillard J, et al. Insomniac complaints interfere with quality of life but not
with absenteeism: respective role of depressive and organic comorbidity. Sleep Med 2006; 7:
585–591.
41. Johnson LC, Spinweber CL, Gomez SA, et al. Daytime sleepiness, performance, mood, nocturnal sleep:
the effect of benzodiazepine and caffeine on their relationship. Sleep 1990; 13:121–135.
42. Daley ME, LeBlanc M, Morin CM. The impact of insomnia on absenteeism, productivity and accidents
rate. Sleep 2005; 28:A247.
43. Nakata A, Ikeda T, Takahashi M, et al. The prevalence and correlates of occupational injuries in
small-scale manufacturing enterprises. J Occup Health 2006; 48:366–376.
44. Partinen M, Hirvonen K, Hublin C, et al. Effects of after-midnight intake of zolpidem and temazepam
on driving ability in women with non-organic insomnia. Sleep Med 2003; 4:553–561.
45. Menzin J, Lang KM, Levy P, et al. A general model of the effects of sleep medications on the risk and
cost of motor vehicle accidents and its application to France. Pharmacoeconomics 2001; 19:69–78.
46. Wereyer S, Dilling H. Prevalence and treatment of insomnia in the community: results from the Upper
Bavarian field study. Sleep 1991; 14:392–398.
47. Kales JD, Kales A, Bixler EO, et al. Biopsychobehavioral correlates of insomnia. Clinical characteristics
and behavior correlates. Am J Psychiatry 1984; 141:1371–1376.
48. Katz DA, McHorney CA. Clinical correlates of insomnia in patients with chronic illness. Arch Intern
Med 1998; 159:1099–1107.
IHBK059-03 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:19 Char Count=
30 LEGER
49. Katz DA, McHorney CA. The relationship between insomnia and health-related quality of life in
patients with chronic illness. J Fam Pract 2002; 51:229–235.
50. Dirksen SR, Epstein DR. Efficacy of an insomnia intervention on fatigue, mood and quality of life in
breast cancer survivors. J Adv Nurs 2008; 61:664–675.
51. National Commission on Sleep Disorders Research. Wake up America: A National Sleep Alert, Vol
1. Executive Summary and Executive Report of National Commission of Sleep Disorders Research.
Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office, 1993.
52. Leger D. The cost of sleep related accidents: a report for the national commission of sleep disorders
research. Sleep 1994; 17:84–93.
53. Stoller MK. Economic effects of insomnia. Clin Ther 1994; 16:873–897.
54. Walsh JK, Engelhardt CL. The direct economic costs of insomnia in the United State for 1995. Sleep
1999; 22(suppl 2):S386–S393.
55. Leger D, Levy E, Paillard M. The direct costs of insomnia in France. Sleep 1999; 22(suppl 2):S394–S401.
56. Ozminkowski RJ, Wang S, Walsh JK. The direct and indirect costs of untreated insomnia in adults in
the United States. Sleep 2007; 30(3):263–273.
57. Godet-Cayré V, Pelletier-Fleury N, Le Vaillant M, et al. Insomnia and absenteeism at work. Who pays
the cost? Sleep 2006; 29(2):179–184.
58. Idzikowski C. Impact of insomnia on health-related quality of life. Pharmacoeconomics 1996; 10(suppl
1):15–24.
59. Iliescu EA, Coo H, McMurray MH, et al. Quality of sleep and health-related quality of life in haemodial-
ysis patients. Nephrol Dial Transplant 2003; 18:126–132.
60. Leger D, Janus C, Pellois A, et al. Sleep, morning alertness and quality of life in subjects treated with
zopiclone and in good sleepers. Study comparing 167 patients and 381 good sleepers. Eur Psychiatry
1995; 10(suppl 3):99–102.
61. Smith IE, Shneerson JM. Is the SF-36 sensitive to sleep disruption? A study in subjects with sleep
apnoea. J Sleep Res 1995; 4:183–188.
62. Zammitt GK, Weiner J, Damato N, et al. Quality of life in people with insomnia. Sleep 1999; 22:S379–
S385.
63. Russel IT. The SF-36 health survey questionnaire: an outcome measure suitable for routine use in the
NHS. Br Med J 1993; 306:1440–1444.
64. Leger D, Scheuermaier K, Philip P, et al. SF-36: evaluation of quality of life in severe and mild
insomniacs compared with good sleepers. Psychosom Med 2001; 63:49–55.
65. Schubert CR, Cruickshanks KJ, Dalton DS, et al. Prevalence of sleep problems and quality of life in an
older population. Sleep 2002; 25:889–893.
66. Pilowski I, Cettenden I, Townley M. Sleep disturbance in pain clinic patients. Pain 1985; 23:27–33.
67. Stark D, Kiely M, Smith A, et al. Anxiety disorders in cancer patients: their nature, associations and
quality of life. J Clin Oncol 2002; 20:3137–3148.
68. Lindley C, Vasa S, Sawyer WT, et al. Quality of life and preferences for treatment following systemic
adjuvant therapy for early-stage breast cancer. J Clin Oncol 1998; 16:1380–1387.
69. Caap-Ahlgren M, Dehlin O. Insomnia and depressive symptoms in patients with Parkinson disease.
Relationship to health-related quality of life. An interview study of patients living at home. Arch
Gerontol Geriatr 2001; 32:23–33.
70. Molnar MZ, Novak M, Szeifert L, et al. Restless legs syndrome, insomnia, and quality of life after renal
transplantation. J Psychosom Res 2007; 63:591–597.
71. Krystal AD, Thakur M, Roth T. Sleep disturbance in psychiatric disorders: effects on function and
quality of life in mood disorders, alcoholism, and schizophrenia. Ann Clin Psychiatry 2008; 20:39–46.
72. Nokes KM, Kendrew J. Correlates of sleep quality in persons with HIV disease. J Assoc Nurses AIDS
Care 2001; 12:17–22.
73. Goldenberg F, Hindmarch J, Joyce CRB,et al. Zopiclone, sleep and health related quality of life. Hum
Psychopharmacol 1994; 9:245–252.
74. Walsh JK, Krystal AD, Amato DA, et al. Nightly treatment of primary insomnia with eszopiclone for
six months: effect on sleep, quality of life, and work limitations. Sleep 2007; 30(8):959–968.
75. Baca E, Estivill E, Hernandez B, et al.; on behalf of Castivil group. Quality of life in insomnia: influence
of zolpidem. J Sleep Res 2002; 11(suppl 1):10.
76. Quesnel C, Savard J, Simard S, et al. Efficacy of cognitive–behavioral therapy for insomnia in women
treated for nonmetastatic breast cancer. J Consult Clin Psychol 2003; 71:189–200.
77. Dixon S, Morgan K, Mathers N, et al. Impact of cognitive behavior therapy on health-related quality
of life among adult hypnotic users with chronic insomnia. Behav Sleep Med 2006; 4(2):71–84.
78. Leger D, Scheuermaier K, Raffray T, et al. HD-16: a new quality of life instrument specifically designed
for insomnia. Sleep Med 2005; 6:191–198.
c04 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:20 Char Count=
INTRODUCTION
As reviewed in the prior two chapters, insomnia can have a variety of daytime consequences for
the individual as well as present a considerable socioeconomic burden to society. As a disease
entity, insomnia also exists as an independent risk factor for psychiatric and medical illness,
compounding its consequences.
In this chapter, we will review the evidence that insomnia is “something that increases
a person’s chances of developing a disease.” This most common definition of risk factor will
be expanded to also include something that increases the chances for diminished treatment
response or in the case of adequate treatment response, increases relapse rates. It is important
to note that several definitions of the term risk factor exist and that the reader is often left
to determine what definition authors have in mind (if any). For that reason we also follow a
definition proffered by Beck (1) that additionally defines a risk factor as being part of the casual
chain, thus requiring temporal evidence, which can only be derived from longitudinal studies.
In the insomnia literature, there is a good deal of cross sectional data linking insomnia to a
variety of disease entities, which will be reviewed. Such studies, however, will be considered
as evidence that insomnia is a risk indicator that Burt (2) has defined as a probable or putative
risk factor.
Another matter of definition arises when reviewing the broad insomnia literature. Namely,
insomnia is not measured or defined in a consistent manner across studies. This is especially
true for data drawn from epidemiologic and community samples, but is also found in studies
in which sleep was not among the primary outcomes. Typically such investigations report on
single item measures of sleep complaints either developed for the study or embedded within
other instruments. For instance, the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (3) has three items,
one each pertaining to early, middle, and late night insomnia. Until recently, this item approach
to sleep or insomnia assessment had not been validated, but Manber et al. (4) have now shown
that, at least in a depressed sample, such approaches can be validated against the assessment of
insomnia via daily sleep diaries traditionally used by sleep researchers. This sleep outcome has
been called variably “insomnia” or “sleep disturbance.” The latter term has some merit when
the sleep items are few and very broad (e.g., one item that asks for a rating of sleep quality).
This chapter has adopted a convention of using the term “insomnia” in cases where there are
not validated measures of insomnia, but the sleep items are consistent with insomnia.
32 PIGEON
posed an increased risk for the development of depression with odds ratios of 40 (CI: 19.8–80)
and 5.4 (2.6–11.3) respectively (10,15), while Dryman and Eaton found an increased risk of
depression [OR: 9.4(4.3–20.3)], but only in women (16).
While the above studies sampled populations of varying ages, others have focused on
younger adults. In a study that began with college-aged men, insomnia in college conferred
a relative risk of 2.0 (1.2–3.3) for developing depression at some point during the following
30 years (17). Breslau et al. (18) sampled 979 members of a health maintenance organization
who were 21 to 30 years old at baseline assessment and reassessed three years later. At baseline,
persons reporting two weeks or more of insomnia nearly every night at any point in their life
were at increased risk of developing new onset depression by the three-year follow-up [OR:
2.4(1.2–4.8)] than those with no history of insomnia even after controlling for prior depressive
symptoms.
In longitudinal studies performed in older cohorts, findings have been slightly mixed.
One study limited by power, found that in 147 patients with no prior history of depression,
the presence of persistent insomnia at baseline was associated with new onset depression one
year later (but only in females) (19). In a study of 524 community-dwelling elders, insomnia at
baseline and three years later was associated with depression at three years compared to those
with no insomnia or those with insomnia at one time point (20). In a reanalysis of this data
set including only subjects with activity limitations and no psychiatric morbidity at baseline,
baseline insomnia was not associated with depression three years later (21). Roberts et al. (22)
found that insomnia at baseline was associated with an increased risk of depression one year
later [OR: 2.5(1.7–3.7)] and insomnia at both time points carried an eightfold risk, though other
factors were more significant predictors. In older adults followed for 12 years (23), baseline
insomnia was an independent predictor of depression at 12 years in women only [OR: 4.1(2.3–
7.2)]. Brabbins (24) determined that in a sample of 771 elders, only initial insomnia (and not
middle of the night or early morning awakening) that occurred at both baseline and three years
was associated with the development of new depression at three years.
Finally, a meta-analysis of studies conducted in older adults found that sleep disturbance,
with an odds ratio of 2.6, was second to recent bereavement (OR 3.3) as a risk factor for late-life
depression (25). In summary, while there is a good body of evidence that suggest that both
incident and persistent insomnia predict new onset depression, the findings are not unanimous,
and insomnia is not the only significant risk factor. Taken as a whole these sets of longitudinal
studies support the insomnia as one risk factor for developing depression.
As Table 1 shows, insomnia and depression do not always co-occur, nor does one neces-
sarily presage the other across all cases. As is the case for any risk factor, insomnia is neither
a necessary condition for nor the sole pathway to depression, but it does, in fact, appear to be
part of the casual chain for many individuals. Two recent publications further this case with
respect to depression. Buysse et al. (26) were able to assess insomnia and depression from an
epidemiologic study in Zurich that included 6 time points over a 20-year span. In their analyses,
the presence of insomnia absent depression at each time point was strongly associated with the
presence of co-occurring insomnia and depression at the subsequent time point. A similar find-
ing occurred with respect to the presence of depression absent insomnia, though the findings
for insomnia were more robust. Departing from the epidemiologic data, Pigeon et al. assessed
insomnia in the context of a depression treatment trial of 1801 older adults in primary care
settings (26,27). They found that patients with insomnia that persisted across a baseline and
three-month assessment had a diminished treatment response at 6 and 12 months compared
to patients with insomnia at one or neither of the baseline and two-month time points. This
expands the notion of insomnia as a risk factor for depression to include comorbid insomnia as
a risk factor for unremitting depression.
While it is not the case that a putative risk factor must be demonstrated to improve a dis-
ease entity when it is removed, there are emerging data in this regard for insomnia in the context
of depression. Two uncontrolled studies have shown that patients presenting with stringent def-
initions of insomnia and depression who completed a course of cognitive-behavioral therapy
for insomnia had improvements in both sleep and depression (28,29). In addition two random-
ized, controlled trials have shown that treating insomnia and depression simultaneously has a
greater impact on both conditions than when treating the depression alone. One of these trials
c04 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:20 Char Count=
34 PIGEON
found that patients treated concomitantly with fluoxetine and eszopiclone exhibited less illness
severity over the course of an eight-week intervention and a faster time to recovery than sub-
jects treated with fluoxetine only (30) and the other that escitalopram and cognitive-behavioral
therapy for insomnia produced a higher rate of remission for depression and insomnia than
escitalopram alone (31). This set of studies supports the assertion that insomnia is a modifiable
risk factor for depression.
predicted an increased risk for eventual death by suicide (49). Goldstein et al. (50) assessed sleep
disturbances in 140 adolescent suicide victims and 131 controls with a psychological autopsy
protocol and semistructured psychiatric interview, respectively. Suicide completers had higher
rates of overall sleep disturbance, insomnia, and hypersomnia compared to controls within the
last week after controlling for affective disorders. These data suggest that insomnia is a risk
indicator for suicidality with some evidence that it is a risk factor for suicide.
36 PIGEON
Anecdotal reports also suggest that particularly in patients acutely recovering from alco-
holism, sleeping problems lead to relapse. This has been borne out in one study, where insomnia
and fragmented sleep were found to predict relapse in 20 abstinent alcoholics (65). In another
sample of 74 recently abstinent alcoholics, 60% with baseline insomnia versus 30% without
baseline insomnia relapsed to any use of alcohol within five months (64). Brower et al. (66) also
recently completed a small randomized controlled trial of gabapentin in 21 patients with active
alcoholism and comorbid insomnia. Albeit a limited sample size, those in the treatment arm
had a reduced time to relapse.
These data indicate that insomnia is a risk indicator for the development of alcoholism;
there is not enough data to posit such an assertion in other forms of substance abuse. The data
also suggest that insomnia represents a risk factor for relapse in alcohol dependence.
sleep deprivation, or with clinical diagnosis (i.e., insomnia) (98–104). As reviewed by Krueger
et al. (101), numerous cytokines appear to have some relation to sleep. A small number of studies
have found that insomnia (as compared to good sleep) tends to be associated with altered innate
immunity. In general, insomnia is associated with decreased NK activity (105,106), and a shift in
the circadian distribution of IL-6 and TNF-␣ from the night to the daytime (107), despite higher
evening levels of Il-6 (107). Only one study has found that immune alterations are correlated
with self-report or PSG measures of disturbed sleep. Burgos et al. (108) found in patients with
insomnia that IL-6 secretion is inversely correlated with self-reported sleep quality and PSG-
measured SWS minutes. While intriguing none of these data point to insomnia as a possible
risk factor for subsequent disease.
Data from three adaptive immune system studies (as opposed to the innate immunity
studies above) are also suggestive. In a companion study to a clinical trail of an experimental
avian influenza vaccine (109), 46 patients receiving the active vaccine were assessed with the
Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) (110). Compared to vaccine responders, the nonrespon-
ders had higher PSQI scores, poorer sleep efficiency, and shorter sleep duration. Cohen et al.
(111), in a study of 276 healthy adults, sought to assess whether social ties were related to
susceptibility to the common cold. All participants were infected with a live rhinovirus and
the development and severity of common cold symptoms were assessed. While the primary
hypothesis was borne out (greater social ties were protective against the development of cold
symptoms), several potential variables were found to moderate the relationship, including
decreased self-reported sleep efficiency. In a follow-up study designed to further test this asso-
ciation in 153 participants, these authors found that sleep durations under seven hours and
sleep efficiency under 92% immediately preceding exposure to a rhinovirus were each associ-
ated with significantly higher rates of infection (112). Thus, preliminary data exist demonstrates
that insomnia is a risk factor for infectious disease.
REFERENCES
1. Beck JD. Risk revisited. Community Dent Oral Epidemiol 1998; 26(4):220–225.
2. Burt BA. Risk factors, risk markers, and risk indicators. Community Dent Oral Epidemiol 1998;
26(4):219.
3. Hamilton M. Development of a rating scale for primary depressive illness. Br J Soc Clin Psychol 1967;
6:278–296.
4. Manber R, Blasey C, Arnow B, et al. Assessing insomnia severity in depression: comparison of
depression rating scales and sleep diaries. J Psychiatr Res 2005; 39(5):481–488.
5. Pigeon W, Perlis ML. Insomnia and depression: birds of a feather? Int J Sleep Disord 2007; 1(3):
82–91.
6. Stewart R, Besset A, Bebbington P, et al. Insomnia comorbidity and impact and hypnotic use by age
group in a national survey population aged 16 to 74 years. Sleep 2006; 29(11):1391–1397.
7. Mahendran R, Subramaniam M, Chan YM. Psychiatric morbidity of patients referred to an insomnia
clinic. Singapore Med J 2007; 48(2):163–165.
8. Mellinger GD, Balter MB, Uhlenhuth EH. Insomnia and its treatment: prevalence and correlates.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1985; 42(3):225–232.
9. Taylor DJ, Lichstein KL, Durrence HH, et al. Epidemiology of insomnia, depression, and anxiety.
Sleep 2005; 28(11):1457–1464.
c04 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:20 Char Count=
38 PIGEON
10. Ford DE, Kamerow DB. Epidemiologic study of sleep disturbances and psychiatric disorders. An
opportunity for prevention? JAMA 1989; 262(11):1479–1484.
11. Benca R. Mood disorders. In: Kryger M, Roth T, Dement W, eds. Principles and Practice of Sleep
Disorders Medicine. Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders Company, 2005:1311–1326.
12. Tsuno N, Besset A, Ritchie K. Sleep and depression. J Clin Psychiatry 2005; 66(10):1254–1269.
13. Perlis ML, Buysse D, Giles DE, et al. Sleep disturbance may be a prodromal symptom of depression.
J Affect Disord 1997; 42(2):209–212.
14. Beck AT, Steer RA, Garbin MG. Psychometric properties of the Beck depression inventory—25 years
of evaluation. Clin Psychol Rev 1988; 8(1):77–100.
15. Weissman MM, Greenwald S, Nino-Murcia G, et al. The morbidity of insomnia uncomplicated by
psychiatric disorders. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 1997; 19(4):245–250.
16. Dryman A, Eaton WW. Affective symptoms associated with the onset of major depression in the
community: findings from the US National Institute of Mental Health Epidemiologic Catchment
Area Program. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1991; 84(1):1–5.
17. Chang PP, Ford DE, Mead LA, et al. Insomnia in young men and subsequent depression. The Johns
Hopkins Precursors Study. Am J Epidemiol 1997; 146(2):105–114.
18. Breslau N, Roth T, Rosenthal L, et al. Sleep disturbance and psychiatric disorders: a longitudinal
epidemiological study of young adults. Biol Psychiatry 1996; 39(6):411–418.
19. Perlis M, Smith LJ, Lyness JM, et al. Insomnia as a risk factor for onset of depression in the elderly.
Behav Sleep Med 2006; 4(2):104–113.
20. Livingston G, Blizard B, Mann A. Does sleep disturbance predict depression in elderly people? A
study in inner London. Br J Gen Pract 1993; 43(376):445–448.
21. Livingston G, Watkin V, Milne B, et al. Who becomes depressed? The Islington community study of
older people. J Affect Disord 2000; 58(2):125–133.
22. Roberts RE, Shema SJ, Kaplan GA, et al. Sleep complaints and depression in an aging cohort: a
prospective perspective. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157(1):81–88.
23. Mallon L, Broman JE, Hetta J. Relationship between insomnia, depression, and mortality: a 12-year
follow-up of older adults in the community. Int Psychogeriatr 2000; 12(3):295–306.
24. Brabbins CJ, Dewey ME, Copeland JR, et al. Insomnia in the elderly: prevalence, gender differences
and relationships with morbidity and mortality. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 1993; 8(6):473–480.
25. Cole MG, Dendukuri N. Risk factors for depression among elderly community subjects: a systematic
review and meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160(6):1147–1156.
26. Buysse DJ, Angst J, Gamma A, et al. Prevalence, course, and comorbidity of insomnia and depression
in young adults. Sleep 2008; 31(4):473–480.
27. Pigeon WR, Hegel MT, Unutzer J, et al. Is insomnia a perpetuating factor for late-life depression in
the IMPACT cohort? Sleep 2008; 31(4):481–488.
28. Morawetz D. Behavioral self-help treatment for insomnia: a controlled evaluation. Behav Ther 1989;
20:365–379.
29. Taylor DJ, Lichstein K, Weinstock J, et al. A pilot study of cognitive-behavioral therapy of insomnia
in people with mild depression. Behav Ther 2007; 38:49–57.
30. Fava M, McCall WV, Krystal A, et al. Eszopiclone co-administered with fluoxetine in patients with
insomnia coexisting with major depressive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2006; 59(11):1052–1060.
31. Manber R, Edinger JD, Gress JL, et al. Cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia enhances depression
outcome in patients with comorbid major depressive disorder and insomnia. Sleep 2008; 31(4):489–
495.
32. Harvey AG. Sleep and circadian rhythms in bipolar disorder: seeking synchrony, harmony, and
regulation. Am J Psychiatry 2008; 165(7):820–829.
33. Plante DT, Winkelman JW. Sleep disturbance in bipolar disorder: therapeutic implications. Am J
Psychiatry 2008; 165(7):830–843.
34. Jackson A, Cavanagh J, Scott J. A systematic review of manic and depressive prodromes. J Affect
Disord 2003; 74(3):209–217.
35. Prigerson HG. When the path of adjustment leads to a dead-end. Bereavement Care 2004; 23:38–40.
36. Horowitz MJ, Siegel B, Holen A, et al. Diagnostic criteria for complicated grief disorder. Am J
Psychiatry 1997; 154(7):904–910.
37. Hamilton BE, Minino AM, Martin JA, et al. Annual summary of vital statistics: 2005. Pediatrics 2007;
119(2):345–360.
38. Reynolds CF, Hoch CC, Buysse DJ, et al. Sleep after spousal bereavement—a study of recovery from
stress. Biol Psychiatry 1993; 34(11):791–797.
39. Prigerson HG, Frank E, Kasl SV, et al. Complicated grief and bereavement-related depression as
distinct disorders: preliminary empirical validation in elderly bereaved spouses. Am J Psychiatry
1995; 152(1):22–30.
c04 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:20 Char Count=
40. Germain A, Caroff K, Buysse DJ, et al. Sleep quality in complicated grief. J Trauma Stress 2005;
18(4):343–346.
41. Richardson SJ, Lund DA, Caserta MS, et al. Sleep patterns in older bereaved spouses. Omega-J Death
Dying 2003; 47(4):361–383.
42. Valdimarsdottir U, Helgason AR, Furst CJ, et al. Long-term effects of widowhood after terminal
cancer: a Swedish nationwide follow-up. Scand J Public Health 2003; 31(1):31–36.
43. Beem EE, Maes S, Cleiren M, et al. Psychological functioning of recently bereaved middle-aged
women: the first 13 months. Psychol Rep 2000; 87(1):243–254.
44. Pronger CE. Insomnia and suicide. Lancet 1914; 184:1356–1359.
45. Sjostrom N, Waern M, Hetta J. Nightmares and sleep disturbances in relation to suicidality in suicide
attempters. Sleep 2007; 30(1):91–95.
46. Bernert RA, Joiner TE, Cukrowicz KC, et al. Suicidality and sleep disturbances. Sleep 2005; 28(9):1135–
1141.
47. Smith MT, Perlis ML, Haythornthwaite JA. Suicidal ideation in outpatients with chronic muscu-
loskeletal pain—an exploratory study of the role of sleep onset insomnia and pain intensity. Clin J
Pain 2004; 20(2):111–118.
48. Liu XC. Sleep and adolescent suicidal behavior. Sleep 2004; 27(7):1351–1358.
49. Bernert R, Turvey C, Conwell Y, et al. Sleep disturbance as a unique risk factor for completed suicide.
Sleep 2007; 30:A334.
50. Goldstein TR, Bridge JA, Brent DA. Sleep disturbance preceding completed suicide in adolescents. J
Consult Clin Psychol 2008; 76(1):84–91.
51. Ohayon MM, Shapiro CM. Sleep disturbances and psychiatric disorders associated with posttrau-
matic stress disorder in the general population. Compr Psychiatry 2000; 41(6):469–478.
52. Neylan TC, Marmar CR, Metzler TJ, et al. Sleep disturbances in the Vietnam generation: findings
from a nationally representative sample of male Vietnam veterans. Am J Psychiatry 1998; 155(7):
929–933.
53. Zayfert C, Deviva JC. Residual insomnia following cognitive behavioral therapy for PTSD. J Trauma
Stress 2004; 17(1):69–73.
54. Harvey AG, Bryant RA. The relationship between acute stress disorder and posttraumatic stress
disorder: a prospective evaluation of motor vehicle accident survivors. J Consult Clin Psychol 1998;
66(3):507–512.
55. Mellman TA, David D, Bustamante V, et al. Dreams in the acute aftermath of trauma and their
relationship to PTSD. J Trauma Stress 2001; 14(1):241–247.
56. Mellman TA, Pigeon WR, Nowell PD, et al. Relationships between REM sleep findings and PTSD
symptoms during the early aftermath of trauma. J Trauma Stress 2007; 20(5):893–901.
57. Krakow BJ, Melendrez DC, Johnston LG, et al. Sleep Dynamic Therapy for Cerro Grande Fire
evacuees with posttraumatic stress symptoms: a preliminary report. J Clin Psychiatry 2002; 63(8):
673–684.
58. Krakow B, Johnston L, Melendrez D, et al. An open-label trial of evidence-based cognitive behavior
therapy for nightmares and insomnia in crime victims with PTSD. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158(12):2043–
2047.
59. Deviva JC, Zayfert C, Pigeon WR, et al. Treatment of residual insomnia after CBT for PTSD: case
studies. J Trauma Stress 2005; 18(2):155–159.
60. Germain A, Shear MK, Hall M, et al. Effects of a brief behavioral treatment for PTSD-related sleep
disturbances: a pilot study. Behav Res Ther 2007; 45(3):627–632.
61. Saletu-Zyhlarz G, Saletu B, Anderer P, et al. Nonorganic insomnia in generalized anxiety disorder. 1.
Controlled studies on sleep, awakening and daytime vigilance utilizing polysomnography and EEG
mapping. Neuropsychobiology 1997; 36(3):117–129.
62. Saletu B, Anderer P, Brandstatter N, et al. Insomnia in generalized anxiety disorder: polysomno-
graphic, psychometric and clinical investigations before, during, and after therapy with a long-
versus a short-half-life benzodiazepine (Quazepam versus Triazolam). Neuropsychobiology 1994;
29:69–90.
63. Fuller KH, Waters WF, Binks PG, et al. Generalized anxiety and sleep architecture: a polysomno-
graphic investigation. Sleep 1997; 20(5):370–376.
64. Brower KJ, Aldrich MS, Robinson EAR,et al. Insomnia, self-medication, and relapse to alcoholism.
Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158(3):399–404.
65. Drummond SP, Gillin JC, Smith TL, et al. The sleep of abstinent pure primary alcoholic patients:
natural course and relationship to relapse. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1998; 22(8):1796–1802.
66. Brower KJ, Kim HM, Strobbe S, et al. Randomized double-blind pilot trial of gabapentin versus
placebo to treat alcohol dependence and comorbid insomnia. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2008; 32(8):1429–
1438.
c04 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:20 Char Count=
40 PIGEON
67. Dew MA, Reynolds CF, Buysse DJ, et al. Electroencephalographic sleep profiles during depression.
Effects of episode duration and other clinical and psychosocial factors in older adults. Arch Gen
Psychiatry 1996; 53(2):148–156.
68. Kripke DF. Mortality risk of major depression. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152(6):962.
69. Althuis MD, Fredman L, Langenberg PW, et al. The relationship between insomnia and mortality
among community-dwelling older women. J Am Geriatr Soc 1998; 46:1270–1273.
70. Kripke DF, Simons RN, Garfinkel L, et al. Short and long sleep and sleeping pills: is increased
mortality associated? Arch Gen Psychiatry 1979; 36:103–116.
71. Dew MA, Hoch CC, Buysse DJ, et al. Healthy older adults’ sleep predicts all-cause mortality at 4 to
19 years of follow-up. Psychosom Med 2003; 65(1):63–73.
72. Atkinson JH, Ancoli-Israel S, Slater MA, et al. Subjective sleep disturbance in chronic back pain. Clin
J Pain 1988; 65(2):225–232.
73. Morin C, Gibson D, Wade J. Self-reported sleep and mood disturbance in chronic pain patients. Clin
J Pain 1998; 14(4):311–314.
74. Pilowsky I, Crettenden I, Townley M. Sleep disturbance in pain clinic patients. Pain 1985; 23(1):27–33.
75. Smith MT, Perlis ML, Smith MS, et al. Sleep quality and presleep arousal in chronic pain. J Behav
Med 2000; 23(1):1–13.
76. Affleck G, Urrows S, Tennen H, et al. Sequential daily relations of sleep, pain intensity, and attention
to pain among women with fibromyalgia. Pain 1996; 68:363–368.
77. Onen SH, Alloui A, Gross A, et al. The effects of total sleep deprivation, selective sleep interrup-
tion and sleep recovery on pain tolerance thresholds in healthy subjects. J Sleep Res 2001; 10(1):
35–42.
78. Onen SH, Alloui A, Jourdan D, et al. Effects of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep deprivation, selective
sleep interruption, and sleep recovery on pain tolerance thresholds in healthy subjects. Brain Res
2001; 900:261–267.
79. Hicks RA, Coleman DD, Ferrante F, et al. Pain thresholds in rats during recovery from REM sleep
deprivation. Percept Mot Skills 1979; 48(3, pt 1):687–690.
80. Moldofsky H, Scarisbrick P. Induction of neurasthenic musculoskeletal pain syndrome by selective
sleep stage deprivation. Psychosom Med 1976; 38(1):35–44.
81. Lentz MJ, Landis CA, Rothermel J, et al. Effects of selective slow wave sleep disruption on muscu-
loskeletal pain and fatigue in middle aged women. J Rheumatol 1999; 26(7):1586–1592.
82. Older SA, Battafarano DF, Danning CL, et al. The effects of delta wave sleep interruption on pain
thresholds and fibromyalgia-like symptoms in healthy subjects; correlations with insulin-like growth
factor I. J Rheumatol 1998; 25(6):1180–1186.
83. Currie SR, Wilson KG, Pontefract AJ, et al. Cognitive-behavioral treatment of insomnia secondary to
chronic pain. J Consult Clin Psychol 2000; 68(3):407–416.
84. Edinger JD, Wohlgemuth WK, Krystal AD, et al. Behavioral insomnia therapy for fibromyalgia
patients—a randomized clinical trial. Arch Intern Med 2005; 165(21):2527–2535.
85. Pigeon W, Jungquist C, Matteson S, et al. Pain, sleep and mood outcomes in chronic pain patients
following cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia. Sleep 2007; 30:A255–A256.
86. Kupperman M, Lubeck DP, Mazonson PD. Sleep problems and their correlates in a working popula-
tion. J Gen Intern Med 1995; 10:25–32.
87. Redeker NS, Ruggiero JS, Hedges C. Sleep is related to physical function and emotional well-being
after cardiac surgery. Nurs Res 2004; 53(3):154–162.
88. Redeker NS. Why is sleep relevant to cardiovascular disease? J Cardiovasc Nurs 2002; 17(1):v–ix.
89. Jenkins CD, Stanton BA, Jono RT. Quantifying and predicting recovery after heart surgery. Psychosom
Med 1994; 56(3):203–212.
90. Jenkins CD. Psychosocial risk factors for coronary heart disease. Acta Med Scand Suppl 1982; 660:123–
136.
91. Schwartz S, McDowell AW, Cole SR, et al. Insomnia and heart disease: a review of epidemiologic
studies. J Psychosom Res 1999; 47(4):313–333.
92. Mander B, Colecchia E, Spiegel K, et al. Short sleep: a risk factor for insulin resistance and obesity.
Diabetes 2001; 50:A45.
93. Ryden AM, Knutson KL, Mander BA, et al. Association between sleep quality and glycemic control
in type 2 diabetic African-American women. Diabetes 2002; 51:A620.
94. Ryder AM, Knutson KL, Mander BA, et al. Sleep in Type II diabetes: a survey study. Sleep 2002;
25:A105–A106.
95. Suka M, Yoshida K, Sugimori H. Persistent insomnia is a predictor of hypertension in Japanese male
workers. J Occup Health 2003; 45(6):344–350.
96. Phillips B, Mannino D. Do insomnia complaints cause hypertension or cardiovascular disease? J Clin
Sleep Med 2007; 3(5):489–494.
c04 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:20 Char Count=
97. Phillips B, Buzkova P, Enright P. Insomnia did not predict incident hypertension in older adults in
the cardiovascular health study. Sleep 2009; 32(1):65–72.
98. Vgontzas AN, Zoumakis E, Bixler EO, et al. Adverse effects of modest sleep restriction on sleepiness,
performance, and inflammatory cytokines. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2004; 89(5):2119–2126.
99. Dinges DF, Douglas SD, Hamarman S, et al. Sleep deprivation and human immune function. Adv
Neuroimmunol 1995; 5(2):97–110.
100. Krueger JM, Toth LA, Floyd R, et al. Sleep, microbes and cytokines. Neuroimmunomodulation 1994;
1(2):100–109.
101. Krueger JM, Obal FJ, Fang J, et al. The role of cytokines in physiological sleep regulation. Ann N Y
Acad Sci 2001; 933:211–221.
102. Opp MR. Cytokines and sleep: the first hundred years. Brain Behav Immun 2004; 18(4):295–297.
103. Opp MR, Toth LA. Neural-immune interactions in the regulation of sleep. Front Biosci 2003; 8:d768–
d779.
104. Irwin M. Effects of sleep and sleep loss on immunity and cytokines. Brain Behav Immun 2002;
16(5):503–512.
105. Cover H, Irwin M. Immunity and depression: insomnia, retardation, and reduction of natural killer
cell activity. J Behav Med 1994; 17(2):217–223.
106. Irwin M, McClintick J, Costlow C, et al. Partial night sleep deprivation reduces natural killer and
cellular immune responses in humans. FASEB J 1996; 10(5):643–653.
107. Vgontzas AN, Zoumakis M, Papanicolaou DA, et al. Chronic insomnia is associated with a shift
of interleukin-6 and tumor necrosis factor secretion from nighttime to daytime. Metabolism 2002;
51(7):887–892.
108. Burgos I, RIchter L, Klein T, et al. Increased nocturnal interleukin-6 excretion in patients with primary
insomnia: a pilot study. Brain Behav Immun 2006; 20(3):246–253.
109. Pigeon WR, Duberstein P, Tang W, Moynihan J. The association of poor sleep to immunogenicity
following an avian influenza vaccine in healthy older adults. Paper presented at: Psychoneuroim-
munology Research Society Meeting; May 29, 2008; Madison, WI.
110. Buysee DJ, Reynolds CF III, Monk TH, et al. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument
for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res 1989; 28(2):193–213.
111. Cohen S, Doyle WJ, Skoner DP, et al. Social ties and susceptibility to the common cold. JAMA 1997;
277(24):1940–1944.
112. Cohen S, Doyle WJ, Alper CM, et al. Sleep habits and susceptibility to the common cold. Arch Intern
Med 2009; 169(1):62–67.
113. Lichstein KL, McCrae CS, Wilson NM. Secondary insomnia: diagnostic issues, cognitive-behavioral
treatment and future directions. In: Perlis M, Lichstein KL, eds. Treating Sleep Disorders: Principles
and Practice of Behavioral Sleep Medicine. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 2003:286–304.
114. Pigeon WR, Perlis ML. The long-term management of chronic insomnia: recommendations for pri-
mary care physicians. INSOM 2005;(5):4–9.
115. Smith MT, Huang MI, Manber R. Cognitive behavior therapy for chronic insomnia occurring within
the context of medical and psychiatric disorders. Clin Psychol Rev 2005; 25(5):559–611.
116. Turek FW. Insomnia and depression: if it looks and walks like a duck. Sleep 2005; 28(11):1362–1363.
117. Weyerer S, Dilling H. Prevalence and treatment of insomnia in the community: results from the upper
Bavarian Field Study. Sleep 1991; 14(5):392–398.
118. Bixler EO, Kales A, Soldatos CR, et al. Prevalence of sleep disorders in the Los Angeles metropolitan
area. Am J Psychiatry 1979; 136:1257–1262.
119. Foley DJ, Monjan AA, Brown SL, et al. Sleep complaints among elderly persons: an epidemiologic
study of three communities. Sleep 1995; 18(6):425–432.
120. Ohayon M. Epidemiological study on insomnia in the general population. Sleep 1996; 19(3 Suppl):S7–
S15.
121. Hohagen F, Kappler C, Schramm E, et al. Sleep onset insomnia, sleep maintaining insomnia and
insomnia with early morning awakening–temporal stability of subtypes in a longitudinal study on
general practice attenders. Sleep 1994; 17(6):551–554.
c05 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:21 Char Count=
The critical role of psychological processes in the cause and/or maintenance of insomnia has
received considerable attention over the past four decades. In this chapter, we begin by describ-
ing an influential overarching psychological theory, the three-P model. We then move on to
review several of the behavioral, cognitive, and hybrid models in chronological order.
A range of psychological processes may contribute to insomnia, including behavior, cog-
nition, affect, and personality. As will become evident, the bulk of the excellent work completed
thus far has targeted the behavioral and/or cognitive types of explanation. These theories vary
across a number of dimensions. First, several of the theories attempt to capture the entire life
course of the disorder, from its inception to its most chronic form, while others take a narrower
focus on just the maintaining or perpetuating factors. The former tend to be more general and
the latter tend to be more detailed or specific. Second, some of the theories examine several
types of explanation (e.g., the hybrid models) while others take on the task of delving into the
detail of one type of explanation. Other psychological processes, such as affect and personality
traits, may also be relevant but have been understudied. We will return to this topic later in this
chapter.
THREE P-MODEL
The three-P model, also referred to as the three-factor or Spielman model, is a diathesis-stress
theory that includes predisposing, precipitating, and perpetuating factors (1,2). The predispos-
ing factors can be biological (e.g., regularly elevated cortisol), psychological (e.g., tendency to
worry), or social (e.g., work schedule incompatible with sleep schedule). These factors represent
a vulnerability for insomnia. Precipitating factors, such as stressful life events, trigger the acute
onset of insomnia. The influence of precipitating factors diminishes over time. Perpetuating
factors, such as maladaptive coping skills or an extension of time in bed, contribute to the acute
insomnia developing into a chronic or longer term disorder. As the perpetuating factors serve
to maintain the insomnia, they are typically the targets of treatment. Importantly, the model
has been refined such that the three types of factors are dynamic over time (3). For exam-
ple, predisposing factors are not necessarily viewed as constant vulnerabilities in the refined
model (Fig. 1).
The unique feature of this model is that it provides a framework across types of explana-
tion; that is, this model is relevant to biological, cognitive, social, and other types of explanation.
However, in the early discussions of the model the behavioral processes were emphasized. In
particular, an important perpetuating factor was identified as extending sleep opportunity, or
lengthening the time in bed by going to bed earlier or getting up later. In this conceptualization,
this behavior results in a discrepancy between time in bed and ability to sleep. This theory has
resulted in a well-established therapy according to American Psychological Association criteria
(4), known as sleep restriction (5), as described in chapter 25.
Perpetuating
INSOMNIA SEVERITY
Insomnia
Threshold Precipitating
Predisposing
TIME
Figure 1 Characteristics contributing to insomnia over time. Source: Adapted from Ref. 3.
leads to a new association of the bed/bedroom with not sleeping. In this model, individuals’
attempts to cope with insomnia may maintain and actually exacerbate the disorder through
strengthening such maladaptive associations. For example, an individual with insomnia may
lie in bed awake for long periods of time, believing it is better to get rest. Or the individual
may get up and do work in the bedroom when sleep is elusive. In both cases, the behavior
results in a lower likelihood that the sleep stimuli of the bed/bedroom will elicit the desired
response of falling asleep. The intervention arising from this theory is another that meets the
American Psychological Association criteria for a well-validated treatment (4), as described in
chapter 24.
NEUROCOGNITIVE MODEL
The neurocognitive model (9) extends behavioral models by explicitly allowing for the possibil-
ity that conditioned arousal may act as a maintaining factor in insomnia. In this model, arousal
includes somatic, cognitive, and cortical arousal. Somatic arousal corresponds to measures of
metabolic rate, cognitive arousal typically refers to mental constructs like worry rumination,
and cortical arousal refers to the level of cortical activation [primarily electroencephalography
(EEG) activity, though cortical arousal may also include all CNS arousal]. Cortical arousal is
hypothesized to occur as a result of classical conditioning and can lead to abnormal levels of
sensory and information processing and long-term memory formation.
In this model, the cortical arousal phenomena become linked to sleep continuity
disturbance and/or sleep state misperception (i.e., the individual judging that he was awake
for a longer duration than actually occurred). In particular, enhanced sensory processing
around sleep onset and during NREM sleep is thought to make the individual vulnerable
to environmental stimuli (e.g., a noise outside on the street) interfering with sleep initiation
and/or maintenance. Enhanced information processing (detection of, and discrimination
between, stimuli and the formation of a short-term memory of the stimulating event) during
NREM sleep may blur the distinction between sleep and wakefulness. That is, one cue for
“knowing” that one is asleep is the lack of awareness for events occurring during sleep.
Enhanced information processing may therefore account for the tendency in insomnia to
judge PSG sleep as wakefulness (i.e., sleep state misperception). Finally, enhanced long-term
memory (e.g., recollection of a stimulating event hours after its occurrence) around sleep onset
and during NREM sleep may interfere with the subjective experience of sleep initiation and
duration. Typically individuals cannot recall information from periods immediately prior to
sleep, during sleep, or during brief arousals from sleep. An enhanced ability to encode and
retrieve information in insomnia would thus be expected to influence judgments about sleep
latency, wakefulness after sleep onset, and sleep duration.
c05 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:21 Char Count=
COGNITIVE MODEL
According to Harvey’s cognitive model of insomnia, insomnia is maintained by a cascade of
cognitive processes that operate at night and during the day. The five key cognitive processes
that comprise the cascade are worry (accompanied by arousal and distress), selective attention
and monitoring, misperception of sleep and daytime deficits, unhelpful beliefs, and counter-
productive safety behaviors (13). Two assumptions are made. First, the cognitive processes that
are proposed to operate at night apply equally to difficulty getting to sleep at the beginning of
the night and to difficulty getting back to sleep after waking during the night and to waking too
early in the morning. Second, the maintaining processes described can “kick in” at any point in
the model and as a consequence of either daytime or nighttime experiences.
This cognitive model suggests that worry in the night activates the sympathetic nervous
system thereby triggering physiological arousal and distress. This combination of worry, arousal,
and distress plunges the individual into an anxiety state (10). Research in cognitive psychology
indicates that when one is anxious the range of stimuli in the environment that is attended to
narrows (14) and attention is preferentially directed toward potential threats (15). On this basis
c05 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:21 Char Count=
the model suggests that the anxious state leads people with insomnia to narrow their attention
and selectively attend to or monitor for sleep-related threats that might be internal stimuli (e.g.,
bodily sensations) and/or external stimuli (e.g., clock). As monitoring for threat increases the
chance of detecting random and meaningless cues that can then be misinterpreted (16) and
the aroused state means that there is likely to be an abundance of body sensations present to
be detected, monitoring is likely to provide further cause for worry. Hence, a vicious cycle is
established.
This model suggests that this cycle may help explain the robust finding that some indi-
viduals with insomnia overestimate the extent to which their sleep is inadequate. For example,
in the time perception literature time seems longer when the number of units of information
processed per unit of time increases (17). Hence, worry may distort the perception of how
long it is taking to get to sleep (18). Monitoring increases the chance of detecting random and
meaningless cues. These may then be misinterpreted as indicative of threat (16,19) that, in turn,
contributes to misperception of sleep and/or daytime functioning. Of course, if an individual
perceives they have not slept adequately a further cause for worry is established, thus fuelling
the vicious cycle.
It is proposed that there are two additional exacerbating processes operating at night. First,
unhelpful beliefs about sleep are likely to exacerbate worry (7). Second, in an attempt to cope
with the escalating anxiety caused by the processes described, individuals with insomnia often
make use of safety behaviors (20), such as drinking alcohol to reduce anxiety and promote sleep
onset. A safety behavior is an overt or covert action that is adopted to avoid feared outcomes.
The problem is that these behaviors (i) prevent the person experiencing disconfirmation of their
unrealistic beliefs and (ii) may make the feared outcome more likely to occur (20).
The same cognitive processes are suggested to be operating during the day. For example,
people with insomnia often worry during the day that they have not obtained sufficient sleep.
This worry, in turn, triggers arousal and distress, selective attention and monitoring for sleep-
related threats, misperception and the use of counterproductive safety behaviors. Each of these
processes serves to maintain the insomnia. In addition, worry, arousal, and distress are likely to
interfere with satisfying and effective daytime performance. Further, the use of safety behaviors
can contribute to distress and worsening of sleep, as well as preventing disconfirmation of
unhelpful beliefs.
It is suggested that these processes culminate in a real sleep deficit. Three points are relevant
here. First, when an individual suffers from a real sleep deficit it is conceived to be the product of
escalating anxiety that is not conducive to sleep onset (10) nor to effective daytime performance
(21). Second, misperception of sleep and a real sleep deficit can coexist. An individual may
report sleeping only two hours on a night but on polysomnography is shown to have slept
for four hours. In this example, the individual is misperceiving his sleep and suffering from
a serious real sleep deficit. Third, for the occasional individual with insomnia who thinks he
is not getting enough sleep but, in reality, is sleeping sufficiently (sleep state misperception),
the cognitive model suggests that such individuals will be at grave risk of getting trapped
into becoming progressively more absorbed by and anxious about their sleep problem. The
unfortunate consequence of this is that they are at high risk of developing a real sleep deficit.
The process of empirically validating predictions from this model is ongoing (see Ref. 22 for a
summary of evidence to date) and a treatment derived from the model has preliminary support
in an open trial (23).
CONTEXTUAL/ENVIRONMENTAL FACTORS
Context likely underlies the expression of and interaction between all the psychological factors
we have discussed, including behavior, cognition, affect, and personality. The role of context,
or environmental factors, is also understudied. Examples of potentially important contextual
c05 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:21 Char Count=
factors include bed partners who may interfere with each other’s sleep through snoring, move-
ment, or dyssynchronous bedtimes. Other contextual factors include the noise and safety levels
of the environment that could lead to sleep disturbance and/or hypervigilance for threat.
Increased use of technology and busier schedules may also impact insomnia through increased
stress and poorer sleep habits.
CONCLUSION
A number of the important psychological models have been reviewed in this chapter, ranging
from behavioral to cognitive to several hybrid cognitive-behavioral models. These substantial
theoretical contributions have led to the development of numerous efficacious treatments (4),
but more work remains. In particular, future research on the role of emotion, personality, and
contextual factors could pave the way for even more successful treatments.
REFERENCES
1. Spielman AJ, Caruso LS, Glovinsky PB. A behavioral perspective on insomnia treatment. Psychiatr
Clin North Am 1987; 10:541–553.
2. Spielman AJ. Assessment of insomnia. Clin Psychol Rev 1986; 6:11–25.
3. Glovinsky P, Spielman A. The insomnia answer: a personalized program for identifying and overcom-
ing the three types of insomnia. New York: Penguin, 2006.
4. Morin CM, Bootzin RR, Buysse DJ, et al. Psychological and behavioral treatment of insomnia: an
update of recent evidence (1998–2004). Sleep 2006; 29:1396–1406.
5. Spielman AJ, Saskin P, Thorpy MJ. Treatment of chronic insomnia by restriction of time in bed. Sleep
1987; 10:45–56.
6. Bootzin RR. Stimulus control treatment for insomnia. In: Proceedings of the American Psychological
Association; 1972; Washington, D.C., 7:395–396.
7. Morin CM. Insomnia: Psychological Assessment and Management. New York: Guilford Press, 1993.
8. Lundh L-G, Broman J-E. Insomnia as an interaction between sleep-interfering and sleep-interpreting
processes. J Psychosom Res 2000; 49:299–310.
9. Perlis ML, Giles DE, Mendelson WB, et al. Psychophysiological insomnia: the behavioural model and
a neurocognitive perspective. J Sleep Res 1997; 6:179–188.
10. Espie CA. Insomnia: conceptual issues in the development, persistence, and treatment of sleep disorder
in adults. Annu Rev Psychol 2002; 53:215–243.
11. Espie CA, Broomfield NM, MacMahon KM, et al. The attention-intention-effort pathway in the devel-
opment of psychophysiologic insomnia: a theoretical review. Sleep Med Rev 2006; 10:215–245.
12. Semler CN, Harvey AG. Monitoring of sleep-related threat in primary insomnia: development and
validation of the sleep associated monitoring inventory (SAMI). Psychosom Med 2004; 66:242–250.
13. Harvey AG. A cognitive model of insomnia. Behav Res Ther 2002; 40:869–894.
14. Easterbrook JA. The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychol Rev
1959; 66:183–201.
15. Dalgleish T, Watts FN. Biases of attention and memory in disorders of anxiety and depression. Clin
Psychol Rev 1990; 10:589–604.
16. Clark DM. Anxiety disorders: why they persist and how to treat them. Behav Res Ther 1999; 37(suppl
1):S5–S27.
17. Thomas EA, Cantor NE. On the duality of simultaneous time and size perception. Percept Psychophys
1975; 18:44–48.
18. Borkovec TD. Insomnia. J Consult Clin Psychol 1982; 50:880–895.
19. Clark DM, Salkovskis PM, Ost L-G, et al. Misinterpretation of body sensations in panic disorder. J
Consult Clin Psychol 1997; 65:203–213.
20. Salkovskis PM. The importance of behaviour in the maintenance of anxiety and panic: a cognitive
account. Behav Psychother 1991; 19:6–19.
21. Eysenck MW. Attention and Arousal, Cognition and Performance. Berlin/New York: Springer-Verlag,
1982.
22. Harvey AG. A cognitive theory of and therapy for chronic insomnia. J Cogn Psychother Int Q 2005;
19:41–60.
23. Harvey AG. An open trial of cognitive therapy for insomnia. Behav Res Ther 2007; 45:2491–2501.
24. Rottenberg J, Johnson SL. Emotion and Psychopathology: Bridging Affective and Clinical Science.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 2007.
25. Gross JJ. The emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative review. Rev Gen Psychol 1998;
2:271–299.
c05 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:21 Char Count=
26. Benca RM, Obermeyer WH, Thisted RA, et al. Sleep and psychiatric disorders: a meta-analysis. Arch
Gen Psychiatry 1992; 49:651–668.
27. Harvey AG. Sleep and circadian rhythms in bipolar disorder: seeking synchrony, harmony, and regu-
lation. Am J Psychiatry 2008; 165:820–829.
28. Morin CM, Rodrigue S, Ivers H. Role of stress, arousal, and coping skills in primary insomnia.
Psychosom Med 2003; 65:259–267.
29. Buysse DJ, Thompson W, Scott J, et al. Daytime symptoms in primary insomnia: a prospective analysis
using ecological momentary assessment. Sleep Med 2007; 8:198–208.
30. Riedel BW, Lichstein KL. Insomnia and daytime functioning. Sleep Med Rev 2000; 4:277–298.
31. Yoo S, Gujar N, Hu P, et al. The human emotional brain without sleep—a prefrontal amygdala
disconnect. Curr Biol 2007; 17:877–878.
32. Adam K, Tomeny M, Oswald I. Physiological and psychological differences between good and poor
sleepers. J Psychiatr Res 1986; 20:301–316.
33. Monroe LJ. Psychological and physiological differences between good and poor sleepers. J Abnorm
Psychol 1967; 72:255–264.
34. Schneider-Helmert D. Twenty-four-hour sleep-wake function and personality patterns in chronic
insomniacs and healthy controls. Sleep 1987; 10:452–462.
35. Kales A, Caldwell AB, Soldatos CR, et al. Biopsychobehavioral correlates of insomnia. II. Pattern
specificity and consistency with the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory. Psychosom Med
1983; 45:341–356.
IHBK059-06 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:24 Char Count=
Mario Terzano
Department of Neurology, University of Parma, Parma, Italy
Kimberly Cote
Department of Psychology, Brock University, St. Catharines, Ontario, Canada
Dieter Riemann
Center for Sleep Research and Sleep Medicine, Department of Psychiatry, Freiburg University, Freiburg, Germany
INTRODUCTION
With the advent of the electroencephalographic study of sleep (1–3) and the more comprehensive
methodology of polysomnography (4), the era of the objective assessment of sleep had arrived.
Along with this technology, came the promise that sleep related complaints could be accounted
for by abnormal physiologic function during sleep. That is, it was believed that the simultaneous
and long duration monitoring of sleep continuity and sleep architecture along with pulmonary,
cardiac, motor, and cortical function would reveal the abnormalities that account for sleep initia-
tion and maintenance problems, the report of shallow and/or fragmented and/or nonrestorative
sleep, and/or the diurnal complaints of sleepiness and fatigue. To a large extent this promise was
realized for the complaint of shallow and/or fragmented and/or nonrestorative sleep, and/or
the diurnal complaints of pathologic sleepiness. The polysomnographic study of sleep revealed
that these complaints were, in large measure, directly related to sleep disordered breathing. The
promise, however, was not realized for the complaint of insomnia (sleep initiation and main-
tenance problems). In fact, the very effort to objectively measure sleep continuity disturbance1
raised more questions than it answered. That is, the objective assessment of sleep with electroen-
cephalography brought into sharp resolution that remarkable discrepancies existed between the
subjective and objective measure of sleep latency, wake after sleep onset, number of awakenings,
and total sleep time (4,5). Further, no evidence was obtained for physiologic abnormalities that
clearly accounted for difficulty initiating or maintaining sleep (e.g., abnormal REM pressure,
slow wave sleep deficiency, sleep onset related central apneic events, sleep induced cardiac or
CNS irregularities, etc.). At best some of these factors were found to be indicative of “Secondary
Insomnia” and particularly the sleep of patients with major depression (6,7).
The lack of obvious abnormalities resulted in a new program of research, one that was
focused on the effort to document that physiologic and/or cognitive hyperarousal is pathog-
nomic for primary insomnia2 . This program of research yielded a variety of findings and the-
ories, all of which provide partial support for the “hyperarousal hypotheses” (8). Interestingly,
these efforts also gave rise to a fundamentally new concept with respect to the etiology and
pathophysiology of insomnia—the concept that the primary defect in primary insomnia may
be less related to “hyperarousal” and more related to “the failure to inhibit wakefulness” (9).
1 Sleep Continuity refers to the collection of variables that correspond to sleep initiation and maintenance,
including Sleep Latency (SL), Wake After Sleep Onset (WASO), Number of Awakenings (NWAK), and Total Sleep
Time (TST). While the term is not formally part of the sleep lexicon, it has the heuristic value of being a global
term whose meaning may be contrasted with the class of variables that correspond to “Sleep Architecture”.
2 For the purposes of this chapter, the term “primary insomnia” is meant to be synonymous with psychophysio-
logic insomnia.
IHBK059-06 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:24 Char Count=
In this chapter, information will be provided regarding the findings from all of the
EEG based technologies including those from Polysomnography (PSG), Quantitative Elec-
troencephalography (QEEG), Cyclic Alternating Patterns analysis (CAPs) and Event-Related
Potentials (ERPs).
52 PERLIS ET AL.
45
40
35
30
25
20
15
10
5
0
PI MDD GS
30
20
10
0
–10
–20
–30
–40
–50
PI MDD GS
Figure 1 In both figures, time in minutes are difference scores between the group average subjective values
and the objective values. For example, if subjective SL = 90 and the objective SL = 30 then the difference score if
60 minutes. Accordingly, these difference scores represent the absolute magnitude of the “sub-ob discrepancies”
as seen in PI, MDD and GS subjects.
Third, insomnia’s characterization, prior to and after 1990, as a disorder of initiation and
maintenance of sleep (DIMS) came with the suggestion that sleep continuity disturbance was
the (vs. a) defining attribute of insomnia and thus many PSG studies of insomnia simply did
not contain sleep architecture data.
With these caveats in mind, the PSG findings in insomnia from 1975 to 1989 insomnia
were characterized by Benca and colleagues in 1992 (7). This metaanalytic study summarized
the results from 25 investigations in patients with insomnia. The findings for patients with
insomnia, and nine other psychiatric patient classifications, were expressed as whether they
significant differed from values calculated for healthy good sleepers. The results from this
study suggested that patients with insomnia exhibit reduced SWS%. REM% and REM latency
were found to be within normal limits. Data for stage 1 and stage 2 were not reported.
In order to assess whether these results are also typical of a more modern cohort, a set
of 17 studies from 1994 to the present was reviewed. All the studies included in this review
(1) used ICSD or DSM-IV definitions of insomnia, (2) reported one or more sleep architectural
findings, and (3) indicated that patients were medication free at the time of study. Most of
the studies for this review had small samples although there were two investigations with
samples of 100 or more subjects (27,28). Table 1 provides a list of these studies, their findings,
and study related information. The sleep architecture findings are summarized below by stage
of sleep.
Stage 1%: One study reported a statistical tendency (p < 0.10) for stage 1% to be increased in
patients with primary insomnia as compared to good sleepers (28). Fifteen studies report
no difference and one study does not provide data (29).
IHBK059-06 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:24 Char Count=
Stage 2: One study reported differences for this stage of sleep. Bonnet & Arand (1995) (30) found
that patients with insomnia exhibited a slight decrease in stage 2 percent. Fourteen studies
report no difference and two studies do not provide these data (29,31).
SWS: Two studies describe a reduction of SWS percent in insomniacs compared to controls
(29,32). Fourteen studies report no difference and one study did not provide these
data (33).
REM: Three studies described significantly reduced REM sleep percent in insomnia (28,30,34).
Eleven studies report no difference and three studies do not provide these data (29,31,33).
REM latency: One study described a statistical tendency for REM latency to be prolonged in
patients with insomnia (30). Seven studies report no difference (27–29,31,32,35–37) and
nine studies do not provide these data.
Apart from naturalistic studies, there are two investigations that probe the issue of SWS
deficiency and/or sleep homeostasis dysregulation as it occurs in patients with insomnia in
response to sleep deprivation. Besset and colleagues (38) conducted a study in which patients
with primary insomnia were subjected to a partial sleep deprivation protocol (three hour sleep)
and then allowed a recovery sleep period of 10 hour on the following night. Both the PI
and GS subjects exhibited increase in total sleep time and SWS percent. When compared to
good sleepers, the patients with PI exhibited a smaller increase in total sleep time (although a
comparable percent) and a smaller increase in SWS minutes (although a comparable percent).
Stepanski et al. (2000) (39) found that patients with primary insomnia, when sleep deprived for
approximately 36 hours, exhibited larger increases in total sleep time, but a smaller percentage
increase in SWS than good sleepers.
In sum, the most conservative interpretation of the findings to date is that patients with
insomnia do not exhibit reliably altered sleep architecture. If trends exist, they do so as cohort
effects or relative to probative conditions. With respect to cohort effects, the earlier generation
of studies tended to show reduced slow wave sleep percent as a feature of insomnia while
the later generation studies tend to show reduced REM percent. With respect to probative
conditions, there is mixed evidence that patients with insomnia exhibit a reduced capacity to
IHBK059-06 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:24 Char Count=
54 PERLIS ET AL.
generate SWS in response to sleep loss. Given these findings, clearly further research is needed
where such studies may benefit by having large samples, the use of both absolute and percent
time (differences may exist for the former where they do not for the latter), the application of
quantitative electroencephalographic techniques to quantify SWS activity, and/or the use of
challenge paradigms to resolve underlying abnormalities.
QUANTITATIVE ELECTROENCEPHALOGRAPHY
Quantitative electroencephalography (QEEG) refers to a group of mathematical techniques
used for detecting periodicities within time series data. The techniques include, but are not
limited to, power spectral analysis (PSA) and digital period analysis (DPA). As employed
within electroencephalography, the technique is used to deconstruct complex waveforms into
their constituent frequencies. In the case of PSA, quantification is accomplished by determining
the amount of voltage that occurs per bin and/or prespecified bandwidth. This technique and
related techniques have been extensively used to quantify delta frequency EEG in good sleepers
(during normal sleep or manipulations like constant routine schedules or sleep deprivation).
The technique has also been used to quantify high frequency EEG activity in the 14 to 45 Hz
range in both good sleepers and in patients with insomnia.
High frequency EEG was first observed in the waking state by Berger in 1937. He hypothe-
sized that such activity, observed during mental arithmetic, must be a “material concomitant of
mental processes” (40). In the last two decades, there has been substantial work indicating that
during wakefulness high frequency EEG activity in the beta (14–35 Hz) and gamma (35–45 Hz)
ranges is associated with attention in animals [e.g., (41,42)] and with attention, perception, and,
more generally, cognitive function in humans (43–58).
Recently, eight investigations have demonstrated that patients with insomnia exhibit an
unusual amount of beta EEG activity during polysomnographically-monitored sleep (59–66).
The majority of the investigations found that the increase in beta activity was apparent in
patients with insomnia compared to good sleeper controls for time period’s at/around sleep
onset and/or during NREM sleep. These findings were based on (1) EEG measurements from
central sites using monopolar or bipolar measures that included C3, Cz or C4, and O2, (2) relative,
as opposed to absolute, measures of power density, and (3) definitions of beta activity ranging
from 14 to 32 Hz. Three studies provided evidence that increased beta activity during sleep
occurs specifically in association with primary, as opposed to secondary, insomnia (63,64,66).
Two studies provided data to suggest that patients with insomnia also have more beta activity
during REM sleep (60,61). One study indicated that beta activity varies with clinical course given
nonpharmacological interventions (65). The most recent study of high frequency EEG activity
in patients with insomnia suggests that women with primary insomnia, but not men, showed
increased high-frequency and low-frequency EEG activity during NREM sleep compared to
good sleepers. Figure 2 represents the all night distribution of delta and beta activity in good
sleepers and in patients with primary insomnia.
While the data acquired from this measurement strategy appear to support the perspec-
tive that cortical arousal may be a biomarker for insomnia (and this is theoretically appealing
to the extent that the increased occurrence of beta and gamma activity is thought to be permis-
sive of increased sensory and information processing), the lack of replication across large scale
contemporary investigations (e.g., 67) and unpublished studies (Buysse D, personal commu-
nication, 2005; Perlis M, personal communication, 2005) suggests that this approach has some
limitations. In our hands, the occurrence of beta and gamma activity varies not only with trait
considerations (diagnostic category) but also appears to be mediated/moderated by a variety
of factors including:
r Whether or not the patient experiences a first night effect or a reverse-first night effect
(a precedent also exists in the published literature) (e.g., 68)
r Whether or not the patient experiences a good night’s sleep in relation to the amount of prior
sleep loss or amount of prior wakefulness (i.e., increased homeostatic pressure for sleep)
r The extent to which circadian dysrhythmia contributes to the insomnia (e.g., delayed sleep
phase syndrome subjects would be expected to show more beta/gamma activity when
attempting to sleep in a nonpreferred sleep phase)
IHBK059-06 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:24 Char Count=
100 10
80 8
GOOD
BETA1
DELTA
SLEEPER 60 6
CONTROLS 40 4
20 2
0 0 0
42
84
126
168
210
252
294
336
378
420
462
504
546
588
630
672
714
756
798
840
882
924
966
SLEEP ONSET
100 10
80 8
BETA1
DELTA
PRIMARY 60 6
INSOMNIA 40 4
20 2
0 0
0
45
90
135
180
225
270
315
360
405
450
495
540
585
630
675
720
765
810
856
901
949
994
SLEEP ONSET DELTA BETA1
Figure 2 In both figures, (1) the left and right ordinates represent relative power [e.g., (beta power/total power)],
(2) the abscissa represents time (from sleep onset, in 30 second increments, to sleep offset), (3) the black boxes
represent delta power and the white boxes represent beta power, (4) the data are average plots for nine individuals
per group.
r Technical considerations regarding how the EEG signals are acquired (e.g., signals transmit-
ted over long cable runs may significantly alter the power spectrum)
r Ambient noise in the recording/sleep environment (e.g., sound, light, temperature, etc.)
The extent to which one or more of these factors obscure simple group contrasts under-
scores the need to identify a biomarker, probe, or challenge paradigm that provides for stable
group characterizations of the phenomena that contribute to the occurrence of and/or severity
of insomnia.
56 PERLIS ET AL.
NCAP
Bipolar EOG
Fp2-F4
F4-C4
C4-P4
P4-02
C3-A2
EMG
Respiratory Flow
EKG
CAP
A B A B A B A B
Bipolar EOG
Fp2-F4
F4-C4
C4-P4
P4-02
C3-A2
EMG
Respiratory Flow
EKG
Figure 3 The letter designations “A” and “B” represent phase A and B CAPS. Fp2-F4, F4-C4, C4-P4, P4–02,
are bipolar EEG derivations. C3-A2 is a monopolar derivation (typically used for sleep scoring) EMG is mentalis
electromyogram EKG is an electrocardiograph tracing.
In the case of patients with insomnia (74), it was found that, under placebo conditions, patients
exhibited a significantly higher CAP rate than did controls (evident for subtypes A1 and A2 but
not A3). Treatment with several medications [zolpidem (10 mg), triazolam (0.25 mg), zopiclone
(7.5 mg), brotizolam (0.25 mg)] significantly reduced CAP rates, although only for subtypes
A1 and A2 (74). Finally, sleep quality was significantly improved by the use of hypnotics, and
improved sleep quality was significantly correlated with CAP rate.
These data suggest that “hyperarousal” within NREM sleep can be assessed in terms of
CAP rate and that the association of CAP rate with sleep quality suggests that this form of
arousal during sleep (or processes associated with this form of arousal) may undermine sleep’s
capacity to be restorative.
Based on ERP studies in good sleepers (77,80,81,85–94) one would expect signs of hyper-
arousal and/or deficits in inhibition in patients with insomnia to appear as larger N1, smaller
P2, and larger P300 amplitudes in wakefulness. One would also expect N350 to be smaller in
poor sleepers. Figure 4 represents the transition from wakefulness to sleep and the changes that
one would expect in patients with chronic insomnia.
To date, there have been two types of studies applying ERPs to investigate the phenomena
of insomnia: (1) a number of studies have examined information processing in patients with
insomnia during wakefulness (95–99) (2) two studies have investigated ERPs at sleep onset or
during early NREM sleep (34,100).
58 PERLIS ET AL.
−10.0 −10.0
−7.5 −7.5
N1 N1
−5.0 −5.0
−2.5 −2.5
µV 0.0 µV 0.0
2.5 2.5
5.0 P2 5.0 P2
7.5 7.5
10.0 10.0 P300
−100.0 0.0 100.0 200.0 300.0 400.0 500.0 600.0 −100.0 0.0 100.0 200.0 300.0 400.0 500.0 600.0
ms ms
N350 N350
−10.0 −10.0
−7.5 −7.5
−5.0 N1 −5.0 N1
−2.5 −2.5
µV 0.0 µV 0.0
2.5 2.5
5.0 5.0
P2 P2 P300
7.5 7.5
10.0 10.0
−100.0 0.0 100.0 200.0 300.0 400.0 500.0 600.0 −100.0 0.0 100.0 200.0 300.0 400.0 500.0 600.0
ms ms
GS
PI
Figure 4 Theoretical ERP waveforms following standard and deviant stimuli in good sleepers (GS) and
patients with primary insomnia (PI) in an alert waking state (1A and 2A) and at sleep onset (1B and 2B).
Panel 1A: In attentive wakefulness, GS have a large N1 and a small P2 peak. PIs should have a larger N1 and
smaller P2 depicting heightened attention. PIs may also show the sleep-related N350 reflecting sleepiness. Panel
1B: At sleep onset in GS, N1 decreases to baseline while P2 increases in amplitude reflecting that attention is
disengaged. For PIs, it is expected that N1 might remain large and P2 small (similar to wakefulness) reflecting
failure to inhibit. Research shows a smaller N350 emerging at sleep onset in PIs compared to controls, reflecting
a failure of sleep inhibition processes. Panel 2A: N1 and P2 appear similar following the standard and deviant
stimuli in GS, but it is possible that early sensory processing of the deviant may differ for PIs. In GS, the large
P300 reflects conscious detection of the deviant in waking-attend conditions. Waking P300 has been reported by
Devoto et al to be larger in amplitude for PIs compared to GS reflecting hyperarousal. Panel 2B: In GS, P300 is
diminished at sleep onset (i.e., present in stage 1 when there is a behavioral response but absent in stage 2). For
PIs, it is theorized that P300 may be larger in early or late stage 1 or remain present in stage 2 sleep.
were investigated during a single night. Participants were instructed to ignore tones in a classic
oddball paradigm. In-wake, N1 to the standard tone was larger for patients than controls on
the fourth night at Pz, and larger on the fourth morning at both Cz and Pz sites. There were no
P2 or P300 differences. These data are in agreement with some previous reports of increased N1
in wakefulness reflecting hyperarousal and attentiveness (98,100). To examine the sleep onset
period, ERPs were averaged to stimuli presented from lights out to any sign of stage 2 sleep,
and the K-complexes were appropriately excluded from averages. The authors observed that
N350 was smaller in patients with insomnia compared to good sleepers at sleep onset, which
reflects a failure of the inhibition processes needed to initiate sleep. In addition, the authors
observed a smaller N1 to standards at Cz, and a larger P2 to deviants at Pz in patients with
IHBK059-06 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:24 Char Count=
insomnia compared to good sleepers. These results are contrary to what would be expected
based on selective attention research, (77) and what has been found to date with waking ERPs
in patients with insomnia. Replication is necessary given that only a single sleep onset period
was observed, ERPs were collapsed across wake and stage 1, and results were not consistent at
all sites or apparent in both categories of stimuli.
The data from these two reports of ERPs at sleep onset and during early NREM sleep are in
agreement in some respects, but not others. It is thus important that this area be pursued through
further study. It is necessary that researchers collect enough ERP trials for reliable averaging (e.g.,
through more stimuli delivered, and more subjects or multiple nights). The findings on N350 are
most compelling to date; N350 may be a sensitive metric of deficits in information processing
associated with sleep initiation problems in particular. It will also be important to carefully select
patient samples, by ensuring they do not have comorbid medical conditions and by system-
atically investigating differences between qualitatively different types of insomnia (e.g., sleep
onset versus maintenance complaints). Finally, the sleep period itself needs to be more system-
atically investigated, particularly for those with sleep maintenance complaints, to investigate
information processing in NREM stages at various times of night and in REM sleep periods.
CONCLUDING COMMENTS
In many ways, the assessment of the sleep EEG in primary insomnia is in its infancy. This is
ironic in that that insomnia is likely to be the oldest established diagnostic entity of the various
sleep disorders and it was likely the first to be studied with most, if not all, EEG techniques.
This said, it is an inescapable fact that each methodological approach has been applied to the
problem of insomnia in only a few dedicated studies where each of the studies are generally
on very small samples. Thus, at this point in time, it is difficult to definitively say whether
subjects with primary insomnia reliably exhibit any form of abnormal sleep EEG and it is
nearly impossible to make claims about how the types (e.g., psychophysiologic, paradoxical,
idiopathic insomnia) subtypes (i.e., initial, middle, late or mixed insomnia) or the comorbid
insomnias (insomnia comorbid with psychiatric or medical illnesses) are similar to, or different
from, primary insomnia on EEG measures.
At the present time, the following claims can be made, albeit tentatively
r Sleep continuity disturbance as measured by PSG tends to be more normal than is measured
by self-report. This suggests that there is a tendency to report more insomnia severity
than exists or that patients are reporting phenomena that are not properly assessed by
polysomnography (102).
r Sleep architecture does not appear reliably altered in patients with primary insomnia, and to
the extent that trends are evident they exist as modest alterations to SWS and REM sleep. This
suggests that systems that are responsible for the regulation of these forms of sleep are not
related to sleep induction and maintenance. This said it seems likely that SWS (as it related
to sleep homeostasis) would be altered in insomnia (103). Resolution of effects within this
domain may require more than naturalistic studies but instead challenge paradigms that use
sleep deprivation, pharmacologic probes or treatment as ways of assessing the functioning
of these systems.
r QEEG abnormalities have been reported where the primary finding is in terms of elevated
beta/gamma frequency activity around sleep onset and during NREM sleep (104). These
findings have been interpreted as supporting that hyperarousal is a primary etiologic factor
in insomnia.
r CAP irregularities have been shown to be correlated with self-reported sleep quality and to
be associated with treatment outcome. These findings have been interpreted as supporting
that hyperarousal is a primary etiologic factor in insomnia.
r ERP activity appears to be altered in patients with insomnia. As compared to healthy controls,
patients with primary insomnia (during sleep onset and/or early NREM sleep) tend to
exhibit larger N1 and smaller P2 amplitudes to deviant tones, and smaller N350 amplitudes
to standard tones. These findings suggest that insomnia (around sleep onset and during
NREM sleep) is characterized by both elevated CNS activation and by an attenuation of the
processes that serve to inhibit sensory and information processing.
IHBK059-06 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:24 Char Count=
60 PERLIS ET AL.
Given the tenuousness of the present sleep EEG findings, further studies are clearly
indicated. The problem with the “call” for further research is that there is a fundamental
disagreement about whether insomnia merits targeted research. The lack of merit owes to a
variety of factors including (1) the widespread belief that insomnia is only a symptom; and
(2) that insomnia, when considered a primary disorder, does not have significant medical,
psychiatric, personal or social consequences.
The former is a conceptual issue that has been under siege from within the sleep commu-
nity for nearly a decade (105,106). At present it is commonly held (within the sleep community)
that insomnia is disorder which, when it occurs with other medical and/or psychiatric illness,
is best characterized as a comorbid disorder. This change in perspective, which has yet to gain
wide spread acceptance outside the sleep community, carries with it the implication that fur-
ther research is warranted and likely to be fruitful with respect to the delineation of specific
biological correlates.
The latter is an empirical issue. That is, there needs to be enough research that demonstrates
the medical, psychiatric, personal or social effects of insomnia to persuade the larger science and
medical communities of the importance of conducting the research needed to reach a detailed
consensus view on the pathophysiology of insomnia based on sleep EEG techniques as well
as other approaches ranging from animal models to functional and anatomical imaging for
both global and regional activation in human subjects (107–109) and the profiling of specific
neurotransmitter system activity (110).
REFERENCES
1. Davis H, Davis PA, Loomis AL, et al. Human brain potentials during the onset of sleep. Neurophysiol
1937; 1:24–38.
2. Dement WC, Kleitman N. Cyclic variations in EEG during sleep and their relation to eye movements,
body motility and dreaming. Neurophysiology 1957; 9:673–690.
3. Rechtschaffen A, Kales A. A Manual of Standardized Terminology, Techniques and Scoring System
for Sleep Stages of Human Subjects. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office, 1968.
4. Carskadon M, Rechtschaffen A. Monitoring and staging human sleep. In: Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement
WC, eds. Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine. Philadelphia, PA: Elsevier-Saunders, 2006:
665–682
5. Monroe LJ. Psychological and physiological differences between good and poor sleepers. J Abnorm
Psychol 1967; 72:255–264.
6. Reynolds CF, Kupfer DJ. Sleep research in affective illness: State of the art circa 1987. [Review]. Sleep
1987; 10:199–215.
7. Benca RM, Obermeyer WH, Thisted RA, et al. Sleep and psychiatric disorders: A meta-analysis. Arch
Gen Psychiatry 1992; 49:651–668.
8. Perlis ML, Pigeon W, Smith MT. Etiology and pathophysiology of insomnia. In: Kryger M, Roth T,
Dement W, eds. Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine, 4th Edn. Philadelphia, PA: W.B. Saunders
Company, 2005:714–725.
9. Espie CA, Broomfield NM, MacMahon KMA, et al. The attention-intention-effort pathway in the
development of Psychophysiologic Insomnia: An invited theoretical review. Sleep Med Rev 2006;
10:215–245.
10. Edinger JD, Fins A. The distribution and clinical significance of sleep time misperceptions among
insomniacs. Sleep 1995; 18:232–239.
11. Coates T, Killen J, Silberman S, et al. Cognitive activity, sleep disturbance, and stage specific differ-
ences between recorded and reported sleep. Psychophysiology 1983; 20:243.
12. Coates TJ, Killen J, George J, et al. Estimating sleep parameters: A multitrait-multimethod analysis. J
Consult Clin Psychol 1982; 50:345–352.
13. Monroe L, Marks P. MMPI differences between adolescent poor and good sleepers. J Consult Clin
Psychol 1977; 45:151–152.
14. Lutz T, Roth T, Kramer M, et al. The relationship between objective and subjective evaluations of
sleep in insomniacs. Sleep Res 1977; 6:178.
15. Frankel BL, Coursey R, Buchbinder R, et al. Recorded and reported sleep in primary chronic insomnia.
Arch Gen Psuchiatry 1976; 33:615–623.
16. Rechtschaffen A, Monroe L. Laboratory studies of insomnia. In: Kales A, ed. Sleep: Physiology and
Pathology. Philadelphia, PA: J.B. Lippincott Company, 1969:158–169.
IHBK059-06 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:24 Char Count=
17. Mercer JD, Bootzin RR, Lack LC. Insomniacs’ perception of wake instead of sleep. Sleep 2002; 25:
564–571.
18. Bixler E, Kales A, Leo I, et al. A comparison of subjective estimates and objective sleep laboratory
findings in insomnia patients. Sleep Res 1973; 2:143.
19. Perlis ML, Smith MT, Orff HJ, et al. Beta/Gamma EEG activity in patients with primary and secondary
insomnia and good sleeper controls. Sleep 2001; 24:110–117.
20. Koukkou M, Lehmann D. EEG and memory storage experiments with humans. Electroencephalogr
Clin Neurophysiol 1968; 25:455–462.
21. Koukkou M, Lehmann D. Human learning and EEG analysis in sleep experiments. In: Jovanovic, UJ,
ed. The Nature of Sleep. Stuttgart, Germany: Fisher Corporation, 1973:146–149.
22. Perlis ML, Kehr EL, Smith MT, et al. Temporal and stagewise distribution of high frequency EEG
activity in patients with primary and secondary insomnia and in good sleeper controls. J Sleep Res
2001; 10:93–104.
23. Mendelson W, Martin J, Stephens H, et al. Effects of flurazapam on sleep, arousal threshold, and
perception of being asleep. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1988; 95:258–262.
24. Mendelson WB, James SP, Garnett D, et al. A psychophysiological study of insomnia. Psychiatry Res
1986; 19:267–284.
25. Borkovec T, Lane T, van Oot P. Phenomenology of sleep among insomniacs and good sleepers:
Wakefulness experience when corticallsy asleep. J Abnorm Psychol 1981; 90:607–609.
26. Mendelson WB. Effects of flurazepam and zolpidem on the perception of sleep in normal volunteers.
Sleep 1995; 18:88–91.
27. Rosa RR, Bonnet MH. Reported chronic insomnia is independent of poor sleep as measured by
electroencephalography. Psychosom Med 2000; 62:474–482.
28. Feige B, Al Shajlawi A, Nissen C, et al. Does REM sleep contribute to subjective wake time in primary
insomnia? A comparison of polysomnographic and subjective sleep in 100 patients. J Sleep Res 2008;
17:180–190.
29. Edinger JD, Glenn DM, Bastian LA, et al. Slow-wave sleep and waking cognitive performance II:
Findings among middle-aged adults with and without insomnia complaints. Physiol Behav 2000;
70:127–134.
30. Bonnet MH, Arand DL. 24-Hour metabolic rate in insomniacs and matched normal sleepers. Sleep
1995; 18:581–588.
31. Edinger JD, Radtke RA, Wohlgemuth WK, et al. The efficacy of cognitive-behavioral therapy for
sleep-maintenance insomnia. Sleep Res 1997; 26:357.
32. Backhaus J, Junghanns K, Born J, et al. Impaired declarative memory consolidation during sleep in
patients with primary insomnia: Influence of sleep architecture and nocturnal cortisol release. Biol
Psychiatry 2006; 60:1324–1330.
33. Bastien CH, Fortier-Brochu E, Rioux I, et al. Cognative Performance and Sleep Quality in the Elderly
Suffering From Chronic Insomnia Relationship Between Objective and Subjective Measures. J Psy-
chosom Res 2003; 54(1):39–49.
34. Bastien CH, St Jean G, Morin CM, et al. Chronic psychophysiological insomnia: Hyperarousal and/or
inhibition deficits? An ERPs investigation. Sleep 2008; 31:887–898.
35. Riemann D, Klein T, Rodenbeck A, et al. Nocturnal cortisol and melatonin secretion in primary
insomnia. Psychiatry Res 2002; 113:17–27.
36. Burgos I, Richter L, Klein T, et al. Increased nocturnal Interleukin-6 excetion in patients with primary
insomnia: A pilot study. Brain Behav Immun 2006; 20:246–253.
37. Riemann D, Klein T, Rodenbeck A, et al. Nocturnal cortisol and melatonin secretion in primary
insomnia. Psychiatry Res 2002; 113:17–27.
38. Besset A, Villemin E, Tafti M, et al. Homeostatic process and sleep spindles in patients with sleep-
maintenance insomnia: Effect of partial (21 h) sleep deprivation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neuro-
physiol 1998; 107:122–132.
39. Stepanski E, Zorick F, Roehrs T, et al. Effects of sleep deprivation on daytime sleepiness in primary
insomnia. Sleep 2000; 23:215–219.
40. Pantev C, Makeig S, Hoke M, et al. Human auditory evoked gamma-band magnetic fields. Proc Natl
Acad Sci U S A 1991; 88:8996–9000.
41. Bouyer JJ, Montaron MF, Rougeul A. Fast fronto-parietal rhythms during combined focused atten-
tive behavior and immobility in cat: Cortical and thalamic localizations. Electroencephalogr Clin
Neurophysiol 1981; 51:244–252.
42. Rougeul A, Bouyer JJ, Dedet JJ, et al. Fast somato-parietal rhythms during combined focal attention
and immobility in baboon and squirrel monkey. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1979; 46:
310–319.
IHBK059-06 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:24 Char Count=
62 PERLIS ET AL.
43. Basar-Eroglu C, Struber D, Schurmann M, et al. Gamma-band responses in the brain: A short review
of psychophysiological correlates and functional significance. [Review] [69 refs]. Int J Psychophysiol
1996; 24:101–112.
44. Galambos R, Makeig S, Talmachoff PJ. A 40-Hz auditory potential recorded from the human scalp.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1981; 78:2643–2647.
45. Goertz R, Jokeit H, Kuchler E. Event related dynamics of 40 Hz electroencephalogram during visual
discrimination task. Int J Neurosci 1994; 79:267–273.
46. Joliot M, Ribary U, Llinas R. Human oscillatory brain activity near 40 Hz coexists with cognitive
temporal binding. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1994; 91:11748–11751.
47. Leung LS. Generation of theta and gamma rhythms in the hippocampus. Neurosci Biobehav Rev
1998; 22:275–290.
48. Llinas R, Ribary U. Coherent 40-Hz oscillation characterizes dream state in humans. Proc Natl Acad
Sci U S A 1993; 90:2078–2081.
49. Loring DW, Ford M, Sheer D. Laterality of 40 Hz EEG and EMG during cognitive performance.
Psychophysiology 1984; 21:34–38.
50. Lutzenberger W, Pulvermuller F, Birbaumer N. Words and pseudowords elicit distinct paterns of
30-Hz EEG responses in humans. Neurosci Lett 1994; 176:115–118.
51. Lutzenberger W, Pulvermuller F, Elbert T, et al. Visual stimulation alters local 40-Hz responses in
humans: An EEG-study. Neurosci Lett 1995; 183:39–42.
52. Makeig S, Inlow M. Lapses in alertness: Coherence of fluctuations in performance and EEG spectrum.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1993; 86:23–35.
53. Pantev C. Evoked and induced gamma-band activity of the human cortex. Brain Topogr 1995; 7:
321–330.
54. Pfurtscheller G, Neuper C, Kalcher J. 40-Hz oscillations during motor behavior in man. Neurosci Lett
1993; 164:179–182.
55. Pulvermuller F, Lutzenberger W, Preissl H, et al. Spectral responses in the gamma-band: Physiological
signs of higher cognitive processes? Neuroreport 1995; 6:2059–2064.
56. Sheer D. Focused arousal and 40 Hz EEG. In: Knight R, Bakker D, eds. The Neuropsychology of
Learning Disorders. Baltimore, MD: University Press, 1976:71–87.
57. Spydell J, Sheer D. Effect of problem solving on right and left hemisphere 40 Hz EEG activity.
Psychophysiology 1982; 19:420–425.
58. Tiitinen H, Sinkkonen J, Reinikainen K, et al. Selective attention enhances the auditory 40-Hz transient
response in humans. Nature 1993; 364:59–60.
59. Buysse DJ, Germain A, Hall ML, et al. EEG spectral analysis in primary insomnia: NREM period
effects and sex differences. Sleep 2008; 31:1673–1682.
60. Freedman R. EEG power in sleep onset insomnia. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1986; 63:
408–413.
61. Merica H, Blois R, Gaillard JM. Spectral characteristics of sleep EEG in chronic insomnia. Eur J
Neurosci 1998; 10:1826–1834.
62. Merica H, Gaillard JM. The EEG of the sleep onset period in insomnia: A discriminant analysis.
Physiol Behav 1992; 52:199–204.
63. Lamarche CH, Ogilvie RD. Electrophysiological changes during the sleep onset period of psy-
chophysiological insomniacs, psychiatric insomniacs, and normal sleepers. Sleep 1997; 20:724–733.
64. Nofzinger EA, Nowell PD, Buysee DJ, et al. Towards a Neurobiology of Sleep Disturbance in Primary
Insomnia and Depression: A Comparison of Subjective, Visually Scored, Period Amplitude, and
Power Spectral Density Sleep Measures. Sleep 1999; 22(supp 1):S99.
65. Jacobs GD, Benson H, Friedman R. Home-based central nervous system assessment of a multifactor
behavioral intervention for chronic sleep-onset insomnia. Behav Ther 1993; 24:159–174.
66. Perlis ML, Andrews PJ, Orff HJ, et al. Fast frequency EEG activity in patients with insomnia and in
good sleeper controls. Sleep 2000; 23 Supplemental #1:A5.
67. Bastien CH, LeBlanc M, Carrier J, et al. Sleep EEG power spectra, insomnia, and chronic use of
benzodiazepines. Sleep 2003; 26:313–317.
68. Curcio G, Ferrara M, Piergianni A, et al. Paradoxes of the first-night effect: A quantitative analysis of
antero-posterior EEG topography. Clin Neurophysiol 2004; 115:1178–1188.
69. Terzano MG, Mancia D, Salati MR, et al. The cyclic alternating pattern as a physiologic component
of normal NREM sleep. Sleep 1985; 8:137–145.
70. Terzano MG, Parrino L. The cyclic alternating pattern (CAP) in human sleep. In: Guilleminault C,
ed. Sleep and its Disorders. Handbook of Clinical Neurophysiology. 2005:79–93.
71. Halasz P, Terzano M, Parrino L, et al. The nature of arousal in sleep. J Sleep Res 2004; 13:1–23.
72. Terzano MG, Parrino L, Smerieri A, et al. Atlas, rules, and recording techniques for the scoring of
cyclic alternating pattern (CAP) in human sleep. Sleep Med 2002; 3:187–199.
IHBK059-06 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:24 Char Count=
73. Terzano MG, Parrino L, Fioriti G, et al. Modifications of sleep structure induced by increasing levels
of acoustic perturbation in normal subjects. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1990; 76:29–38.
74. Terzano MG, Parrino L, Spaggiari MC, et al. CAP variables and arousals as sleep electroencephalo-
gram markers for primary insomnia. Clin Neurophysiol 2003; 114:1715–1723.
75. Handy T. Event-related potentials: A methods handbook. Cambridge: MA: MIT Press.
76. Picton TW, Lins OG, Scherg M. The recording and analysis of event-related potentials. In: Boller F,
Grafman J, eds. Handbook of Neuropsychology vol. 10. Amsterdam: Elsevier. 1995:3–73.
77. Muller-Gass A, Campbell K. Event-related potential measures of the inhibition of information pro-
cessing: I. Selective attention in the waking state. Int J Psychophysiol 2002; 46:177–195.
78. Cote KA. Probing awareness during sleep with the auditory odd-ball paradigm. Int J Psychophysiol
2002; 46:227–241.
79. Bastuji H, Garcia-Larrea L. Evoked potentials as a tool for the investigation of human sleep. Sleep
Med Rev 1999; 3:23–45.
80. Campbell K, Bell I, Bastien C. Evoked potential measures of information processing during natural
sleep. In: Broughton RJ, Ogilvie RD, eds. Sleep, Arousal, & Performance. Boston: Birkhauser, 1992:
88–116.
81. Campbell KB, Colrain IM. Event-related potential measures of the inhibition of information process-
ing: II: The sleep onset period. Int J Psychophysiol 2002; 46:197–214.
82. Naatanen R. The Role of Attention in Auditory Information-Processing As Revealed by Event-Related
Potentials and Other Brain Measures of Cognitive Function. Behav Brain Sci 1990; 13:201–232.
83. Crowley K, Colrain I. A review of the evidence for the P2 being an independent component process:
age, sleep and modality. Clin Neurophysiol 2004; 115:732–744.
84. Picton TW. The P300 wave of the human event-related potential. J Clin Neurophysiol 1992; 9:456–479.
85. Atienza M, Cantero JL, Dominguez-Marin E. Mismatch negativity (MMN): An objective measure of
sensory memory and long-lasting memories during sleep. Int J Psychophysiol 2002; 46:215–225.
86. Campbell K. Event-related potential measures of information processing during sleep. Int J Psy-
chophysiol 2002; 46:159–162.
87. Cote KA, De Lugt DR, Campbell KB. Changes in the scalp topography of event-related potentials
and behavioral responses during the sleep onset period. Psychophysiology 2002; 39:29–37.
88. Campbell KB, Colrain I. Event-related potential measure on the inhibition of information processing:
II. The sleep onset period. Int J Psychophysiol 2002; 46:197–214.
89. Harsh J, Voss U, Hull J, et al. ERP and behavioral changes during the wake/sleep transition. Psy-
chophysiology 1994; 31:244–252.
90. Ogilvie RD, Simons IA, Kuderian RH, et al. Behavioral, event-related potential, and EEG/FFT changes
at sleep onset. Psychophysiology 1991; 28:54–64.
91. Hull J, Harsh J. P300 and sleep-related positive waveforms (P220, P450, and P900) have different
determinants. J Sleep Res 2001; 10:9–17.
92. Colrain IM. The K-complex: A 7-decade history. Sleep 2005; 28:255–273.
93. Cote KA, Etienne L, Campbell KB. Neurophysiological evidence for the detection of external stimuli
during sleep. Sleep 2001; 24:791–803.
94. Perrin F, Garcia-Larrea L, Mauguiere F, et al. A differential brain response to the subject’s own name
persists during sleep. Clin Neurophysiol 1999; 110:2153–2164.
95. Coursey RD. Personality measures and evoked responses in chronic insomniacs. J Abnorm Psychol
1975; 84:239–249.
96. Devoto A, Violani C, Lucidi F, et al. P300 amplitude in subjects with primary insomnia is modulated
by their sleep quality. J Psychosom Res 2003; 54:3–10.
97. Devoto A, Manganelli S, Lucidi F, et al. Quality of sleep and P300 amplitude in primary insomnia: A
preliminary study. Sleep 2005; 28:859–863.
98. Regenstein QR, Dambrosia J, Hallett M, et al. Daytime alertness in patients with primary insomnia.
Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150:1529–1534.
99. Wang W, Zhu SZ, Pan LC, et al. Mismatch negativity and personality traits in chronic primary
insomnia. Funct Neurol 2001; 16:3–10.
100. Yang CM, Lo HS. ERP evidence of enhanced excitatory and reduced inhibitory processes of auditory
stimuli durina sleep in patients with primary insomnia. Sleep 2007; 30:585–592.
101. Colrain IM. P300 and the daytime consequences of disturbed nocturnal sleep: Easy to measure but
difficult to interpret. Sleep 2005; 28:790–792.
102. Perlis ML, Giles DE, Mendelson WB, et al. Psychophysiological insomnia: The behavioural model
and a neurocognitive perspective. J Sleep Res 1997; 6:179–188.
103. Pigeon WR, Perlis ML. Sleep homeostasis in primary insomnia. Sleep Med Rev 2006; 10:247–254.
104. Perlis ML, Merica H, Smith MT, et al. Beta EEG activity and insomnia. Sleep Med Rev 2001; 5:365–376.
105. Lichstein KL. Secondary insomnia: A myth dismissed. Sleep Med Rev 2006; 10:3–5.
IHBK059-06 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:24 Char Count=
64 PERLIS ET AL.
106. McCrae CS, Lichstein KL. Secondary insomnia: Diagnostic challenges and intervention opportunities.
Sleep Med Rev 2001; 5:47–61.
107. Nofzinger EA, Buysse DJ, Germain A, et al. Functional neuroimaging evidence for hyperarousal in
insomnia. Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161:2126–2129.
108. Smith MT, Perlis ML, Chengazi VU, et al. Neuroimaging of NREM sleep in primary insomnia: A
Tc-99-HMPAO single photon emission computed tomography study. Sleep 2002; 25:325–335.
109. Drummond SP, Smith MT, Orff HJ, et al. Functional imaging of the sleeping brain: Review of findings
and implications for the study of insomnia. Sleep Med Rev 2004; 8:227–242.
110. Winkelman JW, Buxton OM, Jensen JE, et al. Reduced brain GABA in primary insomnia: Preliminary
data from 4 T proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy (1 H-MRS). Sleep 2008; 31:1499–1506.
c07 IHBK059-Sateia March 17, 2010 14:9 Char Count=
INTRODUCTION
Insomnia is, by far, the most commonly encountered sleep disorder in medical practice. Its
prevalence varies considerably based on the definition used. While one-fourth to one-third of
the general population reports a complaint of difficulty falling and/or staying asleep, about 10%
present chronic complaints and seek medical help for insomnia (1–11). However, insomnia has
always been and still is an underrecognized and therefore undertreated problem, since about
60% of the people suffering from insomnia never discuss their sleeping difficulties with their
physicians (12,13).
Relatively little is known about the mechanisms, causes, clinical course, and consequences
of this highly prevalent chronic condition (14). Insomnia occurs more frequently in women as
well as in middle aged and older adults. Many studies have established that insomnia is highly
comorbid with psychiatric disorders and is a risk factor for the development of depression,
anxiety, and suicide (14,15). However, despite the evidence that insomnia has significant public
health implications, including impaired occupational performance, increased absenteeism at
work, higher health care costs, and decreased quality of life, there is a paucity of data linking
chronic insomnia with significant medical morbidity, for example, cardiovascular disorders.
This paucity may in part be due to our limited understanding of its neurobiology.
In contrast, two other studies, in which diagnosis of insomnia was based on subjective measures
only, showed opposite results, that is, decreased pupil size (23,24). Overall, these studies show
that poor sleepers, particularly those with objective sleep disturbance, exhibit increased levels
of autonomic system activity compared to good sleepers before and during sleep.
HEART RATE
Heart rate variability is under the control of the sympathetic nervous system activity and
represents another measure of physiologic arousal in humans. An early study by Monroe (17)
showed significantly increased heart rate 30 minutes before sleep in insomniacs as compared
to normal sleepers. During sleep, their heart rate was slightly, but not significantly, faster.
Similarly, in another study insomniacs were found to have increased heart rate prior to sleep,
but these differences were not present during sleep (18). In contrast to these early studies, two
more recent studies found significant changes in nocturnal heart rate and heart rate variability
when comparing insomnia patients who also met objective criteria of sleep disturbance to
normal sleepers (25,26). Specifically, in a 36-hour laboratory study, sleep and EKG measures
were evaluated in insomnia patients and age-, gender-, and BMI (body mass index)-matched
controls. Heart period was decreased, that is, heart rate was increased, and heart rate variability
was decreased in all sleep stages in insomniacs as compared to good sleepers. Furthermore,
spectral analysis of heart rate variability revealed significant increase in low frequency reflecting
sympathetic system activity and decrease in high-frequency power reflecting parasympathetic
system activity in insomniacs as compared to controls. Those changes were present across
all sleep stages. The authors concluded that chronic insomniacs could be at higher risk for the
development of disorders associated with increased sympathetic system, such as coronary artery
disease (25). The other study assessed heart rate in chronic insomnia patients with objective sleep
disturbance and normal sleepers before sleep, during sleep, and in response to acute stress (i.e.,
stressful performance task given in the morning after a night of sleep recording) (26). Nocturnal
heart rate was significantly higher in insomniacs. The morning after no difference was found in
heart rate between the two groups, but there was a significant increase in heart rate during the
stressful performance task. The lack of significant increase in nocturnal heart rate in insomnia
groups in two early studies may be due to the fact that insomnia was defined only subjectively
in contrast to the two recent studies, in which insomniacs met both subjective and objective
polysomnographic criteria.
METABOLIC FUNCTION
340
INSOMNIACS
CONTROLS
320
300
VO2 (mL/min)
280
260
240
220
200
5 10 15 20 25 30 35
Time (hr)
Figure 1 Average best fit VO2 in groups of insomnia patients and normal controls as a function of time of day.
The data represent averaged best fit third degree polynomials. A marker has been added at the right and left
margin of both lines to indicate the group. The average t = 13.10, P < 0.0001.
conclusion that increased whole body metabolic rate is primarily present in insomnia patients
with objective short sleep duration.
Hypothalamic–Pituitary–Adrenal Axis
Stress has been associated with the activation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA)
axis, while corticotrophin-releasing hormone (CRH) and cortisol, products of the hypothalamus
and the adrenals, respectively, are known to cause arousal and sleeplessness in humans and
animals. On the other hand, sleep, and particularly deep sleep, has an inhibitory effect on
the stress system, including its two major components, the HPA axis and the sympathetic
system (33).
In the past, few studies have assessed cortisol levels in insomnia patients and their results
were inconsistent. Two reported no difference between good and poor sleepers in urinary
24-hour cortisol or 17-hydroxysteroid (17-OHCS) excretion (34,35), whereas a third reported a
significantly higher 24-hour rate of urinary free 11-hydroxycorticosteroid (11-OHCS) excretion
in young adult poor sleepers as compared to young adult normal sleepers (36).
More recently, in a preliminary study that investigated the possible association between
chronic insomnia and the activity of the stress system, mean 24-hour urinary free cortisol
(UFC) levels, although within the normal range, were correlated positively with indices of sleep
disturbance, that is, total wake time (TWT) (37). In a following controlled study that tested
24-hour serial adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) and cortisol level in young adult insomnia
patients and age- and BMI-matched healthy controls, ACTH and cortisol were significantly
higher in insomniacs (Fig. 2) (38). Within the 24-hour period the strongest elevations were
observed in the evening and during the first half of the night. Furthermore, within the group of
insomnia patients, the subgroup with high degree of objective sleep disturbance [percent of total
sleep time (%TST) <70] had higher amount of cortisol compared to the subgroup with low degree
of sleep disturbance (Fig. 3). Finally, pulsatile analysis revealed a significantly higher number of
ACTH and cortisol pulses in insomnia patients as compared to normal sleepers, while cosinor
analysis indicated a significant circadian rhythm without differences in the temporal pattern of
ACTH and cortisol secretion between the two groups. Hence, this study suggests that insomnia
is associated with an overall 24-hour increase in ACTH and cortisol secretion, which nonetheless
retains a normal circadian pattern. Based on these two studies the authors concluded that in
chronic insomnia (i) the activity of the stress system is directly proportional to the degree of
objective sleep disturbance and (ii) polysomnographic measures can provide a reliable index of
the biological impact and severity of chronic insomnia (37,38). Similarly, a recent study assessing
plasma cortisol levels in patients with severe chronic primary insomnia found that evening
and nocturnal cortisol levels were significantly increased in insomnia patients as compared to
controls (39). Finally, a study that investigated the effects of the tricyclic antidepressant doxepin
on nocturnal sleep and plasma cortisol levels in patients with primary insomnia showed that
doxepin improved sleep and reduced mean cortisol levels suggesting that beneficial effects of
the medication are at least partially mediated by the normalization of the HPA axis (40).
Contrary to the above findings, two studies found no increase in cortisol secretion in
insomnia patients. It appears that the difference between these two groups of studies is the
degree of polysomnographically documented sleep disturbance. For example, in a study by
Rodenbeck et al. (39) the correlation between area under the curve (AUC) of cortisol and
percentage sleep efficiency was 0.91 suggesting that high cortisol levels are present in insomnia
patients with an objective short sleep duration. In contrast, in a study by Riemann et al. (41),
in which no cortisol differences were observed between insomnia patients and controls, the
objective sleep of insomniacs was very similar to that of controls, that is, sleep efficiency of 88.2%
versus 88.6%. Furthermore, in a study that applied constant routine conditions, all indices of
physiological arousal were increased but not to a significant degree due to lack of power and
uncareful selection of controls (42,43). Interestingly, in this study a visual inspection of cortisol
data suggested an elevation of cortisol values of 15% to 20% in the insomnia group, a difference
similar to that reported in a study by Vgontzas et al. and is considered of clinical significance
(37,42).
Similar findings have been reported in a community sample investigating evening and
morning saliva cortisol levels in 393 children of ages 5 through 11 (44). A univariate analysis
controlling for BMI percentile, age, and gender demonstrated significantly higher morning
c07 IHBK059-Sateia March 17, 2010 14:9 Char Count=
14 3.5 * 21:00–00:30
3.0
2.5
ACTH pmol/L
12 2.0
1.5
1.0
10 0.5
0.0
ACTH (pmol/L)
0
08:00 10:00 12:00 14:00 16:00 18:00 20:00 22:00 00:00 02:00 04:00 06:00
Time (clock hours)
550
180 * 21:00–00:30
160
500
140
Cortisol nmol/L
120
450 100
80
400 60
40
20
Cortisol (nmol/L)
350 0
300
250
200
150
100
50
0
08:00 10:00 12:00 14:00 16:00 18:00 20:00 22:00 00:00 02:00 04:00 06:00
Time (clock hours)
b
Figure 2 The 24-hour plasma ACTH (top) and cortisol (bottom) concentrations in insomnia patients () and
controls ( ). The thick black line on the abscissa indicates the sleep-recording period. Error bar indicates SE,
∗ P < 0.01.
c07 IHBK059-Sateia March 17, 2010 14:9 Char Count=
24
22
20
18
16
14
Cortisol (µg/dl)
12
10
0
08:00 10:00 12:00 14:00 16:00 18:00 20:00 22:00 00:00 02:00 04:00 06:00
Clock time
Figure 3 The 24-hour circadian secretory pattern of cortisol in insomnia patients with low TST () and high ( ) r
TST. The thick black line on the abscissa indicates the sleep-recording period. Error bar indicates SE, ∗ P < 0.05.
levels of cortisol in the insomnia group with objective low sleep efficiency than in both the
control group and insomnia group without objective low sleep efficiency. Evening cortisol
levels were also significantly higher in the insomnia group with objective low sleep efficiency
than in the insomnia group without objective low sleep efficiency. It appears that childhood
insomnia combined with objective low sleep efficiency is associated with high cortisol levels.
The findings of the 24-hour HPA axis activation in primary insomnia are consistent with a
disorder of central nervous system hyperarousal rather than one of sleep loss, which is usually
associated with no change or a decrease in cortisol secretion. Most of the studies investigating
the effects of sleep loss on cortisol secretion have found no change or decrease in the secretion of
cortisol after sleep deprivation. On the other hand, in studies that reported significant increase
of cortisol in the evening following the night of sleep loss, sleep deprivation was associated with
rather stressful experimental conditions, that is, lying in bed in a dimly lit room and receiving
calories through an IV catheter (45–51). Also, the higher evening cortisol levels reported in
studies where sleep was curtailed at the beginning of the night may represent a change of
the 24-hour secretory pattern, for example, a shift of the nadir point of cortisol secretion later
at night.
Catecholamines
Sympathetic system activation results in the release of norepinephrine from sympathetic nerve
terminals and in the secretion of epinephrine from the adrenals.
In an early study that investigated urinary epinephrine levels in middle-aged good and
poor sleepers no significant difference was found, although there was a trend toward higher
epinephrine levels in poor sleepers (35).
c07 IHBK059-Sateia March 17, 2010 14:9 Char Count=
50 50 50
40 40 40
30 30 30
20 20 20
10 10 10
0 0 0
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 0 500 1000 1500 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100
Norepinephrine (µg/24 hr) DOPAC (µg/24 hr) DHPG (µg/24 hr)
Figure 4 Correlation between wake time after sleep onset with 24-hour urinary NE, DHPG, and DOPAC.
About 10 years ago, Vgontzas et al. (37) in a preliminary study in young chronic insomnia
patients correlated 24-hour catecholamines levels with polysomnographic measures of sleep
architecture and continuity. Catecholamine metabolites, DHPG (dihydroxyphenylglycol), and
DOPAC (3,4-dihydroxyphenylacetic acid) were positively correlated with percentage stage 1
(%ST1) sleep and WTASO, while norepinephrine tended to be significantly positively asso-
ciated with %ST1 and WTASO and negatively associated with percentage slow wave sleep
(%SWS) (Fig. 4). In the same study, growth hormone (GH) was detectable in a small percent
of the participants suggesting that GH axis may be suppressed in chronic insomniacs. These
findings were further supported by a controlled study, which found that circulating levels of
norepinephrine during the nocturnal period were elevated in patients with primary insomnia
compared to patients with major depression and normal controls (52). Additionally, sleep effi-
ciency was negatively correlated with norepinephrine levels in insomnia patients but not in
the controls or depressives. Those findings together indicate that sympathetic nervous system
activity as measured by cathecholamines levels is increased in chronic insomnia patients with
objective sleep disturbance.
Finally, based on the previous reports that insomnia with objective short sleep duration
is associated with hypercortisolemia, increased catecholaminergic activity, and increased auto-
nomic activity (e.g., increased heart rate and 24-hour metabolic rate and impaired heart rate
variability), a recent study examined the relationship between insomnia and hypertension in a
large general population sample using objective measures of sleep, that is, polysomnography
(53). After controlling for age, gender, race, BMI, diabetes, smoking, alcohol use, depression,
and sleep disordered breathing, chronic insomnia with objective short sleep duration was found
to be associated with a high risk for hypertension suggesting that objective measures of sleep
duration of insomnia may serve as clinically useful predictors of the medical severity of chronic
insomnia.
7 3.5
6
3.0
TNF (pg/mL)
IL-6 (pg/mL)
2.5
4
3 2.0
2
1.5
1
0.0
8:00 10:00 12:00 14:00 16:00 18:00 20:00 22:00 00:00 02:00 04:00 06:00 8:00 10:00 12:00 14:00 16:00 18:00 20:00 22:00 00:00 02:00 04:00 06:00
r
Figure 5 The 24-hour circadian secretory pattern of IL-6 (left) and TNF␣ (right) in insomnia patients () and
controls ( ). The thick black line on the abscissa indicates the sleep-recording period. Error bar indicates SE, ∗ P
< 0.05.
whereas their TNF␣ secretion was characterized by a regular 4-hour rhythm, a pattern that
was not present in the normal sleepers group. Based on these findings, the authors concluded
that chronic insomnia is associated with a shift of IL-6 and TNF␣ secretion from nighttime to
daytime, which may explain the daytime fatigue and performance decrements associated with
this disorder. Another recent study of IL-6 secretion in chronic insomnia patients of wide age
range showed a significant increase in nocturnal IL-6 levels in insomnia patients compared to
controls (59).
Cytokine secretion is elevated in disorders of “true” excessive daytime sleepiness defined
by falling asleep quickly in objective daytime sleep testing [multiple sleep latency test (MSLT)],
that is, sleep apnea and narcolepsy, and in healthy individuals experiencing daytime sleepiness
secondary to acute and short-term total or partial sleep loss. Cytokine peripheral levels are also
elevated in insomnia patients who experience “fatigue” but not true “sleepiness,” that is, not
falling asleep quickly in objective daytime sleep testing (48,49,54,56,60,61). What modulates the
effect of cytokines so that in one instance they are associated with true “sleepiness” and in
another with “fatigue”? Based on accumulated evidence, we proposed that it is the interaction
of cytokines and cortisol secretion that determines sleepiness versus fatigue (61). Specifically,
the hypersecretion and/or circadian alteration of the cytokines secretion associated with an
HPA axis activation may explain the fatigue and poor sleep associated with insomnia (38,58).
In contrast, hypersecretion and/or circadian alteration of the cytokines that is not associated
with HPA axis activation, that is, sleep deprivation in normal sleepers, may explain their true
sleepiness and poor sleep (48,49,58,62).
of ACTH and cortisol, wakefulness increased significantly only in the middle-aged group (63).
This increase was more pronounced during the first half of the night. Moreover, CRH admin-
istration caused a significant reduction of SWS only in the middle-aged group. These findings
suggest that middle-aged men as compared with the young are more vulnerable to the arousing
effects of CRH. Therefore, the increased prevalence of insomnia in middle age may be the result
of deteriorating sleep mechanisms associated with increased sensitivity to arousal-producing
stress hormones, such as CRH and cortisol, rather than to increased life stresses during this
period. This suggests that a middle-aged individual compared with a young individual is at a
significantly higher risk of developing insomnia when faced with equivalent stressors.
Disturbed sleep also is a common complaint of women entering the menopause phase of
life. A recent study investigating the objective sleep patterns associated with menopause and
hormonal replacement therapy (HRT) in a large population sample found that the objective
sleep of postmenopausal women without HRT was worse than that of men matched for age and
postmenopausal women on HRT (64). Specifically, menopause appears to have a negative effect
on sleep latency (SL) and SWS, that is, postmenopausal women without HRT had a longer SL
and less SWS than women on HRT and compared with men. SL was not significantly different
between premenopausal women and men as well as between menopausal women with HRT and
men. However, postmenopausal women without HRT had significantly longer SL compared
to men and compared to women with HRT. Furthermore, postmenopausal women without
HRT were less likely to have SWS compared to those with HRT. These objective findings are
consistent with the complaint of poor sleep in some postmenopausal women as well as the
symptomatic relief reported by those on HRT (65–72). These data are also consistent with early
epidemiological findings from a large community cohort in which there was a greater increase
of prevalence of insomnia in women compared to men in middle age, a difference that could
not be explained by changes in the prevalence of psychiatric disorders such as depression or
anxiety (73).
CONCLUSION
Consistent data from numerous biological studies conducted over the 30-year period and
assessing different physiological domains including autonomic nervous system status, heart
rate, sympathetic system and HPA axis activity, whole body and brain metabolic rate, and
immune system basal activity suggest that (i) insomnia is a disorder of physiologic hyper-
arousal present throughout 24-hour sleep–wake cycle and (ii) insomnia and sleep loss are two
distinct states. Central nervous system hyperarousal, either as preexisting and/or induced by
psychiatric pathology and worsened by stressful events, as well as aging- and menopause-
related physiological decline of sleep mechanisms appears to be at the core of this common
sleep disorder. Finally, there is emerging evidence that the levels of physiologic hyperarousal
are directly related to the degree of polysomnographically documented sleep disturbance in
insomnia patients, suggesting that objective measures of sleep duration in insomnia may be a
useful marker of the biological severity of the disorder.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank Barbara Green for overall preparation of the manuscript and Athanasia F. Vgontzas
for editing of the manuscript.
REFERENCES
1. National Center for Health Statistics (U.S.). Division of Health Examination Statistics. Selected Symp-
toms of Psychological Distress. Rockville, MD: U.S. Health Services and Mental Health Administration,
1970.
2. Karacan I, Thornby JI, Anch M, et al. Prevalence of sleep disturbances in a primarily urban Florida
county. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1976; 10(5):239–244.
3. Bixler EO, Kales A, Soldatos CR, et al. Prevalence of sleep disorders in the Los Angeles metropolitan
area. Am J Psychiatry 1979; 136(10):1257–1262.
4. Mellinger GD, Balter MB, Uhlenhuth EH. Insomnia and its treatment. Prevalence and correlates. Arch
Gen Psychiatry 1985; 42(3):225–232.
c07 IHBK059-Sateia March 17, 2010 14:9 Char Count=
5. Klink M, Quan SF. Prevalence of reported sleep disturbances in a general adult population and their
relationship to obstructive airways diseases. Chest 1987; 91(4):540–546.
6. Klink M, Quan SF, Kaltenborn WT, et al. Risk factors associated with complaints of insomnia in a
general adult population. Arch Intern Med 1992; 152(8):1634–1637.
7. Dodge R, Cline MG, Quan SF. The natural history of insomnia and its relationship to respiratory
symptoms. Arch Intern Med 1995; 155(16):1797–1800.
8. Foley DJ, Monjan AA, Brown SL, et al. Sleep complaints among elderly persons: an epidemiologic
study of three communities. Sleep 1995; 18(6):425–432.
9. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (DHHS). National Center for Health Statistics. Third
National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey: 1988–1994. NHANES III laboratory data file.
Hyattsville, MD: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 1996.
10. Ancoli-Israel S, Roth T. Characteristics of insomnia in the United States: results of the 1991 National
Sleep Foundation Survey. I. Sleep 1999; 22(2):S347–S353.
11. Ford DE, Kamerow DB. Epidemiologic study of sleep disturbances and psychiatric disorders. JAMA
1989; 262(11):1479–1484.
12. Cortoos A, Verstraeten E, Cluydts R. Neurophysiological aspects of primary insomnia: implications
for its treatment. Sleep Med Rev 2006; 10(4):255–266.
13. Ohayon MM. Prevalence of DSM-IV diagnostic criteria of insomnia: distinguishing insomnia related
to mental disorders from sleep disorders. J Psychiatr Res 1997; 31(3):333–346.
14. National Institutes of Health. NIH state of the science statement on manifestations and management
of chronic insomnia in adults. J Clin Sleep Med 2005; 1(4):412–421.
15. Basta M, Chrousos GP, Vela-Bueno A, et al. Chronic insomnia and the stress system. Sleep Med Clin
2007; 2(2):279–291.
16. Kales A, Kales J. Evaluation and Treatment of Insomnia. New York: Oxford University Press, 1984.
17. Monroe LJ. Psychological and physiological differences between good and poor sleepers. J Abnorm
Psychol 1967; 72(3):255–264.
18. Freedman RR, Sattler HL. Physiological and psychological factors in sleep-onset insomnia. J Abnorm
Psychol 1982; 91(5):380–389.
19. Haynes SN, Follingstad DR, McGowan WT. Insomnia: sleep patterns and anxiety level. J Psychosom
Res 1974; 18(2):69–74.
20. Krauchi K, Wirz-Justice A. Circadian rhythm of heat production, heart rate, and skin and core tem-
perature unmasking conditions in men. Am J Physiol 1994; 267(3, pt 2), R819–R829.
21. Lushington K, Dawson D, Lack L. Core body temperature is elevated during constant wakefulness in
elderly poor sleepers. Sleep 2000; 23(4):1–7.
22. Lichstein K, Johnson RS. Pupillometric discrimination of insomniacs. Behav Res Ther 1994; 32(1):123–
129.
23. Lichstein K, Johnson RS, Sen Gupta S,et al. Are insomniacs sleepy during the day? A pupillometric
assessment. Behav Res Ther 1992; 30(3):283–292.
24. Lichstein K, Wilson NM, Noe SL, et al. Daytime sleepiness in insomnia: behavioral, biological and
subjective indices. Sleep 1994; 17(8):693–702.
25. Bonnet MH, Arand DL. Heart rate variability in insomniacs and matched normal sleepers. Psychosom
Med 1998; 60(5):610–615.
26. Stepanski E, Glinn M, Zorick F, et al. Heart rate changes in chronic insomnia. Stress Med 1994;
10(4):261–266.
27. Bonnet MH, Arand DL. 24-hour metabolic rate in insomniacs and matched normal sleepers. Sleep
1995; 18(7):581–588.
28. Bonnet MH, Arand DL. Physiological activation in patients with sleep state misperception. Psychosom
Med 1997; 59(5):533–540.
29. Bonnet MH, Arand DL. Caffeine use as a model of acute and chronic insomnia. Sleep 1992; 15(6):526–
536.
30. Nofzinger EA, Buysse DJ, Germain A, et al. Functional neuroimaging evidence for hyperarousal in
insomnia. Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161(11):2126–2128.
31. Nofzinger EA, Nissen C, Germain A, et al. Regional cerebral metabolic correlates of WASO during
NREM sleep in insomnia. J Clin Sleep Med 2006; 2(3):316–322.
32. Winkelman JW, Buxton OM, Jensen JE, et al. Reduced brain GABA in primary insomnia: preliminary
data from 4 T proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy (1 H-MRS). Sleep 2008; 311(11):1499–1506.
33. Vgontzas AN, Chrousos GP. Sleep, the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, and cytokines: multi-
ple interactions and disturbances in sleep disorders. Endocrinol Metab Clin North Am 2002; 31(1):
15–36.
34. Frankel BL, Buchbinder R, Coursey RD, et al. Sleep patterns and psychological test characteristics of
chronic primary insomnia. Sleep Res 1973; 2:149.
c07 IHBK059-Sateia March 17, 2010 14:9 Char Count=
35. Adam K, Tomeny M, Oswald I. Physiological and psychological differences between good and poor
sleepers. J Psychiatr Res 1986; 20(4):301–316.
36. Johns MW, Gay TJ, Masterton JP, et al. Relationship between sleep habits, adrenocortical activity and
personality. Psychosom Med 1971; 33(6):499–508.
37. Vgontzas AN, Tsigos C, Bixler EO, et al. Chronic insomnia and activity of the stress system: a prelim-
inary study. J Psychosom Res 1998; 45(1):21–31.
38. Vgontzas AN, Bixler EO, Lin HM, et al. Chronic insomnia is associated with nyctohemeral activa-
tion of the hypothalamic-pituitary–adrenal axis: clinical implications. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2001;
86(8):3787–3794.
39. Rodenbeck A, Huether G, Ruether E, et al. Interactions between evening and nocturnal cortisol
secretion and sleep parameters in patients with severe chronic primary insomnia. Neurosci Lett 2002;
324(2):163–459.
40. Rodenbeck A, Cohrs S, Jordan W, et al. The sleep-improving effects of doxepin are paralleled by
a normalized plasma cortisol secretion in primary insomnia. Psychopharmacology 2003; 170(4):
423–428.
41. Riemann D, Klein T, Rodenbeck A, et al. Nocturnal cortisol and melatonin secretion in primary
insomnia. Psychiatry Res 2002; 113(1–2):17–27.
42. Varkevisser M, Van Dongen HP, Kerkhof GA. Physiologic indexes in chronic insomnia during a
constant routine: evidence for general hyperarousal? Sleep 2005; 28(12):1588–1596.
43. Bonnet MH. Hyperarousal as the basis for insomnia: effect size and significance. Sleep 2005;
28(12):1500–1501.
44. Bixler EO, Tsaoussoglou M, Calhoun S, et al. Insomnia with low sleep efficiency is associated with
high salivary cortisol levels in children. Sleep 2008; 31:A143.
45. Moldofsky H, Lue FA, Davidson JR, et al. Effects of sleep deprivation on human immune functions.
FASEB J 1989; 3(8):1927–1977.
46. Scheen AJ, Byrne MM, Plat L, et al. Relationships between sleep quality and glucose regulation in
normal humans. Am J Physiol 1997; 271(2, pt 1):E261–E270.
47. Brun J, Chambe G, Khalfallah Y, et al. Effect of modafinil on plasma melatonin, cortisol and growth hor-
mone rhythms, rectal temperature and performance in healthy subjects during 36 h sleep deprivation.
J Sleep Res 1998; 7(2):105–114.
48. Vgontzas AN, Zoumakis E, Bixler EO, et al. Adverse effects of modest sleep restriction on sleepiness,
performance, and inflammatory cytokines. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2004; 89(5):2119–2126.
49. Vgontzas AN, Mastorakos G, Bixler EO, et al. Sleep deprivation effects on the activity of hypothalamic–
pituitary-adrenal and growth axes: potential clinical implications. Clin Endocrinol (Oxf) 1999;
51(2):205–215.
50. Leproult R, Copinschi G, Buxton O, et al. Sleep loss results in an elevation of cortisol levels the next
evening. Sleep 1997; 20(10):865–870.
51. Spiegel K, Lerpoult R, Van Cauter E. Impact of sleep debt on metabolic and endocrine function. Lancet
1999; 354(9188):1435–1439.
52. Irwin M, Clark C, Kennedy B, et al. Nocturnal catecholamines and immune function in insomniacs,
depressed patients, and controls subjects. Brain Behav Immun 2003; 17(5):365–372.
53. Vgontzas AN, Liao D, Bixler EO, et al. Insomnia with objective short sleep duration is associated with
a high risk for hypertension. Sleep 2009; 32(4):491–497.
54. Vgontzas AN, Papanicolaou DA, Bixler EO, et al. Elevation of plasma cytokines in disorders of
excessive daytime sleepiness: role of sleep disturbance and obesity. J Clinc Endocrinol Metab 1997;
82(5):1313–1316.
55. Vgontzas AN, Papanicolaou DA, Bixler EO, et al. Sleep apnea and daytime sleepiness and fatigue:
relations with visceral obesity, insulin resistance, and hypercytokinemia. J Clin Endocrinol Metab
2000; 85(3):1151–1158.
56. Vgontzas AN, Papanicolaou DA, Bixler EO, et al. Circadian interleukin-6 secretion and quantity and
depth of sleep. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1999; 84(8):2603–2607.
57. Chrousos GP. The hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in immune-mediated inflammation. N Engl J
Med 1995; 332(2):1351–1362.
58. Vgontzas AN, Zoumakis E, Papanicolaou DA, et al. Chronic insomnia is associated with a shift
of interleukin-6 and tumor necrosis factor secretion from nighttime to daytime. Metabolism 2002;
51(7):887–892.
59. Burgos I, Richter L, Klein T, et al. Increased nocturnal interleukin-6 excretion in patients with primary
insomnia: a pilot study. Brain Behav Immun 2006; 20(3):246–253.
60. Shearer WT, Reuben JM, Mullington JM, et al. Soluble TNF-alpha receptor 1 and IL-6 plasma levels
in humans subjected to the sleep deprivation model of spaceflight. J Allergy Clin Immunol 2001;
107(1):165–170.
c07 IHBK059-Sateia March 17, 2010 14:9 Char Count=
61. Vgontzas AN, Bixler EO, Lin HM, et al. IL-6 and its circadian secretion in humans. Neuroimmunomod-
ulation 2005; 12 (3):131–140.
62. Stepanski E, Zorick F, Roehrs T, et al. Daytime alertness in patients with chronic insomnia compared
to asymptomatic controls subjects. Sleep 1988; 1(1):54–60.
63. Vgontzas AN, Bixler EO, Wittman AM, et al. Middle-aged men show higher sensitivity of sleep to
the arousing effects of corticotrophin-releasing hormone that young men: clinical implications. J Clin
Endocrinol Metab 2001; 86(4):1489–1495.
64. Bixler EO, Papaliaga MN, Vgontzas AN,et al. Women sleep objectively better than men and the sleep
of young women is more resilient to external stressors: effects of age and menopause. J Sleep Res 2009;
18(2):221–228.
65. Young T, Rabago D, Zgierska A, et al. Objective and subjective sleep quality in premenopausal, peri-
menopausal, and postmenopausal women in the Wisconsin Sleep Cohort Study. Sleep 2003; 26(6):667–
672.
66. Kuh DL, Wadsworth M, Hardy R. Women’s health in midlife: the influence of the menopause, social
factors and health in earlier life. Br J Obstet Gynaecol 1997; 104(8):923–933.
67. Matthews KA, Wing RR, Kuller LH, et al. Influences of natural menopause on psychological charac-
teristics and symptoms of middle-aged healthy women. J Consult Clin Psychol 1990; 58:345–351.
68. Kravitz HM, Ganz P, Bromberger J, et al. Sleep difficulty in women at midlife: a community survey of
sleep and the menopausal transition. Menopause 2003; 10(1):19–28.
69. Thomson J, Oswald I. Effect of oestrogen on the sleep, mood, and anxiety of menopausal women. Br
Med J 1977; 2(6098):1317–1319.
70. Scharf MB, McDannold MD, Stover R, et al. Effects of estrogen replacement therapy on rates of cyclic
alternating patterns of hot-flush events during sleep in postmenopausal women: a pilot study. Clin
Ther 1997; 19(2):304–311.
71. Schiff I, Regestein Q, Tulchinsky D, et al. Effects of estrogens on sleep and psychological state of
hypogonadal women. JAMA 1979; 242(22):2405–2407.
72. Hays J, Ockene J, Brunner R, et al. Women’s Health Initiative Investigators. Effects of estrogen plus
progestin on health-related quality of life. N Engl J Med 2003; 348(19):1839–1854.
73. Lugaresi E, Cirignotta F, Zucconi M, et al. Good and poor sleepers: an epidemiological survey of the
San Marino population. In: Guilleminault C, Lugaresi E, eds. Sleep/Wake Disorders: Natural History,
Epidemiology, and Long-Term Evolution. New York: Raven Press, 1983:1–12.
c08 IHBK059-Sateia March 17, 2010 9:16 Char Count=
INTRODUCTION
Clinically, insomnia patients describe overactive minds that do not turn off at night to allow
them to sleep. This chapter will review brain imaging studies that comment on whether or
not there is abnormal brain function in insomnia patients that may in some way relate to
their difficulty sleeping. Interpretations will be aided by review of relevant preclinical studies
of the neurobiology of sleep and recent neuroimaging studies in healthy humans across the
sleep/wake cycle. The organization of this paper will follow a systems neuroscience view of a
hierarchical arousal network in the central nervous system. Abnormalities in any component of
this interactive system may produce insomnia complaints. Finally, evidence will be presented
as to the location where interventions may impact on regional brain function in insomnia
patients.
78 NOFZINGER
Recent work shows that the amygdala is anatomically connected with and functionally
modulates effects on the brainstem centers involved in arousal and sleep regulation (15,16).
Similarly, other components of the ventral emotional system such as the ventral striatum, the
subgenual anterior cingulate cortex, and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex are known to have
both anatomical and functional relationships with brainstem centers thought to play a role in
behavioral state regulation in addition to the primary roles they each play in cortical arousal
(17,18).
Human sleep neuroimaging studies support a role for the ventral emotional system in
sleep processes. Blood flow has been shown to negatively correlate with the presence of NREM
sleep in the anterior cingulate (3–5), the amygdala (5), and in the orbitofrontal cortex (3–5). These
changes suggest that components of the ventral emotion system play a role in modulating sleep
or in the manifestation of sleep. Declining function in the amygdala raises the possibility that
this structure modulates activity in core structures involved in regulating arousal.
Human sleep neuroimaging studies support the role for components of the ventral emo-
tion system in pathological sleep associated with both depression and insomnia. Nofzinger
et al. (19) used [18 F]-FDG PET to define regional cerebral correlates of arousal in NREM
sleep in 9 healthy and 12 depressed patients. They assessed EEG power in the beta high
frequency spectrum as a measure of cortical arousal. They then correlated beta power with
metabolism in NREM sleep. They found that beta power negatively correlated with sleep quality.
Further, beta power positively correlated with ventromedial prefrontal cortex metabolism in
both group of depressed and healthy subjects. They concluded that elevated function in the ven-
tromedial prefrontal cortex, an area associated with obsessive behavior and anatomically linked
with brainstem and hypothalamic arousal centers, may contribute to dysfunctional arousal.
Nofzinger et al. (6) investigated the neurobiological basis of poor sleep in insomnia. Insom-
nia patients and healthy subjects completed regional cerebral glucose metabolic assessments
during both waking and NREM sleep using [18 F]-FDG PET. Healthy subjects reported better
sleep quality than did insomnia subjects. A group × state interaction analysis confirmed that
insomnia subjects showed a smaller decrease than did healthy subjects in relative metabolism
from waking to NREM sleep in the insular cortex, amygdala, hippocampus, anterior cingulate,
and medial prefrontal cortices. This study supports the notion that persistent overactivity in a
limbic/paralimbic level of the arousal system contributes to the nonrestorative sleep in insomnia
patients.
Pharmacotherapy for insomnia may have some of its mechanism of action on these limbic
and paralimbic structures, especially the psychotropic medications. When Kajimura et al. (7)
assessed regional cerebral blood flow during NREM sleep in response to triazolam, they also
found that blood flow in the amygdaloid complexes, in addition to basal forebrain, was lower
than following placebo. While not always consistent, several neuroimaging studies show that
serotonergic antidepressants tend to lower, or inhibit, brain function (blood flow or metabolism)
in ventral limbic and paralimbic cortex including the amygdala (20,21), perhaps via actions on
5-HT1 postsynaptic receptors given their high density in these areas (22). Inhibiting hyper-
arousal in this limbic/paralimbic emotional neural network could benefit insomnia patients.
Recent studies have focused on the role of sleep in memory consolidation utilizing both
cognitive neuroscience and brain imaging of the hippocampus. Nissen et al. (23) compared
sleep-related consolidation of procedural memory in seven patients with primary insomnia
and seven healthy controls. Performance on a mirror tracing task was measured before and
after sleep. Performance in the mirror tracing task before sleep did not differ between the
groups. Both groups performed significantly better in the recall condition after sleep. Healthy
controls showed an improvement of 42.8% ± 5.8% in the mirror tracing draw time, whereas
patients with insomnia showed an improvement of 20.4% ± 14.8% (multivariate analyses of
variance test session × group interaction: F3,10 = 10.9, P = 0.002). These preliminary findings
support the view that sleep-associated consolidation of procedural memories may be impaired
in patients with primary insomnia.
Another study focused on morphology of the hippocampus in insomnia patients (24).
Morphometric analysis of magnetic resonance imaging brain scans was used to investigate
possible neuroanatomic differences between patients with primary insomnia compared to good
sleepers. MRI images (1.5 Tesla) of the brain were obtained from insomnia patients and good
c08 IHBK059-Sateia March 17, 2010 9:16 Char Count=
80 NOFZINGER
sleepers. MRI scans were analyzed bilaterally by manual morphometry for different brain areas
including hippocampus, amygdala, anterior cingulate, orbitofrontal and dorsolateral prefrontal
cortex. Patients with primary insomnia demonstrated significantly reduced hippocampal vol-
umes bilaterally compared to the good sleepers. None of the other regions of interest analyzed
revealed differences between the two groups. While replication of the findings in larger samples
is needed to confirm the validity of the data, these pilot data raise the possibility that chronic
insomnia is associated with alterations in brain structure. The integration of structural, neu-
ropsychological, neuroendocrine, and polysomnographic studies is necessary to further assess
the relationships between insomnia and brain function and structure.
SUMMARY
Cerebral metabolic studies, in light of preclinical work, suggest that there are hierarchical levels
of arousal that may be disturbed in insomnia. Further, interventions may have their effects at
different levels of a hierarchical arousal neural network. Additional work is needed to clarify
the degree to which alterations in these levels of arousal distinguish insomnia patients from
other sleep disorders, or the degree to which distinct levels may be disturbed in individual
patients. Additional work is also needed to match interventions for the treatment of insomnia
with the levels of arousal that are unique to the individual patient.
c08 IHBK059-Sateia March 17, 2010 9:16 Char Count=
82 NOFZINGER
REFERENCES
1. Jones B. Anatomy and neurophysiology. In: Opp M, ed. SRS Basics of Sleep Guide. Westchester, IL:
Sleep Research Society, 2005.
2. Henriksen S. Stimulants. In: Opp M, ed. SRS Basics of Sleep Guide. Westchester, IL: Sleep Research
Society, 2005.
3. Braun AR, Balkin TJ, Wesenten NJ, et al. Regional cerebral blood flow throughout the sleep-wake
cycle. An H2(15)O PET study. Brain 1997; 120:1173–1197.
4. Hofle N, Paus T, Reutens D, et al. Regional cerebral blood flow changes as a function of delta and
spindle activity during slow wave sleep in humans. J Neurosci 1997; 17:4800–4808.
5. Maquet P, Degueldre C, Delfiore G, et al. Functional neuroanatomy of human slow wave sleep. J
Neurosci 1997; 17:2807–2812.
6. Nofzinger EA, Buysse DJ, Germain A, et al. Functional neuroimaging evidence for hyperarousal in
insomnia. Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161:2126–2131.
7. Kajimura N, Nishikawa M, Uchiyama M, et al. Deactivation by benzodiazepine of the basal forebrain
and amygdala in normal humans during sleep: a placebo-controlled [15 O]H2 O PET study. Am J
Psychiatry 2004; 161:748–751.
8. Nofzinger EA, Buysse D, Moul D, et al. Eszopiclone reverses brain hyperarousal in insomnia: evidence
from [18]-FDG PET. Sleep 2008; 31:A232.
9. Phillips ML, Drevets WC, Rauch SL, et al. Neurobiology of emotion perception I: the neural basis of
normal emotion perception. Biol Psychiatry 2003; 54:504–514.
10. Wright CI, Fishcer H, Whalen PJ, et al. Differential prefrontal cortex and amygdala habituation to
repeatedly presented emotional stimuli. Neuroreport 2001; 12:379–383.
11. Blair RJR, Morris JS, Frith CD, et al. Neural responses to sad and angry expressions. Brain 1999;
122:883–893.
12. Isenberg N, Silbersweig D, Engelien A, et al. Linguistic threat activates the human amygdala. Proc
Natl Acad Sci U S A 1999; 96:10456–10459.
13. Phillips ML, Young AW, Scott SK, et al. Neural responses to facial and vocal expressions of fear and
disgust. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1998; 265:1809–1817.
14. Davidson RJ, Kabat-Zinn J, Schumacher J, et al. Alterations in brain and immune function produced
by mindfulness meditation. Psychosom Med 2003; 65:564–570.
15. Morrison AR, Sanford LD, Ross RJ. Initiation of rapid eye movement sleep: beyond the brainstem. In:
Mallick BN, Inoue S, eds. Rapid Eye Movement Sleep. New York: Marcel Dekker, 1999.
16. Sanford LD, Ross RJ, Morrison AR. Serotonergic mechanisms in the amygdala terminate REM sleep.
Sleep Res 1995; 24:54
17. Semba K. The mesopontine cholinergic system: a dual role in REM sleep and wakefulness. In: Lydic
R, Baghdoyan HA, eds. Handbook of Behavioral State Control: Cellular and Molecular Mechanisms.
Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press, 1999:161–180.
18. Sherin JE, Elmquist JK, Torrealba F, et al. Innervation of histaminergic tuberomammillary neurons by
GABAergic and galaninergic neurons in the ventrolateral preoptic nucleus of the rat. J Neurosci 1998;
18:4705–4721.
19. Nofzinger EA, Price JC, Meltzer CC, et al. Towards a neurobiology of dysfunctional arousal in
depression: the relationship between beta EEG power and regional cerebral glucose metabolism
during NREM sleep. Psychiatry Res 2000; 98:71–91.
20. Mayberg HS, Brannan SK, Tekell JL, et al. Regional metabolic effects of fluoxetine in major depression:
serial changes and relationship to clinical response. Biol Psychiatry 2000; 48:830–843.
21. Buchsbaum MS, Wu J, Siegel BV, et al. Effect of sertraline on regional metabolic rate in patients with
affective disorder. Biol Psychiatry 1997; 41:15–22.
22. Tsukada H, Kakiuchi T, Nishiyama S, et al. Effects of aging on 5-HT(1A) receptors and their functional
response to 5-HT(1A) agonist in the living brain: PET study with [carbonyl-(11)C]WAY-100635 in
conscious monkeys. Synapse 2001; 42:242–251.
23. Nissen C, Kloepfer C, Nofzinger EA, et al. Impaired sleep-associated memory consolidation in primary
insomnia. Sleep 2006; 29:1068–1073.
24. Riemann D, Voderholzer U, Spiegelhalder K, et al. Chronic insomnia and MRI-measured hippocampal
volumes: a pilot study. Sleep 2007; 30:955–958.
25. Benington JH, Heller HC. Restoration of brain energy-metabolism as the function of sleep. Prog
Neurobiol 1995; 45:347–360.
26. Tononi G, Cirelli C. Sleep and synaptic homeostasis: a hypothesis. Brain Res Bull 2003; 62:143–150.
27. Steriade M, Amzica F. Coalescence of sleep rhythms and their chronology in corticothalamic networks.
Sleep Res Online 1998; 1:1–10.
28. Werth E, Achermann P, Borbely AA. Brain topography of the human sleep EEG: antero-posterior
shifts of spectral power. Neuroreport 1996; 8:123–127.
c08 IHBK059-Sateia March 17, 2010 9:16 Char Count=
29. Cajochen C, Foy R, Dijk D-J. Frontal predominance of a relative increase in sleep delta and theta EEG
activity after sleep loss in humans. Sleep Res Online 1999; 2:65–69.
30. Harrison Y, Horne JA. The impact of sleep deprivation on decision making: a review. J Exp Psychol
Appl 2000; 6:236–249.
31. Thomas M, Sing H, Belenky G, et al. Neural basis of alertness and cognitive performance impairments
during sleepiness. I. Effects of 24h of sleep deprivation on waking human regional brain activity. J
Sleep Res 2000; 9:335–352.
32. Huber R, Ghilardi MF, Massimini M, et al. Local sleep and learning. Nature 2004; 430:78–81.
33. Miller EK, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu Rev Neurosci 2001;
24:167–202.
34. Davidson RJ, Jackson DC, Kalin NH. Emotion, plasticity, context, and regulation: perspectives from
affective neuroscience. Psychol Bull 2000; 126:890–909.
35. Perez-Jaranay JM, Vives F. Electrophysiological study of the response of medial prefrontal cortex
neurons to stimulation of the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala in the rat. Brain Res 1991; 564:
97–101.
36. Botvinick M, Nystrom LE, Fissell K, et al. Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior
cingulate cortex. Nature 1999; 402:179–181.
37. Nofzinger EA, Buysse DJ, Miewald JM, et al. Human regional cerebral glucose metabolism during
non-rapid eye movement sleep in relation to waking. Brain 2002; 125:1105–1115.
38. Lou HC, Kjaer TW, Friberg L, et al. A 15 O-H2 O PET study of meditation and the resting state of normal
consciousness. Hum Brain Mapp 1999; 7:98–105.
39. Mayberg HS, Brannan SK, Tekell JL, et al. Regional metabolic effects of fluoxetine in major depression:
serial changes and relationship to clinical response. Biol Psychiatry 2000; 48:830–843.
c09 IHBK059-Sateia March 31, 2010 10:5 Char Count=
Scott E. Cologne
American Sleep Medicine, St. Louis, Missouri, U.S.A.
INTRODUCTION
A comprehensive history is the cornerstone of the evaluation process in insomnia. This fact
deserves special emphasis in the case of insomnia, since less than half of primary care physicians
obtain a sleep history despite being confronted with a complaint of insomnia (1). In this chapter,
we will assume that an insomnia complaint has already been established; nevertheless, it is
noteworthy that only a minority of insomnia complaints are actually captured during patient–
physician encounters. Even severe insomnia remains undetected by physicians in 60% of cases
(2,3). Therefore, active inquiry about sleep disturbances is especially important since 70% of
insomnia sufferers do not raise the problem with their physicians (4).
The history should be explored in a systematic fashion. Although a variety of formulations
have been described in the history-taking process, empirical studies of the optimal method are
lacking (5).
SLEEP PATTERNS
The list below summarizes critical elements of the sleep/wake history
1. Bedtime
2. Sleep latency
3. Nocturnal awakenings; number and duration
4. Final morning awakening
5. Out-of-bed time
6. Number, time, and duration of daytime naps
7. Levels of sleepiness and fatigue as the day progresses
Characteristic disturbances in these areas can point to certain circadian rhythm distur-
bances such as delayed sleep phase syndrome and sleep hygiene impairments such as spending
excessive time in bed while awake, among others. It is also useful to ascertain each of these
parameters not only for the “average” day, but also for a sequence of days, as such a tempo-
ral record can demonstrate variability in sleep patterns over time that can, in turn, contribute
to poor sleep. The assessment of variability between workdays/schooldays, weekends, and
vacations can be useful. Poor sleepers characteristically do not maintain rigorous sleep/wake
schedules. Identification and correction of this variability is a primary element of sleep hygiene
education and other cognitive-behavioral techniques.
sources such as family members or a television set; and being disturbed by the movements of
a bed partner. Aerobic exercise increases body temperature, which may contribute to reduced
sleep propensity in the immediate postexercise period. This is followed by a drop within four to
six hours which, in turn, can promote sleep continuity (13). Certain behaviors are compensatory
and intended to address the sleep difficulty, but have a detrimental effect nevertheless. These
include strategies for catching up on lost sleep such as going to bed too early and staying in
bed for extended periods of time after awakening. A similar strategy includes staying in bed
for extended periods of time after nocturnal awakenings.
MEDICATIONS
A history of current and prior medications is always of relevance. The list should include not only
prescribed medications, but also over-the-counter agents, nutraceuticals, herbals substances,
dietary supplements, and even foods. Their effects, side effects, dosages or quantities, and timing
of administration should be recorded. Allergies to medications should also be recorded. The
effects of psychotropic and pain medications on sleep are reviewed in chapters 13 and 14 of this
book.
SUBSTANCES
It is useful to determine the type and extent of use of various substances. Chronic and excessive
use may lead to substance use disorders, including abuse or dependence. The effects of sub-
stances on sleep are reviewed in chapter 16 of this book. Of particular relevance are stimulants
such as caffeine, amphetamines, and cocaine. Sedatives such as alcohol, at low dosages, can
help with sleep initiation, yet chronic and excessive use may lead to disturbed sleep and the
complaint of insomnia (14,15).
FAMILY HISTORY
Certain sleep disorders have a hereditary component. In the case of restless legs syndrome, 50%
of primary cases have a positive family history (16). Other comorbid disorders such as obstruc-
tive sleep apnea may demonstrate familial predisposition by virtue of inherited metabolic or
anatomical conditions. Fatal familial insomnia is a relatively rare disorder in which family
history is clearly key.
CONCLUSIONS
A thorough history provides the clinician with critical information for the formulation of a dif-
ferential diagnosis and establishes the foundation for therapy. As already indicated, approaches
to the clinical history have not been subjected to empirical validation and, indeed, it is difficult
to imagine how a single process might apply to all clinicians and patients. However, this chapter
has provided a framework upon which the history-taking process can be organized. Therefore,
we recommend that clinicians tailor the guidelines and principles that have been provided to
develop history-taking strategies that are most appropriate for their work styles and patient
types in the evaluation of insomnia.
REFERENCES
1. Everitt DE, Avom J, Baker MW. Clinical decision making in the evaluation and treatment of insomnia.
Am J Med 1990; 89(3):357–362.
2. Hohagen F, Rink K, Kappler C, et al. Prevalence and treatment of insomnia in general practice: a
longitudinal study. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1993; 242(6):329–336.
c09 IHBK059-Sateia March 31, 2010 10:5 Char Count=
3. Schramm E, Hohagen F, Kappler C, et al. Mental comorbidity of chronic insomnia in general practice
attenders using DSM-III-R. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1995; 91(1):10–17.
4. Gallup Organization.Sleep in America. Princeton, NJ: Gallup Organization, 1995.
5. Sateia M, Doghramji K, Hauri P, et al. Evaluation of chronic insomnia. Sleep 2000; 23(2):1–66.
6. Anonymous. National Institutes of Health State of the Science Conference Statement. Manifestations
and management of chronic insomnia in adults June 13–15, 2005. Sleep 2005; 28:1049–1057.
7. Ancoli-Israel S, Roth T. Characteristics of insomnia in the United States: results of the 1991 National
Sleep Foundation Survey. I. Sleep 1999; 22(suppl 2):S347–S353.
8. Kales JD, Kales A, Bixler EO, et al., Biopsychobehavioral correlates of insomnia, V: clinical character-
istics and behavioral correlates. Am J Psychiatry 1984; 141(11):1371–1376.
9. Edinger JD, Means MK, Carney CE, et al. Psychomotor performance deficits and their relation to prior
nights’ sleep among individuals with primary insomnia. Sleep 2008; 31(5):599–607.
10. Kupperman M, Lubeck DP, Mazonson PD, et al. Sleep problems and their correlates in a working
population. J Gen Intern Med 1995; 10:25–32.
11. Stutts JC, Wilkins JW, Osbert JS, et al. Driver risk factors for sleep-related crashes. Accid Anal Prev
2003; 35:321–331.
12. Stepanski EJ, Wyatt JK. Use of sleep hygiene in the treatment of insomnia. Sleep Med Rev 2003;
7(3):215–225.
13. Horne JA, Staff LH. Exercise and sleep: body heating effects. Sleep 1983; 6(1):36–46.
14. Roehrs T, Roth T. Sleep, sleepiness and alcohol use. Alcohol Res Health 2001; 25(2):101–109.
15. Brower KJ. Alcohol’s effects on sleep in alcoholics. Alcohol Res Health 2001; 25(2):110–125.
16. Montplaisir J, Allen R, Walters A, et al. Restless legs syndrome and periodic limb movements in sleep.
In: Kryger M, Roth T, Dement W, eds. Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine. 4th ed. Philadelphia:
Elsevier, 2005:839–852.
c10 IHBK059-Sateia March 17, 2010 9:29 Char Count=
Douglas E. Moul
Department of Psychiatry, Louisiana State University at Shreveport School of Medicine,
Shreveport, Louisiana, U.S.A.
INTRODUCTION
Epidemiological studies (1,2) have shown that the most prevalent form of insomnia occurs
concurrently with other medical, psychiatric, or sleep disorders. For researchers studying the
psychophysiological and neurobiological mechanisms underlying insomnia, however, primary
insomnia offers the advantages of being uncomplicated by other conditions. While not entirely
homogenous, primary insomnia also offers the advantage of providing a group of relatively
uncomplicated patients to evaluate treatment efficacy in clinical trials. Nevertheless, the dif-
ferential diagnosis and evaluation of insomnia in epidemiological and treatment effectiveness
studies require similar measurement approaches for insomnia and relevant domains, to thor-
oughly characterize the populations of interest. While most insomnia measurement scales and
methods described here are derived from instruments developed to study primary insomnia,
the goal of capturing insomnia as a distinct and addressable measurement problem remains the
same, irrespective of the cause of insomnia. In this chapter, we discuss evaluation measures and
methods that have been widely used, and that are validated, reliable, and/or recommended
by expert consensus on insomnia measurements (3). These methods are amenable to different
purposes for the evaluation of insomnia: clinical evaluation, research assessments, and epi-
demiological studies. As such, we first discuss the requirements and constraints of these three
contexts in relation to the evaluation of insomnia. Selected measures of insomnia reviewed here
include clinician-administered scales, self-report questionnaires, sleep diaries, and actigraphy.
Polysomnography (PSG) has been used widely as a screening method to rule out other sleep
disorders that may masquerade as insomnia (e.g., sleep apnea, periodic leg movement disor-
der), as a quantitative measurement of sleep disturbances, and as an objective outcome measure
in treatment efficacy studies. However, PSG is not a clinically recommended measurement
method to diagnose insomnia (4). Additionally, PSG does not quantify all features of insomnia,
which also include sleep dissatisfaction, daytime impairments, and related symptoms. These
latter daytime symptoms may partially explain the modest relationships between subjective
and objective sleep measures in patients with sleep disorders (5).
In addition to assessing the nighttime features and daytime symptoms of insomnia, mea-
suring insomnia requires the evaluation of exclusionary criteria of insomnia. Therefore, mea-
sures that do not assess insomnia are also required to enhance the ability to discriminate insom-
nia from other sleep or psychiatric disorders, and to determine whether insomnia is present in
a primary or comorbid form.
insomnia from a comorbid insomnia related to depression or other psychiatric disorders. If the
goal is to derive a “caseness” decision for screening purposes, then some questionnaires with
clinical cut-off scores may assist with population-level management efforts. Outcome measure-
ment is an additional area where questionnaires may help both the clinician and the patient
measure progress with treatment.
For the clinical researcher, self-report instruments, actigraphy, and PSG can document
the relative success of treatments of insomnia. Detailed clinical interviews to accurately select
research participants are essential in obtaining replicable and robust findings. For research
methods and instruments, reliability, validity, precision, and sensitivity to change are highly
desirable, since these instrument characteristics decrease the number of subjects needed to
convincingly answer a research question. An additional task for research is theory testing.
To test theories, the items in questionnaires need to relate directly to the concepts proposed
in a theory of insomnia onset, maintenance, or therapy. For example, if a theory proposes
that particularly anxious thoughts contribute to sustaining insomnia, then the theory-related
questionnaire should ask about the presence and frequency of these thoughts.
Two differing types of epidemiological studies also guide choices of questionnaire. For
analytic epidemiological studies, the goal is to pursue causal theories of insomnia by assessing
potential risk factors. For such analytic studies, questionnaires for primary insomnia need to
be quite detailed and elaborate, since primary insomnia is a diagnosis of exclusion. Similarly,
risk factors also need to be queried with high specificity, so that the relationships derived in
the statistical analysis are not susceptible to measurement errors and biases. The second kind of
epidemiological study is largely descriptive. For such an effort, the main goal of measurement
is to estimate the population burden of chronic insomnia. Here it may be possible to use one
of the instruments listed below to derive some estimate of prevalence. This kind of description
will be helpful to health policy planners and services researchers in planning for health resource
utilization.
MEASURES OF INSOMNIA
Self-Report Questionnaires
Whether they are used alone or in conjunction with clinical assessment measures and methods,
self-report questionnaires of insomnia are by far the most common method for assessing insom-
nia in clinical, research, and epidemiological contexts. There are numerous insomnia scales or
single-item measures that have been developed and used across different types of studies (9).
Some self-report instruments are specifically designed to assess the frequency and/or severity
of sleep and wake symptoms of insomnia. Since instruments vary in their popularity, format,
ease of scoring, and psychometrics, clinicians and researchers can make the best choices when
considering instruments’ characteristics in relation to their specific measurement goals and tar-
get populations. Other more global assessments of sleep quality and sleep patterns have been
widely used in clinical, research, and epidemiological sleep studies, and are also described here.
These instruments may have a role in settings where the population studied contains a variety
of sleep disturbances other than chronic insomnia.
The Insomnia Severity Index (ISI) (10,11) is a seven-item questionnaire that assesses sub-
jective severity of insomnia symptoms, degree of satisfaction with sleep, nature and salience
of daytime impairments, and concerns caused by the sleep difficulties. Each item is rated on a
c10 IHBK059-Sateia March 17, 2010 9:29 Char Count=
0 to 4 point scale. A cut-off score of 14 has been shown to optimize sensitivity and specificity.
Scores from zero to seven reflect no clinically significant insomnia; scores from 8 to 14 reflect
subthreshold insomnia; scores from 15 to 21 reflect clinically significant insomnia of moderate
severity; and scores from 22 to 28 reflect clinically significant, severe insomnia. The ISI is consid-
ered to be one of the best standards for evaluation of treatment efficacy in clinical research (3).
Because of its brevity, the ISI can also be comfortably included in large epidemiological studies.
Drawbacks of the ISI include its lack of items specifically focusing on nighttime symptoms and
its dependence on the cognitive theory of insomnia. The ISI has not yet been studied with Item
Response Theory (IRT) methods.
The Spielman Insomnia Symptom Questionnaire (12) contains 13 100-mm visual analog scales
evaluating the frequency of some nighttime features of insomnia, daytime consequences, and
behavior characteristics of insomnia. The questionnaire items are addressed using a visual
analog scale that ranges from “never” at the far left side of the scale (0 mm) to “always” at the
far right (100 mm). The individual items refer to symptoms and events during the week prior to
completing the questionnaire. The total questionnaire score is the average of the 13-item scores
measured as the distance in millimeters from 0 to 100 on each scale. It has been used in clinical
trials, but has not been studied psychometrically in relation to depression, anxiety, or fatigue
scales. It has a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.92, based on data studying the Pittsburgh Insomnia Rating
Scale (PIRS) (5). Clinical cut-off scores have not been determined.
The Pittsburgh Insomnia Rating Scale, 20-item version (PIRS-20) (13,14) is derived from
the original 65-item version of the PIRS. From the original study of insomnia patients and
control subjects, these 20 items were selected based on both their classical and item response
characteristics, to include daytime and nighttime items, excluding the influence of depressive
or anxiety symptomatology. The PIRS-20 focuses on sleep and wake symptoms as they occurred
in the week prior to completion of the instrument. It does not discriminate between primary or
comorbid insomnia. The PIRS-20 assesses the intensity of distress related to 12 nighttime and
daytime symptoms of insomnia on a 0 (not at all bothered) to 3 (severely bothered) scale; four
items on quantitative sleep parameters (e.g., sleep latency, sleep durations) also scored on a 0
to 3 scale, and four items evaluating quality, regularity, and depth of sleep on a 0 (excellent) to
3 (poor) scale. The total score is the simple sum of the item scores, which provides an index of
insomnia severity. A score higher than 20 (out of a maximum of 60) appears to be a useful cut-
off for clinical insomnia (15). The PIRS-20 has a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.95, and has a test–retest
reliability of 0.92. It responds robustly to clinical change (6). Of note, two items of the PIRS-20,
items #9 (lack of energy because of poor sleep) and #18 (satisfaction with your sleep) comprise the
PIRS-2, which can serve as an ultrashort severity questionnaire for screening or epidemiological
purposes. On the PIRS-2, a score equal to or higher than 2 (out of maximum of 6) may serve
as an index of clinically meaningful insomnia. The PIRS-20 was designed as a comparatively
theory-neutral instrument for scaling insomnia severity.
The Athens Insomnia Scale (AIS) (16) is an instrument for measuring insomnia based on the
diagnostic criteria of the International Classification of Diseases, 10th Edition (17). The AIS refers
to symptoms as they occurred in the past month. It has five-item and eight-item versions, with
scores being the simple sum of the item scores. It contains eight items that ask about daytime
and nighttime symptoms, but without containing items that ask about specific time lengths
associated with sleep. A cut-off score of 6 on the AIS can serve as a clinical case definition for
chronic insomnia. The Cronbach’s alpha of the five- and seven-item versions is 0.87 and 0.89,
respectively (16). Its test–retest reliability was 0.89 at a one-week interval. The AIS has not been
evaluated for its correlation against dimensions of depression, anxiety, or fatigue scaling, but
has served well as an outcome measure.
In most epidemiological studies, insomnia is more common among women than men,
particularly during and after the perimenopausal transition. As part of the Women’s Health
Initiative (WHI), researchers utilized the five-item Women’s Health Initiative Insomnia Rating
Scale in a very large sample of middle-aged women (18,19). The period covered by the rating
scale is the four weeks preceding the completion of the questionnaire, and the score is a simple
sum of item scores. Fatigue items were selected out of the questionnaire. This questionnaire
correlated only 0.21 with the Center for Epidemiology Studies Depression scale (20). Clinical
cut-offs were not described but the scale did serve as a useful outcome measure of sleep quality
c10 IHBK059-Sateia March 17, 2010 9:29 Char Count=
provide a psychometrically advanced brief measure of sleep and wake function that can easily
be translated into clinical, research, and epidemiological contexts.
Sleep Diaries
Sleep diaries are a widely used method to gather nightly information on sleep–wake tim-
ing, patterns, and sleep disturbances prospectively over one or two weeks. Because of the
prospective nature of data collected with sleep diaries, recall bias is minimized compared to
retrospective clinical interviews or self-report questionnaires. Sleep diaries also provide more
information regarding night-to-night variability compared to PSG recordings that can only be
practically collected over two nights. Sleep diaries typically collect information about quantita-
tive sleep parameters, including sleep latency, number and duration of nocturnal awakenings,
sleep timing, and total sleep time, and qualitative assessments of sleep quality and feelings
upon awakenings. Many sleep diaries also include items aimed at gathering information of
daytime habits and behaviors that can influence sleep and wake functions, such as napping or
use of alcohol, stimulants, or medications.
Sleep diaries can be useful for screening purposes, but are a complementary piece of
information necessary for diagnostic purposes. For clinical and research purposes, sleep diaries
provide valuable longitudinal data that have been shown to closely correlate with PSG sleep
measures (27,28), and that are sensitive to change related to pharmacological and behavioral
insomnia treatments (12,29).
Numerous sleep diary formats have been used, but sleep diaries do not have an estab-
lished form that has been validated or standardized for use across clinical or research settings.
Nevertheless, they are typically of two common formats: the graphic format [Fig. 1A] and the
text format [Fig. 1B]. These sleep diaries are typically distributed and used in pencil-and-paper
formats, which do not allow for verification that the diary was indeed completed at the expected
time of day, every day. The use of electronic versions of sleep diaries using handheld comput-
ers, web-based dairies, and interactive voice response systems eliminates such undesirable
retrospective reporting.
Actigraphy
Actigraphs are wrist bracelets worn on the nondominant hand that record objective rest and
activity patterns continuously for several consecutives days and nights, with minimal inter-
ference with one’s daily and nightly routines. The bracelet detects and stores the number of
movements per one-minute epoch. The rest–activity patterns are then downloaded and used
as proxies for episodes or sleep and wakefulness. Algorithms code and classify the actigraphic
data collected to derive estimates of sleep latency, total sleep time, wake time after sleep onset,
and sleep fragmentation. Actigraphy is cost-effective (3,4,30–33). While actigraphy has been
used in insomnia research, for example (30,33–35), its use in clinical practice remains controver-
sial. Actigraphy can be used as a screening measure, and can complement clinical evaluation.
However, actigraphy is neither sufficient nor necessary for either diagnostic purposes or mech-
anistic studies of insomnia. It is practical for use in monitoring adherence and for monitoring
and displaying treatment-related changes in sleep.
Midnight
Sleep Quality
p.m. a.m.
Noon
Date Afternoon Evening Morning
12 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
M
T
W
Th
F
Sa
Su
M
T
W
Th
F
Sa
Su
Instructions: Use the symbols below to indicate your Comments
sleep times in the grid. Rate your sleep quality each
night from 0 (poor) to 10 (excellent).
= Go to bed
= Get out of bed
= Actual sleep
SLEEP DIARY BEDTIME KEEP BY BED SLEEP DIARY WAKETIME KEEP BY BED
Please fill out this part of the diary last thing at night. Please fill out this part of the diary first thing in the morning:
Today, when did you have: breakfast went to bed last night at
(if none, write ‘none’) lights out at
lunch
minutes untill fell asleep
dinner finally woke at
How many of the following did you have in each time period?
(if none, leave blank) Awakened by (check one): alarm clock/radio
before or after breakfast after lunch after someone whom I asked to wake me
with before/with before/with dinner noises
breakfast lunch dinner just woke
caffeinated drinks After falling asleep, woke up this many times during the night (circle)
alcoholic drinks 0 1 2 3 4 5 or more
total number of minutes awake
cigarettes
woke to use bathroom (circle # times)
cigars/pipes/plugs
0 1 2 3 4 5 or more
(of chewing tobacco)
awakened by noises/child/bedpartner (circle # times)
Which drugs and medications did you take today? (prescribed & over the counter) 0 1 2 3 4 5 or more
name time dose awakened due to discomfort or physical complaint (circle # times)
0 1 2 3 4 5 or more
just woke (circle # times)
0 1 2 3 4 5 or more
Ratings (place a mark somewhere along the line):
What exercise did you take today? (if none, check here) Sleep Quality:
very very
bad good
start end type
start end type Mood on Final Wakening:
very very
How many daytime naps did you take today? (if none, write 0) tense calm
give times for each: Alertness on Final Wakening:
very very
start end start end sleepy alert
Figure 1 Sleep diaries. (A) Graphic sleep diary. Patients/participants are asked to indicate time in and out of
bed by arrows pointing down and up, respectively, and to mark time spent asleep during the night or during the
day by side arrows pointing to the start and end time of sleep. Hours of the day and night are provided at the top
of the chart. Sleep quality using a 10-point Likert scale can also be logged. (B) Pittsburgh (text) sleep dairy. The
bedtime section is completed in the evening, prior to bed time, and collects information about daytime habits and
behaviors that can influence sleep. The morning wake time section is completed upon awakening in the morning
and inquires about timing of bed time and wake time, sleep latency, number and duration of nocturnal awakenings,
and feelings upon final awakening. Source: From Ref. 51.
c10 IHBK059-Sateia March 17, 2010 9:29 Char Count=
METHODOLOGICAL ISSUES
In selecting instruments for use in the clinic or in research studies, it is important to select
ones that are best suited to the precise measurement goals of the effort and the expected case
prevalence in the studied population. No single instrument is appropriate for all applications.
If the measurement goal is to find as many cases as possible, then an instrument with a high
sensitivity (# of cases/# test-positives) will be best. If the goal is to be sure that the identified
cases really are true cases, then an instrument with a high specificity (# of noncases/# test-
negatives) will be better. However, before deciding on the basis of sensitivity and specificity
alone, consideration of the expected population prevalence in the setting of measurement should
also be modeled with the sensitivities and specificities. With very high prevalence, it may not
be especially informative to use any questionnaire, since virtually everyone will be a case. With
very low prevalence, most of the test-positives will turn out to be false positives. Calculating
the positive and negative predictive values will be helpful.
If the measurement goal is mainly to scale the severity of symptoms, then other consider-
ations come into play. An ideal scale will behave like a thermometer, where only one dimension
of phenomenology is measured and accuracy is uniform at each point along the scale. Often,
semantically based scales have mixtures of dimensions. For example, people often think of
depression and insomnia together. However, when measuring symptoms of depression and
insomnia, it is best to have one scale for sad mood, another for insomnia, and as little correlation
between the two scales as possible. Separating symptom dimensions avoids confounding one’s
measurements. Accuracy of measurement can also present difficulty. In traditional severity
scales, the estimate of severity is the sum of the item scores in the scale. This method, using
what are called short forms, works reasonably well, but may not afford the greatest accuracy
of measurement. A newer general method, IRT, estimates severity as a point along a latent
dimension of severity. This estimator makes the most use of each item’s semantic severity to the
respondent. For example, the PIRS item “Your satisfaction with your sleep” is less severe than
the item “Lack of energy because of poor sleep.” Judging how strongly a person endorses the
second item even when first is strongly endorsed provides greater measurement accuracy than
the summary score from the two items. Based on this principle, it is possible to use a computer
algorithm to estimate symptom severity using only a few items, rather than deploying an entire
short form. This method is called “computer-adapted testing,” and is likely to be increasingly
used as more is learned about the item characteristics of sleep-related symptom questions in
population studies.
CONCLUSION
Insomnia is a prevalent complaint, whether primary or comorbid with other psychiatric, med-
ical, or sleep disorders. The evaluation of both primary and comorbid insomnia requires cap-
turing both the nighttime symptoms and the daytime consequences of insomnia, whether
the context is clinical, experimental, or epidemiological in nature. The evaluation of insomnia
also necessitates the evaluation of other sleep disorders, psychiatric symptoms, and medical
conditions that can be comorbid to or a risk factor for insomnia. Unstructured and struc-
tured clinician-administered interviews, self-report measures, actigraphy, and sleep diaries are
diverse assessment methods and tools that complement each other, and provide a comprehen-
sive assessment of nighttime and daytime symptoms of insomnia. Not all measures, however,
are amenable to all research contexts. For example, sleep dairies may be required for clinical
trials, but may be difficult to implement in epidemiological studies.
c10 IHBK059-Sateia March 17, 2010 9:29 Char Count=
The assessment and measurement of insomnia requires further empirical validation work
in different areas. There is a clear need for developing and validating a reliable clinician-
administered gold standard for the assessment of insomnia that closely parallels the diagnostic
criteria for insomnia. Guidelines and recommendations for the selection of self-report measures
are available (3,50), and a combination of tools is often necessary to fully cover the different
domains of insomnia symptoms and related impairments and distress. Sleep diaries are also
numerous, but the need to achieve consensus on the most effective format and content items
has not yet been addressed. Actigraphy provides the advantages of allowing for objective
measurements over extended periods with minimal burden to the participant, but data collection
parameters and sleep–wake scoring algorithms vary widely, and few have been validated
against PSG or sleep diary measures in patients with insomnia.
In summary, the selection of tools to assess insomnia in a given context (e.g., experimental
mechanistic study vs. clinical trial vs. epidemiological population study) will be strongly guided
by the research aims to be achieved and by the feasibility of using different measurement meth-
ods. The growing use of advanced measurement methods, such as computerized adaptive test-
ing, can increase the sensitivity, specificity, and practicality of insomnia measurements and pro-
vide tools that enhance comparability of findings across study contexts and across populations.
REFERENCES
1. Ohayon MM, Roth T. Place of chronic insomnia in the course of depressive and anxiety disorders. J
Psychiatr Res 2003; 37:9–15.
2. Ohayon MM. Prevalence of DSM-IV diagnostic criteria of insomnia: distinguishing insomnia related
to mental disorders from sleep disorders. J Psychiatr Res 1997; 31(3):333–346.
3. Buysse DJ, Ancoli-Israel S, Edinger JD, et al. Recommendations for a standard research assessment of
insomnia. Sleep 2006; 29(9):1155–1173.
4. Chesson A, Hartse K, Anderson WM, et al. Practice parameters for the evaluation of chronic insomnia.
An American Academy of Sleep Medicine report. Standards of Practice Committee of the American
Academy of Sleep Medicine. Sleep 2000; 23(2):237–241.
5. Edinger JD, Fins AI, Glenn DM, et al. Insomnia and the eye of the beholder: are there clinical markers
of objective sleep disturbances among adults with and without insomnia complaints? J Consult Clin
Psychol 2000; 68(4):586–593.
6. Schramm E, Hohagen F, Grasshoff U, et al. Test-retest reliability and validity of a structured interview
for sleep disorders according to DSM-III-R(SIS-D). Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150(6):867–872.
7. Ohayon MM, Guilleminault C, Zulley J, et al. Validation of the sleep-EVAL system against clinical
assessments of sleep disorders and polysomnographic data. Sleep 1999; 22(7):925–930.
8. Ohayon MM, Guilleminault C, Priest RG. Night terrors, sleepwalking, and confusional arousals in
the general population: their frequency and relationship to other sleep and mental disorders. J Clin
Psychiatry 1999; 60(4):268–276.
9. Moul DE, Hall M, Pilkonis PA, et al. Self-report measures of insomnia adults: rationales, choices, and
needs. Sleep Med Rev 2004; 8:177–198.
10. Bastien CH, Vallieres A, Morin CM. Validation of the Insomnia Severity Index as an outcome measure
for insomnia research. Sleep Med 2001; 2:297–307.
11. Morin CM. Insomnia: Psychological Assessment and Management. New York/London: The Guilford
Press, 1993.
12. Spielman AJ, Saskin P, Thorpy MJ. Treatment of chronic insomnia by restriction of time in bed. Sleep
1987; 10:45–56.
13. Moul D, Mai E, Miewald J, et al. Psychometric study of the Pittsburgh Insomnia Rating Scale (PIRS)
in an initial calibration sample [Abstract]. Sleep 2007; 30(suppl):A343.
14. Moul DE, Pilkonis PA, Miewald JM, et al. Preliminary study of the test-retest reliability and concurrent
validities of the Pittsburgh Insomnia Rating Scale (PIRS) [Abstract]. Sleep 2002; 25(suppl):A246–A247.
15. Moul DE, Mai E, Miewald J, et al. Psychometric study of the Pittsburgh Insomnia Rating Scale (PIRS)
in an initial calibration sample [Abstract]. Sleep 2007; 30(suppl):A343.
16. Soldatos CR, Dikeos DG, Paparrigopoulos TJ. Athens Insomnia Scale: validation of an instrument
based on ICD-10 criteria. J Psychosom Res 2000; 48(6):555–560.
17. World Health Organization.International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health
Problems—10th revision. 10th revision ed. Geneva: World Health Organization, 1992.
18. Levine DW, Kaplan RM, Kripke DF, et al. Factor structure and measurement invariance of the Women’s
Health Initiative Insomnia Rating Scale. Psychol Assess 2003; 15(2):123–136.
19. Levine DW, Kripke DF, Kaplan RM, et al. Reliability and validity of the Women’s Health Initiative
Insomnia Rating Scale. Psychol Assess 2003; 15(2):137–148.
c10 IHBK059-Sateia March 17, 2010 9:29 Char Count=
20. Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl
Psychol Meas 1977; 1(3):385–401.
21. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH, et al. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for
psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res 1989; 28:193–213.
22. Lacks P, Rotert M. Knowledge and practice of sleep hygiene techniques in insomniacs and good
sleepers. Behav Res Ther 1986; 24(3):365–368.
23. Espie CA, Inglis SJ, Harvey L, et al. Insomniacs’ attributions: psychometric properties of the Dysfunc-
tional Beliefs and Attitudes about Sleep Scale and the Sleep Disturbance Questionnaire. J Psychosom
Res 2000; 48(2):141–148.
24. Edinger JD, Wohlgemuth WK. Psychometric comparisons of the standard and abbreviated DBAS-
10 versions of the dysfunctional beliefs and attitudes about sleep questionnaire. Sleep Med 2001;
2(6):493–500.
25. Edinger JD, Wohlgemuth WK, Radtke RA, et al. Does cognitive-behavioral insomnia therapy alter
dysfunctional beliefs about sleep? Sleep 2001; 24(5):591–599.
26. Cella D, Yount S, Rothrock N, et al. The Patient-Reported Outcomes Measurement Information System
(PROMIS): progress of an NIH Roadmap cooperative group during its first two years. Med Care 2007;
45(5, suppl 1):S3–S11.
27. Coates TJ, Killen JD, George J, et al. Estimating sleep parameters: a multitrait–multimethod analysis.
J Consult Clin Psychol 1982; 50(3):345–352.
28. Riedel BW, Lichstein KL. Objective sleep measures and subjective sleep satisfaction: how do older
adults with insomnia define a good night’s sleep? Psychol Aging 1998; 13(1):159–163.
29. Krystal AD, Walsh JK, Laska E, et al. Sustained efficacy of eszopiclone over 6 months of nightly
treatment: results of a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study in adults with chronic
insomnia. Sleep 2003; 26(7):793–799.
30. Lichstein KL, Stone KC, Donaldson J, et al. Actigraphy validation with insomnia. Sleep 2006; 29(2):232–
239.
31. Morgenthaler T, Alessi C, Friedman L, et al. Practice parameters for the use of actigraphy in the
assessment of sleep and sleep disorders: an update for 2007. Sleep 2007; 30(4):519–529.
32. Sadeh A, Hauri PJ, Kripke DF, et al. The role of actigraphy in the evaluation of sleep disorders. Sleep
1995; 18(4):288–302.
33. Verbeek I, Arends J, Declerck G, et al. Wrist actigraphy in comparison with polysomnography and
subjective evaluation in insomnia. Sleep-Wake Res Neth 1994; 5:163–169.
34. Hauri PJ, Wisbey J. Wrist actigraphy in insomnia. Sleep 1992; 15(4):293–301.
35. Vallieres A, Morin CM. Actigraphy in the assessment of insomnia. Sleep 2003; 26(7):902–906.
36. Johns MW. Reliability and factor analysis of the Epworth Sleepiness Scale. Sleep 1992; 15(4):
376–381.
37. Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth Sleepiness Scale. Sleep
1991; 14(6):540–545.
38. Maislin G, Pack AI, Kribbs NB et al. A survey screen for prediction of apnea. Sleep 1995; 18(3):158–166.
39. Smets EMA, Garssen B, Bonke B, et al. The Multidimensional Fatigue Inventory (MFI) psychometric
qualities of an instrument to assess fatigue. J Psychosom Res 1995; 39:315–325.
40. Krupp LB, LaRocca NG, Muir-Nash J, et al. The fatigue severity scale. Application to patients with
multiple sclerosis and systemic lupus erythematosus. Arch Neurol 1989; 46(10):1121–1123.
41. First M, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, et al. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality
Disorders (SCID-II), Version 2.0. Version 2.0 ed. New York: New York State Psychiatric Institute, 1994.
42. Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1960; 23:56–62.
43. Hamilton M. The assessment of anxiety states by rating. Br J Med Psychol 1959; 32:50–55.
44. Beck AT, Steer RA. Manual for Beck Depression Inventory. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corpora-
tion, 1987.
45. Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, et al. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry
1961; 4:561–571.
46. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Manual for Beck Depression Inventory II (BDI-II). San Antonio, TX:
Psychology Corporation, 1996.
47. Rush AJ, Gullion CM, Basco MR, et al. The Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology (IDS): psycho-
metric properties. Psychol Med 1996; 26(3):477–486.
48. Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G, et al. An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric prop-
erties. J Consult Clin Psychol 1988; 56:893–897.
49. Spielberger CD. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Adults. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists
Press, Inc., 1968.
50. Moul DE, Buysse DJ. Evaluation of insomnia. In: Lee-Chiong TL, ed. Sleep: A Comprehensive Hand-
book. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 2006:117–123.
51. Monk TH, Reynolds CF, Kupfer DJ, et al. The Pittsburgh Sleep Diary. J Sleep Res 1994; 3:111–120.
c11 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:26 Char Count=
INTRODUCTION
Classification in medicine serves several purposes. In the clinical arena, disease classification is
used to identify individuals with a consistent set of symptoms and signs. This permits condi-
tions of a similar type or etiology to be recognized and discriminated from other conditions,
and helps to guide therapeutic interventions. In serving these functions, classification provides
a sort of “shorthand” for communication among professionals. Additionally, classification is
important for administrative functions such as medical record-keeping and billing. Within the
research arena, classification provides consistency in the identification of “cases” for scien-
tific investigation into epidemiology, course, etiology, pathophysiology, and treatment. Ideally,
the conceptualization and classification of disorders informs research into their etiology and
treatment, and such research leads back to refinements of conceptualization and classification.
Generalizations regarding etiology and treatment efficacy depend on reliable and valid diag-
noses. In this chapter, we will briefly review some general aspects of disease classification, and
present an overview regarding classification of insomnia, highlighting findings from empirical
research studies.
Definitions
At the most basic clinical level, symptoms are defined as complaints or other subjective evidence
presented by the patient. Signs are objective indicators of a health, illness, or functioning. Thus,
“difficulty falling asleep” is a symptom; polysomnographic sleep latency value of 32 minutes
is a sign. A syndrome is a characteristic set of symptoms and/or signs, which often follow
a characteristic clinical course. An illness or disorder typically encompasses the elements of a
syndrome, but also includes the connotation of suffering and a deviation from the normal state
of function. Finally, the term disease includes all the elements of a syndrome, but also connotes a
particular etiology. The etiology and pathophysiology of different diseases may vary widely. For
instance, diseases may be defined in terms of morbid anatomy (e.g., mitral stenosis), cellular
pathology (e.g., cancer), molecular pathology (e.g., porphyria) or an infectious agent (e.g.,
tuberculosis). A diagnosis is the determination of the nature of a case of illness or disorder, derived
from the Latin verb meaning “to recognize”. More commonly, diagnosis refers to the label placed
on a specific case of disease, illness or disorder. A nosology is a classification system of diseases, an
organizational structure comprising a set of specific diagnoses. Classification systems may use
categorical or dimensional models of disorders and diseases, or some combination of the two (as is
the case with sleep disorders classifications). A categorical model assumes that individuals with
a disorder/disease differ in some fundamental, noncontinuous way from the remainder of the
population, allowing a clean distinction between affected and unaffected individuals. In sleep
medicine, narcolepsy is an example of a disease, which can be classified in categorical terms;
an individual either has narcolepsy or does not. In dimensional disease models, affected and
unaffected individuals are seen as coming from a single population, with affected individuals
exceeding a specified threshold. In sleep medicine, sleep apnea is an example of a disorder
based on a dimensional threshold.
According to these definitions, insomnia may best be seen as a symptom or a disorder
rather than a true disease. Sometimes “insomnia” is used to refer to an isolated complaint
(symptom); in other cases, it is used to indicate a disorder, i.e., a consistent set of symptoms
and signs which causes distress or impairment, but for which no precise etiology has been
defined. Whether as a symptom or disorder, insomnia is most often conceptualized in dimen-
sional rather than categorical terms, which may help to explain why the percentage of affected
c11 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:26 Char Count=
individuals may vary from study to study, and why there is occasionally confusion as to whether
an individual should be considered to have a disorder at all. However, the ultimate goal of clas-
sification, even in insomnia diagnosis, is to assign individuals to discrete diagnostic categories
for purposes of clinical management or research investigation.
Finally, many disorders and diseases can have variable presentations of symptoms and
signs. For some diseases, pathological structure is the only criterion; cancer is an example. For
other diseases, even those with known pathophysiology, symptoms and signs can quite vari-
able; systemic lupus erythematosus and multiple sclerosis are two common examples in which
the pathophysiology is known, but specific manifestations vary considerably from one individ-
ual to another. Finally, disorders without established pathophysiology or etiology, defined by
symptoms and signs alone, can be highly variable among individuals. For instance, two indi-
viduals who meet the criteria for major depressive disorder may have virtually no symptoms in
common. Insomnia disorders encompass a similar wide degree of variability. For instance, the
criteria for general insomnia disorder according to the International Classification of Sleep Dis-
orders, Second Edition (ICSD-2) encompass individuals who present with sleep onset difficulty
and irritability as well as individuals with nonrestorative sleep and daytime fatigue.
100 BUYSSE
than four weeks (1). This type of classification, however, is recognized as being an intermediate
step toward more specific etiologically-based diagnoses, which often differ in their duration. For
instance, transient insomnia complaints are often associated with situational, environmental, or
medical stresses, and chronic insomnia is more often associated with psychiatric disorders, cir-
cadian rhythm disorders, or primary dysregulation of sleep-wake mechanisms. Duration-based
insomnia disorders are to some extent recognized in more formal nosologies such as ICSD-2
in disorders such as adjustment sleep disorder, with a duration of less than three months, and
idiopathic insomnia, with a lifelong course. Empirical evidence and epidemiological studies
also address potential subtypes based on duration. A Gallup Poll conducted for the National
Sleep Foundation suggested that approximately 30% of adults complained of transient, short-
term, or recurrent insomnia, and 10% complained of chronic problems (9). The cross-sectional
national Comorbidity Survey Replication study demonstrated a more bimodal distribution of
insomnia duration, with approximately 20% of patients reporting symptoms of less than four
weeks duration, and approximately 30% reporting symptoms for greater than one year (10).
Some longitudinal studies suggest a continuum of insomnia duration, including occasional
insomnia, repeated brief insomnia, and more persistent forms (11,12). Studies with follow-up
intervals of one to three years suggest that insomnia symptoms persist in approximately 50% to
80% of individuals, depending on the definition of insomnia symptoms (13,14). Other data sug-
gest that more severe forms of insomnia have greater chronicity. A three-year follow-up study
demonstrated remission in approximately 40% of individuals with insomnia symptoms, whereas
approximately 80% of individuals with insomnia syndrome had a chronic course (15). Data from
the Wisconsin Sleep Cohort indicate that approximately two-third of individuals with insomnia
symptoms have persistent symptoms at 3 and 10 year follow-up (16). Therefore, some empirical
data support the distinction between duration-based insomnia subtypes. However, duration in
itself provides little guidance in terms of appropriate interventions, but merely points toward
likely contributing factors that can be specifically addressed.
Another common, but informal, method for classifying insomnia is by the specific symp-
tom presentation: sleep onset insomnia, sleep maintenance insomnia, or nonrestorative sleep.
Epidemiological and survey data demonstrate that sleep maintenance insomnia is consistently
the most prevalent symptom, and early morning awakening the least common.(17) Moreover,
symptom types vary with age, with sleep onset insomnia and nonrestorative sleep being rela-
tively more common in younger adults, and sleep maintenance insomnia being more common
with increasing age [e.g., (17,18)]. However, such studies also demonstrate the high rate of cooc-
currence of individual symptoms, and the strong correlation between them. For instance, in the
National Comorbidity Survey Replication, each of the symptoms correlated with each of the
others with r values of 0.65 to 0.84 (10), and up to 80% of insomnia patients have more than one
symptom when followed longitudinally (16). Moreover, longitudinal studies demonstrate that
individuals’ specific symptom type frequently changes over time (16,19). Insomnia symptom
types may provide clues to specific contributing factors such as Restless Legs Syndrome or
subclinical circadian phase delays (20) in individuals with sleep onset complaints. Symptom
patterns are also considered in selecting specific treatments. For instance, a patient who presents
with predominantly sleep onset complaints may be an appropriate candidate for a short-acting
benzodiazepine receptor agonist, and a patient with predominantly sleep maintenance com-
plaints may benefit from a longer-acting agent. However, because of the frequent cooccurrence
of symptoms and their poor stability over time, symptom subtype does not appear to be an
adequate basis for diagnostic classification. Formal diagnostic classifications for insomnia have
been proposed as part of more comprehensive sleep disorders nosologies. These classifications
distinguish different insomnia disorders on the basis of specific sleep symptoms, other clinical
features (e.g., duration, polysomnographic features), and the presence or absence of other sleep,
general medical, and psychiatric disorders. Table 1 outlines the major classification systems for
sleep disorders used the past 30 years: the diagnostic classification of sleep and arousal disorders
(DCSAD) (2); the international classification of sleep disorders (ICSD) (21,22); the International
Classification of Sleep Disorders, 2nd Edition (ICSD-2) (23); the Diagnostic and Statistical Man-
ual of Mental Disorders, Third Edition Revised (DSM-III-R) (24); and Fourth Edition (DSM-IV)
(25); and the International Classification of Diseases, 9th Edition, Clinical Modification (ICD-
9CM) (26) and 10th Edition (27). These classification systems differ substantially in terms of their
c11
IHBK059-Sateia
INSOMNIA DIAGNOSIS AND CLASSIFICATION
International
Diagnostic and Statistical International
Statistical Manual Classification of Statistical
Diagnostic of Mental Diseases and Related Classification of
8:26
Classification of International International Disorders, Diagnostic and Health Problems, Diseases and
Sleep and Arousal Classification of Classification of 3rd Edition Statistical Manual of 9th Edition, Clinical Related Health
Disorders Sleep Disorders Sleep Disorders, 2nd Revised Mental Disorders, 4th Modification Problems, 10th
Char Count=
(DCSAD) (2) (ICSD) (21,22) Edition (ICSD-2) (23) (DSM-III-R) (24) Edition (DSM-IV) (25) (ICD-9-CM) (26) Edition (ICD-10) (27)
r Disorders of r Dyssomnias r Insomnia r Dyssomnias r Primary sleep r Sleep disturbances r Sleep disorders
Initiating and – Intrinsic r Sleep-related – Insomnia disorders (organic) (organic)
maintaining – Extrinsic breathing disorders disorder – Dyssomnias r Specific disorders r Sleep disorders
sleep (DIMS) – Circadian rhythm r Hypersomnias of – Hypersomnia – Parasomnias of sleep of due to emotional
r Disorders of sleep disorders central origin disorder r Sleep disorders nonorganic origin causes
excessive sleep r Parasomnias r Circadian rhythm – Sleep–wake related to another
(DOES) r Sleep disorders– sleep disorders schedule mental disorder
r Disorders of the associated with r Parasomnias disorder r Sleep disorders due
sleep–wake mental, neurologic, r Sleep-related r Parasomnias to medical disorder
schedule or medical movement r Substance-induced
r Parasomnias disorders disorders sleep disorder
r Proposed sleep r Isolated symptoms,
disorders apparently normal
variants, and
unresolved issues
r Other sleep
disorders
101
c11 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:26 Char Count=
102 BUYSSE
organizational schemes, the number of specific diagnoses, and their reliance on specific criteria.
An overview of these features is presented in Table 2. Specific insomnia diagnoses within each
classification are outlined in Table 3.
The DCSAD, the first widely used classification of sleep disorders, includes four major
categories of disorders: Disorders of Initiating and Maintaining Sleep (DIMS or insomnias), Dis-
orders of Excessive Sleepiness (DOES or hypersomnias), Disorders of the Sleep/Wake Schedule,
and Parasomnias. This classification is easy to use, given its symptom-based organization. The
number of individual categories is large, and approximately 30 diagnoses could conceivably
include a complaint of insomnia. The DCSAD provides useful clinical descriptions of each dis-
order. However, it also has some important limitations. First, specific clinical and polysomno-
graphic criteria are not included, which necessitates considerable clinical judgment in establish-
ing a diagnosis and is likely to decrease reliability. Second, the symptom-based approach makes
sense clinically but leads to duplicate listings for a single disorder. For instance, periodic limb
movement disorder can produce either insomnia or hypersomnia, so the diagnosis was listed
twice in the classification, in both the DIMS and DOES sections.
The ICSD is a successor to the DCSAD, published in 1990. It differs from the DCSAD clas-
sification in two major ways. First, the organization of disorders is by presumed etiology rather
than symptom presentation. Second, the ICSD includes specific clinical and polysomnographic
criteria, severity ratings, and both “minimal” and “complete” diagnostic criteria for each dis-
order. The three major categories of sleep disorders in ICSD are dyssomnias, parasomnias, and
secondary sleep disorders. Dyssomnias are disorders in which the major signs and symptoms
relate to sleep and/or wakefulness. The dyssomnias are further subdivided into intrinsic dys-
somnias, in which some abnormality of brain function is thought to underlie the symptoms
and signs; extrinsic disorders, in which some external factor leads to the sleep complaint; and
circadian rhythm sleep disorders, which are thought to result from abnormal entrainment or
misalignment of the circadian system with external time cues. Secondary sleep disorders are those
associated with, and presumed to be causally related to, mental, neurologic, and medical disor-
ders. In contrast to dyssomnias, secondary sleep disorders occur within a context of broader set
of symptoms and signs of the “primary” disorder. Of the 84 diagnoses in ICSD, approximately
43 may be associated with insomnia complaints, including those in Dyssomnia and Secondary
Sleep Disorder categories. ICSD defines insomnia as in general as a “complaint of an insuffi-
cient amount of sleep or not feeling rested after the habitual sleep episode.” Daytime symptoms
associated with insomnia, such as restlessness, irritability, anxiety, fatigue, and tiredness, are
described in the severity criteria, rather than in the insomnia disorders themselves.
Although the ICSD has several advantages compared DCSAD, it also has some limitations,
many of which apply to other classifications as well. First, diagnoses are based largely upon
expert opinion, although literature reviews were also used. Second, for most sleep disorders,
the exact pathophysiology remains unknown, despite the broad pathophysiological orientation,
which drives ICSD. Third, most specific diagnostic criteria in the ICSD have not been rigorously
tested against alternative criteria, being based largely upon expert opinion. Fourth, there is
some concern that the ICSD’s large number of categories may represent a form of pseudo
precision. For instance, Reynolds and colleagues (28) argued that sub-typing chronic insomnia
into different disorders may be premature, given the uncertain validity of concepts such as
sleep state misperception, environmental sleep disorder, and inadequate sleep hygiene. Finally,
the specific diagnostic criteria for certain disorders have been questioned by expert interest
groups within those areas. For instance, alternative criteria had been proposed for the diagnosis
of restless leg syndrome and periodic limb movements, (29) sleep apnea syndromes, (30) and
narcolepsy (31,32).
ICSD-2 was published in 2005. The framework of ICSD-2 departs substantially from
ICSD in two major ways. First, ICSD-2 groups sleep disorders into eight major categories
based on major symptom presentation and presumed etiology (Table 2). Insomnia is one of
these categories, and 11 specific insomnia diagnoses are grouped in that section. Brief descrip-
tions of these diagnoses are presented in Table 3. Second, ICSD-2 specifies general criteria for
insomnia which apply to all diagnoses within the Insomnia section (Table 4). The important
elements of this definition are (1) a sleep-related insomnia complaint; (2) adequate oppor-
tunity and circumstances to sleep, clearly demarcating insomnia from problems related to
c11
IHBK059-Sateia
INSOMNIA DIAGNOSIS AND CLASSIFICATION
DCSAD (2) ICSD (21,22) ICSD-2 (23) DSM-III-R (24) DSM-IV (25) ICD-9-CM (26) ICD-10 (27)
Char Count=
Derivation Expert opinion Expert opinion, Expert opinion, Expert opinion Expert opinion, Expert opinion Expert opinion
literature reviews literature reviews literature reviews
Major organizing Symptoms Presumed Symptoms, Symptoms Presumed Presumed Presumed
feature pathophysiology/ Presumed pathophysiology/ pathophysiology/ pathophysiology/
etiology pathophysiology/ etiology etiology etiology
etiology
Breadth of Narrow Narrow Narrow Broad Intermediate Broad Broad
categories
Number of specific 68 88 81 15 25 18 15
disorders
Polysomnographic No Yes, for some Yes, for some No No No No
criteria disorders, but not disorders, but not
insomnia insomnia
103
c11 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:26 Char Count=
104 BUYSSE
(A) A complaint of difficulty initiating sleep, difficulty maintaining sleep, or waking up too early or sleep that
is chronically nonrestorative or poor in quality. In children, the sleep difficulty is often reported by the
caretaker and may consist of observed bedtime resistance or inability to sleep independently.
(B) The above sleep difficulty occurs despite adequate opportunity and circumstances for sleep.
(C) At least one of the following forms of daytime impairment related to the nighttime sleep difficulty is
reported by the patient:
(i) Fatigue or malaise
(ii) Attention, concentration, or memory impairment
(iii) Social or vocational dysfunction or poor school performance
(iv) Mood disturbance or irritability
(v) Daytime sleepiness
(vi) Motivation, energy, or initiative reduction
(vii) Proneness for errors or accidents at work or while driving
(viii) Tension, headaches, or gastrointestinal symptoms in response to sleep loss
(ix) Concerns or worries about sleep
106 BUYSSE
brings the “secondary” insomnia disorders due to mental disorder, medical disorder, or sub-
stance abuse, into the insomnia section. Although ICSD-2 identifies insomnia disorders within
a single section, clinicians must also be mindful of the fact that many, in fact most, other major
sections within the nosology also contain diagnoses that may include an insomnia complaint as
one element of the clinical presentation. For instance, insomnia complaints are ubiquitous in the
circadian rhythm disorders and common among breathing and movement disorders as well.
Even patients with narcolepsy often report insomnia, occasionally as a presenting complaint.
Insomnia symptoms associated with these other major sleep disorders may coexist with major
insomnia diagnoses (primary or comorbid), requiring that both disorders be targeted effectively
in treatment.ICSD-2 employs the terminology of “insomnia due to. . .” in identifying what have
historically been referred to as “secondary” insomnia conditions. As discussed above, current
evidence suggests the importance of moving away from the concept of secondary insomnia and
toward that of comorbid insomnia.
The DSM-III-R and DSM-IV were designed as classifications of mental disorders but also
include sleep disorders sections. DSM-III-R includes two major sections of sleep disorders, dys-
somnias and parasomnias. The dyssomnias include insomnia disorder, hypersomnia disorder, and
other dyssomnias; within insomnia disorder are three subtypes, related to another mental dis-
order, related to a known organic factor, and primary insomnia. In DSM-IV, the major divisions
of sleep disorders are arranged differently, and include a small number of additional categories.
DSM-IV major divisions are Primary Sleep Disorders, which include dyssomnias and parasom-
nias; sleep disorders related to another mental disorder; sleep disorder due to a general medical
disorder (including insomnia type); and substance-induced sleep disorder (including insomnia
type). Thus, there is some homology between the organization of DSM-III-R and DCSAD, and
between DSM-IV and ICSD. The main difference between the psychiatric and sleep medicine
classifications is in the number of specific categories and the breadth of these diagnoses. For
instance, DSM-IV includes only one diagnosis for chronic “primary” insomnia unrelated to
mental, medical, or substance-induced sleep disorders (primary insomnia). This category sub-
sumes more specific ICSD categories such as psychophysiological insomnia, inadequate sleep
hygiene, idiopathic hypersomnia, adjustment sleep disorder, and environmental sleep disorder,
both in theory and in practice (34). Like the ICSD, DSM-IV was derived from expert opinion and
literature reviews, and the organization is based broadly on presumed pathophysiology. Also
like ICSD, it includes clinical criteria, but unlike ICSD, it does not include specific polysomno-
graphic criteria or severity descriptors. Although DSM-IV is appropriate for and appears to be
well- accepted by the psychiatric community, it is not widely used by sleep disorders specialists.
ICD-9CM and ICD-10 include two broad categories of sleep disorders, those of organic
origin, and those of nonorganic origin (due to emotional causes). In ICD-10, Organic sleep disorders
include insomnia, hypersomnia, circadian disorders, sleep apnea, and narcolepsy. Sleep disorders
due to emotional causes include insomnia, hypersomnia, circadian rhythm disorders, sleepwalk-
ing, sleep terrors, and nightmares. Specific diagnostic descriptions are provided only for the
nonorganic sleep disorders. The distinction between organic and nonorganic is largely arbitrary
and may be difficult to operationalize in clinical practice. Therefore, ICD-9CM and ICD-10 are
not widely used clinically for descriptive purposes. However, the diagnosis codes from ICD-
9CM have been cross-referenced to ICSD, ICSD-2, and DSM-IV, and these codes are widely used
for billing and records keeping purposes.
Although it was developed specifically as a research tool, the Research Diagnostic Criteria
for Insomnia (RDC-I) (33) used a rigorous review process and consensus to evaluate reliability
and validity data for various insomnia disorders. More than 400 published research studies of
insomnia were examined to determine how often various criteria were used in the selection
and characterization of study samples, and cross-referenced these actual study selection criteria
against the specific DSM-IV or ICSD diagnosis which was said to be studied. For example, actual
selection criteria included the use of specific insomnia diagnoses, insomnia duration, medical,
psychiatric, and medication exclusions, self-reported symptoms, and polysomnographic find-
ings. Based on this literature review and a consensus process, the work group developed research
criteria for those diagnoses with the greatest empirical support. This led to the development
of research diagnostic criteria for general insomnia disorder, nine specific insomnia disorders,
and normal sleepers. The nine specific disorders include primary insomnia, insomnia due to
c11 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:26 Char Count=
Reliability
Reliability is a measure of the extent to which diagnoses are reproducible across time (test-retest
reliability), across multiple ratings by a single rater (intra-rater reliability), and among different
raters (inter-rater reliability). Several sources of variance contribute to reduced reliability. First,
test-retest reliability may be low because patients’ actual clinical state may change from one
time to another. Inter-rater reliability may be imperfect because the judgments of one clinician
will not perfectly match those of another. In addition, both types of reliability may suffer
from measurement error, either in the sensitivity of particular questions asked of a patient,
or of physiological measures such as EEG sleep studies. Similarly, the interpretation of specific
measurements may vary across raters and over time. Finally, the specific criteria used to establish
a diagnosis may be imprecise. Table 5 summarizes data on inter-rater and test-retest reliability
for insomnia diagnoses using various diagnostic criteria. The table illustrates that the total
number of reliability studies and the total number of insomnia patients assessed has been small.
The most rigorous work has been conducted with ICSD-2 and DSM-IV, although publication of
peer-reviewed findings is pending at this time.
Although not a measure of reliability per se, one would expect similar patterns of insomnia
diagnoses to be made at different sleep medicine centers. A field trial for DSM-IV examined
diagnostic patterns in five sleep disorders centers found similar overall patterns of diagnoses,
but significantly different relative frequencies for specific diagnoses (36). For instance, some
sites made frequent diagnoses of insomnia related to another mental disorder, and very few
108 BUYSSE
diagnoses of Primary Insomnia, whereas other sites had the opposite pattern. This study also
examined diagnostic patterns of two different interviewers at each site, one a sleep specialist
and one a nonsleep specialist clinician. The two types of interviewers had similar patterns of
overall diagnoses, but nonspecialists tended to make more diagnoses of insomnia due to a
medical condition, whereas sleep specialists tended to make more diagnoses such as delayed
sleep phase syndrome.
Other studies have examined more traditional measures of inter-rater reliability. Schramm
et al (37) used a structured diagnostic interview to establish DSM-III-R insomnia diagnoses in
a sample of 68 patients with complaints of sleep disturbances. The overall kappa value for
insomnia disorders was 0.91, indicating excellent inter-rater agreement. With regard to specific
types of insomnia, kappa values ranged from 0.84 to 0.86 for insomnia related to a mental
disorder, insomnia related to a known organic factor, and primary insomnia. Of note, the kappa
for primary insomnia was derived from a total of only 10 cases, which again makes the reliability
of this estimate uncertain.
The DSM-IV field trial included 216 patients clinically referred for a sleep disorder and
evaluated by two interviewers, one an experienced sleep specialist and one a nonsleep specialist
clinician. The two raters used their “usual clinical interview” to assess patients. Kappa values for
the primary diagnosis ranged from 0.35 to 0.56 across the five sites, indicating only moderate
diagnostic agreement. Kappa values for specific diagnoses were somewhat worse, ranging
from 0.28 to 0.59 for primary insomnia and 0.34 to 0.60 for insomnia related to another mental
disorder. The lower degree of inter-rater reliability in this study likely related to the use of clinical
interviews, rather than structured interviews, and clinicians who varied in their sleep expertise.
However, videotaped interviews for a subset of 41 patients were reviewed by sleep specialists
at each of the five sites. The overall kappa value for DSM-IV and ICSD diagnoses ranged from
0.22 to 0.25, in the fair to poor range(38). Thus, it appears that different raters, even among sleep
specialists, make use of clinical information in different ways to establish diagnoses.
Another small study (39) examined inter-rater agreement for DSM-III-R and ICSD diag-
noses in 31 clinically referred patients with insomnia, with diagnoses based on clinical and
polysomnographic information rather than direct interview with the patients. A kappa value
of 0.71 was found for DSM-III-R diagnoses, and 0.68 for ICSD diagnoses, within the moderate
to very good range of reliability.
No published studies have formally assessed the test-retest reliability of insomnia diag-
noses. The study by Schramm and colleagues described above indicates that test-retest reliability
was assessed, but the interval between interviews was only one to three days. In addition, this
study combined elements of both a test-retest and inter-rater reliability study.
More recent preliminary data from a two-site study examined the reliability and validity of
DSM-IV and ICSD-2 insomnia diagnoses in 333 patients.(40) Each patient was interviewed by six
sleep specialists, using various combinations of clinical interview, structured interview, and PSG
information. Inter-rater correlations for DSM-IV diagnoses were highest for disorders that might
be considered secondary or comorbid forms of insomnia, such as alcohol-induced, insomnia
related to another mental or medical disorder, breathing-related sleep disorder, and circadian
rhythm sleep disorder, with inter-rater r values of approximately 0.40 to 0.70. DSM-IV primary
insomnia and other forms of insomnia were least reliable, with r values of approximately 0.10
to 0.30. Among ICSD-2 diagnoses, highest reliability was associated with insomnia related to a
mental or medical disorder, restless legs syndrome, idiopathic insomnia, delayed sleep phase
syndrome, and obstructive sleep apnea (r values approximately 0.34–0.68); lowest reliability
was seen for paradoxical insomnia. Some ICSD-2 insomnia diagnoses were never chosen. The
authors conclude that reliability and validity of some, but not all, DSM-IV and ICSD-2 diagnoses
was supported, and that the former nosology may have too few categories, and the latter too
many. Further data from this study indicated that use of a structured diagnostic interview
was associated with greater reliability against “gold standard” diagnoses than standard clinical
interviews.(41)
In summary, available data suggest moderate inter-rater reliability for insomnia diagnoses
using DSM-IV, ICSD, and ICSD-2 criteria. Test-retest reliability has yet to be adequately assessed.
The finding of significant differences among sleep disorder centers and the diagnoses they
establish, and the low rates of inter-rater agreement among sleep specialists at different sites,
c11 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:26 Char Count=
suggest that different investigators apply the diagnostic criteria for insomnia disorders in very
different ways. Data from a small number of studies also suggest moderate improvements in
reliability with the use of structured interview and/or PSG data.
Validity
Validity is a measure of the extent to which a diagnosis serves its purposes of case identification,
clinical prediction, or communication. Several types of validity are commonly investigated. Face
validity or ecological validity describes how reasonable a diagnosis is based on clinical experience;
in other words, does the diagnosis make sense and reflect a condition seen in the real world?
Construct validity refers to the degree to which a diagnosis (or other measurement scale) measures
the theoretical, etiological, or pathological construct it is designed to measure, and is often
inferred from discriminant and convergent validity. Descriminant validity relates to how well
specific features discriminate one diagnosis from other diagnoses. Convergent validity refers to
the degree to which one diagnosis or set of diagnostic criteria relates to similar diagnoses or
criteria. Predictive validity assesses the degree to which a diagnosis corresponds with a particular
natural history or treatment response.
A large number of studies have been reported which describe distinctive clinical features
or polysomnographic features of one type of insomnia diagnosis compared to other insomnia
diagnoses. For instance, traditional polysomnographic and clinical characteristics that distin-
guish “chronic insomnia” from the insomnia of depression (42,43) or “objective insomnia”
from “subjective insomnia” (44,45) have been evaluated. Other analyses have demonstrated
quantitative EEG sleep differences between subjects with psychophysiological insomnia, para-
doxical insomnia, insomnia associated with depression, and good sleepers (46,47). Two studies
have also demonstrated similarities and differences in the 24-hour metabolic rate patterns of
psychophysological and paradoxical insomnia patients, both of whom differed from controls
(48–50). However, confirmatory analyses and replications have been infrequently reported with
all of these analyses. Likewise, the sensitivity, specificity, or receiver-operating characteristic
curves of specific polysomnographic or clinical features have not been described for one subtype
of insomnia versus another. By contrast, such studies have been conducted for measures such as
reduced REM latency in depression (51,52). Perhaps partly as a result of infrequently-replicated
findings, current clinical recommendations do not support the use of polysomnography for
diagnosis in most patients with insomnia (53,54).
Another method of assessing construct validity is to determine how clinical and
polysomnographic features empirically cluster, and to match such empirical clusters to clinical
diagnoses. Hauri (55) reported a cluster analysis of psychological tests, clinical interview, and
three nights of polysomnography in 89 insomnia patients and 10 controls compared to DCSAD
diagnoses. Factor analysis identified 26 factors derived from these assessments, and subsequent
cluster analysis provided empirical validation for the category of persistent psychophysiolog-
ical insomnia, insomnia associated with affective disorder, and childhood onset (idiopathic)
insomnia. However, six other clusters did not readily correspond to existing DCSAD diagnoses.
A similar study used questionnaire data, polysomnographic data, and interview data to derive
15 factors in 113 insomnia patients and 39 healthy controls (39). The 14 empirically-identified
clusters did not correspond strongly with either DSM-III-R or ICSD diagnoses. Caution is war-
ranted given the small number of patients and controls relative to the large number of factors
in both of these studies.
Construct validity, focusing on distinctions between primary insomnia and insomnia
related to another mental disorder, was also addressed in the DSM-IV field trial described
above (56). Clinicians were asked to rate factors they thought might contribute to the individual
patients insomnia complaint regardless of diagnosis, then assigned a primary diagnosis and
up to three secondary diagnoses for each patient. Patients with insomnia related to a mental
disorder were identified as having less evidence of poor sleep hygiene and negative conditioning
than patients with primary insomnia. Conversely, patients with primary insomnia were felt
to have significantly less psychiatric disturbance contributing to their insomnia. These data
support the distinction between the insomnia subtypes of primary and psychiatric insomnia,
and suggest that additional criteria reflecting sleep hygiene and conditioning factors may help
to improve the reliability of these diagnoses.
c11 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:26 Char Count=
110 BUYSSE
Clinicians also use diagnostic judgments to make treatment decisions. Further data
from the DSM-IV field trial (38) showed that the pattern of diagnostic recommendations for
different specific insomnia diagnoses do in fact differ in significant ways. Specifically, treatment
recommendations for ICSD diagnoses of psychophysiological insomnia, delayed sleep phase
syndrome, inadequate sleep hygiene, insomnia related to mood disorder, and obstructive
sleep apnea syndrome have very distinct patterns of treatment recommendations. Treatment
recommendations are not equivalent to treatment outcomes, but these data do provide some
support for the notion that clinicians believe their diagnoses have predictive validity in the
clinical setting.
Finally, the two-site study of DSM-IV and ICSD-2 diagnoses described above also
addressed questions of convergent validity using multitrait/multi-method matrices based on
the diagnoses of six raters (57). These analyses demonstrated strong validity indices for ICSD-2
idiopathic insomnia, moderate indices for psychophysiological insomnia, environmental sleep
disorder, and inadequate sleep hygiene, and low indices for paradoxical insomnia. DSM-IV
primary insomnia had lower validity indices than all ICSD-2 diagnoses except paradoxical
insomnia. These findings suggest that specific diagnostic criteria may improve the reliability
and validity of insomnia diagnoses, in contrast to the broad and nonspecific criteria used in
DSM-IV primary insomnia.
with careful clinical and physiological phenotyping. Such studies would also help to fulfill the
goals of personalized medicine, by permitting an examination of treatment response in relation
to carefully characterized patient phenotypes.
REFERENCES
1. National Institute of Mental Health. Consensus conference report: Drugs and insomnia—the use of
medication to promote sleep. JAMA 1984; 251:2410–2414.
2. Association of Sleep Disorders Centers, Sleep Disorders Classification Committee, Roffwarg HP. Diag-
nostic classification of sleep and arousal disorders. Sleep 1979; 2:1–137.
3. McCrae CS, Lichstein KL. Secondary insomnia: Diagnostic challenges and intervention opportunities.
Sleep Med Rev 2001; 5(1):47–61.
4. Harvey AG. Insomnia: Symptom or diagnosis? Clin Psychol Rev 2001; 21(7):1037–1059.
5. Nierenberg AA, Keefe BR, Leslie VC, et al. Residual symptoms in depressed patients who respond
acutely to fluoxetine. J Clin Psychiatry 1999; 60(4):221–225.
6. Power JD, Perruccio AV, Badley EM. Pain as a mediator of sleep problems in arthritis and other chronic
conditions. Arthritis Rheum 2005; 53(6):911–919.
7. Vitiello MV, Rybarczyk B, Von Korff M, et al. Cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia improves
sleep and decreases pain in older adults with co-morbid insomnia and osteoarthritis. J Clin Sleep Med
2009; 5(4):355–362.
8. National Institutes of Health. NIH State-of-the-Science Conference Statement on Manifestations and
Management of Chronic Insomnia in Adults, Bethesda, MD, 2005:6–13.
9. National Sleep Foundation. Sleep in America: A National Survey of US Adults, 1991.
10. Roth T, Jaeger S, Jin R, et al. Sleep problems, comorbid mental disorders, and role functioning in the
national comorbidity survey replication. Biol Psychiatry 2006; 60(12):1364–1371.
11. Angst J, Vollrath M, Koch R, et al. The Zurich Study: VII. Insomnia: Symptoms, classification and
prevalence. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1989; 238:285–293.
12. Buysse DJ, Angst J, Gamma A, et al. Prevalence, course, and comorbidity of insomnia and depression
in young adults. Sleep 2008; 31(4):473–480.
13. Dodge R, Cline MG, Quan SF. The natural history of insomnia and its relationship to respiratory
symptoms. Arch Intern Med 1995; 155:1797–1800.
14. Foley DJ, Monjan A, Simonsick EM, et al. Incidence and remission of insomnia among elderly adults:
An epidemiologic study of 6,800 persons over three years. Sleep 1999; 22(Suppl 2):S366–S372.
15. Morin CM, Belanger L, LeBlanc M, et al. The natural history of insomnia: A population-based 3-year
longitudinal study. Arch Intern Med 2009; 169(5):447–453.
16. Buysse DJ, Finn L, Young T. Onset, remission, persistence, and consistency of insomnia symptoms
over 10 years: Longitudinal results from the Wisconsin Sleep Cohort Study (WSCS). Sleep 2004; 27
[abstract Suppl]:A268.
17. Partinen M, Hublin C. Epidemiology of sleep disorders. In: Kryger M, Roth T, Dement W, eds.
Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine. 4th ed. Philadelphia, PA: Elsevier Saunders, 2005:626–647.
18. Lichstein KL, Durrence HH, Riedel BW, et al. Epidemiology of Sleep: Age, Gender, and Ethnicity. New
Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., 2004.
19. Hohagen F, Kappler C, Schramm E, et al. Sleep onset insomnia, sleep maintaining insomnia and
insomnia with early morning awakening—temporal stability of subtypes in a longitudinal study on
general practice attenders. Sleep 1994; 17(6):551–554.
20. Lack LC, Mercer JD, Wright H. Circadian rhythms of early morning awakening insomniacs. J Sleep
Res 1996; 5(4):211–219.
21. American Sleep Disorders Association. International Classification of Sleep Disorders: Diagnostic and
Coding Manual. Rochester, MN: American Sleep Disorders Association, 1990.
22. American Sleep Disorders Association. International Classification of Sleep Disorders, revised: Diag-
nostic and Coding Manual. Rochester, MN: American Sleep Disorders Association, 1997.
23. American Academy of Sleep Medicine. The International Classification of Sleep Disorders, Second
Edition (ICSD-2): Diagnostic and Coding Manual. Second Edition 2005.
24. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-III-R).
3rd ed. Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Association, 1987.
25. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV).
4th ed. Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Association, 1994.
26. World Health Organization. Manual of the international classification of diseases, 9th revision, clinical
modification (ICD-9-CM). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office, 1980.
27. World Health Organization. International statistical classification of diseases and related health prob-
lems, 10th revision (ICD-10). Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization, 1992.
c11 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:26 Char Count=
112 BUYSSE
28. Reynolds CF, Kupfer DJ, Buysse DJ, et al. Subtyping DSM-III-R primary insomnia: A literature review
by the DSM-IV Work Group on sleep disorders. Am J Psychiatry 1991; 148(4):432–438.
29. Walters AS. The International Restless Legs Syndrome Study Group. Toward a better definition of the
restless legs syndrome. Mov Disord 1995; 10(5):634–642.
30. American Sleep Disorders Association Task Force. The Chicago criteria for measurements, definitions,
and severity ratings of sleep-related breathing disorders in adults. 1998; S1–S20..
31. Guilleminault C, Mignot E, Partinen M. Controversies in the diagnosis of narcolepsy. Sleep 1994;
17:S1–S6.
32. Bassetti C, Aldrich MS. Idiopathic hypersomnia—a series of 42 patients. Brain 1997; 120:1423–1435.
33. Edinger JD, Bonnet MH, Bootzin RR, et al. Derivation of research diagnostic criteria for insomnia:
Report of an American Academy of Sleep Medicine Work Group. Sleep 2004; 27(8):1567–1596.
34. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Kupfer DJ, et al. Clinical diagnoses in 216 insomnia patients using the
International Classification of Sleep Disorders (ICSD), DSM-IV and ICD-10 categories: A report from
the APA/NIMH DSM-IV field trial. Sleep 1994; 17(7):630–637.
35. Buysse DJ, Young T, Edinger JD, et al. Clinicans’ use of the International Classification of Sleep
Disorders: Results of a national survey. Sleep 2003; 26(1):48–51.
36. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Hauri PJ, et al. Diagnostic concordance for DSM-IV sleep disorders: A report
from the APA/NIMH DSM-IV field trial. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151(9):1351–1360.
37. Schramm E, Hohagen F, Grasshoff U, et al. Test–retest reliability and validity of a structured interview
for sleep disorders according to DSM-III-R(SIS-D). Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150(6):867–872.
38. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Kupfer DJ, et al. Effects of diagnosis on treatment recommendations in chronic
insomnia: A report from the APA/NIMH DSM-IV field trial. Sleep 1997; 20(7):542–552.
39. Edinger JD, Fins AI, Goeke JM, et al. The empirical identification of insomnia subtypes: A cluster
analytic approach. Sleep 1996; 19(5):398–411.
40. Edinger JD, Wyatt JK, Olsen MK, et al. Comparative validity of the DSM-IV-TR and ICSD-2
insomnia nosologies: How many ways should we slice the insomnia pie? Sleep 2009; 32[Abstract
Supplement]:A263.
41. Edinger JD, Wyatt JK, Olsen MK, et al. Reliability and validity of the Duke structured interview for
sleep disorders for insomnia screening. Sleep 2009; 32[Abstract Supplement]:A265.
42. Gillin JC, Duncan W, Pettigrew KD, et al. Successful separation of depressed, normal, and insomniac
subjects by EEG sleep data. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1979; 36:85–90.
43. Reynolds CF, Taska LS, Sewitch DE, et al. Persistent psychophysiologic insomnia: Preliminary Research
Diagnostic Criteria and EEG sleep data. Am J Psychiatry 1984; 141(6):804–805.
44. Dorsey CM, Bootzin RR. Subjective and psychophysiologic insomnia: An examination of sleep ten-
dency and personality. Biol Psychiatry 1997; 41:209–216.
45. Lichstein KL, Wilson NM, Noe SL, et al. Daytime sleepiness in insomnia: Behavioral, biological and
subjective indices. Sleep 1994; 17(8):693–702.
46. Perlis ML, Smith MT, Andrews PJ, et al. Beta/gamma EEG activity in patients with primary and
secondary insomnia and good sleeper controls. Sleep 2001; 24(1):110–117.
47. Krystal AD, Edinger JD, Wohlgemuth WK, et al. NREM sleep EEG frequency spectral correlates of
sleep complaints in primary insomnia subtypes. Sleep 2002; 25(6):630–640.
48. Bonnet MH, Arand DL. 24-Hour metabolic rate in insomniacs and matched normal sleepers. Sleep
1995; 18(7):581–588.
49. Bonnet MH, Arand DL. Insomnia, metabolic rate and sleep restoration. J Intern Med 2003; 254(1):
23–31.
50. Bonnet MH, Arand DL. Physiological activation in patients with Sleep State Misperception. Psychosom
Med 1997; 59(5):533–540.
51. Giles DE, Roffwarg HP, Rush AJ, et al. Age-adjusted threshold values for reduced REM latency in
unipolar depression using ROC analysis. Biol Psychiatry 1990; 27:841–853.
52. Somoza E, Mossman D. Optimizing REM latency as a diagnostic test for depression using receiver
operating characteristic analysis and information theory. Biol Psychiatry 1990; 27:990–1006.
53. Reite M, Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, et al. The use of polysomnography in the evaluation of insomnia.
Sleep 1995; 18(1):58–70.
54. Vgontzas AN, Kales A, Bixler EO, et al. Usefulness of polysomnographic studies in the differential
diagnosis of insomnia. Int J Neurosci 1995; 82:47–60.
55. Hauri P. A cluster analysis of insomnia. Sleep 1983; 6:326–338.
56. Nowell PD, Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, et al. Clinical factors contributing to the differential diagnosis of
primary insomnia and insomnia related to mental disorders. Am J Psychiatry 1997; 154(10):1412–1416.
57. Carney CE, Edinger JD, Wyatt JK, et al. Should DSM-IV-TR primary insomnia be divided into specific
subtypes? Sleep 2009; 32[abstract Suppl]:A264.
IHBK059-12 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:27 Char Count=
Jason C. Ong
Department of Behavioral Sciences, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, Illinois, U.S.A.
Psychophysiological Insomnia
Psychophysiological insomnia is a primary insomnia subtype characterized by heightened
sleep-related arousal or the presence of associations that are incompatible with sleep. According
to the ICSD-2, the prevalence of psychophysiological insomnia is 12% to 15% among individuals
presenting to a sleep center, regardless of chief complaint. Evidence for psychophysiological
insomnia include any one of the following: (i) excessive focus on and heightened anxiety about
sleep during the day (e.g., avoiding evening events for fear that such events might interfere with
sleep, worrying about sleep during the day); (ii) relative ease of falling asleep during relatively
monotonous activities (e.g., watching TV, reading, etc.) when not intending to sleep; (iii) ability
to sleep better away from home than at home; (iv) intrusive thoughts in bed or inability to calm
the mind; (v) heightened somatic tension in bed reflected by a perceived inability to relax the
body. Sleep-related arousal that is prominent during the intended sleep period may be cognitive
IHBK059-12 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:27 Char Count=
(e.g., racing thoughts or difficulty calming the mind when trying to sleep), emotional (anxiety
or agitation while in bed or anticipatory anxiety prior to bedtime), and/or physiological (e.g.,
muscle tension). Sleep-related arousal can also be evidenced by excessive preoccupation with
sleep during the day.
Paradoxical Insomnia
Paradoxical insomnia (formerly “sleep state misperception” or “subjective insomnia”) is a sub-
type of primary insomnia characterized by a profound mismatch between subjective and objec-
tive sleep estimates. Evidence for paradoxical insomnia include any one of the following: (i)
reporting a chronic pattern of little or no sleep most nights with rare nights during which rel-
atively normal amounts of sleep are obtained; (ii) sleep log data during one or more weeks of
monitoring show an average sleep time well below published age-adjusted normative values,
often with no sleep at all for several nights per week. Typically there is an absence of daytime
naps following such nights; (iii) a marked mismatch between objective findings from PSG or
actigraphy and subjective sleep estimates. Patients with paradoxical insomnia report levels of
daytime impairments that are similar to what is reported by other primary insomnia subtypes
but much less severe than would be expected given the extreme level of sleep deprivation
reported.
While there are no established criteria for a discrepancy threshold between subjective
perception and objective findings, various definitions have been used in research. Krystal et
al. defined paradoxical insomnia in patients with insomnia complaints based on PSG-defined
total sleep time (TST) and sleep efficiency (SE) (9,10). The criteria were age dependent, taking
into account shorter sleep durations in older adults. PSG sleep was deemed “normal” if TST
was ≥6.5 hours. In those with TST between 6 and 6.5 hours, sleep was defined as “normal”
based on their age. For those <60 years old, SE% had to be >85% and those ≥60, SE% had
to be >80%. Individual differences in time estimates and the perception of sleep states (11)
are at the core of a debate on the validity of paradoxical insomnia as a distinct diagnostic
category. Those supporting the validity of the paradoxical insomnia diagnosis (12) claim that
the underestimation of sleep time is unique to a subset of individuals with primary insomnia.
Others claim that underestimation of sleep time is a generic characteristic of insomnia with
some exhibiting larger discrepancy than others (13). According to the ICSD-2, the prevalence of
paradoxical insomnia is less than 5% among individuals presenting to sleep centers, regardless
of chief complaint. A survey of 11 sleep centers in the United States found that 9% of patients
with insomnia received a diagnosis of subjective (now paradoxical) insomnia (6).
Idiopathic Insomnia
Idiopathic insomnia is a subtype of primary insomnia with an onset during infancy or child-
hood, no identifiable precipitating event, and a persistent course with no periods of sustained
remission (8). The RDC specifies an onset prior to age 10 (2). According to the ICSD-2 the preva-
lence of idiopathic insomnia is less than 10% among individuals presenting to sleep centers,
regardless of chief complaint. The diagnosis of idiopathic insomnia requires assessment of the
age of onset of the first insomnia episode and its course. A typical statement volunteered by
individuals with idiopathic insomnia is “I have always had problems sleeping.”
The possibility that some cases of idiopathic insomnia might be related to a delayed
sleep phase should be considered. Children with a delayed sleep phase might be predisposed to
developing sleep onset difficulty because they are expected to sleep at times when their circadian
clocks are generating strong activating signals. Early and repeated associations of efforts to
sleep with heightened arousal may fuel a pattern of insomnia that persists into adulthood. As
adults, these individuals might continue to express a preference for delayed sleep schedules but
experience sleep difficulties even when allowed to choose their preferred sleep schedule, which
precludes a diagnosis of delayed sleep phase syndrome. If their insomnia is primary, they will
meet criteria for idiopathic insomnia. This suggests that a delayed sleep phase might constitute
a predisposition for idiopathic insomnia. Though this possibility has not been directly tested, it
is supported by evidence that children with ADHD and chronic sleep onset insomnia (> 1 year)
have a delayed melatonin rhythm (14).
IHBK059-12 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:27 Char Count=
DIAGNOSTIC ASSESSMENT
Diagnostic assessment of primary insomnia is based primarily on a clinical interview. Sleep
diaries and self-report questionnaires are often used to supplement the interview and are
administered either before the initial interview to facilitate the intake evaluation or after
the initial interview to clarify diagnostic issues that arise during the initial assessment (see
chapter 11). Routine polysomnography is not indicated for the initial diagnosis of primary
insomnia. Polysomnography is indicated when sleep disordered breathing or periodic limb
movement disorder is suspected, when the diagnosis is uncertain, and when violent injurious
behavior during sleep is present (15).
Complaint of difficulty
Prolonged
initiating or
sleep
maintaining sleep
onset latency
Is it a clinical Frequent or
problem? prolonged
awakenings
after
YES NO sleep onset
Phenotypes
(nocturnal
symptoms)
Insomnia Insomnia
disorder symptoms
Early morning
awakenings
Does it
occur exclusively
in the context of another
disorder or substance
use? Nonrestorative
sleep
NO YES
Primary Comorbid
insomnia insomnia
Are there
features related
to presumed
etiology?
Figure 1 A schematic diagram for the diagnostic assessment of primary insomnia and its subtypes. This algo-
rithm provides a general guideline for differential diagnosis of primary insomnia.
insomnia and depressive disorders share symptoms, such as poor sleep, poor concentration,
and low energy.
One practical way to gather these essential data is to ask the patient to describe in detail
specific sleep-related behaviors in the chronological order they occur throughout the evening
and night, starting with the presleep routines and progressing through the sleep period to the
patient’s morning rise time. Table 1 summarizes the type of information that can be obtained
relative to this time line and how it pertains to case conceptualization, as discussed below.
For better understanding of the patient’s insomnia experience, the clinician should pay atten-
tion to and, if necessary, probe the patient’s emotional reactions and behavioral responses
in coping with insomnia. Knowledge of the patient’s routine evening activities, particularly
the hour or two before bedtime, allows the clinician to determine if activities at bedtime are
likely to be interfering with sleep. This information is relevant to the diagnosis of inadequate
sleep hygiene and it identifies specific sleep-interfering activities that need to be addressed in
treatment.
Details about the consumption of substances, such as alcohol, nicotine, and caffeine should
include the amount and timing of such consumption and their potential for disrupting sleep.
The same holds true for prescription and over the counter medications and herbal preparations
taken to promote sleep. Other behaviors that can interfere with sleep and should therefore be
assessed include, clock watching (17), a sedentary lifestyle, and absence of sufficient stimulation
during the day. The latter is particularly important to assess in institutional older adults.
of low energy that may be influenced by psychological and physiological factors. Objectively
measured daytime sleepiness using the Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT) indicates that
following habitual sleep, individuals with primary insomnia are not abnormally sleepy, with
sleepiness scores comparable to that of good sleepers (20–22). Interestingly, when individuals
with primary insomnia are sleep deprived to 80% of their habitual sleep time, objectively
determined daytime sleepiness, as measured with the MSLT is increased (23).
Research suggests that the severity of reported daytime symptoms is impacted by the sub-
jective perception of poor or insufficient sleep. Semler and Harvey manipulated the perception
of sleep by providing individuals with primary insomnia “feedback” about the results of the
actigraphic recording of their sleep in a random counter balanced fashion. Regardless of the
actual observed sleep, participants reported more sleepiness during the day when they were
told that they slept poorly on the previous night (24).
CASE CONCEPTUALIZATION
Beyond establishing a diagnosis of primary insomnia and its subtypes, the goal of the clinical
assessment is reaching a case conceptualization and formulating a treatment plan. To that end the
clinician considers etiological models and theories of insomnia while listening and inquiring for
further information from the patient. We therefore present a prevailing model of insomnia, the
diathesis-stress model, and the arousal/activation theories and discuss how to assess features
relevant to these theories. We also discuss the relevance and assessment of circadian tendency
and treatment history in primary insomnia.
Circadian Contributions
Circadian factors are relevant to the presentation of primary insomnia both in terms of differen-
tial diagnosis and case conceptualization. For example, it has been suggested that individuals
who present with sleep onset difficulties and no sleep maintenance problems have a delayed
sleep phase. Their temperature rhythm markers were approximately 2.5 hours later than the
respective phases of the good sleepers and their usual bedtimes fell within the “wake main-
tenance zone” (phases of the body’s temperature rhythm during which sleep onset is seldom
selected by free running subjects) of their temperature rhythm (40). However, the generalizabil-
ity of this finding is not clear as this study did not report whether the participants with sleep
onset difficulty met criteria for primary insomnia disorder or delayed sleep phase syndrome. The
salient feature differentiating between insomnia and circadian rhythm sleep disorder, delayed
type, is that in the latter case sleep initiation and maintenance are normal when sleep occurs
during the patient’s preferred time (8). It has also been suggested that individuals with pri-
mary insomnia presenting with early morning awakenings have a two to four hours advanced
melatonin rhythm phases (41), and that this phase advance causes early arousal from sleep in
these patients. The salient feature differentiating between insomnia and circadian rhythm sleep
disorder, advanced type, is that in the latter case the patient typically experiences early evening
sleepiness that is usually absent in primary insomnia (8).
In a clinical setting it is most practical to assess the patient’s circadian tendencies either
during the interview or by using validated questionnaires. In the absence of a full syndrome of
delayed sleep phase, some patients with primary insomnia experience their best sleep during
the second half of the night and report prolonged time to feel fully awake after rise time.
These two symptoms suggest a natural evening chronotype that needs to be considered when
recommending bedtime and rise time. Similarly, in the absence of a full syndrome of advanced
sleep phase, some patients with primary insomnia present with an early bedtime, involuntary
evening “naps,” and very early wake-up times. Since comorbid psychiatric disorders that are
associated with early morning awakening have been ruled out during the diagnostic portion
of the assessment, these symptoms suggest a morning chronotype. Other circadian rhythm
considerations concern irregular sleep schedules. The strength of the signal for optimal timing
of sleep delivered by the suprachiasmatic nucleus is weakened when sleep–wake schedules
change dramatically or frequently, as is the case in professions that require multiple shifts in
time zones (e.g., airline industry and other jobs that require frequent changes in time zones)
and in professions that involve varying shifts. Therefore, information about the patient’s work
schedule and travel patterns should be obtained.
other specific treatment approaches. To the best of our knowledge no treatment outcome studies
tested the efficacy of CBT or pharmacological treatments for these two insomnia phenotypes.
The distribution of the four phenotypes has been studied in epidemiological studies and
surveys of the general population and clinic samples (46,47), but due to variations in nosology
and study criteria specific findings for primary insomnia are unclear. Inference from these
studies to primary insomnia cannot be made because the samples that were studied included
individuals that did not meet criteria for an insomnia disorder and/or those whose insomnia
disorder might be related to other medical or psychiatric conditions. The extant literature
suggests that the pure early morning awakening phenotype is the least common among people
with a probable insomnia disorder (4) and that the most common presentation in clinic samples
(45,48) and in population surveys consists of more than one of the four nocturnal symptoms
(4). One could infer from these studies that mixed symptoms are likely to be the most common
presentation among individuals with primary insomnia. However, to the best of our knowledge,
estimates of the symptom distribution among those who meet criteria for primary insomnia are
lacking.
Nonrestorative sleep is the least studied nocturnal symptom of insomnia disorder. This
nocturnal symptom is most commonly defined by reports of feeling unrefreshed upon awaken-
ing. Unlike sleep maintenance phenotype, whose prevalence increases with age, nonrestorative
sleep is not associated with increasing age (6); it is more common among people younger than
55 years (18). Nonrestorative sleep has been studied mostly in individuals with medical dis-
orders associated with fatigue, such as fibromyalgia, chronic fatigue, temporomandibular joint
disorder, and irritable bowel syndrome (49) and in patients with other sleep disorders, such as
sleep apnea (8). There is a fourfold increase in the prevalence of nonrestorative sleep among
those with mood disorders (18). We were unable to find any study of nonrestorative sleep among
individuals with primary insomnia and it is not clear if nonrestorative sleep exists in a pure
form (i.e., without other nocturnal symptom) among individuals with primary insomnia (50).
expectations and encourage the patient to try implementing the full set of stimulus control
instruction consistently for at least two weeks.
POLYSOMNOGRAPHIC FINDINGS
Comparisons of PSG-defined sleep parameters in primary insomnia relative to controls gen-
erally report minimal to modest differences. Average differences are less than 38 minutes for
TST and less than 42 minutes for total wake time, with many studies detecting no significant
difference (42). Individual differences in the perception of sleep states (11) might explain the
inconsistency in the results from these, usually small, studies. In general, sleep time misper-
ceptions are larger when subjective estimation of time awake after sleep onset is longer (51).
Overestimation of time awake is associated with a complaint of nonrestorative sleep and the
conviction that one’s sleep difficulty is organically based (52). The severity of distress and day-
time complaints of people with insomnia is not fully explained by the severity of the disruption
in sleep.
Macro architecture of sleep in individuals with primary insomnia is only slightly different
from that of good sleepers, with more time spent in stage 1 sleep, less time spent in stages 3 and
4, and more stage shifts (53). The micro architecture of sleep, as determined by spectral analyses
of the EEG, reveals increases in fast frequency EEG (beta) among people with primary insomnia
relative to controls (34). Krystal et al. found that individuals with paradoxical insomnia also have
lower delta and greater alpha and sigma EEG activity during non-REM sleep than noninsomnia
control subjects and that less delta EEG activity predicted greater deviation between subjective
and PSG estimates of sleep time (2). These finding might suggest that the origins of the sleep
complaints in patients with paradoxical insomnia are related to high cortical arousal during
sleep (10,54).
Objectively defined sleep onset and sleep discontinuity parameters are not in high agree-
ment with subjective recollections of sleep experiences provided by the patient. Only 12% of
patients with insomnia (less than half of which were likely to have experienced primary insom-
nia) provide estimates of their TST that are congruent (off by no more that 15 minutes) relative
to their PSG-defined TST (55). Approximately 44% had estimates that were lower than PSG TST
by at least 60 minutes (55). Overestimation of sleep difficulties is common in all types of insom-
nia. It is more pronounced in people with paradoxical insomnia. For example, Edinger and Fins
found that patients diagnosed with sleep state misperception (paradoxical insomnia) had actual
(PSG defined) sleep times that were at least twice as large as their subjective time estimates,
whereas those with psychophysiological insomnia or inadequate sleep hygiene estimated their
sleep times more accurately, at a median of 88% of the objective (PSG) estimates (52).
most affected (early middle, and/or late) as well as the pharmacokinetics of sleep inducing
medications and over the counter preparations that the patient is using at the time of the
assessment. The assessment also provides useful information when considering CBT-I. For
example, habitual sleep times guide the initial time in bed prescription associated with sleep
restriction. Evaluation of chronotypes can also help the insomnia therapist chose the optimal
time in bed window. For example, the optimal bedtime for a patient who describes himself as
a night owl will be later than what would be recommended for a patient who describes herself
as a morning person. The information gathered in the assessment can aid in choosing the order
and relative emphasis of the various CBT-I treatment components, allowing the therapist to
work through compliance issues thus improving adherence with treatment recommendations.
CONCLUSION
The goal of this chapter was to provide an overview of the assessment process relevant to
the diagnosis, case conceptualization, and treatment of the patient with primary insomnia.
Evaluation of nonprimary forms of insomnia disorder necessitates additional assessments,
discussed elsewhere in this book. However, the diathesis-stress model of insomnia applies to
insomnia disorders other than primary insomnia, thus rendering much of the content of this
chapter relevant to the assessment of other insomnia disorders.
REFERENCES
1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed.
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association, 1994.
2. Edinger JD, Bonnet MH, Bootzin RR, et al. Derivation of research diagnostic criteria for insomnia:
report of an American Academy of Sleep Medicine Work Group. Sleep 2004; 27(8):1567–1596.
3. Ohayon MM. Prevalence of DSM-IV diagnostic criteria of insomnia: distinguishing insomnia related
to mental disorders from sleep disorders. J Psychiatr Res 1997; 31(3):333–346.
4. Morin CM, LeBlanc M, Daley M, et al. Epidemiology of insomnia: prevalence, self-help treatments,
consultations, and determinants of help-seeking behaviors. Sleep Med 2006; 7(2):123–130.
5. Ohayon MM, Caulet M, Priest RG, et al. DSM-IV and ICSD-90 insomnia symptoms and sleep dissat-
isfaction. Br J Psychiatry 1997; 171:382–388.
6. Coleman RM, Roffwarg HP, Kennedy SJ, et al. Sleep-wake disorders based on a polysomnographic
diagnosis. A national cooperative study. JAMA 1982; 247(7):997–1003.
7. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF III, Kupfer DJ, et al. Effects of diagnosis on treatment recommendations in
chronic insomnia–a report from the APA/NIMH DSM-IV field trial. Sleep 1997; 20(7):542–552.
8. American Academy of Sleep Medicine. International Classification of Sleep Disorders. 2nd ed. Diag-
nostic and Coding Manual. Westchester, IL: American Academy of Sleep Medicine, 2005.
9. Edinger JD, Fins AI, Glenn DM, et al. Insomnia and the eye of the beholder: are there clinical markers
of objective sleep disturbances among adults with and without insomnia complaints? J Consult Clin
Psychol 2000; 68(4):586–593.
10. Krystal AD, Edinger JD, Wohlgemuth WK, et al. NREM sleep EEG frequency spectral correlates of
sleep complaints in primary insomnia subtypes. Sleep 2002; 25(6):630–640.
11. Means MK, Edinger JD, Glenn DM, et al. Accuracy of sleep perceptions among insomnia sufferers
and normal sleepers. Sleep Med 2003; 4(4):285–296.
12. Edinger JD, Krystal AD. Subtyping primary insomnia: is sleep state misperception a distinct clinical
entity? Sleep Med Rev 2003; 7(3):203–214.
13. Trinder J. Subjective insomnia without objective findings: a pseudo diagnostic classification? Psychol
Bull 1988; 103(1):87–94.
14. Bastien CH, Vallieres A, Morin CM. Precipitating factors of insomnia. Behav Sleep Med 2004; 2(1):
50–62.
15. Littner M, Hirshkowitz M, Kramer M, et al. Practice parameters for using polysomnography to
evaluate insomnia: an update. Sleep 2003; 26(6):754–760.
16. Buysse DJ, Ancoli-Israel S, Edinger JD, et al. Recommendations for a standard research assessment of
insomnia. Sleep 2006; 29(9):1155–1173.
17. Tang NK, Anne Schmidt D, Harvey AG. Sleeping with the enemy: clock monitoring in the maintenance
of insomnia. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 2007; 38(1):40–55.
18. Ohayon MM. Prevalence and correlates of nonrestorative sleep complaints. Arch Intern Med 2005;
165(1):35–41.
IHBK059-12 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:27 Char Count=
19. Roth T, Jaeger S, Jin R, et al. Sleep problems, comorbid mental disorders, and role functioning in the
national comorbidity survey replication. Biol Psychiatry 2006; 60(12):1364–1371.
20. Stepanski E, Zorick F, Roehrs T, et al. Daytime alertness in patients with chronic insomnia compared
with asymptomatic control subjects. Sleep 1988; 11(1):54–60.
21. Bonnet MH, Arand DL. 24-Hour metabolic rate in insomniacs and matched normal sleepers. Sleep
1995; 18(7):581–588.
22. Bonnet MH, Arand DL. Activity, arousal, and the MSLT in patients with insomnia. Sleep 2000;
23(2):205–212.
23. Bonnet MH, Arand DL. The consequences of a week of insomnia. II: patients with insomnia. Sleep
1998; 21(4):359–368.
24. Semler CN, Harvey AG. Misperception of sleep can adversely affect daytime functioning in insomnia.
Behav Res Ther 2005; 43(7):843–856.
25. Spielman AJ, Caruso LS, Glovinsky PB. A behavioral perspective on insomnia treatment. Psychiatr
Clin North Am 1987; 10(4):541–553.
26. Pavlova M, Berg O, Gleason R, et al. Self-reported hyperarousal traits among insomnia patients. J
Psychosom Res 2001; 51(2):435–441.
27. Van Egeren L, Haynes SN, Franzen M, et al. Presleep cognitions and attributions in sleep-onset
insomnia. J Behav Med 1983; 6(2):217–232.
28. Espie CA. Insomnia: conceptual issues in the development, persistence, and treatment of sleep disorder
in adults. Annu Rev Psychol 2002; 53:215–243.
29. Wicklow A, Espie CA. Intrusive thoughts and their relationship to actigraphic measurement of sleep:
towards a cognitive model of insomnia. Behav Res Ther 2000; 38(7):679–693.
30. Fichten CS, Creti L, Amsel R, et al. Poor sleepers who do not complain of insomnia: myths and realities
about psychological and lifestyle characteristics of older good and poor sleepers. J Behav Med 1995;
18(2):189–223.
31. Borkovec TD, Lane TW, VanOot PH. Phenomenology of sleep among insomniacs and good sleepers:
wakefulness experience when cortically asleep. J Abnorm Psychol 1981; 90(6):607–609.
32. Harvey AG. A cognitive model of insomnia. Behav Res Ther 2002; 40(8):869–893.
33. Ong JC, Shapiro SL, Manber R. Combining mindfulness meditation with cognitive-behavior therapy
for insomnia: a treatment-development study. Behav Ther 2008; 39(2):171–182.
34. Perlis ML, Smith MT, Andrews PJ, et al. Beta/Gamma EEG activity in patients with primary and
secondary insomnia and good sleeper controls. Sleep 2001; 24(1):110–117.
35. Nofzinger EA, Nissen C, Germain A, et al. Regional cerebral metabolic correlates of WASO during
NREM sleep in insomnia. J Clin Sleep Med 2006; 2(3):316–322.
36. Bonnet MH, Arand DL. Heart rate variability in insomniacs and matched normal sleepers. Psychosom
Med 1998; 60(5):610–615.
37. Yang CM, LO HS. ERP evidence of enhanced excitatory and reduced inhibitory processes of auditory
stimuli during sleep in patients with primary insomnia. Sleep 2007; 30(5):585–592.
38. Bonnet MH, Arand DL. The consequences of a week of insomnia. Sleep 1996; 19(6):453–461.
39. Ong JC, Shapiro SL, Manber R. Mindfulness meditation and CBT for insomnia: a naturalistic 12-month
follow-up. Explore: The Journal of Science and Healing 2009; 5:30–36.
40. Morris M, Lack L, Dawson D. Sleep-onset insomniacs have delayed temperature rhythms. Sleep 1990;
13(1):1–14.
41. Lack LC, Mercer JD, Wright H. Circadian rhythms of early morning awakening insomniacs. J Sleep
Res 1996; 5(4):211–219.
42. Edinger JD, Glenn DM, Bastian LA, et al. Sleep in the laboratory and sleep at home II: comparisons of
middle-aged insomnia sufferers and normal sleepers. Sleep 2001; 24(7):761–770.
43. Morin C, Colecchi C, Stone J, et al. Behavioral and pharmacological therapies for late-life insomnia: a
randomized controlled trial. JAMA 1999; 281:991–999.
44. Sivertsen B, Omvik S, Pallesen S, et al. Cognitive behavioral therapy vs. zopiclone for treatment of
chronic primary insomnia in older adults: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA 2006; 295(24):2851–
2858.
45. Jacobs GD, Pace-Schott EF, Stickgold R, et al. Cognitive behavior therapy and pharmacotherapy for
insomnia: a randomized controlled trial and direct comparison. Arch Intern Med 2004; 164(17):1888–
1896.
46. Karacan I, Thornby JI, Anch M, et al. Prevalence of sleep disturbance in a primarily urban Florida
County. Soc Sci Med 1976; 10(5):239–244.
47. Ohayon MM, Smirne S. Prevalence and consequences of insomnia disorders in the general population
of Italy. Sleep Med 2002; 3(2):115–120.
48. Hryshko-Mullen AS, Broeckl LS, Haddock CK, et al. Behavioral treatment of insomnia: the Wilford
Hall Insomnia Program. Mil Med 2000; 165(3):200–207.
IHBK059-12 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:27 Char Count=
49. Moldofsky H. Nonrestorative sleep, musculoskeletal pain, fatigue, and psychological distress in
chronic fatigue syndrome, fibromyalgia, irritable bowel syndrome, temporal mandibular joint dys-
function disorders. In: Yehuda S, ed. Chronic Fatigue Syndrome. New York: Plenum Press, 1997:95–117.
50. Stone KC, Taylor DJ, McCrae CS, et al. Nonrestorative sleep. Sleep Med Rev 2008; 12(4): 275–288.
51. Vanable PA, Aikens JE, Tadimeti L, et al. Sleep latency and duration estimates among sleep disor-
der patients: variability as a function of sleep disorder diagnosis, sleep history, and psychological
characteristics. Sleep 2000; 23(1):71–79.
52. Edinger JD, Fins AI. The distribution and clinical significance of sleep time misperceptions among
insomniacs. Sleep 1995; 18(4):232–239.
53. Morin CM, Espie CA. Insomnia: A Clinical Guide to Assessment and Treatment. New York: Springer,
2004.
54. Bonnet MH, Arand DL. Physiological activation in patients with Sleep State Misperception. Psychosom
Med 1997; 59(5):533–540.
55. Carskadon MA, Dement WC, Mitler MM, et al. Self-reports versus sleep laboratory findings in 122
drug-free subjects with complaints of chronic insomnia. Am J Psychiatry 1976; 133(12):1382–1388.
56. Vincent N, Lionberg C. Treatment preference and patient satisfaction in chronic insomnia. Sleep 2001;
24(4):411–417.
c13 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:32 Char Count=
INTRODUCTION
Insomnia and psychiatric disorders frequently occur together. Epidemiological studies have
demonstrated that between approximately 25% and 50% of individuals with insomnia in the
general population meet criteria for insomnia comorbid with a psychiatric disorder, including
most commonly the psychiatric comorbidities of depressive disorders, anxiety disorders, and
substance use disorders (1–3). Sleep difficulties are a common symptom of many psychiatric
disorders and in some cases treatment of the psychiatric disorder may lead to improvement or
resolution of the sleep difficulties. However, insomnia commonly persists despite treatment of
the psychiatric illness (e.g., Ref. 4) and can cause significant distress to the patient as well as an
adverse impact on the course of the psychiatric disorder.
Mounting evidence suggests a dynamic and potentially bidirectional relationship between
insomnia and psychiatric disorders. For example, longitudinal research has demonstrated that
individuals with insomnia but without a comorbid psychiatric illness at baseline are at a signifi-
cantly higher risk for developing future depressive, anxiety, and substance abuse or dependence
disorders (1,5–8). Not only is insomnia a risk factor for the onset of psychiatric disorders, but it
has also been shown that insomnia can increase the risk for relapse and exacerbation of psychi-
atric symptoms. For example, individuals with residual insomnia symptoms after treatment for
depression are at a higher risk of relapse of depression than those without residual insomnia
(9). Conversely, treatment of insomnia may improve the course of a depressive episode; recent
research has demonstrated how individuals with depression and insomnia who receive both
general treatment for depression as well as insomnia-specific treatment (e.g., fluoxetine with
eszopiclone or escitalopram with cognitive-behavioral therapy for insomnia) have significantly
greater improvements in depressive symptoms than those who only receive general treatment
for depression (10,11).
Given this dynamic relationship, it is essential to assess both insomnia and psychiatric
symptomatology in patients with either an insomnia or psychiatric complaint so that appropri-
ate treatment planning and implementation can occur. The focus of the current chapter is to pro-
vide recommendations regarding the assessment of insomnia comorbid with psychiatric issues.
The first section of this chapter will discuss the assessment of psychiatric disorders commonly
occurring with insomnia (i.e., depressive disorders, bipolar disorders, generalized anxiety dis-
order, panic disorder, posttraumatic stress disorder, and schizophrenia), including a description,
associated sleep complaints, characteristic abnormalities detected in sleep electroencephalogra-
phy (EEG), and possible screening measures for each psychiatric disorder. The second section
will review the assessment of important factors commonly contributing to insomnia in patients
with psychiatric disorders, including behavioral factors, the effects of psychotropic medications,
and other sleep disorders. With the aim of promoting more standardized insomnia assessment
in clinical practice and facilitating a connection between clinical and research settings, the stan-
dardized insomnia assessment recommendations for research developed by Buysse et al. (12)
will be used as a guide whenever possible. Of note, assessment of insomnia related to substance
issues is not covered in this chapter as this particular comorbidity is covered in chapter 16.
schizophrenia. Despite their high prevalence, psychiatric disorders often remain undiagnosed,
untreated or undertreated in patients who present with insomnia (13). Factors that can contribute
to the lack of diagnosis and treatment include: (a) attribution of psychiatric symptomatology to
sleep problems (e.g., “If I could only sleep, then I would be fine.”); (b) lack of physician training
in psychiatric assessment; (c) insufficient time during clinic visits to assess psychiatric symp-
toms in busy clinical practices; and (d) limited reimbursement for assessment and treatment of
psychiatric disorders.
Descriptions of the psychiatric disorders commonly occurring with insomnia are provided
below. In addition, associated sleep complaints, characteristic abnormalities in sleep EEG, and
suggested screening questions and measures to potentially identify patients at risk for specific
psychiatric disorders are described below and summarized in Table 1. It should be noted
that the basic sleep patterns outlined below, including both sleep complaints and objective
abnormalities in sleep EGG, should not be used as definitive diagnostic markers. The sleep
patterns described are not necessarily present in all individuals with a particular psychiatric
Table 1 Basic Sleep Patterns, Screening Questions, and Screening Questionnaires for Common Psychiatric
Disorders Comorbid with Insomnia
Psychiatric Screening
comorbidity Basic sleep patterns Screening question(s) questionnaires
Depressive disorders • Sleep continuity • “Have you been feeling • Beck Depression
disturbances depressed or down?” Inventory II (17)
• REM sleep abnormalities • “Have you lost interest • Inventory of
• SWS deficits in things you usually Depressive
enjoy?” Symptomatology
Self-Report (18)
• Geriatric Depression
Scale (21)
Bipolar disorders • Sleep continuity • “Have you felt so high, • Mood Disorders
disturbances ‘hyper’, or irritable that Questionnaire (23)
• REM sleep abnormalities people told you that
• SWS deficits you were not your
normal self?”
Generalized anxiety • Sleep continuity • “Have you been • State-Trait Anxiety
disorder disturbances anxious or worried?” Inventory, trait
version (29)
• Penn State Worry
Questionnaire (30)
Panic disorder • Sleep continuity • “Have you ever had a • Brief Panic Disorder
disturbances panic attack when you Screen (36)
• Nocturnal panic attacks suddenly felt anxious
with related fears of going or experienced a lot of
to sleep physical symptoms?”
Posttraumatic stress • Sleep continuity • “Have you experienced • Posttraumatic Stress
disorder disturbances any traumas in your Diagnostic Scale (42)
• REM sleep abnormalities life?”
• Nightmares/flashbacks
with related fears of going
to sleep
Schizophrenia • Sleep continuity • If psychotic symptoms • If psychotic symptoms
disturbances are observed and are observed and
• NREM sleep spindle there is no diagnosis, there is no diagnosis,
deficits in the vertex region an immediate referral an immediate referral
for psychiatric for psychiatric
assessment is assessment is
recommended recommended
Abbreviations: REM, rapid eye movement; SWS, slow wave sleep; NREM, nonrapid eye movement.
c13 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:32 Char Count=
diagnosis and similar sleep abnormalities may be seen in individuals without those disorders,
including healthy, normal individuals. Likewise, the screening questions and measures outlined
below should not be used to make specific diagnoses but instead to identify individuals at greater
risk for particular psychiatric disorder. Further psychiatric assessment is recommended for any
case where clinical suspicion for possible psychiatric disorder is increased based on clinical or
objective sleep abnormalities and/or positive screening questionnaires.
When treating an individual with insomnia and a known psychiatric disorder, the screen-
ing measures outlined below can also be used to track psychiatric symptom severity along with
insomnia symptom severity so that intervention can be better guided. For example, if increases
and decreases in psychiatric and insomnia symptom severity seem to correspond, treatment
could focus more prominently on the psychiatric disorder. Other the other hand, if insomnia
symptom severity remains high in the context of low psychiatric symptom severity, treatment
could center more on insomnia.
Depressive Disorders
Major depressive disorder is characterized by the presence of at least one major depressive
episode (14). The starting criterion for a major depressive episode is the presence of depressed
mood or anhedonia for two weeks or longer. Other symptoms include: (a) significant weight
loss or weight gain, (b) insomnia or hypersomnia, (c) psychomotor agitation or retardation, (d)
fatigue or loss of energy, (e) feelings of worthlessness or guilt, (f) decreased ability to concentrate,
and (g) recurrent thoughts of death and suicide. Five or more symptoms must be present
with at least one of these symptoms being depressed mood or anhedonia. Major depressive
episodes also cause clinically significant distress or impaired social or occupational functioning.
Dysthymic disorder is diagnosed in individuals with chronic depressive symptoms for at least
two years who have significant impairment in functioning, but do not meet full criteria for a
major depressive episode.
to complete. Each item is rated on a 4-point scale ranging from 0 to 3 and summed into a
single score ranging from 0 to 63. Severity of depression based on scores is as follows: 0–13
minimal, 14–19 mild, 20–28 moderate, and 29–63 severe. A cut-off score of 18 or higher yielded
a sensitivity of 94% and a specificity of 92% when predicting a diagnosis of major depressive
disorder as diagnosed by the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ, 19) in a sample of primary
care patients (20).
The IDS-SR is a 30-item self-report scale that includes all DSM-IV symptom domains for
a major depressive episode. This measure takes approximately 15 to 20 minutes to complete.
Each item is rated on a 4-point scale ranging from 0 to 3 and summed into a single score ranging
from 0 to 84. Depression severity ranges are as follows: 0 to 11 normal, 12 to 23 mildly ill, 24
to 36 moderately ill, 37 to 46 moderately to severely ill, and 47 to 84 severely ill. In a sample
of outpatients with and without current symptomatic major depressive disorder as determined
by diagnostic interview, a cut-off score of 18 or higher revealed a sensitivity of 100% and a
specificity of 94% (18).
In comparison to the BDI-II, the IDS-SR is a longer measure that takes more time to
complete; however, this measure also offers the benefits of assessment of a larger number of
depressive symptoms, a wider score range, better detection of depression in less severely ill
populations, and easy accessibility (www.ids-qids.org; 18). Furthermore, the IDS-SR has four
items specifically assessing sleep compared to only one item on the BDI-II.
In addition to these two scales, the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS; 21) may be helpful
for assessment of depressive symptoms in older adults with insomnia (12). The GDS is a 30-item
self-report scale and each item is answered by circling yes or no. Some of the items are reversed
scored, and then the items are summed into a single score ranging from 0 to 30. In one of the
original validation studies for the GDS, a cut-off score of 11 yielded a sensitivity rate of 84%
and a specificity rate of 95% (22). There are two main benefits of using this measure for older
adults. First, the GDS is in a simpler format with yes/no questions in comparison to items with
4-point scales. Second, the scale does not include physical symptoms that may mimic depressive
symptoms in older adults, such as motor retardation, which could result from a chronic medical
condition rather than depression.
Bipolar Disorders
Bipolar disorders are characterized by the occurrence of at least one manic or mixed episode (14).
Criteria for diagnosis of a manic episode include abnormally elevated or irritable mood, lasting
one week or longer, accompanied by three or more of the following symptoms (four or more
if only irritable mood): (a) grandiosity, (b) decreased need for sleep (usually with significantly
decreased amounts of total sleep), (c) talkativeness, (d) racing thoughts, (e) distractibility, (f)
increased goal-directed activity, and (g) excessive involvement in pleasurable activities with
potential for negative consequences. Mixed episodes are diagnosed when the patient meets
criteria for both a manic episode and a depressive episode simultaneously. Both manic and mixed
episodes cause clinically significant distress or impaired social or occupational functioning.
Hypomanic episodes are of lesser severity and associated functional impairment.
of impairment associated with symptoms. The first 13 items are answered in a yes/no response
format and scores range from 0 to 13. Using a cut-off of seven or more endorsed symptoms,
one study demonstrated a sensitivity of 58% and a specificity of 93% in a primary care clinic
sample with and without bipolar disorder as determined by a diagnostic interview (24). Using
this same cut-off and similar methods, sensitivity was higher and specificity was lower (73%
and 90%, respectively) in a psychiatric clinic sample (23).
Panic Disorder
Patients with panic disorder experience repeated panic attacks, which are sudden occurrences
of extreme anxiety accompanied by at least four of the following symptoms: palpitations,
sweating, trembling, shortness of breath, choking, chest pain, nausea, dizziness, derealization
or depersonalization, fear of losing control or dying, numbness or tingling, and chills or hot
flushes (14). Agoraphobia, which is the fear of being in a place from which escape or help might
not be possible should a panic attack ensue, often develops as a secondary feature. Panic attacks
usually occur while awake, but may arise during sleep. Panic attacks occurring during sleep
can be differentiated from night terrors since patients are usually awake and alert during panic
attacks and remember them the next day, whereas they do not always fully awaken during a
night terror or confusional arousal and are less likely to remember them. Night terrors are also
more frequently associated with a frightening dream than are panic attacks.
c13 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:32 Char Count=
Schizophrenia
Schizophrenia is a chronic illness characterized by the presence of a constellation of symptoms
that are associated with significant social or occupational dysfunction (14). The lifetime preva-
lence is 1% and death results from suicide in about 10% of cases. Positive symptoms include
disorganization of speech and behavior, as well as psychotic features such as delusions and
hallucinations. Negative symptoms consist of affective flattening, alogia (poverty of speech and
its content), and avolition (decrease in goal-directed activity). Depressed mood, loss of interest
or pleasure in normally pleasurable activities, anxiety, and irritability also may be present.
c13 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:32 Char Count=
Behavioral Factors
Behavioral factors are always important to assess in the context of insomnia. Furthermore,
given that individuals with insomnia comorbid with depressive and/or anxiety disorders have
been shown to report significantly higher levels of sleep-inhibitory behaviors (e.g., napping,
smoking more than one pack of cigarettes a day, using caffeine within four hours of bedtime,
and not relaxing prior to bedtime) than individuals with primary insomnia (52), assessment
of behavioral factors in psychiatric patients with insomnia complaints is paramount. These
patients often have a lack of daily structure, irregular bedtimes and wake-up times, and incon-
sistent patterns of activity and exercise. They also are prone to spend excessive amounts of time
in bed sleeping and attempting to sleep. Finally, they frequently engage in daytime napping
and accidental dozing, as well as use of caffeine, tobacco, alcohol, and/or recreational drugs.
Collectively, these behaviors that commonly occur in reaction to both sleep loss and psychiatric
symptoms can then perpetuate sleep difficulties through circadian disruption, homeostatic dys-
regulation, weakened associations between the bedroom and sleeping, and increased arousal.
Furthermore, some of these behaviors, particularly daytime napping and accidental dozing,
may be exacerbated by sedating psychiatric medications.
c13 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:32 Char Count=
Table 2 Key Areas and Screening and Assessment/Treatment Recommendations for Factors Commonly
Contributing to Insomnia in Patients with Psychiatric Disorders
Screening Assessment/treatment
Factors Key areas recommendations recommendations
Behavioral factors • Lack of daily structure • Patient’s description of a • Cognitive-behavioral
• Irregular sleep schedule typical day and night therapy for insomnia with
• Lack of exercise • Sleep Hygiene Practice an individual trained in
• Excessive time in bed Scale behavioral techniques
• Daytime napping • Sleep logs
• Accidental dozing • Activity logs
• Substance use • Actigraphy
Psychotropic • Antidepressants • Regular follow-up on • Assess time of day
medications • Antipsychotics sleep-related side medication is taken,
• Stimulants effects when prescribing dose and half-life
any of these medications • Consider alternative
agents if possible
Primary sleep • OSA • OSA: screen for obesity, • If OSA or PLMD
disorders • PLMD upper airway suspected, referral for a
• RLS obstruction, snoring, diagnostic sleep study
• Delayed sleep phase witnessed apneas, poor and consultation with
sleep quality, and sleep specialist
excessive daytime • If RLS, referral to a sleep
sleepiness specialist for
• PLMD: screen for legs consultation
twitching or jerking at • If delayed sleep phase,
night light therapy,
• RLS: screen for chronotherapy,
uncomfortable or odd melatonin, and/or
sensations in legs stimulus control
• Delayed sleep phase:
sleep logs
Abbreviations: OSA, obstructive sleep apnea; PLMD, periodic limb movement disorder; RLS, restless legs syndrome.
As part of the assessment, patients should describe a typical day and night. Questionnaires
and/or daily monitoring with sleep logs and/or actigraphy can also provide information about
sleep habits. The Sleep Hygiene Practice Scale (SHPS; 53), may be a particularly useful instru-
ment for this population, which is especially prone to poor sleep hygiene. This measure has
demonstrated good evidence of validity as it discriminates patients meeting criteria for insom-
nia from those who do not (52,53). Patients can engage in self-monitoring by completing sleep
logs (e.g., 54) and/or activity logs (e.g., 55). An assessment of the 24-hour rest-activity pattern
over days or weeks may also be assessed through the use of actigraphy devices, particularly if
the patient’s psychiatric issues significantly limit his or her ability to appropriately self-report
sleep and waking patterns.
Patients who exhibit sleep-related behaviors that are counterproductive to promoting
sleep (e.g., large amounts of time in bed awake, a variable sleep schedule, daytime napping
or dozing, alcohol use to facilitate sleep) may benefit from cognitive-behavioral therapy for
insomnia (CBT-I; see chapters 24–30). Several recent research studies have demonstrated the
efficacy of CBT-I for insomnia comorbid with depression, showing not only improvements in
sleep outcomes, but also improvements in depressive symptoms (11,56).
medications on sleep within each class are reviewed below. When evaluating the psychiatric
patient with insomnia, it is important to consider the contribution of these medications to sleep
complaints. The care provider should also verify that the patient is taking the medication at the
appropriate time of day and at the appropriate dosage. If sleep-related side effects are clinically
significant and timing and dosage have been optimized, it is recommended that other agents
be tried whenever possible.
wake-up times. Treatment for delayed sleep phase disorder includes morning bright light
therapy for less significantly delayed sleep patterns and chronotherapy for more significantly
delayed sleep patterns (76). Treatment may also include melatonin administration and/or other
behavioral techniques, such as stimulus control therapy (77), if sleep-inhibitory behaviors are
present (76).
SUMMARY
Insomnia and psychiatric disorders frequently occur together. Furthermore, mounting evidence
has shown that insomnia is a risk factor for future onset and/or relapse and exacerbation
of psychiatric disorders, particularly depression. Therefore, the assessment of both sleep and
psychiatric symptomatology in individuals with either an insomnia or a psychiatric complaint is
essential. Psychiatric disorders commonly occurring with insomnia include mood, anxiety, and
psychotic disorders; these have effects on both subjective insomnia as well as affecting objective
sleep EEG patterns. Screening questions and tools are often helpful to identify psychiatric
disorders in patients with insomnia and to monitor the severity of psychiatric symptoms during
the course of treatment. Other factors commonly contributing to insomnia in psychiatric patients
include behavioral factors, the effects of psychotropic medications, and other sleep disorders.
An understanding of the comorbidity between sleep and psychiatric disorders as well as ways
to better screen, assess, and monitor both insomnia and psychiatric disorders should lead to
more optimal management and outcome in insomnia patients.
REFERENCES
1. Ford DE, Kamerow DB. Epidemiologic study of sleep disturbances and psychiatric disorders. JAMA
1989; 262(11):1479–1484.
2. Ohayon MM. Prevalence of DSM-IV diagnostic criteria of insomnia: distinguishing insomnia related
to mental disorders from sleep disorders. J Psychiatr Res 1997; 31(3):333–346.
3. Ohayon MM, Roth T. Place of chronic insomnia in the course of depressive and anxiety disorders. J
Psychiatr Res 2003; 37:9–15.
4. Carney C, Segal Z, Edinger J, et al. A comparison of rates of residual symptoms following phar-
macotherapy or cognitive-behavior therapy for major depressive disorder. J Clin Psychiatry 2008;
68:254–260.
5. Breslau N, Roth T, Rosenthal L, et al. Sleep disturbance and psychiatric disorders: a longitudinal
epidemiological study of young adults. Biol Psychiatry 1996; 39:411–418.
6. Chang P, Ford D, Mead L, et al. Insomnia in young men and subsequent depression: The Johns Hopkins
Precursor Study. Am J Epidemiol 1997; 146:105–114.
7. Johnson E, Roth T, Breslau N. The association of insomnia with anxiety disorder and depression:
exploration of the direction of risk. J Psychiatr Res 2006; 40:700–708.
8. Weissman M, Greenwald S, Niño-Murcia G, et al. The morbidity of insomnia uncomplicated by
psychiatric disorders. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 1997; 19:245–250.
9. Reynolds CF, Frank E, Houck P, et al. Which elderly patients with remitted depression remain well
with continued interpersonal psychotherapy after discontinuation of antidepressant therapy? Am J
Psychiatry 1997; 154:958–962.
10. Fava M, McCall V, Krystal A, et al. Eszopiclone co-administered with fluoxetine in patients with
insomnia coexisting with major depressive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2006; 59:1052–1060.
11. Manber R, Edinger J, Gress J, et al. Cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia enhances depression
outcome in patients with comorbid major depressive disorder and insomnia. Sleep 2008; 31:489–495.
12. Buysse DJ, Ancoli-Israel S, Edinger JD, et al. Recommendations for a standard research assessment of
insomnia. Sleep 2006; 29(9):1155–1173.
13. Roth T. The relationship between psychiatric diseases and insomnia. Int J Clin Pract 2001; 116:3–8.
14. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed, text
revision. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association, 2001.
15. Benca R, Obermeyer W, Thisted R, et al. Sleep and psychiatric disorders. A meta-analysis. Arch Gen
Psychiatry 1992; 49:651–670.
16. Kupfer D. Sleep research in depressive illness: clinical implications—a tasting menu. Biol Psychiatry
1995; 38:391–403.
17. Beck A, Steer R, Brown G. Beck Depression Inventory-II manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological
Corporation, 1996.
c13 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:32 Char Count=
18. Rush A, Gullion B, Basco M, et al. The Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology (IDS): psychometric
properties. Psychol Med 1996; 26:477–486.
19. Spitzer R, Kroenke K, Williams J. Validation and utility of a self-report version of the PRIME-MD: the
PHQ primary care study. JAMA 1999; 282:1737–1744.
20. Arnau R, Meagher M, Norris M, et al. Psychometric evaluation of the Beck Depression Inventory-II
with primary care patients. Health Psychol 2001; 20:112–119.
21. Yesavage J, Brink T, Rose T, et al. Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale:
a preliminary report. J Psychiatr Res 1983; 17:37–49.
22. Brink T, Yesavage J, Lum O, et al. Screening tests for geriatric depression. Clin Gerontol 1982; 1:37–43.
23. Hirschfeld R, Williams J, Spitzer R, et al. Development and validation of a screening instrument for
bipolar spectrum disorder: the Mood Disorder Questionnaire. Am J Psychiatry, 2000; 157:1873–1875.
24. Hirschfeld R, Cass A, Holt D, et al. Screening for bipolar disorder in patients treated for depression in
a family medicine clinic. J Am Board Fam Med 2005; 18:233–239.
25. Belanger L, Morin C, Langlois F, et al. Insomnia and generalized anxiety disorder: effects of cognitive
behavior therapy for GAD on insomnia symptoms. J Anxiety Disord 2004; 18:561–571.
26. Arriaga F, Paiva T. Clinical and EEG sleep changes in primary dysthymia and generalized anxiety
disorder: a comparison with normal controls. Neuropsychobiology 1990–1991; 24:109–114.
27. Papdimitriou G, Kerkhofs M, Kempenaers C, et al. EEG sleep studies in patients with generalized
anxiety disorder. Psychiatry Res 1988; 26:183–190.
28. Saletu-Zyhalrz G, Saletu B, Anderer P, et al. Nonorganic insomnia in generalized anxiety disorder.
Controlled studies on sleep, awakening and daytime vigilance utilizing polysomnography and EEG
mapping. Neuropsychobiology 1997; 36:117–129.
29. Spielberger C, Gorsuch R, Lushene R. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA:
Consulting Psychologists Press, 1970.
30. Meyer T, Miller M, Metzger R, et al. Development and validation of the Penn State Worry Question-
naire. Behav Res Ther 1990; 28:487–495.
31. Behar E, Alcaine O, Zuellig A, et al. Screening for generalized anxiety disorder using the Penn State
Worry Questionnaire: a receiver operating characteristic analysis. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 2003;
34:25–43.
32. Arriaga F, Paiva T, Matos-Pires A, et al. The sleep of non-depressed patients with panic disorder: a
comparison with normal controls. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1996; 93:191–194.
33. Sloan E, Natarajan M, Baker B, et al. Nocturnal and daytime panic attacks-comparison of sleep
architecture, heart rate variability, and response to sodium lactate challenge. Biol Psychiatry 1999;
45:1313–1320.
34. Mellman T, Uhde T. Patients with frequent sleep panic: clinical findings and response to medication
treatment. J Clin Psychiatry 1990; 51:513–516.
35. Mellman T, Unde T. Electroencephalographic sleep in panic disorder. A focus on sleep-related panic
attacks. J Clin Psychiatry 1989; 146:1204–1207.
36. Apeldorf W, Shear M, Leon A, et al. A brief screen for panic disorder. J Anxiety Disord 1994; 8:71–78.
37. Mellman T, Nolan B, Hebding J, et al. Sleep events among veterans with combat-related PTSD,
depressed men, and non-ill controls. Sleep 1997; 20:46–51.
38. Ross R, Ball W, Dinges D, et al. Rapid eye movement sleep disturbance in posttraumatic stress disorder.
Biol Psychiatry 1994; 35:195–202.
39. Ross R, Ball W, Dinges D, et al. Motor dysfunction during sleep in posttraumatic stress disorder. Sleep
1994; 17:723–732.
40. Lavie P. Current concepts: sleep disturbances in the wake of traumatic events. N Engl J Med 2001;
345:1825–1832.
41. Mellman T. Sleep and the pathogenesis of PTSD. In: Shalev A, Yehuda R, McFarlane A, eds. Interna-
tional Handbook of Human Response to Trauma. New York: Plenum Publishing, 2000.
42. Foa EB. Posttraumatic Stress Diagnostic Scale: Manual. Minneapolis, MN: National Computer System,
1995.
43. Sheeran T, Zimmerman M. Screening for posttraumatic stress disorder in a general psychiatric outpa-
tient sample. J Consult Clin Psychol 2002; 70:961–966.
44. Ritsner M, Kurs R, Ponizovsky A, et al. Perceived quality of life in schizophrenia: relationships to
sleep quality. Qual Life Res 2004; 13:783–791.
45. Haffmans P, Hoencamp E, Knegtering H, et al. Sleep disturbance in schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry
1994; 165:697–698.
46. Chemerinski E, Ho B, Flaum M, et al. Insomnia as a predictor for symptom worsening following
antipsychotic withdrawal in schizophrenia. Compr Psychiatry 2002; 43:393–396.
47. Benson K. Sleep in schizophrenia: impairments, correlates, and treatment. Psychiatr Clin North Am
2006; 29:1033–1045.
c13 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:32 Char Count=
48. Chouinard S, Poulin J, Stip E, et al. Sleep in untreated patients with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis.
Schizophr Bull 2004; 30:957–967.
49. Ferrarelli F, Huber R, Peterson M, et al. Reduced sleep spindle activity in schizophrenia patients. Am
J Psychiatry 2007; 164:483–492.
50. Overall J, Gorham D. The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS): recent developments in ascertainment
and scaling. Psychopharmacol Bull 1988; 24:97–99.
51. Kay S, Opler L, Fiszbein A. Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale Manual. North Tonawanda, NY:
Multi-Health Systems, 1994.
52. Kohn L, Espie C. Sensitivity and specificity of measures of the insomnia experience: a comparative
study of psychophysiologic insomnia, insomnia associated with mental disorder, and good sleepers.
Sleep 2005; 28:104–112.
53. Lacks P, Rotert M. Knowledge and practice of sleep hygiene techniques in insomniacs and good
sleepers. Behav Res Ther 1986; 24(3):365–368.
54. Morin C, Espie C. Insomnia: A Clinical Guide to Assessment and Treatment. New York: Kluwer, 2003.
55. Frank E. Treating Bipolar Disorder: A Clinician’s Guide to Interpersonal and Social Rhythms Therapy.
New York: Guilford Press, 2005.
56. Taylor D, Lichstein K, Weinstock J, et al. A pilot study of cognitive-behavioral therapy of insomnia in
people with mild depression. Behav Ther 2007; 38:49–57.
57. DeMartinis N, Winokur A. Effects of psychiatric medications on sleep and sleep disorders. CNS Neurol
Disord Drug Targets 2007; 6:17–29.
58. Argyropoulos S, Wilson S. Sleep disturbances in depression and the effects of antidepressants. Int Rev
Psychiatry 2005; 17:237–245.
59. Lippmann S, Mazour I, Shahab H. Insomnia: therapeutic approach. South Med J 2001; 94:866–873.
60. Mayers AG, Baldwin, DS. Antidepressants and their effect on sleep. Hum Psychopharmacol 2005;
20:533–559.
61. Hofer A, Kemmler G, Eder U, et al. Attitudes toward antipsychotics among outpatient clinic attendees
with schizophrenia. J Clin Psychiatry 2002; 63:49–53.
62. Armitage R, Cole D, Suppes T, et al. Effects of clozapine on sleep in bipolar and schizoaffective
disorders. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2004; 28:1065–1070.
63. Gimenez S, Clos S, Romero S, et al. Effects of olanzapine, risperidone and haloperidol on sleep after a
single oral morning dose in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology 2007; 190:507–516.
64. Keshavan M, Prasad K, Montrose D, et al. Sleep quality and architecture in quetiapine, risperidone,
or never-treated schizophrenia patients. J Clin Psychopharmcol 2007; 27:703–705.
65. Mendelson W, Caruso C. Pharmacology in sleep medicine. In: Pocete J, Mitler M, eds. Sleep Disorders:
Diagnosis and Treatment. Totowa, NJ: Humana Press Inc, 1998:137–160.
66. Krakow B, Melendrez D, Ferreira E, et al. Prevalence of insomnia in patients with sleep-disordered
breathing. Chest 2001; 120:1923–1929.
67. Ohayon M. The effects of breathing-related sleep disorders on mood disturbances in the general
population. J Clin Psychiatry 2003; 64:1195–1200.
68. Winkelman J. Schizophrenia, obesity, and obstructive sleep apnea. J Clin Psychiatry 2001; 62:8–11.
69. Lesage S, Hening W. The restless legs syndrome and periodic limb movement disorder: a review of
management. Semin Neurol 2004; 24:249–259.
70. Rottach K, Schaner B, Kirch M, et al. Restless legs syndrome as side effect of second generation
antidepressants. J Psychiatr Res 2008; 43:70–75.
71. Yang C, White D, Winkleman J. Antidepressants and periodic leg movements of sleep. Biol Psychiatry
2005; 58:510–514.
72. Nofzinger E, Fasiczka A, Berman S, et al. Bupropion SR reduces periodic limb movements associated
with arousals from sleep in depressed patients with periodic limb movement disorder. J Clin Psychiatry
2000; 61:858–862.
73. Taylor D, Jenni O, Acebo C, et al. Sleep tendency during extended wakefulness: insights into adolescent
sleep regulation and behavior. J Sleep Res 2005; 22:571–584.
74. Mansour H, Wood J, Chowdari K, et al. Circadian phase variation in bipolar I disorder. Chronobiol
Int 2005; 22:571–584.
75. Ahn Y, Change J, Joo Y, et al. Chronotype distribution in bipolar I disorder and schizophrenia in a
Korean sample. Bioplar Disord 2008; 10:271–275.
76. Lack L, Wright H. Clinical management of delayed sleep phase disorder. Behav Sleep Med 2007;
5:57–76.
77. Bootzin, R. Stimulus control treatment for insomnia. In: Proceedings of the 80th Annual Convention
of the American Psychological Association; 1972; 7:395–396.80
IHBK059-14 IHBK059-Sateia March 31, 2010 16:30 Char Count=
Michael T. Smith
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Behavioral Sleep Medicine Program, Johns Hopkins
University School of Medicine, Baltimore, Maryland, U.S.A.
COPING
AROUSAL
PAIN
INSOMNIA
DEPRESSION
be elevated in chronic pain patients (e.g., Ref. 12). Although it is commonly assumed that sleep
disturbance associated with pain is primarily a consequence of pain, experimental and cross
sectional evidence spanning over half a century, as well as emerging longitudinal data, support
a bidirectional relationship between sleep and pain. Indeed, for patients with chronic pain
a vicious cycle often develops, with pain disrupting sleep and poor sleep further exacerba-
ting pain.
Importantly, mood disturbance and psychological distress are common among chronic
pain patients (13). Evidence consistently supports elevated rates of both depression and anx-
iety in chronic pain populations, and as demonstrated in Figure 1, it is now known that the
sleep–pain relationship is often mediated by cognitive and affective processes. This suggests
that thorough assessment of emotional disturbance and sleep disorders is especially important
in the evaluation and management of patients with chronic pain conditions. Further, despite
high prevalence rates of insomnia complaints in patients suffering from chronic pain condi-
tions, systematic empirical treatment approaches are not well developed and therefore clinical
management can be especially challenging.
The purpose of this chapter is to review the relation between chronic insomnia and chronic
pain. To this end, we will briefly summarize the literature evaluating the effect of experimental
sleep disturbance on pain sensitivity, as well as the effects of the administration of pain during
sleep to provide important background information. We will next consider evidence supporting
prospective and reciprocal relations between sleep disturbance and pain. Sleep complaints in
several common chronic pain conditions will be reviewed, and key findings from the extant
clinical trials literature, including pharmacological and cognitive-behavioral approaches, will
be discussed. The chapter concludes with clinical recommendations and suggestions for future
research.
somewhat mixed (17), in aggregate these findings support a bidirectional, causal relationship
between sleep and pain sensitivity (18). More recent studies have reported increased sensitivity
to mechanical stimuli following 40 hours of total sleep deprivation (19), a 24% decrease in
mechanical pain threshold, and an increase in inflammatory skin flare response following SWS
disruption among women (20). Recent well-controlled experiments found that restriction to four
hours of sleep and REM disruption (but not NREM disruption) caused thermal hyperalgesia
(21). In a controlled study of 32 healthy females, sleep continuity disturbance induced via
forced awakenings, but not simple sleep curtailment, impaired descending pain inhibitory
functions and induced spontaneous reports of pain (22). While not enough systematic research
is yet available to conclusively explain which types of sleep disruption or loss alter various
dimensions of central pain processing, it is clear that insufficient sleep contributes to pain
amplification and induces pain even in the absence of peripheral input.
with rheumatoid arthritis and found that time awake in bed predicted morning stiffness, joint
tenderness, and subjective pain ratings six months later. Similarly, baseline ratings of pain and
morning stiffness predicted nocturnal wake time as well as changes in sleep architecture at six
months (30).
processing of noxious input. As a result, there is a great deal of overlap in symptoms between
these conditions, which have been increasingly referred to as “central sensitivity syndromes”
(57). Patients diagnosed with one of these disorders frequently meet diagnostic criteria for other
syndromes in this cluster of conditions associated with intractable pain (58,59).
Subjective and objectively measured sleep disturbances are highly prevalent in these pop-
ulations and may exacerbate symptoms through increased psychological distress or disturbed
processing of noxious inputs (e.g., Ref. 22). In clinical settings, sleep disruption is among the
most common of all symptoms reported in idiopathic pain disorders. For example, fibromyalgia
(FM) has a consistent and well-replicated relation with disturbed sleep (11), with the majority of
patients reporting difficulty maintaining sleep and characterizing their sleep quality as shallow
and nonrestorative (60). Complaints of impaired sleep continuity in FM have been verified by
PSG, including increased sleep onset latency (61), increased nocturnal arousals (62–64), more
time awake during the night (62), greater sleep fragmentation (65), decreased sleep efficiency
(64,66,67), and increased alpha intrusion during NREM sleep (64,68).
Patients with myofascial temporomandibular joint disorder (TMD) report similar sleep
disturbances, and recent evidence suggests high rates of diagnosed sleep disorders in this pop-
ulation, including insomnia, obstructive sleep apnea, and sleep bruxism (69a,69b). Polysomno-
graphically measured decrements in sleep efficiency have also been linked to psychophysical
measures of impaired pain inhibitory processes in TMD (70). Sleep fragmentation has been
shown to decrease endogenous pain inhibitory capacity, and dysfunction of these same descend-
ing pain inhibitory systems has been implicated in pathophysiologic models of idiopathic pain
disorders (71).
Sleep disturbance and headache disorders, the most common category of episodic pain
disorder, are also highly comorbid. Insomnia is the most common sleep complaint in this
population (72) with over one half of all headache patients reporting difficulty initiating or
maintaining sleep, early morning awakening, or nonrestorative sleep (73). Boardman et al. (74)
surveyed 2662 adults living in the United Kingdom and found that after controlling for age and
gender, sleep disturbance was monotonically related to headache complaints. Similarly, Ohayon
(75) analyzed data from a large-scale telephone-based survey and found a significant associa-
tion between morning headache and insomnia. Other studies have documented differences in
sleep complaints between patients with migraine or tension-type headache, with tension-type
headache generally being more strongly associated with complaints of insomnia (76,77). Among
patients with tension-type headache, insomnia has also been associated with poorer prognosis
(77). Patients with migraine have changes in the quality of sleep several days before the onset
of a migraine attack, but have largely normal sleep patterns outside the attacks (78). Cluster
headaches (attacks of severe unilateral orbital, supraorbital or temporal pain) frequently occur
during sleep and are believed to be directly associated with chronobiologic mechanisms and
sleep stage shifts. In addition to insomnia, sleep disordered breathing has often been linked
to morning headaches, and current guidelines indicated that headache management should
identify and treat sleep disorders, which may improve or prevent headaches (72). In general,
the particularly strong association between sleep disturbance and central sensitivity syndromes
suggests the possibility that sleep disturbances may play an integral role in the pathophysiology
and clinical course of these syndromes. Research aimed at determining how treatment of sleep
disturbances in these conditions alters pain and related symptoms is needed.
pain conditions include diabetic neuropathies, complex regional pain syndrome, demyelinating
diseases, virally mediated neuropathies (e.g., HIV and postherpetic neuralgia), spinal cord
injuries, and iatrogenic nerve damage due to chemotherapies for cancer. Although there
are many causes of neuropathic pain, treatment targeting neuropathic pain most commonly
includes agents that stabilize cell membranes (e.g., Na+ and K+ channels) associated with
aberrant nerve conduction. Anticonvulsant medications with sedating properties are commonly
used and may improve sleep.
As is the case with other types of chronic pain, mood disturbance is common among
patients experiencing neuropathic pain (79), and not surprisingly, many patients also complain
of sleep disturbance. In one of the few systematic studies of sleep in neuropathic pain disorders,
Zelman et al. (80) compared self-report sleep data from 255 patients with painful diabetic
peripheral neuropathy (DPN). These authors found that relative to the general population (P <
0.001) and patients with chronic medical diseases (P < 0.05), patients with painful DPN reported
significantly poorer sleep. The DPN group also reported greater sleep disturbance than subjects
with postherpetic neuralgia. Average daily pain and anxiety and depression symptoms were
all associated with poorer sleep in this cross sectional study. With increasing prevalence of
obesity and diabetes worldwide and an estimated U.S. diabetes prevalence of 11.2% by the year
2030 (81), these data indicate that systematic research to assess and treat sleep and pain in this
population is a health priority.
Finally, some recent evidence also supports the use of weak opioid agonists for comorbid
pain and sleep disturbance. In two large (N > 1000) posthoc studies of extended-release tramadol
in adults with OA, significant improvement in self-reported sleep parameters were reported as
early as one week and maintained throughout treatment (90). In another study of OA patients,
Kivitz et al (91) reported that 40 and 50 mg, but not 10 mg, extended-release oxymorphone
improved self-reported sleep.
mood improved concurrent with sleep, no changes were detected in subjective pain ratings. In
a sample of cancer patients, Cannici et al. (109) found that PMR led to a significant, 90-minute
reduction in diary-reported sleep onset latency, an improvement that was maintained at follow-
up. Again, no improvements in pain were observed, although baseline pain ratings in this
sample were low. A study of mixed diagnosis outpatients demonstrated that CBT-I could be
effective even when the primary cause of sleep disturbance was believed to be medical or psy-
chiatric (110). Moderate effect sizes were reportedly observed for improvements in wake time
after sleep onset, sleep efficiency, and sleep quality ratings, and these changes were maintained
at three-month follow-up (110). Others have reported similar results (111). Among older adults
with mixed medical comorbidities including OA, CBT-I is associated with improvements in
diary-reported but not actigraphically measured sleep onset latency, sleep efficiency, and sleep
quality (112,113). In addition to these studies employing standard cognitive-behavioral treat-
ment for insomnia, other studies have evaluated the impact of adding sleep education to routine
medical care. For example, Calhoun and Ford (114) employed a randomized, placebo-controlled
design and found that adding cognitive-behavioral instructions for improved sleep was asso-
ciated with a significant reduction in headache complaints among 43 women with migraine.
Further, improvement in headache symptoms was related to the number of changes in sleep
behaviors reported. Although changes in sleep parameters were not reported, this study is
nonetheless notable for the association of a sleep-focused intervention and reductions in report
of chronic pain.
At least two randomized clinical trials have evaluated using CBT-I exclusively in chronic
pain conditions. In the first of these, Currie et al. (115) reported improvements in the CBT-I
condition relative to wait-list control in both diary and actigraphic measures of sleep latency,
wake after sleep onset, sleep efficiency, and sleep quality. As is found in CBT-I for primary
insomnia, effect sizes were large (in the range of 0.80), and treatment gains were maintained at
three months. Although no significant group differences were observed in total sleep time, the
CBT-I group tended to have larger gains that increased during the three-month follow-up period.
More recently, Edinger et al. (116) employed an active control group to test the effectiveness
of CBT-I in FM patients with chronic insomnia. Relative to the control condition, CBT-I was
associated with improvements in diary-based measures of sleep onset latency, sleep efficiency,
and total wake time. Actigraphy-measured sleep latency was also significantly improved in
CBT-I relative to control. In addition, reductions in night-to-night sleep variability were also
observed in the CBT-I group.
Preliminary evidence suggests effectiveness of standard CBT-I for improving sleep in
patients with chronically painful conditions, although further research is needed. Several impor-
tant limitations must also be noted. Only one of the aforementioned studies (116) employed
a credible control condition. Thus, it cannot be concluded that observed changes were due to
specific treatment elements rather than nonspecific factors such as therapist contact. It is par-
ticularly challenging to develop credible placebo control conditions for cognitive-behavioral
interventions. Sleep hygiene, often employed as a control condition, has been associated with
improvements in insomnia complaints (117), further highlighting the need for new approaches
to experimental design in this area.
follow-up, concurrent with the nearly significant reduction in pain. This result raises the possibil-
ity that noticeable analgesic effects may require increases in consolidated sleep time. However,
participants in the sleep hygiene condition of the Edinger et al. (116) study reported significantly
reduced pain even by posttreatment, while those in the CBT-I condition did not. Posthoc anal-
yses of this data revealed that a subset of patients in the sleep hygiene group actually restricted
their sleep opportunity. These individuals reported reduced time in bed and total wake time,
and also showed significant reductions in pain. In summary, although much remains to be
learned, two studies suggest that improving sleep might reduce clinical pain, although these
effects may require time for sleep consolidation to occur. Aggressively treating insomnia asso-
ciated with chronic pain conditions clearly has the potential to improve sleep, pain, mood, and
quality of life.
CLINICAL RECOMMENDATIONS
In clinical practice, a majority of patients suffering from chronic pain will report numerous
overlapping symptoms including difficulty sleeping and psychological distress. Due to the
numerous reciprocal relationships among these symptoms, their casual interactions are difficult
to disentangle. Clinicians treating patients with chronic pain conditions will find themselves
facing chicken-or-egg dilemmas in diagnosis: Are the patients having trouble sleeping because
they are in pain, or do they are in pain because they are hardly sleeping? These relationships
are likely to be dynamic and may change within the patients, requiring frequent re-evaluation
for the treatment plan. It is strongly recommended that clinicians working with patients with
chronic pain conditions adopt a biopsychosocial perspective for assessment and conceptual-
ization. Treatment will frequently require flexibility on the part of the clinician, and providers
will often be asked to employ a balanced treatment approach that addresses multiple com-
plaints simultaneously. A multidisciplinary approach is highly desirable if not essential. Finally,
because enhancing motivation is such an important component of many treatment approaches,
an empathetic, patient-centered approach to maximize treatment outcomes is often most pro-
ductive, as many chronic pain patients have experienced multiple frustrations in their search
for pain relief.
When chronic pain patients seek care for their sleep disturbance, several areas must be
addressed as part of a thorough biopsychosocial assessment (118,119). We review the primary
categories below. In addition, medical records including current medications and previous sleep
study results should be requested and reviewed.
r Sleep disturbance. In addition to thoroughly assessing insomnia complaints, the diagnostic
priority should be to discern sleepiness from fatigue and whether referral for an overnight
sleep study is warranted. Sleep disorders other than insomnia (e.g., sleep disordered breath-
ing, restless legs) will require additional evaluation and treatment. Of note, actigraphy has
recently been validated as a measure of sleep in patients with TMD and may be a useful
adjunct in patients with chronic pain conditions (123).
r Mood states and psychiatric distress. Psychological distress is likely to impair patients’ ability
and motivation to care for themselves, undermining the self-efficacy that patients need to
engage in routine self-care. Numerous validated screening instruments exist for the most
common psychiatric disorders, including depression and anxiety, and clinicians should
also inquire about suicidal ideation/behavior, which is not only elevated in chronic pain
populations (120), but has also been linked to insomnia in chronic pain patients (121).
r Cognitive processes and thought content. Many patients with chronic pain report increased
somatic focus and engage in catastrophic thinking, which predicts poorer functional out-
comes. Clinicians should seek to understand these thought processes, which can be effec-
tively addressed using cognitive restructuring, and patient attitudes that can similarly be
explored and modified in treatment. For example, many pain patients perceive that exer-
cise will exacerbate their pain experience. In reality, gradually building toward consistent
moderate exercise is among the most reliable treatments associated with improvements in
physical function and improved mood among chronic pain patients. Patient beliefs about
the relations between their pain and sleep disturbance can also guide treatment planning.
Treatment acceptability and readiness to change are other important areas for assessment.
IHBK059-14 IHBK059-Sateia March 31, 2010 16:30 Char Count=
REFERENCES
1. International Association for the Study of Pain. Pain terms: a list with definitions and notes on usage.
Pain 1982; 14:205.
2. Gatchel RJ, Peng YB, Peters ML, et al. The biopsychosocial approach to chronic pain: scientific
advances and future directions. Psychol Bull 2007; 133(4):581–624.
3. Basbaum AI, Fields HL. Endogenous pain control systems: brainstem spinal pathways and endorphin
circuitry. Annu Rev Neurosci 1984; 7:309–338.
4. Edwards RR. Individual differences in endogenous pain modulation as a risk factor for chronic pain.
Neurology 2005; 65(3):437–443.
5. Melzack R. Pain and the neuromatrix in the brain. J Dent Educ 2001; 65(12):1378–1382.
6. Diatchenko L, Slade GD, Nackley AG, et al. Genetic basis for individual variations in pain perception
and the development of a chronic pain condition. Hum Mol Genet 2005; 14(1):135–143.
7. Gureje O, Von KM, Kola L, et al. The relation between multiple pains and mental disorders: results
from the World Mental Health Surveys. Pain 2008; 135(1–2):82–91.
8. Smith MT, Huang MI, Manber R. Cognitive behavior therapy for chronic insomnia occurring within
the context of medical and psychiatric disorders. Clin Psychol Rev 2005; 25(5):559–592.
9. Smith MT, Klick B, Kozachik S, et al. Sleep onset insomnia symptoms during hospitalization for
major burn injury predict chronic pain. Pain 2008; 138(3):497–506.
10. von Korff M, Le Resche L, Dworkin SF. First onset of common pain symptoms: a prospective study
of depression as a risk factor. Pain 1993; 55(2):251–258.
11. Smith MT, Haythornthwaite JA. How do sleep disturbance and chronic pain inter-relate? Insights
from the longitudinal and cognitive-behavioral clinical trials literature. Sleep Med Rev 2004; 8(2):119–
132.
IHBK059-14 IHBK059-Sateia March 31, 2010 16:30 Char Count=
12. Gold AR, Dipalo F, Gold MS, Broderick J. Inspiratory airflow dynamics during sleep in women with
fibromyalgia. Sleep 2004; 27(3):459–466.
13. Bair MJ, Robinson RL, Katon W, et al. Depression and pain comorbidity: a literature review. Arch
Intern Med 2003; 163(20):2433–2445.
14. Copperman NR, Mullin FJ, Kleitman N. Further observations on the effects of prolonged sleepless-
ness. Am J Physiol 1934; 107:589–594.
15. Moldofsky H, Scarisbrick P, England R, et al. Musculoskeletal symptoms and non-REM sleep distur-
bance in patients with “fibrositis syndrome” and healthy subjects. Psychosom Med 1975; 37:341–351.
16. Moldofsky H, Scarisbrick P. Induction of neurasthenic musculoskeletal pain syndrome by selective
sleep stage deprivation. Psychosom Med 1976; 38(1):35–44.
17. Older SA, Battafarano DF, Danning CL, et al. The effects of delta wave sleep interruption on pain
thresholds and fibromyalgia-like symptoms in healthy subjects; correlations with insulin-like growth
factor I. J Rheumatol 1998; 25(6):1180–1186.
18. Lautenbacher S, Kundermann B, Krieg JC. Sleep deprivation and pain perception. Sleep Med Rev
2006; 10(5):357–369.
19. Onen SH, Alloui A, Gross A, et al. The effects of total sleep deprivation, selective sleep interruption
and sleep recovery on pain tolerance thresholds in healthy subjects. J Sleep Res 2001; 10(1):35–42.
20. Lentz MJ, Landis CA, Rothermel J, et al. Effects of selective slow wave sleep disruption on muscu-
loskeletal pain and fatigue in middle aged women. J Rheumatol 1999; 26(7):1586–1592.
21. Roehrs T, Hyde M, Blaisdell B, et al. Sleep loss and REM sleep loss are hyperalgesic. Sleep 2006;
29(2):145–151.
22. Smith MT, Edwards RR, McCann UD, et al. The effects of sleep deprivation on pain inhibition and
spontaneous pain in women. Sleep 2007; 30(4):494–505.
23. Lavigne G, Zucconi M, Castronovo C,et al. Sleep arousal response to experimental thermal stimula-
tion during sleep in human subjects free of pain and sleep problems. Pain 2000; 84(2–3):283–290.
24. Drewes AM, Nielsen KD, Arendt-Nielsen L, et al. The effect of cutaneous and deep pain on the
electroencephalogram during sleep—an experimental study. Sleep 1997; 20(8):632–640.
25. Sandrini G, Milanov I, Rossi B, et al. Effects of sleep on spinal nociceptive reflexes in humans. Sleep
2001; 24(1):13–17.
26. Lavigne GJ, Zucconi M, Castronovo V, et al. Heart rate changes during sleep in response to experimen-
tal thermal (nociceptive) stimulations in healthy subjects. Clin Neurophysiol 2001; 112(3):532–535.
27. Bentley AJ, Newton S, Zio CD. Sensitivity of sleep stages to painful thermal stimuli. J Sleep Res 2003;
12(2):143–147.
28. Lavigne G, Brousseau M, Kato T, et al. Experimental pain perception remains equally active over all
sleep stages. Pain 2004; 110(3):646–655.
29. Affleck G, Urrows S, Tennen H, et al. Sequential daily relations of sleep, pain intensity, and attention
to pain among women with fibromyalgia. Pain 1996; 68:363–368.
30. Drewes AM, Nielsen KD, Hansen B, et al. A longitudinal study of clinical symptoms and sleep
parameters in rheumatoid arthritis. Rheumatology (Oxford) 2000; 39(11):1287–1289.
31. Raymond I, Nielsen TA, Lavigne G, et al. Quality of sleep and its daily relationship to pain intensity
in hospitalized adult burn patients. Pain 2001; 92(3):381–388.
32. Stone AA, Broderick JE, Porter LS, et al. The experience of rheumatoid arthritis pain and fatigue:
examining momentary reports and correlates over one week. Arthritis Care Res 1997; 10(3):185–
193.
33. Shaw IR, Lavigne G, Mayer P, et al. Acute intravenous administration of morphine perturbs sleep
architecture in healthy pain-free young adults: a preliminary study. Sleep 2005; 28(6):677–682.
34. Raymond I, Ancoli-Israel S, Choiniere M. Sleep disturbances, pain and analgesia in adults hospital-
ized for burn injuries. Sleep Med 2004; 5(6):551–559.
35. Mikkelsson M, Sourander A, Salminen JJ, et al. Widespread pain and neck pain in schoolchildren. A
prospective one-year follow-up study. Acta Paediatr 1999; 88(10):1119–1124.
36. Edwards RR, Almeida DM, Klick B, et al. Duration of sleep contributes to next-day pain report in the
general population. Pain 2008; 137(1):202–206.
37. Leigh TJ, Bird HA, Hindmarch I, et al. A comparison of sleep in rheumatic and non-rheumatic
patients. Clin Exp Rheumatol 1987; 5(4):363–365.
38. Wilcox S, Brenes GA, Levine D, et al. Factors related to sleep disturbance in older adults experiencing
knee pain or knee pain with radiographic evidence of knee osteoarthritis. J Am Geriatr Soc 2000;
48(10):1241–1251.
39. Gallup Organization. Adult Public’s Experiences with Nighttime Pain. Washington, DC: National
Sleep Foundation, 1997.
40. Davis GC. Improved sleep may reduce arthritis pain. Holist Nurs Pract 2003; 17(3):128–135.
41. Gallup Organization. Sleep and Aging. Washington, DC: National Sleep Foundation, 1996.
IHBK059-14 IHBK059-Sateia March 31, 2010 16:30 Char Count=
42. Ohayon MM. Relationship between chronic painful physical condition and insomnia. J Psychiatr Res
2005; 39(2):151–159.
43. McMillan SC, Tofthagen C, Morgan MA. Relationships among pain, sleep disturbances, and depres-
sive symptoms in outpatients from a comprehensive cancer center. Oncol Nurs Forum 2008; 35(4):603–
611.
44. Drewes AM, Svendsen L, Taagholt SJ, et al. Sleep in rheumatoid arthritis: a comparison with healthy
subjects and studies of sleep/wake interactions. Br J Rheumatol 1998; 37(1):71–81.
45. Hirsch M, Carlander B, Verge M, et al. Objective and subjective sleep disturbances in patients with
rheumatoid arthritis. Arthritis Rheum 1994; 37(1):41–49.
46. Moldofsky H, Lue FA, Smythe HA. Alpha EEG sleep and morning symptoms in rheumatoid arthritis.
J Rheumatol 1983; 10(3):373–379.
47. Lavie P, Epstein R, Tzischinsky O, et al. Actigraphic measurements of sleep in rheumatoid arthri-
tis: comparison of patients with low back pain and healthy controls. J Rheumatol 1992; 19(3):362–
365.
48. Mahowald MW, Mahowald ML, Bundlie SR, et al. Sleep fragmentation in rheumatoid arthritis.
Arthritis Rheum 1989; 32(8):974–983.
49. Crosby LJ. Factors which contribute to fatigue associated with rheumatoid arthritis. J Adv Nurs 1991;
16(8):974–981.
50. Lavie P, Nahir M, Lorber M, et al. Nonsteroidal antiinflammatory drug therapy in rheumatoid
arthritis patients. Lack of association between clinical improvement and effects on sleep. Arthritis
Rheum 1991; 34(6):655–659.
51. Prado GF, Allen RP, Trevisani VM, et al. Sleep disruption in systemic sclerosis (scleroderma) patients:
clinical and polysomnographic findings. Sleep Med 2002; 3(4):341–345.
52. Turner GA, Lower EE, Corser BC, et al. Sleep apnea in sarcoidosis. Sarcoidosis Vasc Diffuse Lung Dis
1997; 14(1):61–64.
53. Verbraecken J, Hoitsma E, van der Grinten CP, et al. Sleep disturbances associated with periodic leg
movements in chronic sarcoidosis. Sarcoidosis Vasc Diffuse Lung Dis 2004; 21(2):137–146.
54. Haack M, Sanchez E, Mullington JM. Elevated inflammatory markers in response to prolonged
sleep restriction are associated with increased pain experience in healthy volunteers. Sleep 2007;
30(9):1145–1152.
55. Management of temporomandibular joint disorders. National Institutes of Health Technology Assess-
ment Conference Statement. J Am Dent Assoc 1996; 127(11):1595–1606.
56. Diatchenko L, Nackley AG, Slade GD, et al. Idiopathic pain disorders—pathways of vulnerability.
Pain 2006; 123(3):226–230.
57. Yunus MB. Fibromyalgia and overlapping disorders: the unifying concept of central sensitivity
syndromes. Semin Arthritis Rheum 2007; 36(6):339–356.
58. Aaron LA, Burke MM, Buchwald D. Overlapping conditions among patients with chronic fatigue
syndrome, fibromyalgia, and temporomandibular disorder. Arch Intern Med 2000; 160(2):221–
227.
59. Hudson JI, Goldenberg DL, Pope HG Jr.,et al. Comorbidity of fibromyalgia with medical and psychi-
atric disorders. Am J Med 1992; 92(4):363–367.
60. Moldofsky H. Sleep and fibrositis syndrome. Rheum Dis Clin North Am 1989; 15(1):91–103.
61. Horne J, Shackell B. Alpha-like EEG activity in non-REM sleep and the fibromyalgia (fibrositis)
syndrome. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1991; 79:271–276.
62. Branco J, Atalaia A, Paiva T. Sleep cycles and alpha-delta sleep in fibromyalgia syndrome. J Rheumatol
1994; 21(6):1113–1117.
63. Jennum P, Drewes AM, Andreasen A, et al. Sleep and other symptoms in primary fibromyalgia and
in healthy controls. J Rheumatol 1993; 20(10):1756–1759.
64. Drewes AM. Pain and sleep disturbances with special reference to fibromyalgia and rheumatoid
arthritis. Rheumatology (Oxford) 1999; 38(11):1035–1038.
65. Shaver JL, Lentz M, Landis CA, et al. Sleep, psychological distress, and stress arousal in women with
fibromyalgia. Res Nurs Health 1997; 20(3):247–257.
66. Delgado JA, Murali G, Goldberg R. Sleep disorders in fibromyalgia. Sleep 2004; 27:A339.
67. Khan SA, Goldberg R, Haber A. Sleep disorders in fibromyalgia. Sleep 2005; 28:A290.
68. Roizenblatt S, Moldofsky H, Benedito-Silva AA, et al. Alpha sleep characteristics in fibromyalgia.
Arthritis Rheum 2001; 44(1):222–230.
69a. Wickwire EM, Kronfli T, Bellinger K, et al. The relations between objective sleep data, sleep disorders,
and signs and symptoms of Temporomandibular Joint Disorder (TMD). Journal of Pain 2008; 9(Suppl
2):P14.
69b. Smith MT, Wickwire EM, Grace EG, et al. Sleep disorders and their association with laboratory pain
sensitivity in temporomandibular joint disorder. Sleep 2009; 32(6):779–790.
IHBK059-14 IHBK059-Sateia March 31, 2010 16:30 Char Count=
70. Edwards RR, Grace E, Peterson S, et al. Sleep continuity and architecture associations with pain-
inhibitory processes in patients with temporomandibular joint disorder. Eur J Pain 2009; 13(10):1043–
1047.
71. Bradley LA, McKendree-Smith NL. Central nervous system mechanisms of pain in fibromyalgia
and other musculoskeletal disorders: behavioral and psychologic treatment approaches. Curr Opin
Rheumatol 2002; 14(1):45–51.
72. Rains JC, Poceta JS, Penzien DB. Sleep and headaches. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep 2008; 8(2):167–175.
73. Rains JC, Poceta JS. Headache and sleep disorders: review and clinical implications for headache
management. Headache 2006; 46(9):1344–1363.
74. Boardman HF, Thomas E, Millson DS, Croft PR. Psychological, sleep, lifestyle, and comorbid associ-
ations with headache. Headache 2005; 45(6):657–669.
75. Ohayon MM. Prevalence and risk factors of morning headaches in the general population. Arch
Intern Med 2004; 164(1):97–102.
76. Rasmussen BK. Migraine and tension-type headache in a general population: precipitating factors,
female hormones, sleep pattern and relation to lifestyle. Pain 1993; 53(1):65–72.
77. Lyngberg AC, Rasmussen BK, Jorgensen T, et al. Incidence of primary headache: a Danish epidemi-
ologic follow-up study. Am J Epidemiol 2005; 161(11):1066–1073.
78. Jennum P, Jensen R. Sleep and headache. Sleep Med Rev 2002; 6(6):471–479.
79. Argoff CE. The coexistence of neuropathic pain, sleep, and psychiatric disorders: a novel treatment
approach. Clin J Pain 2007; 23(1):15–22.
80. Zelman DC, Brandenburg NA, Gore M. Sleep impairment in patients with painful diabetic peripheral
neuropathy. Clin J Pain 2006; 22(8):681–685.
81. Mainous AG III, Baker R, Koopman RJ, et al. Impact of the population at risk of diabetes on pro-
jections of diabetes burden in the United States: an epidemic on the way. Diabetologia 2007; 50(5):
934–940.
82. Drewes AM, Andreasen A, Jennum P, et al. Zopiclone in the treatment of sleep abnormalities in
fibromyalgia. Scand J Rheumatol 1991; 20(4):288–293.
83. Moldofsky H, Lue FA, Mously C, et al. The effect of zolpidem in patients with fibromyalgia: a
dose ranging, double blind, placebo controlled, modified crossover study. J Rheumatol 1996; 23(3):
529–533.
84. Arnold LM, Keck PE Jr, Welge JA. Antidepressant treatment of fibromyalgia. A meta-analysis and
review. Psychosomatics 2000; 41(2):104–113.
85. O’Malley PG, Balden E, Tomkins G, et al. Treatment of fibromyalgia with antidepressants: a meta-
analysis. J Gen Intern Med 2000; 15(9):659–666.
86. Fishbain DA, Hall J, Meyers AL, et al. Does pain mediate the pain interference with sleep problem in
chronic pain? Findings from studies for management of diabetic peripheral neuropathic pain with
duloxetine. J Pain Symptom Manage 2008; 36(6):639–647.
87. Gordh TE, Stubhaug A, Jensen TS, et al. Gabapentin in traumatic nerve injury pain: a randomized,
double-blind, placebo-controlled, cross-over, multi-center study. Pain 2008; 138(2):255–266.
88. Tölle T, Freynhagen R, Versavel M, et al. Pregabalin for relief of neuropathic pain associated with
diabetic neuropathy: a randomized, double-blind study. Eur J Pain 2008; 12(2):203–213.
89. Arnold LM, Russell IJ, Diri EW, et al. A 14-week, randomized, double-blinded, placebo-controlled
monotherapy trial of pregabalin in patients with fibromyalgia. J Pain 2008; 9(9):792–805.
90. Florete OG, Xiang J, Vorsanger GJ. Effects of extended-release tramadol on pain-related sleep param-
eters in patients with osteoarthritis. Expert Opin Pharmacother 2008; 9(11):1817–1827.
91. Kivitz A, Ma C, Ahdieh H, et al. A 2-week, multicenter, randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled,
dose-ranging, phase III trial comparing the efficacy of oxymorphone extended release and placebo
in adults with pain associated with osteoarthritis of the hip or knee. Clin Ther 2006; 28(3):352–364.
92. Astin JA, Beckner W, Soeken K, et al. Psychological interventions for rheumatoid arthritis: a meta-
analysis of randomized controlled trials. Arthritis Rheum 2002; 47(3):291–302.
93. Morley S, Eccleston C, Williams A. Systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled
trials of cognitive behaviour therapy and behaviour therapy for chronic pain in adults, excluding
headache. Pain 1999; 80(1–2):1–13.
94. Wilson KG, Eriksson MY, D’Eon JL, et al. Major depression and insomnia in chronic pain. Clin J Pain
2002; 18(2):77–83.
95. Basler HD, Rehfisch HP. Cognitive-behavioral therapy in patients with ankylosing spondylitis in a
German self-help organization. J Psychosom Res 1991; 35(2–3):345–354.
96. Singh BB, Berman BM, Hadhazy VA, et al. A pilot study of cognitive behavioral therapy in fibromyal-
gia. Altern Ther Health Med 1998; 4(2):67–70.
97. Thieme K, Gromnica-Ihle E, Flor H. Operant behavioral treatment of fibromyalgia: a controlled study.
Arthritis Rheum 2003; 49(3):314–320.
IHBK059-14 IHBK059-Sateia March 31, 2010 16:30 Char Count=
98. Thieme K, Flor H, Turk DC. Psychological pain treatment in fibromyalgia syndrome: efficacy of
operant behavioural and cognitive behavioural treatments. Arthritis Res Ther 2006; 8(4):R121.
99. Thieme K, Hauser W, Batra A, et al. Psychotherapy in patients with fibromyalgia syndrome. Schmerz
2008; 22(3):295–302.
100. Dalton JA, Keefe FJ, Carlson J, et al. Tailoring cognitive-behavioral treatment for cancer pain. Pain
Manag Nurs 2004; 5(1):3–18.
101. Appelbaum K, Blanchard E, Hickling E, et al. Cognitive behavioral treatment of a veteran population
with moderate to severe rheumatoid arthritis. Behav Ther 1988; 19:489–502.
102. O’Leary JF, Mallory TH, Kraus TJ, et al. Mittelmeier ceramic total hip arthroplasty. A retrospective
study. J Arthroplasty 1988; 3(1):87–96.
103. Kashikar-Zuck S, Swain NF, Jones BA, et al. Efficacy of cognitive-behavioral intervention for juvenile
primary fibromyalgia syndrome. J Rheumatol 2005; 32(8):1594–1602.
104. Redondo JR, Justo CM, Moraleda FV, et al. Long-term efficacy of therapy in patients with fibromyal-
gia: a physical exercise-based program and a cognitive-behavioral approach. Arthritis Rheum 2004;
51(2):184–192.
105. Wigers SH, Stiles TC, Vogel PA. Effects of aerobic exercise versus stress management treatment in
fibromyalgia. A 4.5 year prospective study. Scand J Rheumatol 1996; 25(2):77–86.
106. Smith MT, Perlis ML, Carmody TP, et al. Presleep cognitions in patients with insomnia secondary to
chronic pain. J Behav Med 2001; 24(1):93–114.
107. Morin CM, Kowatch RA, Wade JB. Behavioral management of sleep disturbances secondary to
chronic pain. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 1989; 20(4):295–302.
108. Morin CM, Kowatch RA, O’Shanick G. Sleep restriction for the inpatient treatment of insomnia. Sleep
1990; 13(2):183–186.
109. Cannici J, Malcolm R, Peek LA. Treatment of insomnia in cancer patients using muscle relaxation
training. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 1983; 14(3):251–256.
110. Lichstein KL, Wilson NM, Johnson CT. Psychological treatment of secondary insomnia. Psychol
Aging 2000; 15(2):232–240.
111. Perlis ML, Sharpe M, Smith MT, et al. Behavioral treatment of insomnia: treatment outcome and the
relevance of medical and psychiatric morbidity. J Behav Med 2001; 24(3):281–296.
112. Rybarczyk B, Lopez M, Benson R, et al. Efficacy of two behavioral treatment programs for comorbid
geriatric insomnia. Psychol Aging 2004; 17(2):288–298.
113. Rybarczyk B, Stepanski E, Fogg L, et al. A placebo-controlled test of cognitive-behavioral therapy for
comorbid insomnia in older adults. J Consult Clin Psychol 2005; 73(6):1164–1174.
114. Calhoun AH, Ford S. Behavioral sleep modification may revert transformed migraine to episodic
migraine. Headache 2007; 47(8):1178–1183.
115. Currie SR, Wilson KG, Pontefract AJ, et al. Cognitive-behavioral treatment of insomnia secondary to
chronic pain. J Consult Clin Psychol 2000; 68(3):407–416.
116. Edinger JD, Wohlgemuth WK, Krystal AD, et al. Behavioral insomnia therapy for fibromyalgia
patients: a randomized clinical trial. Arch Intern Med 2005; 165(21):2527–2535.
117. Edinger JD, Wohlgemuth WK, Radtke RA, et al. Cognitive behavioral therapy for treatment of chronic
primary insomnia: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA 2001; 285(14):1856–1864.
118. Horowitz SH. The diagnostic workup of patients with neuropathic pain. Med Clin North Am 2007;
91(1):21–30.
119. Peat G, Croft P, Hay E. Clinical assessment of the osteoarthritis patient. Best Pract Res Clin Rheumatol
2001; 15(4):527–544.
120. Smith MT, Edwards RR, Robinson RC, et al. Suicidal ideation, plans, and attempts in chronic pain
patients: factors associated with increased risk. Pain 2004; 111(1–2):201–208.
121. Smith MT, Perlis ML, Smith MS, et al. Sleep onset disturbance discriminates suicidal ideation in
patients with chronic pain. Sleep 2000; 23(suppl #1).
122. Rollnick S, Miller WR, Butler CC, et al. Motivational Interviewing in Health Care: Helping Patients
Change Behavior. COPD 2008; 5(3):203.
123. Wickwire EM, Saletin J, Hoehn J, et al. Performance of actigraphy in temporomandibular joint
disorder. Sleep 2009; 32(Abstract Suppl):A340.
c15 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:34 Char Count=
Medical and neurological disorders frequently have a significant impact on sleep, affecting
overall well-being and quality of life. Further, the lack of restful or sufficient sleep may exacerbate
the underlying disease itself. Numerous investigators have demonstrated increased prevalence
of insomnia in subjects with somatic diseases (1–7). Furthermore, a National Sleep Foundation
survey found that participants’ perception of their sleep quality was highly associated with their
number of medical conditions (6). Although prevalence rates are variable, there is consensus that
rates of chronic insomnia are higher in clinical populations than in the general population. While
numerous disease processes may be associated with insomnia and poor sleep, some medical and
neurological disorders have more consistent associations with sleep-related complaints. Chronic
pain syndromes are also highly associated with insomnia and sleep disturbance, although this
is not included in this chapter as it is discussed more fully in chapter 14.
MEDICAL DISORDERS
Medical treatment of insomnia in patients with COPD is also an option, although again
with some special considerations. Benzodiazepines are commonly prescribed for insomnia,
although they may pose additional risk in patients with COPD. A large number of studies
(18) have reviewed the effects of the benzodiazepines on respiratory function in both normal
individuals as well as those with COPD, with variable results. Most, though not all, studies
demonstrated detrimental effects on a variety of respiratory parameters in patients with and
without lung disease. Studies have been performed with the non-benzodiazepine benzodi-
azepine receptor agonist (BzRA) zolpidem (19–21), suggesting that there is not a significant
impairment in respiratory parameters when administered to patients with COPD, although
studies are limited in number and results must be interpreted cautiously. It remains reasonable
to recommend that these medications may be used with caution in patients with underlying
respiratory abnormalities, although the non-benzodiazepine BzRAs may be a better choice than
standard benzodiazepines in this population. Recent studies have demonstrated the safety of
ramelteon in patients with mild to moderate as well as severe COPD (22,23), although there
is little information on the use of other designated sedative hypnotics in this specific group of
patients.
Diabetes Mellitus
Sleep disturbances are common among individuals with diabetes due to a number of factors.
There is a strong association between impaired glucose tolerance, insulin resistance, obesity, and
sleep disordered breathing (24,25), increasing the likelihood of insomnia complaints related to
nocturnal respiratory disturbances. Beyond this, however, are other factors that lead to increased
insomnia complaints in diabetics and higher rates of hypnotic use (6,26,27).
Rapid changes in glucose levels during sleep have been postulated to cause awakenings
in type 1 diabetics (28). Poor sleep in type 2 diabetics has been associated with poor glycemic
control, with an inverse correlation between hemoglobin A1C and sleep efficiency (29). Thus,
maintaining more constant levels of glucose control may help improve sleep quality in diabetic
individuals.
Leg discomfort may also contribute to disrupted sleep in diabetics. Individuals with dia-
betes have not been found to have an increased risk for developing restless legs syndrome
(26,30). Diabetic peripheral neuropathy with associated pain and paresthesias can lead to dis-
turbed sleep (27,31) and treatments aimed at alleviating these symptoms may be helpful in
improving sleep.
when obtaining a history, and consider polysomnography even if the patient does not report
typical symptoms of sleep disordered breathing, as an occult respiratory or motor disorder may
be contributing to insomnia symptoms.
Treatment of insomnia related to CKD is complex, given the multiple factors that may
contribute to the symptoms. Further, extra care and caution has to be exercised when treating
insomnia in this group of patients, given the potential altered metabolism of medications and
potential interactions with the numerous medications often used in this population. Surprisingly
little work has been done evaluating the use of sedative hypnotics in patients with CKD, despite
their high prevalence of insomnia complaints. One analysis identified a 15% higher mortality rate
in dialysis patients using benzodiazepines or zolpidem (44), although this did not further discern
between the two medication classes or identify specific risks associated with these medications.
The non-benzodiazepines, zaleplon, zolpidem, and zopiclone, are increasingly used to treat
insomnia in the general population. As their primary mode of metabolism is hepatic and renal
function does not contribute significantly to their excretion, these medications may be safer to
use in patients with kidney disease. Both zolpidem (45) and zaleplon (46) have been evaluated in
clinical studies involving patients with CKD and were found to be safe and effective; however,
these were both small studies and sedative hypnotics should be used with caution given the
numerous potential confounding factors in this population. Nonpharmacological interventions
may also be appropriate and a recent study by Chen et al. (47) did demonstrate improvements
in subjective sleep quality in a small group of peritoneal dialysis patients who underwent
cognitive-behavioral therapy for insomnia.
association should not be surprising. Stage of illness has not necessarily correlated with insom-
nia symptoms. Asymptomatic seropositive patients with depression have been found to have
a high likelihood for insomnia in clinical studies (52,58). Conversely, cognitive impairment
due to Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndrome-related central nervous system involvement
associated with more advanced disease is also highly predictive of insomnia (48).
Treatment of insomnia in this population may thus provide further challenges, given the
multiple possible contributors to the sleep symptoms.
Malignancy
Sleep disturbance constitutes a significant source of suffering in patients with cancer, although
prevalence rates vary depending on the type of cancer and method of assessing symptoms. Much
of the work investigating insomnia and cancer has been with breast cancer patients. In this group,
using standardized criteria as defined by Savard and Morin (59) insomnia rates were found to
be double that of the general population. As with the general population, sleep disturbances
can significantly impact quality of life in cancer patients. Women with metastatic breast cancer
rated sleep in the highest quartile of quality of life items (59) and patients undergoing radiation
therapy ranked sleep disturbance as one of the 10 most troubling difficulties of their illness
(60). However, despite the distress associated with insomnia, many patients do not report the
problem to their health care providers, perhaps assuming it is an inevitable aspect of their
illness or that nothing can be done for the symptoms. In fact, one study found that almost 85%
of cancer patients with sleep disturbance did not discuss the problem with their provider (61).
The same study also noted that approximately half of the patients experienced their symptoms
on a nightly basis.
Although the nature of the sleep disturbance may be as heterogeneous as the underlying
illness, frequent awakenings appear to be the most commonly reported disturbance, described
in multiple studies (62–66). In Davidson’s survey (66), 52% of patients attributed their insomnia
to “intrusive thoughts” and 45% attributed their sleep disturbance to physical discomfort.
Although there is limited objective evaluation of sleep in cancer patients, the studies available
would generally concur with subjective reports demonstrating decreased sleep efficiency and
increased awakenings on polysomnography (67) as well as actigraphy (68,69).
As with many of the other illnesses discussed in this chapter, the etiology of insom-
nia related to cancer is likely multifactorial. Physical symptoms, psychological distress, and
medications may all play a role in the sleep disturbance and may be targets for treatment.
Cancer-related fatigue is also a highly prevalent and persistent problem in patients with cancer,
as well as cancer survivors. Roscoe et al. (70) provides a detailed description of the interrela-
tionship between fatigue and sleep disturbance in cancer patients. The relationship between the
two may be bidirectional. The fatigue may be due at least in part to the poor sleep, although
other factors such as cytokines associated with tumors have also been reported as possible con-
tributors to cancer-related fatigue. However, the fatigue may cause patients to extend their sleep
opportunity, spending excessive time in bed with subsequent reductions in sleep efficiency and
worsening feelings of poor sleep. Thus, measures aimed at improving daytime fatigue may also
help with the nocturnal disturbance.
Treatment of insomnia in cancer has typically focused on pharmacologic agents, although
there have been a number of studies demonstrating the effectiveness of cognitive-behavioral
therapy for insomnia (CBT-I). This is particularly important, given that insomnia symptoms can
persist for several years after the end of treatment. More recent studies (71,72) in particular have
modified CBT-I to meet the special needs of cancer patients, more specifically using strategies
to help cope with fatigue (such as encouraging physical activity) and employing education and
cognitive restructuring to address the fatigue.
NEUROLOGICAL DISORDERS
Neurodegenerative Diseases
As the pathophysiology of neurodegenerative diseases becomes better understood there has
been a shift in classification. Traditional classification systems were based on clinical symptom
complexes. More recently many neurodegenerative diseases can be classified as tauopathies or
c15 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:34 Char Count=
Tauopathies
The prototypic tauopathy is Alzheimer’s disease (AD). AD is the most common form of demen-
tia. Short-term memory loss, the hallmark of the disease, tends to be progressive. Other areas
of impairment include executive dysfunction, language, abstraction, and mood. The classic
pathologic findings in this disorder include extracellular plaques of the peptide -amyloid and
intracellular neurofibrillary tangles composed of the protein tau. The most prominent biochem-
ical change is the loss of choline acetyltransferase activity. This enzyme is responsible for the
production of acetylcholine in cholinergic neurons.
Patients with AD exhibit abnormal sleep architecture on polysomnography. These changes
generally reflect lower quality sleep with a reduction in sleep efficiency and total sleep time and
an elevation in stage one sleep along with an increased number of arousals and awakenings (73).
The formed elements of sleep, K-complexes, and sleep spindles may also be reduced. Late in
the disease, a decrease in REM sleep percentage and a prolongation of the latency to REM sleep
develop (73). Cholinesterase inhibitors used to treat dementia have been shown to increase REM
sleep in nondemented patients and may have the same effect in AD patients. Reports of vivid
dreaming in AD patients on cholinesterase inhibitor support this notion (74). The cholinesterase
inhibitor, donepezil, may lead to insomnia whereas this does not appear to be the case with
rivastagmine and galantamine (74).
Degeneration of neurons in the suprachiasmatic nucleus and decreased melatonin secre-
tion may lead to circadian rhythm disruption in AD (75). Circadian rhythm abnormalities
characteristic of this disorder include daytime sleepiness and nocturnal wakefulness (75).
Patients with dementia may develop the irregular sleep–wake type circadian sleep disorder
(76). This disorder is characterized by a lack of clearly defined sleep and wake periods with
at least three sleep episodes in a 24-hour period. The severity of dementia has been correlated
with the severity of the circadian rhythm disturbance (77). Other factors such as medications
that lead to confusion, lack of environmental cues (e.g., limited light exposure), other medical
conditions (e.g., pain), psychiatric conditions (e.g., depression), and inadequate sleep hygiene
(e.g., time spent in bed watching television) may exacerbate or mimic the circadian rhythm
disruption. Use of sleep logs and actigraphy can help provide diagnostic clues. This disrup-
tion of the circadian pattern of sleep and wake is particularly important, as the majority of
caregivers point to nocturnal problems as a factor in their decision to institutionalize elderly
relatives (78).
Sleep disordered breathing may lead to an insomnia complaint. An association between
AD and obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) has been demonstrated. The APOE4 allele commonly
associated with AD has also been associated with OSA (79).
When considering therapies for insomnia in AD patients one must first consider the etiol-
ogy. Addressing the underlying problem driving the insomnia may then lead to improvement
in the symptom of insomnia. For example, a delirium secondary to a medical condition or
medication could be the driving force behind the insomnia in an AD patient (74). A primary
sleep disorder such as RLS or OSA should be addressed directly if possible before addressing
the symptom of insomnia. Assuming these conditions have been met, symptom management
of insomnia can be undertaken. Information on the use of sedative hypnotics in AD patients
is sparse. There are limited data to suggest that the short acting benzodiazepine, triazolam,
and the BzRA, zolpidem, may help with insomnia in AD patients (74). Concerns, however,
about adverse events such as unsteadiness, falls, and worsening of cognitive impairment have
limited their usage. A meta-analysis of hypnotic use in people aged 60 or over concluded
there were small improvements in the sleep quality but an increased risk of adverse events
(80). Antipsychotic medications have been used off-label as hypnotics, especially if nocturnal
c15 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:34 Char Count=
agitation is also present. The safety of these medications for this purpose, however, remains
largely unexplored with some evidence to suggest increased cerebrovascular adverse events
(81,82), cognitive decline (83), and mortality (84) in patients with dementia. Because of the
reduction in melatonin secretion that is greater than expected for age in AD, melatonin supple-
mentation has been attempted as a therapy (85). Studies evaluating melatonin replacement’s
effect on sleep in elderly and AD patients have been mixed (85). In 2003, a multicenter, ran-
domized, double-blind, placebo-controlled clinical trial suggested no benefit of melatonin up
to 10 mg on actigraphically derived measure of sleep in an AD population (86). This may be
explained by the observation that the number of melatonin-1 receptor containing neurons in the
suprachiasmatic nucleus in patients with AD is reduced (85). The data on the use of ramelteon, a
melatonin-1 and 2 receptor agonist, is limited (85). Phototherapy may be helpful in the manage-
ment of the circadian disruption in patients with AD but the specifics on the timing, duration,
and intensity have yet to be determined (75). Some success has been achieved through a combi-
nation of environmental and behavioral changes in nursing home residents. Such changes have
included increased daytime bright light exposure, avoidance of daytime time in bed, structured
bedtime routines, and increased physical activity as well as sleep hygiene education for the
caregiver (87,88).
Progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP), another tauopathy, is a disorder characterized by
parkinsonism, dystonia, gait disturbance, and a supranuclear gaze palsy (impaired voluntary
vertical eye movements initially). Insomnia is common in this disorder and tends to be more
severe than the insomnia in AD or PD (89,90). Insomnia in PSP disorder correlates most closely
with motor impairment and to a lesser degree with cognitive or eye movement impairment
(90,91). Polysomnographic findings demonstrate a reduced sleep efficiency of 58% in one study
(90). The etiology of the insomnia complaint may be a direct result of brain stem pathology,
immobility, depression, dysphagia, and/or nocturia (90,92). Sleep-related breathing disorders
and REM sleep behavior disorder are probably not common in PSP but data are limited (89,90).
Besides the reduced sleep efficiency, other polysomnographic findings in PSP include reduced or
absent eye movements, poorly formed or absent sleep spindles and K-complexes, and increased
alpha activity in stage 1 and 2 sleep (90). Corticobasal degeneration, another tauopathy, has been
associated with periodic limb movements and REM sleep behavior disorder in case reports only
(89). Little is known about sleep in this rare disorder.
Synucleinopathies
Parkinson’s disease is characterized by a resting tremor, bradykinesia, masked fascies, loss of
postural reflexes, and an increased incidence of depression. The hallmark pathologic finding is
loss of dopaminergic neurons in the substantia nigra in the brainstem. Sleep complaints and
problems are multiple in this disorder and are more common with more severe disease (93).
The frequency of reported sleep problems in PD varies among studies from 25% to 98% (93).
RBD is seen in one-third of newly diagnosed patients with PD (76). Hypersomnia, whether it
is from the disease process itself, the dopaminergic agents used to treat PD or coexistent sleep
disorders known to cause excessive sleepiness, is common in PD (94–96). The frequency of the
RLS and OSA appears to be elevated in PD as well and may lead to insomnia and/or daytime
sleepiness (94).
While the sleep complaints are multiple in patients with PD the two most problematic
appear to be sleep maintenance insomnia and nocturia (93). Almost two-thirds of PD patients
complain of sleep onset difficulties but almost 90% may complain about sleep maintenance
issues (97). Akathisia, dystonia, freezing, tremor, nocturia, muscle cramps, and off period-
related urinary incontinence and pain can all play a role in insomnia in PD (94). Some of the
medications used to treat PD, such as selegiline or the dopamine agonists, can be alerting
and contribute to insomnia. The depression associated with PD may also be associated with
insomnia complaints (96).
Polysomnographic findings in patients with PD include decreased sleep efficiency,
increased wake after sleep onset, sleep fragmentation, decreased SWS, decreased REM sleep,
decreased sleep spindles, and increased EMG activity. Findings consistent with RBD such as
REM sleep without atonia may also be present (97). Periodic limb movements consistent with
RLS as well as obstructive respiratory events consistent with OSA may also play a role in sleep
c15 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:34 Char Count=
Cerebrovascular Disease
Strokes have been associated with a number of disorders of sleep and wake. Insomnia com-
plaints, specifically, are common in the poststroke period. Approximately 2/3 of ischemic stroke
patients had insomnia early on and that insomnia persisted for at least 18 months in almost
half of the patients in one study (101). Damage to specific regions of the brain may lead to an
insomnia complaint. Inversion of the sleep–wake cycle with nocturnal agitation and daytime
hypersomnolence may occur in association with subcortical, thalamic, thalamomesencephalic,
and large tegmental pontine strokes (102). Two patients with locked-in syndromes had pro-
longed periods of polysomnographically confirmed insomnia lasting over one month. One had
a pontomesencephalic stroke and the other had a bilateral basal pontine stroke with extension to
the pontine tegmentum (102). Insomnia in the poststroke period may be related to depression.
Depression is a well-documented consequence of stroke (103). Just as in nonstroke patients,
sleep disordered breathing may lead to an insomnia complaint (102). Inadequate sleep hygiene
(i.e., extended periods in bed) secondary to a lack of mobility or independence may lead to
insomnia. Other causes of insomnia in the poststroke period include other medical disorders,
other sleep disorders, medications, inactivity, and environmental disturbances (104). Directly
treating any underlying disorder such as depression or pain that could be leading to insomnia
may also lead to improvement in the insomnia. No large-scale trials of therapy for insomnia
in stroke populations have been undertaken. If pharmacotherapy is used one must take into
account the respiratory suppressant effects as sleep-related breathing disorders are common in
poststroke populations (102).
c15 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:34 Char Count=
Multiple Sclerosis
Multiple sclerosis (MS) is a disorder characterized by central nervous system demyelination.
Difficulty initiating or maintaining sleep is a complaint in approximately 40% of MS patients
(105). This insomnia in MS may have multiple possible causes such as pain, nocturia, medica-
tions, depression, or classic sleep disorders such as RLS and periodic limb movement disorder
(106). Depression may be present in up to 50% of patients with MS and a symptom of that
depression may be insomnia (107). Pain is also very common in MS and may take different
forms such as neuropathic pain or pain related to muscle spasm. Nocturia or urinary inconti-
nence related to a spastic bladder is also very common in MS and may also disrupt sleep (106).
Immunomodulating medications such as interferon and steroids that are commonly used in MS
have been associated with insomnia (106). Addressing the underlying etiology of the insomnia
is the principal means of treatment in MS.
CONCLUSION
A wide range of medical and neurologic diseases have been associated with insomnia com-
plaints. The etiology of the insomnia in these conditions varies with the condition and is also
often multifactorial. The pathologic process of the disease itself may directly lead to the insomnia
complaint as may be the case in AD. Other symptoms or problems attributable to the condition
such as pain, impaired respiration or immobility may also lead to insomnia. In chronic diseases
in particular, the insomnia may be associated with a coexistent mood disorder, rather than
directly with the neurologic or medical disorder. Some of the medications used to treat medi-
cal and neurological disorders can also lead to insomnia, such as -agonist inhalers in COPD.
Primary sleep disorders, such as OSA or the RLS, among others, may occur more frequently in
patients with certain conditions. These primary sleep disorders may manifest as an insomnia
complaint. Therapy for insomnia in medical or neurologic disease is often highly dependent on
the etiology of the insomnia.
REFERENCES
1. Mellinger G, Balter M, Uhlenhuth E. Insomnia and its treatment: prevalence and correlates. Arch
Gen Psychiatry 1985; 42(3):225–232.
2. Gislason T, Almqvist M. Somatic diseases and sleep complaints. Acta Med Scand 1987; 221(5):475–481.
3. Klink M, Quan S, Kaltenborn W, et al. Risk factors associated with complaints of insomnia in a general
adult population. Arch Intern Med 1992; 152(8):1634–1637.
4. Kuppermann M, Lubeck D, Mazonson, P, et al. Sleep problems and their correlates in a working
population. J Gen Intern Med 1995; 10(1):25–32.
c15 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:34 Char Count=
5. Katz D, McHorney C. Clinical correlates of insomnia in patients with chronic illness. Arch Intern
Med 1998; 158(10):1099–1106.
6. Foley D, Ancoli-Israel S, Britz P, et al. Sleep disturbances and chronic disease in older adults: results
of the 2003 National Sleep Foundation Sleep in America Survey. J Psychosom Res 2004; 56(5):497–502.
7. Taylor DJ, Mallory LJ, Lichstein KL, et al. Comorbidity of chronic insomnia with medical problems.
Sleep 2007; 30(2):213–218.
8. Fleetham J, West P, Mezon B, et al. Sleep, arousals and oxygen desaturation in chronic obstructive
pulmonary disease. Am Rev Respir Dis 1982; 126(3):429–433.
9. Klink M, Quan S. Prevalence of reported sleep disturbances in a general adult population and their
relationship to obstructive airway diseases. Chest 1987; 91(4):540–546.
10. Saaresranta T, Irjala K, Aittokallio T, et al. Sleep quality, daytime sleepiness and fasting insulin levels
in women with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Respir Med 2005; 99(7):856–863.
11. George CF, Bayliff CD. Management of insomnia in patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary
disease. Drugs 2003; 63(4):379–387.
12. van Manen JG, Bindels PJ, IJzermans CJ, et al. Prevalence of comorbidity in patients with a chronic
airway obstruction and controls over the age of 40. J Clin Epidemiol 2001; 54(3):287–293.
13. George CF. Perspectives on the management of insomnia in patients with chronic respiratory disor-
ders. Sleep 2000; 23(suppl 1):S31–S35.
14. Cormick W, Olsen LG, Hensley MJ, et al. Nocturnal hypoxemia and quality of sleep in patients with
chronic obstructive lung disease. Thorax 1986; 41(11):846–854.
15. Calverley PM, Brezinova V, Douglas NJ, et al. The effect of oxygenation on sleep quality on chronic
bronchitis and emphysema. Am Rev Respir Dis 1982; 126(2):206–210.
16. Martin RJ, Bartelson BLB, Smith P, et al. Effect of ipratropium bromide on oxygen saturation and
sleep quality in COPD. Chest 1999; 115(5):1338–1345.
17. Sanders MH, Newman AB, Haggerty CL, et al. Sleep and sleep-disordered breathing in adults with
predominantly mild obstructive airway disease. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 2003; 167(1):7–14.
18. Stege G, Vos PJE, van den Elshout FJJ,et al. Sleep, hypnotics and chronic obstructive pulmonary
disease. Respir Med 2008; 102(6):801–814.
19. Girault C, Muir JF, Mihaltan F, et al. Effect of repeated administration of zolpidem on sleep, diurnal
and nocturnal respiratory function, vigilance, and physical performance in patients with COPD.
Chest 1996; 110(5):1203–1211.
20. Steens RD, Pouliot Z, Millar TW, et al. Effect of zolpidem and triazolam on sleep and respiration in
mild to moderate chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Sleep 1993; 16(4):318–326.
21. Murciano D, Armengaud MH, Cramer PH, et al. Acute effects of zolpidem (Z), triazolam (T), and
flunitrazepam (F) on arterial blood gasses and control of breathing in severe COPD. Eur Respir J
1993; 6(5):625–629.
22. Kryger M, Roth T, Wang-Weigand S, et al. The effects of ramelteon on respiration during sleep in
subjects with moderate to severe chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Sleep Breath 2009; 13(1):
79–84.
23. Kryger M, Wang-Weigand S, Zhang J, et al. Effect of ramelteon, a selective MT(1)/MT(2) -
receptor agonist on respiration during sleep in mild to moderate COPD. Sleep Breath 2008; 12(3):
243–250.
24. Punjabi NM, Shahar E, Redline S, et al. Sleep disordered breathing, glucose intolerance, and insulin
resistance: the Sleep Heart Health Study. Am J Epidemiol 2004; 160(6):521–530.
25. Seicean S, Kirchner HL, Gottlieb DJ, et al. Sleep-disordered breathing and impaired glucose
metabolism in normal-weight and overweight/obese individuals: the Sleep Heart Health Study.
Diabetes Care 2008; 31(5):1001–1006.
26. Skomro RP, Ludwig S, Salamon E, et al. Sleep complaints and restless leg syndrome in adult type 2
diabetics. Sleep Med 2001; 2(5):417–422.
27. Sridhar GR, Madhu K. Prevalence of sleep disturbances in diabetes mellitus. Diabetes Res Clin Pract
1994; 23(3):183–186.
28. Pillar G, Schuscheim G, Weiss R, et al. Interactions between hypoglycemia and sleep architecture in
children with type 1 diabetes mellitus. J Pediatr 2003; 142(2):163–168.
29. Trento M, Broglio F, Riganti F, et al. Sleep abnormalities in type 2 diabetes may be associated with
glycemic control. Acta Diabetol 2008; 45(4):225–229.
30. Garcia-Borreguero D, Egatz R, Winkelmann J, et al. Epidemiology of restless legs syndrome: the
current status. Sleep Med Rev 2006; 10(3):153–167.
31. Schmader KE. Epidemiology and impact of quality of life of postherpetic neuralgia and painful
diabetic neuropathy. Clin J Pain 2002; 18(6):350–354.
32. Gusbeth-Tatomir P, Boisteanu D, Seica A, et al. Sleep disorders: a systematic review of an emerging
major clinical issue in renal patients. Int Urol Nephrol 2007; 39:1217–1226.
c15 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:34 Char Count=
33. Iliescu EA, Yeates KE, Holland DC. Quality of sleep in patients with chronic kidney disease. Nephrol
Dial Transplant 2004; 19(1):95–99.
34. De Santo RM, Bartiromo M, Cesare CM, et al. Sleep disorders occur very early in chronic kidney
disease. J Nephrol 2008; 21(suppl 13):S59–S65.
35. Merlino G, Gigli GL, Valente M. Sleep disturbances in dialysis patients. J Nephrol 2008; 21(suppl
13):S66–S70.
36. Bastos JPC, de Sousa RB, Nepumuceno LA, et al. Sleep disturbances in patients on maintenance
hemodialysis: role of dialysis shift. Rev Assoc Med Bras 2007; 53(6):492–496.
37. Novak M, Molnar MZ, Ambrus C, et al. Chronic insomnia in kidney transplant recipients. Am J
Kidney Dis 2006; 47(4):655–665.
38. Sabbatini M, Pisani A, Crispo A, et al. Renal transplantation and sleep: a new life is not enough. J
Nephrol 2008; 21(suppl 13):S97–S101.
39. Molnar MZ, Novak M, Szeifert L, et al. Restless legs syndrome, insomnia and quality of life after
renal transplantation. J Psychosom Res 2007; 63(6):591–597.
40. Kraus MA, Hamburger RJ. Sleep apnea in renal failure. Adv Perit Dial 1997; 13:88–92.
41. Zoccali C, Mallamaci F, Tripepi G, et al. Autonomic neuropathy is linked to nocturnal hypoxaemia
and to concentric hypertrophy and remodeling in dialysis patients. Nephrol Dial Transplant 2001;
16(1):70–77.
42. Jean G, Piperno D, Francois B, et al. Sleep apnea incidence in maintenance hemodialysis patients:
influence of dialysate buffer. Nephron 1995; 71(2):138–142.
43. Parker KP, Bliwise DL. Clinical comparison of hemodialysis and sleep apnea patients with excessive
daytime sleepiness. ANNA J 1997; 24(6):663–665.
44. Winkelmayer WC, Mehta J, Wang PS. Benzodiazepine use and mortality of incident dialysis patients
in the United States. Kidney Int 2007; 72(11):1388–1393.
45. Fillastre JP, Geffroy-Josse S, Etienne I, et al. Pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of zolpidem
following repeated doses in hemodialyzed uraemic patients. Fundam Clin Pharmacol 1993; 7(1):1–9.
46. Sabbatini M, Crispo A, Pisani A, et al. Zaleplon improves sleep quality in maintenance hemodialysis
patients. Nephron Clin Pract 2003; 94(4):c99–c103.
47. Chen H, Chiang C, Wang H, et al. Cognitive behavioral therapy for sleep disturbance in patients
undergoing peritoneal dialysis: a pilot randomized control trial. Am J Kidney Dis 2008; 52(2):
314–323.
48. Rubinstein ML, Selwyn PA. High prevalence of insomnia in an outpatient population with HIV
infection. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr Hum Retrovirol 1998; 19(3):260–265.
49. Bryant PA, Trinder J, Curtis N. Does sleep have a vital role in the immune system? Nat Rev Immunol
2004; 4(6):457–467.
50. Irwin M. Effects of sleep and sleep loss on immunity and cytokines. Brain Behav Immun 2002;
16(5):503–512.
51. Majde JA, Krueger JM. Links between the innate immune system and sleep. J Allergy Clin Immunol
2005; 116(6):1188–1198.
52. Reid S, Dwyer J. Insomnia in HIV infection: a systematic review of prevalence, correlates and man-
agement. Psychosom Med 2005; 67(2):260–269.
53. Freda PU, Bilezikian JP. The hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis in HIV disease. AIDS Read 1999;
9(1):46–47.
54. Silverman MN, Pearce BD, Biron CA, et al. Immune modulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-
adrenal (HPA) axis during viral infection. Viral Immunol 2005; 18(1):41–78.
55. Zapanti E, Terzidis K, Chrousos G. Dysfunction of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in HIV
infection and disease. Hormones 2008; 7(3):205–216.
56. Nuñez M, de Requena DG, Gallego L, et al. Higher efavirenz plasma levels correlate with develop-
ment of insomnia. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr Hum Retrovirol 2001; 28(4):399–400.
57. Fumaz CR, Tuldra A, Ferrer MJ, et al. Quality of life, emotional status, and adherence of HIV-1 infected
patients treated with efavirenz versus protease inhibitor-containing regimens. J Acquir Immune Defic
Syndr Hum Retrovirol 2002; 29(3):244–253.
58. Perkins DO, Leserman J, Stern RA, et al. Somatic symptoms and HIV infection: relationship to
depressive symptoms and indicators of HIV disease. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152(12):1776–1781.
59. Savard J, Morin CM. Insomnia in the context of cancer: a review of a neglected problem. J Clin Oncol
2001; 19(3):895–908.
60. Sutherland HJ, Lockwood GA, Boyd NF. Ratings of the importance of quality of life variables:
therapeutic implications for patients with metastatic breast cancer. J Clin Epidemiol 1990; 43(7):661–
666.
61. Munro AJ, Biruls R, Griffin AV, et al. Distress associated with radiotherapy for malignant disease: a
quantitative analysis based on patient perceptions. Br J Cancer 1989; 60(3):370–374.
c15 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:34 Char Count=
62. Engstrom CA, Strohl RA, Rose L, et al. Sleep alterations in cancer patients. Cancer Nurs 1999;
22(2):143–148.
63. Kaye J, Kaye K, Madow L. Sleep patterns in patients with cancer and patients with cardiac disease. J
Psychol 1983; 114(1st half):107–113.
64. Koopman C, Nouriani B, Erickson V, et al. Sleep disturbances in women with metastatic breast cancer.
Breast J 2002; 8(6):362–370.
65. Savard J, Simard S, Blanchet J, et al. Prevalence, clinical characteristics and risk factors for insomnia
in the context of breast cancer. Sleep 2001; 24(5):583–590.
66. Davidson JR, MacLean AW, Brundage MD, et al. Sleep disturbance in cancer patients. Soc Sci Med
2002; 54(9):1309–1321.
67. Silberfarb PM, Hauri PJ, Oxman TE, et al. Assessment of sleep in patients with lung cancer and breast
cancer. J Clin Oncol 1993; 11(5):997–1004.
68. Ancoli-Israel S. The impact and prevalence of chronic insomnia and other sleep disorders associated
with chronic illness. Am J Manag Care 2006; 12(suppl 8):S221–S219.
69. Fernandes R, Stone P, Andrews P, et al. Comparison between fatigue, sleep disturbance and circadian
rhythm in cancer patients and healthy volunteers: evaluation of diagnostic criteria for cancer-related
fatigue. J Pain Symptom Manage 2006; 32(3):245–254.
70. Roscoe JA, Kaufman ME, Matteson-Rusby SE, et al. Cancer-related fatigue and sleep disorders.
Oncologist 2007; 12(suppl 1):35–42.
71. Quesnel C, Savard J, Simard S, et al. Efficacy of cognitive-behavioral therapy for insomnia in women
treated for non-metastatic breast cancer. J Consult Clin Psychol 2003; 71(1):189–200.
72. Savard J, Simard S, Ivers H, et al. Randomized study on the efficacy of cognitive-behavioral therapy
for insomnia secondary to breast cancer, part I: sleep and psychological effects. J Clin Oncol 2005;
23(25):6083–6096.
73. Avidan AY. Clinical neurology of insomnia in neurodegenerative and other disorders of neurological
function. Rev Neurol Dis 2007; 4(1):21–34.
74. Bliwise DL. Sleep disorders in Alzheimer’s disease and other dementias. Clin Cornerstone 2004;
6(suppl 1A):S16–S28.
75. Avidan AY. Sleep and neurologic problems in the elderly. Sleep Med Clin 2006; 1:273–292.
76. Sateia MJ, ed. The International Classification of Sleep Disorders. 2nd ed. Westchester, IL: American
Academy of Sleep Medicine, 2005.
77. Ancoli-Israel S, Klauber MR, Jones DW, et al. Variations in circadian rhythms of activity, sleep and
light exposure related to dementia in nursing-home patients. Sleep 1997; 20(1):18–23.
78. Pollak CP, Perlick D. Sleep problems and institutionalization of the elderly. J Geriatr Psychiatry
Neurol 1991; 4 (4):204–210.
79. Gottlieb DJ, Destefano AL, Foley DJ, et al. APOE E4 is associated with obstructive sleep
apnea/hypopnea, The Sleep Heart Health Study. Neurology 2004; 63:664–668.
80. Glass J, Lanctot KL, Herrmann N, et al. Sedative hypnotics in older people with insomnia: meta-
analysis of risks and benefits. BMJ 2005; 331(7526):1169.
81. Wooltorton E. Risperidone (Risperdal): increased rate of cerebrovascular events in dementia trials.
CMAJ 2002; 167:1269–1270.
82. Wooltorton E. Olanzapine (Zyprexa): increased incidence of cerebrovascular events in dementia
trials. CMAJ 2004; 170:1395.
83. McShane R, Keene J, Gedline K, et al. Do neuroleptic drugs hasten cognitive decline in dementia?
Prospective study with necropsy follow up. BMJ 1997; 314:266–270.
84. Singh S, Wooltorton E. Increased mortality among elderly patients with dementia using atypical
antipsychotics. CMAJ 2005; 173:252.
85. Wu YH, Swaab DF. Disturbance and strategies for reactivation of the circadian rhythm system in
aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Sleep Med 2007; 8:623–636.
86. Singer C, Tractenber RE, Kaye J, et al. A multicenter, placebo-controlled trial of melatonin for sleep
disturbance in Alzheimer’s disease. Sleep 2003; 26:893–901.
87. Alessi CA, Martin JL, Webber AP, et al. Randomized, controlled trial of a nonpharmacological inter-
vention to improve abnormal sleep/wake patterns in nursing home residents. J Am Geriatr Soc 2005;
53(5):803–810.
88. Martin JL, Marler MR, Harker JO, et al. A multicomponent nonpharmacological intervention
improves activity rhythms among nursing home resident with disrupted sleep/wake patterns. J
Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2007; 62(1):67–72.
89. Bhatt MH, Podder N, Chokroverty S. Sleep and neurodegenerative diseases. Semin Neurol 2005;
25(1):39–51.
90. Aldrich MS, Foster NL, White RF, et al. Sleep abnormalities in progressive supranuclear palsy. Ann
Neurol 1989; 25:577–581.
c15 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:34 Char Count=
91. Perrett JL, Jouvet M. Sleep study of progressive supranuclear paralysis. Electroencephalogr Clin
Neurophysiol 1980; 49:323–329.
92. DeBruin VS, Machado C, Howard RS, et al. Nocturnal and respiratory disturbances in Steele-
Richardson-Olszewski syndrome (progressive supranuclear palsy). Postgrad Med J 1996; 72:293–296.
93. Porter B, Macfarlane R, Walker R. The frequency and nature of sleep disorders in a community-based
population of patients with Parkinson’s disease. Eur J Neurol 2008; 15:50–54.
94. Dhawan V, Healy DG, Pal S, et al. Sleep-related problems of Parkinson’s disease. Age Ageing 2006;
35:220–228.
95. Chaudhuri KR, Martinez-Martin P, Schapira AH, et al. International multicenter pilot study of the
first comprehensive self-completed nonmotor symptoms questionnaire for Parkinson’s disease: the
NMSQuest study. Mov Disord 2006; 21(7):916–923.
96. Chaudhuri KR. Nocturnal symptom complex in PD and its management. Neurology 2003; 61(suppl
3):S17–S23.
97. Chokroverty S. Sleep and degenerative neurological disorders. Neurol Clin 1996; 14(4):807–826.
98. Dhawan V, Dhoat S, Williams AJ. The range and nature of sleep dysfunction in untreated Parkinson’s
disease (PD). A comparative controlled clinical study using the Parkinson’s disease sleep scale and
selective polysomnography. J Neurol Sci 2006; 248:158–162.
99. Hogl BE, Gomez-Arevalo G, Garcia S. A clinical, pharmacologic, and polysomnographic study of
sleep benefit in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology 1998; 50:1332–1339.
100. Provini F, Lombardi C, Lugaresi E. Insomnia in neurological disease. Semin Neurol 2005; 25(1):81–89.
101. Palomaki H, Berg A, Meririnne E. Complaints of poststroke insomnia and its treatment with
mianserin. Cerebrovasc Dis 2003; 15:56–62.
102. Bassetti CL. Sleep and Stroke. Semin Neurol 2005; 25(1):19–32.
103. Kotila M, Numminen H, Waltimo O, et al. Depression after stroke. Results for the FINNSTROKE
study. Stroke 1998; 29:368–372.
104. Bassetti CL. Sleep and stroke. In: Kryger MH, Roth MH, Dement WC, eds. Principles and Practice of
Sleep Medicine. 4th ed. Philadelphia: Elsevier Saunders, 2005:811–830.
105. Tachibana N, Howard RS, Hirsch NP, et al. Sleep problems in multiple sclerosis. Eur Neurol 1994;
34:320–323.
106. Fleming WE, Pollak CP. Sleep disorders in multiple sclerosis. Semin Neurol 2005; 25(1):64–68.
107. Sadovnick AD, Remick RA, Allen J, et al. Depression and multiple sclerosis. Neurology 1996; 46:628–
632.
108. Cohen M, Oksenberg A, Snir D, et al. Temporally related changes of sleep complaints in traumatic
brain injured patients. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1992; 55:313–315.
109. Verma A, Anand V, Verma NP. Sleep disorders in chronic traumatic brain injury. J Clin Sleep Med
2007; 3(4):357–362.
110. Ouellet MC, Beaulieu-Bonneau S, Morin CM. Insomnia in patients with traumatic brain injury. J
Head Trauma Rehabil 2006; 21(3):199–212.
111. Baumann CR, Werth E, Stocker R, et al. Sleep-wake disturbances 6 months after traumatic brain
injury: a prospective study. Brain 2007; 130:1873–1883.
112. Ayalon L, Borodkin MA, Dishon L, et al. Circadian rhythm sleep disorders following mild traumatic
brain injury. Neurology 2007; 68:1136–1140.
113. Flanagan SR, Greenwald B, Weber S, et al. Pharmacological treatment of insomnia for individuals
with brain injury. J Head Trauma Rehabil 2007; 22 (1):67–70.
114. Li Pi Shan RS, Ashworth NL. Comparison of lorazepam and zopiclone for insomnia in patients with
stroke and brain injury: a randomized, crossover, double-blinded trial. Am J Phys Med Rehabil 2004;
83:421–427.
IHBK059-16 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:35 Char Count=
16 Substance-Induced Insomnia
Deirdre A. Conroy
Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan, U.S.A.
J. Todd Arnedt
Department of Psychiatry and Neurology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan, U.S.A.
Kirk J. Brower
Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan, U.S.A.
Alcohol
Intoxication
Acute administration of alcohol to normal healthy volunteers decreases sleep onset latency
(SOL) (6–8), prolongs rapid eye movement onset latency (ROL) (9) and increases slow wave sleep
(SWS) in the first half of the night (6,7,10). In the second half of the night, stage 1 (N1) sleep, wake-
fulness, and the percentage of REM sleep increase (6), while SWS decreases (6,7,10) (Table 1).
IHBK059-16
Table 1 Effect of Substances of Abuse on Sleep and Wakefulness
166
IHBK059-Sateia
Sleep Sleep Sleep
continuity architecture disorders Sleepinessa
Substance of Abuse SOL TST SE ROL REM SWS S1 SDB PLMS MSLT MWT
ALCOHOL
Intoxicationb ⇓6–8 ⇓8,13c ⇑8 ⇑9 ⇓7,9,14,15 ⇑6–8,15,16 ⇓8 ⇑15,17d ⇑18 ⇑11
⇓19
8:35
⇓ 33⇔ 32,33i
CAFFEINE
Char Count=
Intoxication ⇑34,35 ⇓34,36 ⇓34,35 ⇓34k ⇔34k ⇓35,36l ⇑36 ⇑34,37
⇔ ⇔
Withdrawal 34 ⇔34 ⇔34 ⇔34 ⇔34 34 ⇔34 ⇔34
MARIJUANA
Intoxication ⇑38 ⇑38m ⇑38 ⇑38 ⇓40 ⇑40,41 ⇑38m ⇓38
⇓ ⇓ ⇓39 ⇓
Withdrawal ⇑42 ⇑43 ⇓42,43 ⇓41
OPIOIDS
Intoxication ⇑44 ⇔45,46 ⇔45,46 ⇑46 ⇓46–49 ⇓45–47 ⇑48 ⇑46,47,50
⇓44 ⇓47 49 ⇔51
⇓46 ⇓45
Withdrawal ⇑52 ⇑49,52,53 ⇔52
⇓
MDMA “ECSTASY”
CONROY ET AL.
Intoxication ⇔54 ⇓55 ⇓54 ⇓54 ⇑56 ⇑57
⇑
Withdrawal ⇑57 ⇓57 ⇓57j ⇓57 ⇑57j ⇔57 ⇑57
IHBK059-16
SUBSTANCE-INDUCED INSOMNIA
IHBK059-Sateia
COCAINE
Intoxication ⇑58 ⇓58 ⇓58 ⇑58 ⇓58 ⇑59 ⇔58
⇔60
Withdrawal ⇓58,59g ⇓ ⇓62,63 ⇓62,65 ⇑62,64 ⇓59,61 ⇑61 ⇓65
⇔61 59,62– ⇔ 61 ⇔63 ⇔63n
⇑59e 64e ⇑59g
⇑
GHB
Intoxication ⇑69 ⇓69 ⇑69 ⇓69 ⇓69 ⇑69 ⇓69 ⇑70 ⇑69
8:35
⇑
Withdrawal
Char Count=
a
Decrease in sleep onset on the MSLT indicates more sleepiness.
b
Alcohol’s effects on sleep stages change from the 1st half to the 2nd half of the night.
c
Increased number of wake periods in sleep.
d
Only in men.
e
Late withdrawal (>3 weeks).
f
Increased SOL during first 75 min after consumption.
g
Early withdrawal (<3 weeks).
h
Wetter et al (2000) shows SWS decreases at day 3 but then increases from day 3–10.
i
Initially increased, but decreased over time.
j
Not significant.
k
Decreased shortly after caffeine intake but increase with late caffeine (see Table 1, pg. 530 of Bonnet and Arand 34).
l
During the first 1/3rd of the sleep period.
m
Depending on dose of THC and CBD variables; include dose and time into withdrawal.
n
Pace –Schott et al (63) is a binge abstinence protocol.
⇑= Increase, ⇓= Decrease, ⇔= No change, = No data. Abbreviations: MSLT, Multiple Sleep Latency Test; MWT, Maintenance of Wakefulness Test. PLMS, Periodic Limb Movements in Sleep;
REM%, Rapid Eye Movement percentage; ROL, Rapid Eye Movement Sleep Latency; S1, Stage 1 sleep; SDB, Sleep Disordered Breathing; SE, Sleep efficiency; SOL, Sleep onset latency; SWS,
Slow Wave Sleep; TST, Total sleep time.
167
IHBK059-16 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:35 Char Count=
Tolerance to alcohol’s acute effects can develop quickly, within one week in healthy volunteers
(11), resulting in increased alcohol intake to derive the same sleep-promoting effects. Insomnia
may persist even after alcohol use has stopped and treatment has been initiated. In fact, an esti-
mated 36% to 91% of patients in early recovery from alcohol dependence complain of insomnia
(2,12) Figure 1 provides a schematic describing the bidirectional relationship of alcohol use and
sleep disturbance.
Withdrawal
Polysomnographic (PSG) irregularities during alcohol withdrawal have been well characterized.
There is typically an increase in SOL (14,22), a decrease in SWS percentage (%) (16,22) and an
increase in REM sleep (16). Returning to drinking was found to temporarily increase SWS or
decrease REM sleep; however, upon repeat withdrawal, these changes reverted to baseline or
below baseline levels (71).
Sleep architecture (SWS and REM) and sleep continuity (sleep latency, total sleep time,
sleep efficiency) during withdrawal have been studied extensively as predictors of relapse
(22). Reduced SWS percentage was implicated in some early studies, (10,72) but these findings
were only partially replicated in a larger sample (22). REM variables have been consistently
linked to relapse in alcoholic patients (23). Increased REM pressure and reduced ROL, have
been shown to differentiate relapsers from abstainers after follow-up as long as six months
(22,73). A recent study using spectral EEG analysis found evidence for increased hyperarousal
in the REM sleep of alcoholic patients who relapsed within three months compared to abstinent
alcoholics and controls (74). In addition to the consistent REM findings, most clinical studies
have also identified sleep continuity measures, (increased SOL and reduced TST) as being
strongly predictive of relapse (21,22). Sleep disturbances during withdrawal can persist for up
to two years (16).
Comparisons between subjective and objective data may highlight important perceptual
distinctions in SII. Alcohol dependent patients with insomnia who overestimated SOL and
underestimated wake time after sleep onset (WASO) were more likely to relapse (75).
Several mechanisms have been proposed to explain the sleep disruptive properties of alco-
hol. Sleep-generating systems in the brain, including the GABAergic system, adapt to chronic
alcohol administration by downregulation, whereas arousal-generating systems involving glu-
tamate and other neurotransmitters are upregulated. During alcohol withdrawal, dysregulated
neurotransmitter systems such as these are postulated to disrupt sleep (71). One study found
that alcoholics’ homeostatic response (measured via SWS and delta power) to partial sleep
deprivation was impaired compared to controls (76). Circadian rhythm mechanisms are also
disturbed by alcohol (77–79). Insomnia may also be related to alcohol-related sleep disordered
IHBK059-16 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:35 Char Count=
breathing (SDB) (18) or periodic limb movements in sleep (PLMS) (25,80), both of which are
elevated in alcoholic patients.
Nicotine
Nicotine increases catecholamines, vasopressin, growth hormone, ACTH, and endorphins in
the brain, which can disturb sleep. The average cigarette contains 8 to 9 mg of nicotine, but
the amount delivered varies because smokers can adjust dose by puffing volume, depth, rate
or intensity. Many studies, therefore, have employed the nicotine patch to better regulate dose-
dependent effects.
Intoxication
Smokers (particularly females (81)) report more difficulty sleeping compared to nonsmokers
independent of health, demographics, behavioral, and psychological variables (81). Laboratory
studies show that nonsmokers with a nicotine patch took longer to fall asleep, had shorter TST,
lower sleep efficiency (SE), and lower REM sleep % compared to participants with a placebo
patch (26).
Depression history is associated with differences in REM sleep during nicotine exposure
and withdrawal. A randomized controlled trial found that depressed nonsmokers wearing
nicotine patches showed increased REM sleep and short-term mood improvements compared
to nondepressed nonsmokers (29). The increased REM sleep persisted during withdrawal in
depressed patients, while REM sleep in nondepressed patients decreased (32).
Withdrawal
Sleep and mood disturbances have been documented for up to one year following smoking ces-
sation. Heavy smokers (mean smoking history of 24 years) were studied across a smoking week
and a withdrawal week. During the withdrawal week, smokers had more arousals, awakenings,
and stage changes compared to the smoking week (33). In the first long-term study across one
year of abstinence, seven former smokers underwent PSG at months 1, 2, 4, 6, 9, and 12. There
was a reduction of REM latency, N1 sleep, and SWS coupled with an increase in REM sleep
and N2 sleep (31). Interestingly, depression measures were increased and correlated with REM,
ROL from sleep onset and N2, and stage shifts (31). The authors posit that the increases in REM
sleep and levels of depression may be due in part to an increased sensitivity of serotonergic
neurons during withdrawal.
Caffeine
Caffeine, a phosphodiesterase inhibitor with a half-life ranging from three to seven hours, pro-
motes wakefulness by blocking adenosine receptors (82) and is the most widely used substance
for counteracting the daytime effects of sleep loss.
Intoxication
Caffeine attenuates the progressive increase in sleepiness across the day and reduces the quality
of recovery sleep (83), but effects vary depending on an individual’s history of caffeine con-
sumption (84). Administration of caffeine at bedtime has been shown to prolong SOL, shorten
TST, and reduce total SWS and S4 sleep in a dose-dependent manner (34,35,85) During sleep
deprivation, low to moderate caffeine doses (100–200 mg) have been found to increase stage 1
sleep and reduce the slow component of EEG power (0.75–2.0 Hz) relative to placebo; during
recovery sleep, fast EEG components (11.25–20.0 Hz) were enhanced (35). Higher caffeine doses
during sleep deprivation (300 mg) reduce TST, increase S1 sleep, and reduce SWS in the first
third of the night (36).
Low-dose caffeine improves daytime performance more than placebo (86). Concomitant
subjective sleepiness data has been more variable. For example, a recent double-blind parallel-
group design using a forced desynchrony protocol found that low doses of repeated caffeine
(0.3 mg per hour) across an extended 28.57 hour “day” reduced objective sleepiness compared
to placebo, but increased subjective sleepiness (86).
Chronic caffeine use can lead to a physiological state of hyperarousal, a common charac-
teristic in patients with insomnia. One study used caffeine to model insomnia by giving repeated
IHBK059-16 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:35 Char Count=
doses of 400 mg of caffeine to healthy volunteers across one week. The study reported prolonged
SOL, reduced S4, and shortened TST (34). Metabolic rate measured via VO2 also increased, con-
sistent with the hyperarousal theory of insomnia. A later study by the same group examined
electrocardiograms (ECGs) during sleep in 15 healthy volunteers who took 400 mg of caffeine a
half-hour before bedtime. Heart rate variability, defined as the integration of the low frequency
(LF)/high frequency (HF) ratio band was significantly higher during REM sleep in the caffeine
group than in the placebo group. The investigators interpreted this finding as suggestive of a
predominance of sympathetic nervous system activity and an increased risk for adverse cardiac
events (87).
Withdrawal
Withdrawal from chronic caffeine use is associated with distinct physiological and subjective
withdrawal symptoms, due in large part to the hypersensitivity of adenosine receptors during
abstinence (82). Within 12 to 16 hours into caffeine withdrawal, sleepiness, lethargy, headaches,
decreased alertness, depressed mood, irritability, and mental fogginess may occur (83,84). With-
drawal symptoms usually dissipate within 3 to 5 days, but can last as long as one week (88).
In summary, the ability of caffeine to attenuate decrements in performance across prolonged
wakefulness makes it a highly desired substance. However, caffeine can contribute to impair-
ments in sleep if consumed in close proximity to bedtime and withdrawal can be associated
with impairment in cognitive functioning.
Marijuana
The psychoactive ingredient in marijuana, delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC), binds to type
1 cannabinoid (CB1 ) G protein-coupled receptors located in the nociceptive areas of the brain,
spinal cord, and peripheral nervous system (89).
Intoxication
Overall, THC effects on sleep have been found to be variable, owing to variability in methodol-
ogy and samples studied (see also Table 1 –substance table).
Marijuana can induce sleep (38) and decrease REM sleep (40). Administration of THC at
doses of 10, 20, and 30 mg prior to sleep onset decreased SOL after subjects reported achieving a
“high” (90). When large doses (15 mg) of cannabidiol (a major component in marijuana thought
to have minimal psychoactive effects) were added to THC and administered to participants
before sleep, the amount of wakefulness in the night increased (38). Conflicting evidence exists
with respect to the acute effect of THC on SWS [for review see Schierenbeck et al. 2008 (91)].
Some studies have reported an increase in stage 4 (S4) (40) sleep, while others have reported a
decrease in overall SWS (38).
Withdrawal
Sleep disturbance is one of the most common symptoms associated with marijuana with-
drawal. Other common symptoms may include strange dreams, depression, chills, and irri-
tability. Among 1735 frequent users of marijuana (>21 occasions in a single year), 235 (13.5%)
reported difficulty sleeping during withdrawal (92). Laboratory studies show prolonged SOL
(42) and decreased SWS% (42,43) during withdrawal. REM sleep rebound has been documented,
but only in withdrawal from higher doses of THC (70–210 mg) (43). One study tracking common
cannabis withdrawal symptoms, including sleep problems, in 36 cannabis-dependent subjects
reported that these symptoms were not predictive of relapse after an approximately 26-month
period (93).
Opioids
Intoxication
Opioids, (e.g. methadone, morphine, heroin) are perceived as sedating, but can disrupt sleep
quality. In healthy adults, morphine sulfate and methadone have been found to reduce SWS
(45,47) and REM sleep (48,49) When compared to naltrexone in a group of recovering addicts,
methadone patients had increased SOL (44) and WASO and reduced TST (44), SWS (47), and
IHBK059-16 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:35 Char Count=
REM sleep (44). Heroin-dependent patients detoxified with methadone reported even greater
sleep problems than those maintained on methadone during the first months of heroin absti-
nence (94). Sleep disturbance, increased dreams, and nightmares were also associated with
opioids in palliative care patients (95).
Withdrawal
Abstinent morphine-dependent patients showed a dose-related effect of morphine on sleep
with decreases in TST, SWS, and REM sleep (96). Methadone-maintained patients had more
awakenings (44) lower sleep efficiency (SE), and lower SWS (47) than controls. One early study
of six male heroin-dependent prisoners compared sleep for three months prior to methadone
induction, during titration and stabilization of 100 mg daily of methadone, and finally up to
22 weeks after withdrawal. During methadone administration, awake time during the night
decreased and delta bursts increased significantly (53). Across 22 weeks of abstinence, wake-up
time decreased further while REM sleep and delta sleep increased significantly (53).
Animal data suggest that opioids may disrupt sleep by decreasing GABA neurotrans-
mission in the pontine reticular nucleus (97). Symptoms of insomnia may also result from
opioid-induced central sleep apnea (46,47,98), occurring in about 30% of chronic users (99).
Intoxication
Persistent use of ecstasy shortens TST and impairs NREM sleep, particularly S2 sleep (100,101).
An early study examined the effects of MDMA on the sleep of 23 MDMA abstinent users
compared to age- and sex-matched controls with no history of use (55). The MDMA users
had a 19-minute reduction of TST and a slightly shorter ROL than controls (60 minutes versus
75 minutes). In a more recent study, alpha-methyl-para-tyrosine (AMPT), which decreases brain
catecholamines, was administered to abstinent MDMA users to determine whether AMPT
would differentially affect sleep and cognitive performance in abstinent users. Results showed
that exposure to AMPT at 4 PM and 10 PM before a 12 AM bedtime resulted in a trend towards
decreased TST, ROL and significantly lower S2 sleep (102).
MDMA exposure has lasting effects on circadian rhythms in animal experiments. Rats
exposed to MDMA had alterations in sleep and circadian pattern of activity (wheel running) for
up to five days after dosage. In addition, SWS was still altered after one month (103). In vitro
administration of MDMA to rat brain slices impaired the resetting ability of cells in the SCN to
a serotoinin receptor agonist 20 weeks after initial exposure (60).
Cocaine
Cocaine is a stimulant that blocks reuptake of DA, NE, and 5HT and can cause increased energy
and euphoria.
Intoxication
Acute administration of cocaine in a laboratory setting causes sleep impairment, including
increased SOL by several hours, decreased SE, and decreased REM sleep (58). In chronic cocaine
users, administration of cocaine actually increased SWS during cocaine administration (59).
Spectral analyses of the sleep EEG showed that during binge periods, slower EEG bandwidths,
e.g., delta and theta, were higher than the faster bands, e.g., sigma and beta (59).
IHBK059-16 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:35 Char Count=
Withdrawal
Sleep disturbance is one of the most common symptoms of cocaine withdrawal, reported in
71% of a sample of recovering cocaine addicts (20). Sleep quality appears to change from early
abstinence (≤3 weeks) to late abstinence (>3 weeks). Early abstinence in cocaine withdrawal
is associated with decreased SOL (59), increased TST (58,59,62), shortened ROL (61,62), and
increased REM% (62). Late abstinence is associated with prolonged SOL (59,62), decreased TST
(63) and decreased SE (63).
Finally, there appears to be a discrepancy between objective sleep and subjective percep-
tion of sleep during early withdrawal from cocaine. Subjective ratings, including overall sleep
quality, feeling well rested, depth of sleep, and mental alertness all increased over protracted
abstinence in one study, but objectively measured sleep did not (59). Other studies have found
that despite deterioration of sleep quality across cocaine abstinence, estimates of sleep quality
improved (104) or remained unchanged (63). The authors posit that this finding may reflect a
dysregulation of homeostatic sleep drive in chronic cocaine users. Similar discrepancies between
objective and subjective measures have been documented in early abstinence from alcohol (74).
While cocaine is a known stimulant of monoamine neurotransmitter systems via reup-
take blockade, animal studies suggest that circadian mechanisms may also be involved in its
rewarding effects, if not sleep impairment per se (105,106).
Amphetamines
The exact mechanisms of how amphetamines, a major class of stimulants, increase EEG arousal
are uncertain, but are thought to involve adrenergic or dopaminergic transmission (107).
Increased alertness is also highly dependent on the dose and the type of amphetamine.
Intoxication
D-amphetamine is three times more potent than L-amphetamine and 12 times more potent than
L-methamphetamine in increasing wakefulness and reducing SWS (108). Three studies have
examined the effect of amphetamines on daytime sleepiness using the Multiple Sleep Latency
Test (MSLT). All three showed prolonged SOL on nap opportunities across the day (66–68). Sleep
deprivation appears to increase the drug-seeking behaviors associated with amphetamines.
Another similarly acting stimulant, methylphenidate, was chosen more often (88% of days)
after a four-hour time in bed time than it was after an 8-hour time in bed (29%) (68).
Withdrawal
Early amphetamine withdrawal is associated with initial long bouts of sleep and reports of
poor quality sleep. Sleep records obtained by nurses’ observations in abstinent amphetamine-
dependent subjects showed an initial increase in TST followed by reduced TST across 20 nights
following cessation of use (109). Sleep questionnaires given to 21 patients in the first three weeks
of methamphetamine withdrawal showed that TST (day and night) peaked on the fifth day of
abstinence (110). One study evaluated objective sleep parameters across acute (days 3–10) and
subacute withdrawal (days 11–14) in stimulant abusers. From acute to subacute withdrawal,
stimulant abusers had less TST and REM sleep. However, subjective reports of sleep improved
across abstinence. Another study found that self-reported SOL, number of awakenings in the
night, quality of sleep, clear-headedness on awakening, satisfaction with sleep, and depth of
sleep all improved significantly across the three weeks of continued abstinence (110).
EVALUATION
Preliminary Assessment
SII may have varying clinical presentations depending on the patient’s age, gender, weight,
genetics, psychological traits and states, and health status. For example, stimulant-induced
insomnia may be diagnosed more in younger patients (typically adolescents) whereas SII due
to alcohol or hypnotics may be found more in older patients. Substances may also have indi-
rect effects by exacerbating preexisting medical conditions (e.g. caffeine may worsen gastroe-
sophageal reflux disease, which can disrupt sleep).
IHBK059-16 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:35 Char Count=
When assessing substance-using patients for insomnia, several principles should be borne
in mind. First, substances may not be the only cause of the insomnia. Other medical or psychiatric
disorders, sleep-impairing medications, inadequate sleep hygiene, and dysfunctional beliefs
about sleep may play a role. This issue is particularly relevant to patients with substance use
disorders, who have high rates of co-occurring psychiatric and medical disorders. It should
be assumed that substances are part of the problem, even if not necessarily the only cause of
insomnia. Second, PSG should be considered if there is high suspicion of other sleep disorders,
particularly sleep apnea and periodic limb movement (PLM) disorder. Third, assessment of
sleep complaints is aided by asking patients to keep a sleep log for two weeks during early
recovery after acute substance withdrawal symptoms have subsided. Sleep logs have several
advantages, including an assessment of sleep patterns over time, documenting improvement
with abstinence, and engaging the patient in the treatment process.
Differential Diagnoses
In addition to other medical or psychiatric disorders, differential diagnoses of SII include inad-
equate sleep hygiene and psychophysiological insomnia. To distinguish SII from these two
disorders, specific inquiry about substance tolerance, dosage escalation, rebound insomnia in
the absence of the substance, fear of not sleeping (or staying awake) without the substance,
and sporadic use of hypnotics, will help distinguish SII from other sleep disorders. Use of the
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual or the ICSD can be used to diagnose SII, but certain diagnostic
criteria may vary (see Table 2).
Inadequate sleep hygiene is characterized by daily activities that are not compatible with
maintenance of quality sleep. This diagnosis is appropriate when the timing of substance use is
incompatible with the preferred sleep time. For example, a patient’s use of alcohol or nicotine in
the hours before bedtime is clearly associated with the sleep disturbance. However, SII should
be considered if the sleep problem began or got worse after initiation of the substance use or the
sleep difficulties improve markedly during periods of abstinence, or the individual feels unable
to sleep without the substance. Some of the distinguishing characteristics of psychophysiological
insomnia are the perpetuating factors that serve to maintain the insomnia, i.e., heightened
arousal in bed. Substance use may have contributed to poor sleep in the acute and early stages
of the insomnia, but when the maladaptive thoughts and behaviors persist even after the patient
stops using the substance, the diagnosis is more consistent with psychophysiological insomnia
B. SUBSTANCE-INDUCED INSOMNIA
Meets the criteria for insomnia
Insomnia developed within one month of substance exposure, use or abuse, or acute
withdrawal
There is current ongoing dependence on or abuse of a substance known to disrupt
sleep either during use or withdrawal or there is exposure to a toxin known to disrupt
sleep.
Insomnia is temporally associated with the substance use, exposure or withdrawal
Insomnia is not better accounted for by a sleep disorder that is not substance induced,
medical, neurological, or mental disorder
a
DSM-IV, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed. American Psychiatric Association, DSM-IV-TR: Diagnostic
and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed, Text Revision, American Psychiatric Association, Washington DC, 2000.
b
ICSD-2, International Classification of Sleep Disorders, 2nd edition (1).
IHBK059-16 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:35 Char Count=
than SII. Therefore, a timeline with respect to the use of the substance(s) and the onset of the
insomnia is important to obtain.
Finally, insomnia may predate the substance use/dependence; however, there must be
convincing evidence based on the sleep history that the sleep disturbance and substance
use/abuse are intricately related.
TREATMENT
Pharmacological Treatments
Special considerations are required when considering hypnotic therapy with patients recovering
from substance dependence. First, physicians who treat patients with a history of substance
abuse or dependence are reticent to prescribe medications for sleep. This is especially true for
the Schedule IV hypnotics (including benzodiazepine receptor agonists), which are first-line
agents for insomnia in nonabusing patients with insomnia, but their reluctance to prescribe
may also generalize to other available hypnotics. A recent postal survey of addiction medicine
physicians found that less than one-third of alcoholic patients with sleep disturbances during
the first three months of recovery were offered a sleep medication (111). Second, although the
most widely used hypnotic agents have low addiction potential for most patients with insomnia
(112), the benzodiazepine receptor agonist medications have moderate to high abuse liability
in patients with a history of substance abuse and dependence (113).
We outline the most commonly available hypnotic medications and their efficacy for
substance-induced insomnia below. These studies have been conducted almost exclusively
with alcohol-dependent patients. Pharmacological treatment options for patients with other
types of substance use disorders are needed.
Anticonvulsants
Anticonvulsant agents do not lower seizure threshold, which makes them appealing for treating
insomnia in the substance-abusing population. At least two anticonvulsants—carbamazepine
and gabapentin-–have been studied specifically for their effects on sleep in alcoholics. Carba-
mazepine was superior to lorazepam for treating sleep disturbance associated with acute alcohol
withdrawal (123). Gabapentin has the advantages of sleep promotion, non-liver metabolism,
noninterference with metabolism or excretion of other medications, and it does not require blood
monitoring for therapeutic concentrations, hepatotoxicity, and hematological toxicity. Further-
more, it has a favorable side effects profile, low abuse potential, and is not protein-bound.
It exerts its CNS effects by binding to alpha-2-delta receptors, resulting in voltage-sensitive
calcium channel inhibition (124).
Gabapentin may improve the sleep of recovering alcoholic patients (125–127). Karam-
Hage and Brower (125) found that self-reported sleep quality improved after four to six weeks
of treatment with gabapentin (mean dose 953 mg/day) in 15 of 17 consecutively evaluated
alcoholic patients with persistent insomnia. A recent placebo-controlled study, however, found
that, while six weeks of gabapentin 1500 mg nightly delayed the onset to heavy drinking,
no subjective or objective sleep differences were found between the gabapentin and placebo
groups (128). Although gabapentin and pregabalin, a newer anticonvulsant agent, increase SWS
in healthy control subjects, similar evidence is lacking in patients with alcohol dependence.
IHBK059-16 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:35 Char Count=
Antidepressants
Trazodone is the most frequently prescribed medication for sleep by addiction medicine physi-
cians (111). It is effective acutely in the treatment of depressed patients with insomnia (129–132)
and has been used safely in several samples of alcoholics (126,133). In a placebo-controlled study
of trazodone in 16 abstinent alcoholic patients, trazodone significantly reduced wake time after
sleep onset (WASO) and improved SE compared to placebo, but only improvements in WASO
were sustained at night 28. Depression scores were also more improved in the trazodone group
(134). A recently completed study also found superior sleep outcomes of trazodone vs. placebo
more than 12 weeks of treatment in alcohol-dependent patients, but heavy drinking was higher
in the trazodone-treated group compared to the placebo group (135).
Antipsychotics
Quetiapine is an atypical antipsychotic with sedative effects (136) that has been used as a
treatment for insomnia in alcohol-dependent patients. In one of the only studies in substance
users, Monnelly et al (137) found that alcohol-dependent veterans who reported difficulty
sleeping and were treated with 25 to 200 mg of quetiapine had an increased number of days
of abstinence and fewer hospitalizations, but no subjective or objective sleep measure was
included. Moreover, any benefit of quetiapine for use in this population must be weighed
against its potential for akathisia (138) and increased PLMs when used to promote sleep (136).
Other Hypnotics
Melatonin is a sleep-promoting agent that may be particularly useful to treat circadian rhythm
disorders (139). Because the manufacturing and quality of melatonin is not currently regu-
lated in the U.S., the melatonin receptor agonist, ramelteon, may be a better candidate for
study. Over-the-counter remedies such as antihistamines, valerian root extract (from the herbal
plant, Valeriana officinalis), and melatonin have not been widely evaluated in substance-abusing
patients, although they are commonly used.
CONCLUSIONS
There are a number of licit and illicit substances that can lead to SII. Although substance-
induced sleep problems improve with continued abstinence, persistent sleep problems may
occur for at least two reasons. First, long-lasting alterations to the sleep centers of the brain may
IHBK059-16 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:35 Char Count=
occur due to chronic drug exposure. Second, chronic sleep disturbance is typically associated
with multiple perpetuating causes and substance use may be just one of many reasons for sleep
complaints. Assessment should consider the many causes of sleep disturbance. Pharmacological
and nonpharmacological treatments to target insomnia associated with substance use disorders
do exist, but many are either inappropriate or have been inadequately tested in this patient
population. More well-controlled studies are needed to characterize the phenomenology of
sleep during recovery, to determine the efficacy of monotherapy and combined approaches to
sleep treatment in patients with addiction, and to evaluate the impact of such treatments on
relapse and recovery.
REFERENCES
1. The International Classification of Sleep Disorders, Diagnostic and Coding Manual, 2nd ed. American
Academy of Sleep Medicine. Westchester, IL; 2005.
2. Cohn T, Foster J, Peters T. Sequential studies of sleep disturbance and quality of life in abstaining
alcoholics. Addict Biol 2003; 8:455–462.
3. National Sleep Foundation. 2008. Sleep in America poll: summary of findings.
http://www.sleepfoundation.org/sites/default/files/2008%20POLL%20SOF.PDF (Accessed
February 17, 2010).
4. Roehrs T, Papineau K, Rosenthal L, et al. Ethanol as a hypnotic in insomniacs: Self administration
and effects on sleep and mood. Neuropsychopharmacology 1999; 20:279–286.
5. Breslau N, Roth T, Rosenthal L, et al. Sleep disturbance and psychiatric disorders: A longitudinal
epidemiological study of young adults. Biol Psychiatry 1996; 39:411–418.
6. MacLean A, Cairns J. Dose response effects on ethanol on the sleep of young men. J Stud Alcohol
1982; 43:434–444.
7. Williams D, MacLean A, Cairns J. Dose-response effects of ethanol on the sleep of young women. J
Stud Alcohol 1983; 44:515–523.
8. Feige B, Gann H, Brueck R, et al. Effects of alcohol on polysomnographically recorded sleep in healthy
subjects. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2006; 30:1527–1537.
9. Miyata S, Noda A, Atarashi M, et al. REM sleep is impaired by a small amount of alcohol in young
women sensitive to alcohol. Intern Med 2004; 43:679–684.
10. Allen R, Wagman A, Funderburk F. Slow wave sleep changes: Alcohol tolerance and treatment
implications Adv Exp Med Biol 1977; 85A:629–640.
11. Roehrs T, Zwyghuizen-Doorenbos A, Knox M, et al. Sedating effects of ethanol and time of drinking.
Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1992; 16:553–557.
12. Brower KJ, Aldrich M, Robinson EAR,et al. Insomnia, self-medication, and relapse to alcoholism. Am
J Psychiatry 2001; 158:399–404.
13. Van Reen E, Jenni O, Carskadon M. Effects of alcohol on sleep and the sleep electroencephalogram
in healthy young women. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2006; 30:974–981.
14. Papineau K, Roehrs T, Petrucelli N, et al. Electrophysiological assessment (The Multiple Sleep Latency
Test) of the biphasic effects of ethanol in humans. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1998; 22:231–235.
15. Prinz P, Roehrs T, Vitaliano P, et al. Effect of alcohol on sleep and nighttime plasma growth hormone
and cortisol concentrations. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1980; 51:759–764.
16. Drummond S, Gillin J, Smith T, et al. The sleep of abstinent pure primary alcoholic patients: Natural
course and relationship to relapse. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1998; 22:1796–1802.
17. Peppard P, Austin D, Brown R. Association of alcohol consumption and sleep disordered breathing
in men and women. J Clin Sleep Med 2007; 3:265–270.
18. Aldrich MS, Brower KJ, Hall JM. Sleep-disordered breathing in alcoholics. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1999;
23:134–140.
19. Roehrs T, Claiborue D, Knox M, et al. Residual sedating effects of alcohol. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1994;
18:831–834.
20. Brower K, Maddahian E, Blow F, et al. A comparison of self reported symptoms and DSM-III-R
criteria for cocaine withdrawal. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse 1988; 14:347–356.
21. Clark CP, Gillin JC, Golshan S, et al. Increased REM sleep density at admission predicts relapse by
three months in primary alcoholics with a lifetime diagnosis of secondary depression. Biol Psychiatry
1998; 43:601–607.
22. Brower KJ, Aldrich MS, Hall JM. Polysomnographic and subjective sleep predictors of alcoholic
relapse. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1998; 22:1864–1871.
23. Gillin JC, Smith TL, Irwin M, et al. Increased pressure for rapid eye movement sleep at time of
hospital admission predicts relapse in nondepressed patients with primary alcoholism at 3-month
follow-up. Arch Gen psychiatry 1994; 51:189–197.
IHBK059-16 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:35 Char Count=
24. Aldrich M, Shipley J, Tandon R, et al. Sleep-disordered breathing in alcoholics: Association with age.
Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1993; 17:1179–1183.
25. Gann H, Feige B, van Calker D, et al. Periodic limb movements during sleep in alcohol dependent
patients. Eur Arch Psychiatry Neurosci 2002; 252:124–129.
26. Davila D, Hurt R, Offord K, et al. Acute effects of transdermal nicotine on sleep architecture, snoring,
and sleep disordered breathing in nonsmokers. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 1994; 150:469–474.
27. Soldatos C, Kales J, Scharf M, et al. Cigarette smoking associated with sleep difficulties. Science 1980;
207:551–552.
28. Gothe B, Strohl K, Levin S, et al. Nicotine: A different approach to treatment of obstructive sleep
apnea. Chest 1985; 87:11–17.
29. Salin-Pascual R, de la Fuente J, Galicia-Polo L, et al. Effects of transdermal nicotine on mood and
sleep in nonsmoking major depressed patients. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1995; 121:476–479.
30. Wetter DW, Young TB. The relation between cigarette smoking and sleep disturbance. Prev Med
1994a; 23:328–334.
31. Moreno-Coutino A, Calderon -Ezquerro C, Drucker-Colin R. Long-term changes in sleep and depres-
sive symptoms of smokers in abstinence. Nicotine Tob Res 2007; 9:389–396.
32. Wetter DW, Carmack C, Anderson C, et al. Tobacco withdrawal signs and symptoms among women
with and without a history of depression. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2000; 8:88–96.
33. Prosise G, Bonnet M, Berry R, et al. Effects of abstinence from smoking on sleep and daytime
sleepiness. Chest 1994; 105:1136–1141.
34. Bonnet M, Arand D. Caffeine use as a model of acute and chronic insomnia. Sleep 1992; 15:526–536.
35. Landolt H, Retey J, Tonz K, et al. Caffeine attenuates waking and sleep electroencephalographic
markers of sleep homeostasis in humans. Neuropsychopharmacology 2004; 29:1933–1399.
36. LaJambe C, Kamimori G, Belenky G, et al. Caffeine effects on recovery sleep following 27 hours of
total sleep deprviation. Aviat Space Environ Med 2005; 76:108–113.
37. Zwyghuizen-Doorenbos A, Roehrs T, Lipschutz L, et al. Effects of caffeine on alertness. Psychophar-
macology (Berl) 1990; 100:36–39.
38. Nicholson A, Turner C, Stone B, et al. Effect of delta-9- tetrahydrocannabinol and cannabidiol on
nocturnal sleep and early morning behavior in young adults. J Clin Psychopharmacol 2004; 24:305–
313.
39. Freemon F. The effect of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol on sleep. Psychopharmacology 1974; 35:39–
44.
40. Feinberg I, Jones R, Walker J, et al. Effects of marijuana extract and tetrahydrocannabinol on elec-
troencephalographic sleep patterns. Clin Pharmacol Ther 1976; 19:782–794.
41. Pivik R, Zarcone V, Dement W, et al. Delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol and synhexyl; effects on human
sleep patterns. Clin Pharmacol Ther 1972; 13:426–435.
42. Freemon F. The effect of chronically administered delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol upon the polygraph-
ically monitored sleep of normal volunteers. Drug Alcohol Depend 1982; 10:345–353.
43. Feinberg I, Jones R, Walker J, et al. Effects of high dosage delta -9-tetrahydrocannabinol on sleep
patterns in man. Clin Pharmacol Ther 1975; 17:458–466.
44. Staedt J, Wassmuth F, Stoppe G. Effects of chronic treatment with methadone and naltrexone on sleep
in addicts. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1996; 246:305–309.
45. Dimsdale J, Norman D, DeJardin D, et al. The effect of opioids on sleep architecture. J Clin Sleep Med
2007; 3:33–36.
46. Wang D, Teichtahl H, Drummer O, et al. Central sleep apnea in stable methadone maintenance
treatment patients. Chest 2005; 128:1348–1356.
47. Teichtahl H, Prodromidis A, Miller B, et al. Sleep-disordered breathing in stable methadone pro-
gramme patients: A pilot study. Addiction 2001; 96:395–403.
48. Kay D, Pickworth W, Neider G. Morphine-like insomnia from heroin in nondependent human
addicts. Br J Clin Pharmacol 1981; 11:159–169.
49. Lewis S, Oswald I, Evans J, et al, Tompsett S. Heroin and human sleep. Electroencephalogr Clin
Neurophysiol 1970; 28:374–381.
50. Farney R, Walker J, Cloward T, et al. Sleep-disordered breathing associated with long-term opioid
therapy. Chest 2003; 123:623–639.
51. Shaw I, Lavigne G, Mayer P, et al. Acute intravenous administration of morphine perturbs sleep
architecture in healthy pain -free young adults: A preliminary study. Sleep 2005; 28:677–682.
52. Wang D, Teichtahl H. Opioids, sleep architecture, and sleep-disordered breathing. Sleep Med Rev
2007; 11:35–46.
53. Kay D. Human sleep and EEG through a cycle of methadone dependence. Electroencephalogr Clin
Neurophysiol 1975; 38:35–43.
54. Gouzoulis E, Steiger A, Ensslin M, et al. Sleep EEG effects of 3, 4-⇔⇓methylenedioxyethamphetamine
(MDE;“eve”) in healthy volunteers. Biol Psychiatry 1992; 32:1108–1117.
IHBK059-16 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:35 Char Count=
86. Wyatt J, Cajochen C, Ritz-De Cecco A, et al. Low-dose repeated caffeine administration for
circadian-phase dependent performance degredation during extended wakefulness. Sleep 2004; 27:
374–381.
87. Bonnet M, Tancer M, Uhde T, et al. Effects of caffeine on heart rate and QT variability during sleep.
Depress Anxiety 2005; 22:150–155.
88. Hughes J, Oliveto A, Bickel W, et al. Caffeine self-administration and withdrawal: Incidence, indi-
vidual differences and interrelationships. Drug Alcohol Depend 1993; 32:239–246.
89. Pertwee R. Cannabinoid receptors and pain. Prog Neurobiol 2001; 63:569–611.
90. Cousens K, Dimascio A. Delta-9- THC as an hypnotic. An experimental study of 3 dose levels.
Psychopharmacologia 1973; 33:355–364.
91. Schierenbeck T, Riemann D, Berger M, et al. Effect of illicit recreational drugs upon sleep: Cocaine,
ecstasy, and marijuana. Sleep Med Rev 2008; 12:381–389.
92. Wiesbeck G, Schuckit M, Kalmijn J, et al. An evaluation of the history of marijuana withdrawal
syndrome in a large population. Addiction 1996; 91:1469–1478.
93. Arendt M, Rosenberg R, Foldager L, et al. Withdrawal symptoms do not predict relapse among
subjects treated for cannabis dependence. Am J Addict 2007; 16:461–467.
94. Shi J, Zhao L, Epstein D, et al. Long-term methadone maintenance reduces protracted symptoms of
heroin abstinence and cue-induced craving in Chinese heroin abusers. Pharmacol Biochem Behav
2007; 87:141–145.
95. Vella-Brincat J, Macleod A. Adverse effects of opioids on the central nervous systems of palliative
care patients. J Pain Palliat Care Pharmacother 2007; 21:15–25.
96. Kay D, Einstein R, Jasinski D. Morphine effects on human REM state, waking state and NREM sleep.
Psychopharmacologia 1969; 14:404–416.
97. Watson C, Lydic R, Baghdoyan H. Sleep and GABA levels in the oral part of rat pontine reticular
formation are decreased by local and systemic administration of morphine. Neuroscience 2007;
144:375–386.
98. Walker J, Farney R, Rhondeau S, et al. Chronic opioid use is a risk factor for the development of
central sleep apnea and ataxic breathing. J Clin Sleep Med 2007; 3:455–461.
99. Teicher M, Wang D. Sleep disordered breathing with chronic opioid use. Expert Opin Drug Saf 2007;
6:641–649.
100. Parrott A. Human psychopharmacology of Ecstasy (MDMA): A review of 15 years of empirical
research. Hum Psychopharmacol 2001; 16:557–577.
101. Jansen K. Ecstasy (MDMA) dependence. Drug Alcohol Depend 1999; 53:121–124.
102. McCann U, Peterson S, Ricaurte G. The effect of catecholamine depletion by alpha-methyl-para-
tyrosine on measures of cognitive performance and sleep in abstinent MDMA users. Neuropsy-
chopharmacology 2007; 32:1695–1706.
103. Balogh B, Molnar E, Jakus R, et al. Effects of a single dose of 3,4 methylenedioxymethamphetamine
on circadian patterns, motor activity and sleep in drug naive rats and rats previously exposed to
MDMA. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2004; 173:296–309.
104. Morgan P, Mailison R. Cocaine and sleep:early abstinence. Scientific WorldJournal 2007; 7:223–230.
105. Lynch W, Girgenti M, Breslin F, et al. Gene profiling the response to repeat cocaine self-administration
in dorsal striatum: A focus on circadian genes. Brain Res 2008; 1213:166–177.
106. Roybal K, Theobold D, Graham A, et al. Mania-like behavior induced by disruption of CLOCK. Proc
Natl Acad Sci U S A 2007; 104:6406–6411.
107. Nishino S, Mignot E. Wake-promoting medications: Basic mechanisms and pharmacology. In: Kryger
M, Roth T, Dement W, eds. Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine, 4th ed. Philadelphia, PA: Elsevier
Saunders, 2005:468–483.
108. Kanbayashi T, Honda K, Kodama T, et al. Implication of dopaminergic mechanisms in the wake-
promoting effects of amphetamine: a study of D- and L-derivatives in canine narcolepsy. Neuroscience
2000; 99:651–659.
109. Gossop M, Bradley B, Brewis R. Amphetamine withdrawal and sleep disturbance. Drug Alcohol
Depend 1982; 10:177–183.
110. McGregor C, Srisurapanont M, Jittiwutikarn J, et al. The nature, time course and severity of metham-
phetamine withdrawal. Addiction 2005; 100:1320–1329.
111. Friedmann PD, Herman DS, Freedman S, et al. Treatment of sleep disturbance in alcohol recovery:
A national survey of addiction medicine physicians. J Addict Dis 2003; 22:91–103.
112. Mendelson WB, Roth T, Cassella J, et al. The treatment of chronic insomnia: Drug indications, chronic
use and abuse liability. Summary of a 2001 New Clinical Drug Evaluation Unit Meeting Symposium.
Sleep Med Rev 2004; 8:7–17.
113. Griffiths RR, Johnson MW. Relative abuse liability of hypnotic drugs: A conceptual framework and
algorithm for differentiating among compounds. J Clin Psychiatry 2005; 66:31–41.
114. Roehrs T, Roth T. Hypnotics: An update. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep 2003; 3:181–184.
IHBK059-16 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:35 Char Count=
115. Aubin H-J, Goldenberg F, Benoit O, et al. Effects of tetrabamate and of diazepam on sleep polygraphy
during subacute withdrawal in alcohol-dependent patients. Hum Psychopharmacol 1994; 9:191–195.
116. Graham AW. Sleep disorders. In: Graham AW, Schultz T, eds. Principles of Addiction Medicine.
Chevy Chase, MD: American Society of Addiction Medicine, 1998:793–808.
117. Ciraulo DA, Nace EP. Benzodiazepine treatment of anxiety or insomnia in substance abuse patients.
Am J Addict 2000; 9:276–284.
118. Hajak G, Müller WE, Wittchen HU, et al. Abuse and dependence potential for the non-benzodiazepine
hypnotics zolpidem and zopiclone: A review of case reports and epidemiological data. Addiction
2003; 98:1371–1378.
119. Johnson B, Longo LP. Considerations in the physician’s decision to prescribe benzodiazepines to
patients with addiction. Psychiatr Ann 1998; 28:160–165.
120. Kranzler HR. Evaluation and treatment of anxiety symptoms and disorders in alcoholics. J Clin
Psychiatry 1996; 57:15–21.
121. Schuckit MA. Recent developments in the pharmacotherapy of alcohol dependence. J Consult Clin
Psychol 1996; 64:669–676.
122. Swift RM. Pharmacologic treatment for drug and alcohol dependence: Experimental and standard
therapies. Psychiatr Ann 1998; 28:697–702.
123. Malcolm R Jr., Myrick H, Roberts J, et al. The differential effects of medication on mood, sleep
disturbance, and work ability in outpatient alcohol detoxification. Am J Addict 2002; 11:141–150.
124. LaRoche S, Helmers S. The new antiepileptic drug. JAMA 2004; 291:605–614.
125. Karam-Hage M, Brower KJ. Gabapentin treatment for insomnia associated with alcohol dependence.
Am J psychiatry 2000; 157:151.
126. Karam-Hage M, Brower KJ. Open pilot study of gabapentin versus trazodone to treat insomnia in
alcoholic patients. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2003; 57:542–544.
127. Rosenberg KP. Gabapentin for chronic insomnia. Am J Addict 2003; 12:273–274.
128. Brower KJ, Kim HB, Strobbe S, et al. A randomized double-blind pilot trial of gabapentin vs. placebo
to treat alcohol dependence and comorbid insomnia. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2008; 32:1429–1438.
129. Armitage R. The effects of antidepressants on sleep in patients with depression. Can J Psychiatry
2000; 45:803–809.
130. Mendelson WB. A review of the evidence for the efficacy and safety of trazodone for insomnia. J Clin
Psychiatry 2005; 66:469–476.
131. Kaynak H, Kaynak D, Gozukirmizi E, et al. The effects of trazodone on sleep in patients treated with
stimulant antidepressants. Sleep Med 2004; 5:15–20.
132. Nierenberg AA, Adler LA, Peselow E, et al. Trazadone for antidepressant-associated insomnia. Am
J Psychiatry 1994; 151:1069–1072.
133. Janiri L, Hadjichristos A, Buonanno A, et al. Adjuvant trazodone in the treatment of alcoholism: An
open study. Alcohol Alcohol 1998; 33:362–365.
134. Le Bon O, Murphy J, Staner L, et al. Double-blind, placebo controlled study of the efficacy of trazodone
in alcohol post-withdrawal sydrome: Polysomnography and clinical evaluations. J Clin Psychophar-
macol 2003; 23:377–383.
135. Friedmann PD, Rose JS, Swift RM, et al. Trazodone for sleep disturbance after detoxification from
alcohol dependence: A double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. In: American Academy of Addiction
Psychiatry 18th Annual Meeting & Symposium. 2007; Coronado, CA.
136. Cohrs S, Rodenbeck A, Guan Z, et al. Sleep-promoting properties of quetiapine in healthy subjects.
Psychopharmacology 2004; 174:421–429.
137. Monnelly EP, Ciraulo DA, Knapp C, et al. Quetiapine for treatment of alcohol dependence. J Clin
Psychopharmacol 2004; 24:532–535.
138. Catalano G, Grace JW, Catalano MC, et al. Acute akathisia associated with quetiapine use. Psycho-
somatics 2005; 46:291–301.
139. Cajochen C, Krauchi K, Wirz-Justice A. Role of melatonin in the regulation of human circadian
rhythms and sleep. J Neuroendocrinol 2003; 15:432–437.
140. Greeff AP, Conradie WS. Use of progressive relaxation training for chronic alcoholics with insomnia.
Psychol Rep 1998; 82:407–412.
141. Currie SR, Clark S, Hodgins DC, et al. Randomized controlled trial of brief cognitive-behavioural
interventions for insomnia in recovering alcohlics. Addiction 2004; 99:1121–1132.
142. Arnedt J, Conroy D, Rutt J, et al. An open trial of cognitive-behavioral treatment for insomnia
comorbid with alcohol dependence. Sleep Med 2007; 8:176–180.
143. Bootzin RR, Stevens S. Adolescents, substance abuse, and the treatment of insomnia and daytime
sleepiness. Clin Psychol Rev 2005; 25:629–644.
144. Haynes PL, Bootzin RR, Smith L, et al. Sleep and aggression in substance-abusing adolescents: Results
from an integrative behavioral sleep-treatment pilot program. Sleep 2006; 29:512–520.
c17 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 13:53 Char Count=
INTRODUCTION
Circadian rhythm sleep disorders (CRSDs) arise from either an alteration in the function of the
circadian timing system or a misalignment between the circadian rhythm of sleep propensity
and the requirements of the environmental or socially structured sleep schedule. There are six
recognized CRSDs: (i) delayed sleep phase disorder (DSPD), (ii) advanced sleep phase disorder
(ASPD), (iii) irregular sleep–wake disorder (ISWD), (iv) free-running disorder (FRD), also called
nonentrained or non–24-hour sleep–wake disorder, (v) jet lag disorder (JLD), and (vi) shift work
disorder (SWD). Insomnia (difficulty falling asleep or staying asleep) is a major symptom of all
the CRSDs, as well as sleepiness and dysphoria while awake. To meet the full diagnostic criteria
for a sleep disorder, the symptoms must be persistent and involve a significant impairment in
social, occupational, or other areas of function.
DIAGNOSTIC CONSIDERATIONS
Although the formal diagnostic criteria developed by the American Academy of Sleep Medicine
(AASM) (1) are intended to distinguish clinical disorders from normal variability, the dividing
line is not always sharp. Symptoms are likely to occur in otherwise unaffected people if the
circadian system is significantly challenged, as in long distance jet travel or shift work. There is
ambiguity about the relevance of the formal diagnostic criteria to a clinical population since the
criteria have rarely been used in research studies (2,3). In any case, the principles of treatment
are similar whether the symptoms are mild (subclinical) or more severe. The clinician needs
to judge whether a patient meets criteria for a formal diagnosis and decide how aggressive
treatment should be.
182 SACK
for sleep. This misalignment in timing can arise from either exogenous or endogenous factors
(or both). For example, in JLD and SWD, rhythms are misaligned because of an externally
imposed shift in the timing of sleep. In the other CRSDs, misalignment is hypothesized to
involve abnormalities of the circadian system itself; for example, in DSPD the intrinsic circadian
period may be unusually long, and in ASPD, unusually short; or there may be subsensitivity to
the usual circadian time cues. In ISWD associated with dementia, the amplitude of the circadian
signal may be diminished. However, the distinction between exogenous and endogenous factors
is not always sharp; for example, an “owl” (a person with a tendency for DSPS) may have no
problem with insomnia until the requirements of a new job involve going to bed earlier and
getting up earlier.
The opponent process model of sleep regulation, as formulated by Edgar et al. (4), readily
explains the consequences of circadian misalignment. The model postulates that, during the
day, homeostatic sleep drive accumulates in proportion to the duration of prior wakefulness
(Fig. 1). However, the accumulation of sleep drive is not manifest as sleepiness during the day
because it is counteracted (opposed) by a circadian alerting process. As bedtime approaches,
this circadian alerting process wanes, the accumulated sleep drive is unopposed, and normally
a person becomes sleepy and ready for bed. Whether the circadian system actively promotes
sleep at night, or simply does not oppose it, is debated by circadian scientists. During sleep,
the accumulated sleep drive is discharged, and in the morning the cycle begins again. These
homeostatic and circadian processes are normally synchronized with each other and with the
24-hour solar and social day–night cycle.
SL SL
EE Work EE
PD PD
RIV time RIV
E E
GN
GN
SI
SI
G
G
IN IN
RT E RT
E
AL AL
9 AM 3 PM 9 PM 3 AM 9 AM 3 PM 9 PM 3 AM 9 AM
Figure 1 The opponent process model of sleep regulation on a conventional sleep schedule. The opponent
process model proposed by Edgar et al. (4) is illustrated in a double-plotted hypothetical diagram. According to
the model, the level of alertness (sleepiness) is a vector sum derived from the opposing forces of sleep drive,
which accumulates in proportion to the duration of prior wakefulness (shown as a downward force), and an alerting
signal, generated by the circadian pacemaker in the SCN (shown as an upward force). During the day, sleep drive
accumulates, but is counteracted by the opposing alerting signal. In the early evening, the alerting signal peaks
and, even though sleep drive is strong, initiating sleep is difficult. Prior to bedtime, the alerting signal recedes,
sleepiness emerges, sleep commences, and sleep drive dissipates. At the time of final awakening, sleep drive is
at a minimum. After sleep inertia has receded, the daytime level of alertness is restored to a normal zone.
c17 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 13:53 Char Count=
SL
EE SLE
PD EP
RIV DR
E IVE
NESS S
ERT TNES
AL LER
F A
O F
L
L
L
VE
NA
NA
L
VE
LE
IG
IG
LE
S S
NG NG
TI TI
ER ER
AL AL
9 AM 3 PM 9 PM 3 AM 9 AM 3 PM 9 PM 3 AM 9 AM
Figure 2 The opponent process model of sleep regulation in a night shift worker. This diagram illustrates the role
of the opponent process on alertness in a night shift worker that has made no circadian adaptation. Daytime sleep
is undermined and shortened by the circadian alerting process: consequently, overall sleep drive is increased due
to insufficient, nonrestorative sleep. Furthermore, the timing of work is coincident with a recession of the alerting
signal; consequently, the accumulated sleep drive is unopposed by the SCN alerting. With the combination of a
high burden of sleep drive and a lack of circadian alerting, sleep may be very difficult to resist toward the end of
the night or on the drive home.
When homeostatic and circadian processes are out of alignment, the inappropriately timed
circadian alerting process interferes with sleep. Depending on the relationship of the circadian
alerting signal to the timing of attempted sleep, there can be difficulties getting to sleep, staying
asleep, as well as waking up too early. Moreover, during the intervening periods of wake, the
circadian alerting signal is weak or absent, and does not sufficiently counteract the accumulated
sleep drive (or may actively promote sleep) resulting in unwelcome sleepiness and dysphoria.
A multitude of physiological processes are driven by the circadian pacemaker, including
cyclic variations in core body temperature, melatonin, and cortisol secretion, as well as glucose
and lipid metabolism. These abnormalities in circadian synchrony may increase the risk for
cardiovascular and metabolic disorders.
In addition to these endogenous mechanisms, sleep at nonstandard times can be inter-
rupted by ambient noise and light, as well as pressing social obligations. Furthermore, there
is an unavoidable degree of sleep deprivation associated with sudden transitions in the sleep
schedule as occur in SWD and JLD. The various consequences of circadian misalignment as
exemplified in SWD are illustrated in Figure 2 and discussed in the accompanying legend.
DIAGNOSTIC ASSESSMENT
The treatment of CRSDs needs to be preceded by a careful history to rule out other primary
sleep disorders and to characterize behavior patterns (e.g., ill-timed recreational or social activ-
ities) or other factors (e.g., chronic illness, medication side effects, caffeine intake) that may be
exacerbating the problem. Difficulty falling asleep or waking up too early may suggest a CRSD,
but other causes of sleep onset or maintenance insomnia need to be considered, especially
psychophysiological insomnia as well as anxiety and depressive disorders.
c17 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 13:53 Char Count=
184 SACK
A sleep diary is always a useful assessment instrument when a CRSD is suspected. Data
should be collected for at least two weeks and include the times of “light’s off” and “light’s
on,” estimated sleep latency, final awakening, and the recording of medications, alcohol use,
or other factors that influence sleep. Actigraphy, if available, is extremely useful to corroborate
diary data and to provide objective assessment of sleep timing and quality. The Morningness–
Eveningness scale (5) can corroborate the preference for a delayed or advanced sleep schedule,
but cannot replace a clinical interview and evaluation.
An indicator of internal circadian timing (“hands on the clock”) would provide the most
objective way to evaluate the alignment between sleep and circadian phase. However, the two
methods most frequently used in the research setting—the timing of melatonin secretion and
the core body temperature rhythm under constant conditions—have not been applied clinically.
Measuring onset of melatonin secretion in the evening either in serial plasma or saliva samples
(dim light melatonin onset or DLMO) (6) would be feasible in a sleep laboratory, but the
melatonin assay is not readily available as yet. The constant routine protocol (7) required for
valid measurements of core temperature rhythms is very labor intensive and not suitable for
the clinical setting.
individuals) and is least (but not absent) with light exposure in mid-day [reviewed by Duffy
and Wright (8)].
Appropriately timed bright light exposure (3000–10,000 lux) has been shown to produce
robust phase shifts, but even modest intensities (100–550 lux) can produce substantial phase
shifts if subjects have been living in a constant dim light environment. Also, intermittent bright
light exposure can produce almost as much phase shifting as continuous exposure (9). Recently,
specialized nonrod, noncone photoreceptors, associated with the ganglion cells of the retina, that
are maximally sensitive to blue-green light, have been shown to be important for the circadian
phase resetting (10). Clinical trials are underway to determine if exposure to blue-green light
has advantages. Reports of phase shifting with light exposure to the skin (11) have not been
replicated (12,13).
People usually sleep at night, in a dark room, with eyelids closed; thus, the timing of sleep
structures (or “gates”) an individual’s exposure to the light/dark cycle and in this way sleep
can indirectly (but importantly) influence circadian timing. Because sleep and reduced light
exposure occur together, it has been difficult to determine if sleep itself, apart from its gating
effect on light exposure, influences rhythms. Other possible nonphotic time cues, for example,
timed physical activity, may have some influence on circadian rhythms, but are not as potent as
light exposure.
Timed light exposure as a treatment modality usually involves a bright artificial light
source (3000 to 10,000 lux). There have been some safety concerns with light of this intensity,
especially the possibility of phototoxic effects on the lens and/or the retina, but it can be argued
that the intensities are no greater than sunlight on a clear, sunny day. Nevertheless, bright light
sources should be used with caution in patients with ocular pathology (e.g., lenticular cataracts
or retinal degeneration). In early experiments with light therapy “full spectrum” sources that
included UV radiation were employed for light therapy, but UV wavelengths are unnecessary
and should be avoided (14). A diffuser panel placed over the light sources effectively filters UV
radiation.
In summary, the phase-resetting effects of light are dependent on intensity, timing, wave-
length, pattern (intermittent or continuous), duration, exposure history, and the level of contrast
with background light exposure. In clinical practice, it is customary to employ light exposure
from a commercially available light source that generates diffuse illumination with an intensity
of 3000 to 10,000 lux for 30 to 60 minutes, at a time of day that will promote the desired phase-
shifting effect. If light exposure is to be carried out on a regular basis, compliance will be poor
if it is not integrated into some other daily activity (e.g., eating, watching television, reading,
etc.). The light can be indirect; that is, patients do not need to fix their gaze at a light source.
Used in this way, timed bright light exposure appears to be safe within the parameters that have
been tested. Appropriately timed exposure to ordinary daylight, when feasible, can be just as
effective, and is less expensive than an artificial light source.
If the goal is to synchronize the circadian system to the desired (or required) sleep schedule,
appropriately timed light exposure should, in principle, be a helpful intervention for almost
all of the CRSDs, although it may be impractical or even impossible to implement in some
circumstances. Likewise, eliminating (or reducing) the unwanted effects of light on the circadian
system (by staying indoors or wearing goggles) has been shown to inhibit unwanted phase
shifting (15).
186 SACK
Night Day
Phase advance
(reset clock earlier)
Phase delay
(reset clock later)
Light PRC
Melatonin PRC
Phase advance
(reset clock earlier)
Phase delay
(reset clock later)
Endogenous
Melatonin
Profile
12 18 0 6 12
Circadian time
24 6 12 18
Approximate clock time
Figure 3 Schematic phase response curves (PRCs) for light and melatonin administration. The effects of light
and melatonin are dependent on the timing (phase) of administration, a relationship that can be plotted as a
phase response curve (PRC). Light exposure late in the day or melatonin administration in the morning will
cause the circadian clock to shift later (a phase delay); on the other hand, light exposure in the morning or
melatonin administration late in the day will cause the circadian clock to shift earlier (a phase advance). Both light
and melatonin PRCs have an inflection point, the crossover time between advances and delays. According to
convention, circadian time 0 is the beginning of the light phase (daytime) and circadian time 12 is the beginning
of the dark phase (nighttime).
those times of the day when endogenous melatonin is not being secreted. Sleep promoting and
phase-shifting effects may occur concurrently, depending on the timing and dose, and in some
instances, one or the other may be considered undesirable; for example, an early morning dose
of melatonin may promote an intended phase delay but an associated hypnotic effect would
be unwelcome. Likewise, melatonin taken at bedtime for sleep maintenance insomnia related
to an advanced circadian phase might exacerbate the problem by promoting a further phase
advance.
Appropriately timed melatonin and light exposure may have synergistic phase-shifting
effects (17), and melatonin can counteract, to some extent, the effect of light exposure on the
circadian system if light and melatonin are promoting shifts in opposite directions.
A variety of doses of melatonin have been used for phase shifting, and it appears that
the shape of a dose response curve for this effect is rather flat; that is, there is not a great deal
of difference between lower and higher doses. Timing of administration appears to be more
important than dose. Paradoxically, it is possible for a high dose to be less effective than a low
dose. In one report, a low dose (0.5 mg per day) was able to normally entrain a blind person with
free-running rhythms after a high dose (up to 20 mg) had failed (18). The authors suggested
c17 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 13:53 Char Count=
that the higher dose overlapped both the advance and delay portions of the melatonin PRC,
canceling out a phase-resetting effect, while the lower dose targeted just the phase advance
region.
Melatonin is widely available in the United States as a “nutritional supplement” but has
not been approved by the FDA as a drug. Concerns have been raised about the purity of the
available preparations, as well as the reliability of stated doses. Labels that feature a GMP
seal, which stands for Good Manufacturing Practice, provide some assurance of the purity and
accuracy of stated doses.
Commonly available melatonin formulations of 3 mg produce blood levels that are at least
10-fold higher than physiological concentrations; however, no serious adverse reactions have
been attributed to these supra-physiological doses. Recently, a melatonin agonist, ramelteon has
been licensed as a hypnotic in the United States and other melatonin agonists are in development.
Animal studies suggest that ramelteon has phase-shifting effects that are analogous to melatonin
(19) but, at this time, no studies have been reported in humans.
Symptomatic Treatment
Counteracting Insomnia
As discussed above, the circadian pacemaker generates an alerting signal during the day (in
diurnal species) that counteracts the expression of accumulated homeostatic sleep drive (4).
During the normal time for sleep at night, this alerting signal is withdrawn. In CRSDs, there
is a mismatch so that the circadian alerting signal occurs during the desired (or required)
time for sleep, potentially generating insomnia, usually manifested as foreshortened sleep.
Hypnotic drugs or other treatments for insomnia can be used to counteract the unwelcome
clock-dependent alerting in patients with CRSDs.
188 SACK
demands. A tendency for a delayed sleep schedule is very common in adolescence and young
adulthood, but this could reflect life style issues as well as a biological tendency.
DSPD is the most common CRSD diagnosis among patients presenting to a sleep disorders
center; nevertheless, these individuals represent but a small percentage of the patients who
would qualify for the diagnosis that is estimated to be between 360,000 to several million people
in the United States alone (20). The disorder is more common in men than women. Weitzman,
who first described the syndrome (21), originally proposed that a significant number of patients
with a diagnosis of sleep onset insomnia may, in fact, have underlying subsyndromal DSPD.
If true, it would imply that an appreciable subgroup of insomnia patients should respond to
circadian-based interventions.
It is often suggested that patients with DSPS have an intrinsic circadian period that is
longer than average and that is why they have such difficulty resetting their circadian pace-
maker (and sleep schedule) to an earlier time frame; however, other mechanisms; such as a
subsensitivity to the phase-advancing effects of light could explain the disorder. Okawa and
Uchiyama have recently published a detailed review of possible mechanisms (22).
the DLMO and CBT rhythm. On further analysis, the earlier the melatonin was administered
relative to DLMO, the larger was the phase advance.
The optimal time for melatonin administration would be at the peak of the phase advance
portion of the melatonin PRC, but until circadian marker such as the DLMO becomes clinically
available, appropriate timing has to be estimated. In our clinic, we provide the patient with a
schedule (like the one for light therapy) that initiates treatment (0.5 to 3 mg) at about three to
four hours prior to habitual bedtime, and then gradually shifts the timing of both melatonin
administration and bedtime earlier (15 to 30 minutes every few days).
Combination Treatment
In our clinic, evening melatonin and morning bright light are often prescribed in tandem,
utilizing a schedule (provided by a computer-generated worksheet) that gradually advances
the timing of attempted sleep and the timing of both treatments by 15 to 30 minutes every
few days. These gradual shifts in schedule are based on an understanding that large circadian
“phase jumps” are difficult to achieve, especially phase advances, and therefore attempting
to shift sleep times too quickly may lead to failure and the abandonment of treatment. Once
the desired schedule is accomplished, maintenance treatment with light and melatonin can be
continued on a stable, fixed schedule.
Many patients with DSPS have a strong personal preference for being awake late at night
(e.g., they may enjoy the solitude at this time of day) and unless there is a serious consequence
such as failing in school or losing a job, they may not want to change their sleep schedule.
Consequently, cultivating patient motivation is important for the success of any treatment
for DSPD. With adolescents, a great deal of tension has often developed with their parents
regarding sleep schedules. In order to reduce unproductive blaming, an effort may be needed to
educate patients and their parents about the factors that regulate circadian rhythms and sleep,
attempting to frame the problem in a more dispassionate way.
190 SACK
Combination Treatment
As with DSPD, a combination of the treatments can be employed as there are no known adverse
interactions.
with recruited volunteers as subjects. Therefore, the precise nature and boundaries of this
disorder remain rather undefined.
The degree to which night workers reset their circadian rhythms to match their daytime
sleep schedule appears to be quite variable and may depend on a number of factors, especially
baseline circadian phase and the pattern of light exposure. In general, realignment of rhythms
so they are congruent with a day sleep schedule improves the quantity and quality of sleep (36),
although one study of actual shift workers found an unexpectedly weak correlation between
sleep quality and work satisfaction (38). Even if phase congruence could be accomplished, some
night workers would find it undesirable because they would be out of phase on their days off.
192 SACK
responders; that is, they shifted with melatonin treatment but not with placebo. In summary, it
appears that some night workers do not need treatment, others fail melatonin treatment, and
some respond specifically to melatonin treatment.
Melatonin given just prior to daytime sleep may have both a direct hypnotic effect (perhaps
by counteracting the unwelcome daytime circadian alerting signal) as well as a circadian phase-
resetting effect. To date, there have been no studies comparing melatonin to a standard hypnotic
medication for daytime sleep.
Counteracting Insomnia
Hypnotic medications have been shown to promote daytime sleep in several simulated shift
work studies (45,46). However, it was somewhat surprising that the increase in total sleep time
did not necessarily counteract the circadian-mediated dip in nighttime alertness, as measured
with MSLT. In another study, treatment did not improve nighttime alertness as assessed by
MSLT, but did improve scores on the Maintenance of Wakefulness Test (MWT), suggesting a
differential effect on these two dimensions of sleepiness (47).
There is little question that hypnotic medications can lengthen daytime sleep in night
workers, but there is not yet a consensus as to when such treatment would be appropriate (or
inappropriate). For short runs or occasional night shifts, a short course of hypnotic medication
would seem consistent with the generally accepted guidelines for their use. The use of hypnotics
for permanent night workers is more controversial.
in Europe than in the United States. In order to minimize sleep inertia, naps should be kept
relatively brief.
Combination Treatment
Given the wide variety shift work schedules and the various desires of patients, treatment will
need to be tailored to meet the needs of the individual worker. For an occasional overnight duty
(e.g., 24-hour duty in the Emergency Room), phase resetting would not be possible or desirable.
A stimulant medication to maintain alertness during the shift and perhaps a hypnotic for
daytime sleep might be indicated. For workers on a steady night shift (who maintain a relative
nocturnal orientation on the their days off), promoting phase resetting with light exposure or
melatonin would be more logical. For a worker who is at risk for costly mistakes (e.g., a nuclear
power plant supervisor) more aggressive treatment may be required. Recently, Eastman et al.
have shown that using light treatment to reset rhythms to a compromise phase, along with
prescribed sleep scheduling, in between a night work and conventional schedule, may be the
best way to maximize sleep and alertness for both work days and days off. Some people are
very intolerant of shift work and may need a medical authorization to be excused.
Melatonin Administration
The benefits of melatonin for jet lag have been demonstrated in a number of double-blind,
placebo-controlled studies (55). Improvement in sleep, accelerated phase shifting, and a decrease
in jet lag symptoms have been reported in various studies. Doses ranging from 0.5 to 10 mg
for up to three days prior to departure and up to five days upon arrival at the destination
c17 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 13:53 Char Count=
194 SACK
have been employed. Most of these studies have tested melatonin for eastward flight, when a
bedtime dose would promote a desired phase advance and might have a hypnotic effect as well.
With westward flight melatonin should be taken (according to the melatonin PRC) upon early
morning awakening in order to facilitate phase delays; if taken at bedtime, it could stimulate
the advance portion of the melatonin PRC and thereby inhibit clock resetting in the desired
delay direction. In order to minimize a sleep-promoting effect, a morning dose should be low;
for example, 0.5 mg.
Counteracting Insomnia
JLD-related insomnia is the result, not only of circadian misalignment, but also of attempting to
sleep in an upright, sometimes noisy, airplane seat, and later, in an unfamiliar hotel bed. Because
JLD-insomnia is self-limited, a short course of hypnotic medication can be readily justified. A few
studies that have tested hypnotics for this indication and they were shown to be generally safe
and effective (56). Occasional adverse events have been reported; for example, global amnesia
following the use of triazolam (and some alcohol) during flight (57). Also, hypnotic use during
a flight could increase immobility and raise the risk for deep vein thrombosis.
Combination Treatment
Melatonin, for its phase-resetting effect, and a hypnotic medication, for insomnia, could be taken
together as there are no known adverse pharmacological interactions. The hypnotic effects might
be additive.
Combination Treatment
Non–24-hour CRSD may be an extreme form of DSPS and similar treatment strategies can be
tried; for example, morning light exposure, evening melatonin administration, and intermittent
hypnotics (as described above).
196 SACK
were provided training in behavior management skills. A control group received education
about dementia and caregiver support. Sleep was monitored actigraphically. The active treat-
ment group had fewer nighttime awakenings and a reduction in total wake time during the
night. These benefits persisted to six-month follow-up.
REFERENCES
1. Evans R. Letter to the editor: practicing under the influence of fatigue [comment]. Adv Neonatal Care
2006; 6(2):61–62.
2. Sack RL, Auckley D, Auger RR, et al. Circadian rhythm sleep disorders: part II, advanced sleep phase
disorder, delayed sleep phase disorder, free-running disorder, and irregular sleep-wake rhythm. An
American Academy of Sleep Medicine review. Sleep 2007; 30(11):1484–1501.
3. Sack RL, Auckley D, Auger RR, et al. Circadian rhythm sleep disorders: part I, basic principles, shift
work and jet lag disorders. An American Academy of Sleep Medicine review. Sleep 2007; 30(11):1460–
1483.
4. Edgar DM, Dement WC, Fuller CA. Effect of SCN lesions on sleep in squirrel monkeys: evidence for
opponent processes in sleep-wake regulation. J Neurosci 1993; 13(3):1065–1079.
5. Horne JA, Ostberg O. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in
human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol 1976; 4(2):97–110.
6. Lewy AJ, Sack RL. The dim light melatonin onset as a marker for circadian phase position. Chronobiol
Int 1989; 6(1):93–102.
7. Czeisler CA, Duffy JF, Shanahan TL, et al. Stability, precision, and near-24-hour period of the human
circadian pacemaker. Science 1999; 284(5423):2177–2181.
8. Duffy JF, Wright KP Jr. Entrainment of the human circadian system by light. J Biol Rhythms 2005;
20(4):326–338.
9. Gronfier C, Wright KP Jr, Kronauer RE, et al. Efficacy of a single sequence of intermittent bright light
pulses for delaying circadian phase in humans. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 2004; 287(1):E174–E181.
10. Hankins MW, Peirson SN, Foster RG. Melanopsin: an exciting photopigment. Trends Neurosci 2008;
31(1):27–36.
11. Campbell SS, Murphy PJ. Extraocular circadian phototransduction in humans. Science 1998;
279(5349):396–399.
12. Eastman CI, Martin SK, Hebert M. Failure of extraocular light to facilitate circadian rhythm reentrain-
ment in humans. Chronobiol Int 2000; 17(6):807.
13. Wright KP Jr, Czeisler CA. Absence of circadian phase resetting in response to bright light behind the
knees. Science 2002; 297(5581):571.
14. Reme CE, Rol P, Grothmann K, et al. Bright light therapy in focus: lamp emission spectra and ocular
safety. Technol Health Care 1996; 4(4):403–413.
15. Revell VL, Eastman CI. How to trick mother nature into letting you fly around or stay up all night. J
Biol Rhythms 2005; 20(4):353–365.
16. Lewy AJ, Ahmed S, Jackson JM, et al. Melatonin shifts human circadian rhythms according to a
phase-response curve. Chronobiol Int 1992; 9(5):380–392.
c17 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 13:53 Char Count=
17. Revell VL, Burgess HJ, Gazda CJ, et al. Advancing human circadian rhythms with afternoon melatonin
and morning intermittent bright light. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2006; 91(1):54–59.
18. Lewy AJ, Emens JS, Sack RL, et al. Low, but not high, doses of melatonin entrained a free-running
blind person with a long circadian period. Chronobiol Int 2002; 19(3):649.
19. Hirai K, Kita M, Ohta H, et al. Ramelteon (TAK-375) accelerates reentrainment of circadian rhythm
after a phase advance of the light-dark cycle in rats. J Biol Rhythms 2005; 20(1):27–37.
20. Wyatt JK. Delayed sleep phase syndrome: pathophysiology and treatment options. Sleep 2004;
27(6):1195.
21. Weitzman ED, Czeisler CA, Coleman RM, et al. Delayed sleep phase syndrome. A chronobiological
disorder with sleep-onset insomnia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1981; 38(7):737.
22. Okawa M, Uchiyama M. Circadian rhythm sleep disorders: characteristics and entrainment pathology
in delayed sleep phase and non-24-h sleep-wake syndrome. Sleep Med Rev 2007; 11(6):485–496.
23. Rosenthal NE, Joseph-Vanderpool JR, Levendosky AA, et al. Phase-shifting effects of bright morning
light as treatment for delayed sleep phase syndrome. Sleep 1990; 13(4):354.
24. Dahlitz M, Alvarez B, Vignau J, et al. Delayed sleep phase syndrome response to melatonin. Lancet
1991; 337(8750):1121.
25. Dagan Y, Yovel I, Hallis D, et al. Evaluating the role of melatonin in the long-term treatment of delayed
sleep phase syndrome (DSPS). Chronobiol Int 1998; 15(2):181.
26. Kayumov L, Brown G, Jindal R, et al. A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled crossover study
of the effect of exogenous melatonin on delayed sleep phase syndrome. Psychosom Med 2001; 63(1):40.
27. Czeisler CA, Richardson GS, Coleman RM, et al. Chronotherapy: resetting the circadian clocks of
patients with delayed sleep phase insomnia. Sleep 1981; 4(1):1–21.
28. Jones CR, Campbell SS, Zone SE, et al. Familial advanced sleep-phase syndrome: a short-period
circadian rhythm variant in humans [see comment]. Nat Med 1999; 5(9):1062.
29. Toh KL, Jones CR, He Y, et al. An hPer2 phosphorylation site mutation in familial advanced sleep
phase syndrome. Science 2001; 291(5506):1040.
30. Dijk DJ, Duffy JF. Circadian regulation of human sleep and age-related changes in its timing, consoli-
dation and EEG characteristics. Ann Med 1999; 31(2):130–140.
31. Carrier J, Paquet J, Morettini J, et al. Phase advance of sleep and temperature circadian rhythms in the
middle years of life in humans. Neurosci Lett 2002; 320(1–2):1–4.
32. Campbell SS, Dawson D, Anderson MW. Alleviation of sleep maintenance insomnia with timed
exposure to bright light. J Am Geriatr Soc 1993; 41(8):829–836.
33. Suhner AG, Murphy PJ, Campbell SS. Failure of timed bright light exposure to alleviate age-related
sleep maintenance insomnia. J Am Geriatr Soc 2002; 50(4):617–623.
34. Lack L, Wright H, Kemp K, et al. The treatment of early-morning awakening insomnia with 2 evenings
of bright light [see comment]. Sleep 2005; 28(5):616–623.
35. Moldofsky H, Musisi S, Phillipson EA. Treatment of a case of advanced sleep phase syndrome by
phase advance chronotherapy. Sleep 1986; 9(1):61.
36. Boivin DB, Tremblay GM, James FO. Working on atypical schedules. Sleep Med 2007; 8(6):
578–589.
37. Ohayon MM, Lemoine P, Arnaud-Briant V, et al. Prevalence and consequences of sleep disorders in a
shift worker population. J Psychosom Res 2002; 53(1):577.
38. Roden M, Koller M, Pirich K, et al. The circadian melatonin and cortisol secretion pattern in permanent
night shift workers. Am J Physiol 1993; 265(1, pt 2):R261–R267.
39. Burgess HJ, Sharkey KM, Eastman CI. Bright light, dark and melatonin can promote circadian adap-
tation in night shift workers. Sleep Med Rev 2002; 6(5):407–420.
40. Crowley SJ, Lee C, Tseng CY, et al. Complete or partial circadian re-entrainment improves performance,
alertness, and mood during night-shift work. Sleep 2004; 27(6):1077–1087.
41. Boivin DB, James FO. Circadian adaptation to night-shift work by judicious light and darkness expo-
sure. J Biol Rhythms 2002; 17(6):556.
42. Sharkey KM, Eastman CI. Melatonin phase shifts human circadian rhythms in a placebo-controlled
simulated night-work study. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 2002; 282(2):R454.
43. Smith L, Tanigawa T, Takahashi M, et al. Shiftwork locus of control, situational and behavioural effects
on sleepiness and fatigue in shiftworkers. Ind Health 2005; 43(1):151–170.
44. Sack RL, Lewy AJ. Melatonin as a chronobiotic: treatment of circadian desynchrony in night workers
and the blind. J Biol Rhythms 1997; 12(6):595–603.
45. Hart CL, Ward AS, Haney M, et al. Zolpidem-related effects on performance and mood during
simulated night-shift work. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2003; 11(4):259.
46. Monchesky TC, Billings BJ, Phillips R, et al. Zopiclone in insomniac shiftworkers. Evaluation of its
hypnotic properties and its effects on mood and work performance. Int Arch Occup Environ Health
1989; 61(4):255.
c17 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 13:53 Char Count=
198 SACK
47. Porcu S, Bellatreccia A, Ferrara M, et al. Performance, ability to stay awake, and tendency to fall asleep
during the night after a diurnal sleep with temazepam or placebo. Sleep 1997; 20(7):535.
48. Czeisler CA, Walsh JK, Roth T, et al. Modafinil for excessive sleepiness associated with shift-work
sleep disorder [see comment]. N Engl J Med 2005; 353(5):476–486.
49. McLellan TM, Bell DG, Kamimori GH. Caffeine improves physical performance during 24 h of active
wakefulness. Aviat Space Environ Med 2004; 75(8):666–672.
50. Wyatt JK, Cajochen C, Ritz-De Cecco A, et al. Low-dose repeated caffeine administration for circadian-
phase-dependent performance degradation during extended wakefulness. Sleep 2004; 27(3):374.
51. Czeisler CA, Moore-Ede MC, Coleman RH. Rotating shift work schedules that disrupt sleep are
improved by applying circadian principles. Science 1982; 217(4558):460–463.
52. Daan S, Lewy AJ. Scheduled exposure to daylight: a potential strategy to reduce “jet lag” following
transmeridian flight. Psychopharmacol Bull 1984; 20(3):566–568.
53. Waterhouse J, Reilly T, Atkinson G, et al. Jet lag: trends and coping strategies. Lancet 2007;
369(9567):1117–1129.
54. Eastman CI, Gazda CJ, Burgess HJ, et al. Advancing circadian rhythms before eastward flight: a
strategy to prevent or reduce jet lag. Sleep 2005; 28(1):33–44.
55. Herxheimer A. Jet lag. Clin Evid 2005; 13:2178–2183 [update of Clin Evid 2004; 12:2394–2400; PMID:
15865798].
56. Jamieson AO, Zammit GK, Rosenberg RS, et al. Zolpidem reduces the sleep disturbance of jet lag.
Sleep Med 2001; 2(5):423–430.
57. Morris HH III, Estes ML. Traveler’s amnesia. Transient global amnesia secondary to triazolam. JAMA
1987; 258(7):945–946.
58. Pierard C, Beaumont M, Enslen M, et al. Resynchronization of hormonal rhythms after an eastbound
flight in humans: effects of slow-release caffeine and melatonin. Eur J Appl Physiol 2001; 85(1–2):144.
59. Sack RL, Lewy AJ, Blood ML, et al. Circadian rhythm abnormalities in totally blind people: incidence
and clinical significance. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1992; 75(1):127–134.
60. Lockley SW, Skene DJ, James K, et al. Melatonin administration can entrain the free-running circadian
system of blind subjects. J Endocrinol 2000; 164(1):R1–R6.
61. Sack RL, Brandes RW, Kendall AR, et al. Entrainment of free-running circadian rhythms by melatonin
in blind people. N Engl J Med 2000; 343(15):1070–1077.
62. Lewy AJ, Bauer VK, Hasler BP, et al. Capturing the circadian rhythms of free-running blind people
with 0.5 mg melatonin. Brain Res 2001; 918(1–2):96.
63. Hack LM, Lockley SW, Arendt J, et al. The effects of low-dose 0.5-mg melatonin on the free-running
circadian rhythms of blind subjects. J Biol Rhythms 2003; 18(5):420–429.
64. Singer C, Tractenberg RE, Kaye J, et al. A multicenter, placebo-controlled trial of melatonin for sleep
disturbance in Alzheimer’s disease. Sleep 2003; 26(7):893.
65. Jan JE, Freeman RD. Melatonin therapy for circadian rhythm sleep disorders in children with multiple
disabilities: what have we learned in the last decade? [see comment]. Dev Med Child Neurol 2004;
46(11):776–782.
66. McArthur AJ, Budden SS. Sleep dysfunction in Rett syndrome: a trial of exogenous melatonin treat-
ment. Dev Med Child Neurol 1998; 40(3):186.
67. McCurry SM, Gibbons LE, Logsdon RG, et al. Nighttime insomnia treatment and education for
Alzheimer’s disease: a randomized, controlled trial. J Am Geriatr Soc 2005; 53(5):793–802.
c18 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:51 Char Count=
Definition
Restless legs syndrome (RLS) is a major cause of comorbid insomnia. It is characterized by
four basic symptoms: an urge to move the legs, usually accompanied by unpleasant sensations;
worsening of symptoms by rest; relief of symptoms by activity; and worsening of symptoms in
the evening or night (1,2). The unpleasant sensations are typically described as creepy-crawly
or worm-like, but patients use varied descriptors and not uncommonly find it hard to label
the quality of the discomfort. A minority will describe the sensations as painful and some will
experience only the need to move without associated sensations. Usually the symptoms are
experienced bilaterally, but one limb may predominate, with discomfort sometimes alternating
between different sides. In some patients the symptoms may also be experienced in other areas
of the body, especially the arms (3). The urge to move is precipitated by physical rest such
as sitting or lying down and may be especially severe in prolonged situations of enforced
quiescence, such as traveling in a car or plane or sitting in a theater. Reduced alertness may
enhance the severity of RLS and, conversely, stimulating mental activities may help alleviate
the discomfort. Activities such as walking, stretching or bicycling result in relief but symptoms
recommence after the activity is discontinued. Soaking or massaging the affected limb may also
provide temporary relief. The characteristic circadian rhythmicity of RLS results in symptoms
frequently being most severe in bed, either before sleep onset or on waking during the night.
Diagnostic Assessment
The diagnosis of RLS can be made in most patients by obtaining a careful history and diagnostic
testing is not generally needed. Polysomnography to detect PLMS has low sensitivity and speci-
ficity and immobilization tests (12) to detect periodic limb movements of wakefulness are rarely
used clinically. Other potentially confusing conditions have different clinical features, usually
allowing easy differentiation. Nocturnal leg cramps abruptly awaken the patient with severe
pain in a palpably contracted muscle. Arthritis pain is localized to joints and the discomfort of
a sensory peripheral neuropathy is predominantly felt in the feet. While neurogenic pain may
be worse at night or at rest, it is not generally associated with an urge to move. Fibromyalgia is
c18 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:51 Char Count=
200 SILBER
minimum of twice weekly (17). Sleep-related symptoms were noted by 75.5% of patients, with
48.1% describing difficulty falling asleep, 39.2% difficulty staying asleep, 60.6% experiencing
disturbed or interrupted sleep and 40.1% insufficient sleep. The most troublesome symptom
was listed as sleep disturbance by 37.8% of patients.
Several controlled, population-based studies of RLS symptoms have been reported. A
study of 1000 randomly selected adults from Norway and 1005 from Denmark revealed a
prevalence of RLS of 11.5% (38). Of those subjects indicating that insomnia was usually or
always present, 23% had RLS compared to 9.3% of those indicating insomnia to be sometimes
or never present. Multivariate analysis demonstrated that RLS was significantly associated
with insomnia that was always present (odds ratio 2.75), usually present (odds ratio 3.16) and
sometimes present (odds ratio 1.71). Depressive mood did not correlate significantly with the
presence of RLS. Of 1000 randomly selected Swedish adults, 5% were diagnosed with RLS
(39). Symptoms of insomnia were described by 51% of the RLS patients compared to 24.3%
of the controls (p = 0.0001), whereas depressed mood was present in 18% of the RLS patients
compared to 6.7% of the controls (p = 0.01). As part of the 2005 National Sleep Foundation Poll,
1506 randomly selected United States adults were interviewed. RLS was identified in 9.7% of
the sample (40). Compared to controls, RLS subjects were more likely to sleep <6 hours a night,
to endorse symptoms of insomnia, to stay up longer than they planned more than a few nights
a week and to take >30 minutes to fall asleep.
A few polysomnographic (PSG) studies of RLS patients have addressed the question of
sleep disruption. In an uncontrolled study of 133 consecutive patients with RLS (4), the mean
PSG sleep latency was 20.5 minutes and the mean sleep efficiency was 75%. A mean of 7.4
awakenings per night greater than two minutes was reported. The PSG findings correlated with
the patients’ subjective reports. The mean sleep latency for those complaining of difficulty falling
asleep was 22.9 minutes compared to 10.6 minutes for those reporting no initial insomnia. The
mean sleep efficiency was 73.2% for those complaining of difficulty maintaining sleep compared
to 86.4% for those reporting no sleep maintenance problems. The periodic limb movement index
did not, however, correlate with patients’ complaints of sleep onset or maintenance insomnia, the
number of objective awakenings or sleep efficiency, suggesting that periodic limb movements
are not the primary cause of the sleep disturbances in RLS patients. In a study of the effects
of ropinirole on moderate to severe RLS, baseline PSG data was reported for 59 patients (41).
Mean preintervention sleep efficiency was 81.2% for the drug group and 81.9% for the placebo
group. However, preintervention mean sleep latency was relatively short: only 16.7 minutes for
the drug group and 8.9 minutes for the placebo group.
A small controlled PSG study of 12 RLS patients compared to 12 controls showed increased
wake time after sleep onset (mean 92.4 minutes compared to 36.2 minutes), reduced sleep
efficiency (73.2% compared to 86.6%), shorter total sleep time (326.3 minutes compared to
383.3 minutes) and more awakenings (12.2 compared to 7.4) (42). A larger controlled PSG
study of 45 RLS patients and controls showed that the patients had significantly reduced
sleep efficiency (80% compared to 87.6%), higher arousal index (23.4 compared to 12.4), more
awakenings (26.8 compared to 20.8) and more wake after sleep onset (15.6% compared to 7.9%)
(43). Percentage stages 2 and REM sleep were significantly reduced in the RLS group and REM
latency was prolonged. Sleep onset latency did not differ between the groups, but latency to 10
minutes persistent sleep was significantly longer in the RLS subjects (41.6 minutes compared to
25.4 minutes).
In summary, questionnaire and PSG studies show conclusively that untreated RLS causes
serious disruption of sleep. In population based studies, approximately one half to three quar-
ters of RLS patients describe sleep-related symptoms, with higher percentages reported in
studies based on primary care or specialist practices. Both sleep onset and sleep mainte-
nance difficulties are described. In objective PSG studies, sleep maintenance difficulties are
confirmed, with reduced sleep efficiency together with increased arousals and awakenings.
Conventionally calculated sleep latency is less consistently prolonged but latency to sustained
sleep lengthens, suggesting that many patients fall asleep rapidly but wake shortly thereafter
with recurrence of symptoms. While there is limited data on the role of periodic limb move-
ments, one study suggests that they do not significantly contribute to the sleep disturbances of
RLS (4).
c18 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:51 Char Count=
202 SILBER
adequate absorption. In particular, the drugs should not be given at bedtime to patients whose
symptoms are maximal in bed before sleep onset. If the initial dose is given in the afternoon
or early in the evening, a second dose before bed may be needed. In patients who have both
RLS and primary insomnia, a benzodiazepine agonist may be helpful, either as a first-line or
supplementary drug. Similarly, gabapentin is generally sedating and may be helpful in these
circumstances.
204 SILBER
presence nor severity of PLMS (68). No association was found between sleep quality assessed
on a standardized questionnaire and the PLMS index in 78 patients with PLMS (narcolepsy
and sleep apnea patients excluded). There was also no association detected in a subgroup of 22
patients diagnosed with primary insomnia (69). A study of 34 patients with daytime sleepiness
and PLMS showed no correlation between the PLMS index and mean latencies on a multiple
sleep latency test (70).
Similar results have been found in studies of normal subjects. In a study of 100 com-
munity dwelling older subjects, the presence of PLMS was not significantly associated with
complaints of insomnia or sleepiness, nor with any abnormal sleep log measurements (11).
However, in a similar study of 420 subjects, PLMS were associated with complaints of reduced
sleep satisfaction (9). Seventy healthy middle-aged subjects were assessed by PSG and sleep
questionnaires. There was no association between PLMS severity and any PSG measure, includ-
ing sleep latency and sleep efficiency. Sleep quality assessed with the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality
Index was significantly lower in men with higher numbers of PLMS but not for women (71).
Another approach to understanding the significance of PLMS comes from analysis of
arousals. In a detailed study of 10 patients complaining of insomnia or sleepiness, including
six with RLS, the relationship between PLMS and arousal phenomena was assessed. Of 3916
EEG arousals occurring 10 seconds before or after the onset of a PLMS, 49.2% occurred before
leg movements, 30.6% simultaneous with leg movements and 23.2% after leg movements.
Alpha activity was significantly higher during the 10 seconds before movements compared to
the 10 seconds after movements. These data suggest that PLMS may be manifestations of an
underlying arousal disorder rather than the primary cause (72). Six patients with RLS were
treated with levodopa or placebo and the effect on PLMS and arousals assessed. Levodopa
resulted in a significant reduction in the PLMS index compared to placebo but the frequency of K-
alpha complexes remained unchanged, suggesting that the leg movements were not responsible
for the arousals (73). Transient increases in heart rate (74,75) and changes in EEG theta and delta
activity (74) have been reported following PLMS, even when unassociated with EEG arousals.
However, the clinical relevance, if any, of these physiologic changes has not been established.
Thus, in summary, there is little data to suggest that PLMS alone play an important role
in causing insomnia or sleepiness in the absence of other sleep disorders. PLMD appears to
be a rare condition and should be diagnosed with care. A sustained response of the primary
complaint of insomnia or hypersomnia with the use of dopaminergic agonists can result in
increased confidence in the correctness of the initial diagnosis (62). However, while it may
seem intuitive to treat PLMD with drugs known to improve RLS and the associated PLMS, no
controlled clinical trials of any agents for pure PLMD have been reported (76), apart from studies
using clonazepam (77) and ropinirole (78) for a single night compared to a night with placebo.
SLEEP-RELATED BRUXISM
Sleep-related bruxism (tooth grinding or clenching) is associated with either tonic contraction
of the masseter muscle or rhythmic masticatory muscle activity (RMMA), a series of repetitive
contractions at approximately 1 Hz each lasting 0.25 to 2 seconds (57). Bruxism can occur in any
stage of sleep, but occurs most frequently in light NREM sleep. Bruxism is common, but in order
to qualify as a disorder, not only must the patient report or be aware of tooth grinding sounds or
tooth clenching in the night, but one or more clinical consequence must occur. According to the
second edition of the ICSD, these are abnormal wear of the teeth; jaw muscle discomfort, fatigue
or pain and jaw lock on awakening; and masseter muscle hypertrophy. In adults the prevalence
of sleep related tooth grinding has been estimated at 8% in a Canadian sample of 2,019 subjects,
with prevalence dropping from 13% at ages 18 to 29 years to 3% at ages 60 years and older (13).
In a European sample of 13,057 subjects, the prevalence of sleep-related bruxism was 8.8% in
women and 7.5% in men (79). Prevalence was 5.5% in the 15 to 18 year age group, 8.8% to 10.5%
in adults aged 19 to 64 years and 3% in subjects older than 64 years. It should be understood
that these figures (13,79) reflect the motor phenomenon alone and not its consequences. In the
European study, the prevalence of bruxism as a disorder using the ICSD (second edition) criteria
was also assessed (79). Overall prevalence was 4.6% in women and 4.1% in men with the lowest
prevalence (1.1%) in subjects older than 64 years and maximal prevalence in those age 19 to 44
years (5.8%).
c18 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:51 Char Count=
It should be noted that the ICSD criteria for bruxism do not include any resultant changes
in sleep or wakefulness. In an age and gender matched PSG study of 18 patients with bruxism
and 18 controls, there was no significant difference in total sleep time, sleep efficiency, sleep
latency or arousals between the two groups (80). In a similar study of six patients and six
controls, again no significant differences in total sleep time, wake time after sleep onset or
sleep latency were noted (81). A further PSG study of 10 patients and 10 controls also failed to
detect any differences between sleep duration, sleep efficiency and arousals (82). In a European
study of 13,057 subjects, the disorder of sleep bruxism was not associated with insomnia disorder
diagnoses but tooth grinding was associated with perception of disrupted sleep in a multivariate
model (79). In a study of 917 subjects, mostly shift workers, frequent occurrence of bruxism was
associated with symptoms of difficulty initiating sleep and disrupted sleep (83) Thus, there is no
evidence that bruxism, either as a motor phenomenon or an arbitrarily defined disorder, causes
significant insomnia. It is possible that there may be an association with perceived disrupted
sleep, but other data suggests that bruxism may actually be the result of sleep fragmentation
rather than its cause.
In a study of 10 patients with bruxism, a significant increase in EEG alpha band activity
was seen in a four second window preceding the onset of RRMA (82). Similarly, during the
10 heart beats preceding onset of RRMA, the mean heart rate significantly increased. These
findings suggest that a microarousal occurred before the start of an episode of bruxism and thus
may have been instrumental in its causation. Similarly, in a study of six patients with bruxism,
EMG activity commenced during phase A of the cyclic alternating pattern and especially during
subtype A3 (81). Phase A indicates a state of arousal and subtype A3 specifically suggests the
presence of microarousals. The results of these studies match with the clinical observation that
episodes of bruxism sometimes occur with arousals from episodes of obstructive sleep apnea.
Because the major consequences of bruxism are related to dental damage and not to sleep
disruption, management usually involve the use of oral appliances rather than medications.
206 SILBER
some neuromuscular disorders, idiopathic cramps are common, especially in older persons (87).
Their pathophysiology is uncertain. Leg cramps, when frequent, can cause clinically relevant
arousals and may precipitate periods of sleep maintenance insomnia. However, no systematic
studies of these potential consequences have been reported.
Treatment of leg cramps can be challenging. While quinine has been recommended, con-
trolled trials have shown at most modest benefits. A meta-analysis showed a relative risk
reduction of only 21% with frequent side effects reported, especially tinnitus (88). The potential
for more serious complications of therapy, such as thrombocytopenia and cardiac arrhythmias,
should also be noted. Gabapentin and verapamil have been suggested as alternative possible
therapies, but adequate trials have not been performed (89).
SUMMARY
Restless legs syndrome is a common disorder and is one of the most important causes of
comorbid insomnia. It can usually be diagnosed clinically with a careful history. The pathogen-
esis is not fully understood, but involves genetic factors, low brain iron stores and dopamine
deficiency. Population studies show that 50% to 75% of RLS patients complain of insomnia,
confirmed by PSG studies showing reduced sleep efficiency, increased arousals and prolonged
latency to sustained sleep. Appropriate treatment of RLS can be a vital component in managing
insomnia.
Periodic limb movements of sleep accompany RLS but probably play little role in the
sleep disturbances. True periodic limb movement disorder is uncommon and, in the absence
of other disorders, rarely results in insomnia. Bruxism, while common, has not been shown
to cause sleep disruption, but may sometimes occur as a motor phenomenon accompanying
arousals from other causes. Rhythmic movement disorder consists of rhythmic movements of
large muscle groups but there is little evidence that the movements cause sleep disruption. On
the contrary, they may be seen as a sleep induction technique by patients with other causes of
insomnia. Sleep related leg cramps are little understood and their severe pain can disrupt sleep.
Treatment can be challenging.
REFERENCES
1. Allen RP, Picchietti D, Hening WA, et al. Restless legs syndrome: Diagnostic criteria, special consid-
erations, and epidemiology. A report from the restless legs syndrome diagnosis and epidemiology
workshop at the National Institutes of health. Sleep Med 2003; 4:101–119.
2. American Academy of Sleep Medicine. Sleep Related Movement Disorders. International Classifica-
tion of Sleep disorders, 2nd ed. Diagnostic and Coding Manual. Westchester, IL: American Academy
of Sleep Medicine, 2005.
3. Michaud M, Chabli A, Lavigne G, et al. Arm restlessness in patients with restless legs syndrome. Mov
Disord 2000; 15:289–293.
4. Montplaisir J, Boucher S, Poirier G, et al. Clinical, polysomnographic, and genetic characteristics of
restless legs syndrome: A study of 133 patients diagnosed with new standard criteria. Mov Disord
1997; 12:61–65.
5. Fry JM, DiPhillipo MA, Pressman MR. Periodic leg movements in sleep following treatment of obstruc-
tive sleep apnea with nasal continuous positive airway pressure. Chest 1989; 96:89–91.
6. Montplaisir J, Michaud M, Denesle R, et al. Periodic leg movements are not more prevalent in insom-
nia or hypersomnia but are specifically associated with sleep disorders involving a dopaminergic
mechanism. Sleep Med 2000; 1:163–167.
7. Fantini ML, Michaud M, Gosselin N, et al. Periodic leg movements in REM sleep behavior disorder
and related autonomic and EEG activation. Neurology 2002; 59:1889–1894.
8. Wetter TC, Collado-Seidel V, Pollmacher T, et al. Sleep and periodic leg movement patterns in
drug-free patients with Parkinson’s disease and multiple system atrophy. Sleep 2000; 23:361–
367.
9. Ancoli-Israel S, Kripke DF, Klauber MR, et al. Periodic limb movements in sleep in community-
dwelling elderly. Sleep 1991; 14(6):496–500.
10. Bixler EO. Nocturnal myoclonus and nocturnal myoclonic activity in a normal population. Res Com-
mun Chem Path Pharmacol 1982; 36:129–140.
11. Dickel MJ, Mosko SS. Morbidity cut-offs for sleep apnea and periodic leg movements in predicting
subjective complaints in seniors. Sleep 1990; 13:155–166.
12. Montplaisir J, Boucher S, Nicolas A, et al. Immobilization tests and periodic leg movements in sleep
for the diagnosis of restless leg syndrome. Mov Disord 1998; 13:324–329.
c18 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:51 Char Count=
13. Lavigne GJ, Montplaisir JY. Restless legs syndrome and sleep bruxism: Prevalence and association
among Canadians. Sleep 1994; 17:739–743.
14. Phillips B, Young T, Finn L, et al. Epidemiology of restless legs symptoms in adults. Arch Int Med
2000; 160:2137–2141.
15. Tippmann-Peikert M, Silber MH, Park JP, et al. Pathologic gambling in patients with restless leg
syndrome treated with dopaminergic agnostics. Neurology 2007; 68: 301–303.
16. Hogl B, Kiechl S, Willeit J, et al. Restless legs syndrome: A community-based study of prevalence,
severity and risk factors. Neurology 2005; 64:1920–1924.
17. Allen RP, Walters AS, Montplaisir J, et al. Restless legs syndrome prevalence and impact. REST general
population study. Arch Intern Med 2005; 165:1286–1292.
18. Nichols DA, Allen RP, Grauke JH, et al. Restless legs syndrome symptoms in primary care. A preva-
lence study. Arch Intern Med 2003; 163:2323–2329.
19. Walters AS, Hickey K, Maltzman J, et al. A questionnaire study of 138 patients with restless legs
syndrome: The ‘Night-Walkers’ survey. Neurology 1996; 46:92–95.
20. Desautels A, Turecki G, Montplaisir J, et al. Identification of a major susceptibility locus for restless
legs syndrome on chromosome 12q. Am J Hum Genet 2001; 69:1266–1270.
21. Bonati MT, Ferini-Strambi L, Aridon P, et al. Autosomal dominant restless legs syndrome maps on
chromosome 14q. Brain 2003; 126:1485–1492.
22. Chen S, Ondo WG, Rao S, et al. Genome wide linkage scan identifies a novel susceptibility locus for
Rls on chromosome 9p. Am J Hum Genet 2004; 74:876–885.
23. Levchenko A, Provost S, Montplaisir JY, et al. A novel autosomal dominant restless legs syndrome
locus maps to chromosome 20p13. Neurology 2006; 67:900–901.
24. Pichler I, Marroni F, Volpato CB, et al. Linkage analysis identifies a novel locus for restless legs
syndromeon chromosome 2q in a South Tyrolean population isolate. Am J Hum Genet 2006; 79:
716–723.
25. Winkelmann J, Schormair B, Lichtner P, et al. Genome-wide association study of restless legs syndrome
identifies common variants in three genomic regions. Nat Genet 2007; 39:1000–1006.
26. Stefansson H, Rye DB, Hicks A, et al. A genetic risk factor for periodic limb movements in sleep. N
Eng J Med 2007; 357:639–647.
27. O’Keeffe ST, Gavin K, Lavan JN. Iron status and restless legs syndrome in the elderly. Age Ageing.
1994; 23:200–203.
28. Sun ER, Chen CA, Ho G, et al. Iron and the restless legs syndrome. Sleep 1998; 21:371–377.
29. Earley CJ, Connor JR, Beard JL, et al. Abnormalities in CSF concentrations of ferritin and transferrin
in restless legs syndrome. Neurology 2000; 54:1698–1700.
30. Allen RP, Barker PB, Wehrl F, et al. MRI measurement of brain iron in patients with restless legs
syndrome. Neurology 2001; 56(2):263–265.
31. Connor JR, Boyer PJ, Menzies SL, et al. Neuropathological examination suggests impaired brain iron
acquisition in restless legs syndrome. Neurology 2003; 61:304–309.
32. Collado-Seidel V, Kohnen R, Samtleben W, et al. Clinical and biochemical findings in uremic patients
with and without restless legs syndrome. Am J Kidney Dis 1998; 31:324–328.
33. Ondo W, Jankovic J. Restless legs syndrome: Clinicoetiologic correlates. Neurology 1996; 47:
1435–1441.
34. Polydefkis M, Allen RP, Hauer P, et al. Subclinical sensory neuropathy in late-onset restless legs
syndrome. Neurology 2000; 55:1115–1121.
35. Ondo WG, Vuong KD, Jankovic J. Exploring the relationship between Parkinson’s disease and restless
legs syndrome. Arch Neurol 2002; 59:421–424.
36. Bassetti CL, Mauerhofer D, Gugger M, et al. Restless legs syndrome: A clinical study of 55 patients.
Eur Neurol 2001; 45:67–74.
37. Hening W, Walters AS, Allen RP, et al. Impact, diagnosis and treatment of restless legs syndrome (RLS)
in a primary care population: The REST (RLS epidemiology, symptoms, and treatment) primary care
study. Sleep Med 2004; 5:237–246.
38. Bjorvatn B, Leissner L, Ulfberg J, et al. Prevalence, severity and risk factors of restless legs syndrome
in the general adult population in two Scandinavian countries. Sleep Med 2005; 6:307–312.
39. Ulfberg J, Bjorvatn B, Leissner L, et al. Comorbidity in restless legs syndrome among a sample of
Swedish adults. Sleep Med 2007; 8:768–772.
40. Phillips B, Hening W, Britz P, et al. Prevalence and correlates of restless legs syndrome. Results from
the 2005 National Sleep Foundation poll. Chest 2006; 129:76–80.
41. Allen R, Becker PM, Bogan R, et al. Ropinirole decreases periodic leg movements and improves sleep
parameters in patients with restless legs syndrome. Sleep 2004; 27:907–914.
42. Saletu B, Anderer P, Saletu M, et al. EEG mapping, psychometric, and polysomnographic studies in
restless legs syndrome (RLS) and periodic limb movement disorder (PLMD) patients as compared
with normal controls. Sleep Med 2002; 3:S35–S42.
c18 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:51 Char Count=
208 SILBER
43. Hornyak M, Feige B, Voderholzer U, et al. Polysomnography findings in patients with restless legs
syndrome and in healthy controls: A comparative observational study. Sleep 2007; 30:861–865.
44. Hening W, Allen R, Earley C, et al. The treatment of restless legs syndrome and periodic limb movement
disorder. An American Academy of Sleep Medicine Review. Sleep 1999; 22:970–999.
45. Hening W, Allen R, Earley C, et al. An update on the dopaminergic treatment of restless legs syndrome
and periodic limb movement disorder. An American Academy of Sleep Medicine review. Sleep 2004;
27:560–583.
46. Bogan RK, Fry JM, Schmidt MH, et al; TREAT RLS US Study Group. Ropinirole in the treatment
of patients with restless legs syndrome: A US-based randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled
clinical trial. Mayo Clin Proc 2006; 81:17–27.
47. Montplaisir J, Karrasch J, Haan J, et al. Ropinirole is effective in the long-term management of restless
legs syndrome: A randomized controlled trial. Mov Disord 2006; 21:1627–1635.
48. Trenkwalder C, Garcia-Borreguero D, Montagna P, et al. Ropinirole in the treatment of restless legs
syndrome: Results from the TREAT RLS 1 study, a 12 week, randomised, placebo controlled study in
10 European countries. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2004; 75:92–97.
49. Walters AS, Ondo W, Dreykluft T, et al. Ropinirole is effective in the treatment of restless legs syndrome.
TREAT RLS 2: A 12-week, double blind, randomized parallel-group, placebo-controlled study. Mov
Disord 2004; 19:1414–1423.
50. Oertel WH, Stiasny-Kolster K, Bergtholdt B, et al. Efficacy of pramipexole in restless legs syndrome:
A six-week, multicenter, randomized, double-blind study (effect-RLS study). Mov Disord 2007; 22:
213–219.
51. Benes H, Kurella B, Kummer J, et al. Rapid onset of action of levodopa in restless legs syndrome: A
double-blind, randomized, multicenter, crossover trial. Sleep 1999; 22:1073–1081.
52. Collado-Seidel V, Kazenwadel J, Wetter TC, et al. A controlled study of additional sr-L-dopa
in L-dopa-responsive restless legs syndrome with late-night symptoms. Neurology 1999; 52:
285–290.
53. Garcia-Borreguero D, Larrosa E, De la Llave Y, et al. Treatment of restless legs syndrome with
gabapentin. A double-blind, cross-over study. Neurology 2002; 59:1573–1579.
54. Walters AS, Wagner M, Hening W, et al. Successful treatment of the idiopathic restless legs syndrome
in a randomized double-blind trial of oxycodone versus placebo. Sleep 1993; 16:327–332.
55. Montagna P, Sassoli de Bianchi L, Zucconi M, et al. Clonazepam and vibration in restless legs syndrome.
Acta Neurol Scand 1984; 69:428–430.
56. Silber MH, Ehrenberg BL, Allen RP. An algorithm for the management of restless legs syndrome.
Mayo Clin Proc 2004; 79:916–922.
57. Iber C, Ancoli-Israel S, Chesson A, et al; for the American Academy of Sleep Medicine. The AASM
Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events: Rules, Terminology and Technical Specifica-
tions. Westchester, Illinois: American Academy of Sleep Medicine, 2007.
58. Zucconi M, Ferri R, Allen R, et al. The official World Association of Sleep Medicine (WASM) standards
for recording and scoring periodic leg movements in sleep (PLMS) and wakefulness (PLMW) devel-
oped in collaboration with a task force from the International Restless Legs Syndrome Study Group
(IRLSSG). Sleep Med 2006; 7:175–183.
59. Ferri R, Zucconi M, Manconi M, et al. New approaches to the study of periodic leg movements during
sleep in restless legs syndrome. Sleep 2006; 29:759–769.
60. Al-Awali A, Mulgrew A, Tench E, et al. Prevalence, risk factors and impact on daytime sleepiness and
hypertension of periodic leg movements with arousals in patients with obstructive sleep apnea. J Clin
Sleep Med 2006; 2:281–287.
61. Wetter TC, Collado-Seidel V, Pollmacher T, et al. Sleep and periodic leg movement patterns in drug-free
patients with Parkinson’s disease and multiple system atrophy. Sleep 2000; 23:361–367.
62. Silber MH. Controversies in Sleep Medicine: Periodic limb movements. Sleep Med 2001; 2:367–369.
63. Mahowald MW. Assessment of periodic leg movements is not an essential component of an overnight
sleep study. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 2001; 164:1340–1341.
64. Walters AS. Assessment of periodic leg movements is an essential component of an overnight sleep
study. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 2001; 164:1339–1340.
65. Coleman RM, Pollack CP, Weitzman ED. Periodic movements in sleep (nocturnal myoclonus): Relation
to sleep disorders. Ann Neurol 1980; 8:416–421.
66. Mendelson W. Are periodic limb movements associated with clinical sleep disturbances? Sleep 1996;
19:219–223.
67. Youngstedt SD, Kripke DF, Klauber MR, et al. Periodic leg movements during sleep and sleep distur-
bances in elders. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 1998; 53:M391–M394.
68. Hilbert J, Mohsenin V. Can periodic limb movement disorder be diagnosed without polysomnogra-
phy? A case-control study. Sleep Med 2003; 4:35–41.
c18 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:51 Char Count=
69. Hornyak M, Riemann D, Voderholzer U. Do periodic leg movements influence patients’ perception of
sleep quality? Sleep Med 2004; 5:597–600.
70. Nicolas A, Lesperance P, Montplaisir J. Is excessive daytime sleepiness with periodic leg movements
during sleep a specific diagnostic category? Eur Neurol 1998; 40:22–26.
71. Carrier J, Frenette S, Montplaisir J, et al. Effects of periodic leg movements during sleep in middle-aged
subjects without sleep complaints. Mov Disord 2005; 20:1127–1132.
72. Karadeniz D, Ondze B, Besset A, et al. EEG arousals and awakenings in relation with periodic leg
movements during sleep. J Sleep Res 2000; 9:273–277.
73. Montplaisir J, Boucher S, Gosselin A, et al. Persistence of repetitive EEG arousals (K-alpha complexes)
in RLS patients treated with L-DOPA. Sleep 1996; 19:196–199.
74. Sforza E, Nicolas A, Lavigne G, et al. EEG and cardiac activation during periodic leg movements in
sleep: Support for a hierarchy of arousal responses. Neurology 1999; 52:786–791.
75. Winkelman JW. The evoked heart rate response to periodic leg movements of sleep. Sleep 1999;
22:575–580.
76. Hornyak M, Feige B, Riemann D, et al. Periodic leg movements in sleep and periodic limb movement
disorder: Prevalence, clinical significance and treatment. Sleep Med Rev 2006; 10:169–177.
77. Saletu M, Anderer P, Saletu-Zyhlarz G, et al. Restless legs syndrome (RLS) and periodic limb move-
ment disorder (PLMD): Acute placebo-controlled sleep laboratory studies with clonazepam. Eur
Neuropsychopharmacol 2001; 11(2):153–161.
78. Saletu M, Anderer P, Saletu B, et al. Sleep laboratory studies in periodic limb movement disorder
(PLMD) patients as compared with normals and acute effects of ropinirole. Hum Psychopharmacol
2001; 16:177–187.
79. Ohayon MM, Li KK, Guilleminault C. Risk factors for sleep bruxism in the general population. Chest
2001; 119:53–61.
80. Lavigne GJ, Rompre PH, Montplaisir JY. Sleep bruxism: Validity of clinicial research diagnostic criteria
in a controlled polysomnographic study. J Dent Res 1996; 75:546–552.
81. Macaluso GM, Guerra P, Di Giovanni G, et al. Sleep bruxism is a disorder related to periodic arousals
during sleep. J Dent Res 1998; 77:565–573.
82. Kato T, Rompre P, Montplaisir JY, et al. Sleep bruxism: An oromotor activity secondary to micro-
arousal. J Dent Res 2001; 80:1940–1944.
83. Ahlberg K, Jahkola A, Savolainen A, et al. Associations of reported bruxism with insomnia and
insufficient sleep symptoms among media personnel with or without irregular shift work. Head Face
Med 2008; 4:4
84. Dyken ME, Lin-Dyken DC, Yamada T. Diagnosing rhythmic movement disorder with video-
polysomnography. Pediatr Neurol 1997; 16:37–41.
85. Neveus T, Cnattingius S, Olsson U, et al. Sleep habits and sleep problems among a community sample
of schoolchildren. Acta Paediatr 2001; 90:1450–1455.
86. Jankovic SM, Sokic DV, Vojvodic NM, et al. Multiple rhythmic movement disorders in a teenage boy
with excellent response to clonazepam. Mov Disord 2008; 23:767–775.
87. Butler JV, Mulkerrin EC, O’Keeffe ST. Nocturnal leg cramps in older people. Postgrad Med 2002;
78:596–598.
88. Man-Son-Hing M, Wells G, Lau A. Qunine for nocturnal leg cramps. A meta-analysis including
unpublished data. J Gen Intern Med 1998; 13:600–606.
89. Guay DR. Are there alternatives to the use of quinine to treat nocturnal leg cramps? Consult Pharm
2008; 23:141–156.
c19 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:56 Char Count=
Michael T. Smith
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Behavioral Sleep Medicine Program, Johns Hopkins
University School of Medicine, Baltimore, Maryland, U.S.A.
Nancy A. Collop
Division of Pulmonary and Critical Care, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore,
Maryland, U.S.A.
INTRODUCTION
Recent years have seen a rapidly growing interest in the frequent co-occurrence of insomnia
disorder and sleep related breathing disorders (SRBD) (1). The two conditions were once con-
sidered orthogonal, or insomnia was considered a symptom of SRBD. However, evidence from
clinical, research, and population samples, as well as clinical experience, consistently suggest
that insomnia disorder and SRBD co-exist as distinct disease entities and warrant independent
treatment in many patients. The purpose of this chapter is to review the association between
insomnia and SRBD, the two most common sleep disorders among adults. The chapter begins
with a review of the causes and consequences of SRBD, followed by a discussion of the liter-
ature regarding the cooccurrence of chronic insomnia and SRBD. Treatment of these disorders
when they are comorbid is considered, along with the potential interactions among treatments,
including pharmacological approaches. Clinical recommendations and suggestions for future
research are offered.
60.00s 60.00s
4:15:00 AM 4:15:05 AM 4:15:10 AM 4:15:15 AM 4:15:20 AM 4:15:25 AM 4:15:30 AM 4:15:35 AM 4:15:40 AM 4:15:45 AM 4:15:50 AM 4:15:55 AM
EDGL
EDGR
S2 S2
D1A2
C3A2
Resp Arousal
EMGch
ECG
EMGat
Hypopnea (21.15s)
NCPT
AFth
Chest
Abd
98
98
97
97
97
97
97
97
97
Desaturation (27.00s) [7.0 %]
97
97
97
97
97
97
97
97
97
97
97
97
97
97
97
97
97
97
97
97
96
96
96
96
96
96
96
96
SpO2
96
95
95
95
94
94
94
94
94
92
92
9
91
9
9
9
9
9
9
9
90
91
Figure 1 Hypopnea This figure shows a polysomnographic recording of an obstructive hypopnea lasting 21
seconds. Airflow (NCPT and AFth ) is decreased but effort (Chest and Abd) increases as the event proceeds.
The event is terminated with an arousal from sleep and oxygen saturation falls by 7%. Abbreviations: EOGL ,
EOGR, electrooculogram; O1 A2 , Occipital electroencephalogram; C3 A2 ,Central electroencephalogram; EMGch ,
Chin electromyogram; ECG, Electrocardiogram; EMGat , Anterior tibialis (leg) electromyogram; NCPT, Airflow as
measured by a nasal cannula pressure transducer; AFth , Airflow as measured by a thermocouple; Chest Abd,
Respiratory effort of chest and abdomen; SpO 2, Pulse oxygen saturation.
Sequelae of SRBD
The short and long-term health consequences of OSA can be severe. Common short-term symp-
toms of OSA include excessive daytime sleepiness, irritability, depressed mood, poor execu-
tive function including cognitive and memory impairment, and other performance deficits.
For example, a recent review reported that 23 of 27 studies found a significant relation
between OSA and risk for motor vehicle accident (3). Equally striking are the long-term disease
correlates of OSA. Well-documented relationships exist between OSA and hypertension, stroke,
cardiovascular death, and overall mortality (4).
Of particular relevance to health care professionals working with insomnia patients is
the complex and often subtle presentation of SRBD. Symptoms of OSA can include restless or
nonrestorative sleep, rapid weight gain, lethargy, excessive daytime sleepiness, reduced libido,
irritability, difficulty concentrating, and hyperactivity in children. Such complaints can easily
be mistaken for numerous psychological or medical disorders, including depression, anxiety,
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and cognitive impairment (5). At the very least, to
maximize diagnostic accuracy, routine assessments across medical and psychological disciplines
should include the question, “Do you snore?”
Epidemiology of SRBD
Obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) is the second most common sleep disorder, with overall pop-
ulation prevalence estimates ranging from 3% to 7% (6). Men suffer OSA at roughly two to
three times the rate of women (7), although clinic referrals display a greater gender discrepancy,
with five or more times as many men being referred for evaluation than women. Perhaps due
to decreasing patency in the upper airway and changes in the pharyngeal structure (8,9), the
prevalence of OSA increases steadily with age until the sixth decade of life, when a plateau
appears to be reached (7). Although obstructive apnea increases with age for both genders, in
women, menopause is associated with significant increases in incidence. In addition to age and
gender, members of certain ethnic and socioeconomic groups are also at increased risk relative
c19 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:56 Char Count=
Causes of SRBD
The exact cause of OSA is unknown. However, certain anatomical and behavioral factors have
been consistently associated with this condition (16). There also appears to be a genetic predis-
position for obstructive sleep apnea (17). Craniofacial structures differ across racial groups, and
these differences have also been associated with higher prevalence of OSA. In terms of behavior,
weight gain has consistently been associated with increased prevalence of the disorder. In one
community-based longitudinal study, individuals whose weight increased by 10% were found
to have a 32% increase in AHI and were at six times the risk for development of moderate or
severe obstructive sleep apnea, relative to individuals whose weight remained constant (18).
Other factors may also contribute to development or exacerbation of OSA. Alcohol consumption
decreases muscle tone in the upper airway and can precipitate apneic episodes in otherwise
normal breathing individuals, or worsen the severity of apneic events and oxyhemoglobin desat-
uration in patients with established OSA (19). Smoking and exposure to second-hand smoke
have been associated with snoring and obstructive sleep apnea (20). (Readers are referred to
reference #7, Punjabi 2008, for a detailed review of the epidemiology of OSA.)
Assessment of SRBD
An overnight sleep study, or polysomnogram (PSG), is usually performed to confirm the diag-
nosis. The PSG is a test that records a number of parameters during sleep: brain activity (via
electroencephalogram); electrical activity of muscles (via surface electromyography); eye move-
ments (via electrooculogram); leg movement; heart rate and rhythm; airflow, snoring; blood
oxygen saturation; and chest and abdominal movement. Most sleep centers also use video
recording, which allows treatment providers to observe sleeping position, body movements, or
other unusual behaviors during sleep.
A diagnosis of sleep apnea is made based on the number of breathing “events” (i.e., apneas
and hypopneas) observed during the night, which are calculated into an apnea-hypopnea index
(AHI—the number of events/hr of sleep time). Unfortunately, there is a lack of consensus in
interpreting the significance of the AHI, and this is reflected in inconsistent diagnostic cutoffs
among various sleep centers. Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Services (CMS) define mild
sleep apnea as having an AHI between 5 and 15 events/hr, with an associated daytime com-
plaint; an AHI of 15 to 30 is labeled moderate disease severity, and individuals with an AHI
greater than 30 are described as having severe sleep apnea (21). In terms of prevalence estimates
and determining the relationship between SRBD and insomnia, the sensitivity of these cutoffs
is important and can lead to notably divergent results, as will be discussed below.
c19 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:56 Char Count=
Treatment of SRBD
Depending on disease severity and individual patient characteristics, most SRBD patients are
currently treated using one of four approaches. For mild cases of OSA, conservative treatment
includes weight loss, careful monitoring of symptom progression, and mechanical interventions,
such as sleeping with additional pillows or sewing a tennis ball into the back of a t-shirt, to
prevent the patient from sleeping on his or her back. The next least invasive approach involves
the use of a customized oral appliance designed to keep the airway open by pulling the lower
jaw forward. In a recent metaanalysis, Lim and colleagues (22) reported significant reduction
in disease severity when use of an oral appliance was compared to a control condition. Surgical
alternatives such as uvulopalatopharyngoplasty (UPPP) represent the most invasive treatments.
Although surgery may be effective in certain carefully selected patients, a recent review (23)
found insufficient evidence to support the use of surgical treatment approaches in SRBD.
Continuous positive airway pressure (CPAP) is the currently the most commonly
employed and “gold standard” treatment for SRBD and an overwhelming majority of SRBD
patients are treated using positive airway pressure therapy. CPAP consists of small machine
that generates a prescribed level of air pressure, delivered to the upper airway via a hose and
mask that covers the nose or nose and mouth. The air pressure functions as a pneumatic splint,
pushing the airway open and allowing the patient to breathe normally. CPAP is the most effec-
tive first-treatment available. The major challenge to successful long-term therapy is patient
adherence. Careful follow-up and resolution of complicating factors is required to achieve com-
pliance. Readers are referred to Reference (24) for review of minimally invasive treatments of
SRBD as well as modifiable psychological factors related to CPAP adherence.
Table 1 Retrospective Chart Review Studies of Insomnia Complaints in Patients Referred for Evaluation of
SRBD
Insomnia Prevalence of
References N criteria OSA criteria insomnia Major findings
26 358 Questionnaire RDI > 5 OI in women: Profile of SRBD-related
items 27.9%; OI in men: sleep disturbances
21.9% may differ b/t men
and women, (e.g.,
women more SOL
complaints than men
but not snoring or
EDS; Women also
complain ↑ EDS and
↑ SOL together,
more often than men.
27 157 4 questionnaire AHI > 5, 42% ≥1 complaint: OI: ↓ AHI; MI: ↑ EDS;
items AHI > 15 OI (6%), MI results similar w/AHI
(26%), EMA > 5 and AHI > 15
(19%)
28 105 ISI > 15, self-report, chart 39% Suspected OSA: 2x
duration > 6 review (AHI likely INS; INS +
mo, SOL or not reported) SRBD vs. SRBD
WASO > 30 alone: ↑ depression,
m on PSG ↑ anxiety, ↑ stress
29 357 3 questionnaire AHI > 10 OI: 15.6% if AHI > OI: ↓ SRBD; lie awake
items 60, 18.2% if AHI w/intense thoughts: ↓
> 30, 20.9% if SRBD; MI: ↑ SRBD
AHI > 10; MI:
73.9% if AHI >
60, 62.9% if AHI
> 30, 58.2% if
AHI > 10
30 231 3 questionnaire AHI > 5 50% INS + SRBD vs SRBD
items alone: ↑ SOL (65 vs
17 m), ↓ TST (5.6 vs
7.2 h), ↑ psych probs
31 255 3 questionnaire AHI > 5, 54.9% ≥1 INS: ↓ SRBD; INS: ↑
items AHI > 15 complaint: OI pain, psych probs,
(33.4%), MI RLS, PLMS; AHI >
(38.8%), EMA 10 vs AHIÃ10: ↓ INS
(31.4%) (51.8% vs 81.5%)
32 119 4 questionnaire AHI > 5 33% NOA, 21% OI: ↓ AHI vs MI or no
items EMA, 16% insomnia; MI is most
WASO, 9% SOL common complaint in
SRBD; many SRBD
pts have OI
33 232 3 questionnaire AHI > 5 37% > 1 complaint: Patients reporting
items OI (16.6%), MI maintenance
(23.7%), EMA insomnia were less
(20.6%) adherent to CPAP
prescription at
clinical follow-up.
Insomnia complaints
were unrelated to
AHI.
Abbreviations: INS, insomnia; SRBD, sleep related breathing disorder; OI, onset insomnia; MI, maintenance insomnia; EMA,
early morning awakenings; SOL, sleep onset latency; WASO, wake after sleep onset; NOA, number of nocturnal awakenings;
PSG, polysomnogram; AHI, apnea-hypopnea index; ISI, insomnia severity index; RLS, restless legs syndrome; PLMS, periodic
limb movements.
c19 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:56 Char Count=
including significantly more difficulty initiating sleep (66.7%), maintaining sleep (59.2%), and
early morning awakenings (51.8%).
Other investigators have reported an inverse relationship between insomnia complaints
and severity of SRBD. Gold et al. (29) analyzed data collected from 357 consecutive patients,
including 220 OSA (AHI > 10) patients and 137 patients diagnosed with UARS. Insomnia
complaints were quantified using three Likert items from an intake questionnaire. Consis-
tent with previous findings (31), greater sleep onset latency was associated with less severe
SRBD. Similarly, Chung (27) also found that complaints of onset insomnia were associated with
lower respiratory disturbance. In this study, an insomnia complaint was reported by 42% of
157 patients diagnosed with OSA (AHI > 5). Individuals with more severe SRBD likely expe-
rience greater daytime sleepiness, which may explain the inverse relationship between SRBD
and onset insomnia. We will return to this issue in a later section.
during the PSG evaluation. In the Smith et al. (28) study, individuals with both insomnia and
SRBD experienced lower sleep efficiency and total sleep time, as well as more time awake in
bed. This finding is consistent with Krakow et al. (30), who observed that patients with both
insomnia and OSA (AHI>5) reported significantly longer SOL (17 m vs. 65 m), shorter TST
(5.6 h vs. 7.2 h), and lower SE (75% vs. 92%) as measured during PSG.
A similar pattern emerges in daytime performance. Patients with both insomnia and SRBD
report significantly more depression, anxiety, and stress than did patients with only SRBD (28).
Gooneratne et al. (40) found that individuals with both insomnia and SRBD had slower psy-
chomotor reaction times and greater functional impairment, including daytime sleepiness as
measured by MSLT, than did patients with SRBD alone. At least one study has found no signif-
icant differences between individuals with insomnia and SRBD versus individuals with insom-
nia alone (35); however, small samples size in this study limited power to identify differences
between groups.
model, insomnia symptoms were the only predictors of nonimprovement. Similarly, in a sample
of 232 patients referred for evaluation of SRBD and seen in clinical follow-up, complaints of sleep
maintenance insomnia at baseline independently predicted poorer rates of CPAP acceptance
at follow-up (33). This study was the first to provide empirical support for the very common
clinical phenomenon of patients ascribing difficulty acclimating to CPAP to difficulty initiating
or maintaining sleep.
Only a few pioneering studies (summarized in Table 3) have explored the use of behavioral
interventions for insomnia in patients with SRBD, or interventions for SRBD in patients with
insomnia. In perhaps the first related study, Guilleminault and colleagues (47) subdivided a
sample of older adult females with chronic insomnia into those with upper airway resistance
syndrome versus those with no sleep related breathing disorder. None of the sample met criteria
for obstructive sleep apnea, however. Half of the subjects with UARS received either CPAP or
turbinectomy and the other half received a six-week CBT-I intervention. The group without
UARS received either CBT-I or a sleep hygiene education control condition. Interestingly none
of the groups showed improvement in Epworth sleepiness scores, and all groups showed
improvement in sleep quality, PSG sleep latency, and PSG wake after sleep onset time. This
suggests that CBT-I alone may have some long-term benefit for patients with UARS. Only
the group receiving CPAP or turbinectomy demonstrated significant reduction in fatigue and
actigraphic measure of arousal at six months. This indicates that in select cases intervening
on upper airway resistance may be a critical intervention to add to traditional interventions
for insomnia. Another major finding from the study was that subjects who underwent CBT-I
outperformed the sleep hygiene control groups as well as the SRBD treatment only groups
on PSG measures of sleep latency and total sleep time. While this study did not contrast a
treatment package that combined CPAP or turbinectomy with CBT-I, the differential gains with
both interventions suggest that a combination approach may prove superior.
The only other studies exploring treatment approaches to patients with chronic insomnia
and signs/symptoms of sleep related breathing disorder were conducted by Krakow and col-
leagues (45). In the only study of patients meeting criteria for obstructive sleep apnea, Krakow
and colleagues (45) conducted a chart review of patients who had initially presented with
symptoms of insomnia but who had achieved only partial remission of insomnia symptoms
from standard CBT-I. Nineteen patients subsequently underwent PSG and an adequate CPAP
titration trial. Comparing the titration night PSG against the diagnostic baseline, these authors
found that CPAP was associated with decreased sleep latency, decreased sleep wake transitions
and increased percentage of REM sleep.
In a prospective open label study of trauma survivors with OSA and insomnia, Krakow
and colleagues (45) studied outcomes at the end of CBT-I. Patients without complete cure from
CBT-I were then enrolled in a CPAP or mandibular positioning device (MPD) arm with outcomes
assessed three months posttreatment. Only subjects showing good adherence to CPAP or the
MPD (one subject had turbinectomy) were included in the analyses. At the end of CBT-I, all
groups demonstrated subjective improvement in insomnia. Improvement increased further at
three months on all measures. Of particular interest is that the vast majority of insomniacs
(88%) treated for sleep related breathing disorder demonstrated subclinical levels of insomnia
symptoms at three months posttreatment. This suggests that insomniacs refractory to insomnia
therapies may benefit from further evaluation and treatment of sleep related breathing disorder.
More recently, Krakow and colleagues (46) studied the effects of an external nasal dila-
tor strip in normal weight, sleep maintenance insomniacs reporting some symptoms of sleep
disordered breathing. Subjects were randomly assigned to four weeks of nasal dilator strips
versus a contact educational control condition. At one month, the nasal dilator group showed
large differential effects on self-reported insomnia severity and improvements in sleep quality.
Moderate but significant improvements were observed on measures of quality of life, sleepiness
impact, and diary symptoms of sleep related breathing disorder. While this study is suggestive,
conclusions must be tempered as no diagnostic PSGs were conducted and there was no placebo
condition. Since only subjective measurements were conducted with no blinded placebo con-
trol, it remains possible that these data might be a function of expectancies for improvement or
lack of improvement. This said, the effect sizes were large and future placebo controlled studies
using objective measurement and diagnosis is warranted.
c19
IHBK059-Sateia
218
Table 3 Studies Treating Sleep Related Breathing Disorder in Patients with Chronic Insomnia
References Sample Design Endpoints OSA criteria Major findings Comments
44 Two groups of Post Clinical Trial, 4 Baseline & 6 mo No OSA, AHI < 5 No group showed improved combined RX of CBT-I
+UARS RX, although
8:56
sig. ↓ relative to B,C, & D
c. No UARS + CBT-I CBT-I > PSG SL reductions
(n = 34) CBT-I in non UARS had > ↑PSG
d. No UARS + Sleep
Char Count=
TST
hygiene Waitlist
45 Study 1: Chronic Study 1: Uncontrolled, Study 1: PSG sleep AHI ≥ 5 plus daytime Study 1: Compared to DX night, Highly selected groups
Insomnia, failed CBT-I chart review comparing continuity & architecture SX CPAP Night : ↓ Sleep-wake of treatment compliant
with sleep apnea CPAP titration night Study 2: Self-report transition; ↓ AHI; ↓ SL↑ REM% subjects and lack of
(N = 19) versus DX night sleep quality, insomnia Study 2: Both CBT-I and SRBD Rx control limit
Study 2: Trauma Study 2: Open label trial severity, & sleepiness phase demonstrated improvement conclusions &
survivors with insomnia, CBT-I followed impairment @ baseline, on all measures. ↓ Insomnia generalizability
refractory to CBT-I, and sequentially by SRBD post CBT-I and @ 3 mo: severity post SRBD RX @ 3 mo
compliant with SRBD RX (CPAP, 3 mo, oral ISI, PSQI, FOSQ FU 88% achieved nonclinical ISI
RX (N = 17) appliance, score after SRBD RX
turbinectomy)
46 Nonobese, Sleep Randomized controlled Baseline and 4 wk. Self-reported SX Nasal dilator strips associated Lack of PSG diagnosis
maintenance insomnia clinical trial of nasal Primary outcomes: consistent with mild to with large (effect sizes >1) of SRBD, a placebo
with self-reported dilators strips (4 wk (n = Self-report sleep quality, moderate, reductions in insomnia severity condition, and objective
mild-moderate sleep 42) vs. contact, SRBD insomnia severity, & uncomplicated SRBD. ISI, and improvements in sleep measurement limits
related breathing education control sleepiness impairment No PSG assessment. quality. conclusions.
disorder symptoms (n = 38) (ISI, PSQI, FOSQ, and Moderate improvements in
(N = 80) quality of life QLSEQ) sleepiness impact and quality of
Secondary Outcomes: life FOSQ.
WICKWIRE ET AL.
prospective diaries of Diary data revealed self-reported
sleep-related SX improvement in sleep quality and
sleep related breathing symptoms
Abbreviations: FOSQ, Functional outcomes of sleep questionnaire; ISI, Insomnia severity index; PSQI, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; QLSEQ, Quality of Life Enjoyment and Satisfaction Questionnaire.
c19 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:56 Char Count=
Sedatives
Benzodiazepines including diazepam and midazolam, have been shown to preferentially reduce
upper airway muscle tone more than diaphragmatic muscle tone (48–50) This is thought to
worsen OSA due to the imbalance of muscle tone. Benzodiazepines also reduce the arousal
threshold so patients may have longer events prior to arousing from sleep. Finally, these medi-
cations likely also reduce hypoxic and hypercapneic ventilatory responses.
Alcohol
Many patients with insomnia use alcohol to induce sleep. Alcohol has numerous effects on sleep,
both in terms of breathing and architecture. Alcohol induces deep (slow wave) sleep but as the
alcohol wears off, REM sleep rebound occurs, although this rebound is typically fragmented
by arousal and awakenings (51). In addition to its effects on sleep, alcohol is a respiratory
depressant and has been shown in several studies to induce sleep apnea in susceptible persons,
and worsen the degree of apnea in patients known to have the disorder (19,52,53). Specifically,
alcohol has been shown to cause selective reduction in genioglossus and hypoglossal motor
nerve activity increased edema of the nasal mucosa, and reduction of the arousal response
(54–57). These depressant effects have been shown to occur even with levels of alcohol below
the “legal blood alcohol concentration” (58).
Narcotics
There are a number of case reports describing adverse events in OSA patients following surgery
in which narcotics were used for pain control (59–62). In general, most textbooks and guidelines
suggest caution in prescribing opioids to OSA patients due to their respiratory depressive
effects. In fact, however, there are relatively few studies that have actually examined the use of
these drugs in OSA (63).
Hypnotics
Medications specifically formulated as sleep aids have been studied in relation to their effects
on SRBD. Trials are usually short (1–5 nights) and placebo-controlled. Table 4 lists many of the
trials that have evaluated hypnotic use in patients with OSA. Some of the trials evaluated using
hypnotics while patients were on CPAP, to determine if the CPAP requirements changed when
under the influence of those drugs.
In general, the newer hypnotic agents (zolpidem, eszopiclone/zopiclone, zaleplon,
ramelteon) appear to have minimal effects on OSA. Typically, apnea-hypopnea indices do not
change significantly and changes in oxygen saturation are minimal and likely not clinically sig-
nificant. Some of the older agents, which were in the benzodiazepine class, did show some more
significant changes in AHI and oxygen saturation, but these were not dramatic. Of particular
interest, use of a hypnotic in one study did not show any improvement in CPAP usage, although
patients were unselected and insomnia complaints not utilized as a screen for enrollment. (Ref
61, Table 4)
In summary, it appears that some medications (benzodiazepines, opioids) should be used
with caution in OSA patients unless they are undergoing concomitant treatment with CPAP.
Further studies should be performed to determine their effects under that condition however.
Alcohol should also be avoided in OSA patients around the sleep hours, as it clearly worsens
SRBD. Using CPAP following alcohol ingestion does not appear to change CPAP efficacy-–but
again, more study is needed (76). Finally, the newer hypnotic agents appear to be relatively safe
to use in OSA patients although, again, caution should be exercised in patients with severe OSA
or with other respiratory disorders.
c19 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:56 Char Count=
CLINICAL RECOMMENDATIONS
Patients experiencing both sleep related breathing disorder and insomnia are likely to be seen
in a variety of health care environments. In each setting, health care providers should inquire,
at minimum, about difficulty initiating and maintaining sleep, as well as snoring and witnessed
apnea. The diagnostic priority should be to distinguish sleepiness from fatigue, while keeping in
mind that patients with comorbid insomnia disorder and SRBD will be likely to report complex
symptoms that may involve sleepiness and fatigue. Further, patient self-reports will be affected
by their knowledge of sleep and sleep disorders. Due to the nature of insomnia as a subjective
and highly distressing condition, patients may self-diagnose with insomnia when in reality,
SRBD or other factors play an equal or larger role in the overall sleep pathology. In addition, the
influence of social desirability factors must not be overlooked. For some patients, both male and
female, insomnia may be a more socially acceptable condition than snoring and sleep apnea,
or the opposite may be equally true. Practitioners are encouraged to consider their patients’
symptom and disease awareness and to elicit as much corroborating information as necessary
to reliably discern sleepiness from fatigue.
In terms of treatment, the overlap between insomnia and SRBD presents the equally
challenging dilemma of selecting the first targets for treatment. Not surprisingly, there are a
number of factors for providers to consider: severity of sleep complaints, comorbid medical or
psychiatric conditions, and patient preference or readiness for demanding treatments such as
CBT-I or CPAP, to name just a few although clinical judgment should guide the decision-making
process, there are a few rules of thumb. Regardless of the severity of insomnia complaints,
patients who report sleepiness while driving, who have fallen asleep unexpectedly during
daytime hours, or who exhibit other overt signs of sleepiness should be referred for a sleep
study, especially if they snore or have witnessed apneas during sleep. Similarly, if patients
describe chronic problems falling asleep or staying asleep, referral to behavioral sleep medicine
c19 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:56 Char Count=
specialist should be considered. A recent report (77) of combined insomnia and poor CPAP
adherence documented the psychoeducation, motivational approach (78), and give and take
often necessary when treating patients with complicated presentations.
CONCLUSIONS AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS
Insomnia is the second most common medical complaint after pain, and sleep related breathing
disorder is the second most common sleep disorder. Health care providers in all areas, including
physicians, psychologists, nurses, social workers, and especially sleep medicine professionals,
will see patients with combined insomnia and sleep related breathing disorder. In terms of
practice, accurate assessment is essential to ensure patients receive state of the art, appropriate
treatment. Students and health care providers should be trained in the basics of assessing fatigue
and sleepiness and realize that symptoms of both insomnia and SRBD can appear as other
medical or psychological disorders. Future research will help illuminate any potential shared
mechanisms underlying comorbid insomnia and SRBD and guide the further development and
refinement of effective treatments.
REFERENCES
1. Smith MT, Huang MI, Manber R. Cognitive behavior therapy for chronic insomnia occurring within
the context of medical and psychiatric disorders. Clin Psychol Rev 2005; 25(5):559–592.
2. Exar EN, Collop NA. The upper airway resistance syndrome. Chest 1999; 115(4):1127–1139.
3. Ellen RL, Marshall SC, Palayew M, et al. Systematic review of motor vehicle crash risk in persons with
sleep apnea. J Clin Sleep Med 2006; 2(2):193–200.
4. Collop N. The effect of obstructive sleep apnea on chronic medical disorders. Cleve Clin J Med 2007;
74(1):72–78.
5. Haynes PL. The role of behavioral sleep medicine in the assessment and treatment of sleep disordered
breathing. Clin Psychol Rev 2005; 25(5):673–705.
6. Punjabi NM. The epidemiology of adult obstructive sleep apnea. Proc Am Thorac Soc 2008; 5(2):
136–143.
7. Young T, Palta M, Dempsey J, et al. The occurrence of sleep-disordered breathing among middle-aged
adults. N Engl J Med 1993; 328(17):1230–1235.
8. Malhotra A, Huang Y, Fogel R, et al. Aging influences on pharyngeal anatomy and physiology: The
predisposition to pharyngeal collapse. Am J Med 2006; 119(1):72–14.
9. Eikermann M, Jordan AS, Chamberlin NL, et al. The influence of aging on pharyngeal collapsibility
during sleep. Chest 2007; 131(6):1702–1709.
10. Redline S, Kirchner HL, Quan SF, et al. The effects of age, sex, ethnicity, and sleep-disordered breathing
on sleep architecture. Arch Intern Med 2004; 164(4):406–418.
11. Ancoli-Israel S, Klauber MR, Stepnowsky C, et al. Sleep-disordered breathing in African-American
elderly. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 1995; 152(6Pt 1):1946–1949.
12. Li KK, Powell NB, Kushida C, et al. A comparison of Asian and white patients with obstructive sleep
apnea syndrome. Laryngoscope 1999; 109(12):1937–1940.
13. Ong KC, Clerk AA. Comparison of the severity of sleep-disordered breathing in Asian and Caucasian
patients seen at a sleep disorders center. Respir Med 1998; 92(6):843–848.
14. Lam B, Ip MS, Tench E, et al. Craniofacial profile in Asian and white subjects with obstructive sleep
apnoea. Thorax 2005; 60(6):504–510.
15. O’Connor GT, Lind BK, Lee ET, et al. Variation in symptoms of sleep-disordered breathing with race
and ethnicity: The Sleep Heart Health Study. Sleep 2003; 26(1):74–79.
16. Peppard PE, Young T, Palta M, et al. Longitudinal study of moderate weight change and sleep-
disordered breathing. JAMA 2000; 284(23):3015–3021.
17. Redline S, Tosteson T, Tishler PV, et al. Studies in the genetics of obstructive sleep apnea. Am Rev
Respir Dis 1992; 145:440–444.
18. Newman AB, Foster G, Givelber R, et al. Progression and regression of sleep-disordered breath-
ing with changes in weight: The Sleep Heart Health Study. Arch Intern Med 2005; 165(20):
2408–2413.
19. Taasan VC, Block AJ, Boysen PG. Alcohol increase in sleep apnea and alcohol desaturation in asymp-
tomatic men. Am J Med 1981; 71:240–245.
20. Wetter DW, Young TB, Bidwell TR, et al. Smoking as a risk factor for sleep-disordered breathing. Arch
Intern Med 1994; 154(19):2219–2224.
21. Kushida CA, Littner MR, Hirshkowitz M, et al. Practice parameters for the use of continuous and
bilevel positive airway pressure devices to treat adult patients with sleep-related breathing disorders.
Sleep 2006; 29(3):375–380.
c19 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:56 Char Count=
22. Lim J, Lasserson TJ, Fleetham J, et al. Oral appliances for obstructive sleep apnoea. Cochrane Database
Syst Rev 2006; (1):CD004435.
23. Sundaram S, Bridgman SA, Lim J, et al. Surgery for obstructive sleep apnoea. Cochrane Database Syst
Rev 2005; (4):CD001004.
24. Wickwire EM. Behavioral management of sleep-disordered breathing. Prim Psychiatry 2009; 16(2):34–
41.
25. Guilleminault C, Eldridge FL, Dement WC. Insomnia with sleep apnea: A new syndrome. Science
1973; 181(102):856–858.
26. Lavie P. Insomnia and sleep-disordered breathing. Sleep Med 2007; 8(suppl 4):S21–S25.
27. Chung KF. Insomnia subtypes and their relationships to daytime sleepiness in patients with obstructive
sleep apnea. Respiration 2005; 72(5):460–465.
28. Smith S, Sullivan K, Hopkins W, et al. Frequency of insomnia report in patients with obstructive sleep
apnoea hypopnea syndrome (OSAHS). Sleep Med 2004; 5(5):449–456.
29. Gold AR, Gold MS, Harris KW, et al. Hypersomnolence, insomnia and the pathophysiology of upper
airway resistance syndrome. Sleep Med 2007; 9(6):675–683.
30. Krakow B, Melendrez D, Ferreira E, et al. Prevalence of insomnia symptoms in patients with sleep-
disordered breathing. Chest 2001; 120(6):1923–1929.
31. Krell SB, Kapur VK. Insomnia complaints in patients evaluated for obstructive sleep apnea. Sleep
Breath 2005; 9(3):104–110.
32. Chung KF. Relationships between insomnia and sleep-disordered breathing. Chest 2003; 123(1):310–
311.
33. Wickwire EM, Smith MT, Birnbaum S, et al. The relation between insomnia complaints and CPAP use
in a patient sample. Sleep. 2009; 31(Abstract suppl):A227.
34. Morin CM. Insomnia: Psychological Assessment and Management. New York, NY: Guilford Press,
1993.
35. Stone J, Morin CM, Hart RP, et al. Neuropsychological functioning in older insomniacs with or without
obstructive sleep apnea. Psychol Aging 1994; 9(2):231–236.
36. Lichstein KL, Riedel BW, Lester KW, et al. Occult sleep apnea in a recruited sample of older adults
with insomnia. J Consult Clin Psychol 1999; 67(3):405–410.
37. Guilleminault C, Palombini L, Poyares D, et al. Chronic insomnia, postmenopausal women, and sleep
disordered breathing: Part 1. Frequency of sleep disordered breathing in a cohort. J Psychosom Res
2002; 53(1):611–615.
38. Guilleminault C, Palombini L, Poyares D, et al. Chronic insomnia, postmenopausal women, and sleep
disordered breathing: Part 1. Frequency of sleep disordered breathing in a cohort. J Psychosom Res
2002; 53(1):611–615.
39. Bixler EO, Vgontzas AN, Lin HM, et al. Insomnia in central Pennsylvania. J Psychosom Res 2002;
53(1):589–592.
40. Gooneratne NS, Gehrman PR, Nkwuo JE, et al. Consequences of comorbid insomnia symptoms and
sleep-related breathing disorder in elderly subjects. Arch Intern Med 2006; 166(16):1732–1738.
41. Krakow B, Melendrez D, Pedersen B, et al. Complex insomnia: Insomnia and sleep-disordered breath-
ing in a consecutive series of crime victims with nightmares and PTSD. Biol Psychiatry 2001; 49(11):948–
953.
42. Krakow B, Haynes PL, Warner TD, et al. Nightmares, insomnia, and sleep-disordered breathing in
fire evacuees seeking treatment for posttraumatic sleep disturbance. J Trauma Stress 2004; 17(3):
257–268.
43. Machado MA, de Carvalho LB, Juliano ML, et al. Clinical co-morbidities in obstructive sleep apnea
syndrome treated with mandibular repositioning appliance. Respir Med 2006; 100(6):988–995.
44. Guilleminault C, Palombini L, Poyares D, et al. Chronic insomnia, premenopausal women and sleep
disordered breathing: Part 2. Comparison of nondrug treatment trials in normal breathing and UARS
post menopausal women complaining of chronic insomnia. J Psychosom Res 2002; 53(1):617–623.
45. Krakow B, Melendrez D, Lee SA, et al. Refractory insomnia and sleep-disordered breathing: A pilot
study. Sleep Breath 2004; 8(1):15–29.
46. Krakow B, Melendrez D, Sisley B, et al. Nasal dilator strip therapy for chronic sleep-maintenance
insomnia and symptoms of sleep-disordered breathing: A randomized controlled trial. Sleep Breath
2006; 10(1):16–28.
47. Guilleminault C, Palombini L, Poyares D, et al. Chronic insomnia, premenopausal women and sleep
disordered breathing: Part 2. Comparison of nondrug treatment trials in normal breathing and UARS
post menopausal women complaining of chronic insomnia. J Psychosom Res 2002; 53(1):617–623.
48. Nishino T, Shirahata M, Yonezawa T, et al. Comparison of changes in the hypoglossal and the phrenic
nerve activity in response to increasing depth of anesthesia in cats. Anesthesiology 1984; 60(1):19–24.
49. Drummond GB. Comparison of sedation with midazolam and ketamine: Effects on airway muscle
activity. Br J Anaesth 1996; 76(5):663–667.
c19 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 8:56 Char Count=
50. Nozaki-Taguchi N, Isono S, Nishino T, et al. Upper airway obstruction during midazolam sedation:
Modification by nasal CPAP. Can J Anaesth 1995; 42(8):685–690.
51. Guilleminault C. Benzodiazepines, breathing, and sleep. Am J Med 1990; 88(3A):25S–28S.
52. Landolt HP, Roth C, Dijk DJ, et al. Late-afternoon ethanol intake affects nocturnal sleep and the sleep
EEG in middle-aged men. J Clin Psychopharmacol 1996; 16(6):428–436.
53. Berry RB, Bonnet MH, Light RW. Effect of ethanol on the arousal response to airway occlusion during
sleep in normal subjects. Am Rev Respir Dis 1992; 145(2 Pt 1):445–452.
54. Collop NA. Medroxyprogesterone acetate and ethanol-induced exacerbation of obstructive sleep
apnea. Chest 1994; 106(3):792–799.
55. Krol RC, Knuth SL, Bartlett D. Selective reduction of genioglossal muscle activity by alcohol in normal
human subjects. Am Rev Respir Dis 1984; 129(2):247–250.
56. Robinson RW, White DP, Zwillich CW. Moderate alcohol ingestion increases upper airway resistance
in normal subjects. Am Rev Respir Dis 1985; 132(6):1238–1241.
57. Bonora M, Shields GI, Knuth SL, et al. Selective depression by ethanol of upper airway respiratory
motor activity in cats. Am Rev Respir Dis 1984; 130(2):156–161.
58. St John WM, Bartlett D Jr., Knuth KV, et al. Differential depression of hypoglossal nerve activity
by alcohol. Protection by pretreatment with medroxyprogesterone acetate. Am Rev Respir Dis 1986;
133(1):46–48.
59. Scanlan MF, Roebuck T, Little PJ, et al. Effect of moderate alcohol upon obstructive sleep apnoea. Eur
Respir J 2000; 16(5):909–913.
60. Samuels SI, Rabinov W. Difficulty reversing drug-induced coma in a patient with sleep apnea. Anesth
Analg 1986; 65(11):1222–1224.
61. Keamy MF III, Cadieux RJ, Kofke WA, et al. The occurrence of obstructive sleep apnea in a recovery
room patient. Anesthesiology 1987; 66(2):232–234.
62. Etches RC. Respiratory depression associated with patient-controlled analgesia: A review of eight
cases. Can J Anaesth 1994; 41(2):125–132.
63. Ostermeier AM, Roizen MF, Hautkappe M, et al. Three sudden postoperative respiratory arrests
associated with epidural opioids in patients with sleep apnea. Anesth Analg 1997; 85(2):452–460.
64. Berry RB, Kouchi K, Bower J, et al. Triazolam in patients with obstructive sleep apnea. Am J Respir
Crit Care Med 1995; 151(2 Pt 1):450–454.
65. Dolly FR, Block AJ. Effect of flurazepam on sleep-disordered breathing and nocturnal oxygen desat-
uration in asymptomatic subjects. Am J Med 1982; 73(2):239–243.
66. Hoijer U, Hedner J, Ejnell H, et al. Nitrazepam in patients with sleep apnoea: A double-blind placebo-
controlled study. Eur Respir J 1994; 7(11):2011–2015.
67. Camacho ME, Morin CM. The effect of temazepam on respiration in elderly insomniacs with mild
sleep apnea. Sleep 1995; 18(8):644–645.
68. Rosenberg R, Roach JM, Scharf M, et al. A pilot study evaluating acute use of eszopiclone in patients
with mild to moderate obstructive sleep apnea syndrome. Sleep Med 2007; 8(5):464–470.
69. Bradshaw DA, Ruff GA, Murphy DP. An oral hypnotic medication does not improve continu-
ous positive airway pressure compliance in men with obstructive sleep apnea. Chest 2006; 130(5):
1369–1376.
70. Berry RB, Patel PB. Effect of zolpidem on the efficacy of continuous positive airway pressure as
treatment for obstructive sleep apnea. Sleep 2006; 29(8):1052–1056.
71. Quera-Salva MA, McCann C, Boudet J, et al. Effects of zolpidem on sleep architecture, night time
ventilation, daytime vigilance and performance in heavy snorers. Br J Clin Pharmacol 1994; 37(6):
539–543.
72. Cirignotta F, Mondini S, Zucconi M, et al. Zolpidem-polysomnographic study of the effect of a new
hypnotic drug in sleep apnea syndrome. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1988; 29(4):807–809.
73. Lofaso F, Goldenberg F, Thebault C, et al. Effect of zopiclone on sleep, night-time ventilation, and
daytime vigilance in upper airway resistance syndrome. Eur Respir J 1997; 10(11):2573–2577.
74. Coyle MA, Mendelson WB, Derchak PA, et al. Ventilatory safety of zaleplon during sleep in patients
with obstructive sleep apnea on continuous positive airway pressure. J Clin Sleep Med 2005; 1(1):97.
75. Kryger M, Wang-Weigand S, Roth T. Safety of ramelteon in individuals with mild to moderate obstruc-
tive sleep apnea. Sleep Breath 2007; 11(3):159–164.
76. Berry RB, Desa MM, Light RW. Effect of ethanol on the efficacy of nasal continuous positive airway
pressure as a treatment for obstructive sleep apnea. Chest 1991; 99(2):339–343.
77. Wickwire E, Schumacher J, Richert A, et al. Combined insomnia and poor CPAP compliance: A case
study and discussion. Clinical Case Studies 2008; 4(7):267–286.
78. Aloia MS, Arnedt JT, Riggs RL, et al. Clinical management of poor adherence to CPAP: Motivational
enhancement. Behav Sleep Med 2004; 2(4):205–222.
c20 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:2 Char Count=
Sonia Ancoli-Israel
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California, U.S.A.
INTRODUCTION
Insomnia is the most common sleep-related complaint in the elderly and its prevalence increases
with age. In one study of more than 9000 older adults, 42% reported difficulty falling or staying
asleep (1). The high frequency of insomnia has led to a popular myth that disturbed sleep is a
part of the normal aging process. In fact, it is the factors associated with aging, but not aging per
se, that contribute to poor sleep. Therefore, it is not surprising that insomnia is so prevalent in
the elderly given the multiplicity of both internal and external factors that can have a negative
impact on sleep.
Aside from the increased prevalence, insomnia is of particular concern in the elderly
because of its related consequences. In addition to the sequelae seen in younger adults, such
as problems with concentration and poor quality of life, insomnia in older adults is associated
with greater risk of falls, difficulty with ambulation and balance, and visual impairment, over
and above the effects of medications (2,3). The increased fall risk is particularly concerning
since this is a strong predictor of nursing home placement (4). Compared to older adults
without sleep complaints, those with insomnia demonstrate slower reaction times and greater
cognitive dysfunction in domains such as memory (5). Thus age-related cognitive decline can
be exacerbated by insomnia. Perhaps most significantly, disturbed sleep increases the risk of
all-cause mortality, even after controlling for important covariates (6).
It is important to keep in mind the definition of insomnia. For an individual to receive
a diagnosis of insomnia (7,8) there must be both disturbed sleep and a subjective complaint
that the disturbance is associated with daytime distress or impairment. Not all individuals with
disturbed sleep meet criteria for insomnia. Although this phenomenon occurs at all ages it may
be particularly salient for older adults who might not express distress over poor sleep because
of the belief that it is just a natural consequence of aging. Fichten and colleagues separated
subjects in a study of older adults into three groups defined as good sleepers, poor sleepers
with a sleep-related distress, and poor sleepers without distress (9). The largest groups consisted
of clear good and poor sleepers (i.e., reported sleep quantity and rating of sleep-related distress
were in agreement). However, there was also a sizeable group who had disturbed sleep based on
quantitative criteria but who did not complain of poor sleep. The field of sleep research contains
a number of studies of ‘insomnia’ that did not assess for distress or impairment and hence
can only be considered studies of ‘disturbed sleep’ or ‘insomnia symptoms’ which represents a
more heterogeneous phenotype. As such an effort will be made in this chapter to use the term
insomnia only for studies that utilized this criterion.
Sleep Architecture
There are both quantitative and qualitative changes in sleep changes with age. Polysomno-
graphic studies of older adults that did not focus exclusively on those with insomnia complaints
found longer sleep onset latency, decreased total sleep time, and greater number of awakenings
during the night, despite greater time spent in bed (10). There is an associated change in the
distribution of sleep stages with more time spent in lighter stages N1 and N2 and less time spent
c20 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:2 Char Count=
in REM sleep. There is also a gradual decline in slow wave sleep (SWS), beginning earlier in life
during young and middle adulthood, resulting in reduction of delta wave amplitude (11). The
fact that these sleep changes occur with age irrespective of complaints of poor sleep has led to a
popular myth that sleep need declines with age. However, evidence suggests that it is not sleep
need but rather sleep ability that changes (12).
There are also changes in sleep/wake patterns during the day. The frequency and duration
of napping increase with age (13). This trend is maintained even within the elderly population
such that “old old” individuals reported more napping than the “young old” (14). Napping is
often a coping strategy used to compensate for poor sleep as suggested by data demonstrating a
relationship between nighttime sleep fragmentation and increased daytime napping (15). There
are some data to indicate that napping can improve daytime functioning. For example, Monk
and colleagues found that an afternoon nap decreased objective sleepiness as measured on the
Multiple Sleep Latency Test (16). However, this same study found that napping was associated
with reduced total sleep time and earlier wake times the following night. It has been argued
that daytime napping reduces homeostatic sleep drive that has accumulated prior to the nap, so
at bedtime there is less drive available to initiate and maintain sleep, although data examining
this relationship are mixed (13).
Homeostatic Processes
Age-related changes in sleep architecture are likely a reflection of changes in underlying brain
processes for sleep and wakefulness. According to current models of sleep/wake regulation,
arousal state is determined by homeostatic (process S) and circadian (process C) mechanisms
(17). The homeostatic process is a drive for sleep that accumulates during wakefulness and is
discharged during sleep. Insomnia, in particular sleep onset complaints, could be produced
by an insufficient build-up of sleep drive such that at bedtime sleep-promoting processes are
weaker than those promoting wakefulness. Sleep maintenance complaints, on the other hand,
could be the result of a decline in process S during sleep that is too rapid. This would create
a situation in which there is not sufficient sleep drive to keep the individual asleep in the
latter part of the night, particularly after the minimum of core body temperature when the
drive for wakefulness increases. There is evidence that homeostatic processes change with age.
Delta activity during sleep is a marker of sleep drive, so the age-related decline in SWS can
be interpreted as indicating reduced drive (18). Theta EEG activity, which acts similarly as
a marker of homeostatic drive during wakefulness (19), has been found to decline with age
although this may reflect an overall decline in EEG power across frequency bands and not
a decline in homeostatic drive (20). Sleep deprivation experiments also provide evidence of
reduced homeostatic drive in the elderly. Following a period of sleep deprivation, and therefore
enhanced accumulation of sleep drive, good sleepers exhibit subsequent recovery sleep that is
of longer duration and is characterized by greater delta EEG activity compared to baseline sleep.
In contrast, studies of individuals with insomnia have found reduced rebound in delta activity
following sleep deprivation compared to good sleepers, although the effects on total sleep
time were mixed (18). In contrast to research on the accumulation of sleep drive in insomnia, no
studies have examined the rate of decline of slow wave activity during the night. Given the high
prevalence of sleep maintenance complaints in the elderly this possible etiological mechanism
need to be explored.
Circadian Rhythms
The second sleep/wake regulatory process is circadian rhythmicity. Most physiological pro-
cesses follow predictable rhythmic patterns with a periodicity of approximately 24 hours. These
circadian rhythms are generated endogenously by the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) of the
hypothalamus and are influenced by environmental factors that act as zeitgebers or ‘time cues,’
both of which can change with age. There is a substantial body of research documenting age-
related changes in circadian rhythms in humans and various animal species. Some have even
speculated that deterioration of circadian rhythms may be a central component of the aging
process (21). Early free-running studies in animals (22) and humans (23) suggested that there
is a decrease in the intrinsic period of circadian rhythms. The human data have been criticized,
c20 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:2 Char Count=
however, because of more recent findings that demonstrate that self-selected light exposure
during free-running protocols affects estimates of period derived from these studies (24). In
a more recent constant routine study comparing core body temperature rhythms in younger
and older subjects, Czeisler and colleagues found almost identical periods in the two groups
(25). This and similar studies suggest that, at least in humans, there is no decrease in period
associated with aging.
A second age-related change is a decrease in the amplitude of the circadian rhythms,
including in the rhythm of body temperature, melatonin, urinary electrolytes, and prolactin
(10). In one study younger subjects had an average core body temperature amplitude that was
40% greater than in older subjects (26). Data on the amplitude of rest-activity rhythms, which
are less of a ‘pure’ measure of circadian rhythmicity, are mixed (e.g., 27).
A phase advance of circadian rhythms has been consistently found in older compared to
younger adults. During constant routine studies, a phase advance of approximately 90 minutes
was found in older adults (10). These results are in accordance with those found for circadian
rhythms of melatonin (28) and cortisol (29) in older adults. This phase advance manifests as
difficulties with sleepiness in the evening prior to the desired bedtime and early morning
awakenings with difficulty returning to sleep (see below).
Lastly, there is some evidence that there is a desynchronization of circadian rhythms with
age. Wever found that 70% of subjects between the ages of 40 and 70 years became internally
desynchronized during free running compared to only 22% of younger subjects (30). In another
study, it was found that 18% of younger subjects met criteria for desynchronization of rhythms
compared to 50% of older adults (31).
There are several etiological pathways for the age-related decline in circadian rhythmicity
including reductions in endogenous circadian drive, environmental cues, and entrainment.
Swaab et al. found that the volume of the SCN decreased by 41% with age in subjects more than
80 years of age (32). In animals, lesions that destroy a large portion of the SCN are sufficient
to cause disruptions in circadian rhythms that produced nocturnal wakefulness and daytime
napping, similar to sleep changes seen in aging (33). Recently Malatesta and colleagues found
evidence for changes in CLOCK protein activity in older compared to younger animals (34),
suggesting that there may be age-related declines in the fundamental molecular pathways that
generate circadian rhythms.
According to the “environmental hypothesis,” deficient zeitgebers, which entrain endoge-
nous rhythms to the external environment, are the cause of disrupted circadian rhythms in aging
(35). Bright light plays an important role as a zeitgeber and studies have found that many older
adults receive very little exposure to bright light. One study reported that older adults were
exposed to bright light (>1000 lux) for only 35 minutes/day compared to 102 minutes in
younger adults (36). In a study of community-dwelling elderly, investigators reported that the
median duration of exposure to light more than 1000 lux was only 4% (37). Exposure to bright
light is the strongest, but not the only environmental cue that can entrain circadian rhythms.
Older adults have also been shown to have less exposure to other zeitgebers such as physical
activity (36).
Whereas light acts as the primary zeitgeber during the day, melatonin plays an important
role in sleep/wake regulation at night. A number of investigators have reported age-related
decreases in melatonin production. Kennaway and colleagues conducted a meta-analysis of
these studies and found a decrease of peak plasma melatonin levels of 36% and 43% in adults age
50 to 65 and 65 to 80, respectively, compared to adults between the ages of 20 and 35 (38). Several
theories have been proposed to explain the decline in melatonin with age including decreased
beta-adrenergic receptors in the pineal, increased clearance of melatonin in the brain, and
reduced exposure to bright light (39). Recent data also suggest that there is an age-related decline
in responsiveness of the SCN to melatonin (40). However, data on the relationship between
melatonin production and insomnia complaints in the elderly are very mixed. Lushington and
colleagues found no differences in urinary melatonin metabolites between elderly with sleep
maintenance insomnia and good sleepers (41). The therapeutic effects of exogenous melatonin
on insomnia have been equally variable, with several large controlled studies failing to find any
significant benefit (42). The relationship between melatonin and sleep in older adults is clearly
complex and further research will be necessary to untangle the intricacies.
c20 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:2 Char Count=
Aging may be associated with a failure of the entrainment process itself. Light exposure
affects activity of the SCN via the retinohypothalamic tract, which is the primary neural pathway
by which bright light entrains circadian rhythms. Once photic (light-related) information reaches
the SCN it induces the expression of immediate early genes, such as c-fos and jun-B, at times
of day that correspond to the phase response curve to light (43). Sutin et al. found that there
was an age-related decrease in the response of immediate early genes to photic stimuli (44)
suggesting a possible site of molecular changes with age that impact the entrainment of circadian
rhythms.
The results of these and other studies suggest that there are definite changes in circadian
rhythms with age that reflect a variety of etiological pathways. Coupled with the changes in
homeostatic processes, it is clear that increasing age is associated with a greater vulnerability to
insomnia due to alterations in basic sleep/wake regulation. This is consistent with the concept
of predisposing factors in Spielman’s 3-P’s model of insomnia (45). Increased vulnerability to
insomnia in older adults can interact with precipitating events that trigger an episode of disturbed
sleep.
Medical Factors
Advancing age is associated with, on average, a greater number of physical health problems.
In the 2003, National Sleep Foundation poll of sleep in older adults it was found that the likeli-
hood of disturbed sleep increased in parallel with increases in the number of medical conditions
reported (46). This relationship exists across a wide range of medical conditions including car-
diac and pulmonary disease, but the strongest relationships are found in conditions associated
with pain. Sleep maintenance is particularly affected with 81% of arthritis patients and 85% of
chronic pain patients reported difficulty staying asleep (47). Aside from pain, chronic medical
conditions more prevalent in the older adult such as nocturia secondary to an enlarge prostate,
shortness of breath in congestive heart failure or chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, and
neurological damage related to cerebrovascular accidents or dementia can negatively impact
sleep in other ways (48–50). As the number of medical conditions increases these factors can
have a cumulative effect on sleep.
Psychiatric Disorders
Psychiatric illness is a frequent comorbidity with insomnia. Insomnia is a diagnostic criterion for
major depression and generalized anxiety disorder, and sleep disturbance is a common correlate
of psychopathology in general (51). The relationship between insomnia and depression/anxiety
is bi-directional. The onset of psychiatric illness is often associated with a worsening of sleep.
However, several longitudinal studies have now found that insomnia in psychiatrically healthy
individuals is a risk factor for later development of psychopathology, particularly depression
and anxiety (52). The prevalence of psychiatric illness is lower in the elderly compared to
younger and middle-aged adults (53) so the higher rate of insomnia in the aged population is
not a consequence of greater psychopathology. Although insomnia often occurs in the context
of psychiatric illness (so called ‘secondary insomnia’) data are emerging that resolution of the
illness is often not associated with an improvement in sleep (54). It appears that over time the
insomnia develops into an independent condition requiring sleep-specific interventions. It is
likely that a portion of the elderly insomnia population first experienced disturbed sleep in the
context of psychopathology earlier in life, with perpetuation of the insomnia over time despite
the absence of ongoing comorbid psychopathology.
c20 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:2 Char Count=
Dementia
Although the prevalence of psychopathology in general declines with age, there is an increase
in dementia (53), which is often associated with insomnia. In one study of community dwelling
adults with dementia, between 19% and 44% complained of disturbed sleep (55). Polysomno-
graphic studies have documented lower sleep efficiency, greater nocturnal awakenings, and
more time spent in lighter stages of sleep compared to nondemented older adults (10). Fragmen-
tation of sleep at night is accompanied by disruption of wakefulness during the day. Patients
with dementia often sleep during the day, with one study of institutionalized patients with
Alzheimer’s dementia finding that patients were rarely asleep for a full hour at night or awake
for a full hour during the day (56). Insomnia can exacerbate the difficulties of providing care
for an individual with dementia because they may be awake and wandering around during the
night. Pollak and Perlick found that disturbed sleep was one of the primary reasons caregivers
cited for decisions regarding institutionalization (57). During the day, cognitive functioning
may be further impaired as a result of poor sleep.
It is not clear if the sleep disturbance is the result of neurological damage associated with
the dementia or if it is related to the other factors such as pain or depression (58). Many of the
age-related changes in sleep/wake processes are magnified in dementia including deterioration
of the SCN (32) and reduced circadian rhythmicity of melatonin (59) and activity (60). There is
often also a paucity of exposure to zeitgebers, particularly bright light. Campbell et al found that
community-dwelling elderly with mild Alzheimer’s dementia received, on average, less than
30 minutes of bright light exposure per day (61). The situation is even worse for those in nursing
institutions that are often dimly lit (62). Increased prevalence of sleep disordered breathing (SDB)
and periodic limb movements in sleep (PLMS) in patients with dementia further complicates
the situation (63).
Sleep Disorders
In the older adult, primary sleep disorders also contribute to the decreased ability to get sufficient
sleep, and may present with a primary complaint of insomnia. SDB is characterized by recurrent
apneic events throughout the night due to obstruction of airflow or reduced respiratory effort.
Reduced blood oxygen triggers an arousal that stimulates a resumption of normal airflow.
These arousals are often very brief and are typically not remembered the next day; however
they often can lead to full awakenings. Even if each awakening is brief, their cumulative impact
can be experienced as a sleep maintenance problem. This may be further compounded by
increasing difficulty returning to sleep in some individuals. The apnea/insomnia comorbidity
is particularly relevant in older adults, as both conditions occur with higher prevalence than
in younger adults. In a randomly selected community sample of older adults Ancoli-Israel and
colleagues found that 81% had an apnea hypopnea index (AHI) of at least five events/hr (64).
Even at a more stringent cutoff of 20 events/hr, 44% of the samples were categorized as having
SDB. These rates are considerably higher than in young and middle aged adults. In one case-
control study of older adults with insomnia complaints there was an additive impact of daytime
functioning such that impairment was greatest in subjects with both conditions compared to
those with only one or zero conditions (65). Rates of SDB are even higher in the portion of
the elderly population with dementia (see below). The gold standard treatment for SDB is
continuous positive airway pressure (CPAP), with oral appliances and upper airway surgery as
alternatives. There is currently little known regarding the impact of CPAP treatment on insomnia
for individuals with both conditions, although it is unlikely that CPAP alone will eliminate
difficulties falling or staying asleep (see Collop chapter for further discussion of this issue).
Similarly, the prevalence of PLMS, the core feature of periodic limb movement disorder
(PLMD), and restless legs syndrome (RLS) increases with age. Rates of the disorders in the
elderly have been found to be as high as 45% for PLMS (66) and between 9% and 20% for RLS
(67). PLMS are characterized by repetitive limb movements throughout the night, usually in the
legs. As in sleep disordered breathing, PLMS can cause repetitive awakenings from sleep and
lead to a complaint of sleep maintenance insomnia. RLS, on the other hand, is associated with
uncomfortable, tingling sensations in the legs during rest and/or an irresistible urge to move
the legs. This typically occurs in the evening or at night, especially when sitting or lying down.
The sensations are only relieved with movement. The discomfort of RLS can interfere with sleep
c20 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:2 Char Count=
onset and make it more difficult to fall back to sleep when awake during the night. PLMD and
RLS are currently treated with medications that either target the leg movements directly or
reduce the arousal threshold. Dopaminergic agents are the most commonly prescribed class of
medications (see Chapter 18).
A third sleep disorder common in the older adult is advanced sleep phase (ASP), as
described above. Patients with ASP get sleepy early in the evening and awaken early in the
morning hours. Two scenarios are likely to occur. In the first scenario, the older adult, although
sleepy, may force himself/herself to stay awake to a more “acceptable” bedtime, yet still awaken
between 3:00 AM and 5:00 AM. This results in daytime sleepiness and perhaps daytime napping.
In the second scenario, the older patient falls asleep after dinner while reading or watching
television and then has a difficult time falling asleep when going to bed later in the evening.
That patient still awakens early in the morning. These patients then complain of both difficulty
falling asleep and difficulty staying asleep, both of which really result form the advanced phase
and poor sleep habits. This type of “insomnia” is best treated with light therapy (see below).
Pharmacologic
The first line treatment of insomnia has traditionally been pharmacologic. A wide range of med-
ications has been used to treat insomnia including sedative-hypnotics, antihistamines, antide-
pressants, antipsychotics, and anticonvulsants. Commonly used medications include those that
are indicated for the treatment of insomnia or off-label medications, used due to their sedating
side-effects. There are no data available in the elderly to support the use of agents other than
sedative-hypnotics so those medications should be used cautiously, if at all. The 2005 State of the
Science Conference on Insomnia concluded that antihistamines, antidepressants, antipsychotics
and anticonvulsants all have more risks than benefits associated with them and should not be
used for the treatment of insomnia (70).
Sedative-hypnotic medications can be classified as benzodiazepines (e.g., temazepam),
nonbenzodiazepine receptor agonists (i.e., zaleplon, zolpidem, zolpidem MR, eszopiclone),
or melatonin receptor agonists (i.e., ramelteon). Pharmacologic treatment has the advantage
c20 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:2 Char Count=
of rapid treatment response but there are important disadvantages including potential resid-
ual daytime sedation, dependence, tolerance, and withdrawal. Daytime sedation is especially
important to consider in the elderly who may be at increased risk of falls. Several studies
have found an association between use of benzodiazepines and other psychotropic drugs
and an increased risk of falls (71) although there are limited data regarding the use of the
newer sedative-hypnotic medications. Brassington and colleagues attempted to separate the
risks posed by medications from those due to disturbed sleep in a large sample of community-
dwelling elderly (2). Their results suggested that sleep problems rather than the medications was
associated with an increased risk of falls. A recent study by Formiga et al followed adults older
than age 89 years and found no difference in use of benzodiazepines between those that fell and
those that did not (72). These mixed results suggest that caution is warranted when prescribing
sedative-hypnotic medications for the elderly, especially with the older benzodiazepines and
longer-acting agents. Other factors that need to be factored into decisions for pharmacologic
treatment in the elderly are drug interactions given the high prevalence of polypharmacy, and
possible age-related changes in drug metabolism and excretion that may narrow the therapeutic
index. While there have been few studies of the effect of the older benzodiazepines or sedating
antidepressants in older adults, the newer sedative hypnotics have been shown to be safe and
effective in older patients with insomnia.
Given some of the problems associated with sedative-hypnotic medications, there has
been interest in the use of exogenous melatonin as a treatment for insomnia, particularly in
the elderly. There have been a number of randomized trials of melatonin in the elderly, both
with and without dementia, including a large multisite trial of 157 patients with Alzheimer’s
disease (42). Although some studies have found beneficial effects on sleep (ex. 73), a 2005 NIH
Consensus Conference on Insomnia concluded that there was not sufficient evidence of efficacy
(74). Melatonin receptor agonists, such as the drug ramelteon, may have more potent hypnotic
properties because they selectively bind to MT1 /MT2 receptors with a 1,000-fold greater affinity
than melatonin (75).
Cognitive-Behavioral Treatment
Although pharmacologic management is the most commonly employed treatment option, the
risks of side effects and potential need for long-term use suggest that alternative approaches may
be desirable, either alone or in combination with hypnotic medications. Cognitive-behavioral
treatment of insomnia (CBT-I) is a nonpharmacologic approach that uses a collection of strategies
to break the cycle of insomnia and reestablish a healthy sleep/wake pattern. The first component
of CBT-I is sleep hygiene, a set of habits and practices in which patients can engage in order to
promote healthy sleep. Some sleep hygiene guidelines that are particularly relevant for older
adults include reducing daytime napping and engaging in regular physical activity. For many
patients with insomnia, the bed and bedroom are not a cue for sleep because nonsleep activities
such as reading and watching TV takes place in bed. McCrae and colleagues assessed sleep
hygiene practices in community-dwelling older adults and found that those with insomnia
did not engage in a greater number of poor sleep hygiene practices, but may have been more
susceptible to their sleep-disrupting effects (76). Stimulus control is designed to reestablish an
association between the bed and sleep by eliminating sleep-incompatible activities in bed (77).
Often, patients with insomnia believe they can achieve a reasonable amount of sleep by spending
excessive time in bed, but this results in a low sleep efficiency. The technique of sleep restriction
has patients curtail time spent in bed in order to increase sleep efficiency (78). The rationale
behind this approach is that reducing time in bed will lead to an acute reduction in total sleep
time and hence an accumulation of homeostatic sleep drives. Over the course of several weeks
the increased sleep drive leads in increase in sleep efficiency. As sleep efficiency improves, the
patient is allowed to increase time in bed. There has been concern about using sleep restriction
in the elderly due to the potential sleep-deprivation related increased risk of falls. To minimize
this risk, a variant of sleep restriction, called sleep compression (79), can be used that reduces
time in bed more gradually. Patients with insomnia often report that they have difficulty in
sleeping because of excessive mental activity. Cognitive strategies are designed to alter both the
content and process of mental activity in order to facilitate sleep (80). Although there are several
cognitive techniques available, a commonly-used approach is to help patients identify distorted
c20 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:2 Char Count=
beliefs about sleep that are strongly held (ex: “I must get 8 hours of sleep in order to function
the next day.”) During the night when there is difficulty with sleep initiation, these thoughts
are activated and exacerbate the disturbance. Patients are taught to challenge these distorted
patterns of thinking, which, over time, can lead to changes in patterns of sleep-related thinking.
Meta-analyses of studies testing the efficacy of CBT-I have demonstrated significant
improvement in symptoms comparable to pharmacotherapy (81,82) including an analysis lim-
ited to studies in the elderly (83). A particularly noteworthy study by Morin and colleagues
compared CBT-I with temazepam in older adults with insomnia with a follow-up of two years
(84). Outcomes were comparable across treatment groups by the end of the active treatment
phase. However, CBT-I produced more durable improvements that were significantly better
than pharmacotherapy alone on follow-up assessment. This study also raises the question of
whether pharmacotherapy and CBT-I should be combined in order to capitalize on the quicker
treatment response of mediation and the long-lasting effects of CBT-I. They found, however,
that the combined treatment group had worse outcomes long-term than CBT-I alone (84).
More research is needed to determine if a combined treatment approach can be an efficacious
option.
Light Therapy
Aging is associated with changes in the circadian system, as described previously. Treatment
strategies that target the circadian system may therefore improve insomnia in the elderly. Given
that bright light is the most potent zeitgeber, exposure to bright light, (ie, light therapy) should
strengthen rhythms. There have been a number of studies of light therapy in elderly with
insomnia related to circadian rhythm disturbance. The specific timing of light therapy can
produce a phase delay, phase advance, or no shift in phase. The advanced sleep phase commonly
seen with aging can be treated with evening light exposure (85). Bright light should be avoided
in the morning in these individuals, which may necessitate wearing sunglasses when outside.
Although a delayed sleep phase is less common in the elderly, morning bright light exposure is
effective for producing a phase advance (85).
SUMMARY
In summary, many older adults suffer from insomnia. There is an increased vulnerability to
insomnia due to changes in basic sleep/wake processes, as well as a number of factors that
can contribute to disturbed sleep. Treatment options are generally consistent with those used in
younger adults, but there are additional considerations when working with the elderly.
REFERENCES
1. Foley D, Monjan A, Brown S, et al. Sleep complaints among elderly persons: An epidemiologic study
of three communities. Sleep 1995; 18:425–432.
2. Brassington GS, King AC, Bliwise DL. Sleep problems as a risk factor for falls in a sample of community-
dwelling adults aged 64–99 years. J Am Geriatr Soc 2000; 48:1234–1240.
3. Goldman SE, Stone KL, Ancoli-Israel S, et al. Poor sleep is associated with poorer physical performance
and greater functional limitations in older women. Sleep 2007; 30:1317–1324.
4. Tinetti ME, Williams CS. Falls, injuries due to falls, and the risk of admission to a nursing home. N
Engl J Med 1997; 337:1279–1284.
5. Crenshaw MC, Edinger JD. Slow-wave sleep and waking cognitive performance among older adults
with and without insomnia complaints. Physiol Behav 1999; 66:485–492.
6. Dew MA, Hoch CC, Buysse DJ, et al. Healthy older adults’ sleep predicts all-cause mortality at 4 to 19
years of follow-up. Psychosom Med 2003; 65:63–73.
7. APA. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed. Washington, D.C.: American
Psychiatric Association, 1994.
8. American Academy of Sleep Medicine. International Classification of Sleep Disorders, 2nd ed. Amer-
ican Academy of Sleep Medicine, Westchester, IL 2005.
9. Fichten CS, Creti L, Amsel R, et al. Poor sleepers who do not complain of insomnia: Myths and realities
about psychological and lifestyle characteristics of older good and poor sleepers. J Behav Med 1995;
18:189–223.
10. Bliwise DL. Sleep in normal aging and dementia. Sleep 1993; 16:40–81.
c20 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:2 Char Count=
11. Ohayon MM, Carskadon MA, Guilleminault C, et al. Meta-analysis of quantitative sleep parameters
from childhood to old age in healthy individuals: Developing normative sleep values across the human
lifespan. Sleep 2004; 27:1255–1273.
12. Ancoli-Israel S. Sleep problems in older adults: Putting myths to bed. Geriatrics 1997; 52:20–30.
13. Ancoli-Israel S, Martin JL. Insomnia and daytime napping in older adults. J Clin Sleep Med 2006;
2:333–342.
14. Metz ME, Bunnell DE. Napping and sleep disturbances in the elderly. Fam Pract Res J 1990; 10:47–56.
15. Goldman SE, Hall M, Boudreau R, et al. Association between nighttime sleep and napping in older
adults. Sleep 2008; 31:733–740.
16. Monk TH, Buysse DJ, Carrier J, et al. Effects of afternoon “siesta” naps on sleep, alertness, performance,
and circadian rhythms in the elderly. Sleep 2001; 24:680–687.
17. Borbely AA. A two process model of sleep regulation. Hum Neurobiol 1982; 1:195–204.
18. Pigeon WR, Perlis ML. Sleep homeostasis in primary insomnia. Sleep Med Rev 2006; 10:247–254.
19. Finelli L, Baumann H, Borbely A, et al. Dual electroencephalogram markers of human sleep home-
ostasis: Correlation between theta activity in waking and slow-wave activity in sleep. Neuroscience
2000; 3:523–529.
20. Breslau J, Starr A, Sicotte N, et al. Topographic EEG changes with normal aging and SDAT. Electroen-
cephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1989; 72:281–289.
21. Samis HV. Aging: The loss of temporal organization. Perspect Biol Med 1968; 12:95–102.
22. Pittendrigh CS, Daan S. A functional analysis of circadian pacemakers in nocturnal rodents I. The
stability and lability of spontaneous frequency. J Comp Physiol 1976; 106:223.
23. Weitzman ED, Moline ML, Czeisler CA, et al. Chronobiology of aging: Temperature, sleep-wake
rhythms and entrainment. Neurobiol Aging 1982; 3:299–309.
24. Klerman EB, Dijk D-, Kronauer RE. Simulations of effects of light on the human circadian pacemaker:
Implications for assessment of intrinsic period. Am J Physiol 1996; 270:R271–R282.
25. Czeisler CA, Duffy JF, Shanahan TL, et al. Stability, precision, and near-24-hour period of the human
circadian pacemaker. Science 1999; 284:2177–2181.
26. Czeisler CA, Dumont M, Duffy JF, et al. Association of sleep-wake habits in older people with changes
in output of circadian pacemaker. Lancet 1992; 340:933–936.
27. Girardin J-, Kripke DF, Ancoli-Israel S, et al. Circadian sleep, illumination, and activity patterns in
women: Influences of aging and time reference. Physiol Behav 2000; 68:347–352.
28. Van Coevorden A, Mockel J, Laurent E. Neuroendocrine rhythms and sleep in aging men. Am J Physiol
1991; 260:E651–661.
29. Van Cauter E, Leproult R, Kupfer DJ. Effects of gender and age on the levels and circadian rhythmicity
of plasma cortisol. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1996; 81:2468–2473.
30. Wever RA. The Circadian System of Man: Results of experiments under temporal isolation. Berlin:
Springer-Verlag, 1979.
31. Cahn HA, Folk GEJ, Huston PE. Age comparison of human day-night physiological differences.
Aerosp Med 1968; 39:608.
32. Swaab DF, Fliers E, Partiman TS. The suprachasmatic nucleus of the human brain in relation to sex,
age and senile dementia. Brain Res 1985; 342:37–44.
33. Eastman CI, Mistlberger RE, Rechtschaffen A. Suprachiasmatic nuclei lesions eliminate circadian
temperature and sleep rhythms in the rat. Physiol Behav 1984; 32:357–368.
34. Malatesta M, Fattoretti P, Baldelli B, et al. Effects of ageing on the fine distribution of the circadian
CLOCK protein in reticular formation neurons. Histochem Cell Biol 2007; 127:641–647.
35. Harper DG, Stopa EG, McKee AC, et al. Differential circadian rhythm disturbances in men with
Alzheimer Disease and frontotemporal degeneration. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001; 58:353–360.
36. Sanchez R, Ge Y, Zee PC. A comparison of the strength of external zeitgebers in young and older
adults. Sleep Res 1993; 22:416.
37. Espiritu RC, Kripke DF, Ancoli-Israel S, et al. Low illumination by San Diego adults: Association with
atypical depressive symptoms. Biol Psychiatry 1994; 35:403–407.
38. Kennaway DJ, Lushington K, Dawson D, et al. Urinary 6-sulfatoxymelatonin excretion and aging:
New results and a critical review of the literature. J Pineal Res 1999; 27:210–220.
39. Pandi-Perumal SR, Zisapel N, Srinivasan V, et al. Melatonin and sleep in aging population. Exp
Gerontol 2005; 40:911–925.
40. von Gall C, Weaver DR. Loss of responsiveness to melatonin in the aging mouse suprachiasmatic
nucleus. Neurobiol Aging 2008; 29:464–470.
41. Lushington K, Lack L, Kennaway DJ, et al. 6-Sulfatoxymelatonin excretion and self-reported sleep in
good sleeping controls and 55–80-year-old insomniacs. J Sleep Res 1998; 7:75–83.
42. Singer C, Tractenberg RE, Kaye J, et al. A multicenter, placebo-controlled trial of melatonin for sleep
disturbance in Alzheimer’s disease. Sleep 2003; 26:893–901.
c20 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:2 Char Count=
43. Kornhauser JM, Nelson DE, Mayo KE, et al. Photic and circadian regulation of c-fos gene expression
in the hamster suprachiasmatic nucleus. Neuron 1990; 5:127–134.
44. Sutin EL, Dement WC, Heller HC, et al. Light-induced gene expression in the suprachiasmatic nucleus
of young and aging rats. Neurobiol Aging 1993; 14:441–446.
45. Spielman A, Caruso L, Glovinsky P. A behavioral perspective on insomnia treatment. Psychiatr Clin
North Am 1987; 10:541–553.
46. Foley D, Ancoli-Israel S, Britz P, et al. Sleep disturbances and chronic disease in older adults: Results
of the 2003 National Sleep Foundation Sleep in America Survey. J Psychosom Res 2004; 56:497–502.
47. Wilcox S, Brenes GA, Levine D, et al. Factors related to sleep disturbance in older adults experiencing
knee pain or knee pain with radiographic evidence of knee osteoarthritis. J Am Geriatr Soc 2000;
48:1241–1251.
48. Klink ME, Quan SF, Kaltenborn WT, et al. Risk factors associated with complaints of insomnia in
a general adult population. Influence of previous complaints of insomnia. Arch Intern Med 1992;
152:1634–1637.
49. Quan SF, Katz R, Olson J, et al. Factors associated with incidence and persistence of symptoms of
disturbed sleep in an elderly cohort: The Cardiovascular Health Study. Am J Med Sci 2005; 329:163–
172.
50. Garcia-Borreguero D, Larrosa O, Bravo M. Parkinson’s disease and sleep. Sleep Med Rev 2003; 7:115–
129.
51. Benca RM, Obermeyer WH, Thisted RA, et al. Sleep and psychiatric disorders: A meta-analysis. Arch
Gen Psychiatry 1992; 49:651–670.
52. Riemann D, Voderholzer U. Primary insomnia: A risk factor to develop depression? J Affect Disord
2003; 76:255–259.
53. Regier DA, Boyd JH, Burke JD Jr, et al. One-month prevalence of mental disorders in the
United States. Based on five Epidemiologic Catchment Area sites. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1988; 45:
977–986.
54. Stepanski EJ, Rybarczyk B. Emerging research on the treatment and etiology of secondary or comorbid
insomnia. Sleep Med Rev 2006; 10:7–18.
55. McCurry SM, Reynolds CF, Ancoli-Israel S, et al. Treatment of sleep disturbance in Alzheimer’s disease.
Sleep Med Rev 2000; 4:603–628.
56. Pat-Horenczyk R, Klauber MR, Shochat T, Ancoli-Israel S. Hourly profiles of sleep and wakefulness in
severely versus mild-moderately demented nursing home patients. Aging Clin Exp Res 1998; 10:308–
315.
57. Pollak CP, Perlick D. Sleep problems and institutionalization of the elderly. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol
1991; 4:204–210.
58. Dauvilliers Y. Insomnia in patients with neurodegenerative conditions. Sleep Med 2007; 8(suppl
4):S27–S34.
59. Uchida K, Okamoto N, Ohara K, et al. Daily rhythm of serum melatonin in patients with dementia of
the degenerate type. Brain Res 1996; 717:154–159.
60. Van Someren EJW, Hagebeuk EEO, Lijzenga C, et al. Circadian rest-activity rhythm disturbances in
Alzheimer’s Disease. Biol Psychiatry 1996; 40:259–270.
61. Campbell S, Kripke DL, Gillin JC, et al. Exposure to light in healthy elderly subjects and Alzheimer’s
patients. Physiol Behav 1988; 42:141–144.
62. Ancoli-Israel S, Klauber MR, Jones DW, et al. Variations in circadian rhythms of activity, sleep, and
light exposure related to dementia in nursing-home patients. Sleep 1997; 20:18–23.
63. McCurry S, Ancoli-Israel S. Sleep dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease and other dementias. Curr Treat
Options Neurol 2003; 5:261–272.
64. Ancoli-Israel S, Kripke DF, Klauber MH. Sleep disordered breathing in community-dwelling elderly.
Sleep 1991; 14:486–495.
65. Gooneratne NS, Gehrman PR, Nkwuo JE, et al. Consequences of comorbid insomnia symptoms and
sleep-related breathing disorder in elderly subjects. Arch Intern Med 2006; 166:1732–1738.
66. Ancoli-Israel S, Kripke DF, Klauber MR. Periodic limb movements in sleep in community-dwelling
elderly. Sleep 1991; 14:496–500.
67. Hornyak M, Trenkwalder C. Restless legs syndrome and periodic limb movement disorder in the
elderly. J Psychosom Res 2004; 56:543–548.
68. Stein MD, Friedmann PD. Disturbed sleep and its relationship to alcohol use. Subst Abus 2005; 26:
1–13.
69. National Sleep Foundation/WB&A Market Research. Sleep in American Survey. Washington, D.C.:
National Sleep Foundation, 2005.
70. NIH State-of-the-Science Conference Statement on manifestations and management of chronic insom-
nia in adults. NIH Consens State Sci Statements 2005; 22(2):1–30.
c20 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:2 Char Count=
71. Kelly KD, Pickett W, Yiannakoulias N, et al. Medication use and falls in community-dwelling older
persons. Age Ageing 2003; 32:503–509.
72. Formiga F, Ferrer A, Duaso E, et al. Falls in Nonagenarians Living in their Own Homes: The Nonas-
antfeliu Study. J Nutr Health Aging 2008; 12:273–276.
73. Dowling GA, Mastick J, Colling E, et al. Melatonin for sleep disturbances in Parkinson’s disease. Sleep
Med 2005; 6:459–466.
74. National Institutes of Health. National Institutes of Health State of the Science Conference statement
on Manifestations and Management of Chronic Insomnia in Adults, June 13–15, 2005. Sleep 2005;
28:1049–1057.
75. Kato K, Hirai K, Nishiyama K, et al. Neurochemical properties of ramelteon (TAK-375), a selective
MT1/MT2 receptor agonist. Neuropharmacology 2005; 48:301–310.
76. McCrae CS, Rowe MA, Dautovich ND, et al. Sleep hygiene practices in two community dwelling
samples of older adults. Sleep 2006; 29:1551–1560.
77. Bootzin RR, Nicassio PM. Behavioral treatments for insomnia. In: Hersen M, Eisler RM, Miller PM,
eds. Progress in Behavior Modification, Vol. 6. New York, NY: Academic Press, Inc, 1978:1–45.
78. Spielman AJ, Saskin P, Thorpy MJ. Treatment of chronic insomnia by restriction of time in bed. Sleep
1987; 10:45–56.
79. Lichstein KL, Riedel BW, Wilson NM, et al. Relaxation and sleep compression for late-life insomnia:
A placebo-controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psychol 2001; 69:227–239.
80. Harvey AG, Tang NK, Browning L. Cognitive approaches to insomnia. Clin Psychol Rev 2005; 25:593–
611.
81. Morin CM, Culbert JP, Schwartz MS. Non-pharmacological interventions for insomnia: A meta-
analysis of treatment efficacy. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151:1172–1180.
82. Smith MT, Perlis ML, Park A, et al. Comparative meta-analysis of pharmacotherapy and behavior
therapy for persistent insomnia. Am J Psychiatry 2002; 159:5–11.
83. Montgomery P, Dennis J. Cognitive behavioural interventions for sleep problems in adults aged 60+.
Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2003; (1):CD003161.
84. Morin CM, Colecchi C, Stone J, et al. Behavioral and pharmacological therapies for late-life insomnia:
A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 1999; 281:991–999.
85. Sack RL, Auckley D, Auger RR, et al. Circadian rhythm sleep disorders: Part II, advanced sleep phase
disorder, delayed sleep phase disorder, free-running disorder, and irregular sleep-wake rhythm. An
American Academy of Sleep Medicine review. Sleep 2007; 30:1484–1501.
c21 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:7 Char Count=
21 Pediatric Insomnia
Bobbi Hopkins and Daniel Glaze
Baylor College of Medicine, Texas Children’s Hospital Children’s Sleep Center, Houston, Texas, U.S.A.
INTRODUCTION
Pediatric insomnia is defined as a “repeated difficulty with sleep initiation, duration, consolida-
tion, or quality that occurs despite age appropriate time and opportunity for sleep and results in
some form of daytime functional impairment for the child and/or the family” (1). The etiology
of the insomnia varies based on age and is frequently multifactorial. Factors contributing to
insomnia in children vary, but include bedtime resistance, inability to fall asleep easily, fre-
quent or prolonged night awakenings, early morning awakenings, circadian rhythm disorders,
parasomnias, sleep-related movement disorders, and sleep-related breathing disorders (2,3).
Children with insomnia and decreased sleep duration are more likely to have difficulties
with attention, hyperactivity, excessive daytime sleepiness, interpersonal relationships, and
health (4,5). Tantrums and behavior issues are more likely to occur in young children with
sleep problems compared to those without sleep problems. Up to 20% of school-aged children
with sleep difficulties have failed at least one year of school (6). Depression and anxiety are
increased in children with sleep problems (7). When children do not sleep well, their families do
not sleep well. Mothers of infants with sleep problems are more likely to report depression (8).
For every one-hour reduction in the number of hours of sleep per night, the odds of childhood
obesity increase by 41% (9). Unfortunately, children with early sleep difficulties are more likely
to have problems sleeping when they are older (2). Therefore, early diagnosis and treatment is
paramount to a child’s success.
in infants (16,17). Maternal separation anxiety is associated with increased night waking (18).
Frequent nocturnal awakenings are associated with increased accidental injury in toddlers (19).
EPIDEMIOLOGY
Insomnia is a common problem for children and the etiology tends to vary based on age. Up
to 40% of infants and up to 50% of preschool aged children have difficulty initiating sleep or
frequent night awakenings (26–30). Of school-aged children 4 to 12 years, 15% take longer than
30 minutes to fall asleep and 12% experience frequent nighttime awakenings more than once
per week (2). Approximately one quarter of adolescents have symptoms of insomnia (31).
Family history plays a role in the development of insomnia. Of persons with childhood-
onset insomnia, 55% have a positive family history (32). Persons with mood disorders who
are homozygous for the Clock 3111C variant are more likely to have difficulties initiating and
maintaining sleep and are more likely to have early morning awakenings (33).
Insomnia occurs more frequently in children with neurological and psychiatric disor-
ders. More than 40% of toddlers with developmental delays or autism suffer from insomnia
(34). Children with autism and sleep difficulties have more affective problems compared to
those children with autism who were considered to be good sleepers (35). Children with
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and sleep onset insomnia have delayed sleep phase
and delayed melatonin release compared to children with attention deficit and hyperactivity
disorder (ADHD) and no difficulties initiating sleep (36).
c21 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:7 Char Count=
DIFFERENTIAL DIAGNOSIS
Before diagnosing a patient with a specific type of insomnia it is important to evaluate the
patient for other sleep disorders which could present in a similar fashion.
Delayed sleep phase syndrome is a common disorder, which typically presents during ado-
lescence. It is characterized by difficulty falling asleep until later than the desired bedtime.
Once asleep, the patient sleeps for normal durations, if allowed, often waking late in the day
(37). Teenagers may experience problems initiating sleep at conventional hours and insufficient
sleep because of early awakening to meet school start times. Treatment for delayed sleep phase
syndrome may include chronotherapeutic approaches to reset the circadian clock, use of bright
light upon awakening and dim light prior to bed, melatonin, and/or pharmacotherapy (65).
(Chapter 18)
Restless leg syndrome and periodic limb movement disorders are increasingly recognized in
children. Children with restless leg syndrome may have difficulty initiating and maintaining
sleep. Older children and adolescents may have sleep disturbances dating back to infancy
(66). Periodic limb movements in sleep may contribute to decreased REM sleep and increased
arousals (67). Because of the chronic nature of both of these disorders, they may mimic idiopathic
insomnia if left untreated. These disorders have been associated with low serum ferritin (68).
Treatment strategies are poorly studied in children, but may include dietary (iron supplement,
decrease caffeine), sleep hygiene, and pharmacologic agents (68).
Obstructive sleep apnea is associated with snoring, gasping and coughing during sleep,
and early morning headaches. It can contribute to frequent arousals from sleep and
night awakenings, mimicking insomnia. First line treatment is often tonsillectomy and
c21 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:7 Char Count=
Psychophysiologic Insomnia
r Symptoms present for > 1 mo r Not well studied in children
r Anxiety associated with sleep r 46% of adults with chronic insomnia cite adverse
r Trouble sleeping in bed, but able to fall events in childhood with persisting insomnia (38)
asleep in other places not typically meant r Anecdotally, children frequently present with
for sleep or away from home longstanding difficulty initiating sleep accompanied by
r Heightened arousal when trying to go to anxiety associated with sleep and hyperarousal when
sleep in bed. These children (and their families) become
excessively focused on their inability to sleep.
r Can give rise to multiple problems involving sleep
hygiene including sleeping during class or taking an
afternoon nap, use of media, and use of substances
both to stay awake and to promote sleep.
Paradoxical Insomnia
r Symptoms present for at least 1 mo r Pediatric prevalence unknown, but thought to be
r Report of chronic, severe, almost nightly rare (37)
sleep deprivation r Be aware of discrepancies between parent and child
r Increased awareness of environmental sleep time reporting.
stimuli and/or conscious thoughts r If this diagnosis is considered, sleep diaries combined
r Daytime impairment is less than expected with actigraphy may be helpful.
given the severity of the reported sleep
deprivation
r There may be a discrepancy between
subjective sleep logs and objective
measures (actigraphy, polysomnography)
Idiopathic Insomnia
r Persistent unremitting insomnia starting r Prevalence for 15–18 yr = 0.7%; In younger ages, the
during infancy or childhood prevalence is unknown (22)
r Diagnosis of exclusion, no cause identified r In a small sample of adults reporting onset of
insomnia as a child, more reported neurological
conditions including dyslexia, hyperkinesis, abnormal
electroencephalograms, or “minimal brain damage”
compared to persons with adult onset insomnia (32).
r Consider behavioral insomnia of childhood in younger
children.
r Older children may develop inadequate sleep hygiene,
medication dependence, or features of
psychophysiologic insomnia (39).
c21 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:7 Char Count=
Table 1 Summary of ICSD Second Edition Classification of Insomnia: Pediatric Associations (Continued )
(Continued)
c21 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:7 Char Count=
Table 1 Summary of ICSD Second Edition Classification of Insomnia: Pediatric Associations (Continued )
This table was paraphrased in part from the ICSD 2nd Edition Classification of Insomnia. (37) For a full discussion of the criteria
for the various types of insomnia, please see this resource.
Source: From Ref. 37.
adenoidectomy, but treatment of allergies, weight loss, and positive airway pressure are options
as well (69).
Disorders of arousal include confusional arousals, sleep terrors, and sleepwalking (37). Par-
ents may believe that their child is fully aroused and describe the children as having insomnia.
However, children with disorders of arousal usually appear confused during the event and do
not remember the event of the next day. Management of disorders of arousal usually involves
reassuring the parents, ensuring that the child has an age appropriate amount of sleep, tak-
ing appropriate steps to ensure safety, and screening for other primary sleep disorders, which
may provoke events. However, in some cases, children require behavioral or pharmacologic
therapies (70).
EVALUATION
In general, all children should be screened for sleep problems. A parent report or a child or
adolescent’s self- report of sleep difficulties requires a thorough characterization of the sleep
problem and related factors (Table 2). This should include determination that the child or adoles-
cent has a developmentally and age appropriate opportunity to sleep. The history should include
details about the bedtime routine and the parent’s and the child’s response to night awakening.
Evaluation of the quality of sleep is also important. The clinician should determine if the child
awakes refreshed, if there are additional sleep disorders such as obstructive sleep apnea and
periodic limb movement disorder, and if there are medical or psychiatric conditions that may
disrupt sleep. Because some medications contribute to insomnia, a list of over-the-counter, pre-
scription, and illicit drugs should be obtained. In addition, identification of previous treatment
techniques, medications, and the duration of the time that they were used are necessary. The
clinician should also characterize significant daytime symptoms including excessive daytime
sleepiness, irritability, hyperactivity, and difficulty with concentration. Older children should be
interviewed separately from their parents to discuss safety at home and at school, exposure to
nicotine, alcohol, and illicit drugs, and to determine if there is an underlying psychiatric disorder.
The general physical examination should screen for medical disorders such as reflux,
asthma, and pain that may contribute to insomnia. Polysomnography is not typically indi-
cated unless there is concern for obstructive sleep apnea or periodic limb movement disorder
contributing to insomnia. A two-week sleep diary or actigraphy can help to better delineate
bedtimes, sleep times, and wake times. These tools can help distinguish between delayed sleep
phase syndrome and paradoxical insomnia.
MANAGEMENT
Management of pediatric insomnia depends on the underlying etiology. A discussion regard-
ing realistic developmentally appropriate bedtime, wake time, naptime, and sleep hygiene
(Table 3) should be considered for all children. The majority of cases of behavioral insomnia of
childhood are best managed through behavioral interventions including teaching new positive
sleep habits (49). In select cases, pharmacologic treatment may be used on a short-term basis
in combination with the behavioral interventions. For example, if a child’s insomnia is severe
c21 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:7 Char Count=
Ensure that the patient has a r What time does your child go to bed?
developmentally appropriate r What time does your child go to sleep?
number of hours of sleep in a 24 hr r What time does your child wake for the day?
period r Does your child take any naps?
r How many hours of sleep does your child receive in a 24 hour
period?
r How many awakenings does your child have per night?
r How long are the awakenings?
Is there evidence for adjustment or r Does the patient lay awake at night worrying about going to
psychophysiologic insomnia? sleep?
r Does the patient appear tired, but have difficulty going to sleep
once he/she goes to bed?
r Does the patient sleep better when not sleeping in his/her own
bed?
r Was the sleep difficulty provoked by a stressful event?
Diet r Caffeine?
r Eating prior to going to bed
Sleep Routine
r Determine the latest time the child can wake up and still get to school on time. Make this the wake up
time. This should be observed consistently even on weekends until the sleep problem resolves.
r Determine the developmentally appropriate number of hours of sleep and count backwards from the wake
up time. This is the new bedtime. This should be observed consistently even on weekends until the sleep
problem resolves.
r Schedule developmentally appropriate nap times if indicated.
r Create a calm prebedtime routine that fits with the child and the family. This should be started 30–60 min
prior to the expected bedtime.
r Children should be allowed to fall asleep alone to avoid sleep-onset association with parents.
r Expose the child to bright light upon awakening and dim light for one hour prior to bedtime.
r Discontinue use of all media (TV, computer, video games, cell phones, texting) 30 min prior to bed. Media
may need to be removed from the child’s room.
r Discuss the parents’ response to night awakening (young children) and the child’s response to night
awakening (older children).
Lifestyle Changes
r The child should get plenty of exercise during the day. However, vigorous activity should be limited during
the hour prior to bedtime.
r Some children take a bath directly prior to bed. This is acceptable if it is a warm, calming bath. However, if
the child is young and the bath is fun and exciting, the parents should consider moving the bath to earlier
in the evening or in the morning.
r Keep the child’s bedroom temperature below 75◦ F.
r Remove environmental barriers to sleep if possible (i.e., pets, other siblings, noise, potential sources for
allergens).
enough to significantly affect the family’s ability to implement the recommended behavioral
interventions, medication may be used to improve the patient’s sleep and bring about behavioral
changes sooner (3). Additionally, there are some children with neurological or medical disorders
who require long-term pharmacologic treatment for insomnia. Unfortunately, research evalu-
ating the safety and efficacy of pharmacologic agents in the treatment of pediatric insomnia is
lacking (71).
Behavioral Interventions
Positive routines r Provide the patient with a specific bedtime and wake time.
r Prior to bed the family should develop a brief, calm routine to be
performed every night prior to bed.
Faded bedtime with response r The child is put to bed close to the time of their current sleep onset.
cost r After the child starts to go to sleep easily at this time, the bedtime is
advanced (moved earlier) by 15 min. Once the child is falling asleep
easily the bedtime is advanced again.
r This process is repeated until the desired bedtime is achieved.
r If the child is not falling asleep easily, the child is removed from the bed
briefly and then placed back in bed.
Scheduled awakenings r The patient is awakened 15–30 min prior to their nocturnal awakenings.
r The time between scheduled awakenings should be increased over
time.
r Parents respond to their children during these awakenings with their
typical routine.
Parental education r Help families establish age appropriate consistent bedtimes, wake
times, and nap times.
r Remind families to avoid creating an association that requires parental
involvement.
scheduled awakenings, and parental education are effective in correcting bedtime resistance and
night wakings (64). (Table 4)
Extinction techniques are designed to decrease unwanted behaviors (49). Unmodified
extinction involves putting the child to bed at a prescribed time and leaving the child in the
room, ignoring the child’s signaling (crying) until it is time to wake in the morning. The parents
should remain extremely consistent in their responses to bargaining and night waking and
avoid positive reinforcement for unwanted behaviors. However, if the parent is concerned
that the child is ill or hurt, he/she is encouraged to investigate. Parents should be aware that
once positive sleep habits are established, illness or a change in routine (i.e., vacation) may
result in reemergence of the unwanted behavior. Consistently ignoring the child’s signaling
will help the child reestablish self-sufficient sleep. Some families find listening to the signaling
(crying) of their child stressful and may be unable to ignore their child for a sufficient amount
of time to allow the technique to work. A modification of the extinction technique involves the
family staying in the room with the child, but not interacting with the child (64,72). Graduated
extinction is another technique that may decrease parental stress. This technique involves the
parent ignoring the child’s behavior for a predetermined period of time (i.e., 5 minutes) and
c21 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:7 Char Count=
then checking on the child. When the parent checks on the child, the interaction should be
minimal and last less than 15 seconds to avoid reinforcing negative behavior. The periods
of time between checking on the child are gradually increased (i.e., 5 minutes, 10 minutes,
15 minutes) (64).
Difficulty with the extinction techniques may be encountered when the child does not stay
in his/her room. This can be counteracted by confining the children to a space using the crib or
closing the door of the room. Parents should ensure that the rooms are safe by covering electrical
outlets and removing hard toys and climbable surfaces. If the parent is concerned about the child
being in the room alone, securely attached stacked baby gates, half doors, or cameras will allow
parents to be able to see in the child’s room. Extinction techniques are effective if performed
with consistency and result in decreased bedtime problems, decreased night awakenings, and
improved sleep continuity (64).
Positive bedtime routines help to encourage appropriate behaviors. The family should
create a positive, but calm prebed routine. This routine may consist of a warm relaxing bath,
reading books, listening to music, etc. Dim light during the prebed routine is recommended
(48). The goal of this technique is to help the child slowly decrease their activity and to create a
positive association with bedtime (64).
For those children who have difficulty initiating sleep, the faded bedtime with response
cost may be beneficial. The child is put to bed close to the time when they typically fall asleep.
Once the child is going to sleep easily at the later time, the bedtime is advanced by 15 minutes
until the goal bedtime is reached. If the child does not fall asleep, the child is removed from bed
for a specific period of time and then placed back in bed. Children should wake up at a specific
time each morning. Naps, other than developmentally appropriate prescribed naps, should be
avoided (64).
Children with frequent night awakenings may benefit from scheduled night awakenings
to ultimately improve the amount of consolidated sleep. Children are put to bed at their usual
time, but are awakened by the parent about 15 to 30 minutes prior to the typical time of the
nighttime awakening. Parents are allowed to respond to the awakening with their usual routine.
Over time, the period between the scheduled awakenings is increased (64).
Parental education has been used in several studies starting as early as the prenatal
period. This technique focuses on educating parents on appropriate bedtimes, parental role in
sleep initiation, and response to nocturnal awakenings and is geared toward preventing the
development of unwanted behaviors (64). Early maternal sleep education is associated with
long-term decreased incidence of maternal depression and decreased sleep problems in their
children (73).
One behavioral technique has not been proven to be more effective than another in the
treatment of behavioral insomnia of childhood (64). Unmodified extinction may produce results
faster than scheduled awakenings (74). More than 80% of children treated with behavioral
techniques improved and had lasting effects for three to six months (64). No adverse sec-
ondary effects have been identified in participating children and parental energy and mood are
reported to improve following the implementation of the behavioral interventions (64,75,76).
The practitioner should consider the unique situation for the child and the family before
prescribing a particular technique and many practitioners combine techniques for maximum
efficacy (64).
the child falls asleep easily, the bedtime is then advanced by 15 minutes every four to seven
days. This process continues until the child achieves a developmentally appropriate amount
of sleep.
Parents may wish to incorporate positive reinforcement (i.e. sticker chart) to encourage
children to participate in the various treatment techniques. Children with mood disorders
or ADHD often complain that they cannot stop thinking when they go to bed. Relaxation
techniques have also been found helpful to decrease physiologic arousal as well as provide
a focus for their attention as they try to go to sleep (78). Combinations of these techniques
along with sleep education and sleep hygiene are reported to result in significant improvement
in sleep latency, night awakenings, and sleep duration in adolescents receiving treatment for
substance abuse (78).
Pharmacologic Interventions
Despite a lack of research support and FDA approval for pharmacologic treatment of pediatric
insomnia, multiple medications have been used by both physicians as well as parents without
physician guidance to help children sleep. Up to 25% of children 0 to 18 months of age have
been prescribed medication to help them sleep (79). In a large cross-sectional study between
1993 and 2004, 18,640,820 pediatric patient visits (aged 0 to 17 years) for sleep related difficulties
were evaluated (80). Physicians prescribed nutritional counseling for 7% and behavioral therapy
for 22%. However, 81% of patients were prescribed a medication (33% antihistamines, 26% ␣2
agonists, 15% benzodiazepines, 6% nonbenzodiazepines, 6% antidepressants). Nineteen percent
of patient visits received both pharmacological and behavioral treatment (80). In a separate
study, more than 75% of practitioners had recommended nonprescription medications at least
once for pediatric insomnia. Antihistamines were the most frequently recommended over the
counter medication. Fifteen percent of practitioners reported prescribing melatonin or herbal
remedies (81).
Due to the lack of controlled, well-powered studies evaluating the safety and efficacy of
hypnotics in children, pharmacotherapy should be reserved for those children who experience
significant negative consequences as a result of their chronic sleep difficulties, children requiring
a rapid solution to their insomnia, and/or children who have a disorder (e.g., neurological con-
dition) that precludes appropriate response to behavioral interventions (71). Pharmacotherapy
should be paired with a behavioral intervention for maximum efficacy (48). Choice of the med-
ication is guided by whether the patient has trouble initiating or maintaining sleep, comorbid
conditions, and the individual characteristics of the medication (Table 5). Caution is advised
with regard to the use of any agents which promote sleep in children. These agents should
not be combined with other central nervous system depressants or used in high doses due to
concern for respiratory depression. Commonly prescribed medications are discussed below.
This discussion is not meant to be a comprehensive review of these medications and readers are
encouraged to check manufacturer prescribing guidelines for dose recommendations, cautions,
and potential side effects prior to recommending these medications.
Antihistamines
Antihistamines including diphenhydramine hydrochloride and hydroxyzine are the most com-
monly prescribed agents in the treatment of pediatric insomnia (80). These medications compete
with histamine at H1 -receptors, promoting mild sedation at the prescribed dosing (82). They
have been reported to decrease sleep latency and nighttime awakenings (83). However, in a ran-
domized placebo controlled trial evaluating sleep disturbance in infants aged 6 to 15 months,
diphenhydramine hydrochloride showed no improvement compared to placebo (97). In addi-
tion, antihistamines may lead to insomnia in some children, may interfere with sleep quality, and
may contribute to daytime drowsiness (82). These medications are not recommended for chil-
dren less than two years of age (98). Persons taking antihistamines regularly develop tolerance
resulting in decreased effectiveness of the medication with chronic use (99).
Table 5 Pharmacotherapy
Pediatric dosing
r No recommendations are available for pediatric sedation
r Study in children with ADHD: 50–800 g (mean 157 g) (87)
Available formulations
r Tablet
r Transdermal patch
Melatonin (88) Mechanism of action and effects on sleep
r Phase shift
r Hypnotic
Side effects
r Morning sedation
r Dysphoria
r Regulation of growth hormone and gonadotropic hormone
secretion
Pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics
r Rapid absorption
r Peak plasma level at 1 hr
Pediatric dosing
r No recommendations for dosing in children are available.
r Start with low dose 0.5 or 1 mg 1–5 hr prior to bedtime and
increase q 1–2 wk up to a maximum of 10 mg.
r Consider controlled release formulations for night awakenings
Available formulations
r Rapid release
◦ Tabs
◦ Spray
◦ Sublingual
◦ Liquid
r Controlled release
◦ Tabs
Benzodiazepines (48,71) Mechanism of action:
r GABA agonist
Side Effects
r Drowsiness
r Confusion
r Paradoxical excitement
r Decreased respiratory rate
r Apnea
r Hypotension
r Bradycardia
r Abrupt discontinuation is associated with seizures
Diazepam (48,89) Pharmacodyndamics and pharmacokinetics
r Half-life
◦ Infants: 40–50 hr
◦ Children: 15–21 hr
◦ Adults: 20–50 hr
Pediatric dosing
r 0.04–0.25 mg/kg at bedtime
Available formulations
r Oral
r Rectal
(Continued)
c21 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:7 Char Count=
Pediatric dosing
r No recommendations are available for pediatric sedation
r For enuresis > 6 yr
◦ 10–25 mg PO at bedtime
r For depression
◦ 1.5 mg/kg/day with max dose of 5 mg/kg/d divided
1–4 times/day
◦ Monitor carefully doses > 3.5 mg/kg/day
Available formulations
r Capsule
r Tablet
Amitriptyline (95) Pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics
r Peak serum concentration: 4 hr
r Half-life in adults: 9–25 hr with mean of 15 hr
Pediatric dosing
r No recommendations are available for pediatric sedation
r For depression
◦ Children: 1 mg/kg/day divided in 3 doses
◦ Adolescents: 25–50 mg/day
Trazodone (96) Mechanism of action
r Inhibits reuptake of serotonin
r ␣-adrenergic blockade
Side effects
r Blurred vision
r Prolonged priaprism
r Postural hypotension
r Drowsiness
Pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics
r Peak serum concentration: 1–2 hr
r Half-life: 5–9 hr
Pediatric Dosing
r Not FDA approved for depression or sedation in children
r Children 2–18 yr of age
◦ 1.5–2 mg/kg/day in 3 divided doses
◦ Max dose 6 mg/kg/day
r Adolescents
◦ 25–50 mg/day with max of 150 mg/day in divided doses
r Adults
◦ 150 mg/day in 3 divided doses with max of 600 mg/day
This table serves as an outline of medications that have been used to treat pediatric insomnia. It is not comprehensive and
providers are encouraged to look at manufacturer prescribing information for side effects, and cautions prior to recommending
any of these medications to a patient.
in sedation is unclear (71). Clonidine has primarily been used to promote sedation in children
with ADHD (71). The benefit in this patient population is thought to relate to clonidine’s effect
on both ADHD symptoms as well as the side effect of sedation (87). One study found that the
degree of drowsiness decreases with time, which may mean that the dose required inducing
sedation, will increase over time (100).
Clonidine has also been found to reduce sleep latency and night awakening in children
with autism spectrum disorders (101). Despite the fact that clonidine has not been studied in
healthy children without comorbid disorders, general pediatricians commonly prescribe cloni-
dine for children with difficulty initiating sleep (102). Some children will experience insomnia
with this medication (85). Anecdotally, rebound insomnia appears to occur approximately
four to six hours following administration of the medication. Caution is recommended when
prescribing clonidine due to adverse effects such as dysphoria, bradycardia, hypotension, and
c21 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:7 Char Count=
rebound hypertension (71). The FDA recommends a thorough cardiac evaluation prior to the
initiation of this medication due to concern regarding sudden cardiac death (85). Because of the
side effect profile and lack of research in normal healthy children, clonidine is not recommended
for insomnia in healthy children (71).
Melatonin
Melatonin has been used to treat insomnia in adults as well as children. In vivo, levels increase
with darkness and decrease with light, playing a role in the circadian rhythm of sleep and
wakefulness (71). As with many medications used to treat insomnia in children, large studies
have not been performed to determine efficacy or safety. In a small randomized double blind
placebo controlled trial, short term use of melatonin was reported to be effective for phase
advancement, decreasing sleep latency, and increasing total sleep duration in children with
chronic insomnia (103). It has also been found to be efficacious in special patient populations
including children with blindness, intellectual disabilities, Angelman syndrome, Rett syndrome,
and autism (63,104–107). Although many studies evaluating the short-term efficacy in various
pediatric populations are available, studies evaluating the pharmocokinetics and long-term
safety and efficacy have not been performed in children. Significant adverse effects have not
been documented, but daytime somnolence has been reported (88). In addition, several children
have reported vivid dreams, but this warrants further study. There is an association between
melatonin and regulation of growth hormone and gonadotropic hormone (88,108). Therefore,
the practitioner should inform families of the theoretical risk of affecting these hormones in
children. Recommended dosing for children is not available. Based on studies in children, doses
of 0.5 mg up to 10 mg have been reported to be effective (48). Melatonin can be used prior to
bedtime as a hypnotic or up to four hours prior to bedtime as treatment for delayed sleep phase.
Controlled release melatonin is available for children with difficulties with both sleep initiation
and maintenance (109).
Benzodiazepines
Benzodiazepines bind to the ␥ -aminobutyric acid (GABA) receptor complex and modulate
action of GABA at the receptor site. As a result, benzodiazepines not only induce sleep and
decrease partial arousals during sleep transitions, but also contribute to decreased muscle tone
and decreased anxiety (71,110). Choosing a specific benzodiazepine depends on the patient’s
specific sleep disturbance. Patients with difficulty initiating sleep may benefit from a ben-
zodiazepine that is rapidly absorbed with a short half-life. A benzodiazepine with a longer
half-life may be chosen to treat nighttime or early morning awakenings (71). Although effec-
tive in treating insomnia, benzodiazepines may be associated with tolerance, rebound partial
arousals, and daytime somnolence (71). In addition, discontinuation following longer-term use
in children may result in increased insomnia in addition to anxiety, agitation, diarrhea, fever,
sweating, and tachypnea (111). In children, benzodiazepines have primarily been used to treat
disorders of arousal such as sleep terrors and confusional arousals (71). However, controlled
clinical trials using benzodiazepines in children do not exist. They are not approved for seda-
tion in children (71). Due to the significant side effects associated with benzodiazepines, these
medications are not recommended for patients with preexisting CNS depression, decreased
pulmonary function, apnea, or those patients taking medications that contribute to CNS
depression (48).
be mild and self-limited (93). Common side effects in adults include dizziness, headaches, and
somnolence (92).
Antidepressants
Antidepressants have been prescribed for insomnia associated with depression or anxiety in
adults (113). Studies evaluating the safety and efficacy of antidepressants in children are not
available. Caution is advised when prescribing antidepressants to children due to the black box
warning regarding increase risk of suicide in children taking these medications (114).
Tricyclic antidepressants are thought to decrease arousal from sleep but may also impair
deep sleep as well as REM sleep (71,113). Most studies in adults have not found lasting sleep
associated benefits from treatment with tricyclic antidepressants (113). In children, imipramine
hydrochloride is prescribed most frequently for control of nocturnal enuresis and it has been
reported to be beneficial in decreasing disorders of arousal such as sleep terrors and confusional
arousals in children (71). Side effects include dry mouth, orthostatic hypotension, and cardiac
dysrhythmias (71).
Trazodone is a 5-HT2 -receptor antagonist with weak ␣2 -receptor antagonism (113). It
is associated with an increase in total sleep time, increase in the percentage of slow wave
sleep, and a decrease in arousals in adults (115). In a group of adolescents with depression
it was found to minimally decrease the time for insomnia resolution compared to fluoxetine
alone (116). Side effects of trazodone include sedation, dizziness, psychomotor impairment, and
priaprism (117).
Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) or serotonin-norepinephrine reuptake
inhibitors (SNRIs) are not generally used as primary sleep-promoting agents (113). Positive
effects on insomnia may result as part of the treatment of an underlying mood disorder.
SUMMARY
Pediatric insomnia is common and the etiology is often multifactorial. It has significant con-
sequences for children as well as their families. Behavioral insomnia of childhood is the most
common etiology of sleep disturbance in young children. Older children are susceptible to the
other types of insomnia seen in adults. In adolescents, delayed sleep phase is an especially
common cause of sleep initiation problems and morning sleepiness. A comprehensive history
and evaluation is important to determine the exact etiology for a child’s insomnia. Treatment
is based on parental education, a discussion of sleep hygiene, and behavioral techniques. For
select children who need pharmacologic intervention, medications are available, but must be
used with caution.
REFERENCES
1. Mindell JA, Emslie G, Blumer J, et al. Pharmacologic management of insomnia in children and
adolescents: Consensus statement. Pediatrics 2006; 117(6):e1223–e1232.
2. Stein MA, Mendelsohn J, Obermeyer WH, et al. Sleep and behavior problems in school-aged children.
Pediatrics 2001; 107(4):E60.
3. Glaze DG. Childhood insomnia: Why Chris can’t sleep. Pediatr Clin North Am 2004; 51(1):33–50, vi.
4. Nixon GM, Thompson JM, Han DY, et al. Short sleep duration in middle childhood: Risk factors and
consequences. Sleep 2008; 31(1):71–78.
5. Roberts RE, Roberts CR, Duong HT. Chronic insomnia and its negative consequences for health and
functioning of adolescents: A 12-month prospective study. J Adolesc Health 2008; 42(3):294–302.
6. Kahn A, Van de Merckt C, Rebuffat E, et al. Sleep problems in healthy preadolescents. Pediatrics
1989; 84(3):542–546.
7. Forbes EE, Bertocci MA, Gregory AM, et al. Objective sleep in pediatric anxiety disorders and major
depressive disorder. J Am Acad Child and Adolesc Psychiatry 2008; 47(2):148–155.
8. Hiscock H, Wake M. Infant sleep problems and postnatal depression: A community-based study.
Pediatrics 2001; 107(6):1317–1322.
9. Ievers-Landis CE, Storfer-Isser A, Rosen C, et al. Relationship of sleep parameters, child psychological
functioning, and parenting stress to obesity status among preadolescent children. J Dev Behav Pediatr
2008; 29(4):243–252.
10. Iglowstein I, Jenni OG, Molinari L, et al. Sleep duration from infancy to adolescence: Reference values
and generational trends. Pediatrics 2003; 111(2):302–307.
c21 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:7 Char Count=
11. Parmelee AH Jr., Schulz HR, Disbrow MA. Sleep patterns of the newborn. J Pediatr 1961; 58:241–250.
12. Anders TF, Keener M. Developmental course of nighttime sleep-wake patterns in full-term and
premature infants during the first year of life. I. Sleep 1985; 8(3):173–192.
13. Adair RH, Bauchner H. Sleep problems in childhood. Curr Probl Pediatr 1993; 23(4):147–170; discus-
sion 2.
14. Anders TF, Halpern LF, Hua J. Sleeping through the night: A developmental perspective. Pediatrics
1992; 90(4):554–560.
15. Goodlin-Jones BL, Burnham MM, Gaylor EE, et al. Night waking, sleep-wake organization, and
self-soothing in the first year of life. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2001; 22(4):226–233.
16. Simard V, Nielsen TA, Tremblay RE, et al. Longitudinal study of preschool sleep disturbance: The pre-
dictive role of maladaptive parental behaviors, early sleep problems, and child/mother psychological
factors. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2008; 162(4):360–367.
17. Touchette E, Petit D, Paquet J, et al. Factors associated with fragmented sleep at night across early
childhood. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2005; 159(3):242–249.
18. Scher A. Maternal separation anxiety as a regulator of infants’ sleep. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2008;
49(6):618–625.
19. Schwebel DC, Brezausek CM. Nocturnal awakenings and pediatric injury risk. J Pediatr Psychol
2008; 33(3):323–332.
20. Dworak M, Schierl T, Bruns T, et al. Impact of singular excessive computer game and television expo-
sure on sleep patterns and memory performance of school-aged children. Pediatrics 2007; 120(5):978–
985.
21. Wolfson AR, Carskadon MA. Sleep schedules and daytime functioning in adolescents. Child Dev
1998; 69(4):875–887.
22. Ohayon MM, Roberts RE. Comparability of sleep disorders diagnoses using DSM-IV and ICSD
classifications with adolescents. Sleep 2001; 24(8):920–925.
23. Pagel JF, Forister N, Kwiatkowki C. Adolescent sleep disturbance and school performance: The
confounding variable of socioeconomics. J Clin Sleep Med 2007; 3(1):19–23.
24. Camhi SL, Morgan WJ, Pernisco N, et al. Factors affecting sleep disturbances in children and adoles-
cents. Sleep Med 2000; 1(2):117–123.
25. Roberts RE, Roberts CR, Chen IG. Impact of insomnia on future functioning of adolescents. J Psy-
chosom Res 2002; 53(1):561–569.
26. Richman N. A community survey of characteristics of one- to two- year-olds with sleep disruptions.
J Am Acad Child Psychiatry 1981; 20(2):281–291.
27. Bernal JF. Night waking in infants during the first 14 months. Dev Med Child Neurol 1973; 15(6):760–
769.
28. Carey WB. Night waking and temperament in infancy. J Pediatr 1974; 84(5):756–758.
29. Lozoff B, Wolf AW, Davis NS. Sleep problems seen in pediatric practice. Pediatrics 1985; 75(3):477–
483.
30. Ottaviano S, Giannotti F, Cortesi F, et al. Sleep characteristics in healthy children from birth to 6 years
of age in the urban area of Rome. Sleep 1996; 19(1):1–3.
31. Ohayon MM, Roberts RE, Zulley J, et al. Prevalence and patterns of problematic sleep among older
adolescents. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2000; 39(12):1549–1556.
32. Hauri P, Olmstead E. Childhood-onset insomnia. Sleep 1980; 3(1):59–65.
33. Serretti A, Benedetti F, Mandelli L, et al. Genetic dissection of psychopathological symptoms: Insom-
nia in mood disorders and CLOCK gene polymorphism. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet
2003; 121B(1):35–38.
34. Goodlin-Jones BL, Sitnick SL, Tang K, et al. The Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire in toddlers
and preschool children. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2008; 29(2):82–88.
35. Malow BA, Marzec ML, McGrew SG, et al. Characterizing sleep in children with autism spectrum
disorders: A multidimensional approach. Sleep 2006; 29(12):1563–1571.
36. Van der Heijden KB, Smits MG, Van Someren EJ, et al. Idiopathic chronic sleep onset insomnia in
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A circadian rhythm sleep disorder. Chronobiol Int 2005;
22(3):559–570.
37. American Academy of Sleep Medicine. Insomnia. In: Sateia M, ed. The International Classification
of Sleep Disorders : Diagnostic and Coding Manual, 2nd ed. Westchester, IL.: American Academy of
Sleep Medicine, 2005:1–31.
38. Bader K, Schafer V, Schenkel M, et al. Adverse childhood experiences associated with sleep in primary
insomnia. J Sleep Res 2007; 16(3):285–296.
39. Hon KL, Lam MC, Leung TF, et al. Are age-specific high serum IgE levels associated
with worse symptomatology in children with atopic dermatitis? Int J Dermatol 2007; 46(12):
1258–1262.
c21 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:7 Char Count=
40. Ivanenko A, Barnes ME, Crabtree VM, et al. Psychiatric symptoms in children with insomnia referred
to a pediatric sleep medicine center. Sleep Med 2004; 5(3):253–259.
41. Johnson EO, Roth T, Schultz L, et al. Epidemiology of DSM-IV insomnia in adolescence: Lifetime
prevalence, chronicity, and an emergent gender difference. Pediatrics 2006; 117(2):e247–e256.
42. Mehl RC, O’Brien LM, Jones JH, et al. Correlates of sleep and pediatric bipolar disorder. Sleep 2006;
29(2):193–197.
43. Kowatch RA, Youngstrom EA, Danielyan A, et al. Review and meta-analysis of the phenomenol-
ogy and clinical characteristics of mania in children and adolescents. Bipolar Disord 2005; 7(6):
483–496.
44. Corkum P, Panton R, Ironside S, et al. Acute impact of immediate release methylphenidate adminis-
tered three times a day on sleep in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Pediatr
Psychol 2008; 33(4):368–379.
45. Corkum P, Moldofsky H, Hogg-Johnson S, et al. Sleep problems in children with attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Impact of subtype, comorbidity, and stimulant medication. J Am
Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1999; 38(10):1285–1293.
46. Van den Bulck J. Television viewing, computer game playing, and Internet use and self-reported time
to bed and time out of bed in secondary-school children. Sleep 2004; 27(1):101–104.
47. Hitze B, Bosy-Westphal A, Bielfeldt F, et al. Determinants and impact of sleep duration in children
and adolescents: Data of the Kiel Obesity Prevention Study. Eur J Clin Nutr 2009; 63(6):739–746.
48. Sheldon S. Disorders of initiating and maintaining sleep. In: Sheldon S, Ferber R, Kryger M, eds.
Principles and Practice of Pediatric Sleep Medicine. Elsevier Saunders, USA, 2005:127–160.
49. Morgenthaler TI, Owens J, Alessi C, et al. Practice parameters for behavioral treatment of bedtime
problems and night wakings in infants and young children. Sleep 2006; 29(10):1277–1281.
50. Johnson EO, Breslau N. Sleep problems and substance use in adolescence. Drug Alcohol Depend
2001; 64(1):1–7.
51. Scadding GK. Corticosteroids in the treatment of pediatric allergic rhinitis. J Allergy Clin Immunol
2001; 108(suppl 1):S59–S64.
52. Hinds PS, Hockenberry MJ, Gattuso JS, et al. Dexamethasone alters sleep and fatigue in pediatric
patients with acute lymphoblastic leukemia. Cancer 2007; 110(10):2321–2330.
53. Orbeta RL, Overpeck MD, Ramcharran D, et al. High caffeine intake in adolescents: Associa-
tions with difficulty sleeping and feeling tired in the morning. J Adolesc Health 2006; 38(4):
451–453.
54. Strunk RC, Sternberg AL, Bacharier LB, et al. Nocturnal awakening caused by asthma in children with
mild-to-moderate asthma in the childhood asthma management program. J Allergy Clin Immunol
2002; 110(3):395–403.
55. Carr MM, Brodsky L. Severe non-obstructive sleep disturbance as an initial presentation of gastroe-
sophageal reflux disease. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 1999; 51(2):115–120.
56. Bandla H, Splaingard M. Sleep problems in children with common medical disorders. Pediatr Clin
North Am 2004; 51(1):203–227, viii.
57. Bloom BJ, Owens JA, McGuinn M, et al. Sleep and its relationship to pain, dysfunction, and disease
activity in juvenile rheumatoid arthritis. J Rheumatol 2002; 29(1):169–173.
58. Zamir G, Press J, Tal A, Tarasiuk A. Sleep fragmentation in children with juvenile rheumatoid arthritis.
J Rheumatol 1998; 25(6):1191–1197.
59. Byars AW, Byars KC, Johnson CS, et al. The relationship between sleep problems and neuropsycho-
logical functioning in children with first recognized seizures. Epilepsy Behav 2008; 13(4):607–613.
60. Allik H, Larsson JO, Smedje H. Sleep patterns of school-age children with Asperger syndrome or
high-functioning autism. J Autism Dev Disord 2006; 36(5):585–595.
61. Allik H, Larsson JO, Smedje H. Insomnia in school-age children with Asperger syndrome or high-
functioning autism. BMC Psychiatry 2006; 6:18.
62. Newman CJ, O’Regan M, Hensey O. Sleep disorders in children with cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child
Neurol 2006; 48(7):564–568.
63. Braam W, Didden R, Smits MG, et al. Melatonin for chronic insomnia in Angelman syndrome: A
randomized placebo-controlled trial. J Child Neurol 2008; 23(6):649–654.
64. Mindell JA, Kuhn B, Lewin DS, et al. Behavioral treatment of bedtime problems and night wakings
in infants and young children. Sleep 2006; 29(10):1263–1276.
65. Owens JA, Palermo TM, Rosen CL. Overview of current management of sleep disturbances in
children: II–Behavioral Interventions. Curr Ther Res 2002; 63(suppl B):B38–B52.
66. Picchietti DL, Stevens HE. Early manifestations of restless legs syndrome in childhood and adoles-
cence. Sleep medicine 2008; 9(7):770–781.
67. Crabtree VM, Ivanenko A, O’Brien LM, et al. Periodic limb movement disorder of sleep in children.
J Sleep Res 2003; 12(1):73–81.
c21 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:7 Char Count=
68. Mindell J, Owens J. Restless leg syndrome and periodic limb movement disorder. In: Mindell J,
Owens J, eds. A Clinical Guide to Pediatric Sleep: Diagnosis and Management of Sleep Problems.
Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, 2003:123–134.
69. Capdevila OS, Kheirandish-Gozal L, Dayyat E, et al. Pediatric obstructive sleep apnea: Complications,
management, and long-term outcomes. Proc Am Thorac Soc 2008; 5(2):274–282.
70. Hopkins B, Glaze D. Disorders of arousal in children. Pediatr Ann 2008; 37(7):481–487.
71. Reed MD, Findling RL. Overview of Current Management of Sleep Disturbances in Children:
I–Pharmacotherapy. Curr Ther Res 2002; 63(suppl B):B18–B37.
72. Williams CD. The elimination of tantrum behavior by extinction procedures. J Abnorm Soc Psychol
1959; 59:269.
73. Hiscock H, Bayer JK, Hampton A, et al. Long-term mother and child mental health effects of a
population-based infant sleep intervention: Cluster-randomized, controlled trial. Pediatrics 2008;
122(3):e621–e627.
74. Rickert VI, Johnson CM. Reducing nocturnal awakening and crying episodes in infants and young
children: A comparison between scheduled awakenings and systematic ignoring. Pediatrics 1988;
81(2):203–212.
75. Eckerberg B. Treatment of sleep problems in families with young children: Effects of treatment on
family well-being. Acta Paediatr 2004; 93(1):126–134.
76. Hiscock H, Wake M. Randomised controlled trial of behavioural infant sleep intervention to improve
infant sleep and maternal mood. BMJ (Clinical research ed 2002; 324(7345):1062–1065.
77. Morin CM, Culbert JP, Schwartz SM. Nonpharmacological interventions for insomnia: A meta-
analysis of treatment efficacy. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151(8):1172–1180.
78. Bootzin RR, Stevens SJ. Adolescents, substance abuse, and the treatment of insomnia and daytime
sleepiness. Clin Psychol Rev 2005; 25(5):629–644.
79. Ounsted MK, Hendrick AM. The first-born child: Patterns of development. Dev Med Child Neurol
1977; 19(4):446–453.
80. Stojanovski SD, Rasu RS, Balkrishnan R, et al. Trends in medication prescribing for pediatric sleep
difficulties in US outpatient settings. Sleep 2007; 30(8):1013–1017.
81. Owens JA, Rosen CL, Mindell JA. Medication use in the treatment of pediatric insomnia: Results of
a survey of community-based pediatricians. Pediatrics 2003; 111(5 Pt 1):e628–e635.
82. Diphenhydramine, Lexi-Comp OnlineTM , Pediatric Lexi-Drugs OnlineTM . Hudson, Ohio: Lexi-Comp,
Inc; 2008. Accessed September 18, 2008.
83. Russo RM, Gururaj VJ, Allen JE. The effectiveness of diphenhydramine HCI in pediatric sleep disor-
ders. J Clin Pharmacol 1976; 16(5–6):284–288.
84. Hydroxyzine, Lexi-Comp OnlineTM , Pediatric Lexi-Drugs OnlineTM . Hudson, Ohio: Lexi-Comp,
Inc;2008. Accessed September 18, 2008.
85. Clonidine, Lexi-Comp OnlineTM , Pediatric Lexi-Drugs OnlineTM . Hudson, Ohio: Lexi-Comp, Inc;
2008. Accessed September 18, 2008.
86. Guanfacine, Lexi-Comp OnlineTM , Pediatric Lexi-Drugs OnlineTM . Hudson, Ohio: Lexi-Comp,
Inc;2008. Accessed September 18, 2008.
87. Prince JB, Wilens TE, Biederman J, et al. Clonidine for sleep disturbances associated with attention-
deficit hyperactivity disorder: A systematic chart review of 62 cases. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
Psychiatry 1996; 35(5):599–605.
88. Melatonin, Lexi-Comp OnlineTM , Poisoning and Toxicology. Hudson, Ohio: Lexi-Comp, Inc; 2008.
Accessed September 18, 2008.
89. Diazepam, Lexi-Comp OnlineTM , Pediatric Lexi-Drugs OnlineTM . Hudson, Ohio: Lexi-Comp, Inc;
2008. Accessed September 18, 2008.
90. Clonazepam, Lexi-Comp OnlineTM , Pediatric Lexi-Drugs OnlineTM . Hudson, Ohio: Lexi-Comp, Inc;
2008. Accessed September 18, 2008.
91. Lorazepam, Lexi-Comp OnlineTM , Pediatric Lexi-Drugs OnlineTM . Hudson, Ohio: Lexi-Comp, Inc;
2008. Accessed September 18, 2008.
92. Zolpidem, Lexi-Comp OnlineTM , Pediatric Lexi-Drugs OnlineTM . Hudson, Ohio: Lexi-Comp, Inc;
2008. Accessed September 18, 2008.
93. Blumer JL, Reed MD, Steinberg F, et al. Potential pharmacokinetic basis for zolpidem dosing in
children with sleep difficulties. Clin Pharmacol Ther 2008; 83(4):551–558.
94. Imipramine, Lexi-Comp OnlineTM , Pediatric Lexi-Drugs OnlineTM . Hudson, Ohio: Lexi-Comp, Inc;
2008. Accessed September 18, 2008.
95. Amitriptyline, Lexi-Comp OnlineTM , Pediatric Lexi-Drugs OnlineTM . Hudson, Ohio: Lexi-Comp, Inc;
2008. Accessed September 18, 2008.
96. Trazodone, Lexi-Comp OnlineTM , Pediatric Lexi-Drugs OnlineTM . Hudson, Ohio: Lexi-Comp, Inc;
2008. Accessed September 18, 2008.
c21 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:7 Char Count=
97. Merenstein D, Diener-West M, Halbower AC, et al. The trial of infant response to diphenhydramine:
The TIRED study–a randomized, controlled, patient-oriented trial. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2006;
160(7):707–712.
98. Public Health Advisory: FDA recommends that over-the-counter (OTC) cough and cold prod-
ucts not be used for infants and children under 2 years of age. 2010. http://www.fda.gov/
drugs/drugsafety/publichealthadvisories/ucm051137.html. Accessed March 5, 2010.
99. Richardson GS, Roehrs TA, Rosenthal L, et al. Tolerance to daytime sedative effects of H1 antihis-
tamines. J Clin Psychopharmacol 2002; 22(5):511–515.
100. Daviss WB, Patel NC, Robb AS, et al. Clonidine for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: II. ECG
changes and adverse events analysis. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2008; 47(2):189–198.
101. Ming X, Gordon E, Kang N, et al. Use of clonidine in children with autism spectrum disorders. Brain
Dev 2008; 30(7):454–460.
102. Schnoes CJ, Kuhn BR, Workman EF, et al. Pediatric prescribing practices for clonidine and other
pharmacologic agents for children with sleep disturbance. Clin Pediatr 2006; 45(3):229–238.
103. Smits MG, Nagtegaal EE, van der Heijden J, et al. Melatonin for chronic sleep onset insomnia in
children: A randomized placebo-controlled trial. J Child Neurol 2001; 16(2):86–92.
104. Palm L, Blennow G, Wetterberg L. Long-term melatonin treatment in blind children and young adults
with circadian sleep-wake disturbances. Dev Med Child Neurol 1997; 39(5):319–325.
105. Braam W, Didden R, Smits M, et al. Melatonin treatment in individuals with intellectual disabil-
ity and chronic insomnia: A randomized placebo-controlled study. J Intellect Disabil Res 2008;
52(Pt 3):256–264.
106. Andersen IM, Kaczmarska J, McGrew SG, et al. Melatonin for insomnia in children with autism
spectrum disorders. J Child Neurol 2008; 23(5):482–485.
107. Miyamoto A, Oki J, Takahashi S, et al. Serum melatonin kinetics and long-term melatonin treatment
for sleep disorders in Rett syndrome. Brain Dev 1999; 21(1):59–62.
108. Karasek M, Stawerska R, Smyczynska J, et al. Increased melatonin concentrations in children with
growth hormone deficiency. J Pineal Res 2007; 42(2):119–124.
109. Wasdell MB, Jan JE, Bomben MM, et al. A randomized, placebo-controlled trial of controlled release
melatonin treatment of delayed sleep phase syndrome and impaired sleep maintenance in children
with neurodevelopmental disabilities. J Pineal Res 2008; 44(1):57–64.
110. Dahl RE. The pharmacologic treatment of sleep disorders. Psychiatr Clin North Am 1992; 15(1):161–
178.
111. Ista E, van Dijk M, Gamel C, et al. Withdrawal symptoms in critically ill children after long-term
administration of sedatives and/or analgesics: A first evaluation. Crit Care Med 2008; 36(8):2427–
2432.
112. Armour A, Gottschlich MM, Khoury J, et al. A randomized, controlled prospective trial of zolpidem
and haloperidol for use as sleeping agents in pediatric burn patients. J Burn Care Res 2008; 29(1):238–
247.
113. Wilson S, Argyropoulos S. Antidepressants and sleep: A qualitative review of the literature. Drugs
2005; 65(7):927–947.
114. US Food and Drug Administration.Safety Alerts for Drugs, Biologics, Medical Devices,
and Dietary Supplements: Antidepressant Medication Products. 2007. http://www.fda.gov/
medwatch/safety/2007/safety07.htm#Antidepressant. Accessed September 18, 2008.
115. Kaynak H, Kaynak D, Gozukirmizi E, et al. The effects of trazodone on sleep in patients treated with
stimulant antidepressants. Sleep Med 2004; 5(1):15–20.
116. Kallepalli BR, Bhatara VS, Fogas BS, et al. Trazodone is only slightly faster than fluoxetine in relieving
insomnia in adolescents with depressive disorders. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol 1997; 7(2):
97–107.
117. Mendelson WB. A review of the evidence for the efficacy and safety of trazodone in insomnia. J Clin
Psychiatry 2005; 66(4):469–476.
c22 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:8 Char Count=
As reviewed in the first two sections of this volume, insomnia is a prevalent health problem in
the general population and in medical practice. Advances in our understanding of the origins
and evaluation of insomnia have been paralleled by advances in clinical treatment, reviewed in
this final section.
The treatment of insomnia falls into two major categories; psychological-behavioral treat-
ments, and pharmacologic treatments. Other treatments that do not fall into these categories,
such as bright light, exercise, and various somatic treatments, have also been examined. How-
ever, these are far less developed than the psychological-behavioral and pharmacologic treat-
ments, and for that reason are not reviewed in this volume.
The efficacy of psychological-behavioral treatments and pharmacological treatments has
been well established. Published reviews have systematically evaluated the evidence for effi-
cacy of psychological-behavioral treatments, and suggest that standard treatments such as
multicomponent cognitive behavioral therapy lead to both statistically and clinically significant
improvements in patients with primary and comorbid insomnias (1,2). Similarly, published
metaanalyses regarding benzodiazepine receptor agonist therapy demonstrate the efficacy of
this approach (3,4). Greater variability is evident in reviews of the efficacy of pharmacologic
treatments; some of these have suggested more limited benefits relative to observed adverse
effects, particularly among the elderly (5). Although it is often difficult to compare behavioral
and pharmacologic treatment studies because of differences in study design, available evidence
suggests that the two types of treatment are broadly comparable in their efficacy (4). Individual
studies have sometimes indicated larger treatment effects for behavioral versus pharmacologic
treatments (e.g., 6,7), but small sample sizes and methodologic features that favor one treatment
or another make such studies difficult to conduct and interpret.
Despite the proven efficacy of behavioral and pharmacologic treatments for insomnia,
a number of important questions remain. Early studies have begun to address some of these
questions, as indicated in subsequent chapters. These questions fall into four general areas.
BEHAVIORAL TREATMENTS
What are the effective elements of multimodal cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia (CBT-I)?
Although the efficacy of CBT-I is now well demonstrated, treatments can be made more efficient
if they focus on those elements that have the greatest efficacy. In order to identify these elements,
dismantling trials are needed to compare the various components of CBT-I.
What is the minimal effective “dose” of behavioral treatment? One objection sometimes raised
to the more widespread use of CBT-I and other behavioral treatment is the effort required
to provide six to eight individual treatment sessions. Some evidence already suggests that a
smaller number of treatments may have similar efficacy (10,11). Although the ideal number of
sessions is likely to vary for different individuals, further evidence is needed to clarify the range
of effective treatment duration.
How can we most effectively disseminate behavioral treatments for insomnia? Recognizing the
efficacy of behavioral treatments for insomnia, we must also confront the mismatch between
the number of trained behavioral sleep medicine specialists and the number of individuals in
the population who experience insomnia. As outlined in this section, early evidence suggests
that alternate forms of treatment and treatment delivery may be part of the answer. Briefer,
more focused treatments, the use of group therapy, and the use of self-guided treatments such
as internet-based therapy may all provide useful methods for dissemination. These methods
must also be accompanied by additional efforts toward education of other health professionals
and building the workforce of trained behavioral sleep medicine practitioners.
PHARMACOLOGIC TREATMENT
What are the viable targets for new pharmacotherapies? Sleep–wake regulation is complex, and
the number of neurotransmitters and neuromodulators involved is large. The good news is
that this creates a number of viable targets for insomnia therapy; the bad news is that no single
neurochemical is sufficient to reliably alter sleep–wake balance and treat insomnia. Nevertheless,
the long standing reliance on benzodiazepine receptor agonists is likely to give way to a greater
variety of treatments, including those affecting targets such as histamine, orexin, and serotonin
receptors.
What are the ideal delivery systems? Insomnia pharmacotherapy has been provided only in
the form of oral tablets or capsules. Alternate delivery systems, including transdermal systems,
sublingual, and nasal delivery systems may all be feasible for insomnia treatments. These
systems are currently under investigation; their place in the pharmacotherapy armamentarium
remains to be seen.
What is the optimal duration of treatment? Until the last five to ten years, the widespread
assumption was that insomnia pharamacotherapy should be restricted to short term. As evi-
dence is accumulated regarding the longer term efficacy of pharmacotherapy agents, as well as
the chronic nature of insomnia, reasonable questions emerge regarding the optimal length of
pharmacotherapy. Although it seems undesirable to consign a patient to lifelong pharmacologic
treatment, there is currently very little evidence suggesting how to optimize the duration of
treatment.
258 BUYSSE
Clinical state
and circumstances
Clinical expertise
Patients’
preferences Research evidence
and actions
Figure 1 The optimal management of insom-
nia patients depends on research evidence, the
patientí s clinical state, circumstances, and pref-
erences, and the practitioner’s clinical expertise.
Source: From Refs. 12 and 13.
these treatments should be combined, or when patients should be treated sequentially with
the different modalities is largely unknown. Identifying the most effective treatment sequences
will help to address the problem of treatment nonresponse and residual symptoms, which have
heretofore received little attention.
How does treatment affect insomnia and its comorbidities? Convincing evidence indicates that
insomnia is a risk factor for mental disorders, and newer evidence suggests that insomnia may
be a risk factor for, or worsen the experience, of medical conditions such as hypertension and
chronic pain. An important question for future research is whether treating insomnia leads to
reduced risk for adverse health outcomes, and whether it actually improves the symptoms of
comorbid medical and psychiatric disorders.
Can we develop effective evidence-based treatment guidelines for insomnia? Basic evidence
regarding treatment efficacy is available for behavioral and pharmacologic treatments, although
more sophisticated evidence, such as treatment matching and optimal sequencing strategies,
is not. Initial treatment guidelines have been developed for insomnia, but a larger empirical
database is clearly needed to make these guidelines more meaningful and effective in clinical
practice. While there is clearly a need for evidence-based guidelines in insomnia, it is also impor-
tant to consider patients’ preferences in the selection of treatments as well. As Haynes wrote,
“the term evidence based medicine was developed to encourage practitioners and patients to
pay due respect—no more no less—to current best evidence in making decisions” (12) or, as
stated even more succinctly by DaCruz, “evidence does not make decisions, people do” (13).
SUMMARY
Many components of the effective treatment of insomnia have been addressed by previous
research, as presented in detail in this section. However, many other questions remain. The
optimal treatment of insomnia patients will depend on the continued collection of new research
evidence, as well as understanding and respect for the patient’s clinical state, preferences and
actions, and the practitioner’s expertise (Fig. 1).
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
Supported in part by NIH grants MH024652, AG020677 and AR052155.
REFERENCES
1. Morin CM, Bootzin RR, Buysse DJ, et al. Psychological and behavioral treatment of insomnia: An
update of recent evidence (1998–2004). Sleep 2006; 29(11):1398–1414.
c22 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:8 Char Count=
2. Morgenthaler T, Kramer M, Alessi C, et al. Practice parameters for the psychological and behav-
ioral treatment of insomnia: An update. An american academy of sleep medicine report. Sleep 2006;
29(11):1415–1419.
3. Nowell PD, Mazumdar S, Buysse DJ, et al. Benzodiazepines and zolpidem for chronic insomnia: A
meta-analysis of treatment efficacy. JAMA 1997; 278(24):2170–2177.
4. Smith MT, Perlis ML, Park A, et al. Comparative meta-analysis of pharmacotherapy and behavior
therapy for persistent insomnia. Am J Psychiatry 2002; 159(1):5–11.
5. Glass J, Lanctot KL, Herrmann N, et al. Sedative hypnotics in older people with insomnia: Meta-
analysis of risks and benefits. BMJ 2005; 331(7526):1169.
6. Jacobs GD, Pace-Schott EF, Stickgold R, et al. Cognitive behavior therapy and pharmacotherapy for
insomnia: A randomized controlled trial and direct comparison. Arch Intern Med 2004; 164(17):1888–
1896.
7. Sivertsen B, Omvik S, Pallesen S, et al. Cognitive behavioral therapy vs zopiclone for treatment of
chronic primary insomnia in older adults: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 2006; 295(24):2851–
2858.
8. Edinger JD, Bonnet MH, Bootzin RR, et al. Derivation of research diagnostic criteria for insomnia:
Report of an American Academy of Sleep Medicine Work Group. Sleep 2004; 27(8):1567–1596.
9. American Academy of Sleep Medicine. International classification of sleep disorders, 2nd ed: Diag-
nostic and coding manual, American Academy of Sleep Medicine, Westchester, IL 2005.
10. Edinger JD, Means MK. Cognitive-behavioral therapy for primary insomnia. Clin Psychol Rev 2005;
25(5):539–558.
11. Germain A, Moul DE, Franzen PL, et al. Effects of a brief behavioral treatment for late-life insomnia:
Preliminary findings. J Clin Sleep Med 2006; 2(4):403–406.
12. Haynes RB, Devereaux PJ, Guyatt GH. Physicians’ and patients’ choices in evidence based practice.
BMJ 2002; 324(7350):1350.
13. DaCruz D. Good governance must be introduced globally. BMJ 2002; 324(7333):364.
c23 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:11 Char Count=
Jamie M. Zeitzer
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, and Psychiatry Service,
VA Palo Alto Health Care System, Palo Alto, California, U.S.A.
Martin S. Mumenthaler
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, California, U.S.A.
INTRODUCTION
Everyday behaviors can have a profound impact on sleep and modification of these behaviors is
considered a useful nonpharmacologic approach in treating many manifestations of insomnia.
Four behaviors commonly targeted in such treatments are the amount and daily timing of
alcohol and caffeine consumption, the nature, amount and timing of physical exercise, and the
scheduling of sleep and wake.
Most codified nonpharmacologic treatments for insomnia have included regulation of
some or all of these behaviors as treatment components [e.g., scheduling in stimulus control
(1)] or bundled with other recommended daily health practices as an independent behavioral
treatment called “sleep hygiene” [e.g., (2)]. Sleep hygiene has also been used as a control
treatment for other nondrug approaches [e.g., (3,4)]. The components of sleep hygiene vary but
the basic paradigm is consistent: the prescription of some practices thought to improve sleep
and the limitation or prohibition of others thought to harm sleep.
Although there have been studies of sleep hygiene as a package, it is difficult to test
the validity of sleep hygiene as a stand-alone treatment because there is no formal consensus
as to what components should be included nor have the instructions for these components
been standardized (5). Stepanski and Wyatt (5) have suggested that given the inconsistency in
the components of sleep hygiene treatments, evidence for specific health practices should be
reviewed independent of one another. Thus, we will review the extant research on the effects of
alcohol, caffeine, exercise and scheduling on sleep quality and quantity and, when available, the
effects of modifying these behaviors on disturbed sleep. In this chapter, the impact of routine
alcohol and caffeine use as components of inadequate sleep hygiene is discussed. Abuse of
these substances, and others, as primary causative factors in insomnia is discussed in detail in
Chapter 17.
ALCOHOL
Most nonpharmacologic treatments for insomnia advise patients to limit alcohol intake or
abstain from alcohol before bedtime. In some of the published techniques, the instructions are
vague [e.g., the patient is advised to “moderate alcohol consumption and eliminate ‘night caps’”
(6) or “practice light to moderate use of alcoholic beverages” (7)] though some are more specific,
such as those endorsed by a recent American Academy of Sleep Medicine wellness booklet: “Do
not drink alcoholic beverages within four to six hours of bedtime” (8).
The soporific effects of alcohol have long been known; witness the drunken sleep of Noah
in Genesis. Because of its initial sleep-inducing effects, alcohol is frequently used to self-medicate
insomnia. Alcohol is a global depressant of the central nervous system (CNS) and acts through
the inhibitory GABA-A receptor complex to increase the length of time it is opened after its
chloride channel is activated by GABA. Notably, this same receptor complex is also modulated
c23 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:11 Char Count=
by barbiturates and benzodiazepines, though these act at receptor complex sites distinct from
those at which alcohol acts (9).
Alcohol is undoubtedly the most commonly used sleep-promoting substance (10), but
the worm in this apple is that the sedative effects are transient. After intake before bedtime,
even in healthy individuals without insomnia, alcohol reduces sleep onset, increases nonrapid
eye movement (NREM) and reduces rapid eye movement (REM) sleep early in the night. This
seeming immediate benefit of alcohol vanishes relatively quickly since alcohol is metabolized
rapidly (at the approximate rate of one glass of wine or a half-pint of beer per two hours).
The second part of the night after evening alcohol ingestion is characterized by light, disturbed
sleep, with more REM sleep and associated increased dream or nightmare recall and sympathetic
arousal, including tachycardia and sweating (11). Gastric irritation, headache, and a full bladder
may also be among the effects that interrupt sleep in the latter part of the night. The negative
impact of alcohol on sleep can continue long after blood alcohol concentrations reach zero,
and lead to the so-called “hangover effect” (7). This may reduce overall vigor and daytime
alertness, leading to daytime sleep and a subsequent disruption of nighttime sleep due to sleep
fragmentation. In addition to the sleep-specific effects, alcohol increases the risk of nocturnal
falling (10) that may lead to an increase in injuries in certain vulnerable populations (e.g., older
individuals).
A further complication of alcohol intake late in the day is its impact on breathing, particu-
larly during sleep, in those who have compromised ventilation (7). In individuals with mild or
moderate sleep apnea, even moderate blood alcohol levels (0.07 gms/dL) can increase the fre-
quency of obstructive apneas and the mean sleep heart rate (12). In addition to the acute effects
of alcohol on sleep, chronic alcohol intake is likely to affect sleep as well. The amplitude and
incidence of K-complexes are reduced in older alcoholics (13) and this effect may be reversible
through extended alcohol abstinence. Although the incidence and prevalence of chronic alcohol
use as a factor contributing to insomnia is not known, a community-based survey found that
those who met insomnia criteria reported more than twice as much use of alcohol over the
course of a week than age and sex-matched controls not meeting criteria (14).
In sum, while alcohol is often used as both a short- and long-term self-treatment for
insomnia, in addition to being used in an unrelated recreational fashion, it has overall detri-
mental effects on sleep as it changes both the timing and nature of sleep-related physiology, the
consequence of which for long-term health is unknown.
CAFFEINE
If alcohol is the most widely used substance to self-medicate nocturnal sleeplessness, caffeine
is the most commonly used substance to self-medicate excessive sleepiness. While not quite
antediluvian, Chinese folklore dating from the third century reports a tale regarding the alerting
effects of caffeine (in tea). According to the tale, a Chinese general cut off his eyelids in order to
stay awake and a tea tree sprang up from the site where the eyelids fell to the ground (15).
Although the specific details vary, almost all behavioral insomnia treatments caution
against drinking caffeinated beverages late in the day. Some narrowly address coffee and tea
while others include chocolate and caffeinated soft drinks (16). Hygiene directions for limiting
caffeine intake vary from very stringent instructions (e.g., avoid caffeine use completely for four
weeks, followed by limiting caffeine use to three cups of coffee prior to 10 AM) (7) to more liberal
approaches [e.g., caffeine (should) be discontinued four to six hours before bedtime] (17).
Caffeine is a mild CNS stimulant that acts through the inhibition of A1 and A2A adenosine
receptors (18). As adenosine receptors are present throughout the brain, it has been difficult to
specify the location or locations at which caffeine inhibition acts to induce wakefulness. While
still controversial, the cholinergic basal forebrain is likely one such locus (19). Both coffee and
tea are beverages with high caffeine content; a standard cup of coffee has approximately 80
mg of caffeine while a similar sized drink of tea contains approximately 40 mg of caffeine (20).
There is, however, very large variation in the caffeine content of coffees and teas, ranging from
<0.05 mg/mL in decaffeinated coffees and teas to more than 3.5 mg/mL in some espressos
(21,22). Caffeine is also present in carbonated colas, pepper drinks, citrus drinks, root beers, cold
teas, and the growing market segment of energy drinks. The caffeine content of these drinks
can range from 0 (these are naturally uncaffeinated—the caffeine is added as a supplement) to
c23 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:11 Char Count=
120 mg in some energy drinks (23). Other less commonly imagined sources of caffeine include
chocolate (∼10–30 mg/43 g bar) and both sleepiness-specific (e.g., caffeine pills, ∼200 mg) and
nonspecific medications (e.g., migraine and diet medication, ∼30 mg) (24,25). Worldwide, tea is,
however, second only to water as the most widely consumed beverage (26). Because of its utility
as a stimulant, and no doubt because of its relatively low cost, near ubiquitous availability, and
apparent health safety both acutely and chronically, (18) caffeine is used extensively as a benign
drug to maintain alertness. The per capita consumption in the United States has been estimated
at 200 mg/day (20).
Contrary to alcohol, caffeine delays sleep initiation. Caffeine also increases the proportion
of sleep spent in lighter stages (N1 and N2), while reducing both slow wave sleep (N3) and total
sleep time, increases the time to first REM period, and increases the number of changes between
sleep stages (18). Caffeine ingestion even early in the day can have objectively quantifiable
effects on nocturnal sleep (27) possibly by disrupting the effects of adenosine, one hypothesized
mechanism underlying homeostatic sleep pressure (19). Individuals may be unaware of these
nocturnal effects as ingestion of low doses of caffeine during the daytime may fail to induce an
increase in subjective alertness during the day, but can still disrupt sleep at night (26). Daytime
caffeine ingestion can also lead to significant amounts of caffeine being present in the blood
when attempting to initiate sleep, as the half-life of caffeine is approximately 4.5 hours (25) and
its metabolites, notably theophylline, have potent inhibitory effects on the same A1 and A2A
adenosine receptors as does caffeine (18).
There is wide inter-individual variability in sleep responses to caffeine, with some indi-
viduals being more sensitive to the effects of caffeine than others (28,29). There is likely a genetic
component to this variability as a common genetic polymorphism in the adenosine A2A receptor
is related to both the subjective and objective effects of caffeine on sleep (29). The issue of caf-
feine sensitivity, however, is complex as a number of studies have shown that individuals both
with and without insomnia self-regulate caffeine intake such that those who rated themselves
as ‘caffeine sensitive’ would drink fewer caffeinated beverages (29,30). A further complexity is
that the change in sleep onset behavior that can be associated with mild caffeine intake may
be innately more worrisome, and therefore be perceived as more disruptive of sleep, for some
individuals than for others (31). The anxiety or worry associated with a delayed sleep onset
may exacerbate the mild sleep disruption that would have otherwise occurred after exposure
to a low dose of caffeine. Caffeine use is also associated with a subjective increase in tolerance
to the alerting effects of caffeine that is not accompanied by a change in metabolism or objective
effects on performance (32–34). This can lead to increasing caffeine intake during the day in an
effort to increase subjective alertness, which will secondarily increase caffeine levels during the
night, potentially disrupting sleep.
Given the complexities of interindividual difference in caffeine metabolism and sensitivity,
as well as issues of tolerance and subjective response to caffeine-induced alertness, it is not
surprising that there are no specific consensus instructions related to moderating caffeine intake
in individuals with insomnia. It should be noted that the International Classification of Sleep
Disorders, second edition (ICSD-2) makes diagnostic distinctions regarding both caffeine and
alcohol use. Persons whose use of either alcohol or caffeine is inappropriate in relation to their
sleep would be diagnosed as having insomnia due to inadequate sleep hygiene; whereas those
who are dependent on caffeine or alcohol or use them excessively would be assigned a diagnosis
of insomnia due to substance abuse (35). Education about the sources and possible long-lasting
effects of caffeine should be part of any nonpharmacologic treatment of insomnia.
EXERCISE
For more than 25 years, regular, daily exercise has been suggested as a component of nonphar-
macologic methods for treating insomnia (2). Exercise is still promoted widely as a nonpharma-
cologic method for improving the quality and quantity of sleep (8,36).
One metaanalysis of the acute effects of exercise on sleep, found support for small to
moderate effects of acute exercise on slow wave sleep, REM sleep latency, total time spent in
REM sleep, and total time asleep (37). Despite these findings, as Driver and Taylor note, the
sleep-promoting effect of exercise in both normal and clinical populations has not yet been
conclusively demonstrated (38).
c23 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:11 Char Count=
One hypothesized mechanism of the effects of exercise on sleep involves the production
of heat during exercise and subsequent cooling. In theory, this exercise-induced cooling could
enhance the normal sleep onset related cooling mechanisms that appear essential for normal
sleep induction and maintenance (39). The temperature-sensitive neurons in the hypothalamus
(40) that can respond to cooling and are sleep active may be the ultimate physiologic mechanism
responsible for exercise-induced changes in sleep. This would also help explain why exercise
that increases core temperature, when scheduled too close to the onset of sleep, can lead to
sleep disruption (41). Even passive forms of cooling (e.g., hot bath followed by evaporative
cooling several hours before intended bedtime) have been suggested as being beneficial to sleep
onset (42).
Another hypothetical mechanism underlying the effects of exercise on sleep is exercise
induction of cytokines, notably interleukin-6 (43). Low concentrations of cytokines, associated
with mild to moderate exercise, can promote drowsiness while elevated concentrations are
associated with intense exercise and increased wake. There are many other changes in endocrine
activity associated with exercise that may also underlie the changes associated with sleep (e.g.,
endorphins, cortisol).
One of the problems in assessing the effects of exercise on sleep is the variation in the
length, intensity, and timing of exercise, as well as the efficacy of acute and chronic exercise
routines. Each of these, as well as sex, age, and overall fitness level are likely to impact the effects
of exercise on sleep (44). Understanding the specific mechanism, by which exercise improves
sleep in individuals with insomnia, as well as in individuals without insomnia, will be a critical
step in understanding how to maximize its effects. The timing of exercise may also lead to an
indirect effect, which is greater exposure to daylight when the exercise intervention is scheduled
to take place outdoors (e.g., tennis, walking, jogging) (45). In older individuals whose reduced
daytime light exposure may lead to circadian-associated sleep problems (see below), outdoor
exercise may be especially important.
While there is yet to be a definitive link established between exercise and improved sleep,
there is sufficient evidence to suggest that there is a likely connection. A better understanding
of the mechanistic connection between the two will lead to an improved, more thoughtful
recommendation of exercise as a nonpharmacologic treatment of insomnia.
SCHEDULING
Bon vivant that he was, it is questionable whether Ben Franklin followed his own “early to bed,
early to rise” advice but he certainly was on to something important as chronic insomnia is
often associated with irregular sleep timing (30). Regularity in time-in-bed scheduling supports
the entrainment of sleep and wake to the basic daily alternation of day and night. Regular sleep
and wake patterns lead to regular exposure to light. Light is the primary zeitgeber (Gm., time
cue) of the circadian timing system. Thus, regular sleep and wake patterns lead to regular dark
and light exposure that leads to a regular position of the circadian clock relative to sleep and
wake. Such a pattern leads to circadian timing that is maximally supportive of proper sleep
and wake (46,47). Not only does regularly timed light exposure lead to a proper entrainment of
the circadian system, but it also maximizes circadian amplitude (48), which may be especially
important in older individuals who may have an innately decreased amplitude of their circadian
clock (49,50).
Given the nature of agricultural work in pre-industrial times, an inherent tie existed
between the daily alternation of light and dark and peoples’ work and sleep schedules. The
invention of the electric light likely accelerated our movement away from a natural light/dark
exposure and highly regular entraining signals for the circadian system. In our advanced tech-
nological society, there are fewer built-in factors that support a consistently followed daily
schedule. Advances in science and technology have led to great latitude in the timing of work
and play, much of which can theoretically occur now at any time of the day. Twenty-four hour
access to supermarkets and computers and businesses maximizing utilization of equipment and
space by scheduling work around the clock are just a few examples of infrastructure changes
that weaken the practice of fixed day/night schedules. This circumstance is thought to contribute
to the high prevalence of insomnia in modern societies and is also likely linked to decreased
daytime alertness (51,52).
c23 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:11 Char Count=
Scheduling or timing is in some sense at the core of the other health practices discussed in
this chapter. As seen above, a major concern regarding the consumption of alcohol, and caffeine,
and exercise practices is their timing in relation to sleep. Notably, the question is not necessarily
how much, but when. From its earliest formulations [e.g., (53)] to the most recent formulation of
approved guidelines for treatment of insomnia (54), the admonition to keep a regular schedule
has been included in nonpharmacological treatments for insomnia. An important caveat is that
regularity or scheduling alone is not sufficient, however, for patients to get adequate amounts
of sleep. It is necessary for them to allocate sufficient time in bed to get an adequate amount of
sleep (52). The various nonpharmacologic methods used for treating insomnia have different
instructions relating to regular sleep schedules. Stimulus control, given its goal of strengthening
the association between bed and sleep, has a very flexible into bedtime. A central stimulus con-
trol instruction is to get into bed “only when sleepy.” On the other hand, the morning instruction
is to set a standard fixed wake-up time (1). Cognitive behavioral therapy for the treatment of
insomnia also prescribes adherence to a standard rise time (55). The essential treatment mech-
anism in sleep restriction therapy involves scheduling. The time allotted in bed is manipulated
in order to build up sufficient sleep debt so that there will be adequate homeostatic pressure for
sleep during the scheduled time in bed. Although the restriction of time in bed to the amount
of time the individual actually sleeps is the central mechanism of sleep restriction therapy,
the regularity of sleep schedules is intrinsic to this therapy, even if not emphasized (56). Sleep
restriction therapy, along with some of the other nonpharmacologic treatments for insomnia,
also proscribe daytime napping. Not only does this increase the likelihood of being able to
maintain a regular nocturnal sleep schedule, but it also reduces daytime exposure to darkness.
The human circadian system is sensitive to light during the daytime (57) and the change in light
exposure through napping may also deleteriously influence the position of the circadian clock.
CONCLUSION
The benefit of each specific health practice described above remains to be definitively answered.
The most recent consensus guidelines for the treatment of insomnia concluded that “although
there is insufficient evidence of the effectiveness of sleep hygiene as an insomnia treatment the
general principles of sleep hygiene should be included in other behavioral treatments,” and for
the most part they have been (54).
Although the effectiveness and determination of the optimal parameters of the four health
practices described in this chapter are still in question, the overall evidence from our reading
of the research literature and our clinical experience is sufficient for us to suggest following
the four basic health practices discussed above. At worst, the suggested practices appear to do
no harm. At best, if any or all of these health practices have been the source of an individual’s
sleep problem or have exacerbated it, correcting these practices may help. Finally, they are all
supportive of good general mental and physical health independent of their effects on sleep
and this indirectly supports good sleep.
We suggest the following:
1. The collection of baseline sleep logs, which is widely recommended as a first step in behav-
ioral treatments, can generate helpful information as to what should be treatment targets.
For example, when patient information reveals regular late afternoon and evening use of
caffeine, this should be explored as a source of sleep disruption and insomnia complaints
(58). Many who have used these techniques have noted that in the process of collecting these
self-report data, individuals often become aware that some of their behavior is antithetical
to good sleep. This awareness helps increase compliance.
2. Limit evening drinking of alcoholic beverages to ONE alcoholic drink with dinner and NO
alcoholic drinks within three hours of bedtime. Information about alcohol and its paradoxical
effects on sleep initiation and sleep maintenance should be disseminated.
3. Limit caffeine (tea, coffee, cola, caffeinated soft drinks, chocolate) use no more than two
cups taken no later than lunchtime. The impact of caffeine metabolism should be explained
such that individuals with insomnia understand that what is a great boost in the daytime
is a negative at night. There are decaffeinated drinks that many have substituted for that
habitual beverage that people reflexively take throughout the day.
c23 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:11 Char Count=
4. Schedule some form of moderate (or greater) physical activity (walking, swimming, jogging,
gardening) for at least 30 min every day to be completed no later than four hours before
bedtime. Most people are aware of the importance of exercise for their general health, and
if only because exercise improves health it should contribute to good sleep. This exercise
is also likely to synergistically support circadian variation in core body temperature and
contribute to good sleep.
5. Set a sleep schedule that fits with usual habits as recorded by two weeks of sleep log
information. Once a realistic schedule is determined, the alarm should be set for the same
time, seven days a week. The evening into-bed-time can be scheduled within a one-hour
time frame to allow for that essential phone call or must-see TV program. If there are special
events that result in delayed bedtime, keep the alarm set at its regular time and wake as
usual. Keeping the same wake time instead of sleeping-in ensures the buildup of sufficient
homeostatic sleep pressure to maintain sleep the next evening and does not disrupt the
normal light exposure in the morning that is critical for keeping sleep rhythm coupled to
circadian rhythm.
Two parting thoughts: “Bad sleep hygiene is seldom the sole cause of any sleep problem,
but rare is the sleep problem that is not partially maintained by poor sleep hygiene,” (2) and, as
another early sleep hygiene investigator noted, “. . .removing the [sleep hygiene] abuses from
the daily routines of abusers is. . .probably. . .a good idea” (59).
REFERENCES
1. Bootzin RR. Nicassio P. Behavioral treatments for insomnia. In: Hersen M, Eisler RM, Miller PM, eds.
Progress in Behavior Modification, Vol 6. New York, NY: Academic Press, 1978:1–45.
2. Hauri PJ. Current Concepts: The Sleep Disorders, 2nd ed. Kalamazoo, MI: The Upjohn Company,
1982.
3. Friedman L, Benson K, Noda A, et al. An actigraphic comparison of sleep restriction and sleep hygiene
treatments for insomnia in older adults. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 2000; 13:17–27.
4. Friedman L, Zeitzer JM, Kushida CA, et al. Scheduled bright light for treatment of insomnia in older
adults. J Am Geriatr Soc 2009; 57:441–452.
5. Stepanski EJ. Wyatt JK. Use of sleep hygiene in the treatment of insomnia. Sleep Med Rev 2003;
7:215–225.
6. Espie CA, MacMahon KM, Kelly HL, et al Randomized clinical effectiveness trial of nurse-
administered small-group cognitive behavior therapy for persistent insomnia in general practice.
Sleep 2007; 30:574–584.
7. Zarcone V. Sleep hygiene. In: Kryger, MH, Roth T, Dement WC, eds. Principles and Practice of Sleep
Medicine, 3rd ed. Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders Company, 2000:657–661.
8. American Academy of Sleep Medicine. Sleep Hygiene: Behaviors that help promote sound sleep.
Westchester, IL: American Academy of Sleep Medicine, 2005.
9. Hevers W, Luddens H. The diversity of GABAA receptors. Pharmacological and electrophysiological
properties of GABAA channel subtypes. Mol Neurobiol 1998; 18:35–86.
10. Gillin JC, Drummond SPA, Clark CP, et al. Medication and substance abuse. In: Kryger MH, Roth
T, Dement WC, eds. Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine, 4th ed. Philadelphia, PA: Elsevier
Saunders, 2005:1345–1358.
11. Kobayashi T, Misaki K, Nakagawa H, et al. Alcohol effect on sleep electroencephalography by fast
Fourier transformation. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1998; 52:154–155.
12. Scanlan MF, Roebuck T, Little PJ, et al. Effect of moderate alcohol upon obstructive sleep apnoea. Eur
Respir J 2000; 16:909–913.
13. Colrain IM, Crowley KE, Nicholas CL, et al. The impact of alcoholism on sleep evoked delta frequency
responses. Biol Psychiatry 2009; 66:177–184.
14. Jefferson CD, Drake CL, Scofield HM, et al. Sleep hygiene practices in a population-based sample of
insomniacs. Sleep 2005; 28, 611–615.
15. Yü L. The Classic of Tea (Ch’a Ching). Boston, MA: Little, Brown, and Company, 1974.
16. Hauri PJ. Sleep hygiene, relaxation therapy, and cognitive interventions. In: Hauri PJ, ed. Case Studies
in Insomnia. New York, NY: Plenum Publishing Corp, 1991.
17. Morin CM. Insomnia: Psychological Assessment and Management. New York, NY: Guilford,
1993.
18. Fredholm BB, Battig K, Holmen J, et al. Actions of caffeine in the brain with special reference to factors
that contribute to its widespread use. Pharmacol Rev 1999; 51:83–133.
c23 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:11 Char Count=
19. Porkka-Heiskanen T, Strecker RE, McCarley RW. Brain site-specificity of extracellular adenosine con-
centration changes during sleep deprivation and spontaneous sleep: An in vivo microdialysis study.
Neuroscience 2000; 99:507–517.
20. Barone JJ. Roberts HR. Caffeine consumption. Food Chem Toxicol 1996; 34:119–129.
21. McCusker RR, Fuehrlein B, Goldberger BA, et al. Caffeine content of decaffeinated coffee. J Anal
Toxicol 2006; 30:611–613.
22. McCusker RR, Goldberger BA, Cone EJ. Caffeine content of specialty coffees. J Anal Toxicol 2003;
27:520–522.
23. McCusker RR, Goldberger BA, Cone EJ. Caffeine content of energy drinks, carbonated sodas, and
other beverages. J Anal Toxicol 2006; 30:112–114.
24. Durrant,KL. Known and hidden sources of caffeine in drug, food, and natural products. J Am Pharm
Assoc (Wash) 2002; 42:625–637.
25. Somani SM, Gupta P. Caffeine: A new look at an age-old drug. Int J Clin Pharmacol Ther Toxicol 1988;
26:521–533.
26. Hindmarch I, Rigney U, Stanley N, et al. A naturalistic investigation of the effects of day-long con-
sumption of tea, coffee and water on alertness, sleep onset and sleep quality. Psychopharmacology
(Berl) 2000; 149:203–216.
27. Landolt HP, Werth E, Borbely AA, et al. Caffeine intake (200 mg) in the morning affects human sleep
and EEG power spectra at night. Brain Res 1995; 675:67–74.
28. Drake CL, Jefferson C, Roehrs T, et al. Stress-related sleep disturbance and polysomnographic response
to caffeine. Sleep Med 2006; 7:567–572.
29. Retey JV, Adam M, Khatami R, et al. A genetic variation in the adenosine A2A receptor gene
(ADORA2A) contributes to individual sensitivity to caffeine effects on sleep. Clin Pharmacol Ther
2007; 81:692–698.
30. Cheek RE, Shaver JL. Lentz MJ. Variations in sleep hygiene practices of women with and without
insomnia. Res Nurs Health 2004; 27:225–236.
31. Omvik S, Pallesen S, Bjorvatn B, et al. Night-time thoughts in high and low worriers: Reaction to
caffeine-induced sleeplessness. Behav Res Ther 2007; 45:715–727.
32. Parsons WD. Neims AH. Effect of smoking on caffeine clearance. Clin Pharmacol Ther 1978; 24:
40–45.
33. George J, Murphy T, Roberts R, et al. Influence of alcohol and caffeine consumption on caffeine
elimination. Clin Exp Pharmacol Physiol 1986; 13:731–736.
34. Kalow W. Variability of caffeine metabolism in humans. Drug Res 1985; 35:319–324.
35. American Academy of Sleep Medicine. International Classification of Sleep Disorders: Diag-
nostic and Coding Manual, 2nd ed. Westchester, IL: American Academy of Sleep Medicine,
2005.
36. American Sleep Disorders Association. Sleep hygiene: Behaviors that help promote sound sleep.
Rochester, MN: American Sleep Disorders Association, 1997.
37. Youngstedt SD, O’Connor PJ, Dishman RK. The effects of acute exercise on sleep: A quantitative
synthesis. Sleep 1997; 20:203–214.
38. Driver HS, Taylor SR. Exercise and sleep. Sleep Med Rev 2000; 4:387–402.
39. Krauchi K, Cajochen C, Werth E, et al. Warm feet promote the rapid onset of sleep. Nature 1999;
401:36–37.
40. McGinty D, Szymusiak R. Keeping cool: A hypothesis about the mechanisms and functions of slow-
wave sleep. Trends Neurosci 1990; 13:480–487.
41. Edinger JD, Morey MC, Sullivan RJ, et al. Aerobic fitness, acute exercise and sleep in older men. Sleep
1993; 16:351–359.
42. Hauri PJ, Linde SM. No More Sleepless Nights. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons, 1990.
43. Santos RV, Tufik S, De Mello MT. Exercise, sleep and cytokines: Is there a relation? Sleep Med Rev
2007; 11:231–239.
44. Kubitz KA, Landers DM, Petruzzello SJ, et al.The effects of acute and chronic exercise on sleep. A
meta-analytic review. Sports Med 1996; 21:277–291.
45. Montgomery P, Dennis J. A systematic review of non-pharmacological therapies for sleep problems
in later life. Sleep Med Rev 2004; 8:47–62.
46. Dijk DJ. Czeisler CA. Contribution of the circadian pacemaker and the sleep homeostat to sleep
propensity, sleep structure, electroencephalographic slow waves, and sleep spindle activity in humans.
J Neurosci 1995; 15:3526–3538.
47. Dijk DJ, Duffy JF, Czeisler CA. Circadian and sleep/wake dependent aspects of subjective alertness
and cognitive performance. J Sleep Res 1992; 1:112–117.
48. Jewett ME, Kronauer RE, Czeisler CA. Phase-amplitude resetting of the human circadian pacemaker
via bright light: A further analysis. J Biol Rhythms 1994; 9:295–314.
c23 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:11 Char Count=
49. Dijk DJ, Duffy JF, Riel E, et al. Ageing and the circadian and homeostatic regulation of human
sleep during forced desynchrony of rest, melatonin and temperature rhythms. J Physiol 1999; 516
(Pt 2):611–627.
50. van Someren EJ, Mirmiran M. Swaab DF. Non-pharmacological treatment of sleep and wake dis-
turbances in aging and Alzheimer’s disease: Chronobiological perspectives. Behav Brain Res 1993;
57:235–253.
51. Billiard M, Alperovitch A, Perot C, et al. Excessive daytime somnolence in young men: Prevalence
and contributing factors. Sleep 1987; 10:297–305.
52. Manber R, Bootzin RR, Acebo C, et al. The effects of regularizing sleep-wake schedules on daytime
sleepiness. Sleep 1996; 19:432–441.
53. Hauri P. The Sleep Disorders. Current Concepts. Upjohn, Kalamazoo MI: Scope Publications, 1977.
54. Schutte-Rodin S, Broch L, Buysse D, et al. Clinical guideline for the evaluation and management of
chronic insomnia in adults. J Clin Sleep Med 2008; 4:487–504.
55. Edinger JD, Means MK. Cognitive-behavioral therapy for primary insomnia. Clin Psychol Rev 2005;
25:539–558.
56. Spielman AJ, Saskin P, Thorpy MJ. Treatment of chronic insomnia by restriction of time in bed. Sleep
1987; 10:45–56.
57. Khalsa SB, Jewett ME, Cajochen C, et al. A phase response curve to single bright light pulses in human
subjects. J Physiol 2003; 549:945–952.
58. Mitler MM, O’Malley MB. Wake-promoting medications: Efficacy and adverse effects. In: Kryger MH,
Roth T, Dement WC, eds. Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine, 4th ed. Philadelphia, PA: Elsevier
Saunders, 484–498.
59. Lacks P, Rotert M. Knowledge and practice of sleep hygiene techniques in insomniacs and good
sleepers. Behav Res Ther 1986; 24:365–368.
c24 IHBK059-Sateia March 23, 2010 19:41 Char Count=
Leisha J. Smith
Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona, U.S.A.
Peter L. Franzen
Sleep Medicine Institute and Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine,
Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, U.S.A.
Shauna L. Shapiro
Department of Counseling Psychology, Santa Clara University, Santa Clara, California, U.S.A.
Stimulus control therapy (SCT) for insomnia is based primarily upon an operant conditioning
model of the development and maintenance of insomnia in which falling asleep is conceptu-
alized as an instrumental act emitted to produce the reinforcement, sleep. Difficulty in falling
asleep, then may be due to inadequate stimulus control (1). This could be either because of the
lack of discriminative stimuli that facilitate sleep or the presence of stimuli that are associated
with activities that interfere with sleep.
The role of stimuli in facilitating or interfering with sleep is not the only learning principle
that affects sleep. Pavlovian conditioning is also important in that the bed and bedroom can
become cues for the anxiety and frustration associated with trying to fall asleep (1). Internal
cues, such as mind racing, anticipatory anxiety, and physiological arousal can become cues for
further arousal and sleep disruptions (2). SCT aims to reduce cues associated with arousal as
well as cues that are discriminative stimuli for activities that are incompatible with sleep.
Stimulus control therapy was designed to help individuals suffering from insomnia to
strengthen the bed and bedroom as cues for sleep, to weaken the bed and bedroom as cues
for arousal, and to develop a consistent sleep–wake schedule to help maintain improvement.
Stimulus control therapy for the treatment of insomnia was proposed by Bootzin in 1972 (3).
The instructions were expanded during the next few years (1,4) and have remained unchanged
to the present. The six instructions that comprise SCT are as follows:
While the instructions are concerned mainly with sleep onset, they may also be used
with individuals with sleep maintenance problems by repeating instructions three and four
c24 IHBK059-Sateia March 23, 2010 19:41 Char Count=
during mid-night awakenings. Each of the instructions stated above is important in reestab-
lishing the bed and bedroom as cues for sleep rather than wakefulness. It is important that the
rationale for each instruction be described to patients in detail. Although the instructions seem
straightforward, they may be difficult for patients to follow, and if the reasons behind the
instructions are unclear, compliance may be even more challenging.
Instruction 1: The first instruction is designed to help individuals become more aware
of their bodies’ cues for sleepiness. Frequently, individuals with insomnia decide to go to bed
because of clock time as a result of a calculation of how much sleep they feel they must have
before awakening in the morning. This may produce increasing anxiety as sleeplessness persists
coupled with excessive time in bed for the amount of sleep that is obtained.
Initially, individuals with insomnia may rarely use internal cues of sleepiness. Thus,
instruction one should be viewed as a goal to be achieved gradually over the first few weeks
rather than as an imperative to be started immediately. Becoming sensitive to internal cues of
sleepiness will aid patients in determining an appropriate time to go to bed based on sleepiness,
not on the clock. Going to bed when sleepy will also make it easier for patients to fall asleep
more quickly.
Instruction 2: Instruction two is intended to help strengthen the cues of the bed and
bedroom with sleep and weaken the cues of bed and bedroom with arousal and wakefulness.
Often individuals with insomnia will engage in activities in bed that interfere with falling
asleep, such as reading, watching television, talking on the phone, text-messaging, or working.
This behavior establishes the bed and bedroom as conditioned stimuli for wakefulness. By
prohibiting any activity associated with arousal, other than sexual activity, from taking place in
the bed and bedroom, the second instruction of SCT serves to reduce the bed and bedroom as
cues for wakefulness, and reestablish them as cues for sleep.
Patients are typically asked to engage in activities associated with arousal in a different
room in the house. For example, if a patient typically thinks about and plans the following day’s
activities as they are lying in bed, he or she would be asked to do this planning in another room
before going into the bedroom. This may help individuals with insomnia create a new bedtime
routine that is better suited to facilitate sleep onset.
Instructions 3 and 4: The third and fourth instructions are core elements of SCT. Instructing
patients to get out of bed, if they are not sleeping, limits them from being awake in bed and
further strengthens the association between the bed and bedroom and sleep. While SCT is
focused primarily on sleep onset problems, instruction four is incorporated for use with sleep
maintenance issues. Getting out of bed to engage in other activities when unable to sleep can also
give individuals a sense of control over their insomnia. This makes the problem less distressing
and more manageable for the patient.
There are a few important issues in successfully implementing these instructions. First,
how long should someone with insomnia be in bed before getting out of bed? The instructions
place a premium on getting out-of-bed quickly, within 10 minutes. However, some individuals
with insomnia become anxious by such a recommendation and will constantly check the clock
to see if it is time to get out of bed. To avoid that, patients are typically instructed to turn the face
of the clock away from them, so that clock checking does not occur. If time-pressure produces
increased anxiety, the third instruction is often modified to put emphasis on the internal cues of
frustration and anxiety rather than on how much time has passed. Thus, while it is permissible
to be in bed while in the process of falling asleep, the patient should get out of bed at the first
signs of frustration with not falling asleep.
Importantly, however, it is not permissible to stay in bed for long periods of time waiting
to fall asleep (such as 45, 60 minutes, or longer) even if not frustrated. The goal of the SCT
instructions remains to associate the bed and bedroom with falling asleep quickly. Research has
indicated a quarter-hour rule (staying in bed before falling asleep no more than 15 minutes) is
manageable and effective in producing improved sleep in those with insomnia (5).
Second, once up, how long should the patients stay awake and what should they do?
A good clinical rule of thumb is that patients should stay out-of-bed long enough to feel that
they might successfully be able to fall asleep if they return to bed. This is an opportunity to
practice paying attention to internal cues of sleepiness and using them as a guide. Generally,
this means staying awake at least 15 or more minutes before trying to go to sleep again. As for
c24 IHBK059-Sateia March 23, 2010 19:41 Char Count=
the activities to be engaged in, patients should be encouraged to do something relaxing and
enjoyable.
Because of the increasing evidence that even room light can alter sleep-wake circadian
schedules (e.g., 6), we have placed additional emphasis on keeping lights dim when out-of-
bed during the night. Reading with a reading light and watching television from a distance
are acceptable. We discourage patients from doing anything on the computer, even checking
email, since the amount of light from the monitor when sitting close to it is brighter than most
individuals realize and activities done on the computer are usually arousing.
Finally, many adults with sleep maintenance problems elect to start the day at 4 AM or
5 AM rather than return to the bed for additional sleep. This is not a wise strategy since even
30 or 60 minutes of additional sleep increases alertness and reduces fatigue during the day. As
long as the usual final wake-up time is maintained, returning to bed is recommended when
there is 45 minutes or more until wake-up time.
Instruction 5: One of the goals of instruction five is to set a more consistent wake-up time
for all seven days of the week, with less than one hour of discrepancy between days off and
workdays. Keeping a consistent wake time helps establish a more regular sleep schedule. Many
people with insomnia stay in bed later in the morning in hopes of catching up on the sleep
they missed the night before. However, irregular schedules weaken the association between
the cues of the bed and bedroom and sleep. Maintaining regular sleep schedules has been
found to reduce daytime fatigue and sleepiness (7). Consequently, a regular schedule helps
both strengthen cues for sleep and reduce daytime problems associated with sleep disturbance.
Instruction 6: The final instruction encourages patients to maintain more regular sleep
patterns by not napping during the day. In addition, the lack of daytime naps after a poor
night’s sleep when following SCT allows the homeostatic drive for sleep to build throughout
the day, making it more likely that the patient will successfully fall asleep faster the next night.
This strengthens the cues of the bed and bedroom with falling asleep and provides a success
experience for the patient to help maintain compliance with the instruction.
It should be emphasized that we are not opposed to all naps. Instruction six is intended
to increase the likelihood that SCT will successfully change a dysfunctional sleep pattern. With
some individuals, such as the elderly, however, it may be wise to have a brief nap (30 min or
less) scheduled at the same time every day. It is the irregularity of napping that produces and
maintains irregular sleep schedules.
in total sleep time found by Morin and colleagues (13) Perlis, and colleagues (15) found a 29%
improvement in total sleep time along with a 56% reduction in wake after sleep onset and a
34% reduction in sleep onset latency.
Other studies have focused on the comparison of cognitive behavioral treatment for
insomnia (CBTi) with pharmacotherapy and for patients who have poor sleep despite using
medication. One study compared a CBTi intervention (including stimulus control instructions)
to zopiclone and placebo for the treatment of insomnia in older adults (16). Participants who
received CBTi improved on measures of sleep efficiency, amount of slow wave sleep, and time
spent awake during the night at both posttreatment and six-month follow-up assessments.
Participants in the drug and placebo groups did not show similar improvements. Another
study evaluated the effectiveness of CBTi for continuing insomnia in older adults who were
dependent on sedative-hypnotic medications (17). Participants were assigned to either a CBT
group, which included stimulus control instructions, or a placebo group. The group that received
CBTi showed clinically significant and subjective improvement on measures of sleep quality,
whereas the placebo group did not.
In multicomponent treatment studies, it is difficult to identify which components are
most effective. Harvey, Inglis, & Espie (18) surveyed 90 participants at the one year follow-up
to determine which of 10 components of their cognitive-behavioral insomnia treatment were
associated with changes in sleep measures. The combination of stimulus control and sleep
restriction was the best predictor of improved sleep latency and total sleep time, and cognitive
restructuring, a form of cognitive therapy, significantly predicted a reduction in wakefulness.
Relaxation, although most commonly endorsed by patients, did not predict improvement on
any variable. Likewise, sleep hygiene was unrelated to changes in sleep.
It should be stressed that it is important to include components in multicomponent treat-
ments that have been evaluated and found effective as single interventions. All of the multicom-
ponent treatments discussed here used SCT as a core element. From a theoretical perspective
it is easy to see how different components might cover a broader range of problems, but it is
desirable to empirically test that proposition by comparing a single component, such as SCT,
against a multiple component intervention.
Accelerating Improvement
In the previous section, we discussed how technology might be used to accelerate improvement.
In this section, we describe an entirely new approach to help the individual with insomnia to
relearn to fall asleep quickly. In Intensive Sleep Retraining, individuals with insomnia were
invited into the sleep laboratory during which participants received 40 hours of sleep depriva-
tion and were given 50 opportunities, one each half hour, to fall asleep for no more than four
minutes (37). These sleep opportunities began two hours before the participants usual bedtimes
c24 IHBK059-Sateia March 23, 2010 19:41 Char Count=
and continued for 25 hours. The idea being evaluated was whether those who had difficulty
falling asleep quickly could learn to do so when given 50 trials at falling asleep when sleep
deprived.
On average, across the 50 trials, the participants were able to fall asleep quickly and fell
asleep in 6.9 minutes as measured by polysomnography. Posttreatment assessment indicated
substantial improvements in sleep onset latency, wake after sleep onset, and total sleep time, as
well as on broader measures of fatigue and vigor, cognitive anticipatory anxiety, and self-efficacy
for sleep. These improvements were maintained at the six week follow-up assessment.
In a subsequent clinical trial, 68 insomnia participants were randomly assigned to four
treatment conditions: (1) Sleep hygiene control, (2) Intensive Sleep Retraining, (3) Four-week
SCT, and (4) Combined Intensive Sleep Retraining and SCT (38). Preliminary analyses indicate
that the three active treatment conditions were all more effective than the sleep hygiene control.
ISR produced improvement faster than SCT but both reached the same degree of improvement.
The combined treatment provided both faster initial treatment gains plus additional improve-
ment that led to trends for the best overall outcomes.
education, sleep hygiene, and MBSR in an integrative framework. The treatment was delivered
in 6 weekly sessions lasting 90 to 120 minutes.
There were statistically significant changes in both sleep measures (total wake time, sleep
onset latency, wake after sleep onset, number of awakenings, sleep efficiency, and ratings of sleep
quality) and measures of arousal (cognitive and somatic presleep arousal and the Hyperarousal
Scale). Overall clinical significance was demonstrated using intent-to-treat analyses of the
30 participants who attended the first session. At posttreatment, 87% of the sample would
no longer meet the inclusion criteria and 15 of the 30 participants showed a 50% or greater
reduction in the primary sleep outcome measure of total wake time.
In the study of substance-abusing adolescents, frequency of mindfulness meditation prac-
tice, but not duration of practice, was significantly related to improvement in total sleep time
and to improvement in self-efficacy about sleep problems (44). Similarly, in the adults with
insomnia study, frequency of meditation practice, but not duration, was significantly related
to reductions in the Hyperarousal Scale (43). Both studies suggest that emphasis should be
placed more on the frequency than the duration of mindfulness meditation practice. Further,
both studies indicate that mindfulness meditation may make a contribution to improvement of
sleep problems and reduction in arousal in multicomponent treatment studies for insomnia.
CONCLUSION
Stimulus control therapy for insomnia is a behavioral treatment that has been consistently
documented to be effective in the treatment of insomnia, whether delivered as a single-
component treatment or part of a multicomponent behavioral or cognitive-behavioral inter-
vention. Although the six instructions of SCT are relatively straightforward, individuals fare
better when a rationale is provided.
Recent studies have focused on multicomponent treatments and new components such
as the use of bright light to change circadian rhythms and the use of mindfulness meditation to
reduce symptoms of arousal are strong additions. Recent studies have also suggested that even
brief interventions that include SCT improve insomnia, which may be important for treating
the vast numbers of people with insomnia symptoms. Some method for helping patients find
the appropriate level of care will need to be considered.
The development of entirely new treatments based on SCT such as Intensive Sleep Retrain-
ing and using technology to bring treatment into the bedroom is exciting. We can be justifiably
proud of how much progress has been made in developing and evaluating cognitive and behav-
ioral treatments for insomnia. Nevertheless, many challenges in prevention, public policy, as
well as treatment, remain. Effective treatments and new directions are very welcome.
REFERENCES
1. Bootzin RR, Nicassio P. Behavioral treatments for insomnia. In: Hersen M, Eisler R, Miller P, eds.
Progress in Behavior Modification, Vol. 6. New York, NY: Academic Press, 1978:1–45.
2. Bootzin RR, Epstein DR. Stimulus control instructions. In: Lichstein KL, Morin CM, eds. Treatment of
Late-Life Insomnia. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage 2000:167–187
3. Bootzin RR. Stimulus control treatment for insomnia. Proceedings of the 80th Annual Convention of
the American Psychological Association 1972; 7:395–396.
4. Bootzin R. Effects of self-control procedures for insomnia. In: Stuart R, ed. Behavioral Self-
Management: Strategies and Outcomes. New York, NY: Brunner/Mazel, 1977.
5. Malaffo M, Espie CA. Insomnia: The quarter of an hour rule (QHR), a single component of stimulus
control, improves sleep. Sleep 2006; 29:A257.
6. Zeitzer JM, Dijk DJ, Kronauer RE, et al. Sensitivity of the human circadian pacemaker to nocturnal
light: Melatonin phase resetting and suppression. J. Physiol 2000; 526(Pt.3): 695–702.
7. Manber R, Bootzin RR, Acebo C, et al. The effects of regularizing sleep-wake schedules on daytime
sleepiness. Sleep 1996; 19:432–441.
8. Morin CM, Hauri PJ, Espie CA, et al. Nonpharmacologic treatment of chronic insomnia: An American
Academy of Sleep Medicine Review. Sleep 1999; 22:1134–1156.
9. Chesson AL, McDowell WA, Littner M, et al. Practice parameters for the nonpharmacologic treatment
of chronic insomnia. Standards of Practice Committee of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine.
Sleep 1999; 22(8):1128–1133.
c24 IHBK059-Sateia March 23, 2010 19:41 Char Count=
10. Morin CM, Bootzin RR, Buysse DJ, et al. Psychological and behavioral treatment of insomnia: Update
of the recent evidence (1998–2004). Sleep 2006; 29(11):1398–1414.
11. Morgenthaler T, Kramer M, Alessi C, et al. Practice parameters for the psychological and behav-
ioral treatment of insomnia: An update. An american academy of sleep medicine report. Sleep 2006;
29(11):1415–1419.
12. Chambers MJ, Alexander SD. Assessment and prediction of outcome for a brief behavioral insomnia
treatment program. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 1992; 23:289–297.
13. Morin CM, Stone J, McDonald K, et al. Psychological management of insomnia: A clinical replication
series with 100 patients. Behavior Therapy 1994; 25:291–309.
14. Perlis M, Aloia M, Millikan A, et al. Behavioral treatment of insomnia: A clinical case series study. J
Behav Med 2000; 23:149–161.
15. Perlis ML, Sharpe M, Smith MT, et al. Behavioral treatment of insomnia: Treatment outcome and the
relevance of medical and psychiatric morbidity. J Behav Med 2001; 24:281–296.
16. Sivertsen B, Omvik S, Pallesen S, et al. Cognitive behavioral therapy vs zopiclone for treatment of
chronic primary insomnia in older adults: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 2006; 295(24):2851–
2858.
17. Soeffing JP, Lichstein KL, Nau SD, et al. Psychological treatment of insomnia in hypnotic-dependant
older adults. Sleep Med 2008; 9(2):165–171.
18. Harvey L, Inglis SJ, Espie CA. Insomniacs’ reported use of CBT components and relationship to
long-term clinical outcome. Behav Res Ther 2002; 39:45–60.
19. Backhaus J, Hohagen F, Voderholzer U, et al. Long-term effectiveness of a short-term cognitive-
behavioral group treatment for primary insomnia. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2001; 251(1):
35–41.
20. Epstein DR. A Behavioral Intervention to Enhance the Sleep-Wake Patterns of Older Adults with
Insomnia. Unpublished dissertation. Tucson, AZ: University of Arizona, 1994.
21. Ström L, Pettersson R, Andersson G. Internet-based treatment for insomnia: A controlled evaluation.
J Consult Clin Psychol 2004; 72:113–120.
22. Verbeek I, Declerck G, Neven AK, et al. Sleep information by telephone: Callers indicate positive
effects on sleep problems. Sleep and Hypnosis 2002; 4:47–51.
23. Oosterhuis A, Klip EC. The treatment of insomnia through mass media: The results of a televised
behavioural training programme. Soc Sci Med 1997; 8:1223–1229.
24. Mimeault V, Morin CM. Self-help treatment of insomnia: Bibliotherapy with and without professional
guidance. J Consult Clin Psychol 1999; 67:511–519.
25. Morawetz D. Behavioral self-help treatment for insomnia: A controlled evaluation. Behavior Therapy
1989; 20:365–379.
26. Bootzin RR. Is brief behavioral treatment for insomnia effective? Invited editorial commentary on Ger-
main A, Moul DE, Franzen PL, et al. Effects of a brief behavioral ent for late-life insomnia: Preliminary
findings. J Clin Sleep Med 2006; 2:403–406.
27. Smith MT, Neubauer DN. Cognitive behavior therapy for chronic insomnia. Clinical Cornerstone 2003;
5:28–40.
28. Baillargeon L, Demers M, Ladouceur R. Stimulus-control: Nonpharmacologic treatment for insomnia.
Can Fam Physician 1998; 44:73–79.
29. Espie CA, Inglis SJ, Tessier S, et al. The clinical effectivenss of cognitive behaviour therapy for chronic
insomnia: Implementaion and evaluation of a sleep clinic in general medical practice. Behav Res Ther
2001; 39:45–60.
30. Edinger JD, Sampson WS. A primary care “friendly” cognitive behavioral insomnia therapy. Sleep
2003; 26:177–182.
31. Germain A, Moul DE, Franzen PL, et al. Effects of a brief behavioral treatment for late-life insomnia:
Preliminary findings. J Clin Sleep Med 2006; 2(4):407–408.
32. Buysse DJ, Germain A, Moul DE, et al. Efficacy of brief behavioral treatment for insomnia (BBTI) in
older adults with insomnia. Sleep 2009; 32:A282.
33. Goodie JL, Isler WC, Hunter C, et al. Using behavioral health consultants to treat insomnia in primary
care: A clinical case series. J Clin Psychol 2009; 65(3):294–304.
34. McCrae CS, McGovern R, Lukefahr R, et al. Research Evaluating Brief Behavioral Sleep Treatments for
Rural Elderly (RESTORE): A preliminary examination of effectiveness. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2007;
15(11):979–982.
35. Carter PA. A brief behavioral sleep intervention for family caregivers of persons with cancer. Cancer
Nurs 2006; 29(2):95–103.
36. Kaplan R, Wang Y, Loparo K, et al. Evaluation of an automated system for in-home behavioral
treatment of chronic insomnia: Part III. Sleep 2007; 30:A346.
c24 IHBK059-Sateia March 23, 2010 19:41 Char Count=
37. Harris J, Lack L, Wright H, et al. Intensive Sleep Retraining treatment for chronic primary insomnia:
A preliminary investigation. J Sleep Res 2007; 16(3):276–284.
38. Harris J, Lack L, Bootzin R. Randomized controlled trial of an accelerated insomnia therapy. Sleep
2007; 30:A261.
39. Shapiro SL, Carlson LE. The Art and Science of Mindfulness. Washington, DC: American Psychology
Association Books 2009.
40. Bootzin RR, Stevens SJ. Adolescents, substance abuse, and the treatment of insomnia and daytime
sleepiness. Clin Psychol Rev 2005; 25(5):629–644.
41. Haynes PL, Bootzin RR, Smith L, et al. Sleep and aggression in substance abusing adolescents: Results
from an integrative, behavioral sleep treatment pilot program. Sleep 2006; 29:512–520.
42. Manber R, Edinger JD, Gress JL, et al. Cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia enhances depression
outcome in patients with comorbid major depressive disorder and insomnia. Sleep 2008; 31:489.
43. Ong JC, Shapiro SL, Manber R. Combining mindfulness meditation with cognitive-behavior therapy
for insomnia: A treatment-development study. Behavior Therapy 2008; 29:171–182.
44. Britton WB, Bootzin RR, Cousins JC, et al. The contribution of mindfulness practice to a multi-
component behavioral sleep intervention following substance abuse treatment in adolescents: A pilot
study. Substance abuse. In press.
c25 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 13:55 Char Count=
Chien-Ming Yang
Department of Psychology, The Research Center for Mind, Brain & Learning, National Cheng-Chi University,
Taipei, Taiwan
Paul B. Glovinsky
Cognitive Neurosciences Doctoral Program, Department of Psychology, The City College of New York, City
University of New York, New York, and Department of Medicine, Section of Psychology, St. Peter’s Hospital,
St. Peter’s Sleep Center, Albany, New York, U.S.A.
The SRT instruction to spend less time in bed (TIB) effectively reduces total sleep time.
This partial sleep deprivation in turn activates a homeostatic response (5). The composition of
sleep stages across the night known as “sleep architecture” is changed; sleep becomes deeper
as it becomes more compact (6,7). Light NREM stage 1 sleep and wakefulness are reduced
under the pressure of sleep restriction. The amount of deep NREM slow wave sleep (currently
designated stage N3) remains the same but increases as a proportion of total sleep time. REM
sleep changes are more inconsistent. Some studies show a decrease in REM % of total sleep
time and REM minutes while other studies show REM% remains the same. The heightened
homeostatic response triggered by reducing sleep time also leads to shorter sleep latencies. All
of these changes recommended sleep restriction as a means of addressing both sleep-onset and
sleep-maintenance insomnia.
The near immediacy of SRT’s initial effects provides another boon to treatment: it inter-
rupts the circular, self-fulfilling prophecies concerning the prospects for sleep that ultimately
maintain chronic insomnia. At the time SRT was developed theorists were beginning to
understand how psychological dynamics could account for both the appearance of distress and
its perpetuation. Within the realm of sleep, for example, individuals who tend to internalize
psychological problems were seen to have heightened emotional arousal, which in turn
produces trouble sleeping (8). The poor sleep provides a new focus for concern, increasing
emotional distress and further exacerbating the sleep disturbance. This circularity of cause and
effect, called cyclical psychodynamics, represented a major theoretical integration of behavior
therapy and psychodynamics and captured the imagination of clinicians (9). Responding to this
insight, SRT attempted to short-circuit the apprehensive worry about poor sleep that served to
maintain that very condition.
By the late 1970s human circadian rhythm organization was under intense investigation.
The importance of a habitual wake-up time and morning exposure to temporal cues for purposes
of entrainment were widely appreciated. In an early clinical application of chronobiology,
individuals with severe difficulty falling asleep reported much shorter sleep latencies when
they went to bed later than usual on weekends (10). Going to bed later and setting a fixed
wake-up time, staples of SRT, are in line with this chronobiological understanding that sleep
onset is typically more rapid later at night, and that providing regular temporal cues helps
stabilize the sleep–wake cycle.
The improved safety and effectiveness of the benzodiazepines over earlier hypnotic med-
ications encouraged more individuals to seek treatment for chronic sleep disturbance. While
helpful in reducing sleep latency and promoting sustained sleep, these agents were not without
significant limitations especially when used chronically and upon discontinuation (11). Chronic
use of hypnotics with long elimination half-lives was prone to produce daytime hangover while
agents with short half-life were associated with morning anxiety or early morning awakening.
Memory problems and moodiness were reported as well. Finally, the development of drug
tolerance and dependence were of concern.
Thus by the early 1980s, the time was ripe for new approaches to insomnia treatment.
Behavioral therapies for a wide range of psychological problems were showing much promise.
A deepening understanding of the circadian organization of life was starting to shape clinical
interventions. There was much room for improvement in the pharmacological management
of insomnia. Finally, the nascent specialty of behavioral sleep medicine, fostered in newly
established sleep disorders centers as well as academic departments, was for the first time
conceiving of insomnia as not merely the symptom of various underlying problems, but as a
worthy target for systematic treatment trials. SRT drew upon all of these developments.
In the years since its introduction, SRT has been deployed as a stand alone treatment in
a number of studies (Table 1), often in a form slightly modified from its original description.
It was quickly incorporated into the multicomponent treatment known as cognitive behavior
therapy for insomnia (CBT-I; Table 2). The effectiveness of CBT-I for a wide range of insomnias
has been consistently demonstrated; it is now considered the frontline treatment for insomnia
(12). The 2006 American Academy of Sleep Medicine’s Practice Parameters for the treatment
of insomnia rates SRT as a Guideline (13). The Practice Parameters deemed CBT-I a Standard,
describing its behavioral components as follows:
3.6 Cognitive behavior therapy, with or without relaxation therapy is effective and recom-
mended therapy in the treatment of chronic insomnia. [4.2, 4.6] (Standard) . . . The behavioral
c25
IHBK059-Sateia
INSOMNIA: SLEEP RESTRICTION THERAPY
Table 1 Studies of SRT and modified SRT: Summary of methods and results
Treatment Mean Treatment No. of
Author(s) condition N Age Format Sessions Measures Med. F/U Results
Spielman et al. SRT (no control 35 46 Individual 8 weekly Call-ins Sleep Hypnotics: 12 36 wks Both post-treatment & F/U: TST↑,
13:55
Friedman et al. 1: Modified SRT SRT:10 RT:12 69.7 Individual 4 weekly Call-ins BZD: 4 No 3 mons SRT: Post-treatment: TST↑, SOL↓,
(16) 2: Relaxation sessions med: 18 WASO↓, SE↑; F/U: maintain
therapy (RT) treatment effects except for SOL RT:
TST↑ & SOL↓, but no effect in the
Char Count=
other variables
Brooks et al. Nap mod. SRT 9 67.7 Individual 4 weekly Actigraph No med N/A Actigraph: TST↑, SOL = , WASO↓,
(17) (no control sessions Sleep log SE↑ Sleep log: TST = , SOL↓,
condition) WASO↓, SE↑
Riedel et al. 1: Self-help 25 subjects for 67.4 1:Self-help 1:2 Video Sleep log No med 2 mons 1: TST↑, SOL = , WASO↓, SE↑, sleep
(18) Video each groups Video viewing Rating scale satisfaction↑, SSS↓; all effects
(Modified 2:Video + sessions 2:4 SSS maintained at F/U 2: TST = , SOL↓,
SRT-sleep Individual weekly WASO↓, SE↑, sleep satisfaction↑,
compression + sessions SSS↓; all effects maintained and
Sleep TST↑ at F/U 3: TST = , SOL↓,
Education) 2: WASO↓, SE↑, SSS↓
VIDEO +
therapist
guidance
3:Waiting- list
control
Bliwise et al. 1: Modified SRT 16 subjects for 68.7 Individual 4 weekly Call-ins N/A 3 mons 1: Post-treatment: TST = , SOL↓; F/U:
(19) 2: Relaxation both groups sessions + a TST↑ 2: Similar to SRT, but TST
Therapy (RT) wrap-up improved less at F/U
session
Friedman et al. 1: Modified SRT 1:12 64.2 Individual 4 weekly Actigraph No med. 3 mons Actigraph: Post-treatment: no sig.
(20) + sleep 2:12 session + a Sleep log effects, TST↓; F/U: TST = Sleep
hygiene 3:13 wrap-up log: Post-treatment: SE↑ for both
2: Nap mod. session SRT conditions; F/U: SE↑ for all
SRT + sleep conditions
hygiene
3:Sleep hygiene
279
(Continued)
c25
IHBK059-Sateia
280
Table 1 Studies of SRT and modified SRT: Summary of methods and results (Continued )
Treatment Mean Treatment No. of
Author(s) condition N Age Format Sessions Measures Med. F/U Results
13:55
desensitization WASO: decreased for all groups both
(PL) post-treatment and at F/U
TST: interact w/ fatigue level
High fatigue: increased at
Char Count=
post-treatment and F/U for RT, at
F/U for PL; no diff for SRT
Low fatigue: increased at
post-treatment for RT, at F/U for
SRT; no diff for PL
SE: interact w/ fatigue level
High fatigue: increased at
post-treatment and F/U for RT &
SRT; increased at F/U for PL
Low fatigue: increased at F/U for SRT;
increased at post-treatment for PL;
no diff for RT
Abbreviations: TST, total sleep time; TWT, total waking time; SOL, sleep onset latency; WASO, wake after sleep onset; No. Awakening, Number of awakenings; SE, sleep efficiency; ISQ, Insomnia Symptom Questionnaire; SSS,
Stanford Sleepiness Scale; ESS, Epworth Sleepiness Scale; post-tx, post treatment; F/U, follow-up.
Friedman et al. (16): The SRT was modified. Subject tolerance was weighed heavily in the assignment of the allowed TIB at start of treatment. TIB was not reduced for failure to reach criterion.
Friedman et al. (20): Modified SRT-Subject tolerance was weighed heavily in the assignment of the allowed TIB at start of treatment. TIB was not reduced for failure to reach criterion. Patients were given weekly increments of TIB
according to a fixed algorithm based on their initial TST. TIB is only increased by getting into bed earlier at the beginning of the sleep period. The algorithm for determining increased TIB was based on patients’ reported sleepiness
and their initial TIB. All patients received seven-hours TIB by the end of the 4th treatment week.
Nap modification SRT-Optional daytime naps were included. Patients were encouraged to take a 30-min nap daily within a 1:00 to 3:00 PM window.
Brooks et al. (17) : Mandatory daytime naps were included in the protocol because some subjects object to SRT due to daytime sleepiness.
Riedel et al. (18): Sleep compression allows patients to gradually reduce TIB to more closely match TST, and the prescribed TIB is not increased in response to higher SE. During the first session, patients were advised to reduce
SPIELMAN ET AL.
TIB by one-half of the difference between baseline TIB and baseline TST. During session 2 and 3, TIB was further reduced by one-fourth of the difference between baseline TIB and baseline TST.
Bliwise et al. (19): TIB was extended when the floating mean SE over the preceding five days equaled or exceeded 85%. Flexibility in the procedure was employed in the assignment of initial TIB.
Riedel & Lichstein (21): During the first session, patients were recommended to reduce TIB by one-fifth of the difference between baseline TIB and TST. Over the next four sessions, gradual reductions of TIB were recommended to
approach baseline TST.
c25 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 13:55 Char Count=
Table 2 Studies of Insomnia Utilizing Sleep Restriction Therapy as Part of Cognitive Behavior Therapy
(a partial list)
component may include therapies such as stimulus control therapy, sleep restriction, or relax-
ation training.
The review paper (49) accompanying the Practice Parameter paper highlights a dozen
key treatment studies supporting the efficacy of CBT-I. Only six of these studies incorporated
Relaxation Therapy as part of the multicomponent therapy, whereas in all twelve of the studies
SRT was a component.
One early study asked subjects to remain in bed for nearly 24 hours. Sleep broke into
fragments, but the duration of these stints of sleep summed up to more than 14 hours or about
60% of TIB (50). In another study of prolonged bed rest, this time to sixty hours, sleep expanded
to fill nearly 50% of TIB; once again sleep was frequently interrupted by awakenings (51).
Whether the greater percentage of time given over to sleep in these studies reflected the effects
of prior sleep restriction or merely the opportunity presented by extra time in bed is open to
interpretation.
Another study (51) looked at the effects of chronic rather than acute extension of bedtime.
Normal control subjects were exposed to a conventional photoperiod (16 hours, followed by
8 hours in bed) for one week and a short photoperiod (10 hours, followed by 14 hours in
bed) for four weeks. Sleep duration increased by about one hour a night under conditions of
prolonged bed rest. As seen in the studies of acute bedtime extension, sleep efficiency was
reduced and sleep fragmentation increased. However, a distinct pattern emerged with repeated
10:14 light/dark cycles: Sleep was split into two equal bouts, the first on the “evening” end of the
prolonged bed rest period, the second on the “morning” end, separated by one to three hours of
wakefulness. The author suggests that this biphasic sleep pattern may be more representative
of sleep in humans over hundreds of thousands of years until the recent past, when artificial
light became available to produce a fixed, prolonged photoperiod across the seasons, resulting
in the monophasic, highly efficient sleep we think of as “normal.”
Another protocol employed prolonged bed rest to investigate the presence of objective
sleepiness [a sleep latency less than or equal to six minutes on a multiple sleep latency test
(MSLT)] in individuals with no subjective sleepiness complaint (52). One group of subjects
spent 10 hours in bed and the other spent their habitual time in bed (averaging about 8 hours)
for 14 days. Nocturnal sleep recordings followed by daytime MSLTs occurred periodically across
the two weeks. Consistent with the studies cited above, the group following an extended TIB
schedule showed increased sleep time and reduced sleep efficiency at night, as well as reduced
objective sleepiness during the day, compared to those following a self-selected TIB schedule. It
is interesting to note with regard to the present discussion that a subgroup of individuals in this
study did not demonstrate reduced objective sleepiness after extending TIB. Sleep efficiency
plummeted immediately after TIB was increased for these subjects. This response is similar
to the experience of individuals with insomnia, who try to accumulate additional sleep by
spending more time in bed and wind up instead with more frequent or longer awakenings.
A second component of the rationale for SRT is that it focuses on practices perpetuating
insomnia that are more amenable to treatment. According to our conceptualization of insomnia
(known as the 3P Model, Figure 1 53,14) the factors that predisposed a particular patient to
PRECIPITATING/PERPETUATING FACTORS
CONTRIBUTING TO INSOMNIA OVER TIME
INSOMNIA SEVERITY
PERPETUATING
E.G., PRECIPITATING
Insomnia SLEEP EXTENSION PREDISPOSING
Threshold E.G.,
LIFE
STRESS
E.G.,
HYPERMETABOLIC
Time
Figure 1 The 3P model of insomnia depicting changing influences during the course of insomnia. Source: From
Ref. (53, Adapted from 14; modified by Max Hirschkowitz and Michael Perlis).
c25 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 13:55 Char Count=
insomnia as well as those that precipitated the disturbance may be too remote or too entrenched
to serve as fruitful targets for short-term treatment. However, insomnia can still be effectively
treated by addressing factors, which perpetuate the sleep disturbance. Compensating for sleep
loss by spending an excessive amount of time in bed is, as discussed above, a frequently
encountered factor maintaining chronic insomnia—one that is directly targeted by SRT.
In some cases patients may readily concur that the factors that originally triggered their
sleep disturbance are not operative at present. For example, insomnia may have appeared in the
midst of a patient’s bitter divorce, yet his sleeplessness persists in the context of a successful new
relationship. Or it may have followed the arrival of a colicky newborn, who three years later, is
now sleeping through the night while her mother still struggles with frequent awakenings.
Even when the original trigger of insomnia still looms large as a source of distress, it
may be advantageous to target seemingly secondary perpetuating factors to achieve rapid
improvement. For example, a woman’s sleeplessness may be clearly related to the ongoing
stress of contending with a tyrannical boss who never appears satisfied with her performance.
A holistic approach to this problem may include advocating changes in behavior (such as
negotiating clearly defined benchmarks that will serve as the basis of regular reviews) as well
as attitude (such as challenging the idea that any shortfalls in meeting objectives necessarily
reflect incompetence). While these changes are being supported, it still may make clinical sense
to take steps that directly impact sleep. It may not be easy to adhere to an early alarm, but at least
this is an action that is fully under the patient’s control, and one that should yield predictable
results. It counters the loss of efficacy that can prove so corrosive to self confidence, regardless
of whether that sense of helplessness is encountered on the job or when trying to sleep.
The 3P Model also posits that patients may, over the course of a chronic insomnia, become
sensitized to potential sources of sleep disruption. Prior to the onset of the disturbance, sleep is
often seen as more resilient, and behaviors such as drinking caffeinated or alcoholic beverages
at dinner or oversleeping on weekends could apparently be engaged in with impunity. Seven
and a half hours in bed would hardly be deemed generous. We explain that “that was then”
and that now, at least for the time being, a patient in a sensitized or hyperaroused state may
not be capable of consistently sustaining seven or more hours of sleep. It is better under these
conditions to more closely match bedtime with expected total sleep time, even if that is currently
six hours. While we do not guarantee that seven and a half hours of sleep will eventually be
attained on a nightly basis, we do hold out the reasonable expectation that both bedtime and
sleep time will gradually increase over the course of treatment, and invite patients to partner
with us in determining what the optimal balance between sleep restriction and sleep quality is
for them.
A third major benefit of SRT is that it improves the experience of going to bed. Tossing
and turning for hours on end is not pleasant. The bed starts to be dreaded as a place of torment
rather than as a haven from waking concerns. A self-fulfilling prophecy is set in motion, one
in which the expectation of another miserable night and the prospects of having to muddle
through the next day increases anxiety and arousal, thereby leading to that very result. By
contrast, with SRT patients come to expect that bedtime will be short—and sweet, in the sense
that a greater proportion of it will be spent in blissful oblivion. They may actually look forward
to the designated time when they can “finally get into bed.”
A fourth reason why SRT benefits people with insomnia is that it strengthens the circadian
sleep–wake cycle. It replaces a welter of short stints of sleep occurring at various times around
the clock with a single nocturnal period that is more predictably filled with sleep. Patients are
more likely to be awoken by an alarm when under this treatment, leading to consistently timed
exposure to morning light and the various demands of waking life. These cues help keep sleep
synchronized with other biological rhythms, which in turn leads to the more reliable appearance
of sleep shortly after bedtime, and deeper sleep once it arrives.
Offering a considered and detailed rationale for the effectiveness of SRT satisfies patients’
‘need to know’ while acknowledging the counterintuitive nature of the treatment. Taking the
time to thoroughly prepare patients for this therapy will motivate them for the difficulties
ahead. Hopefully they will begin to think differently about their prospects for sleep, about
what it means to be awake and out of bed later into the night or earlier in the morning, and
more generally about their ability to shape their nocturnal experience over the long term. These
c25 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 13:55 Char Count=
cognitive changes are critical therapeutic components that work in concert with behavioral
change to foster improved sleep.
IMPLEMENTING SLEEP RESTRICTION THERAPY
SRT begins with a two-week sleep diary. The average subjective sleep time over the course of
this diary is calculated. This value is then prescribed, as the amount of time in bed (TIB) patients
should spend at the start of treatment. Changes in sleep efficiency (SE, calculated as subjective
sleep time/time in bed X 100%) more than a five day moving window trigger increases and
decreases in TIB (see Table 4) (1).
Setting specific times of retiring and rising to delimit an initial time in bed requires
thoughtful analysis. The patient’s sleep diary may offer clues as to what period would be most
likely to maximize sleep at the start of treatment. In the case of a patient who has no difficulty
falling asleep but who awakens too early in the morning, with difficulty falling back to sleep,
TIB should start at the habitual time of retiring at night. As a result the wake-up time will
be earlier than usual. In patients who typically get their best sleep in the middle of the night,
or whose sleep is quite broken with awakenings scattered haphazardly across the night, the
assigned sleep schedule should start later and end earlier than usual. Finally, if a patient’s
primary difficulty is one of falling asleep, the assigned retiring and rising times should be later
than usual, within the constraint of meeting starting time for work.
Some patients will have obligations such as variable work shifts or shared childcare
responsibilities that necessitate rising early on an intermittent basis. In the interests of entraining
a robust circadian sleep–wake cycle, the clinician may take the opportunity to set a fixed early
morning rising time as part of the initial SRT instructions. The choice of retiring and rising times
should also take various practical concerns into account such as possible disturbance of bed
partners or nighttime needs of pets.
It should be stressed that although SRT can be summarized with a few quantitatively
based prescriptions, this does not relieve the clinician of the need to exercise judgment. Patients’
forbearance for the challenges posed by the treatment is typically limited to one good effort, and
this chance for improvement may be squandered if the clinician’s approach is too rigid. Some
individuals become so concerned at the prospect of a limited bedtime (“. . .but I always sleep so
much worse when I know I have to get up early”) that clinical judgment trumps strict adherence
to the SRT method. Negotiation with such patients is required to come up with an acceptable
level of sleep restriction. In anticipation of their difficulties adhering to even modified SRT
schedules, it is recommended that the clinician ‘go on record’ as follows: “OK, we will permit a
bit more time in bed for you, but it may take longer for your sleep to improve. Therefore, for this
to work it is imperative that you limit your time in bed to the specific hours we have agreed on.”
A number of variations on the original SRT procedure have emerged in the two decades
since the therapy was introduced. Some investigators and clinicians set the initial TIB equal to
the average sleep obtained from the sleep diary plus half-hour, since even good sleepers do not
have 100% sleep efficiency. The extra bedtime therefore allows a more realistic possibility of
garnering the targeted amount of sleep. Another method called sleep compression (54) restricts
TIB minimally at first. As treatment proceeds time in bed is further reduced. This approach is
easier for patients to adhere to at the start of treatment. We have employed an SRT variation
that runs in the opposite direction, with initial TIB initially quite restricted as in standard SRT,
followed by weekly extension of TIB in 15 or 30 minute increments, continued so long as TIB
contains less than 45 minutes of wakefulness (15). This method avoids the dispiriting effect of
having to reduce TIB repeatedly—the patient knows that “the worst is over” in terms of bedtime
restriction after the first week or so.
Patient characteristics should be taken into account before prescribing SRT. There is little
reason, for example, to recommend SRT to individuals whose sleep is already compact. (These
individuals may still present with insomnia, typically complaining of insufficient sleep due to
early morning awakenings.) Consider the patient who can fall asleep soon after getting into
bed at 11 PM and obtain a solid five hours of sleep, but who then awakens around 4 AM and
ruminates on the day ahead for two hours before getting out of bed. Her sleep efficiency is
low at 71% and she is complaining of daytime fatigue. Restricting bedtime to about five hours
via SRT is not likely to significantly heighten her sleep drive, because it will not appreciably
reduce total sleep time. She is likely to continue to have a compact but short sleep period of
five hours on SRT. Employing stimulus control instructions may present the better option here,
with the aim of weakening the association between this patient’s early morning awakenings
and the opportunity they present to obsess in bed. It would allow the possibility of “resetting”
her frame of mind through a period of distracting activity such as reading and then returning
to bed and to sleep. If, on the other hand, you deduce that a patient is in fact flitting in and
out of sleep for the last two hours in bed, perhaps understandably not counting this as “really
sleeping,” SRT offers an appropriate treatment choice.
When conducting SRT in patients with Paradoxical Insomnia (previously called Sleep
State Misperception) the original rules for increasing time in bed do not work well. It is true
that sleep efficiency can be marginally increased when the assigned time in bed of patients
who report, say, one hour of sleep is decreased well below the minimum duration of about five
hours we typically employ. However, their sleep efficiency will rarely approach even a relaxed
criterion for increasing time in bed, as these patients typically do not perceive and report any
increases in sleep time (unpublished data). In these patients, weekly increases in time in bed
following a severe initial reduction, one of the SRT variants discussed above, may improve
daytime functioning although nighttime complaints will likely persist.
High
Benefits
Costs
Low
Figure 2 A cost/benefit model of sleep restriction therapy utilizing sleep efficiency and daytime functioning as
markers of sleep need. Costs are indexed by time spent in bed and vulnerability to insomnia while benefits are
reflected by duration of sleep time and daytime function.
As treatment proceeds levels of TIB are gradually increased while maintaining sleep
efficiency near an optimal level. Both costs and benefits rise, but initially the rate of beneficial
change greatly outpaces the rise in costs. Consistent accumulation of well-consolidated sleep,
even if of modest duration, yields better daytime performance than had short stints of sleep
scattered haphazardly across the clock. Relatively few hours are being spent in bed, while
residual sleep loss guards against the resurgence of insomnia. Through a process of bedtime
titration, a point is reached where both sleep efficiency and daytime functioning are at relatively
high levels, where sleep need has been satisfied, but not sated to the point of inviting the
reappearance of insomnia. The benefit/cost ratio will be highest on this bedtime schedule. It
should be maintained going forward at least until such time that other salient factors which
had predisposed, precipitated and/or perpetuated insomnia have changed. For example, once
anticipatory anxiety over what each night will bring has subsided, a slightly longer TIB may be
indulged in without courting harm to sleep.
As TIB approaches baseline levels, the slope of the curve representing incurred costs
accelerates. A relatively large amount of time is again being spent in bed. More critically, the
potential for sleep disturbance rises disproportionately at higher levels of TIB. There is little
residual sleep loss at this point that may be counted upon to quickly induce and sustain sleep.
Meanwhile, benefits plateau when TIB is minimally restricted. Neither sleep time nor functional
capacity can be expected to continue to rise much, if at all, as sleep efficiency begins to ebb.
Thus sleep need may still be partially unmet after successful treatment. Optimally balancing
sleep efficiency and daytime functioning does not mean that either nocturnal sleep or daytime
capacity alone will be maximized. There are many intransigent aspects of insomnia (typically
categorized in the 3P Model as predisposing factors, such as hyperarousal) that may require
concessions of benefits received from sleep, both by night and day, in the interest of maximizing
the overall sleep/wake experience.
CONCLUSION
In the 21 years since SRT was first introduced, awareness of the health, economic and public
safety issues raised by sleep disturbance has crystallized. Insomnia is no longer exclusively
confined to the role of symptom or side effect, but rather recognized as at times a primary
complaint, with its own course and indications for treatment (55). A new generation of hypnotic
medication has been developed to remedy sleep initiation and maintenance difficulties, and a
new paradigm of long-term reliance on such medication has gained adherents. In this context,
the role played by psychological factors such as anticipatory anxiety and drug dependence in
c25 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 13:55 Char Count=
perpetuating insomnia looms large. CBT-I, with SRT now firmly ensconced among its corner-
stones, is uniquely positioned to address the psychophysiological, behavioral and cognitive
challenges posed by chronic insomnia. A multitude of patients stand to benefit as their physi-
cians and clinicians gain a stronger appreciation of these demonstrably effective treatments.
REFERENCES
1. Spielman AJ, Saskin P, Thorpy MJ. Treatment of chronic insomnia by restriction of time spent in bed.
Sleep 1987; 10(1):45–56.
2. Jacobson E. You Must Relax. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill Book, Co, 1934:201.
3. Bootzin RR. Stimulus control treatment for insomnia. Proc Am Psychol Assoc 1972; 7:395–396.
4. Hauri PJ. The Sleep Disorders: Current Concepts, 2nd ed. Kalamazoo, MI: Upjohn, 1982.
5. Borbely AA, Achermann P. Sleep homeostasis and models of sleep regulation. In: Kryger M, Roth T,
Dement W, eds. Principles and Practices of Sleep Medicine, 4th, ed. Toronto, Canada: W.B. Saunders
Co, 2005: 405–417.
6. Webb WB, Agnew HW. The effects of a chronic limitation of sleep length. Psychophysiology. 1974;
11:265–274.
7. Mullaney DJ, Johnson LC, Naitoh JP, et al. Sleep during and after gradual sleep reduction. Psychophys-
iology 1977; 14:237–244.
8. Kales A, Caldwell AB, Preston TA, et al. Personality patterns in insomnia. Theoretical implications.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1976; 33(9):1128–1124.
9. Wachtel P. Psychoanalysis and Behavior Therapy: Toward an Integration. New York, NY: Basic Books,
Inc, 1977.
10. Weitzman ED, Czeisler C, Coleman R, et al. Delayed sleep phase syndrome: A chronobiological
disorder with sleep onset insomnia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1981; 38:737–746.
11. Mendelson WB, Roth T, Cassella J, et al. The treatment of chronic insomnia: Drug indications, chronic
use and abuse liability. Summary of a 2001 new clinical drug evaluation unit meeting symposium.
Sleep Med Rev 2004; 8:7–17.
12. Morgenthaler T, Kramer M, Alessi C, et al. Practice parameters for the psychological and behavioral
treatment of insomnia: An update. an american academy of sleep medicine report standards of practice.
Sleep 2006; 29(11):1415–1419.
13. Morin CM, Bootzin RR, Buysse DJ. Psychological and behavioral treatment of insomnia: Update of
the recent evidence (1998–2004). Sleep 2006; 29(11):1398–1414 .
14. Spielman AJ, Caruso L, Glovinsky P. A behavioral perspective on insomnia treatment. Psychiatr Clin
North Am 1987; 10(4):541–553.
15. Morin CM, Kowatch RA, O’Shanick G. Sleep Restriction for the inpatient treatment of insomnia. Sleep
1990; 13(2):183–186.
16. Friedman L, Bliwise DL, Yesavage JA, et al. A preliminary study comparing sleep restriction amd
relaxation treatment for insomnia in older adults. J Gerontol 1991; 46:1–8.
17. Brooks JO 3rd,Friedman L, Bliwise DL, et al. Use of the wrist actigraph to study insomnia in older
adults. Sleep 1993; 16(2):51–55.
18. Riedel BW, Lichstein KL, Dwyer WO. Sleep compression and sleep education for older insomniacs:
Self-help versus therapist guidance. Psychol Aging 1995; 10:54–63.
19. Bliwise DL, Friedman L, Nekich JC, et al. Prediction of outcome in behaviorally based insomnia
treatments. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 1995; 26(1):17–23.
20. Friedman L, Benson K, Noda A, et al. An actigraphic comparison of sleep restriction and sleep hygiene
treatments for insomnia in older adults. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 2000; 13(1):17–27.
21. Riedel BW, Lichstein KL. Strategies for evaluating adherence to sleep restriction treatment for insom-
nia. Behav Res Ther 2001; 39:201–212.
22. Lichstein KL, Riedel BW, Wilson NM, et al. Relaxation and sleep compression for late-life insomnia:
A placebo-controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psychol 2001; 69 (2): 227–239.
23. Morin CM, Kowatch RA, Barry T, et al. Cognitive-behavior therapy for late-life insomnia. J Consult
Clin Psychol 1993; 61 (1):137–146.
24. Morin CM, Colecchi C, Stone J, et al. Behavioral and pharmacological therapies for late-life insomnia:
A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 1999; 281 (11):991–999.
25. Mimeault V, Morin CM. Self-help treatment for insomnia: Bibliotherapy with and without professional
guidance. J Consult Clin Psychol 1999; 67 (4):511–519.
26. Verbeek I, Schreuder K, Declerck G. Evaluation of short-term nonpharmacological treatment of insom-
nia in a clinical setting. J Psychosom Res 1999; 47 (4):369–383.
27. Currie SR, Wilson KG, Pontefract AJ, et al. Cognitive-behavioral treatment of insomnia secondary to
chronic pain. J Consult Clin Psychol 2000; 68 (3): 407–416.
c25 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 13:55 Char Count=
28. Perlis M, Aloia M, Millikan A. et al. Behavioral treatment of insomnia: A clinical case series study. J
Behav Med 2000; 23(2):149–161.
29. Espie CA, Inglis SJ, Tessier S, et al. The clinical effectiveness of cognitive behaviour therapy for chronic
insomnia–implementation and evaluation of a sleep clinic in general medical practice. Behav Res Ther
2001; 39(1):45–60.
30. Perlis ML, Sharpe M, Smith MT, et al. Behavioral treatment of insomnia: Treatment outcome and the
relevance of medical and psychiatric morbidity. J Behav Med 2001; 24(3):281–296.
31. Edinger JD, Wohlgemuth WK, Radtke RA, et al. Cognitive behavioral therapy for treatment of chronic
primary insomnia: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 2001; 285(14):1856–1864.
32. Harvey L, Inglis SJ, Espie CA. Insomniacs’ reported use of CBT components and relationship to
long-term clinical outcome. Behav Res Ther 2002; 40(1):75–83.
33. Currie SR, Wilson KG, Curran D. Clinical significance and predictors of treatment response to
cognitive-behavior therapy for insomnia secondary to chronic pain. J Behav Med 2002; 25 (2):135–
153.
34. Rybarczyk B, Lopez M, Schelble K, et al. Home-based video CBT for comorbid geriatric insomnia: A
pilot study using secondary data analyses. Behav Sleep Med 2005; 3 (3):158–175.
35. Edinger JD, Sampson WS. A primary care “friendly” cognitive behavioral insomnia therapy. Sleep
2003; 26:177–182.
36. Ouellet MC, Morin CM. Cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia associated with traumatic brain
injury: A single-case study. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2004; 85(8):1298–1302.
37. Morin CM, Bastien C, Guay B, et al. Randomized clinical trial of supervised tapering and cognitive
behavior therapy to facilitate benzodiazepine discontinuation in older adults with chronic insomnia.
Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161 (2):332–342.
38. Morgan K, Dixon S, Mathers N, et al. Psychological treatment for insomnia in the regulation of
long-term hypnotic drug use. Health Technol Assess (Rockv) 2004; 8(8):iii–iv, 1–68.
39. Currie SR, Clark S, Hodgins DC, El-Guebaly N. Randomized controlled trial of brief cognitive-
behavioural interventions for insomnia in recovering alcoholics. Addiction 2004; 99:1121–1132.
40. Perlis ML, Smith MT, Orff H, et al. The effects of modafinil and cognitive behavior therapy on sleep
continuity in patients with primary insomnia. Sleep 2004; 27 (4):715–725.
41. Jacobs GD, Pace-Schott EF, Stickgold R, et al. Cognitive behavior therapy and pharmacotherapy for
insomnia: A randomized controlled trial and direct comparison. Arch Intern Med 2004; 164 (17):1888–
1896.
42. Dopke CA, Lehner RK, Wells AM. Cognitive-behavioral group therapy for insomnia in individuals
with serious mental illnesses: A preliminary evaluation. Psychiatr Rehabil J 2004; 27:235–242.
43. Stiefel F, Stagno D. Management of insomnia in patients with chronic pain conditions. CNS Drugs
2004; 18(5);285–296.
44. Rybarczyk B, Stepanski E, Fogg L, et al. A placebo-controlled test of cognitive-behavioral therapy for
comorbid insomnia in older adults. J Consult Clin Psychol 2005; 73 (6):1164–1174.
45. Savard J, Simard S, Ivers H, et al. Randomized study on the efficacy of cognitive-behavioral therapy
for insomnia secondary to breast cancer, part I: Sleep and psychological effects. J Clin Oncol 2005;
23(25):6083–6096.
46. Sivertsen B, Omvik S, Pallesen S, et al. Cognitive behavioral therapy vs. zopiclone for treatment of
chronic primary insomnia in older adults: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 2006; 295(24):2851–
2858.
47. Ouellet MC, Morin CM. Efficacy of cognitive-behavioral therapy for insomnia associated with trau-
matic brain injury: A single-case experimental design. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2007; 88(12):1581–1592.
48. Dirksen SR, Epstein DR. Efficacy of an insomnia intervention on fatigue, mood and quality of life in
breast cancer survivors. J Adv Nurs 2008; 61(6):664–675.
49. Nakagawa Y. Continuous observation of EEG patterns at night and daytime of normal subjects under
restrained conditions. I. Quiescent state when lying down. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol
1980; 49 (5–6):524–537.
50. Campbell SS. Duration and placement of sleep in a “disentrained” environment. Psychophysiology
1984; 21 (1):106–113.
51. Wehr TA. In short photoperiods, human sleep is biphasic. J Sleep Res 1992; 1 (2):103–107.
52. Roehrs T, Shore E, Papineau K, et al. A two-week sleep extension in sleepy normals. Sleep 1996; 19
(7):576–582.
53. Spielman AJ. Assessment of insomnia. Clin Psychol Rev 1986; 6:11–25.
54. Glovinsky PB, Spielman AJ. The Insomnia Answer. New York, NY: A Perigee Book, division of The
Penguin Group, 2006.
55. National Institutes of Health. National institutes of health state of the science conference statement on
manifestations and management of chronic insomnia in adults, June 13–15, 2005. Sleep 2005; 28:1049–
1057.
c26 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 13:56 Char Count=
There are a wide variety of nonpharmacological treatments for insomnia including stimulus
control, sleep restriction, and cognitive therapy (1,2), which are all covered extensively elsewhere
in this volume. There are also “other” types of nonpharmacological treatments sometimes used
to treat insomnia discussed within this chapter. Some of these methods, such as progressive
muscle relaxation and paradoxical intention, are commonly used, have significant empirical
backing, and have been listed as “effective and recommended therapies” by the Standards of
Practice Committee of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine (3). Others (e.g., exercise,
acupuncture, aromatherapy) have less rigorous empirical backing, often consisting of case
studies or case-series designs without control conditions, but were deemed worthy of review
because they are used by some practitioners and have not received adequate review elsewhere.
The current chapter addresses these “other” nonpharmacological treatments of insomnia with
a description of efficacy/effectiveness studies. In terms of description of efficacy/effectiveness
studies, more focus was given to those treatments which have received less systematic review
in the current literature.
RELAXATION TECHNIQUES
Relaxation techniques are some of the most frequently used treatments of insomnia (2,4,5). The
rationale and proposed mechanisms of action underlying the use of relaxation methods in the
treatment of insomnia stem from the hypothesis that people with insomnia suffer from increased
somatic and cognitive arousal. Relaxation methods serve to reduce somatic arousal and/or
cognitive arousal, thereby increasing the likelihood that patients will fall asleep. Although a
variety (e.g., breathing retraining, imagery, meditation, biofeedback) of relaxation interventions
have been tested as treatments for insomnia, progressive muscle relaxation (PMR) (6,7) has the
most evidence as a treatment for insomnia (3).
Few studies have compared relaxation procedures, so the fact that PMR has the greatest
evidence may be due to a greater interest in this procedure during the early days of insomnia
treatment research, with little subsequent research focusing on single methods of treatment. It
is possible that other relaxation treatments are equally efficacious, but have not been rigorously
evaluated. In practice, many if not most clinicians actually combine multiple forms of relaxation
treatment (e.g., progressive relaxation, breathing retraining, and autogenics). Detail about the
actual techniques of the various relaxation treatments available and the myriad of issues (e.g.,
variants, therapist issues, common factors) involved in each are covered in much greater depth
in numerous texts (e.g., 9–11).
Biofeedback
Because only one placebo-controlled trial has been performed to date, the Standards of Practice
Committee of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine did not identify this treatment as
“empirically supported,” instead listing it as “probably efficacious” because two studies have
shown it was better than wait-list control and one study showed it was better than no treatment.
Biofeedback is a specific form of relaxation treatment that differs from those mentioned
above in that it actually provides sensory feedback (usually visual or auditory), either mechan-
ically (i.e., thermometers) or more frequently with computers and amplifiers, to help patients
learn how to control physiological parameters such as finger temperature or muscle tension, in
order to reduce somatic arousal (3). For instance, frontalis electromyography (EMG) biofeed-
back, the most commonly studied, teaches subjects to reduce muscle tension in the muscles of
the forehead and face. Biofeedback seems to help patients attain states of mental and physical
relaxation and become more aware of their own bodily sensations and responses to stressors.
Biofeedback actively involves the patient in the therapeutic process and provides immediate
measures of progress. One difficulty with evaluating the effectiveness of biofeedback is that it is
often paired with some form of relaxation exercise, making it difficult to parse out the indepen-
dent effects of each. In addition, improvements appear to be comparable to PMR, which takes
less time for the patient to learn and requires no expensive equipment. Therefore, this method
is not recommended over stimulus control, sleep restriction, or PMR, unless the patient fails to
benefit from those other methods.
Yoga
Kundalini Yoga was popularized by Yogi Bhajan in the late 1960s as a means of general life
enhancement and to explore altered states of consciousness without the use of drugs. Yoga
involves the awareness of breath (pranayama) and thought processes in addition to a series of
postures (asanas) designed to stretch and strengthen the body. Yoga as traditionally practiced is
often combined with aspects of PMR (especially in “corpse pose”) and meditation. As one can
see, many of these elements overlap with the relaxation techniques already discussed.
There is growing evidence supporting yoga as an alternative treatment for insomnia, and
yoga research has recently received funding from the National Institute of Complementary and
Alternative Medicine at the National Institutes of Health (15). So far, many trials investigating
the use of yoga for the purpose of improving sleep have been limited by small sample size and
lack of replication (16). One randomized controlled trial found that yoga was more effective than
wait-list control in reducing self-reported sleep disturbance in elderly residents in a care facility.
To date, the majority of the data concerning the efficacy of yoga consists of case series designs
using mainly self-report measures of improvement (17,18). This is less than optimal because
these forms of assessment are more susceptible to social desirability and placebo effects. It is
also unknown how much of the benefit gleaned from Yoga in these previous studies would
have been found by just using traditional relaxation procedures, due to the lack of comparison
groups.
c26 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 13:56 Char Count=
PARADOXICAL INTENTION
Paradoxical intention aims to reduce the patient’s performance anxiety about falling asleep
through instructions to get in bed and passively remain awake rather than try to fall asleep. This
persuasion to engage in a feared behavior (staying awake) is thought to alleviate performance
anxiety, thereby allowing sleep to come more easily (2).
All research on this technique has focused on patients with sleep-onset insomnia, with
two studies showing it to be more effective than placebo control, and one study showing it to be
better than wait-list control. Because the effects of this intervention are generally smaller than
the single treatments of stimulus control, sleep restriction, and PMR, it is rarely recommended
over those interventions, but may be useful to those who do not benefit from other methods
(2,4,5).
EXERCISE
Exercise and physical activity have known benefits on health (e.g., improved cardiovascular
fitness, weight loss, increased maximal oxygen uptake) (26). There is also a known relationship
between physical activity and sleep, although the exact nature of that relationship is unknown.
The strongest theoretical basis for this relationship comes from the thermodynamic hypothesis of
c26 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 13:56 Char Count=
sleep. This theory is founded on the drop in body temperature that accompanies the onset of
sleep, and which may serve as a signal to the body that it is time for sleep. If the rapid decrease
in temperature that precedes sleep serves as a signal for sleep onset, it is possible that by raising
the body temperature (e.g., via exercise), the internal signal will either be enhanced (i.e., if
exercise occurs at the right time to have the resultant cooling of the body coincide with sleep
onset), or delayed (i.e., if exercise occurs too close to sleep) (27,28).
Generally, research shows that moderate to high intensity exercise sessions four to eight
hours before bedtime results in improved sleep quality in a normal population (29–31). There
is also evidence that discrete episodes of physical activity either less than four or more than
eight hours before bed time can actually be detrimental to sleep (29). Unfortunately, there is
little information regarding the effects of physical activity on insomnia. Most of the research
that has examined these two variables has been epidemiological in nature. For example, a
longitudinal study in older adults found lower levels of physical activity consistently predicted
insomnia status, along with depressed mood and lower physical health, over an eight-year
period (32). Another epidemiological study found that regular exercise was associated with a
lower prevalence of insomnia (33).
One intervention study, in older adults with moderate sleep complaints, showed that a
16-week moderate intensity exercise program was better than a wait-list control at improving
global Pittsburg Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) (34) scores as well as sleep onset latency, sleep
duration, and rated sleep quality, as assessed by the PSQI and sleep diaries (31). Another study
that did not specifically target insomnia, but whose sample met the research diagnostic criteria
for insomnia at baseline, also found positive results following a four-week moderate intensity
exercise intervention (35). However, this was a very small study that also included an afternoon
nap as part of the intervention; thus, the results are not as easily interpreted.
While epidemiologic research provides some information regarding the relationship
between insomnia and physical activity, there is a definite need for additional randomized
controlled trials examining the effects of a physical activity intervention in a population with
insomnia and on the specific sleep parameters that make up insomnia.
ACUPUNCTURE
Based in Traditional Chinese Medicine, acupuncture involves the insertion of very fine needles
into the skin at specific points to influence the body’s functioning. These points are considered
to rest on ‘meridians’, or channels of a network of energy called ‘chi’ that flows throughout
the body. Each meridian is thought to be related to specific internal functions and imbalances
in the flow of chi are thought to lead to disease processes in whichever internal function
the imbalanced meridian governs. Acupuncture is thought to correct this imbalance, thereby
alleviating the disease process. The neurological mechanisms of acupuncture are beyond the
scope of this chapter, and are more thoroughly covered in other texts (e.g., 36,37).
One open clinical trial of 18 anxious adult subjects found that acupuncture significantly
increased nighttime endogenous melatonin secretion, as well as improving PSG measures of
SOL, TST, and SE (38). Although this was not a group of people with insomnia, we do know
that insomnia and anxiety are closely related (39,40), and it is reasonable to assume that if
acupuncture can improve sleep in those without a diagnosed insomnia disorder, then it may
produce similar or greater changes in those with insomnia. To date, very little efficacy data
exists on the effects of acupuncture on insomnia. Almost all of the data come from case-series
studies showing improvement of sleep in individuals with disorders other than insomnia (e.g.,
HIV, stress) (41,42). In addition, many have publication or location biases (43).
Alternate Forms
Acupuncture is sometimes used in combination with other forms of traditional Eastern
medicine, such as moxibustion. Moxibustion is the combustion of mugwort herb that has
been ground into a powder; it is either applied directly to the skin or held over acupuncture
points and is thought to warm and stimulate the circulation and chi. Electroacupuncture is the
application of a pulsating electrical current to acupuncture needles. This is thought to provide
stronger and more prolonged stimulation to the acupuncture point than could be attained
via finger stimulation alone. Acupressure involves the application of finger pressure, not
c26 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 13:56 Char Count=
MASSAGE
Massage is manipulation of the body’s soft tissues (muscles, connective tissue, lymphatic vessels,
etc.) either manually or with aids such as rollers or rocks. Various types of massage exist, from
Swedish “relaxation” massage to deep tissue “shiatsu” massage. Each can be applied to various
parts of the body, including feet, back, shoulders, and face. Among the many goals of massage
(physical, therapeutic, psychological) it has the potential to improve sleep by reducing somatic
arousal and/or cognitive arousal, similar to the previously reviewed relaxation methods (44).
To date, no studies have specifically examined the effects of massage on insomnia. Several
case-series studies have been performed which examine the effect of various types of massage
on sleep in populations without insomnia. For instance, a back massage technique similar to
Effleurage (i.e., skim or lightly touch the skin) has been shown to improve sleep in elderly
hospital patients. One technique of massage combined with sesame oil, increased post-massage
sleep time versus no-treatment control in healthy infants versus control group (45).
AROMATHERAPY
Aromatherapy is a rapidly growing subfield of Complementary and Alternative Medicine (46),
but little evidence exists for the efficacy of aroma alone. Aromatic essential oils reported to have
calming, relaxing or sedating effects include: Bergamot (Citrus bergamia), Roman Chamomile
(Chamomelium nobilis), Jasmine (Jasminum grandiflorum), Lavender (Lavandula angustifolia), Man-
darin (Citrus deliciosa), Marjoram (Origanum majorana), Melissa (Melissa officinalis), Neroli (Neroli
bigarade), Patchouli (Pogostemon cabin), Egypt Rose (Rosa damascene), Ylang-ylang (Cananga
odourata), and Vetiver (Vetivera zizanoides) (47). Aromatherapy is dispersed via mists or a special-
ized misting machine, sprayed on pillows, placed in sachets, potpourri, or scented oil warmers,
or combined with massage. As with herbal therapies, the exact “mechanisms of action are
speculative and unclear ”(48).
Lavender
One small (N = 10) randomized controlled trial using a cross-cover design showed a trend (p =
0.07) for Lavender oil (Lavandula angustifolia) to improve scores on the PSQI (48). Another small
(N = 12) study of children aged 12 to 15 with autism and learning difficulties showed no benefit
on sleep patterns of aromatherapy massage with lavender oil (49). In a single-blind repeated
measures study of 42 female college students, diluted lavender fragrance had a beneficial effect
on several sleep parameters—sleep latency, insomnia severity, and sleep quality (50). Other
studies have shown some improvements in sleep of “healthy” sleepers, but these have not been
translated to insomnia patients (51). Effective proportions of lavender oil/carrier oil have yet to
be confirmed through further studies.
Sandalwood
A study using inhaled diluted sandalwood oil in sleep-disturbed rats showed significant
decreases in total wake time and an increase in total nonrapid eye movement sleep (52). This
method of aromatherapy may be useful in individuals with difficulty maintaining sleep, but
human replication of this study is lacking.
Aromatherapy is not recommended in critically ill patients due to the unclear research on
safety and efficacy in this population (53). Contraindications to the use of aromatherapy include
pregnancy, recent surgery, thrombosis, fractures/wounds, and some medications (54).
SUMMARY
There are a wide variety of “other” types of nonpharmacological treatments used to treat
insomnia, not including stimulus control, sleep restriction, and cognitive therapy (1,2), which
are covered elsewhere in this volume. Progressive muscle relaxation and paradoxical intention
both have significant empirical backing, and have been listed as “efficacious” by the Standards
of Practice Committee of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine, while biofeedback has
c26 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 13:56 Char Count=
good empirical backing and is listed as “probably efficacious” (3). Other forms of relaxation
therapy, yoga, bright light therapy, exercise, acupuncture, massage, and aromatherapy, all show
some promise as treatments for insomnia, but do not have sufficient empirical support to
be recommended as treatments. Future studies are needed to more effectively evaluate these
alternate treatments of insomnia.
APPENDIX A
RELAXATION PROCEDURE
Please close your eyes and get as comfortable as possible. Keep your eyes closed throughout the
procedure and listen to my instructions. (If legs or arms are crossed ask them to uncross them.)
I am going to help you achieve a deeper level of relaxation with the following procedures. Most
people find this is an enjoyable experience. It is not hypnosis. You will not lose consciousness
and will not lose control.
I am going to ask you to tense different muscles of your body. When I do, I want you to
focus all your attention on those muscles until I say, “relax”. As soon as I say, “relax” I want you
to relax muscles immediately. Throughout the tensing and relaxing phases it is very important
for you to focus all of your attention on the sensation coming from your muscles. It is also
important to only tense the one muscle group at a time while leaving the others as relaxed as
possible. Even if this means you cannot fully tense the target muscle group.
FOREHEAD
This time when I say “now”, I want you to tense the muscles of your forehead by raising your
eyebrows as high as they will go and wrinkling your forehead. “NOW” Keep your muscles
tight. . . I want you to feel the strain and tension. . . “RELAX” Relax immediately. . . Just give
up control of the muscles. . . Smooth out the muscles on your forehead letting all tension slip
away. . . Feel the muscles relax and become loose and limp. . . The more carefully you focus your
attention on calmness and tranquility, the greater the relaxation effect you will enjoy. . .
(Relax phase should take 45 seconds).
MOUTH
This time when I say “now”, I want you to tense the lower part of your face by pursing your lips,
pressing your teeth together and pressing your tongue against the roof of your mouth. “NOW”
Keep the muscles tight. . . The muscles are working very hard. “RELAX.” Relax immediately
and completely. . . Let your teeth part, and let all the muscles in your jaw and around your
mouth relax. . . Let the tension in those muscles melt away. . . Let your muscles go loose and
limp. . . Soft and calm. . .
(Relax phase should take 45 seconds).
RIGHT BICEP
This time when I say “NOW,” go ahead and tense the bicep of your right arm, by bending your
arm at the elbow and flexing. Remember, I want you to try to keep your hand and forearm
relaxed, as well as your shoulder. Make sure not to make a fist with your hand. “NOW”. Keep
it tight. . . feel the strain. . . feel the tension. (wait 7 seconds) “RELAX.” Relax completely and
immediately. . .
Think about how relaxed your muscles feel. . . Imagine the tightness and pain flowing out
of your bicep. . . Let your muscles go loose and limp. . . Soft and calm. . .
(relax phase should take 45 seconds).
LEFT HAND AND FOREARM (see RIGHT HAND AND FOREARM above)
RIGHT CALF
When I say “now”, tense your right calf by pointing your foot and toes forward. Don’t strain
too hard, this muscle has a tendency to cramp. “NOW” Tighten the muscle. . . (only 3 seconds
here). “RELAX” Relax completely. . . Focus on the stillness. . . Just give up control of the muscles
and then let them lie there quietly. . . Compare in your mind the feeling of tension you were
feeling just a few minutes ago to the restful feeling that is now gradually emerging. . . Let
the comfortable feelings of tranquility grow deeper and deeper. . .deeper and deeper. Feel the
peaceful. . . calm sensations. (Relax phase should take 45 seconds).
LEFT CALF (see RIGHT CALF above) (tense for only 3 seconds)
REFERENCES
1. Chesson AL Jr, Anderson WM, Littner M, et al. Practice parameters for the nonpharmacologic treat-
ment of chronic insomnia. An American Academy of Sleep Medicine report. Standards of Practice
Committee of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine. Sleep 1999; 22(8):1128–1133.
2. Morin CM, Hauri PJ, Espie CA, et al. Nonpharmacologic treatment of chronic insomnia. An American
Academy of Sleep Medicine review. Sleep 1999; 22(8):1134–1156.
3. Morgenthaler T, Kramer M, Alessi C, et al. Practice parameters for the psychological and behav-
ioral treatment of insomnia: An update. An american academy of sleep medicine report. Sleep 2006;
29(11):1415–1419.
c26 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 13:56 Char Count=
4. Morin CM, Culbert JP, Schwartz SM. Nonpharmacological interventions for insomnia: A meta-analysis
of treatment efficacy. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151(8):1172–1180.
5. Murtagh DR, Greenwood KM. Identifying effective psychological treatments for insomnia: A meta-
analysis. J Consult Clin Psychol 1995; 63(1):79–89.
6. Jacobson E. Progressive relaxation. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press, 1929.
7. Wolpe J. Psychotherapy by reciprocal inhibition. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press, 1958.
8. Jacobson E. Progressive relaxation; a physiological and clinical investigation of muscular states and
their significance in psychology and medical practice. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press,
1929.
9. Lichstein KL. Clinical relaxation strategies. New York, NY: Wiley, 1988.
10. Morin CM, Espie CA. Insomnia : A clinical guide to assessment and treatment. New York, NY: Kluwer
Academic/Plenum Publishers, 2003.
11. Smith JC. Relaxation, meditation and mindfulness. New York, NY: Springer Pub Co, 2005.
12. Hammond DC. What is neurofeedback? J Neurother 2007; 10(4):25–36.
13. Bell JS. The use of EEG theta biofeedback in the treatment of a patient with sleep-onset insomnia.
Biofeedback Self Regul 1979; 4(3):229–236.
14. Hauri PJ, Percy L, Hellekson C, et al. The treatment of psychophysiologic insomnia with biofeedback:
A replication study. Biofeedback self Regul 1982; 7(2):223–235.
15. Shannahoff-Khalsa DS. Kundalini Yoga Meditation Techniques for the Treatment of Obsessive-
Compulsive and OC Spectrum Disorders. Brief Treat Crisis Interv 2003; 3(3):369–382.
16. Gooneratne NS. Complementary and alternative medicine for sleep disturbances in older adults. Clin
Geriatr Med 2008; 24(1):121–138, viii.
17. Chen KM, Tseng WS. Pilot-testing the effects of a newly-developed silver yoga exercise program for
female seniors. J Nurs Res 2008; 16(1):37–46.
18. Khalsa SB. Treatment of chronic insomnia with yoga: A preliminary study with sleep-wake diaries.
Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback 2004; 29(4):269–278.
19. Rajput V, Bromley SM. Chronic insomnia: A practical review. Am Fam Physician 1999; 60(5):1431–1438,
discussion 41–42.
20. Sack RL, Auckley D, Auger RR, et al. Circadian rhythm sleep disorders: Part II, advanced sleep phase
disorder, delayed sleep phase disorder, free-running disorder, and irregular sleep-wake rhythm: An
American academy of sleep medicine review. Sleep 2007; 30(11):1484–1501.
21. Lingjaerde O, Bratlid T, Hansen T. Insomnia during the “dark period” in northern Norway. An
explorative, controlled trial with light treatment. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1985; 71(5):506–512.
22. Campbell SS, Dawson D, Anderson MW. Alleviation of sleep maintenance insomnia with timed
exposure to bright light. J Am Geriatr Soc 1993; 41(8):829–836.
23. Suhner AG, Murphy PJ, Campbell SS. Failure of timed bright light exposure to alleviate age-related
sleep maintenance insomnia. J Am Geriatr Soc 2002; 50(4):617–623.
24. Friedman L, Zeitzer JM, Kushida C, et al. Scheduled bright light for treatment of insomnia in older
adults. J Am Geriatr Soc 2009; 57(3):441–452.
25. Terman M, Terman JS. Bright light therapy: Side effects and benefits across the symptom spectrum. J
Clin Psychiatry 1999; 60(11):799–808, quiz 9.
26. Fletcher GF, Balady G, Blair SN, et al. Statement on exercise: Benefits and recommendations for
physical activity programs for all Americans. A statement for health professionals by the Committee on
Exercise and Cardiac Rehabilitation of the Council on Clinical Cardiology, American Heart Association.
Circulation 1996; 94(4):857–862.
27. Horne JA, Moore VJ. Sleep EEG effects of exercise with and without additional body cooling. Elec-
troencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1985; 60(1):33–38.
28. Horne JA, Staff LH. Exercise and sleep: Body-heating effects. Sleep 1983; 6(1):36–46.
29. Youngstedt SD, O’Connor PJ, Dishman RK. The effects of acute exercise on sleep: A quantitative
synthesis. Sleep 1997; 20(3):203–214.
30. Singh NA, Clements KM, Fiatarone MA. A randomized controlled trial of the effect of exercise on
sleep. Sleep 1997; 20(2):95–101.
31. King AC, Oman RF, Brassington GS, et al. Moderate-intensity exercise and self-rated quality of sleep
in older adults. A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 1997; 277(1):32–37.
32. Morgan K. Daytime activity and risk factors for late-life insomnia. J Sleep Res 2003; 12(3):231–238.
33. Sherrill DL, Kotchou K, Quan SF. Association of physical activity and human sleep disorders. Arch
Intern Med 1998; 158(17):1894–1898.
34. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF III, Monk TH, et al. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: A new instrument
for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res 1989; 28(2):193–213.
35. Tanaka H, Taira K, Arakawa M, et al. Effects of short nap and exercise on elderly people having
difficulty in sleeping. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2001; 55(3):173–174.
c26 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 13:56 Char Count=
36. Stux G, Pomeranz B. Acupuncture textbook and atlas with 98 figures and an acupuncture selector.
Berlin, Germany: Springer-Verlag, 1987.
37. Baldry P. Acupuncture, Trigger Points, and Musculoskeletal Pain: A Scientific Approach to Acupunc-
ture for use by Doctors and Physiotherapists in the Diagnosis and Management of Myofascial Trigger
Point Pain, 2nd ed. Edinburgh, New York, NY: Churchill Livingstone, 1993.
38. Spence DW, Kayumov L, Chen A, et al. Acupuncture increases nocturnal melatonin secretion and
reduces insomnia and anxiety: A preliminary report. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2004; 16(1):
19–28.
39. Taylor DJ, Lichstein KL, Durrence HH. Insomnia as a Health Risk Factor. Behav Sleep Med 2003;
1(4):227–247.
40. Taylor DJ, Lichstein KL, Durrence HH, et al. Epidemiology of insomnia, depression, and anxiety. Sleep
2005; 28(11):1457–1464.
41. Phillips KD, Skelton WD. Effects of individualized acupuncture on sleep quality in HIV disease. J
Assoc Nurses AIDS Care 2001; 12(1):27–39.
42. Sommers E, Porter K, DeGurski S. Providers of complementary and alternative health services in
Boston respond to September 11. Am J Public Health 2002; 92(10):1597–1598.
43. Vickers A, Goyal N, Harland R, et al. Do certain countries produce only positive results? A systematic
review of controlled trials. Control Clin Trials 1998; 19(2):159–166.
44. Soden K, Vincent K, Craske S, et al. A randomized controlled trial of aromatherapy massage in a
hospice setting. Palliat Med 2004; 18(2):87–92.
45. Agarwal KN, Gupta A, Pushkarna R, et al. Effects of massage & use of oil on growth, blood flow &
sleep pattern in infants. Indian J Med Res 2000; 112:212–217.
46. Zollman C, Vickers A. ABC of complementary medicine. BMJ 2000; 319(7216):1050–1053.
47. Perry N, Perry E. Aromatherapy in the management of psychiatric disorders: Clinical and neurophar-
macological perspectives. CNS Drugs 2006; 20(4):257–280.
48. Lewith GT, Godfrey AD, Prescott P. A single-blinded, randomized pilot study evaluating the aroma
of Lavandula augustifolia as a treatment for mild insomnia. J Altern Complement Med 2005; 11(4):
631–637.
49. Williams TI. Evaluating effects of aromatherapy massage on sleep in children with autism: A pilot
study. Evid Based Complement Alternat Med 2006; 3(3):373–377.
50. Lee IS, Lee GJ. Effects of lavender aromatherapy on insomnia and depression in women college
students. Taehan Kanho Hakhoe Chi 2006; 36(1):136–143.
51. Goel N, Kim H, Lao RP. An olfactory stimulus modifies nighttime sleep in young men and women.
Chronobiol Int 2005; 22(5):889–904.
52. Ohmori A, Shinomiya K, Utsu Y, et al. Effect of santalol on the sleep-wake cycle in sleep-disturbed
rats. Nihon Shinkei Seishin Yakurigaku Zasshi 2007; 27(4):167–171.
53. Richards K, Nagel C, Markie M, et al. Use of complementary and alternative therapies to promote
sleep in critically ill patients. Crit Care Nurs Clin North Am 2003; 15(3):329–340.
54. Long L, Huntley A, Ernst E. Which complementary and alternative therapies benefit which conditions?
A survey of the opinions of 223 professional organizations. Complement Ther Med 2001; 9(3):178–185.
c27 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:43 Char Count=
Jason Ellis
Northumbria Centre for Sleep Research, School of Psychology and Sports Science, Northumbria University,
Newcastle upon Tyne, U.K.
INTRODUCTION
Cognitive therapy is a generic term, referring to a broad set of therapeutic techniques designed
to address the maladaptive thought processes associated with a specific disease or disorder.
As the term cognition encompasses all aspects of thinking (i.e., perception, attention, memory,
problem solving, attitudes, beliefs, attributions, and expectations), the breadth and range of
cognitive therapies is extensive, and most certainly has not yet been exhausted. It should
also be acknowledged that even predominantly behavioral interventions (Chapter 24) should
be considered within a cognitive context, in that, behavior is often determined, and as such
challenged and changed, through cognitive processes. However, the aim of this chapter is to
examine the contribution of cognitive therapies to the management of insomnia, and therefore
the focus will be on interventions that are predominantly cognitively orientated.
Table 1 Cognitive Therapies for Insomnia and their Proposed Action Upon Cognitive Attentional Mechanisms
(After 6)
against thought content that would otherwise occupy quiet wakefulness; and imagery training
and the lesser known articulatory suppression technique distract or block unwanted intrusive
thinking.
Finally, we suggest that there are useful passive-paradoxical techniques. Unlike the cognitive
therapies, which at some level emphasize control or at least active management of thoughts and
behaviors, paradoxical intention is a disengagement method and mindfulness an acceptance-
based approach. Indeed paradox might even be regarded as the extreme/ultimate end of the
acceptance dimension.
Dysfunctional sleep-related attitudes and beliefs are generally accepted to be a central fea-
ture of insomnia. For example, Morin, Stone, Trinkle et al (10) found that people with insomnia
endorsed more dysfunctional beliefs about sleep compared to controls. However, the extent to
which they mediate the relationship between perceptions of sleep, catastrophic interpretations
about the consequences of poor sleep, and attributions for insomnia is still unclear, but thought
to be significant (11). In addition, it has been shown that the endocrine system reacts negatively
when there is a discrepancy between what is expected and what really exists (12). Although
Eriksen et al’s (12) study related to stress, it has been shown that subjective sleep quantity and
quality are better indicators of insomnia reporting than objective sleep measures (13). Addi-
tionally, researchers have found that irrespective of the actual sleep obtained, those who scored
highly on the Dysfunctional Beliefs and Attitudes to Sleep Scale (DBAS) were also those who
were more likely to complain of disrupted sleep (14,15).
The primary mechanism by which dysfunctional beliefs affect sleep is thought to be
through the relationship between attitudes and sleep-incompatible behaviors. An individual
who believes that they need eight hours of sleep is more likely to reduce their sleep efficiency
by attempting to obtain sleep during the day, lying in bed when not asleep, or going to bed
earlier, all of which are sleep incompatible behaviors and are likely to exacerbate the problem.
Dysfunctional beliefs may be particularly pertinent for older adults, whose sleep patterns
can change as a function of normal ageing. Changes in the timing of the circadian rhythm,
as well as structural changes in sleep architecture, can render the belief that eight hours of
consolidated sleep is essential for normal functioning as even more unrealistic (16) and the
dissonance between this expectation and perceived reality becomes greater and more anxiety
provoking.
The notion of general sleep education falls within the remit of cognitive restructuring in
so far as its intention is to provide information about a) what constitutes ‘normal’, or more
likely, ‘typical’ sleep over the life span and the intra-individual differences therein, b) what
sleep disruption is, c) an exploration of the factors which exacerbate or increase the likelihood of
sleep disruption, and d) an exploration of the factors which increase the likelihood of good sleep
occurring. In this instance, exploring the individuals’ constructs of ‘sleep’ and ‘sleep disorder’
can offer a platform for identifying sleep-related dissonance and provide the beginnings of an
intervention aimed at reducing this dissonance.
COGNITIVE CONTROL
It is well documented that intrusive, unwanted thoughts and images are a prominent feature of
most psychological disorders (20) and that attempts to suppress intrusive thoughts usually result
in a rebound effect. Clark and Rhyno (21) define intrusive thoughts as ‘any distinct, identifiable
cognitive event that is unwanted, unintended, and recurrent. It interrupts the flow of thoughts, interferes
in task performance, is associated with negative affect, and is difficult to control.’ (p. 4)
The role of intrusive thoughts, and the subsequent use of thought control strategies to
deal with them in insomnia, is well documented (22–24). In addition, when the suppression of
presleep cognitive activity has been experimentally manipulated, those told to suppress had
longer sleep-onset latencies than nonsupressors (25).
c27 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:43 Char Count=
You may find this technique particularly useful for thoughts that have to do with the past day and planning for
the following day. The aim is to put the day to bed, along with your plans for the next day . . . so that you can
get to sleep!. If you can manage to stop the thinking you usually do in bed, before it happens, then you should
sleep better.
To put the day to rest :-
1. Set aside 20 minutes in the early evening (say around 7 pm.) and sit down with a pen and a notebook
2. Think of what has happened during the day, how it has gone, and how you feel about it – evaluate things
3. Write down anything you need to do on a ‘to do’ list, and any steps that you can take to complete any
loose ends
4. Try to use your 20 minutes to leave you feeling more organized and in control and close the notebook
when you are done
5. When it comes to bedtime remind yourself that you have already dealt with things when they come to your
mind
6. If new thoughts come up note them down on a piece of paper at your bedside, to be dealt with the next
day
Source: From Ref. 33, pp. 97–98.
Harvey (26) examined the individual contributions of the different forms of thought
control in insomnia; namely distraction (focusing away from the thought), punishment (self-
chastisement for the thought), reappraisal (logical interpretation of the thought), worry (wor-
rying about the thought), and social control (seeking support about the thought’s validity). She
found that poor sleepers were more likely to use distraction, reappraisal, and worry, as opposed
to punishment and social control. However, to explain paradoxical effects, Harvey makes a
distinction between suppression and replacement when distraction is used as a thought control
strategy, believing suppression to be a negatively toned thought control strategy and replace-
ment to be a positive one.
Perhaps surprisingly, Ellis and Cropley (27) found that distraction (not separated into
suppression and replacement) was related to not developing chronic insomnia, and that the main
strategies used by people with chronic insomnia were worry and punishment, with punishment
being associated with a longer reported duration of insomnia. This pattern of thought control
use conforms to findings from research on other anxiety related disorders (28). Further support
for the role of thought control in the maintenance of insomnia comes from Watts, Coyle and
East (29). They found the presleep cognitions of nonworrying people with insomnia focused on
not sleeping, whereas worrying insomniacs focused on a diversity of topics. In a more recent
study, intrusive thoughts and avoidance behaviors were not strongly related to subjective sleep
quality, only objective sleep latency (30) suggesting that the outcome measures used may be an
important factor in determining the importance of cognitive intrusions. Similarly, Bonnet and
Arand (31) found no increases in subjective anxiety after a period of poor sleep in 10 patients
with insomnia. These findings together suggest that thought control does play a role in insomnia
but may not lead to, or exacerbate further, cognitive activity directly.
The only specific cognitive control procedure used in the treatment of insomnia was
initially outlined in a case study first described some 20 years ago (32). It is really an extension
of the idea of stimulus control (Chapter 24), but recognizes that it may be primarily thoughts
and worries that seem to be incompatible with successful sleep. The great majority of people
with insomnia report excessive mental arousal in bed. They complain of difficulty in emptying
their minds and of racing thoughts. Cognitive control comprises a simple set of procedures
to remove mental activity from the bed and bedroom environment, or at least to reduce the
influence of cognitive activity upon sleep. The instructional set is provided in Table 3.
ARTICULATORY SUPPRESSION
As the review of cognitive factors in the previous section might suggest ‘thought-blocking’
techniques have obvious intuitive appeal in insomnia. Although, there is some evidence that
thought suppression may be a counterproductive strategy employed by poor sleepers (37),
Morin & Espie (33) report that there is one form of thought-blocking that might be recommended.
Articulatory suppression is a technique widely used in studies of working memory. The
phonological component of the central executive is referred to as the articulatory loop, which
serves to hold in store the verbal elements required in any cognitive task. Levey et al. (38) applied
articulatory suppression techniques to the treatment of insomnia. They thought that by blocking
up this short-term store with semantically meaningless phonemes no other mental information
would be processed. They presented an interesting case series supporting this contention,
particularly for sleep maintenance insomnia (i.e. using the technique at any wakening from
sleep). The instructions for articulatory suppression are summarized in Table 4.
IMAGERY TRAINING
Harvey (39) suggests that unwanted cognitive activity can be visual as well as verbal, and
controlling or replacing these images, using distraction techniques, has been shown to result
in reduced arousal in several anxiety-related disorders (40–42). It has been demonstrated that
poor sleepers report more negative images than normal sleepers and the imagery of poor
sleepers tends to be catastrophic (envisioning the worst possible outcome), in that it is usually
distressing and related to physical sensation (43). They also showed that levels of visual imagery
in poor sleepers related to an increased subjective Sleep Onset Latency (43). Using replacement
distraction visual imagery when faced with a visually catastrophic stimulus has been shown to
be effective in samples of poor sleepers (44,45).
The aim of imagery training is to block or distract the individual from intrusive and
preoccupying sleep-related thoughts, using visualization techniques. Imagery techniques are
useful for some but not all patients, probably because there are individual differences in the
ability to visualize. First, there is need to establish the patient’s ability to visualize, and their
degree of comfort with the process. This can be achieved by asking them to close their eyes and
try to picture some objects (a boat under sail in a gentle breeze, a clock face with a ticking second
hand). Second, it is better to get patients to decide upon an imaginable scene for them to use
rather than to leave it literally to their imagination at the time. For example, if it is something
like walking through a favorite piece of parkland and gardens, they should prepare the scene
and the sequence in advance, so it is like ‘rolling the tape’ when it comes to using the imagery.
Finally, practice is crucial to train the imagery if it is going to be useful (33).
thoughts over time or whether this is artefact remains to be seen. Commonly imagery is used
alongside (other) relaxation techniques in clinical practice, and is used in some multicomponent
CBT-I interventions.
PARADOXICAL INTENTION
Although it could be argued that paradoxical intention is not exclusively a cognitive therapy,
in that it was not developed within a cognitive framework being more grounded in learning
theory, throughout its refinement it has applied cognition to its existing elements.
The term paradox was termed long before the intention part was introduced by Frankl
(54,55). Dubois (56), for example, suggested humor ‘paradoxically’ should be advocated for
patients to help them in dealing with their symptoms. Furthermore, Dunlap (57,58) suggested
one method that could be utilized to break a bad habit was to repetitively perform that particular
habit. Frankl (59), who was also a proponent of humor in the therapeutic encounter, refined these
early ideas, suggesting that in order to break vicious cycles of anticipatory anxiety surrounding
a thought or behavior, individuals must focus in on that particular problem or perform that
particular behavior repeatedly. Alongside paradoxical intention Frankl also outlined another
therapeutic technique; dereflection. Because Frankl believed the roots of most psychological
problems were caused by an overemphasis on the self, shifting attention away from the self
might also be used to alleviate the problem (an early version of thought blocking/distraction).
The main advocate of paradoxical intention, since its inception, has been Ascher, who
with various colleagues demonstrated its effectiveness in areas as diverse as urinary retention
and agoraphobia (60,61). In each case, the use of paradoxical intention has lead to a reduction
in symptom reporting, and in some cases, increases in self-efficacy.
c27 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:43 Char Count=
Table 5 Suggested Rationale and Instructions for Using Paradoxical Intention Therapy in Insomnia
If you can’t get to sleep it might seem reasonable to ask someone who is a good sleeper how he/ she
manages it. What do you think they would say? Something like I just fall asleep . . . it just happens . . . I don’t
do anything really? You might think this is not very helpful, but the secret is right there – they do precisely
nothing!
Sleep is a natural process which happens involuntarily. The good sleeper doesn’t make it happen, or have
some kind of method that you don’t know about it. You are the one with all the methods and tactics, and none
of them works! To become a good sleeper you need to learn to abandon all your efforts to sleep because they
simply get in the way of the natural process. They make you too self-conscious about your sleep and about
your sleep failures. You know how sometimes you lie awake for ages or toss and turn until it’s getting close to
morning time? And you feel some relief as you think that soon you can get up? Why do you sometimes fall
asleep at that point? That’s because you give up trying then and you give up being concerned. How about
having that as a general approach. Try this:-
1. When you are in bed lie in a comfortable position and put the light out
2. In the darkened room, keep your eyes open, and try to keep them open ‘just for just a little while longer’.
That’s your catch phrase
3. As time goes by congratulate yourself on staying awake but relaxed
4. Remind yourself not to try to sleep but to let sleep overtake you, as you gently try to resist it
5. Keep this mind set going as long as you can, and if you get worried at staying awake remind yourself
that that is the general idea, so you are succeeding
6. Don’t actively prevent sleep by trying to rouse yourself. Be like the good sleeper, let sleep come to you
Source: From Ref. 33.
Within the framework of insomnia, paradoxical intention aims to reduce the anxiety and
frustration often experienced by people with insomnia at sleep-onset by recommending that
the individual do the opposite of their normal behavior (Table 5). In this case, to lie down
with their eyes open and attempt to stay awake for as long as possible. This strategy operates
on the premise that, in insomnia, sleep onset is prevented because the patient is attempting
to place sleep under voluntary control, resulting in arousal of the autonomic nervous system.
In other words, the more effortful monitoring that is employed to sleep, the more likely it is
to keep the individual awake, creating performance anxiety which then leads to catastrophic
interpretations about daytime functioning, fuelling a vicious cycle of performance anxiety.
Evidence for this comes from the use of measures such as the Glasgow Sleep Effort Scale,
Sleep Associated Monitoring Index and Sleep Preoccupation Scale which, independently, have
identified people with insomnia as being more effortful in their attempts to sleep, spending
more time self-monitoring, and becoming increasingly anxious and sleep preoccupied when
these efforts fail (62–64). As such, sleep may be facilitated by breaking the pathway between
attention to sleep-related cues – increased intention to sleep – and increased effort to sleep (4).
when attempting to determine the relative value of cognitive therapy within CBT-I, there is
undoubtedly very strong evidence that CBT-I as a “package” is an effective treatment for
persistent insomnia. Cognitive components are perhaps integral to this success.
To date, there has been only one study that has formally examined cognitive therapy as a
stand-alone treatment (76). Based on Harvey’s (2) cognitive model, and using a mainly Socratic
questioning approach, this therapy involved three phases (case formulation, personalized exper-
iments with guided discovery, and planning for continued success whilst preventing against
relapse). The therapeutic aim was to reverse the five main cognitive processes thought to main-
tain insomnia; namely, worry and rumination, attentional bias and monitoring for sleep-related
threat, unhelpful beliefs about sleep, the use of safety behaviors that maintain unhelpful beliefs,
and misperception of sleep and daytime deficits, (77). In terms of efficacy, significant improve-
ments in sleep diary measures as well as significant reductions in anxiety and depression scores
were obtained and maintained at 12 months follow up.
Although the lack of a control group or comparable intervention and the small sample size
limit the reliability and generalizability of the Harvey et al. (76) study, it represents emerging
evidence that cognitive therapy may prove to be effective for persistent insomnia. Whether it
would outperform CBT-I as a standard therapy, however, remains questionable.
Another welcome development in the literature has been the examination of cognitive
processes in insomnia beyond the actual intervention itself. Recently, interpersonal aspects
surrounding the patient/therapist interaction have come under scrutiny. For example, one
study has suggested that increased levels of perceived therapeutic alliance may provide additive
efficacy to CBT-I (52). As such it may not be just the intervention itself, but the mode of delivery
that can increase the chances of treatment success.
REFERENCES
1. Perlis ML, Giles DE, Mendelson WB, et al Psychophysiological insomnia: The behavioural model and
a neurocognitive perspective. J Sleep Res 1997; 6:179–188.
2. Harvey AG. A cognitive model of insomnia. Behav Res Ther 2002; 40:869–893.
3. Espie CA. Insomnia: Conceptual issues in the development, persistence, and treatment of sleep disor-
der in adults. Annu Rev Psychol 2002; 53:215–243.
4. Espie CA, Broomfield NM, MacMahon KMA,et al. The attention-intention-effort pathway in the
development of Psychophysiologic Insomnia: An invited theoretical review. Sleep Med Rev 2006;
10:215–245.
5. Perlis ML, et al. Etiology and pathophysiology of insomnia. In: Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement WC, eds.
Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine, 4th ed. Philadelphia, PA: WB Saunders, 2005.
6. Espie CA. The Psychological Treatment of Insomnia. England, Chichester: John Wiley and Sons Ltd.,
1991.
7. Beck AT. Depression: Causes and Treatment. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press, 1972.
8. Beck A. Cognitive therapy: Nature and relation to behavior therapy. Behav Ther 1970; 1(2):184–200.
9. Ellis A. Rational psychotherapy and individual psychology. J Individ Psychol 1957; 13:38–44.
10. Morin CM, Stone J, Trinkle D, et al. Dysfunctional Attitudes about Sleep Among Older Adults With
and Without Insomnia Complaints. Psychol Aging 1993; 8(3):463–467.
c27 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:43 Char Count=
11. Ellis J, Hampson SE, Cropley M. The role of sleep preoccupation in attributions for poor sleep. Sleep
Med 2007; 8(3):277–280.
12. Eriksen HR, Olff M, Murison R, et al. The Time Dimension in Stress Responses: Relevance for Survival
and Health. Psychiatry Res 1999; 85(1):39–50.
13. Riedel BW, Lichstein KL. Objective Sleep Measures and Subjective Sleep Satisfaction: How do Older
Adults with Insomnia Define a Good Night’s Sleep. Psychol Ageing 1998; 13(1):159–163.
14. Edinger JD, Glenn DM, Bastian LA, et al. Slow-wave sleep and waking cognitive performance II:
Findings among middle-aged adults with and without insomnia complaints. Physiol Behav 2000;
70(1–2):127–134.
15. Edinger JD, Glenn DM, Bastian LA, et al. The roles of dysfunctional cognitions and other person
factors in mediating insomnia complaints. Sleep 21:S144.
16. Lichstein KL, Fischer SM. Insomnia: In: Hersen M, Bellack AS, eds. Handbook of Clinical Behavior
Therapy with Adults. New York, NY: Plenum Press, 1985:319–352.
17. Edinger JD, Wohlgemuth WK, Radtke RA, et al. Does Cognitive Behaviour Therapy alter dysfunctional
beliefs about sleep? Sleep 2001; 24(5):591–599.
18. Espie CA, Inglis SJ, Harvey L, et al. Insomniacs’ attributions: Psychometric properties of the Dysfunc-
tional Beliefs and Attitudes about Sleep scale and the Sleep Disturbance Questionnaire. J Psychosom
Res 2000; 48:141–148.
19. Morin CM, Blais F, Savard J. Are Changes in Beliefs and Attitudes about Sleep Related to Sleep
Improvements in the Treatment of Insomnia. Behav Res Ther 2002; 40:741–752.
20. Wenzlaff RM, Wegner DM, Roper D. Depression and mental control: The resurgence of unwanted
negative thoughts. J Pers Soc Psychol 1988; 55:882–892.
21. Clark DA. Rhyno S. Unwanted intrusive thoughts in nonclinical individuals. In: Clark’s DA, eds.
Intrusive Thoughts in Clinical Disorders: Theory, research, and Treatment. New York, NY: Guilford
Press, 2004:1–29.
22. Borkovec TD. Insomnia. J Consult Clin Psychol 1982; 50:880–895.
23. Gendron L, Blais FC, Morin CM. Cognitive activity among insomniac patients. Sleep 1998; 21:130.
24. Lichstein KL, Rosenthal TL. Insomniacs’ perceptions of cognitive versus somatic determinants of sleep
disturbance. J Abnorm Psychol 1980; 89(1):105–107.
25. Gross RT, Borkovec TD. Effects of Cognitive Intrusion Manipulation on the Sleep-Onset Latency of
Good Sleepers. Behav Ther 1982; 13:112–116.
26. Harvey AG. I Can’t Sleep, My Mind is Racing! An Investigation of Strategies of Thought Control in
Insomnia. Behav Cogn Psychother 2001; 29:3–11.
27. Ellis J, Cropley M. An examination of thought control strategies employed by acute and chronic
insomniacs. Sleep Med 2002; 3(5):393–400.
28. Warda G, Bryant RA. Cognitive Bias in Acute Stress Disorder. Behav Res Ther 1998; 36(12):1177–
1184.
29. Watts FN, Coyle K, East MP. The Contribution of Worry to Insomnia. Br J Clin Psychol 1994; 33:211–220.
30. Hall M, Buysse DJ, Dew MA, et al. Intrusive Thoughts and Avoidance Behaviours are Associated with
Sleep Disturbances in Bereavement-Related Depression. Depress Anxiety 1997; 6(3):106–112.
31. Bonnet MH, Arand DL. The Consequences of a Week of Insomnia II: Patients with Insomnia. Sleep
1998; 21(4):359–368.
32. Espie CA, Lindsay WR. Cognitive strategies for the management of severe sleep-maintenance insom-
nia: A preliminary investigation. Behav Psychother 1987; 15:388–339.
33. Morin CM, Espie CA. Insomnia: A Clinical Guide to Assessment and Treatment. New York, NY:
Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers, 2003.
34. Morgenthaler T, Kramer M, Alessi C, et al. Practice parameters for the psychological and behav-
ioral treatment of insomnia: An update. An american academy of sleep medicine report. Sleep 2006;
29(11):1415–1419.
35. Morin CM, Bootzin RR, Buysse DJ, et al. Psychological and behavioural treatment of insomnia. Update
of the recent evidence (1998–2004) prepared by a Task Force of the American Academy of Sleep
Medicine. Sleep 2006; 29:1398–1414
36. Morin CM, Hauri P, Espie CA, et al. Nonpharmacologic treatment of chronic insomnia: An American
Academy of Sleep Medicine Review. Sleep 1999; 22:1134–1156.
37. Harvey AG. Attempted suppression of pre-sleep cognitive activity in insomnia. Cognit Res Ther 2001.
38. Levey AB, Aldaz JA, Watts FN, et al. Articulatory suppression and the treatment of insomnia. Behav
Res Ther 1991; 29:85–89.
39. Harvey AG. Pre-sleep cognitive activity: A comparison of sleep-onset insomniacs and good sleepers.
Br J Clin Psychol 2000; 39:275–286.
40. Borkovec TD, Hu S. The effect of worry on cardiovascular response to phobic imagery. Behav Res Ther
1990; 28(1):69–73.
c27 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:43 Char Count=
41. Borkovec TD, Ray WJ, Stober J. Worry: A cognitive phenomenon intimately linked to affective, phys-
iological, and interpersonal behavioral processes. Cognit Ther Res 1998; 22:561–576.
42. Freeston MH, Dugas MJ, Ladouceur R. Thoughts, Images, Worry and Anxiety. Behav Res Ther 1996;
20:265–273.
43. Nelson J, Harvey AG. Pre-sleep imagery under the microscope: A comparison of patients with insom-
nia and good sleepers. Behav Res Ther 2003; 41(3):273–284.
44. Harvey AG, Payne S. The management of unwanted pre-sleep thoughts in insomnia: Distraction with
imagery versus general distraction. Behav Res Ther 2002; 40(3):267–277.
45. Means MK, Lichstein KL, Epperson MT, et al. Relaxation Therapy for Insomnia: Nighttime and Day
Time Effects. Behav Res Ther 2000; 38:665–678.
46. Morin CM, Azrin NH. Stimulus Control and Imagery Training in Treating Sleep-Maintenance Insom-
nia. J Consult Clin Psychol 1987; 55(2):260–262.
47. Morin CM, Azrin NH. Behavioral and cognitive treatments of geriatric insomnia. J Consult Clin
Psychol 1988; 56:748–753.
48. Kabat-Zinn J. Full Catastrophe Living: Using the wisdom of your body and mind to face stress, pain
and illness. London: Piatkus, 1990.
49. Wicklow A, Espie CA. Intrusive Thoughts and their Relationship to Actigraphic Measurement of
Sleep: Towards a Cognitive Model of Insomnia. Behav Res Ther 2000; 38:679–693.
50. Lundh L. The role of acceptance and mindfulness in the treatment of insomnia. J Cogn Psychother
2005; 19:29–39.
51. Lundh L, Hindmarsh I. Can meta-cognitive observation be used in the treatment of insomnia? A pilot
study of cognitive-emotional self-observation task. Behav Cogn Psychother 2002; 30:233–236.
52. Ong J, Shapiro C, Manber R. Combining mindfulness meditation with cognitive-behavior therapy for
insomnia: A treatment-development study. Behav Ther 2008; 39(2):171–182.
53. Yook K, Lee SH, Ryu M, et al. Usefulness of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for treating insomnia
in patients with anxiety disorders: A pilot study. J Nerv Ment Dis 2008; 196(6):501–503.
54. Frankl VE. The Doctor and the Soul. Berlin: Knopf, 1955.
55. Frankl VE. Paradoxical intention: A logotherapueutic technique. Am J Psychother 1960; 14:520–
535.
56. DuBois (1908) Cited In: Frankle VE, DuBois JM, eds On the Theory and Therapy of Mental Disorders:
An Introduction to Logotherapy and Existential Analysis op.cit. London: Routledge, 2004:
57. Dunlap K. Repetition in the breaking of habits. Sci Mon 1930; 30:66–70.
58. Dunlap K. Habits Their Making and Unmaking. New York, NY: Liveright, 1932.
59. Frankl VE. (1963). (I. Lasch, Trans.) Man’s Search for Meaning: An Introduction to Logotherapy. New York:
Washington Square Press.
60. Ascher LM. Paradoxical intention in the treatment of urinary retention. Behav Res Ther 1979; 17(3):267–
270.
61. Turner RM, Ascher LM. Controlled comparison of progressive relaxation, stimulus control, and para-
doxical intention therapies for insomnia. J Consult Clin Psychol 1979; 47(3):500–508.
62. Broomfield NM, Espie CA. Towards a valid, reliable measure of sleep effort. J Sleep Res 2005; 14(4):401–
407.
63. Ellis J, Mitchell K, Hogh H. Sleep preoccupation in poor sleepers: Psychometric properties of the Sleep
Preoccupation Scale. J Psychosom Res 2007; 63(6):579–585.
64. Neitzert Semler C, Harvey AG. Monitoring for sleep-related threat: A pilot study of the Sleep Associ-
ated Monitoring Index. Psychosom Med 2004; 66:242–250.
65. Ascher LM, Efran JS. Use of paradoxical intention in a behavioral program for sleep onset insomnia. J
Consult Clin Psychol 1978; 46(3):547–550.
66. Espie CA, Lindsay WR. Paradoxical intention in the treatment of chronic insomnia: six case studies
illustrating variability in therapeutic response. Behav Res Ther 1985; 23(6):703–709.
67. Ladouceur R, Gros-Louis Y, Paradoxical intention vs stimulus control in the treatment of severe
insomnia. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry.1986; 17(4):267–269.
68. Ott BD, Levine BA, Ascher LM. Manipulating the explicit demand of paradoxical intention instruc-
tions. Behav Psychother 1983; 11:25–35.
69. Relinger H, Bornstein PH. Treatment of Sleep Onset Insomnia by Paradoxical Instruction. Behav Modif
1979; 3(2):203–222.
70. Ascher LM, Turner R. Paradoxical intention and insomnia: An experimental investigation. Behav Res
Ther 1979; 17(4):408–411.
71. Espie CA, Lindsay WR, Brooks DN, et al. A controlled comparative investigation of psychological
treatments for chronic sleep-onset insomnia. Behav Res Ther 1989; 27(1):79–88.
72. Lacks P, Bertelson AD, Gans I, et al. The effectiveness of three behavioural treatments for different
degrees of sleep-onset insomnia. Behav Ther 1983; 14:593–605.
c27 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:43 Char Count=
73. Lacks P, Bertelson AD, Sugerman J, et al. The treatment of sleep-maintenance insomnia with stimulus-
control techniques. Behav Res Ther 1983; 21(3):291–295.
74. Turner RM, Ascher LM. Therapist factor in the treatment of insomnia. Behav Res Ther1982; 20(1):
33–40.
75. Broomfield NM, Espie CA. Initial insomnia and paradoxical intention: An experimental investiga-
tion of putative mechanisms using subjective and actigraphic measurement of sleep. Behav Cogn
Psychother 2003; 31:313–324
76. Harvey AG, Sharpley AL, Ree MJ, et al. An open trial of cognitive therapy for chronic insomnia. Behav
Res Ther 2007; 45(10):2491–2501.
77. Harvey AG. Toward a cognitive theory and therapy for chronic insomnia. J Cogn Psychother 2005;
19:41–59.
78. Belanger L, Savard J, Morin CM. Clinical management of insomnia using cognitive therapy. Behav
Sleep Med 2006; 4(3):179–198.
c28 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:44 Char Count=
Natalie D. Dautovich
Department of Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida, U.S.A.
Joseph M. Dzierzewski
Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida, U.S.A.
INTRODUCTION
The efficacy of cognitive behavioral treatments of insomnia has been well-documented in adults
of all ages (1). A major challenge facing behavioral sleep medicine experts is how to best
disseminate cognitive behavioral treatments to patients in primary care settings. A major barrier
to the routine provision of such interventions in primary care settings is the length of the time
required for treatment. Cognitive behavioral interventions have traditionally been administered
over the course of 6 to 10 sessions lasting 50 to 90 minutes each (1). Another barrier is the common
approach of providing treatment on a one-to-one basis. It is not difficult to see how lengthy
intervention periods and individually administered treatment combine to consume a great deal
of clinician time-–a valuable and often limited resource in primary care settings. As a result,
access to cognitive behavioral treatments for insomnia is frequently limited outside of a few
select academic medical settings and specialized sleep treatment centers. Both of the intervention
approaches presented in this chapter represent methods of treatment administration that offer
potential solutions to these barriers. For example, shortened protocols and group treatment
(treating more than one patient at a time) can make the best use of available healthcare provider
resources by reducing demands on clinician time. Importantly, evidence suggests that these
alternative approaches to administering cognitive behavioral treatment of insomnia can be
adopted without compromising treatment quality. As will be presented in this chapter, research
indicates that both brief and group approaches can be as efficacious in the treatment of insomnia
as traditional cognitive behavioral protocols. Additionally, although the majority of research on
these approaches has examined their efficacy, a smaller body of evidence provides promising
preliminary support for their effectiveness as well. The first half of this chapter provides an
overview of brief interventions, while the second half focuses on group therapy.
BRIEF INTERVENTIONS
A growing number of researchers have examined briefer approaches to insomnia treatment
using cognitive behavioral therapy. The majority of studies have contrasted multicompo-
nent cognitive behavioral approaches to ‘usual care’ treatment (typically consisting of sleep
hygiene/sleep education). The number of sessions examined has ranged from one to five ses-
sions. In a review of psychological and behavioral treatments for insomnia, Morin and colleagues
(2006) described the average number of treatment sessions as 5.7 meetings (1). Consequently,
for the purpose of this chapter, therapies not exceeding five treatment sessions will be reviewed
and designated as ‘brief approaches’.
(SHE). Treatment was primarily administered in two in-person sessions and two telephone ses-
sions. Results indicated that the MBT approach resulted in greater improvements as measured
by sleep diary for sleep-onset latency and sleep efficiency compared to SHE (Table 1). Addition-
ally, the results for MBT had greater clinical significance with 10 participants no longer meeting
the criteria for insomnia after receiving the MBT versus three for the SHE treatment.
The efficacy of four treatment sessions for alleviating sleep complaints was examined in a
sample of older adults with secondary insomnia (10). The treatment package consisted of sleep
hygiene, stimulus control, and relaxation exercises. Compared to the delayed treatment condi-
tion, participants receiving the treatment showed significant improvement at posttreatment for
the sleep diary measures of wake time after sleep onset, sleep efficiency, and sleep quality. The
results appeared durable at three months with improvements maintained for wake time after
sleep onset, sleep efficiency, and sleep quality.
Another study also examined the efficacy of four treatment sessions for treating insomnia
in older adults but compared various components of behavioral treatments (12). One treat-
ment group received a combination of sleep hygiene and relaxation exercises while another
treatment involved sleep hygiene and stimulus control components. There were significant
differences in pre/post scores for the treatment groups compared to the waitlist controls. The
sleep hygiene/relaxation group and the sleep hygiene/stimulus control group significantly
improved on the sleep diary measures of sleep-onset latency, total sleep time, and sleep effi-
ciency compared to the control group. The significant improvement for the two treatment groups
was maintained at follow-up. There were no significant differences between the two treatment
groups suggesting that both approaches were efficacious.
The efficacy of a four session brief treatment approach was assessed in an older adult sam-
ple using one of three treatment conditions (sleep hygiene education, sleep restriction and sleep
hygiene, and a nap restriction condition) (7). The nap restriction condition encouraged partici-
pants to partake in a daily 30 minute nap between 1 PM and 3 PM. Results indicated significant
improvement at posttreatment for the sleep and nap restriction conditions for actigraphically
measured total sleep time and for sleep diary sleep efficiency and time in bed.
Finally, two studies by Perlis and colleagues (38,39) examined the behavioral treatment
for insomnia using a case based approach in which the number of sessions ranged from three to
nine. Due to the variability in the number of sessions, these studies are not reviewed in depth
here.
9:44
wks therapies, cognitive intensive other than CQOLC SOL for CASI control at week 5
therapy, sleep training session insomnia vs. control (14 (decrease of – (PSQI) - PSQI
hygiene (60 min) min) at wk 5 4.5) after 4mo for CASI vs.
– 2nd session: review (one wk after (actigraphy) control,
Char Count=
and rates goal second – significantly d = 1.03 after
attainment (60 min) session) lower SOL 4mths
(decrease of (actigraphy)
No significant 5.4 min) after 2 – SOL d = 0.79
differences mo and higher after 2mths and
between TST (increase TST d = 1.15
intervention and of 1.1hr) after after 4mths for
control for CES-D 4mo for CASI CASI
or CQOLC vs. control vs. control
Chambers & 1–3 in-person – included stimulus – clinical – 69 women, 34 – organic sleep SQAW SOL ↓30 min, postassessment overall small/
Alexander (4) sessions control, sleep psychologist men disorders TST ↑68 min, was conducted at medium to large
Unable to restriction, sleep – age (m = 39.9, – poor physical WASO ↓23 min follow-up (on effect sizes SOL
ascertain hygiene, & cognitive range 19–75) health Secondary average 222 days d = 0.42 TST
intersession restructuring – acute outcomes: later; see results d = 0.75 WASO
interval (typically delivered in psychiatric Significant section for d = 0.57
one 2—3 hr session) conditions improvement in post-assessment
– follow-up sessions – required daytime findings)
(1–2) were used for prescription of sleepiness,
some to monitor anxiolytics, daytime fatigue,
progress and antidepressant alertness during
encourage or neuroleptic
McCRAE ET AL.
the day, & general
compliance med well-being
– no differences in following
treatment outcome treatment
for number of
sessions
c28
IHBK059-Sateia
Edinger, et al. 1, 2, 4, & 8 1st session: sleep – 2 Clinical – 43 women, 43 – pregnancy sleep diaries – 1 and 4 6 month follow-up Overall medium to
9:44
control & – PLM 4 sessions: – TWT min
actigraphy
sleep – primary sleep – ↓52.7 min for d = 0.41 and
restriction disorder other 4 sessions: sleep diary d = 0.80 for
– ↑4.7% for WASO
Char Count=
provided than PI - PSG actigraphy
Intersession sleep time ≥ 2 actigraphy SE – ↑6.1% for
intervals: x higher than actigraphy SE 4 sessions:
ISQ scores – TWT min ↓30.1 – d = 0.60 for
– 2 sessions sleep diary
improved from for actigraphy actigraphy SE
(3 wk) sleep time
baseline to
– 4 sessions posttreatment for Follow-up:
(2 wks) 1,4, & 8 sessions – 1,2, 4 sessions:
– 8 sessions and from baseline – d = 0.92,
(no interval to follow-up for 1 d = 1.00, and
between & 4 sessions d = 1.52 for
sessions) sleep diary SE
– TST d = 0.70,
d = 1.11 and
d = 0.87 for
sleep diary
– TWT d = 0.86,
d = 0.78 and
d = 1.24 for
sleep diary
4 sessions:
– d = 1.39 for
sleep diary
WASO
– d = 0.59 for
actigraphy SE
– TWT d = 0.60
for actigraphy
313
(Continued)
c28
IHBK059-Sateia
314
9:44
length of Therapist
intersession Content of qualifica- Exclusionary Durability of
Study interval sessions tions Population criteria Measures Results Effects Effect Size∗
Char Count=
Edinger & 2 in-person (ACBT) – beginning- 2 women, 18 men – sleep- sleep diary Sleep diaries: 3mo after Insufficient
Sampson (6) sessions – 1st session: level clinical – age (m = 51.0 disruptive med ISQ – ACBT: WASO treatment information to
pamphlet with review of sleep psycholo- ± 13.7) condition SES ↓43 min, SE Sleep diaries: calculate effect
behavioral logs, sleep gist – terminal illness DBAS ↑9%, SQ ↑.3/5 – ACBT: sizes
recommenda- education, – received – other primary – SHC: WASO WASO ↓51
tions & audio stimulus control, training and sleep disorder ↑1min, SE min, SE
cassette with sleep restriction monthly – Axis I disorder ↑1%, SQ no ↑12%, SQ
treatment – 2nd session: supervision – use of change ↑.2/5
guidelines review from 1st hypnotics / – SHC:
provided instructions, Secondary WASO
author alcohol as
2 wk trouble shoot outcome ↑8min, SE
sleep aid
intersession – sessions ∼ 25 measures: ↑3%,
interval min ACBT group SQ.1/5
showed
(SHC) significant Secondary
– 1st session: improvement outcome
review of sleep compared to SCH measures:
logs (no problem group on ISQ, ACBT group
solving), sleep DBAS showed
education, sleep significant
hygiene improvement
– 2nd session: compared to
McCRAE ET AL.
review SCH group on
instructions, ISQ,
trouble shoot sleep-related
– sessions ∼ self-efficacy,
25min DBAS
c28
IHBK059-Sateia
Friedman 4 in-person Control condition Not able to 24 women, 11 – sleep apnea actigraphy Actigraphy 3mths after overall medium to
9:44
between 4 medications for NR and SR – significant
restriction therapy condition for improvement at
in-person as described in – not free of
sessions sleeping posttreatment
Friedman et al. SE ↑8.6% & for NR and SR
Char Count=
(1991) was medications for 15.2%, ↓TIB
3 wk condition for SE
provided 55min & 95min d = 0.80 & d =
– MMSE
Nap Restriction 1.07, ↓TIB d =
(NR): 0.92 & d = 2.04
Identical protocol to
SR group except
were encouraged
to take a 30 min
daily nap between
1–3 pm
(Continued)
315
c28
IHBK059-Sateia
316
9:44
sessions, &
length of
intersession Content of Therapist Exclusionary Durability of
Effect Size∗
Char Count=
Study interval sessions qualifications Population criteria Measures Results Effects
Germain 2 sessions (BBTI) – Masters-level (BBTI) PSQI Significant n/a overall
et al. (8) with 2 wk – 1st session: adult – 12 women, 5 sleep diary differences medium to
intersession sleep education, psychiatric men HRSD between the two large effect
interval sleep hygiene, and primary – age (m = 70.9 HRSA groups were sizes
(BBTI) stimulus control, care nurse ± 5.3) reported for the (sleep diaries)
– in-person sleep restriction practitioner following: SOL d = 0.80
– wkbook (45min) (IC) (sleep diaries) WASO d =
– trained in
– 2nd session: BBTI – 13 women, 5 – BBTI: SOL ↓22 0.67
(IC) review sleep men min, WASO ↓33 (PSQI)
intervention
– in-person education, – age (m = 69.6 min d = 1.37
– telephone treatment ± 7.3) – IC: SOL ↓3 min,
follow-up adherence, – 33 participants WASO ↓12 min
– pamphlets modify sleep were
schedule as Caucasian (PSQI)
needed (30 min) – 14 participants – BBTI: ↓ 3.94
were using – IC ↑.06
(IC) hypnotics
– 1st session: (clinical
given 3 AASM significance)
brochures 53% BBTI, 17% IC
met criteria for
McCRAE ET AL.
remission
Moderate
improvement in
depression
(HRSD) for BBTI
group
c28
IHBK059-Sateia
SHORT-TERM AND GROUP TREATMENT APPROACHES
Germain, 1 in-person – session – delivered by – participants – substance PSQI – small post- overall medium
et al. (9) session & consisted of a doctoral-level met diagnostic abuse PSQI-A improvements assessment effect size
follow-up combination of practitioner criteria for – psychotic Pittsburgh Sleep for PSQI was PSQI d = 0.66
telephone call imagery PTSD /bipolar Diary (p<0.10) conducted
(3 wk later) rehearsal – 4 women, 3 disorder, major – marked 6–8 wk post-
9:44
sleep &
nightmares,
rationale &
Char Count=
practice of
imagery
rescripting &
rehearsal,
stimulus control,
sleep restriction
– 90 min session
– follow-up phone
call was
scheduled 3wks
later to asses
time in bed
prescription
(Continued)
317
c28
IHBK059-Sateia
Table 1 Summary of Research Employing Brief Treatment Approaches for Insomnia (Continued )
318
#, Format of
sessions, &
length of
intersession Content of Therapist Exclusionary Durability of
Study interval sessions qualifications Population criteria Measures Results Effects Effect Size∗
9:44
Participants were SE ↑11%, improvement at
detailed sleep apnea, for WASO d =
assigned to either SQ↑0.5 follow-up relative
treatment periodic limb 0.42, SE d =
a treatment group to baseline for
manual, mock movement, No significant 0.69, SQ d =
or delayed
Char Count=
therapy narcolepsy) differences were WASO↓31min, 0.71
treatment SE ↑11%,
sessions, – irregular sleep observed for
condition SQ↑0.5 Sleep Diary:
observation, schedule due secondary
No significant Treatment
discussion, to shift work measures
differences were group at
and weekly follow-up for
supervision observed for
secondary WASO d =
provided by 1st 0.59, SE d =
author measures
0.77, SQ d =
0.77
Means, et al. 3 in-person Treatment 3 graduate – 85 women, 33 – medication sleep diary Sleep Diary: No follow-up overall
(11) sessions consisted of a 16 students men age (m = taken IIS Treatment group assessment medium to
Intersession muscle group Students 21.2 ± 5.2) specifically for DBAS had significantly large effect
interval (sessions relaxation received sleep ESS better WASO sizes
occurred 3–7 performed at – presence of FSS ↓9min, SE Sleep Diary:
days apart) least 3 practice other sleep PSWQ ↑3.6%, and SQ Treatment
Treatment group sessions, had disorders (e.g., ↑0.3 than control group WASO
was compared to competence sleep apnea, group at d = 0.58, SE
a waitlist control assessed by periodic limb posttreatment d = 0.52, and
group 2nd author, movement, No group SQ d = 0.75
and therapy narcolepsy) differences were at
Authors also
was – irregular sleep observed for posttreatment
assessed sleep
McCRAE ET AL.
supervised by schedule due daytime
and daytime
2nd author to shift work measures
functioning of a
– chronic illness
subset of
– persistent
participants not
anxiety or
complaining of
depression
insomnia
c28
IHBK059-Sateia
SHORT-TERM AND GROUP TREATMENT APPROACHES
McCrae, 2 in-person – MBT 1st session: – mental – 13 women, 7 – significant med sleep diary (Sleep diaries) n/a overall large
et al. (2) sessions stimulus control, health men condition – MBT: SOL ↓25 effect size for
2 telephone sleep restriction counselor – age (m = 77.2 affecting sleep min, SE ↑16% treatment
sessions (∼50mins) – social ± 8.0) – major psy- (significantly group
Materials – 2nd session: worker chopathology greater than (Sleep
9:44
on weekly basis) no longer met
session: sleep
insomnia criteria SE d = 0.99
education &
sleep hygiene
Char Count=
– 2nd session:
sleep education
– 2 telephone
follow-ups:
progress
review &
troubleshooting
319
c28
IHBK059-Sateia
320
Table 1 Summary of Research Employing Brief Treatment Approaches for Insomnia (Continued )
#, Format of
sessions, &
9:44
interval (sessions standard sleep – participants differences at treatment
were conducted treatment with affective delayed treatment posttreatment group
on a weekly – relaxation tape and anxiety groups (no were maintained Sleep Diary:
basis) involved oral disorders, difference
Char Count=
at follow-up pre to post
instructions with patients who between difference
Immediate treatment groups)
components used hypnotics,
Treatment SH+R:
consisting of and those with SH+R:
groups:
restless legs SOL d = 0.57
progressive SOL ↓20 mins,
– Sleep hygiene mins, TST d
relaxation, syndrome were TST ↑26mins,
and relaxation = 0.50, SE d
passive focal included to SE↑7%
tape = 0.79
attention, and increase
– Sleep hygiene SH+SC: SH+SC:
active expiration. external validity
and stimulus SOL ↓31 mins, SOL d = 0.49
control Sleep hygiene and TST ↑30mins, mins, TST d
stimulus control SE↑12% = 0.39, SE d
Waitlist control (SH+SC): There were no = 0.89
– consisted of significant Sleep Diary:
sleep hygiene differences pre to
advice described between the follow-up
above immediate and difference
– stimulus control delayed treatment
consisted of 6 groups for SH+R:
standard daytime SOL d = 0.53
stimulus control measures mins, TST d
instructions = 0.69, SE d
= 1.00
McCRAE ET AL.
SH+SC:
SOL d = 0.52
mins, TST d
= 0.42, SE d
= 0.94
c28
IHBK059-Sateia
SHORT-TERM AND GROUP TREATMENT APPROACHES
Riedel et al. 4 groups: Stimulus control – graduate – 11 women, 10 – current sleep diaries No significant Follow-up was overall small
(14) – stimulus control sessions: psychology men nonpharmaco- use of hypnotic improvement for conducted 8wks to large effect
with medication – consisted of students – age (m = 56.6 logical medication stimulus control after size for
withdrawal standard recom- – therapists ± 13.8) treatment for ESS participants from posttreatment stimulus
– stimulus control mendations were trained – individuals insomnia BDI pre to post Significant control
9:44
met with therapist medication on disorders (e.g., stimulus control stimulus
through
for 2 one-hour sleep and side sleep apnea or participants: control
weekly
in-person effects meetings periodic limb – TST ↑33 mins, participants:
SE ↑6%, SQ – TST d =
Char Count=
sessions for – provided movement)
stimulus control instructions for ↑0.3 0.39, SE d
treatment gradual = 0.47, SQ
withdrawal from Nonmedicated d = 0.59
There was no stimulus control
intersession sleep medication
participants: Nonmedicated
interval (sessions – TST ↑60 mins, stimulus
were held on SE ↑14.5%, control
consecutive SQ ↑0.6 participants:
weeks)
The two groups Significantly less – TST d =
undergoing daytime 0.75, SE ↑
medication sleepiness for d = 1.23,
withdrawal stimulus control SQ d =
participated in a participants at 0.57
sleep medication follow-up
withdrawal
program (1
session)
(Continued)
321
c28
IHBK059-Sateia
322
Table 1 Summary of Research Employing Brief Treatment Approaches for Insomnia (Continued )
9:44
consisted of 5 restriction, stimulus – age (m = 44.1 related medication index pre/post conducted after improve-
consecutive wks control, information ± 12.0) self-reported DBAS improvement in 9mths) saw ment in
No intersession about sleep respiratory treatment TWT (↓55 improvements TWT d =
Char Count=
treatment hygiene, cognitive problems credibility mins), TST maintained over 0.79, TST d
intervals restructuring, – restless legs (↑34mins), and pretreatment for = 0.46, and
Two groups: information about syndrome SE (↑10%) for Sleep Diary: SE d =
Treatment group medication – depression or treatment – SE (↑10%), 0.71 for
(sleep withdrawal, and anxiety group TST (↑51min), treatment
management applied relaxation – sleep compared to TWT (↓47 min), group
program) and difficulties control group SQ (↑0.54)
– improvements Treatment
waitlist control explained by
in SOL, NWAK, Waitlist control group
physical
All information WASO, early group (follow-up (follow-up)
symptoms
was presented morning conducted 6mths improvements
– night-shift work
via the internet. awakening, after completing over
or regular
TIB, and SQ treatment): pretreatment
daytime sleep
were noted for for:
– previous or – SE improved
ongoing CBT both treatment – SE d =
compared to
for insomnia and control 0.86, TST d
pretreatment
– insomnia being group = 0.87,
(↑7%) and
a minor – control group TWT d =
problem also saw 0.82, SQ d
improvements = 1.39
Waitlist
control group
McCRAE ET AL.
for follow-up:
– SE
improved
compared
to
c28
IHBK059-Sateia
SHORT-TERM AND GROUP TREATMENT APPROACHES
Strom et al. Questions, on TWT, TST, and posttreatment pretreatment
(15) questionnaires, SE (↑3%), TST d = 0.72 and
and diaries were – greater improved posttreatment
submitted through reduction of compared to d = 0.35 ,
the internet DBAS and pretreatment TST improved
9:44
responded to
questions (↓23 min), SOL pretreatment
improved d = 0.73 and
compared to posttreatment
Char Count=
pretreatment d = 0.51,
(↓12min), SQ SOL improved
improved compared to
compared to pretreatment
pretreatment d = 0.41, SQ
(↑0.64) improved
compared to
pretreatment
d = 1.77
Notes: ∗Effect Size Estimates represent Cohen’s d.
Abbreviations: BDI, Beck Depression Inventory (16); CES-D, Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (17); CQOLC, Caregiver Quality of Life Index-Cancer (18); DBAS, Dysfunctional
Beliefs and Attitudes about Sleep Scale (19); ESS, Epworth Sleepiness Scale (20); FSS, Fatigue Severity Scale (21); GDS, Geriatric Depression Scale (22); HADS, Hospital Anxiety and Depression
Scale (23); HRSA, Hamilton Rating Scale for Anxiety (24); HRSD, Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (25); IIS, Insomnia Impact Scale (26); ISQ, Insomnia Symptom Questionnaire (27); MSLT,
Multiple Sleep Latency Test (28); POMS, Profile of Mood States (29); PSWQ, Penn State Worry Questionnaire (30); PSQI, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (31); PSQI-A, PSQI addendum for PTSD
(32); SES, sleep related Self-Efficacy Scale (33); SII, Sleep Impairment Index (34); SSS, Stanford Sleepiness Scale (35); STAI, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (36); SQAW, Sleep Questionnaire and
Assessment of Wakefulness (37);
323
c28 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:44 Char Count=
sleep hygiene/education treatment (6). Participants in the ACBT group experienced decreased
wake time after sleep onset, improved sleep efficiency, and higher sleep quality ratings. The
authors also compared the two groups at a three-month follow-up period. The results indicate
the durability of ACBT intervention with the significant gains of the ACBT group maintained
over time.
Significant improvements in sleep were seen for college students diagnosed with insom-
nia using a three-session treatment approach (11). Treatment consisted of a 16-muscle group
progressive relaxation exercise. The group receiving the relaxation treatment showed signifi-
cantly better improvement at posttreatment compared to the waitlist control for the sleep diary
variables of wake time after sleep onset, sleep efficiency, and sleep quality.
A brief approach of two to four sessions was used to reduce sleep medication use and
improve insomnia among individuals with hypnotic dependent insomnia and primary insom-
nia (14). Three treatment groups (stimulus control with medication withdrawal, stimulus control
alone, medication withdrawal alone) were compared to a waitlist control. The results suggested
that while there was no significant improvement for the groups at posttreatment, follow-up
testing conducted eight weeks after treatment indicated significant improvement for both stim-
ulus control groups in terms of the sleep diary variables. The stimulus control with medication
withdrawal group experienced an increase in total sleep time, an increase of sleep efficiency, and
an increase in sleep quality. The nonmedicated stimulus control group experienced an increase
in total sleep time, an increase in sleep efficiency, and an increase in sleep quality. Additionally,
in terms of secondary outcome measures, the groups receiving stimulus control experienced
significantly less daytime sleepiness at posttreatment and follow-up.
Brief Approaches with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) Patients and Caregivers
The applicability of brief cognitive behavioral intervention for insomnia to different populations
was demonstrated in two studies. A brief, one session, cognitive behavioral intervention was
developed to treat individuals diagnosed with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and poor
sleep (9). The cognitive behavioral intervention included a combination of imagery rehearsal
therapy (IRT), CBT to reduce posttraumatic nightmares, and CBT for insomnia. The results sug-
gested small (p<0.10) improvements in self-reported sleep quality as measured by the Pittsburgh
sleep quality index (PSQI) (31) and marked improvements in PTSD symptoms (9).
Second, the effectiveness of a brief CBT intervention for caregivers was demonstrated
in a study (3) which examined the effectiveness of a two-session caregiver sleep intervention
(CASI) (3). The results indicated that compared to controls, the participants who received the
CASI treatment showed significantly greater improvements in sleep-onset latency as measured
by sleep diaries compared to the baseline period. Additionally, follow-up testing revealed
significantly lower sleep-onset latency and higher total sleep time as measured by actigraphy
for those receiving the CASI treatment.
patients received one in-person CBT session lasting 45 to 60 minutes. The session consisted
of sleep education, stimulus control, and sleep restriction. Additional treatment for patients
receiving more than one session consisted of in-person 15 to 30 minute sessions designed to
troubleshoot and modify the time-in-bed prescription as needed. Results indicated that the
one and four-session CBT dose resulted in significant pre- to post-treatment improvement in
actigraphy total wake time as well as several sleep diary variables, including sleep efficiency,
wake time after sleep onset, and total wake time. Additionally, the four-session CBT dose
resulted in significant improvement in actigraphy sleep efficiency.
In terms of the durability of outcomes at a six month follow-up period, the participants
receiving the one, two, and four session dose of CBT showed significant improvements com-
pared to baseline measures for sleep diary sleep efficiency, total sleep time, and total wake time.
Additionally, the four-session CBT dose saw continued improvement in actigraphy sleep effi-
ciency and total wake time. As mentioned above, the sessions subsequent to the first treatment
session were primarily ‘follow-up’ sessions designed to troubleshoot any treatment difficulties.
In summary, while improvements were seen posttreatment with the one and four dose
treatments and at follow-up with the one, two, and four dose treatments, it appears that the
four dose treatment demonstrated the greatest improvements on both sleep diary and actig-
raphy measures of sleep. Importantly, the four-session dose consisted of bi-weekly delivery of
treatments (compared to a three week intersession interval for the group receiving two sessions
and no intersession interval for the group receiving eight sessions). The authors hypothesize
that scheduling session bi-weekly may provide sufficient time for the patients to independently
develop behavioral changes while still receiving support.
Conclusions
Examination of the literature investigating brief behavioral approaches to treating insomnia
suggests that treatment sessions ranging from one to five can be effective for the treatment
of insomnia. While the majority of studies relied on subjective measures of sleep, significant
improvements were seen with both subjective and objective measures of sleep (3,5,7,42). In
addition to significant posttreatment improvements in sleep, brief behavioral approaches appear
to exhibit durability up to six to eight weeks (9,14), three (6,7,10), four (3), six (5,12,42), seven
(43), and nine (15) months after treatment. Consequently, it appears that brief approaches are
efficacious in the short and longer term (up to nine months) for individuals ranging in age from
21 to 77 years of age.
GROUP INTERVENTION
The ability to simultaneously treat the insomnia symptoms of multiple individuals is an intrigu-
ing concept. A growing number of research studies have employed group treatment approaches.
Although the majority of these studies appear to have used a group approach due to the cost
effectiveness of treating multiple patients at the same time, a few studies have explicitly exam-
ined the impact of different aspects of the ‘group process’ in the treatment of insomnia.
1. Installation of hopea
2. Universalitya
3. Imparting informationa
4. Altruism
5. The corrective recapitulation of the primary family group
6. The development of socializing techniques
7. Imitative behaviora
8. Interpersonal learninga
9. Cohesiveness
10. Catharsisa
11. Existential factors
a
Factors that could be employed in the group treatment of insomnia.
Source: From Ref. 44.
c28 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:44 Char Count=
In conjunction with these factors, Yalom (1995) also states that group treatment approaches must
be inherently distinct from individual approaches to change. These groups should encompass
more than a simple increase in the number of patients present. Group treatment should be
based on the group, not on the individualized treatment approach. According to Yalom, in
group treatment approaches “it is the group that is the agent of change” ((44), p. 109).
In this vein of reasoning, Yalom (1995) has proposed four main factors considered to be
essential to patient satisfaction with group treatment (Table 3). These factors generally include
the group meeting the patient’s needs in alleviating symptoms and the patient’s interaction
in the group and with group members. While traditional cognitive behavioral treatments of
insomnia have proven very efficacious in symptom reduction, the group implementation of such
cognitive behavioral treatments seems to be lacking a focus on the group interaction factors.
Only a handful of studies employing a group treatment approach have examined aspects of the
group interaction.
1. Normalizing insomnia
2. Flexibility to meet the individual needs and expectation of members
3. Individual differences are recognized and discussed
4. Change is possible following education, and identification and insight within a supportive environment
5. Group validation and acceptance allows individuals to move beyond suffering
6. Individual presentation allows for multiple hypotheses and explanations for sleep disturbances
7. Secure, structured group environments facilitate self-disclosure
Special Populations
The effectiveness of group CBT-I has been assessed in numerous special populations. For exam-
ple, the utility of group cognitive behavioral treatment of insomnia for improving sleep and
preventing recidivism in adolescents with substance abuse problems has been studied (56).
Adolescents (between the age of 13 and 19) underwent a multicomponent group treatment and
stress reduction administration. Importantly, only 42% of the sample attended at least four treat-
ment sessions. Sleep improvements in individuals that attended at least four treatment sessions
were found for sleep efficiency, sleep-onset latency, number of awakenings, total sleep time,
and sleep quality. These individuals also strongly endorsed the program at post-treatment. All
adolescents increased their substance use during the course of treatment. The authors suggest
that the effects of improved sleep on substance abuse in adolescents may manifest over a longer
period of time, but group cognitive behavioral treatment of insomnia did produce improved
sleep for the adolescents who completed treatment.
The effectiveness of group intervention to treat insomnia secondary to chronic pain has
been examined via seven weekly sessions of multicomponent CBT-I. Results indicated sig-
nificant improvements in sleep (by diary and actigraphy) from pre- to post-treatment for the
treated patients compared to controls for sleep onset latency, sleep efficiency, wake time after
c28 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:44 Char Count=
sleep onset, and PSQI Quality. These reductions were maintained at three-month follow-up. No
significant improvements in pain, depression, or medications usage were noted (57).
Cancer patients with insomnia were treated with stimulus control, relaxation, sleep edu-
cation, worry time, and cognitive restructuring in groups for seven weeks and found to have
significant improvements two weeks following treatment for: number of awakenings, wake time
after sleep onset, total sleep time, sleep efficiency, sleep quality, and SII (58). Importantly, no
participants continued to meet the diagnostic criteria for insomnia at two weeks post-treatment.
Likewise, the effectiveness of group cognitive behavioral treatment of insomnia for indi-
viduals with a “serious mental illness” has been investigated. Multicomponent CBT-I was
delivered over 10 weekly sessions. From pre- to post-treatment, patients rated their sleep as
significantly better (via the SII.). However, no changes in self-reported sleep parameters were
noted. This is suspected to be largely due to the small (n = 10) sample size (59).
Short-term, group cognitive behavioral treatment of insomnia for “severely mentally ill
patients” has been examined (60). Treatment was delivered over two weekly sessions and
components used include: sleep education, stimulus control, sleep restriction, and sleep hygiene.
From pre- to post-treatment, patients rated their sleep as significantly improved in terms of sleep
onset latency and wake time after sleep onset. These improvements were maintained at three-
month follow-up.
9:44
disorder stimulus (+53 min), SE 1.051 3-mo follow-up
control (+18%), and SOL d = 0.673, TST d =
(modified), BDI (-2.5 0.837 , SE d = 1.222
1-yr follow-up SOL d =
Char Count=
thought points)
stopping, and 0.529, TST d = 0.932,
cognitive SE d = 1.134 3-yr
restructuring followp-up SOL d =
0.366, TST d = 0.826,
SE d = 0.957
Bastien, et 45 Presence of a 4–6 patients 8 weekly Certified Stimulus SD, ISI, Significant pre 82% had SE Overall medium/large
al. (48). Community- sleep disorder sessions/ clinical control, sleep DBAS, BDI, to post above 80% at effect sizes
recruited other than 90 min each psychologists restriction, and BAI improvements 6 mo follow-up Group condition
volunteers, insomnia, session and doctoral cognitive for TWT (+64 Sleep diary:
mean age = presence of students in therapy, and min), SE Pre/post TWT d = 1.346,
41.8 yr (SD = an Axis I psychology sleep hygiene (+11%), SE d = 1.193, WASO d
9.9 yr) disorder, WASO (-31 = .764, SQR d = .965
sleep min), SQR 3-moh follow-up TWT
disturbance (+6 points), d = 1.088, SE d =
due to ISI (-11 0.942, WASO d = 0.605,
substance points), DBAS SQR d = 1.206 6-mo
use, use of (-26 points), follow-up TWT d =
hypnotics, BDI (-8 0.890, SE d = 0.477,
current points), and WASO d = 0.662, SQR
involvement in BAI (-6 points) d = 1.033
psychological
treatment
(Continued)
331
c28
IHBK059-Sateia
Table 5 Summary of Research Employing Group Treatment for Insomnia (Continued )
332
Patient Exclusionary Treatment Treatment
Source sample critieria Group size length Therapist components Measures Results Durability Effect Sizea
Biancosino, et 36 Insomnia No sleep Not reported 2 weekly Not reported Sleep SD, SWAI Significant pre Improvements Overall small effect sizes
al. (60) patients with problem, sessions/ 60 educations, to post maintained at Sleep diary: Pre/post
co-morbid mental min each stimulus improvements 3 mo SOL d = 0.271, WASO d
9:44
completed disturbance or a week break students light, sleep PSWQ, ESS, (-19 min), 12-mo d = 1.593, SOL d =
drug abuse daytime between hygiene, GMHI, and NWAK (-.88 follow-up 1.173, NWAK d = 1.596,
counseling, impairment, session 5 and cognitive drug use awakenings), TST d = .944, SQR d =
Char Count=
mean age = not 6/ 90 min therapy, and TST (+61 1.628
16.13 yr (SD completing or each session Mindfulness- min), and
= 1.215 yr, recently Based Stress SQR (+.76
range = completed Reduction points). No
13–19 yr) substance actigraphy
abuse improvements
treatment
Borkovec, et 24 Less than 30 4 patients 3 weekly Clinical Relaxation Daily ques- Posttreatment No follow-up Not enough information
al. (49). Community- min SOL, use sessions graduate alone, desen- tionnaires improvements provided.
recruited of drugs, students sitization with in SOL (-16
volunteers, no current relaxation, min) and rated
age treatment desensitiza- difficulty in
information tion without falling asleep
reported relaxation and NWAK
Currie, et al. 60 Patients Have 5–7 patients 7 weekly Doctoral Education, SD and SD: SOL (-27 Maintained at Overall medium/large
(57). with fibromyalgia, sessions/ 2 students or sleep actigraphy, min), SE 3-mo effect sizes
co-morbid no sleep hours each interns in restriction, pain, (+13%), follow-up Group vs wait list
insomnia and complaint, session clinical stimulus emotional WASO (-50 Sleep diary:
chronic pain, over the age psychology control, distress, and min), and Posttreatment SOL d =
mean age = of 60, have relaxation medication PSQI Quality .735, SE d = 1.006,
45 yr (SD = 8 major medical training, usage (-5 points) WASO d = .925 PSQI:
McCRAE ET AL.
yr, range = and cognitive SQR d = 1.130 3-mo
29–59 yr) psychiatric restructuring, follow-up
conditions, and sleep Sleep diary: SOL d =
not willing to hygiene 0.646, SE d = 0.962,
undergo WASO d = 0.830 PSQI:
randomization SQR d = 1.534
c28
IHBK059-Sateia
↓ TWT, ↓
9:44
Davidson, et 12 Cancer Currently 4–6 patients 7 weekly Doctoral Stimulus SD, SII, and NWAK (-.72 No follow-up Overall large effect sizes
al. (58). patients with receiving sessions with students in control, HADS awakenings), Pre/post Sleep diary:
insomnia, cancer four weeks clinical relaxation WASO (-31 NWAK d = 1.041,
mean age = treatment (or between psychology training, sleep min), TST WASO d = 1.557, TST
Char Count=
54.7 yr (SD = within 1 mo to session 5 and education, (+35 min), SE d = .559, SE d = 1.899,
10.4 yr) baseline), 6/ 60 – 90 min ‘worry time’, (+16%), SQR SQR d = 1.48
suspicion of a each session and cognitive (+.9 points),
sleep disorder restructuring and SII (-11
other than points)
insomnia,
medical or
psychiatric
disorder
effecting
sleep, and
medication
(hypnotics,
prednisone,
dexametha-
sone,
opioids)
(Continued)
333
c28
IHBK059-Sateia
334
9:44
Dopke, et 11 Insomnia None Unknown 10 weekly Staff clinical Sleep SD and SII Increased No follow-up Overall small effect sizes
al. (59). patients with sessions/ 50 psychologist education, sleep quality Pre/post
comorbid min each and doctoral effects of via SII. No Sleep diary:
Char Count=
mental illness, session student substances, changes in SOL d = .320, WASO
mean age = medical SD. d = .170
45.6 yr (SD = conditions,
9.85 yr) psychiatric
conditions, life
circum-
stances, and
sleep habits
on sleep,
stimulus
control and
sleep
restriction,
and physical,
cognitive, and
behavioral
relaxation
Espie, et 201 General Deteriorating 4–6 patients 5 weekly Community Stimulus SD, SOL (-23.3 SD not Overall medium effect
al. (53). practice health or sessions/ 60 nurses based control, sleep actigraphy, min) and SE maintained at sizes Group condition
patients with dementia, min per in primary restriction, and PSQI (+9%) and 6-mo Sleep diary: Pre/post
insomnia, incapacitating session care teams and cognitive improved follow-up, SOL d = .497, SE d =
mean age =
McCRAE ET AL.
pain or illness, therapy PSQI PSQI .522 6-mo follow-up SOL
54.2 yr (SD = untreated maintained d = 0.391, SE d = 0.417
14.9 yr) mental health
problems,
other sleep
disorders
c28
IHBK059-Sateia
Jansson & 136 Not fulfill the 6–10 patients 6 weekly Certified Problems
9:44
cognitive
therapy, stress
management,
Char Count=
sleep beliefs,
sleep
medications,
coping, and
sleep
management
Schramm, 28 Physician Insomnia 4–8 patients 11 weekly Cognitive- Behavior SD, PSQI, Significant PSQI Overall small/medium
et al. (54). referred, “secondary” sessions/ behavioral analysis, and PSG improvements maintained at effect sizes
mean age = to psychiatric 90 min each therapist and relaxation, in overall 3- and 12-mo Sleep diary:
47.5 yr (SD = or medical session graduate sleep sleep quality follow-up. Pre/post TST d = .305,
13.5 yr) conditions, student in education, (PSQI). TST SE d = .620, NWAK
sleep disorder clinical sleep (+28 min), SE d = .282
other than psychology restriction, (+10%), and
insomnia stimulus NWAK (-.28
control, awakenings).
cognitive No PSG
restructuring, improvement.
stress-
management,
problem
solving, and
increasing
daytime
activities
(Continued)
335
c28
IHBK059-Sateia
336
Char Count=
Patient Exclusionary Treatment Treatment
Source sample critieria Group size length Therapist components Measures Results Durability Effect Sizea
Verbeek, 40 Clinically Psychopathology 5–7 patients 6 weekly Unknown Psychoeducation, SD, DBAS, Improvements Maintained at Overall
et al. (47). referred (depression or sessions/ 2 stimulus control, and SII at post- 9-mo follow up small/medium/large
patients with anxiety, refusal of hor and 30 sleep hygiene, treatment effect sizes Sleep
chronic group treatment, min each sleep restriction, were seen for diary: Pre/post
insomnia, regular hypnotic session cognitive SOL, WASO, SOL d = .610,
mean age = use restructuring, and TST, and SE TST d = .310, SE
43.68 yr (SD relaxation d = 1.136, WAS0
= 10.10 yr) d = .513 9-mo
follow-up SOL
d = 0.567, TST
d = 0.467, SE
d = 0.941, WAS0
d = 360
Vincent & 50 Shift work, sleep 5–6 patients 7 weekly Staff Sleep hygiene, SD, PSQI, SOL (-20 No follow-up Overall large
Hameed Community- disorder other sessions/ psychologist stimulus control, DBAS, and ISI min), TST effect sizes
(51). recruited than insomnia, 90 min each medication (+54 min), SE Sleep diary:
volunteers brain injury, session withdrawal, (+6%), DBAS Pre/post TST
and bipolar disorder, relaxation, sleep (-19 points), d = .833
McCRAE ET AL.
respirologist schizophrenia, restriction, PSQI (-4.48
referred serious medical cognitive therapy, points), and
patients, condition stress ISI (-6.6
mean age = management, points).
51.4 yr (SD = and problem
11.4 yr) solving
c28
IHBK059-Sateia
SHORT-TERM AND GROUP TREATMENT APPROACHES
Vincent & 43 Shift work, sleep 5–7 patients 6 weekly Psychologist Sleep hygiene, SD and SII No sleep data No follow-up Not applicable
Lionberg Community- disorder other sessions and doctoral stimulus control, reported
(61). recruited than insomnia, student medication
volunteers, brain injury, withdrawal,
mean age = bipolar disorder, relaxation, sleep
9:44
patients, 90 min each relaxation, ISI. BDI, Sleep effect sizes
mean age = session stimulus control, PSWQ, and Duration, and Sleep diary:
49.7 yr (SD = sleep restriction, CGI SOL as Pre/post SE
d = .815, TST
Char Count=
12.0 yr) medication measured by
withdrawal, and the PSQI. SD: d = .655, SOL
cognitive therapy SE (+9%), d = .453
TST (+42
min), and SOL
(-12 min).
Improvements
for the DBAS,
ISI, PSWQ,
and BDI
a
Effect Size Estimates represent Cohen’s d and were calculated for pre to post treatment changes for the group condition alone (when no control condition was employed) or represent differences (if
adequate control group was used), when sufficient information was available, in subjectively recorded sleep only.
Abbreviations: SD, sleep diary; PSQI, Pittsburg Sleep Quality Index (31); BDI, Beck Depression Inventory (16); BAI, Beck Anxiety Inventory (62); PSG, polysomnography; SOL, sleep-onset latency;
WASO, wake time after sleep onset; TWT, total wake time; TST, total sleep time; SE, sleep efficiency; SQR, sleep quality rating; DBAS, Dysfunctional Beliefs and Attitudes About Sleep (19); ISI, Insomnia
Severity Index (34); PSWQ, Penn State Worry Questionnaire (63); HADS, Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (23); STAI, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (36); CGI, Clinical Global Improvement Scale;
SII, Sleep Impairment Index (34); GMHI, General Mental Health Distress Index (64); SWAI, Sleep-Wake Activity Inventory (65); and ESS, Epworth Sleepiness Scale (20).
337
c28 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:44 Char Count=
that group treatment can be as efficacious and effective as traditional behavioral approaches for
treating insomnia.
Future Directions
The research that has previously been conducted on the group treatment of insomnia provides
great hope for this modality of implementation. However, many questions are left unanswered.
What is the optimal number of patients per group? Are there patients who are better or worse
suited for group treatment approaches? What potential patient characteristics might distin-
guish patients who might most benefit from group treatment? How are these potential patient
characteristics best assessed? Are there personal characteristics of group leaders that may lead
to improved efficacy of group treatments? Is a purely interpersonal approach to the group
treatment of insomnia practical? What is the best way to integrate group processes into the
group treatment of insomnia? Which traditional cognitive behavioral treatments of insomnia
techniques are best suited for implementation in group contexts?
All of these questions are intriguing. In the age of Health Maintenance Organizations
(HMOs), it is critically important to provide empirical evidence of the effectiveness of treatment
approaches. The cost-effectiveness of treating multiple patients simultaneously may potentially
lead to group-based cognitive behavioral treatment of insomnia as the treatment of choice. While
the extant literature suggests that a group approach is an effective treatment for insomnia in
many different sub-populations of individuals with insomnia, much work is still needed. Given
the high prevalence of insomnia and the preliminary results of group treatment approaches,
this line of research warrants continuation and expansion. Furthermore, as presented in Table
5, many of the studies that have employed a group-based treatment approach have included
patients with several comorbid conditions. Traditional logic suggests that patients with comor-
bid conditions are the hardest to successfully treat. Thus far, insomnia comorbid with drug
abuse (56), chronic pain (57), cancer (58), and mental illnesses (59) have all been successfully
treated within a group approach.
Conclusions
The group treatment of insomnia is a natural extension of individually administered cognitive
behavioral treatment of insomnia. While preliminary work suggests these group approaches to
be highly efficacious, the majority have apparently not capitalized on the unique therapeutic
aspects that group therapy engenders. As Yalom (1995) stated, “groups resting solely on other
assumptions, such as psycho-educational or cognitive-behavioral principles, fail to reap the full
therapeutic harvest of group therapy. Each of these forms of therapy can. . . be made even more
effective by incorporating a focus on interpersonal process” (Yalom, 1995, p. xiv).
Results from the small number of research studies that have employed a group treatment
approach are very promising. Given its cost effectiveness, group treatment may prove to be a
viable early intervention for insomnia (50) and appears to be suitable for delivery by trained
nurses (53). Furthermore, its application to a wide range of clinical populations attests to the
durability of this treatment modality. Lastly, patient perceptions of interactions with other
individuals with insomnia as therapeutic provide some evidence of the added benefit of group
treatment (47).
settings (academic research), more research on the effectiveness of such approaches is needed
in order to fully understand how well such interventions translate to ‘real world’ primary care
settings. To date, the few studies of effectiveness in this area have produced promising results
(15,19–21,31,53). More research on the effectiveness of these approaches is needed, because
several important questions remain regarding the best approach(es) to administering brief
and/or group interventions in applied settings (e.g., Who is best suited to administer such
treatment?; What is the ideal length of treatment?; What is the optimal group size for group
treatment?).
REFERENCES
1. Morin CM, Bootzin RR, Buysse DJ, et al. Psychological and behavioral treatment of insomnia: Update
of the recent evidence (1998–2004). Sleep 2006; 29(11):1398–1414.
2. McCrae CS, McGovern R, Lukefahr R, et al. Research evaluating brief Behavioral sleep treatments for
rural elderly (RESTORE): A preliminary examination of effectiveness. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2007;
15(11):979–982.
3. Carter PA. A brief behavioral sleep intervention for family caregivers of persons with cancer. Cancer
Nurs 2006; 29(2):95–103.
4. Chambers MJ, Alexander SD. Assessment and prediction of outcome for a brief behavioral insomnia
treatment program. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 1992; 23(4):289–297.
5. Edinger JD, Wohlgemuth WK, Radtke RA, et al. Dose-response effects of cognitive-behavioral insom-
nia therapy: A randomized clinical trial. Sleep 2007; 30(2):203–212.
6. Edinger JD, Sampson WS. A primary care “friendly” cognitive behavioral insomnia therapy. Sleep
2003; 26(2):177–182.
7. Friedman L, Benson K, Noda A, et al. An actigraphic comparison of sleep restriction and sleep hygiene
treatments for insomnia in older adults. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 2000; 13(1):17–27.
8. Germain A, Moul DE, Franzen PL, et al. Effects of a brief behavioral treatment for late-life insomnia:
Preliminary findings. J Clin Sleep Med 2006; 2(4):3.
9. Germain A, Shear MK, Hall M, et al. Effects of a brief behavioral treatment for PTSD-related sleep
disturbances: A pilot study. Behav Res Ther 2007; 45(3):627–632.
10. Lichstein KL, Wilson NM, Johnson CT. Psychological treatment of secondary insomnia. Psychol Aging
2000; 15(2):232–240.
11. Means MK, Lichstein KL, Epperson MT, et al. Relaxation therapy for insomnia: Nighttime and day
time effects. Behav Res Ther 2000; 38(7):665–678.
12. Pallesen S, Nordhus IH, Kvale G, et al. Behavioral treatment of insomnia in older adults: An open
clinical trial comparing two interventions. Behav Res Ther 2003; 41(1):31–48.
13. Bech P, Gudex C, Johansen KS. The WHO (Ten) well-being index: Validation in diabetes. Psychother
Psychosom 1996; 65(4):183–190.
14. Riedel BW, Lichstein KL, Peterson BA, et al. A comparison of the efficacy of stimulus control for
mediated and nonmedicated insomniacs. Behav Modif 1998; 22(1):3–28.
15. Strom L, Pettersson R, Andersson G. Internet-based treatment for insomnia: A controlled evaluation.
J Consult Clin Psychol 2004; 72(1):113–120.
16. Beck AT, Steer RA, Garbin MG. Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five
years of evaluation. Clin Psychol Rev 1988; 8:77–100.
17. Radloff LS. The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for reseach in the general population. Appl
Psychol Meas 1977; 1:385–401.
18. Weitzner MA, McMillan SC. The Caregiver Quality of Life Index-Cancer (CQOLC) scale: Revalidation
in a home hospice setting. J Palliat Care 1999; 15(2):13–20.
19. Morin CM, Stone J, Trinkle D, et al. Dysfunctional beliefs and attitudes about sleep among older adults
with and without insomnia complaints. Psychol Aging 1993; 8(3):463–467.
20. Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness – The Epworth Sleepiness Scale. Sleep
1991; 14(6):540–545.
21. Krupp LB, Larocca NG, Muirnash J, et al. The Fatigue Severitiy Scale – Appliation to patients with
Multiple-Schlerosis and Systematic Lupus-Erythmatosus. Arch Neurol 1989; 46(10):1121–1123.
22. Yesavage JA, Brink TL, Rose TL, et al. Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening
scale – A preliminary report. J Psychiatr Res 1983; 17(1):37–49.
23. Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1983;
67(6):361–370.
24. Hamilton M. The assessment of anxiety states by rating. British Journal of Medical Psychology 1959.
25. Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1960; 23:56–62.
c28 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:44 Char Count=
26. Hoelscher TJ, Ware JC, Bond T. Initial validation of the Insomnia Impact Scale. Sleep Res 1993; 22:149.
27. Spielman AJ, Saskin P, Thorpy MJ. Treatment of chronic insomnia by restriction of time in bed. Sleep
1987; 10(1):45–56.
28. Carskadon MA, Dement WC, Mitler MM, et al. Guidelines for the Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT)
– A standard measure of sleepiness. Sleep 1986; 9(4):519–524.
29. McNair DM, Lorr M, Droppleman LF. Manual for the Profile of Mood States. San Diego, CA: EDITS,
1971.
30. Meyer TJ, Miller ML, Metzger RL, et al. Development and validation of the Penn State Worry Ques-
tionnaire. Behav Res Ther 1990; 28(6):487–495.
31. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH, et al. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: A new instrument for
psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res 1989 28:193–213.
32. Germain A, Hall M, Krakow B, et al. A brief sleep scale for posttraumatic stress disorder: Pittsburgh
Sleep Quality Index Addendum for PTSD. J Anxiety Disord 2005; 19:233–244.
33. Lacks P. Behavioral Treatment for Persistent Insomnia. New York, NY: Pergamon Press, 1987.
34. Morin CM. Insomnia. Psychological Assessment and Management. New York, NY: Guilford Press,
1993.
35. Hoddes E, Zarcone V, Smythe H, et al. Quantification of sleepiness – New approach. Psychophysiology
1973; 10(4):431–436.
36. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene R, et al. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, Form Y. Palo Alto, CA:
Consulting Psychologists Press, 1983.
37. Miles L. A sleep questionnaire. In: Guilleminault C, ed. Sleeping and Waking Disorders: Indications
and Techniques. Stoneham, MA: Butterwork, 1982:Appendix I.:383–413.[CE: Please check highlighted
text in this ref.]
38. Perlis ML, Sharpe M, Smith MT, et al. Behavioral treatment of insomnia: Treatment outcome and the
relevance of medical and psychiatric morbidity. J Behav Med 2001; 24(3):281–296.
39. Perlis M, Aloia M, Millikan A, et al. Behavioral treatment of insomnia: A clinical case series study. J
Behav Med 2000; 23(2):149–161.
40. Irwin MR, Cole JC, Nicassio PM. Comparative meta-analysis of behavioral interventions for insomnia
and their efficacy in middle-aged adults and in older adults 55+years of age. Health Psychol 2006;
25(1):3–14.
41. Murtagh DRR, Greenwood KM. Identifying effective psychological treatments for insomnia – A meta-
analysis. J Consult Clin Psychol 1995; 63(1):79–89.
42. Edinger JD, Wohlgemuth WK, Radtke RA, et al. Cognitive behavioral therapy for treatment of chronic
primary insomnia – A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 2001; 285(14):1856–1864.
43. Chambers MJ, Alexender SD. Assessment and prediction of outcome for a brief behavioral insomnia
treatment program. J Abnorm Psychol 1992.
44. Yalom ID. The Theory and Practice of Group Psychotherapy Fourth Edition. New York, NY: Basic-
Books, 1995.
45. Kupych-Woloshyn N, MacFarlane J, Shapiro CM. A group-approach for the management of insomnia.
J Psychosom Res 1993; 37:39–44.
46. Constantino M, Manber R, Ong J, et al. Patient expectations and therapeutic alliance as predictors of
outcome in group cognitive-behavioral therapy for insomnia. Behav Sleep Med 2007; 5(3):210–228.
47. Verbeek I, Konings G, Aldenkamp A, et al. Cognitive behavioral treatment in clinicially referred
chronic insomniacs: Group versus indivdiual treatment. Behav Sleep Med 2006; 4(3):135–151.
48. Bastien CH, Morin CM, Ouellet MC, et al. Cognitive-behavioral therapy for insomnia: Comparison of
individual therapy, group therapy, and telephone consultations. J Consult Clin Psychol 2004; 72(4):653–
659.
49. Borkovec TD, Steinmar.Sw, Nau SD. Relaxation training and single-item desensitization in group
treatment of insomnia. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 1973; 4(4):401–403.
50. Jansson M, Linton SS. Cognitive-behavioral group therapy as an early intervention for insomnia: A
randomized controlled trial. J Occup Rehabil 2005; 15(2):177–190.
51. Vincent N, Hameed H. Relation between adherence and outcome in the group treatment of insomnia.
Behav Dleep Med 2003; 1(3):125–139.
52. Backhaus J, Hohagen F, Voderholzer U, et al. Long-term effectiveness of a short-term cognitive-
behavioral group treatment for primary insomnia. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2001; 251(1):
35–41.
53. Espie CA, MacMahon KMA, Kelly HL, et al. Randomized clinical effectiveness trial of nurse-
administered small-group cognitive behavior therapy for persistent insomnia in general practice.
Sleep 2007; 30(5):574–584.
54. Schramm E, Hohagen F, Backhaus J, et al. Effectiveness of a multicomponent group treatmetn for
insomnia. Behav Cogn Psychother 1995; 23(2):109–127.
c28 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 9:44 Char Count=
55. Vincent N, Penner S, Lewycky S. What predicts patients’ perceptions of improvement in insomnia?
J Sleep Res 2006; 15(3):301–308.
56. Bootzin RR, Stevens SJ. Adolescents, substance abuse, and the treatment of insomnia and daytime
sleepiness. Clin Psychol Rev 2005; 25(5):629–644.
57. Currie SR, Wilson KG, Pontefract AJ, et al. Cognitive-behavioral treatment of insomnia secondary to
chronic pain. J Consult Clin Psychol 2000; 68(3):407–416.
58. Davidson JR, Waisberg JL, Brundage MD, et al. Nonpharmacologic group treatment of insomnia: A
preliminary study with cancer survivors. Psychooncology. 2001; 10(5):389–397.
59. Dopke CA, Lehner RK, Wells AM. Cognitive-behavioral group therapy for insomnia in individuals
with serious mental illnesses: A preliminary evaluation. Psychiatr Rehabil J 2004; 27(3):235–242.
60. Biancosino B, Rocchi D, Dona S, et al. Efficacy of short-term psychoeducational intervention for
persistent non-organic insomnia in severely mentally ill patients. A pilot study. Eur Psychiatry 2004;
21:460–462.
61. Vincent N, Lionberg C. Treatment preference and patient satisfaction in chronic insomnia. Sleep
2001;24(4):411–417.
62. Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G, et al. An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: Psychometric prop-
erties. J Consult Clin Psychol 1988; 56:863–897.
63. Carstensen LL, Pasupathi M, Mayr U, et al. Emotional experience in everyday life across the adult life
span. J Pers Soc Psychol 2000; 79(4):644–655.
64. Dennis ML. Global Appraisal of Individual Needs – Version 1299. Bloomington, IL: Chestnut Health
Systems, 1999.
65. Rosenthal L, Roehrs TA, Roth T. The Sleep-Wake Inventory: A self-report measure of daytime sleepi-
ness. Biol Psychiatry 1993; 34:810–820.
c29 IHBK059-Sateia March 25, 2010 9:39 Char Count=
Jack D. Edinger
Psychology Service, VA Medical Center, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University
Medical Center, Durham, North Carolina, U.S.A.
Stimulus control r A structured behavioral regimen that instructs the Conditioned arousal
patient to (1) establish a standard rising time; (2) go associated with the
to bed only when sleepy; (3) get out of bed whenever bed/bedroom and
awake for long periods; (4) avoid reading, watching attempts to sleep
TV, eating, worrying and other sleep-incompatible Improper sleep–wake
behaviors in the bed/bedroom; and (5) refrain from scheduling that
daytime napping. interferes with circadian
r The goal of this treatment is that of reassociating the control over sleep
bed and bedroom with successful sleep attempts. Daytime napping that
reduces homeostatic
sleep drive
Sleep restriction r Sleep restriction therapy reduces nocturnal sleep Excessive time spent in
disturbance primarily by restricting the time allotted for bed that reduces
sleep each night so that the time spent in bed closely homeostatic sleep drive.
matches the individual’s presumed sleep requirement.
Sleep hygiene r Patients are educated about normative sleep Patients’ misconceptions
education requirements, healthy sleep behaviors and about sleep.
sleep-conducive environmental conditions. Use of sleep-disruptive
r Typically they are encouraged to exercise daily, substances
eliminate the use of caffeine, alcohol, and nicotine, Lifestyle &
eat a light snack at bedtime, and ensure that the environmental factors
sleeping environment is quiet, dark, and comfortable. that disrupt sleep
to expect that a person, who presents with a significant worry component and good sleep
habits, should receive the same single component therapy as a patient who presents primarily
with an irregular sleep–wake schedule. Since multimodal CBT includes several stand-alone
interventions, it presumably casts a wider treatment net, thus providing a more broadly effective
therapy across a range of insomnia subtypes (i.e., those with sleep onset insomnia, those with
sleep maintenance insomnia, those complaining predominantly of cognitive arousal, etc.).
Given its inclusion of several of the first-generation behavioral treatment strategies, Mul-
timodal CBT is a more complex therapy than are its predecessors. Yet multimodal CBT does
not require more time in therapy than do the single component treatments. In fact, CBT is typ-
ically administered in one to eight sessions. Given the accumulation of supportive data, CBT
arguably has become the nonpharmacologic treatment of choice for insomnia, and most recent
RCTs involving psychological interventions for insomnia have focused on various forms of this
multicomponent approach.
troublesome thoughts or cognitive arousal should be improved in this modality. Indeed, studies
suggest that CBT improves several cognitive factors such as sleep-interfering beliefs (44), sleep-
related self-efficacy (e.g., confidence in one’s ability to produce sleep) (21), and cognitive arousal
(24–26). Likewise, CBT appears to address sleep-interfering behavioral factors such as spending
excessive time in bed (27), or varying bedtime and rise times (4,15,17,28). Moreover, although
limited in number, the studies that have examined CBT mechanisms have shown that changes
in selected, CBT-targeted cognitive and behavioral perpetuating mechanisms appear to mediate
improvements in sleep and global insomnia symptoms. Thus, the outcomes achieved with CBT
apparently result, at least in part, from the fact that this therapy effectively targets cognitive and
behavioral mechanisms critical to sustaining insomnia problems.
MODES OF DELIVERY
Despite the efficacy of multimodal CBT for managing insomnia, access to this treatment can
be limited due to the scarcity of providers with CBT expertise. Indeed the number of patients
who might benefit from such an intervention currently exceeds the capacity of the number of
trained providers of this treatment. This state of affairs has created significant impetus for the
development of innovative methods of CBT delivery so as to allow for more facile dissemination
of this treatment. Whereas efforts to enhance CBT dissemination is a fairly new effort, there have
been a number of innovative CBT delivery methods proposed that might lead to greater public
accessibility of this treatment.
Like many other psychological treatments, multimodal CBT is easily adapted to a group
therapy format. The obvious advantage of this format is that it allows one trained provider to
administer treatment to multiple patients simultaneously. Moreover, a group therapy format
can have the added benefit of providing patients peer support as they proceed through the
treatment process. Although a previous metaanalytic review (29) suggested a slight superiority
of individually administered treatments over group therapy, several controlled evaluations have
shown that group CBT models involving six to eight sessions produce notable improvements
in subjective/objective sleep patterns, general mood status and unhelpful beliefs about sleep
(4,17,19,30). Studies comparing the relative efficacy of individual versus group CBT have been
limited, but one recent study did show comparable outcomes for insomnia patients assigned to
either group or individualized CBT therapy. Nonetheless, more studies are needed to further
explore this issue.
Because most insomnia patients who seek care do so in primary care clinics (31), there
has been some interest in developing multimodal CBT protocols suitable for such medication
settings. Espie and colleagues (19,20) tested a group CBT protocol administered by office nurses
working in outpatient medical clinics and found this approach to be more effective than usual
care for managing insomnia patients seen in such settings. However, the treatment effect sizes for
this form of intervention appeared to be somewhat smaller than those reported for trials wherein
treatment was administered by doctoral-level providers and/or sleep specialists (20,32). As an
alternative to this approach, some investigators (15) have proposed abbreviated two-session CBT
protocols suitable for delivery by primary care providers. Whereas results from pilot tests of
these interventions have been promising, it is yet to be demonstrated that MD-level providers
are willing and able to effectively administer such protocols in their busy medical practices.
Thus, continued consideration of CBT models suitable for primary care would seem beneficial.
Of course many insomnia sufferers do not seek care from professionals but rather attempt
to manage their sleep problems on their own by using over-the-counter sleep aids or other less
than optimal “home remedies” such as alcohol (33). With these sorts of individuals in mind,
Mimeault and Morin (13) tested a self-help, CBT bibliotherapy with and without supportive
phone consultations against a wait list control group. Compared to the control condition, those
treated with the bibliotherapy showed substantially greater sleep improvements than control
patients, and these improvements were maintained at a three-month follow up. The addition
of phone consultations with a therapist conferred some advantage over bibliotherapy alone at
posttreatment, but these benefits disappeared by follow up. Oosterhuis and Klip (34) reported
promising results from a novel study wherein a multimodal behavioral insomnia therapy was
provided via a series of eight, 15-minute educational programs broadcast on radio and television.
More than 23,000 people ordered the accompanying course material, and data from a random
c29 IHBK059-Sateia March 25, 2010 9:39 Char Count=
subset of these showed sleep improvements and reductions in hypnotic use, medical visits, and
physical complaints were achieved among those who took part in this educational program.
More recently, Strom and colleagues (35) tested a five-week self-help interactive CBT program
delivered to insomnia patients via the Internet. Although the treated group did show greater
reductions in unhelpful attitudes toward sleep than did untreated patients, the treatment and
control groups did not differ on their self-reported sleep improvements. These findings provide
a mixed view of self-help protocols but do imply that some guidance from trained therapists
(via phone consultation or mass media) may enhance outcomes for these self-help interventions.
Hence, the question of how best to optimize the efficacy of these self-help CBT interventions
merits research attention.
100
85
80
60 54
% 48
40
20
0
CBT Medication CBT + Taper
Taper
Figure 1 Proportion of patients who were able to stop hypnotic use with treatment
undergo CBT are able to reduce or stop their hypnotic use during the course of this treatment.
Perhaps as a result of such observations, some investigators have tested the usefulness of
combining multimodal CBT for insomnia with structured medication tapering protocols to aid
hypnotic-dependent patients who wish to discontinue sleep medication use. In one such study,
Morgan et al. (39) compared a treatment consisting of CBT + medication tapering against a
waitlist condition and found the former treatment was superior to the control condition for
both improving sleep and producing at least a 50% reduction in sleep medication use by a six-
month follow-up. In a subsequent study, Morin compared the relative efficacy of CBT, a guided
medication tapering protocol, and a combined CBT + guided taper protocol (40). As shown by
Figure 1, the proportion of patients who were able to stop their hypnotic use was substantially
higher in the combined treatment condition than in the other two unitary treatment approaches.
Patients who received CBT reported greater sleep improvements than those who received only
the guided medication tapering protocol. Furthermore, overall hypnotic abstinence rates at a
long-term follow-up favored the combined CBT + guided tapering condition over the other
two treatments. Given these findings, multimodal CBT appears to be a very useful therapy for
helping patients reduces or discontinue their use of hypnotic medications.
Treatment matching relies on a case formulation approach. For example, Patient X and
Y complain primarily of cognitive arousal, thus we expect a cognitive approach would be
superior to a less cognitively oriented therapy. Patient X is randomly assigned to a cognitively
oriented therapy and Patient Y to a predominantly behavioral therapy, and we test whether
matching Patient X to a cognitive intervention produces superior treatment outcome to the more
behavioral approach. It would be useful to test a matching approach with contemporary treat-
ment approaches and comprehensive outcome measures to determine if tailored approaches
are superior to “packaged” treatment approaches.
In this regard, there is also need for research to determine if tailored or augmented CBT
protocols need to be developed for complex forms of insomnia. As discussed previously, studies
have supported the efficacy of CBT for treating primary insomnia and insomnia with comorbid
medical and psychiatric conditions. Nonetheless, to date, no studies have been conducted to
evaluate the relative efficacy of CBT delivered in a fixed dose and format to primary and
comorbid insomnia patients. Those with comorbid disorders often have symptoms (e.g., pain,
lethargy, anxiety, etc.) that can confound sleep and treatment acceptance/adherence. Thus, it is
possible that those with comorbid insomnia may respond less well to the CBT dose and format
that is optimal for primary insomnia patients. Of course, a corollary of this speculation is that
cognitive/behavioral insomnia treatments may benefit from special tailoring or augmentation
(41) to better fit the needs of those with various forms of comorbid insomnia. Hopefully, future
research will address these questions and speculations.
Lastly, dissemination remains a challenge to this very effective treatment. Given its supe-
rior durability to pharmacologic approaches and absence of potentially dangerous side effects,
CBT should be a more common first-line approach in primary care and mental health settings
(42). This, however, is not the case, so dissemination of the effectiveness of the treatment as well
as dissemination of available treatment providers in the community is needed. CBT training
facilities are growing but we need more treatment providers to match the prevalence of this dis-
order. Perhaps a growing number of the recently reported (e.g., self-help, internet, CBT tailored
for primary care) (13,15) and yet to be developed alternative delivery modes will increase the
treatment accessibility for this very effective intervention.
REFERENCES
1. Morin CM, Stone J, Trinkle D, et al. Dysfunctional beliefs and attitudes about sleep among older
adults with and without insomnia complaints. Psychol Aging 1993; 8:463–467.
2. Hoelscher T, Edinger JD. Treatment of sleep-maintenance insomnia in older adults: Sleep period
reduction, sleep education and modified stimulus control. Psychol Aging 1988; 3:258–263.
3. Edinger JD, Hoelscher TJ, Marsh GR, et al. A cognitive-behavioral therapy for sleep-maintenance
insomnia in older adults. Psychol Aging 1992; 7:282–289.
4. Morin CM, Kowatch RA, Barry T, et al. Cognitive-behavior therapy for late-life insomnia. J Consult
Clin Psychol 1993; 61:137–147.
5. Edinger JD, Fins AI, Glenn DM, et al. Insomnia and the eye of the beholder: Are there clinical markers
of objective sleep disturbances among adults with and without insomnia complaints? J Consult Clin
Psychol 2000; 68:586–593.
6. Carney CE, Waters WF. Effects of a Structured Problem-Solving Procedure on Pre-Sleep Cognitive
Arousal in College Students with Insomnia. Behav Sleep Med 2006; 4(1):13–28.
7. Morin CM. Insomnia: Psychological Assessment and Management. New York, NY: Guilford Press,
1993.
8. Edinger JD, Carney CE. Overcoming Insomnia: A Cognitive Behavior Therapy Approach Therapist
Guide. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, 2008.
9. Bonnet MH, Arand DL. 24-hour metabolic rate in insomniacs and matched normal sleepers. Sleep
1995; 18:581–588.
10. Edinger JD, Wohlgemuth WK. The significance and management of persistent primary insomnia.
Sleep Med Rev 1999; 3:101–118.
11. Borkovec T, Hennings BL. The role of physiological attention-focusing in the relaxation treat-
ment of sleep disturbance, general tension, and specific stress reaction. Behav Res Ther 1978; 16:
7–19.
12. Riedel BW. Sleep hygiene. In: Lichstein KL, Morin CM, eds. Treatment of Late Life Insomnia. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc, 2000:125–146.
c29 IHBK059-Sateia March 25, 2010 9:39 Char Count=
13. Mimeault V, Morin CM. Self-help treatment for insomnia: Bibliotherapy with and without professional
guidance. J Consult Clin Psychol 1999; 67(4):511–519.
14. Rybarczyk B, Lopez M, Benson R, et al. Efficacy of two behavioral treatment programs for comorbid
geriatric insomnia. Psychol Aging 2002; 17:288–298.
15. Edinger JD, Sampson WS. A primary care “friendly” cognitive behavioral insomnia therapy. Sleep
2003; 26:177–182.
16. Leger D, Guilleminault C, Bader G, et al. Medical and socio-professional impact of insomnia. Sleep
2002; 25:621–625.
17. Morin CM, Colecchi C, Stone J, et al. Behavioral and pharmacological therapies for late-life insomnia:
A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 1999; 281:991–999.
18. Jacobs GD, Pace-Schott EF, Stickgold R, et al. Cognitive behavior therapy and pharmacotherapy for
insomnia: A randomized controlled trial and direct comparison. Arch Intern Med 2004; 164:1888–
1896.
19. Espie CA, Inglis SJ, Tessier S, et al. The clinical effectiveness of cognitive behaviour therapy for chronic
insomnia: Implementation and evaluation of a sleep clinic in general medical practice. Behav Res Ther
2001; 39:45–60.
20. Espie CA, MacMahon KMA, Kelly H, et al. Randomized Clinical Effectiveness Trial of Nurse-
Administered Small-Group Cognitive Behavior Therapy for Persistent Insomnia in General Practice.
Sleep 2007; 30(5):574–584.
21. Currie SR, Wilson KG, Pontefract AJ, et al. Cognitive-behavioral treatment of insomnia secondary to
chronic pain. J Consult Clin Psychol 2000; 68:407–416.
22. Edinger JD, Wohlgemuth WK, Krystal AD, et al. Behavioral insomnia therapy for Fibromyalgia
patients: A randomized clinical trial. Arch Intern Med 2005; 165:2527–2535.
23. Morin CM, Stone J, McDonald K, et al. Psychological management of insomnia: A clinical replication
series with 100 patients. Behav Ther 1994; 25:291–309.
24. Mitchell KR. Behavioral treatment of presleep tension and intrusive cognitions in patients with severe
predormital insomnia. J Behav Med 1978; 2:57–69.
25. Mitchell KR, White RG. Self-management of severe predormital insomnia. J Behav Ther Exp Psychi-
atry 1977; 8:57–63.
26. Sanavio E. Pre-Sleep Cognitive Intrusions and Treatment of Onset-Insomnia. Behav Res Ther 1988;
26(6):451–459.
27. Spielman AJ, Saskin P, Thorpy MJ. Treatment of chronic insomnia by restriction of time in bed. Sleep
1987; 10:45–55.
28. Bootzin RR. Stimulus control treatment for insomnia. Proceedings of the 80th Annual Meeting of the
American Psychological Association. 1972;7:395–396.
29. Morin CM, Culbert JP, Schwartz SM. Nonpharmacological interventions for insomnia: A meta-
analysis of treatment efficacy. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151:1172–1180.
30. Backhaus J, Hohagen F, Voderholzer U, et al. Long-term effectiveness of a short-term cognitive-
behavioral group treatment for primary insomnia. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2001; 251:35–41.
31. Richardson GS. Managing insomnia in the primary care setting: Raising issues. Sleep 2000; 23
(Suppl. 1):S9–S12.
32. Smith MT, Perlis ML, Park A, et al. Comparative Meta-Analysis of pharmacotherapy and behavior
therapy for persistent insomnia. Am J Psychiatry 2002; 159:5–11.
33. Mellinger GD, Balter MB, Uhlenhuth EH. Insomnia and its treatment: Prevalence and correlates. Arch
Gen Psychiatry 1985; 42:225–232.
34. Oosterhuis A, Klip EC. The treatment of insomnia through mass media: The results of a televised
behavioral training programme. Soc Sci Med 1997; 45:1223–1229.
35. Strom L, Pettersson R, Andersson G. Internet-based treatment for insomnia: A controlled evaluation.
J Consult Clin Psychol 2004; 72:113–120.
36. Walsh JK, Schweitzer PK. Ten-year trends in the pharmacological treatment of insomnia. Sleep 1998;
22:371–375.
37. Verbeek I, Schreuder K, Declerck G. Evaluation of short-term non-pharmacological treatment of
insomnia in a clinical setting. J Psychosom Res 1999; 47:369–383.
38. Wu R, Jinfeng B, Chungai Z, et al. Comparison of sleep condition and sleep-related psychologi-
cal activity after cognitive behavior and pharmacological therapy for chronic insomnia. Psychother
Psychosom 2006; 75:220–228.
39. Morgan K, Dixon S, Mathers S, et al. Psychological treatment for insomnia in the management of
long-term hypnotic use: A pragmatic randomised controlled trial. Br J Gen Pract 2003; 53:923–928.
40. Morin CM, Bastien CH, Guay B, et al. Randomized clinical trial of supervised tapering and cognitive
behavior therapy to facilitate benzodiazepine discontinuation in older adults with chronic insomnia.
Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161:332–342.
c29 IHBK059-Sateia March 25, 2010 9:39 Char Count=
41. Smith MT, Huang MI, Manber R. Cognitive Behavior Therapy for chronic insomnia occuring within
the context of medical and psychiatric disorders. Clin Psychol Rev 2005; 25:559–592.
42. Stepanski EJ. Hypnotics should not be considered for the initial treatment of chronic insomnia. J Clin
Sleep Med 2005; 1(2):125–128.
43. Spielman AJ, Caruso LS, Glovinsky PB. A behavioral perspective on insomnia treatment. Psychiatr
Clin North Am 1987; 10:541–553.
44. Morin CM, Blais F, Savard J. Are changes in beliefs and attitudes related to sleep improvements in
the treatment of insomnia? Behav Res Ther 2002;40:741–752.
c30 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:10 Char Count=
Bruce Rybarczyk
Department of Psychology, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, Virginia, U.S.A.
Haley R. Dillon
Department of Psychology, The University of Alabama, Tuscaloosa, Alabama, U.S.A.
INTRODUCTION
By the mid 1980s, cognitive-behavior therapy for insomnia (CBTi) was a mature science in many
respects (1). A substantial literature had accumulated that boasted strong, durable treatment
effects with rigorous methodology.
However, the bulk of the research to that point had focused on primary insomnia in
young and middle-aged adults. Anticipating possible diminished efficacy and potential safety
concerns, individuals with hypnotic-dependent insomnia, individuals with sleep active comor-
bid conditions, and older adults with insomnia were systematically screened from nearly all
of these studies. It was difficult to justify allocating scarce healthcare/scientific resources when
confronted by gloomy outcome expectations.
It is now clear that young adult and middle-aged, primary insomnia accounts for a
narrow sliver of the insomnia population (see chapter 1 in this volume). Neglecting more
complex, severe insomnia also ignores the most needy patients. The literature that has emerged
on comorbid and late-life insomnia is now about two decades old and has soundly refuted the
apprehensions that denied CBTi treatment to so many in the past.
The present chapter will review the literature in the domains of CBTi for comorbid and
late-life insomnia with the intent of estimating clinical efficacy. We will also assess the method-
ological rigor of these studies to gauge the confidence of their findings. To identify the relevant
literature, we searched PsycINFO and MEDLINE for the years 1965 through 2007. We also
examined the citations from retrieved studies to identify additional articles.
COMORBID INSOMNIA
General Characteristics
Until recent years chronic insomnia has been classified into two broad categories: as either
a ‘primary’ disorder or one that is ‘secondary’ to a primary medical or psychiatric disorder
(2). The different diagnostic systems for sleep disorders and treatment recommendations for
insomnia reflected this basic dichotomy. When a primary medical or psychiatric disorder was
present, the standard approach was for treatment to be aimed at the primary disorder, with the
expectation that insomnia will improve once the primary disorder remits. A study of specific
treatment recommendations made by sleep specialists for patients with insomnia confirmed
that this approach was present in actual practice (3).
However, a rapid shift in this clinical logic coincided with the beginning of the 21st cen-
tury. In a 2000 book chapter, Lichstein (4) outlined the difficulty of determining whether various
coexisting medical and psychiatric conditions predate and/or exacerbate the insomnia. Further-
more, beginning in that same year with Lichstein, Wilson and Johnson’s (5) study, randomized
controlled trials with state-of-the-art behavioral treatments were expanded to populations that
were previously overlooked on the grounds that they had secondary insomnia. In the majority
of these studies the issue of whether the medical or psychiatric condition predated the insomnia
c30 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:10 Char Count=
was not addressed in the inclusion criteria (2). Finally, a 2005 State-of-the-Science Conference (6)
recommended that secondary insomnia be removed from the clinical taxonomy and be replaced
with the more descriptive diagnostic term, comorbid insomnia. Similarly, treatment guidelines
have also shifted such that behavioral methods are recommended as a first-line of treatment
for all chronic insomnias, regardless of the presence of a comorbid medical and/or psychiatric
condition. The behavioral treatment literature in support of this paradigm shift will be outlined
and summarized in the next sections.
Literature Search
We identified 21 published behavioral intervention studies between the years 1989 and 2007.
Three additional case studies were published between 1980 and 1990 and four case series
studies have been published since 1994. These seven papers were not included in the count of
21 because they were not designed as a priori studies. Another paper that focused on caffeine
reduction among HIV patients (7) was not included in the count because it employed a single
sleep hygiene treatment strategy. Among the 21 studies, 11 were classified as randomized
clinical trials (RCT, Table 1), with the remainder being quasi-experimental designs, multiple
case studies, or multiple baseline case studies.
Unlike the gradual integration of older adults into the CBTi literature (see below), 15 of the
21 studies have occurred since the year 2000. Consequently, it may not be an exaggeration to say
that the comorbid insomnia subset, largely ignored until the 21st century, is now receiving the
majority of attention in CBTi research. We will likely to see a wide range of RCT studies applied
to different comorbid conditions in the near future and this will be a welcome contribution to
the empirical evidence and will hopefully translate into higher levels of behavioral treatment
in the clinical setting.
Methodology
Studies of CBTi for comorbid insomnia have employed the full spectrum of methods, including
single case studies (e.g., 8–10), multiple case designs (11–15), a large uncontrolled trial (16), a
large trial with a convenience sample as a control group (17), RCTs with no-treatment control
groups (Table 1), and RCTs with a placebo treatment or other treatment comparisons (Table 1).
There have been four additional case-series reports of treatment cases at sleep clinics with a
range of comorbidities, usually of a psychiatric nature (18–21).
A variety of types of comorbid conditions have been targeted in the RCTs. In the domain
of medical comorbidity, these studies have included mixed chronic illnesses (5,22,23), chronic
pain (24), cancer (17,25,26), coronary artery disease (27), osteoarthritis (27), fibromyalgia (28),
Alzheimer’s disease (29) and pulmonary disease (27). In the domain of psychiatric comorbidity,
only two RCT studies have been published and both of those included individuals with or
recovering from alcoholism (30,32). Additional, uncontrolled studies included crime victims
with posttraumatic stress disorder (16) and individuals with depression and insomnia (15).
Accordingly, there is a critical need in CBTi research with comorbid insomnia to address the
treatment efficacy of psychiatric comorbidities, including but not limited to anxiety disorders,
bipolar disorder, eating disorders and substance abuse disorders.
Most of the RCT studies have been conducted since multicomponent CBTi was established
as the “gold standard” for intervention, so all but the two oldest RCTs used this omnibus treat-
ment approach. The specific components within this omnibus approach were fairly uniform,
using a combination of stimulus control, sleep restriction, sleep hygiene recommendations,
guidelines for discontinuing or reducing hypnotics, and cognitive methods (31). The one excep-
tion was relaxation training, which was not included in three of nine multicomponent RCTs
(24,25,28). The two earliest RCT studies both employed single modality relaxation training inter-
ventions (32,33). The RCT with Alzheimer’s patients (29) also employed a necessarily different
treatment model, which included caregiver administered changes in activities, sleep windows
and light exposure (using a light box). Among the nonRCT studies, multicomponent CBTi
was also the norm, but one older study employed only stimulus control and sleep restriction
(13). Three studies tested home treatments as comparison groups for in-person multicompo-
nent treatments. These included relaxation training with audiotapes (22), multicomponent CBT
delivered via videotape and book instruction (23), and CBT book instruction alone (30).
c30 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:10 Char Count=
The nine RCT intervention studies employing multicomponent treatment were split rela-
tively evenly between group and individual treatment sessions, with the quantity of treatment
ranging between four one-hour individual sessions (5) to as many as eight two-hour group
sessions (27). Group treatments are necessarily longer due to the support and interactive con-
tent of the classes.
Therapists in the RCTs have ranged from behavioral sleep medicine clinicians (16), psy-
chologists (22,27), masters level mental health professionals (14,25,26) and psychology graduate
students (5). All of the group interventions have been led by two clinicians, either two psycholo-
gists (22,27) or a psychologist and a graduate student (16). All of the studies delivered treatment
in conventional health care settings, with the exception of the self-help interventions (22,23,30)
and the innovative caregiver-administered intervention for Alzheimer’s patients (29). This inter-
vention included six one-hour in-home sessions delivered by geropsychologists experienced in
behavioral interventions for dementia patients and their caregivers.
Outcomes and Meta-Analyses (MA)
As above, the review of outcomes will focus mainly on the RCT findings for multicomponent
interventions. Uncontrolled studies are reviewed in a limited way and case studies are not
addressed. We were only able to locate a quasi MA for CBTi for comorbid insomnia, published
in 2005 (34). Effect sizes were calculated and presented in a table of the literature, along with
a systematic review of current and recommended research in this area. Overall, the effect sizes
for the RCT studies for self-report variables are in the “moderate” to “strong” range, based
on Cohen’s (35) criteria, and are generally consistent with those reported in MA for CBTi
for primary insomnia (e.g., 36). Thus far, no studies with null findings for most or all sleep
variables have been reported in the small body of CBTi literature with comorbid insomnia. One
cautionary note is that not all studies employed intent-to-treat analyses, so the effect sizes may
be somewhat inflated.
As has been shown with larger MA examining the efficacy of CBTi for primary insomnia
(e.g., 36) these effect sizes were strongest with sleep efficiency, wake after sleep onset and sleep
quality ratings and absent or smaller for increases in total sleep time. Additionally, as has been
the case with primary insomnia (36), these effect sizes were largely maintained at long-term
follow-up assessments, ranging from three months to one year in duration. Taken together,
these findings suggest that CBTi treatment for comorbid insomnia is both highly effective and
robust in its long-term effects on subjective evaluations of sleep.
The findings are much more sparse and inconclusive when it comes to objective measures
of sleep. Among the six RCT studies that employed objective outcome measures (i.e., either
actigraphy or polysomnography) three found weaker effects compared to self-report (24,28,29)
and three found no effects (22,26,30). Only one of the RCT studies employed polysomnography
and that study found no significant effects (26). In contrast, two uncontrolled studies (13,14)
employed polysomnography measures and both found significant changes on sleep parameters.
One of the studies (14) had effect sizes that were 50% smaller compared to sleep diary (though
still in the “strong” range) and the other had effect sizes that were comparable to those obtained
for self-report for three chronic pain subjects in the study (13). The fact that a number of
RCT studies have failed to obtain actigraphy outcomes may be partially due to the limitations
of the scoring algorithms for particular populations, as some evidence suggests that it may be
suboptimal as a measurement of insomnia in older adults (37) and other specialized populations.
In the comorbid CBTi literature, systematic comparisons of different intervention types
are not possible because 9 of 11 of the RCTs employed similar, multicomponent interventions.
The two studies employing relaxation interventions (32,33) did obtain strong effects, though
outcomes were only measured and analyzed for limited variables. In the three studies which
included a self-administered CBTi treatment group, all three found significant effects for self-
help treatment, with some additional benefits for professional-led treatment compared to self-
help. In the Rybarczyk et al. (22) and Rybarczyk et al. (23) studies, the rate of clinically significant
improvement was approximately 50% greater for the in-person treatment compared to self-
help treatment. In the Currie et al. (30) study with recovering alcoholics, the outcomes were
comparable at posttreatment but professional-led treatment was superior at the six-month
follow-up on a measure of clinical significance.
Based on a review of the effect sized calculated in the Smith et al. MA (34), it is apparent
that the three case series studies show smaller effect sizes than most of the RCTs. This is most
c30 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:10 Char Count=
likely due to the fact that relative to carefully screened and incentivized research subjects,
“real world” clients often have complicating factors (e.g., lower motivation, lack of treatment
adherence) that attenuate treatment efficacy. Also important to note is that in most studies
individuals who were using hypnotics were not excluded and this variable was shown to have
no impact on the efficacy of the intervention. In fact, several studies determined that hypnotic
usage was reduced as a result of the intervention (26,30).
It has been hypothesized that the bi-directional relationship between insomnia and day-
time functioning is heightened in comorbid insomnia, for both medical and psychiatric con-
ditions (2). For example, it has been shown that in addition to insomnia being aggravated by
increased pain, pain is also increased following nights of insomnia (38). Accordingly, most of the
11 RCT studies hypothesized that secondary daytime medical, mental health and quality of life
benefits would follow from improved sleep. In general, secondary findings have been limited to
the RCTs addressing insomnia in cancer and Alzheimer’s patients. The Quesnel et al. (14) study
demonstrated that cancer patients receiving CBTi showed improvements in mood, general and
physical fatigue, and global and cognitive dimensions of quality of life. The Savard et al. (26)
study showed that cancer patients in the treatment group had lower depression and anxiety
as well as greater global quality of life compared to controls. The one study with Alzheimer’s
patients (29) showed that treatment group participants had reduced symptoms of depression
and greater levels of activity compared to controls.
In contrast, in the two studies by Rybarczyk and colleagues (22,27), assessing an array of
secondary outcome measures that were relevant to chronic illness, only a single global rating
of daytime effects of insomnia produced significant effects in one of the two studies (27).
Currie et al. (24) found no secondary benefits among chronic pain patients and Edinger et al.
(28) only found pain benefits in his fibromyalgia sample for the sleep hygiene only treatment
condition compared to controls. Although the RCTs produced inconsistent findings, all of the
seven uncontrolled studies that included daytime functioning measures reported significant
findings. However, given the uncontrolled nature of these studies, it is likely that regression to
the mean and other threats to internal validity are responsible for a portion of the variance in
these findings.
LATE-LIFE INSOMNIA
General Characteristics
Definition
Insomnia is more prevalent, more severe, more chronic, and more impairing in older adults
than in any other age group (39–42). Late-life insomnia is simply defined as insomnia occurring
in older adults. There is no standard age cut-off for qualifying, but 60 years is commonly used.
It may also be salient to acknowledge that no CBTi study has distinguished between late-
life insomnia that initiates in late-life versus chronic middle-aged insomnia that persists into
later life. The clinical implications of this distinction are unknown (43).
Literature Search
We identified 41 articles between the years 1974 and 2007 and a frequency distribution of their
publication dates are given in Figure 1. The level of interest in CBTi for late-life insomnia is
somewhat misleading from this figure. The first six publications either included sleep as a sec-
ondary outcome or did not focus on insomnia in older adults, they just did not exclude them and
typically included correlations between age and outcome to assess age as a moderating variable.
The first study to exclusively focus on CBTi in older adults was published in 1983 (44). The first
RCT with an active control group was published in 1999 (45). During the 1990s, interest leveled
off to about one article a year and that rate about doubled in the first five years of the 21st century.
The prorated rate of publication during the years 2005 to 2007 is greater than two per year.
Five of the 41 studies were on comorbid insomnia in older adults (5,22,23,27,29). These
fit equally well in the comorbid section and the late-life section. We arbitrarily chose to include
them in the comorbid section above and have omitted them from the reporting of outcomes
below to avoid redundant discussion.
c30 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:10 Char Count=
8
# publications
0
∗
70-74 75-79 80-84 85-89 90-94 95-99 00-04 05-07
5 year period
Figure 1 Publication count of the 41 CBT for insomnia studies in five-year periods. ∗ Signifies that the last period,
05–07, is only three years.
Methodology
A variety of methodologies have been used to study late-life insomnia including case studies
(e.g., 46,47), single subject experimental designs (e.g., 48,49), RCTs with wait-list control groups
(Table 2), and RCTs with placebo or other treatment comparisons (Table 2).
A variety of types of late-life insomnia have been targeted including primary insomnia
(e.g., 50,51), comorbid insomnia (e.g., discussed in the above section), hypnotic-dependent
insomnia (e.g., 52,53), and insomnia in caregivers (e.g., 54,55).
Therapists have mostly been experienced behavioral sleep medicine clinicians (e.g., 48,50)
or psychology graduate students (e.g., 5,56). One study trained nondoctoral counselors and a
social worker to present CBTi (57).
Most of the studies delivered treatment in conventional health care settings, but two
studies were distinctive. One presented CBTi in primary care (58) and the other in a rural
clinic (57).
Outcome Studies
For purposes of efficiency, this discussion will not consider case studies or single-subject design.
Excluding the several comorbid insomnia studies reported above, we found six studies that
included placebo or treatment comparison controls and 10 studies with a wait list control.
A summary of these studies is given in Table 2. This discussion will emphasize the most
methodologically rigorous of these studies, those with the placebo or treatment comparison
control.
To get a sense of which treatments have received the most attention, we did a frequency dis-
tribution among these 16 studies. Stimulus control was the most common (12 studies included
a form of this treatment), sleep hygiene, which was usually included as a secondary treat-
ment component, was next (11 studies), and other treatments in decreasing order were sleep
restriction/compression (8 studies), relaxation (8 studies), and cognitive therapy (5 studies).
Thorough descriptions of these clinical procedures are available elsewhere (59). Every study
used sleep diaries as a dependent measure. Actigraphy was also included in one study and
polysomnography in six studies.
Of the six placebo/treatment comparison studies, the study by Freidman et al. (60) had the
weakest results. The two active treatment groups, sleep restriction fortified with sleep hygiene
c30 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:10 Char Count=
Morin et al. (45) 1 MT (SH, SC, SR, SD SD: 1 = 2 = 3 > 4 No between group
CT) PSG PSG: 1 = 2 = 3 comparisons at
2 PCT SII 1=2=4 follow-up
3 MT/PCT CS 3>4
4 PL SII: 1 = 3 > 2 = 4
CS: 1 = 3 > 4 (SE)
1 = 2 = 3 (SE)
2 = 4 (SE)
1 = 2 = 3 > 4 (SII)
Morin et al. (63) 1 MW SD SD: 2 = 3 > 1 SD: ns
2 MT (SH, SC, SR, PSG PSG: ns PSG: ns
CT) MED MED: 3 > 1 = 2 MED: ns
3 MW/MT
Sivertsen et al. (64) 1 MT (SH, SC, SR, SD SD: ns SD: 1 > 2 (TWT)
CT, RE) PSG PSG: 1 > 2 = 3 (TWT, PSG: 1 > 2 (TWT,
2 PCT CS SWS) SWS, SE)
3 PL 1 > 3 (SE) CS: 1 > 2
1 = 2 (SE) No follow-up data for
2 = 3 (SE) group 3
CS: 1 > 2
and sleep restriction fortified with sleep hygiene and a daytime nap, demonstrated a stronger
effect than sleep hygiene alone at posttreatment and only on self-reported sleep efficiency. All
other diary and actigraphy results at posttreatment and follow-up were nonsignificant. Indeed,
had a Bonferroni correction been applied to control for testing multiple measures, this one
significant effect would have been nullified. These results are particularly discouraging because
sleep hygiene is a weak unitary treatment (61).
Lichstein et al. (62) observed modest self-reported sleep gains at posttreatment and follow-
up associated with their two active treatments, sleep compression (a method similar to sleep
restriction) and relaxation, compared to placebo treatment. No significant PSG effects were
demonstrated at follow-up and PSG data were not collected at posttreatment. Clinical signifi-
cance markers favored sleep compression. As was predicted, differential treatment effects were
associated with presence or absence of daytime impairment. Subjects with high daytime fatigue
(implying they needed more sleep) responded better to relaxation and patients with low daytime
fatigue (implying they did not need more sleep) responded better to sleep compression.
In a recent effectiveness study, McCrae et al. (57) utilized existing providers in rural health
clinics to deliver brief multicomponent behavior therapy (stimulus control, sleep restriction,
and relaxation) or sleep hygiene education. The group receiving multicomponent therapy fared
better at posttreatment on subjective sleep reports (sleep latency and sleep efficiency) and clinical
significance markers than participants receiving sleep hygiene education alone. Although no
follow-up data were reported, these initial results support the feasibility and value of providing
brief behavioral interventions for insomnia in primary care settings.
When compared to a placebo group at posttreatment, Morin et al. (45) observed signif-
icant improvements in self-reported sleep for all three active treatment groups, CBT alone,
pharmacotherapy alone, and a combination of the two. Only the combined treatment group
performed better than the placebo on posttreatment PSGs. While there were no significant
differences among the three active treatment conditions, a trend was noted for the combined
treatment to result in slightly greater improvements on sleep diary and PSG data than either
c30 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:10 Char Count=
treatment component alone. However, despite these initial gains, long-term outcomes were
highly variable in the combined treatment group and some participants reported significantly
worse sleep patterns at follow-up. Participants in the pharmacotherapy only group gradually
lost sleep gains over time, while those in the CBT only group maintained improvements in
sleep up to 24-months later. Also of note, participants in the combined treatment and CBT
only conditions rated themselves as significantly more improved at posttreatment, more satis-
fied, and having less interference with daytime functioning than participants in the other two
conditions.
Another study conducted by Morin et al. (63) tested the efficacy of three interventions for
benzodiazepine discontinuation; supervised medication taper alone, CBT alone, and a combi-
nation of medication tapering and CBT. In regards to benzodiazepine reduction, the combined
treatment group had significantly more medication-free patients at posttreatment than either
CBT alone or medication taper alone conditions. The combined treatment group also had
more medication-free patients at 3- and 12-month follow-ups, although group differences were
no longer statistically significant. PSG data demonstrated sleep gains at posttreatment for all
three groups, with no differences between treatment conditions. However, participants receiv-
ing insomnia-specific treatments (either CBT alone or CBT combined with medication taper)
reported greater improvements in self-reported sleep than the medication taper only condition.
Interestingly, all three conditions showed additional gains in self-reported sleep from posttreat-
ment to follow-up, though no significant differences were observed between groups. These
results suggest that improvements in sleep may not be noticeable until several months after
discontinuing or reducing medication use.
Sivertsen et al. (64) compared CBT with pharmacotherapy and placebo treatment. At
six-weeks posttreatment, there were no differences between the three groups on self-reported
sleep, although PSG data favored the CBT condition on most sleep variables. The CBT group
demonstrated greater increases in PSG-recorded slow wave sleep compared to pharmacother-
apy or placebo conditions, while the pharmacotherapy condition actually showed decreases
in slow wave sleep at posttreatment. Six-month follow-up data for the two active treatments
indicated that the pharmacotherapy group maintained sleep gains, but the CBT group pro-
duced significantly better results on all self-reported and PSG outcome variables except for
total sleep time. Clinical significance markers also favored CBT at both posttreatment and
follow-up.
Daytime functioning is plausibly expected to profit from sleep improvement but it is
not always measured before and after treatment. Only three of the active controlled studies
assessed changes in daytime functioning. One study reported substantial daytime functioning
gains accruing to CBTi (45) and two others did not (60,62).
Most of the studies reviewed above included a multicomponent treatment condition that
consisted of several behavioral and cognitive techniques for addressing insomnia. Of the stud-
ies comparing specific interventions, two trials found that behavioral treatments were more
effective than sleep hygiene alone, although neither study included a placebo or additional
control group for comparison (57,60). Lichstein et al. (62) also reported sleep gains for two
different behavioral treatments, with stronger effects for sleep compression. The two trials that
compared psychological treatments with pharmacotherapy found conflicting results for phar-
macotherapy conditions, but both found positive treatment effects for CBT (45,65). In particular,
the study by Morin et al. (45) raises question as to the lasting benefits of combining CBT with
pharmacotherapy and whether the addition of pharmacotherapy might undermine the long-
term effectiveness of CBT. A trial focused on chronic users of benzodiazepine medication found
that supervised medication tapering is safe and effective with older adults and suggested that
combining CBT with a medication taper may facilitate benzodiazepine discontinuation by tar-
geting insomnia and withdrawal symptoms (63). Taken together, the most consistent finding
across these controlled studies is that including components of CBTi when treating late-life
insomnia generally produces statistically and clinically significant improvements in subjective
sleep that are well maintained over time. More research is needed to determine the individual
components of CBTi that are responsible for these treatment effects and to clarify the risks and
benefits of using pharmacotherapy by itself or in conjunction with other treatments for late-life
insomnia.
c30 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:10 Char Count=
Meta-Analyses
Meta-Analyses (MA) have the ability to digest large numbers of articles efficiently and provide
a broad perspective of the field from a quantitative perspective. The three MA presented below
are all based on self-reported sleep data.
One of the first MA on CBT for insomnia found that treatment effects did not significantly
vary with age (36). Two MA focused on CBT for older adults with insomnia. The first reported
results for 13 studies (65). Effect sizes based on therapeutic increment above control group
changes found the largest effect for WASO, d = 0.61, the smallest effect for TST, d = 0.15,
with NWAK and SOL intermediate. More recently, a MA compared CBT effects on insomnia in
middle-aged and older individuals (66). There was no significant difference in treatment effects
in these two age groups for sleep quality ratings, SOL, or WASO. The younger group exhibited
stronger treatment effects for TST and SE.
To summarize the MA findings, CBTi for late-life insomnia is often as effective as with
younger adults. Differential effectiveness has been observed indicating greater treatment effects
are sometimes obtained in younger adults. Interestingly, sleep maintenance insomnia (charac-
terized by WASO), which may be the most common form of insomnia in older adults, does not
exhibit diminished responsiveness in older samples.
CONCLUSIONS
CBTi for comorbid and late-life insomnia is effective, and the pattern of characteristics in these
two literatures is very similar. The following conclusions apply equally well to both domains.
r Strong insomnia efficacy is consistently demonstrated.
r There exists good methodological rigor in many of these studies including well- controlled
RCTs.
r There is good long-term maintenance of sleep effects.
r Self-reported sleep improvement is often stronger than objective findings. This may be
disquieting to researchers but is good news for patients.
r There is inconsistent improvement in daytime functioning associated with sleep improve-
ment.
Overall, we strongly conclude that earlier reluctance to apply CBT to comorbid and late-
life insomnia was not justified and deprived patients of effective treatment.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENT
Preparation of this chapter was supported in part by National Institute on Drug Abuse grant
DA13574 awarded to the first author and National Institute of Aging grant AG017491 awarded
to the second author.
REFERENCES
1. Lichstein KL, Fischer SM. Insomnia. In: Hersen M, Bellack AS, eds. Handbook of clinical behavior
therapy with adults. New York, NY: Plenum Press, 1985:319–352.
2. Stepanski E, Rybarczyk B. Emerging research on the treatment and etiology of secondary or comorbid
insomnia. Sleep Med Rev 2006; 10:7–18.
3. Buysse D, Reynolds C, Kupfer D, et al. Effects of diagnosis on treatment recommendations in chronic
insomnia—a report from the APA/NIMH DSM-IV field trial. Sleep 1997; 20;542–552.
4. Lichstein KL. Secondary insomnia. In: Lichstein KL, Morin CM eds. Treatment of Late-life Insomnia.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications Inc., 2000.
5. Lichstein KL, Wilson NM, Johnson CT. Psychological treatment of secondary insomnia. Psychol Aging
2000; 15:232–240.
6. State-of-the-Science Panel. National Institutes of Health State of the Science Conference Statement:
Manifestations and management of chronic insomnia in adults, June 13–15, 2005. Sleep 2005; 28:1049–
1057.
7. Dreher HM. The effect of caffeine reduction on sleep quality and well being in persons with HIV. J
Psychosom Res 2003; 54:191–198.
8. Morin CM, Kowatch RA, O’Shanick G. Sleep restriction for the inpatient treatment of insomnia. Sleep
1990; 13:183–186.
c30 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:10 Char Count=
9. Stam HJ, Bultz BD. The treatment of severe insomnia in a cancer patient. J Behav Ther Exp Res 1986;
17:33–37.
10. Varni JW. Behavioral treatment of disease-related chronic insomnia in a hemophiliac. J Behav Ther
Exp Psychiatry 1980; 11:143–145.
11. Davidson JR, Waisberg JL, Brundage MD, et al. Nonpharmacologic group treatment of insomnia: A
preliminary study with cancer survivors. Psychooncology 2001; 10:389–397.
12. DeViva JC, Zayfert C, Pigeon WR, et al. Treatment of residual insomnia after CBT for PTSD: Case
studies. J Trauma Stress 2005; 18:155–159.
13. Morin CM, Kowatch RA, Wade JB. Behavioral management of sleep disturbances secondary to chronic
pain. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 1989; 20:295–302.
14. Quesnel C, Savard J, Simard S, et al. Efficacy of cognitive-behavioral therapy for insomnia in women
treated for nonmetastatic breast cancer. J Consult Clin Psychol 2003; 71:189–200.
15. Taylor DJ, Lichstein KL, Weinstock J, et al. A pilot study of cognitive-behavioral therapy of insomnia
in people with mild depression. Behav Ther 2007; 38(1):49–57.
16. Krakow B, Johnston L, Melendrez D. An open-label trial of evidence-based cognitive behavior therapy
for nightmares and insomnia in crime victims with PTSD. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158(12):2043–2047.
17. Simiet R, Deck R, Conta-Marx B. Sleep management training for cancer patients with insomnia.
Supportive Care Cancer 2004; 12:176–183.
18. Dashevsky BA, Kramer M. Behavioral treatment of chronic insomnia in psychiatrically ill patients. J
Consult Clin Psychol 1998; 59:693–699.
19. Morawetz, D. Depression and insomnia: Which comes first? Aus J Counsel Psychol 2001; 3(1):19–24.
20. Morin CM, Stone J, McDonald K, et al. Psychological management of insomnia: A clinical replication
series with 100 patients. Behav Ther 1994; 25:291–309.
21. Perlis ML, Sharpe M, Smith MT, et al. Behavioral treatment of insomnia: Treatment outcome and the
relevance of medical and psychiatric morbidity. J Behav Med 2001; 24:281–296.
22. Rybarczyk B, Lopez M, Benson R, et al. Efficacy of two behavioral treatment programs for comorbid
geriatric insomnia. Psychol Aging 2002; 17:288–298.
23. Rybarczyk B, Lopez M, Schelble K, et al. Home-based video CBT for comorbid geriatric insomnia: A
pilot study using secondary data analyses. Behav Sleep Med 2005; 3:158–175.
24. Currie SR, Wilson KG, Pontefract AJ, et al. Cognitive-behavioral treatment of insomnia secondary to
chronic pain. J Consult Clin Psychol 2000; 69:50–57.
25. Epstein DR, Dirksen SR. Randomized trial of a cognitive-behavioral intervention for insomnia in
breast cancer survivors. Oncol Nurs Forum 2007; 34(5):E51–E59.
26. Savard J, Simard S, Ivers H, et al. Randomized study on the efficacy of cognitive-behavioral therapy
for insomnia secondary to breast cancer, part I: Sleep and psychological effects. J Clin Oncol 2005;
23(25):6083–6096.
27. Rybarczyk B, Stepanski E, Fogg L, et al. A placebo-controlled test of cognitive-behavioral therapy for
comorbid insomnia in older adults. J Consult Clin Psychol 2005; 73:1164–1174.
28. Edinger JD, Wohlgemuth WK, Krystal AD, et al. Behavioral insomnia therapy for fibromyalgia patients:
A randomized clinical trial. Arch Intern Med 2005; 165(21):2527–2535.
29. McCurry SM, Gibbons LE, Logsdon RG, et al. Nighttime insomnia treatment and education for
Alzheimer’s disease: A randomized, controlled trial. J Am Geriatr Soc 2005; 53:793–802.
30. Currie SR, Clark S, Hodgins DC, et al. Randomized controlled trial of brief cognitive-behavioral
interventions for insomnia in recovering alcoholics. Addiction 2004; 9:1121–1132.
31. Morin CM. Insomnia: Psychological assessment and management. New York: Guilford, 1993.
32. Greeff AP, Conradie WS. Use of progressive relaxation training for chronic alcoholics with insomnia.
Psychol Rep 1998; 82:407–412.
33. Cannici J, Malcolm R, & Peek LA. Treatment of insomnia in cancer patients using muscle relaxation
training. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 1983; 14:251–256.
34. Smith MT, Huang MI, Manber R. Cognitive behavior therapy for chronic insomnia occurring within
the context of medical and psychiatric disorders. Clin Psychol Rev 2005; 25(5):559–592.
35. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. New York, NY: Academic Press,
1977:274–281.
36. Morin CM, Culbert JP, Schwartz SM. Nonpharmacological interventions for insomnia: A meta-analysis
of treatment efficacy. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151:1172–1180.
37. Sivertsen B, Omvik S, Havik OE, et al. A comparison of actigraphy and polysomnography in older
adults treated for chronic primary insomnia. Sleep 2006; 29:1353–1358
38. Roizenblatt S, Moldofsky H, Benedito-Silva A, et al. Alpha sleep characteristics in fibromyalgia.
Arthritis Rheum 2001; 44:222–230.
39. Lichstein KL, Durrence HH, Riedel BW, et al. Epidemiology of sleep: Age, gender, and ethnicity.
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, 2004.
c30 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:10 Char Count=
40. Morgan K. Sleep and aging. In: Lichstein KL, Morin CM, eds. Treatment of late-life insomnia. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage, 2000:3–36.
41. Morphy H, Dunn KM, Lewis M, et al. Epidemiology of insomnia: a longitudinal study in a UK
population. Sleep 2007; 30:274–280.
42. Stewart R, Besset A, Bebbington P, et al. Insomnia comorbidity and impact and hypnotic use by age
group in a national survey population aged 16 to 74 years. Sleep 2006; 29:1391–1397.
43. Morgan K, Clarke D. Longitudinal trends in late-life insomnia: Implications for prescribing. Age
Ageing 1997; 26:179–184.
44. Puder R, Lacks P, Bertelson AD, et al. Short-term stimulus control treatment of insomnia in older
adults. Behav Ther 1983; 14:424–429.
45. Morin CM, Colecchi C, Stone J, et al. Behavioral and pharmacological therapies for late-life insomnia:
A randomized controlled trial. J Am Med Assoc 1999; 281:991–999.
46. Lichstein KL. Sleep compression treatment of an insomnoid. Behav Ther 1988; 19:625–632.
47. McCurry SM, Logsdon RG, Teri L. Behavioral treatment of sleep disturbance in elderly dementia
caregivers. Clin Gerontol 1996; 17(2):35–50.
48. Edinger JD, Hoelscher TJ, Marsh GR, et al. A cognitive-behavioral therapy for sleep-maintenance
insomnia in older adults. Psychol Aging 1992; 7:282–289.
49. Morin CM, Colecchi CA, Ling WD, et al. Cognitive behavior therapy to facilitate benzodiazepine
discontinuation among hypnotic-dependent patients with insomnia. Behav Ther 1995; 26:733–745.
50. Friedman L, Bliwise DL, Yesavage JA, et al. A preliminary study comparing sleep restriction and
relaxation treatments for insomnia in older adults. J Gerontol 1991; 46:P1–P8.
51. Lichstein KL, Johnson RS. Relaxation for insomnia and hypnotic medication use in older women.
Psychol Aging 1993; 8:103–111.
52. Baillargeon L, Landreville P, Verreault R, et al. Discontinuation of benzodiazepines among older
insomniac adults treated with cognitive-behavioural therapy combined with gradual tapering: A
randomized trial. Can Med Assoc J 2003; 169:1015–1020.
53. Cooper TV, Lichstein KL, Aguillard RN. Hypnotic dependent insomnia in an older adult with
addiction-prone personality. Clin Case Studies 2003; 2:247–258.
54. McCrae CS, Tierney CG, McNamara JPH. Behavioral intervention for insomnia: Future directions for
nontraditional caregivers at various stages of care. Clin Gerontol 2005; 29(2):95–114.
55. McCurry SM, Logsdon RG, Vitiello MV, et al. Successful behavioral treatment for reported sleep
problems in elderly caregivers of dementia patients: A controlled study. J Gerontol Psychol Soc Sci
1998; 53B:P122–P129.
56. Morin CM, Azrin NH. Behavioral and cognitive treatments of geriatric insomnia. J Consult Clin
Psychol 1988; 56:748–753.
57. McCrae CS, McGovern R, Lukefahr R, et al. Research evaluating brief behavioral sleep treatments for
rural elderly (RESTORE): A preliminary examination of effectiveness. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2007;
15:979–982.
58. Morgan K, Dixon S, Mathers N, et al. Psychological treatment for insomnia in the management of
long-term hypnotic drug use: A pragmatic randomised controlled trial. Br J Gen Pract 2003; 53:923–
928.
59. Lichstein KL, Morin CM, eds. Treatment of late-life insomnia. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 2000.
60. Friedman L, Benson K, Noda A, et al. An actigraphic comparison of sleep restriction and sleep hygiene
treatments for insomnia in older adults. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 2000; 13:17–27.
61. Stepanski EJ, Wyatt JK. Use of sleep hygiene in the treatment of insomnia. Sleep Med Rev 2003;
7:215–225.
62. Lichstein KL, Riedel BW, Wilson NM, et al. Relaxation and sleep compression for late-life insomnia:
A placebo-controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psychol 2001; 69:227–239.
63. Morin CM, Bastien C, Guay B, et al. Randomized clinical trial of supervised tapering and cognitive
behavior therapy to facilitate benzodiazepine discontinuation in older adults with chronic insomnia.
Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161:332–342.
64. Sivertsen B, Omvik S, Pallesen S, et al. Cognitive behavioral therapy vs zopiclone for treatment of
chronic primary insomnia in older adults: A randomized controlled trial. J Am Med Assoc 2006;
295:2851–2858.
65. Pallesen S, Nordhus IH, Kvale G. Nonpharmacological interventions for insomnia in older adults: A
meta-analysis of treatment efficacy. Psychotherapy 1998; 35:472–482.
66. Irwin MR, Cole JC, Nicassio PM. Comparative meta-analysis of behavioral interventions for insomnia
and their efficacy in middle-aged adults and in older adults 55 + years of age. Health Psychol 2006;
25:3–14.
67. Rybarczyk BD, Stepanski E, Alsten C, et al. Applying cognitive-behavioral treatment to comorbid
insomnia: Successes and challenges. Paper presented at: The Annual Meeting of the American Psy-
chological Association; 2007; San Francisco, CA.
c30 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:10 Char Count=
68. Creti L, Libman E, Bailes S, et al. Effectiveness of cognitive-behavioral insomnia treatment in a com-
munity sample of older individuals: More questions than conclusions. J Clin Psychol Med Settings
2005; 12:153–164.
69. Davies R, Lacks P, Storandt M, et al. Countercontrol treatment of sleep-maintenance insomnia in
relation to age. Psychol Aging 1986; 1:233–238.
70. Engle-Friedman M, Bootzin RR, Hazlewood L, et al. An evaluation of behavioral treatments for
insomnia in the older adult. J Clin Psychol 1992; 48:77–90.
71. Morin CM, Kowatch RA, Barry T, et al. Cognitive-behavior therapy for late-life insomnia. J Consult
Clin Psychol 1993; 61:137–146.
72. Pallesen S, Nordhus IH, Kvale G, et al. Behavioral treatment of insomnia in older adults: An open
clinical trial comparing two interventions. Behav Res Ther 2003; 41:31–48.
73. Riedel BW, Lichstein KL, Dwyer WO. Sleep compression and sleep education for older insomniacs:
Self-help versus therapist guidance. Psychol Aging 1995; 10:54–63.
c31 IHBK059-Sateia March 31, 2010 10:59 Char Count=
INTRODUCTION
Gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) is an amino acid neurotransmitter, first discovered more
than 50 years ago (1). Approximately 30% of synapses in the vertebrate central nervous system
have been show to contain GABA, making it the most abundant inhibitory neurotransmitter.
Consequently it is not surprising that GABAergic neurotransmission plays a key role in the
normal physiological function of the brain, including sleep regulation (2–5), as well as various
pathophysiological sequelae (6). GABA receptors have traditionally been classified according
to pharmacological criteria into three groups, GABAA , GABAB and GABAC receptors. GABAA
and GABAC receptors are ligand-gated ion channels whereas GABAB receptors are G protein-
coupled receptors (7). Molecular cloning studies have revealed the structural basis for this
classification, a heterogeneity hitherto unrealized (particularly in the case of GABAA receptors)
and that GABAC receptors should be considered part of the GABAA receptor family (7,8).
GABAA receptors posses a rich pharmacology with a number of distinct binding sites for
a variety of molecular entities that are separate to that of the agonist GABA. These binding sites
are often, though not exclusively, modulatory such that their occupation leads to an allosteric
modulation of the action of GABA or other agonists. Benzodiazepines, barbiturates, alcohol
and certain anesthetics all bind to GABAA receptors and modulate their activity. From the
perspective of sleep medicine it is clearly the benzodiazepine binding site that has attracted the
most interest. Elucidation of the molecular composition of GABAA receptors has revealed that
compounds that interact with the benzodiazepine site can modulate GABA-induced receptor
activity in a positive or negative fashion.
Members of the GABAA receptor family are the targets for both the traditional benzodi-
azepine hypnotics and as well as the newer non-benzodiazepine “Z” drugs (zaleplon, zolpidem,
zopiclone and eszopiclone) (9). The realization from basic science studies that the GABAA recep-
tor is not a single entity but a family of related receptors has opened the door to the possible
targeting of specific GABAA receptor subtypes in the pursuit of novel therapeutic interven-
tions for sleep and other disorders. This chapter provides an overview of GABAA receptor
pharmacology, with particular reference to classic and newer hypnotics. It also discusses the
ongoing elucidation of GABAA receptor heterogeneity and how this might be relevant to the
understanding of the physiology sleep, its disorders and their treatment. The rich pharmacol-
ogy of this receptor family and the specific distribution patterns and physiological functions
of its members means that there is significant potential for the further development of novel
hypnotics that act at members of this important receptor family.
366 BATESON
their classification into seven isoform groups (␣, , ␥ , ␦, ε, , , ; 1,7,9,16). In mammals there are
six ␣, three , three ␦, one each of ε, and , and three subunit genes, the latter being previously
classified as GABAC receptors (7,8). Alternate splice forms have been described for a number
of GABAA receptor gene transcripts, thereby potentially increasing diversity. Combinatorial
association of these subunits into pentameric receptors results in the production of a large
number of GABAA receptor subtypes. The actual number of GABAA receptor subtypes in
mammalian brain is not currently known, but although the theoretical number of combinations
is immense, evidence exists for a few dozen rather than thousands of different GABAA receptor
subtypes (8,15). ␣ and  subunits commonly occur together with ␥ subunits, although ␦ subunits
do occur in place of a ␥ subunit. The most abundant receptor subtype comprises of two ␣1, two
2 and one ␥ 2 subunit (7,9,16).
Many postsynaptic GABAA receptors contain ␥ 2 subunits, which given the linked role
of these subunits with that of gephyrin in postsynaptic localization is perhaps not surprising
(21). This is not a simple one-to-one correlation, however, as ␥ 2 subunit-containing GABAA
receptors have been shown to be located beyond the immediate postsynaptic area (18) and
different neuronal types exhibit differing GABAA receptor subtypes at postsynaptic sites (15).
In contrast to postsynaptic GABAA receptors, some of the best characterized extrasynaptic
GABAA receptor subtypes possess kinetic properties that allow them to function effectively in
the absence of the high concentrations of GABA seen in the synaptic cleft immediately following
phasic release. Low millimolar concentrations of “ambient” GABA are found extrasynaptically
which arise from spillover or leakage from GABAergic synapses and are not likely to exhibit
the rapid changes in concentration seen within a GABAergic synaptic cleft (16,22). Thus, the
combination of ␣6, 2 or 3, and ␦ subunits, which is found in cerebellar granule cells, and
the ␣42/3␦ combination, which is found in dentate gyrus granule neurones and thalamocor-
tical neurons of the ventro-basal complex, are located extrasynaptically (16,15,24,26,28). These
subunit combinations result in receptors with high affinity for GABA and slow desensitiza-
tion properties. Their high affinity for GABA means that they bind the neurotransmitter even
when it is present at the low concentrations found outside of the synapse. Further, the slow
desensitization properties of some extrasynaptic GABAA receptors means that once they are
activated they produce a longer lasting (tonic) inhibition. These properties are particular to
those receptor subtypes that contain ␣4 or ␣6 subunits in combination with a ␦ subunit (16,22).
It has been estimated that under these particular conditions a greater charge transfer occurs
via such extrasynaptic GABAA receptor-mediated tonic inhibition than that which occurs via
phasic inhibition by postsynaptic GABAA receptors (29). Extrasynaptic GABAA receptors have
been identified that comprise of other subunit combinations, and which exhibit different kinetic
properties; the consequences of this are currently under investigation (18,28).
368 BATESON
benzodiazepine recognition and functional properties in combination with ␣ subunits (37). For
example, in contrast to those containing ␥ 2 or ␥ 3 subunits, ␥ 1-containing receptors show little
affinity for the antagonist flumazenil (Ro15–1788). Further, methyl -carboline-3-carboxylate
(CCM), which binds to GABAA receptors at the benzodiazepine site, is a positive modulator at
some receptor subunit compositions and a negative modulator at others (37). Thus both ␣ and
␥ subunits determine the recognition and functional properties of the benzodiazepine binding
site of the GABAA receptor.
The ␦ subunit has a discrete expression profile and is found in combination with a variety
of ␣ and  subunits in place of a ␥ subunit. ␦ subunit-containing receptor subtypes exhibit an
interesting pharmacology in that they do not bind classical benzodiazepine agonists but play
an important role in the mechanism of action of the natural modulators neurosteroids (25).
Recent studies combining DNA mutagenesis, transgenic technology and psychopharma-
cology have shed light on the role played by specific GABAA receptor subtypes in the phar-
macological profile of drugs that act at GABAA receptors (18,38,39). For example, the sedative
properties of the classical benzodiazepine diazepam are mediated via ␣1 subunit-containing
receptors while its anxiolytic properties are mediated by ␣2 subunit-containing receptors at low
receptor occupancy and both ␣2 and ␣3 subunit-containing receptors at high receptor occu-
pancy (28,38). Thus the principle of targeting a single GABAA receptor subtype (or a limited
number) in order to achieve a specific pharmacological outcome has been established.
It is clear therefore that the subunit combination, as well as the site(s) of expression, both
with respect to cell compartment and cell type, should be taken into account when considering
GABAA receptors as therapeutic targets.
Most classical benzodiazepine hypnotics appear not to differentiate between different GABAA
receptor subtypes, although there are a few studies, which indicate that quazepam and
lormetazepam show a 10- and 3-fold, respectively, selectivity for receptors containing ␣1
subunits (30,44).
Significant advances in our understanding of the mechanisms of action of classical benzo-
diazepines have occurred more than the past 10 years, largely due to use of transgenic animal
studies. Therefore we now have a much clearer, though by no means complete, understanding
of which GABAA receptor subtypes mediate specific aspects of the complex pharmacologi-
cal profile of classical benzodiazepine agonists. Unfortunately, these studies have largely used
diazepam as an exemplar and hence we can only draw conclusions based upon the very similar
pharmacodynamic profile that diazepam shares with the currently prescribed classical benzo-
diazepine hypnotics indicated above.
Two sorts of transgenic studies have shed light on this area: gene knock-out mice, where
the expression of a specific GABAA receptor subunit gene is ablated; and knock-in mice, where
a specific amino acid is altered in one subunit to alter its pharmacology (e.g., to change its
recognition properties from diazepam sensitive to diazepam insensitive) (39). These allow the
analysis of the sedative-hypnotic properties of benzodiazepine-site compounds when combined
with such behavioral tests as locomotor activity and loss of righting reflex (39). However, such
studies are not without problems, particularly in knock-out animals where compensation for
the loss of expression of one GABAA receptor gene has been seen in the form of up regulation
of the expression of other GABAA receptor genes (e.g., 45) making interpretation difficult. Thus
in ␣1-subunit knock-out mice diazepam’s hypnotic action was increased, which was opposite
to the effects of the ␣1 subunit-specific hypnotic zolpidem (39,45). In contrast, using ␣1-subunit
knock-in mice it was clearly demonstrated that the sedative effects of diazepam were mediated
via the ␣1 subunit (46,47). Changes in EEG patterns caused by diazepam during sleep are well
known but experiments with knock-in mice suggested that the ␣1 (48) and ␣3 (49) subunits
do not mediate these particular effects, but there is an involvement of ␣2 subunit-containing
receptors in the diazepam-mediated modulation of delta-wave activity (50).
NON-BENZODIAZEPINE HYPNOTICS
There are a number of nonbenzodiazepine compounds of various structural classes that bind
to the benzodiazepine site of the GABAA receptor and which are very effective hypnotics.
These include the so-called “z-drugs” zaleplon (pyrazolopyrimidine), zopiclone and eszopi-
clone (cyclopyrrolones), and zolpidem (imidazopyridine), as well as others such as indiplon
(pyrazolopyrimidine). Some of these are licensed in the USA (43) but, somewhat controversially,
while the “z-drugs” are licensed in the UK, they are not recommended by the National Institute
for Health and Clinical Excellence (42,51,52).
In general these hypnotics all show selectivity for ␣1-subunit containing GABAA receptors,
although to varying degrees. Data from published studies indicate a range of values for affinity
and potency at defined GABAA receptors subtypes, dependent to an extent on the methodology
used by different laboratories.
Zaleplon shows a ∼10 fold higher affinity for ␣1 subunit-containing receptors over those
containing ␣2 or ␣3 subunits, and a ∼30 fold higher affinity over those containing the ␣5 sub-
unit (53). The difference in zopiclone’s affinity for ␣1 versus ␣2, ␣3 or ␣5 subunit-containing
receptors is less (2–6 fold: 53,54,55). Zolpidem shows the greatest selective affinity for ␣1 subunit-
containing receptors compared to those containing ␣2 (5–24 fold), ␣3 (14–19 fold), or ␣5 (>1000
fold) subunits (32,54). One study reported similar values for zolpidem for ␣1 and ␣3 but not
␣2 subunit-containing receptors where they were unable to detect binding of zolpidem (53).
Although not tested on defined receptor subtypes, the S(+)-enantiomer of zopiclone (eszopi-
clone) displays approximately 50- and 25-fold higher affinities for GABAA receptor benzo-
diazepine binding sites in mouse brain than the racemate zolpidem or the R(+)-enantiomer,
respectively (56).
Studies on the relative potency of these drugs at different receptor subtypes further
demonstrates their selectivity for ␣1 subunit-containing receptors, although the actual val-
ues vary markedly from study to study. Overall, it is clear however, that zaleplon, zopi-
clone, eszopiclone, zolpidem and indiplon are more potent at ␣1 compared to ␣2 (2–9 fold)
c31 IHBK059-Sateia March 31, 2010 10:59 Char Count=
370 BATESON
CONCLUSION
The benzodiazepine site of the GABAA receptor has proven to be a most effective site for the
pharmacological manipulation of sleep. Our growing understanding of this receptor family,
in particular: the pharmacological distinctiveness of GABAA receptor subtypes; the diverse
expression patterns of individual GABAA receptor subtypes; and the emerging physiological
roles of specific GABAA receptors subtypes; all indicate that this receptor family is likely to
provide a rich vein for the development of GABAA receptor-mediated hypnotics in the future.
REFERENCES
1. Owens DF, Kriegstein AR. Is there more to GABA than synaptic inhibition? Nat Rev Neurosci 2002;
3:715–727.
2. Ebert B, Wafford KA, Deacon S. Treating insomnia: Current and investigational pharmacological
approaches. Pharmacol Ther 2006;112:612–629.
3. Franks NP. General anaesthesia: From molecular targets to neuronal pathways of sleep and arousal.
Nat Rev Neuro 2008; 9:370–386.
4. Saper CB, Scammell TE, Lu J. Hypothalamic regulation of sleep and circadian rhythms. Nature 2005;
437:1257–1263.
5. Watson CJ, Soto-Calderon S, Lydic R, et al. Pontine reticular formation (PnO) administration of
hypocretin-1 increases PnO GABA levels and wakefulness. Sleep 2008; 31:453–464.
6. Mohler H. GABAA receptors in central nervous system disease: Anxiety, epilepsy, and insomnia. J
Recept Signal Transduct Res 2006a; 26:731–740.
7. Barnard EA, Skolnick P, Olsen RW, et al. International Union of Pharmacology. XV. Subtypes of
␥ -aminobutyric acid A receptors: Classification on the basis of subunit structure and receptor function.
Pharmacol Rev 1998; 50:291–313.
8. Olsen RW, Sieghart W. International Union of Pharmacology. LXX. Subtypes of ␥ -aminobutyric
acid(A) receptors: Classification on the basis of subunit composition, pharmacology, and function.
Update. Pharmacol Rev 2008; 60:243–260.
9. Bateson AN. The benzodiazepine site of the GABAA receptor: An old target with new potential? Sleep
Med 2004; 5(suppl 1):S9–S15.
10. Unwin N. Refined structure of the nicotinic acetylcholine receptor at 4 A resolution J Mol Biol 2005;
346:967–989.
c31 IHBK059-Sateia March 31, 2010 10:59 Char Count=
372 BATESON
11. Barrera NP, Betts J, You H, et al. Atomic force microscopy reveals the stoichiometry and subunit
arrangement of the ␣43␦ GABAA receptor. Mol Pharmacol 2008; 73:960–967.
12. Nayeem N, Green TP, Martin IL, et al. Quaternary structure of the native GABAA receptor determined
by electron microscopic image analysis. J Neurochem 1994; 62:815–818.
13. Tsang SY, Ng SK, Xu Z, et al. The evolution of GABAA receptor-like genes. Mol Biol Evol 2007;
24:599–610.
14. Cockcroft VB, Osguthorpe DJ, Barnard EA, et al. Ligand-gated ion channels. Homology and diversity.
Mol Neurobiol 1990; 4:129–169.
15. Mohler H. GABA(A) receptor diversity and pharmacology. Cell Tissue Res 2006b; 326:505–516.
16. Farrant M, Nusser Z. Variations on an inhibitory theme: Phasic and tonic activation of GABAA
receptors. Nat Rev Neurosci 2005; 6:215–229.
17. Doble A, Martin IL. Multiple benzodiazepine receptors: No reason for anxiety. Trends Pharmacol Sci
1992; 13:76–81.
18. Mohler H, Fritschy JM, Rudolph U. A new benzodiazepine pharmacology. J Pharmacol Exp Ther
2002; 300:2–8.
19. Laurie DJ, Seeburg PH, Wisden W. The distribution of 13 GABAA receptor subunit mRNAs in the rat
brain. II. Olfactory bulb and cerebellum. J Neurosci 1992; 12:1063–1076.
20. Wisden W, Laurie DJ, Monyer H, et al. The distribution of 13 GABAA receptor subunit mRNAs in the
rat brain. I. Telencephalon, diencephalon, mesencephalon. J Neurosci 1992; 12:1040–1062.
21. Luscher B, Keller CA. Regulation of GABAA receptor trafficking, channel activity, and functional
plasticity of inhibitory synapses. Pharmacol Ther 2004; 102(3):195–221.
22. Wafford K, Ebert, B. Gaboxadol – a new awakening in sleep. Curr Opin Pharmacol 2006; 6:30–36.
23. Yu W, Jiang M, Miralles CP, et al. Gephyrin clustering is required for the stability of GABAergic
synapses. Mol Cell Neurosci 2007; 36:484–500.
24. Bateson AN. Further potential of the GABAA receptor in the treatment of insomnia. Sleep Med 2006;
7(suppl 1):S3–S9.
25. Belelli D, Peden DR, Rosahl TW, et al. Extrasynaptic GABAA receptors of thalamocortical neurons: A
molecular target for hypnotics. J Neurosci 2005; 25:11513–11520.
26. Jia F, Pignataro L, Schofield CM, et al. An extrasynaptic GABAA receptor mediates tonic inhibition in
thalamic VB neurons. J Neurophysiol 2005; 94:4491–4501.
27. Borden LA. GABA transporter heterogeneity: Pharmacology and cellular localization. Neurochem
Int. 1996; 29:335–356.
28. Rudolph U, Mohler H. GABA-based therapeutic approaches: GABAA receptor function. Curr Op
Pharmacol 2006; 6:18–23.
29. Brickley SG, Cull-Candy SG, Farrant M. Development of a tonic form of synaptic inhibition in rat
cerebellar granule cells resulting from persistent activation of GABAA receptors. J Physiol 1996;
497:753–759.
30. Pritchett DB, Sontheimer H, Shivers BD, et al. Importance of a novel GABAA receptor subunit for
benzodiazepine pharmacology. Nature 1989; 338:582–585.
31. Schofield PR, Darlison MG, Fujita N, et al. Sequence and functional expression of the GABAA receptor
shows a ligand-gated receptor super-family. Nature 1987; 328:221–227.
32. Pritchett DB, Seeburg PH. ␥ -aminobutyric acidA receptor alpha 5-subunit creates novel type II ben-
zodiazepine receptor pharmacology. J Neurochem 1990; 54:1802–1804.
33. Pritchett DB, Lüddens H, Seeburg PH. Type I and type II GABAA -benzodiazepine receptors produced
in transfected cells. Science 1989; 245:1389–1392.
34. Pritchett DB, Seeburg PH. ␥ -Aminobutyric acid type A receptor point mutation increases the affinity
of compounds for the benzodiazepine site. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1991;88:1421–1425.
35. Lüddens H, Pritchett DB, Köhler M, et al. Cerebellar GABAA receptor selective for a behavioural
alcohol antagonist. Nature 1990; 346:648–651.
36. Wisden W, Herb A, Wieland H, et al. Cloning, pharmacological characteristics and expression pattern
of the rat GABAA receptor a4 subunit. FEBS Lett 1991; 289:227–230.
37. Bateson AN. Basic pharmacological mechanisms involved in benzodiazepine tolerance and with-
drawal. Curr Pharm Des 2002; 8:5–21.
38. Reynolds DS. The value of genetic and pharmacological approaches to understanding the complexities
of GABAA receptor subtype functions: The anxiolytic effects of benzodiazepines. Pharm Biochem
Behav 2008; 90:37–42.
39. Rudolph U, Mohler H. Analysis of GABAA receptor function and dissection of the pharmacology
of benzodiazepines and general anesthetics through mouse. Ann Rev Pharmacol Toxicol 2004; 44:
475–498.
40. Sigel E, Buhr A. The benzodiazepine binding site of GABAA receptors. Trends Pharmacol Sci 1997;
18:425–429.
c31 IHBK059-Sateia March 31, 2010 10:59 Char Count=
41. Mendelson WB, Roth T, Cassella J, et al. The treatment of chronic insomnia: Drug indications, chronic
use and abuse liability. Sleep Med Rev 2004; 8:7–17.
42. British National Formulary 58. 2010. http://www.bnf.org/
43. Passarella S, Duong MT. Diagnosis and treatment of insomnia. Am J Health Syst Pharm 2008; 65:
27–34.
44. Ozawa M, Nakada Y, Sugimachi K, et al. Interaction of the hypnotic lormetazepam with central
benzodiazepine receptor subtypes omega-1, omega-2 and omega-3. Nippon Yakurigaku Zasshi (Folia
Pharmacol Jap) 1991; 98:399–408
45. Kralic JE, O’Buckley TK, Khisti RT, et al. GABAA receptor ␣1 subunit deletion alters receptor subtype
assembly, pharmacological and behavioral responses to benzodiazepines and zolpidem. Neuropharm
2002; 43:685–694.
46. McKernan RM, Rosahl TW, Reynolds DS, et al. Sedative but not anxiolytic properties of benzodi-
azepines are mediated by the GABAA receptor ␣1 subtype. Nat Neurosci 2000; 3:587–592.
47. Rudolph U, Crestani F, Benke D, et al. Benzodiazepine actions mediated by specific gamma-
aminobutyric acid(A) receptor subtypes. Nature 1999; 401:796–800.
48. Tobler I, Kopp C, Deboer T, et al. Diazepam-induced changes in sleep: Role of the ␣1 GABAA receptor
subtype. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2001; 98:6464–6469.
49. Kopp C, Rudolph U, Keist R, et al. Diazepam-induced changes on sleep and the EEG spectrum in
mice: Role of the ␣3-GABAA receptor subtype. Eur J Neurosci 2003; 17:2226–2230.
50. Kopp C, Rudolph U, Löw K, et al. Modulation of rhythmic brain activity by diazepam: GABAA
receptor subtype and state specificity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2004; 101:3674–3679.
51. NICE. Guidance on the use of zaleplon, zolpidem and zopiclone for the short-term management of
insomnia. 2005; http://www.nice.org.uk/TA077guidance.
52. Nutt DJ. NICE: The National Institute of Clinical Excellence – or Eccentricity? Reflections on the
Z-drugs as hypnotics. J Psychopharmacol 2005; 19:125–127.
53. Damgen K, Luddens H. Zaleplon displays a selectivity to recombinant GABAA receptors different
from zolpidem, zopiclone and benzodiazepines. Neurosci Res Comm 1999; 25:139–148.
54. Smith A, Alder L, Silk J, et al. Effect of a subunit on allosteric modulation of ion channel function
in stably expressed human recombinant ␥ -aminobutyric acidA receptors determined using 36 Cl ion
flux. Mol Pharmacol 2001; 59:1108–1118.
55. Davies M, Newell JG, Derry JM, et al. Characterization of the interaction of zopiclone with
␥ -aminobutyric acid type A receptors. Mol Pharmacol 2000; 58:756–762.
56. Blaschke G, Hempel G, Muller W. Preparative and analytical separation of the zopiclone enantiomers
and determination of their affinity to the benzodiazepine receptor binding site. Chirality 1993; 5:
419–421
57. Petroski RE, Pomeroy JE, Das R, et al. Indiplon is a high-affinity positive allosteric modulator
with selectivity for ␣1 subunit-containing GABAA receptors. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 2006; 317:369–
377.
58. Sanna E, Busonero F, Talani G, et al. Comparison of the effects of zaleplon, zolpidem, and triazolam
at various GABAA receptor subtypes. Eur J Pharmacol 2002; 451:103–110.
59. Fleck MW. Molecular actions of (S)-desmethylzopiclone (SEP-174559), an anxiolytic metabolite of
zopiclone. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 2002; 302:612–618.
60. Foster AC, Pelleymounter MA, Cullen MJ, et al. In vivo pharmacological characterization of indiplon,
a novel pyrazolopyrimidine sedative-hypnotic. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 2004; 311:547–559.
61. Noguchi H, Kitazumi K, Mori M, et al. Binding and neuropharmacological profile of zaleplon, a novel
nonbenzodiazepine sedative/hypnotic. Eur J Pharmacol 2002; 434:21–28.
62. Sanger DJ, Morel E, Perrault G. Comparison of the pharmacological profiles of the hypnotic drugs,
zaleplon and zolpidem. Eur J Pharmacol 1996; 313:35–42.
63. Sullivan SK, Petroski RE, Verge G, et al. Characterization of the interaction of indiplon, a novel
pyrazolopyrimidine sedative-hypnotic, with the GABAA receptor. J Pharmacol Ther 2004; 311;
537–546.
64. Crestani F, Martin JR, Möhler H, et al. Mechanism of action of the hypnotic zolpidem in vivo. Br J
Pharmacol 2000; 131:1251–1254.
65. Rosenberg R, Roth T, Scharf MB, et al. Efficacy and tolerability of indiplon in transient insomnia. J
Clin Sleep Med 2007: 3:374–379.
66. van Rijnsoever C, Tauber M, Choulli MK, et al. Requirement of ␣5-GABAA receptors for the devel-
opment of tolerance to the sedative action of diazepam in mice. J Neurosci 2005; 24:6785–6790.
67. Wafford KA. GABAA receptor subtypes: Any clues to the mechanism of benzodiazepine dependence?
Curr Opin Pharmacol 2005; 5:47–52.
68. Evans SM, Funderburk FR, Griffiths RR. Zolpidem and triazolam in humans: Behavioral and subjec-
tive effects and abuse liability. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1990; 255:1246–1255.
c31 IHBK059-Sateia March 31, 2010 10:59 Char Count=
374 BATESON
69. Hajak G, Muller WE, Wittchen HU, et al. Abuse and dependence potential for the non-benzodiazepine
hypnotics zolpidem and zopiclone: A review of case reports and epidemiological data. Addiction 2003;
98:1371–1378.
70. Roth T. A physiological basis for the evolution of pharmacotherapy for insomnia. J Clin Psychiatry
2007; 68(suppl 5):13–18.
71. Brown N, Kerby J, Bonnert TP, et al. Pharmacological characterization of a novel cell line expressing
human ␣43␦ GABAA receptors. Br J Pharmacol 2002; 136:965–974.
72. Davies PA, Hanna MC, Hales TG, et al. Insensitivity to anaesthetic agents conferred by a class of
GABAA receptor subunit. Nature 1997; 385:820–823.
73. Belelli D, Lambert JJ. Neurosteroids: Endogenous regulators of the GABAA receptor. Nat Rev Neuro
2005; 6:565–575.
74. Damianisch K, Rupprecht R, Lancel M. The influence of subchronic administration of the neurosteroid
allopregnanolone on sleep in the rat. Neuropsychopharm 2001; 25:576–584.
75. Steiger A, Trachsel L, Guldner J, et al. Neurosteroid pregnenolone induces sleep-EEG changes in man
compatible with inverse agonistic GABAA -receptor modulation. Brain Res 1993; 615:267–274.
76. Hosie AM, Wilkins ME, da Silva HM, et al. Endogenous neurosteroids regulate GABAA receptors
through two discrete transmembrane sites. Nature 2006; 444:486–449.
c32 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 13:58 Char Count=
INTRODUCTION
Many different agents are prescribed to treat insomnia. These agents represent at least 12
different medication classes. Among these classes are agents which mediate their therapeutic
effect by binding to sites on the gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) type A receptor complex,
thereby enhancing the inhibition that occurs when GABA binds to this receptor (1,2). These
agents represent a set of chemically related compounds referred to as benzodiazepines and
include triazolam, temazepam, flurazepam, diazepam, alprazolam, lorazepam, and oxazepam
(2) (Table 1). The benzodiazepines can have a diverse set of effects via their modulation of
GABAA receptors. These effects include sedation, anxiolysis, myorelaxation, antiseizure effects,
and psychomotor impairment (2). There appears to be variation among the benzodiazepines
in their profile of relative potency for these effects; however, all of them have some degree of
sleep enhancement. However, only five of these agents are indicated for insomnia treatment by
the U.S. Food and Drug Administration (FDA): triazolam, temazepam, flurazepam, estazolam,
and quazepam (Table 1) (3).Agents that modulate GABAA receptors at the same binding sites as
the benzodiazepines but that are unrelated chemically to these medications have been referred
to as “nonbenzodiazepines” (3). Nonbenzodiazepines indicated for the treatment of insomnia
in the United States include zolpidem, zolpidem CR, zaleplon, and eszopiclone. Together the
benzodiazepines and nonbenzodiazepines have been referred to as “benzodiazepine receptor
agonists” (BzRAs). While these agents are actually allosteric modulators and not agonists,
because this is the most commonly used term for these medications, it will be adopted here as
well (1). The BzRAs have dominated the pharmacologic management of insomnia for the last
50 years. They represent nearly all of the medications that have been approved by the FDA for the
treatment of insomnia during this period. There is a substantial body of literature documenting
their therapeutic effects in the treatment of insomnia. This chapter reviews this literature with
the goal of providing an overview of their indications, the evidence for efficacy, and the data
related to treatment outcome.
INDICATIONS
The first step in determining whether a BzRA might be indicated in a given patient is deter-
mining whether there is an indication to implement treatment for insomnia. This decision
should involve weighing the risks/costs associated with not treating a patient for insomnia
(the adverse effects of the untreated insomnia) against the anticipated benefits and risks of
the available insomnia therapies (3). Factors that should be taken into account when assess-
ing risks include medical and psychiatric conditions the patient may have such as obstruc-
tive sleep apnea, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, pregnancy, etc., which may alter the
risks of administering particular treatments. Treatment should be administered if at least one
insomnia therapy is expected to deliver improvements in quality of life and/or function that
outweigh the associated side effects and costs. The greater the impairment an individual expe-
riences because of insomnia, the greater is the cost of not implementing treatment and the
greater the possible benefit with treatment and therefore, the greater the motivation to institute
treatment (35).
Once a decision has been made to implement treatment for insomnia, it is then nec-
essary to decide which treatment to administer. In addition to the BzRAs, available options
primarily include nonpharmacologic therapies such as cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT)
c32 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 13:58 Char Count=
376 KRYSTAL
Sleep-
FDA Sleep-onset maintenance
Agentc indication T max (hr) t1/ (hr) Metabolism efficacya efficacya
2
EFFICACY
This section reviews the available double-blind, placebo-controlled trials of the treatment of
insomnia with BzRAs. Efficacy has been assessed via continuous measures of self-reported or
polysomnographic sleep-onset latency or sleep maintenance (either wake time after sleep onset
or number of awakenings) as has long been the standard in insomnia research. The results are
organized for presentation into the following subsections: (1) efficacy in adults with primary
insomnia, (2) efficacy in older adults with primary insomnia, (3) efficacy in children, (4) efficacy
in comorbid insomnia, (5) efficacy as a function of treatment duration, (6) efficacy in intermittent
dosing, and (7) comparative efficacy studies.
c32 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 13:58 Char Count=
378 KRYSTAL
380 KRYSTAL
set minimum and maximum requirements for pill-taking each week, provide the best available
indicators of the properties of “as needed” dosing of insomnia medication. Studies of true
“as needed” dosing will be needed to determine if the required pill-taking frequencies in these
studies affected the results. In terms of the clinical application of these findings, we currently lack
data to indicate which patients with insomnia are best treated with this regimen. From a practical
viewpoint, the effective implementation of intermittent dosing requires that an individual must
be able to predict prior to going to bed that they are likely to have sleep difficulty (3). While
there may be some individuals who can effectively implement dosing following unsuccessful
attempts to sleep, there is a concern that this practice might be problematic from a behavioral
viewpoint. Such a practice may increase the focus on sleep and contribute to the perpetuation
of conditioned insomnia through allowing patients to experience ongoing sleep difficulty (3).
OUTCOME
The studies of the efficacy of BzRAs reviewed in the prior section provide an important basis
for making treatment decisions. However, they reflect only one aspect of outcome. In clinical
practice it is necessary to take other considerations into account, such as treatment-emergent
adverse effects and the degree of improvement in function occurring with treatment. Here, we
have reviewed the available literature on the effects of BzRAs on nonsleep aspects of outcome
as well as global outcome measures. The following five types of outcomes are reviewed in
the subsequent sections: (1) syndromal insomnia measures, (2) patient and clinician global
impressions, (3) measures of daytime function, (4) the severity of comorbid conditions, and (5)
adverse effects of treatment.
syndromal insomnia measure that has been used in several BzRA trials is the Insomnia Severity
Index (ISI) (68). The ISI consists of seven items, each of which is a 5-point Likert scale rating
of a DSM-IV defined insomnia symptom. Several thresholds for defining levels of insomnia
severity have been established—ISI 0 to 7: not clinically significant; ISI 8 to 14: subthreshold
insomnia; ISI 15 to 21: moderate insomnia; ISI 22 to 28: severe insomnia (62,68,69). Significantly
greater improvement versus placebo in the ISI total score has been reported in a number of
recent controlled trials of eszopiclone in adults and elderly with primary insomnia, insomnia
comorbid with MDD, and in menopausal insomnia (15,49,54,58,62). In three of these studies,
active treatment led to a significantly greater percentage of the subjects meeting ISI criteria for
subthreshold and not clinically significant insomnia and a lower percentage meeting criteria
for moderate and severe insomnia compared with placebo (49,54,62). These studies provide
evidence that treatment leads to improvement in the overall severity of the insomnia syndrome
and significantly increases the percentage of individuals who are not substantively affected by
the disorder. These studies illustrate how a syndromal measure could have relevance to clinical
practice and speak to the need to adopt consensus response and remission criteria and employ
them in controlled trials of BzRA treatment.
382 KRYSTAL
findings indicate the potential for BzRAs to improve daytime function and suggest the need for
better integration of measures of daytime function into insomnia research and clinical practice.
are zolpidem 5 mg 10% versus zaleplon 10 mg 4% versus placebo 2% (6) and eszopiclone 2 mg
6.6% versus placebo 5.5% (15).
Other adverse effects: Temazepam dosed at 7.5 to 30 mg was associated with an 8% overall
incidence of adverse effects compared with 11% for placebo treatment (16). Zolpidem 5 mg was
noted to have a rate of CNS adverse effects of 25% versus 14% with placebo and zaleplon 10 mg
(6). Eszopiclone 2 mg led to unpleasant taste in 12% compared with 0% in placebo, dizziness in
6% versus 2% in placebo, and dry mouth in 7% compared with 2% in the placebo group (15).
In summary, the available data would suggest a relatively low rate of adverse effects for
the BzRAs compared with placebo. However, adverse effects data were available for a limited
number of the BzRAs.
CONCLUSIONS
BzRAs consist of the benzodiazepines and a chemically unrelated set of medications (zolpidem,
zaleplon, eszopiclone), which affect sleep via a related mechanism. These agents have domi-
nated the pharmacologic treatment of insomnia for the last 50 years. Nine BzRAs are indicated
for insomnia treatment by the FDA. A risk–benefit analysis should be used to decide whether
to administer treatment for insomnia in an individual, whether to use a BzRA, and which BzRA
to use. This analysis should be based on the published placebo-controlled trials of insomnia
therapies. More studies have been carried out documenting the efficacy and adverse effects of
BzRAs than any other agents. A number of BzRAs have been documented to have efficacy in
the treatment of sleep onset and/or maintenance difficulties with relatively minimal risk of
adverse effects. The available literature has some significant limitations, most notably the
complete absence of any controlled trials of insomnia pharmacotherapy in children. Another
gap in the literature is limited availability of studies of some of the most commonly prescribed
insomnia agents in key comorbid conditions. In addition to data from placebo-controlled trials,
there are some other factors that should be considered when deciding whether a BzRA is indi-
cated for a given patient, including abuse potential, route of metabolism, and the availability
and feasibility of other treatments. There is also a small amount of evidence that BzRAs have
therapeutic effects on syndromal measures of insomnia, global outcome, and daytime function.
Further studies employing such outcomes are needed to better characterize the treatment effects
of BzRAs.
REFERENCES
1. Downing SS, Lee YT, Farb DH, et al. Benzodiazepine modulation of partial agonist efficacy and
spontaneously active GABA(A) receptors supports an allosteric model of modulation. Br J Pharmacol
2005; 145(7):894–906.
2. Katzung BG, ed. Basic & Clinical Pharmacology, 8th ed. New York: Lange Medical Books/McGraw-
Hill, 2001.
3. Krystal AD. A compendium of placebo-controlled trials of the risks/benefits of pharmacologic treat-
ments for insomnia: The empirical basis for U.S. clinical practice. Sleep Med Rev. 2009; 13(4):265–274.
4. Krystal AD, Erman M, Zammit GK, et al. Long-term efficacy and safety of zolpidem extended-release
12.5 mg, administered 3 to 7 nights per week for 24 weeks, in patients with chronic primary insomnia:
A 6-month, randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel-group, multicenter study. Sleep.
2008; 31(1):79–90.
5. Aden GC, Thatcher C. Quazepam in the short-term treatment of insomnia in outpatients. J Clin
Psychiatry 1983; 44(12):454–456.
6. Ancoli-Israel S, Walsh JK, Mangano RM, et al. A novel nonbenzodiazepine hypnotic, effectively treats
insomnia in elderly patients without causing rebound effects. Prim Care Companion J Clin Psychiatry
1999; 1(4):114–120.
7. Drake CL, Roehrs TA, Mangano RM, et al. Dose-response effects of zaleplon as compared with
triazolam (0.25 mg) and placebo in chronic primary insomnia. Hum Psychopharmacol 2000; 15(8):595–
604.
8. Elie R, Frenay M, Le Morvan P, et al. Efficacy and safety of zopiclone and triazolam in the treatment
of geriatric insomniacs. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 1990; 5(suppl 2):39–46.
9. Elie R, Ruther E, Farr I, et al. Sleep latency is shortened during 4 weeks of treatment with zaleplon, a
novel nonbenzodiazepine hypnotic. Zaleplon Clinical Study Group. J Clin Psychiatry 1999; 60(8):536–
44.
c32 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 13:58 Char Count=
384 KRYSTAL
10. Fabre LF Jr, Brachfeld J, Meyer LR, et al. Multi-clinic double-blind comparison of triazolam (Halcion)
and placebo administered for 14 consecutive nights in outpatients with insomnia. J Clin Psychiatry
1978; 39(8):679–682.
11. Fillingim JM. Double-blind evaluation of the efficacy and safety of temazepam in outpatients with
insomnia. Br J Clin Pharmacol 1979; 8(1):73S–77S.
12. Hajak G, Clarenbach P, Fischer W, et al. Zopiclone improves sleep quality and daytime well-being in
insomniac patients: Comparison with triazolam, flunitrazepam and placebo. Int Clin Psychopharmacol
1994; 9:251–261.
13. Hedner J, Yaeche R, Emilien G, et al. Zaleplon shortens subjective sleep latency and improves subjective
sleep quality in elderly patients with insomnia. The Zaleplon Clinical Investigator Study Group. Int J
Geriatr Psychiatry 2000; 15(8):704–712.
14. Krystal AD, Walsh JK, Laska E, et al. Sustained efficacy of eszopiclone over six months of nightly
treatment: Results of a randomized, double-blind, placebo controlled study in adults with chronic
insomnia. Sleep 2003; 26:793–799.
15. McCall WV, Erman, M, Krystal AD, et al. A polysomnography study of eszopiclone in elderly patients
with insomnia. Curr Med Res Opin 2006; 22:1633–1642.
16. Morin CM, Colecchi C, Stone J, et al. Behavioral and pharmacological therapies for late-life insomnia:
A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 1999; 281(11):991–999.
17. Nair NP, Schwartz G, Dimitri R, et al. A dose-range finding study of zopiclone in insomniac patients.
Int Clin Psychopharmacol 1990; 5(suppl 2):1–10.
18. Perlis ML, McCall WV, Krystal AD, et al. Long-term, non-nightly administration of zolpidem in the
treatment of patients with primary insomnia. J Clin Psychiatry 2004; 65(8):1128–1137.
19. Reeves RL. Comparison of triazolam, flurazepam, and placebo as hypnotics in geriatric patients with
insomnia. J Clin Pharmacol 1977; 17(5–6):319–323.
20. Roehrs T, Zorick F, Lord N, et al. Dose-related effects of estazolam on sleep of patients with insomnia.
J Clin Psychopharmacol 1983; 3(3):152–156.
21. Roehrs T, Zorick F, Wittig R, et al. Efficacy of a reduced triazolam dose in elderly insomniacs. Neurobiol
Aging 1985 Winter; 6(4):293–296.
22. Roth T, Seiden D, Sainati S, et al. Effects of ramelteon on patient-reported sleep latency in older adults
with chronic insomnia. Sleep Med 2006; 7(4):312–318.
23. Scharf M, Erman M, Rosenberg R, et al. A 2-week efficacy and safety study of eszopiclone in elderly
patients with primary insomnia. Sleep 2005; 28(6):720–727.
24. Scharf MB, Roth PB, Dominguez RA, et al. Estazolam and flurazepam: A multicenter, placebo-
controlled comparative study in outpatients with insomnia. J Clin Pharmacol 1990; 30(5):461–
467.
25. Scharf MB, Roth T, Vogel GW, et al. A multicenter, placebo-controlled study evaluating zolpidem in
the treatment of chronic insomnia. J Clin Psychiatry 1994; 55(5):192–199.
26. Sunshine A. Comparison of the hypnotic activity of triazolam, flurazepam hydrochloride, and placebo.
Clin Pharmacol Ther 1975; 17(5):573–577.
27. Walsh JK, Erman M, Erwin CW, et al. Subjective hypnotic efficacy of trazodone and zolpidem in
DSM-III-R primary insomnia. Hum Psychopharmacol 1998; 13:191–198.
28. Walsh JK, Roth T, Randazzo A, et al. Eight weeks of non-nightly use of zolpidem for primary insomnia.
Sleep 2000; 23(8):1087–1096.
29. Walsh JK, Targum SD, Pegram V. A multi-center clinical investigation of estazolam: Short-term efficacy.
Curr Ther Res 1984; 36:866–874.
30. Walsh JK, Vogel GW, Scharf M, et al. A five week, polysomnographic assessment of zaleplon 10 mg
for the treatment of primary insomnia. Sleep Med 2000; 1(1):41–49.
31. Zammit GK, McNabb LJ, Caron J, et al. Efficacy and safety of eszopiclone across 6-weeks of treatment
for primary insomnia. Curr Med Res Opin 2004; 20:1979–1991.
32. Piccione P, Zorick F, Lutz T, et al. The efficacy of triazolam and chloral hydrate in geriatric insomniacs.
J Int Med Res 1980; 8(5):361–367.
33. Scharf M, Erman M, Rosenberg R, et al. A 2-week efficacy and safety study of eszopiclone in elderly
patients with primary insomnia. Sleep 2005; 28(6):720–727.
34. Krystal AD, Erman M, Zammit GK, et al. Long-term efficacy and safety of zolpidem extended-release
12.5 mg, administered 3 to 7 nights per week for 24 weeks, in patients with chronic primary insomnia:
A 6-month, randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel-group, multicenter study. Sleep.
2008; 31(1):79–90.
35. Krystal AD. Treating the health, quality of life, and functional impairments in insomnia. J Clin Sleep
Med 2007; 3(1):63–72.
36. Morin CM, Bootzin RR, Buysse DJ, et al. Psychological and behavioral treatment of insomnia: Update
of the recent evidence (1998–2004). Sleep 2006; 29(11):1398–1414.
c32 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 13:58 Char Count=
37. Walsh JK. Drugs used to treat insomnia in 2002: Regulatory-based rather than evidence-based
medicine. Sleep 2004; 27(8):14441–14442.
38. Hajak G. A comparative assessment of the risks and benefits of zopiclone: A review of 15 years’ clinical
experience. Drug Saf 1999; 21:457–469.
39. Roehrs T, Pedrosi B, Rosenthal L, et al. Hypnotic self administration and dose escalation. Psychophar-
macology (Berl) 1996; 127:150–154.
40. Crone CC, Gabriel GM. Treatment of anxiety and depression in transplant patients: Pharmacokinetic
considerations. Clin Pharmacokinet 2004; 43(6):361–394.
41. Friedman H, Redmond DE Jr, Greenblatt DJ. Comparative pharmacokinetics of alprazolam and
lorazepam in humans and in African Green Monkeys. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1991; 104(1):103–
105.
42. Fukasawa T, Suzuki A, Otani K. Effects of genetic polymorphism of cytochrome P450 enzymes on the
pharmacokinetics of benzodiazepines. J Clin Pharm Ther 2007; 32(4):333–341.
43. Greenblatt DJ, Harmatz JS, Engelhardt N, et al. Pharmacokinetic determinants of dynamic differences
among three benzodiazepine hypnotics. Flurazepam, temazepam, and triazolam. Arch Gen Psychiatry
1989; 46(4):326–332.
44. Locniskar A. Greenblatt DJ. Oxidative versus conjugative biotransformation of temazepam. Biopharm
Drug Dispos 1990; 11:499–506.
45. Edinger JD, Sampson WS. A primary care “friendly” cognitive behavioral insomnia therapy. Sleep
2003; 26(2):177–182.
46. Wohlgemuth WK, Krystal AD. Hypnotics should be considered for the initial treatment of chronic
insomnia. Pro. J Clin Sleep Med 2005; 1(2):120–124.
47. Dorsey CM, Lee KA, Scharf MB. Effect of zolpidem on sleep in women with perimenopausal and post-
menopausal insomnia: A 4-week, randomized, multicenter, double-blind, placebo-controlled study.
Clin Ther 2004; 26(10):1578–1586.
48. Fava M, Asnis GM, Shrivastava R, et al. Zolpidem extended-release improves sleep and next-day
symptoms in comorbid insomnia and generalized anxiety disorder. J Clin Psychopharmacol 2009;
29(3):222–230.
49. Fava M, McCall WV, Krystal A, et al. Eszopiclone co-administered with fluoxetine in patients with
insomnia co-existing with major depressive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2006; 59:1052–1060.
50. Krystal AD, Fava M, Rubens R, et al. Evaluation of eszopiclone discontinuation after co-therapy with
fluoxetine for insomnia with co-existing depression. J Clin Sleep Med 2007; 3:48–55.
51. Londborg PD, Smith WT, Glaudin V, et al. Short-term cotherapy with clonazepam and fluoxetine:
Anxiety, sleep disturbance and core symptoms of depression. J Affect Disord 2000; 61:73–79.
52. Nolen WA, Haffmans PM, Bouvy PF, et al. Hypnotics as concurrent medication in depression. A
placebo-controlled, double-blind comparison of flunitrazepam and lormetazepam in patients with
major depression, treated with a (tri)cyclic antidepressant. J Affect Disord 1993; 28(3):179–188.
53. Pollack M, Kinrys G, Krystal A, et al. Eszopiclone coadministered with escitalopram in patients with
insomnia and comorbid generalized anxiety disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2008; 65(5):551–562.
54. Soares CN, Joffe H, Rubens R, et al. Eszopiclone in patients with insomnia during perimenopause and
early postmenopause: A randomized controlled trial. Obstet Gynecol 2006; 108(6):1402–1410.
55. Steens RD, Pouliot Z, Millar TW, et al. Effects of zolpidem and triazolam on sleep and respiration in
mild to moderate chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Sleep 1993; 16(4):318–326.
56. Timms RM, Dawson A, Hajdukovic RM, et al. Effect of triazolam on sleep and arterial oxygen satu-
ration in patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Arch Intern Med 1988; 148(10):2159–
2163.
57. Walsh JK, Muehlbach MJ, Lauter SA, et al. Effects of triazolam on sleep, daytime sleepiness, and
morning stiffness in patients with rheumatoid arthritis. J Rheumatol 1996; 23(2):245–252.
58. Roth T, Price JM, Amato DA, et al. The effect of eszopiclone in patients with insomnia and coexisting
rheumatoid arthritis: a pilot study. Prim Care Companion J Clin Psychiatry 2009; 11(6):292–301.
59. Fava M, Asnis GM, Shrivastava R, et al. Zolpidem extended-release improves sleep and next-day
symptoms in comorbid insomnia and generalized anxiety disorder. J Clin Psychopharmacol 2009;
29(3):222–230.
60. Mindell JA, Emslie G, Blumer J, et al. Pharmacologic management of insomnia in children and ado-
lescents: Consensus statement. Pediatrics 2006; 117(6):e1223–e1232.
61. Ancoli-Israel S, Richardson GS, Mangano RM, et al. Long-term use of sedative hypnotics in older
patients with insomnia. Sleep Med 2005; 6(2):107–113.
62. Walsh JK, Krystal, AD, Amata DA, et al. Nightly treatment of primary insomnia with eszopiclone for
six months: Effect on sleep, quality of life, and work limitations. Sleep 2007; 30(8):959–968.
63. National Institutes of Health. Consensus conference. Drugs and Insomnia. The use of medications to
promote sleep. JAMA 1984; 251(18):2410–2414.
c32 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 13:58 Char Count=
386 KRYSTAL
64. Estivill E. Behavior of insomniacs and implication for their management. Sleep Med Rev 2002; 6
(suppl 1):S3–S6.
65. Erman MK, Zammit G, Rubens R, et al. A polysomnographic placebo-controlled evaluation of the
efficacy and safety of eszopiclone relative to placebo and zolpidem in the treatment of primary
insomnia. J Clin Sleep Med 2008; 4(3):229–234.
66. Piccione P, Zorick F, Lutz T, et al. The efficacy of triazolam and chloral hydrate in geriatric insomniacs.
J Int Med Res 1980; 8(5):361–367.
67. Jacobs GD, Pace-Schott EF, Stickgold R, et al. Cognitive behavior therapy and pharmacotherapy for
insomnia: a randomized controlled trial and direct comparison. Arch Intern Med 2004; 164(17):1888–
1896.
68. Morin CM. Insomnia: Psychological Assessment and Management. New York: Guilford Press; 1993.
69. Smith S, Trinder J. Detecting insomnia: Comparison of four self-report measures of sleep in a young
adult population. J Sleep Res 2001; 10:229–235.
70. Edinger JD, Bonnet MH, Bootzin RR, et al. American Academy of Sleep Medicine Work Group.
Derivation of research diagnostic criteria for insomnia: report of an American Academy of Sleep
Medicine Work Group. Sleep 2004; 27(8):1567–1596.
71. Scharf M, Erman M, Rosenberg R, et al. A 2-week efficacy and safety study of eszopiclone in elderly
patients with primary insomnia. Sleep 2005; 28(6):720–727.
72. Asnis GM, Chakraburtty A, DuBoff EA, et al. Zolpidem for persistent insomnia in SSRI-treated
depressed patients. J Clin Psychiatry 1999; 60:668–676.
73. Ford DE, Kamerow DB. Epidemiologic study of sleep disturbances and psychiatric disorders. An
opportunity for prevention? JAMA 1989; 262:1479–1484.
74. Sunshine, 1979.
75. Roth T, Soubrane C, Titeux L; Zoladult Study Group. Efficacy and safety of zolpidem-MR: A double-
blind, placebo-controlled study in adults with primary insomnia. Sleep Med 2006; 7(5):397–406.
76. Roth T, Walsh JK, Krystal A, et al. An evaluation of the efficacy and safety of eszopiclone over 12
months in patients with chronic primary insomnia. Sleep Med 2005; 6(6):487–495.
c33 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:45 Char Count=
INTRODUCTION
The major side effect and safety issues associated with benzodiazepine receptor agonist (BzRA)
hypnotic use include psychomotor and cognitive (i.e., anterograde amnesia) impairment,
parasomnia-like episodes, discontinuation effects, and dependence liability. Some of these side
effects are mediated by the primary pharmacodynamic activity, sedation, of BzRAs and directly
relate to the pharmacokinetic properties of specific BzRAs. Other side effects can be attributed
to both pharmacokinetics and receptor selectivity of the drug. Finally, drug dose, duration of
use, and concomitant medications, comorbid medical, or sleep disorders may determine side
effects.
The side effect–safety profile of the BzRAs has to be weighed against the various alterna-
tives to treating insomnia, including no treatment, self-treatment, and alternative nonhypnotic
medications (i.e., drugs used as hypnotics that have sedative effects but are indicated for another
disorder). Unfortunately, comparisons to alternative nonhypnotics are limited by the fact that
there is little systematic information regarding the safety of the various alternative medica-
tions being used as hypnotics. This includes an absence of data at the lower doses frequently
used for sleep and for the effects on sleep-related activity (e.g., rebound insomnia, effects on
sleep-related breathing disorders). Importantly, the risks associated with nontreatment and self-
treatment also have to be compared with those of the BzRAs. The risks of nontreatment and
self-treatment are known.
This chapter will review the evidence and discuss the determinants of the side effects of
BzRA hypnotics. It will compare these risks to the risks associated with alternative drugs used
as hypnotics and self-treatment, as well as those risks associated with no treatment. Finally,
we will discuss ways by which the clinician can minimize the various side effect–safety risks
associated with BzRAs.
Psychomotor Impairment
Psychomotor impairment with BzRAs has been shown on various measures (i.e., as slowed
reaction times, response errors, tracking errors, lapses of attention, and driving deviations) in
laboratory performance testing and on actual roadway driving assessments. At their peak
plasma concentrations BzRA-associated impairments relate directly to the level of plasma
concentration, which is a function of dose. To illustrate, a study compared the daytime admin-
istration effects of 0.125, 0.25, and 0.50 mg triazolam; 5, 10, and 20 mg zolpidem; and 15,
30, and 60 mg temazepam (1). These BzRAs were compared on the basis of their differing
pharmacokinetics and receptor selectivity. Temazepam is known to be longer acting than zolpi-
dem and triazolam, and zolpidem is considered to be more receptor selective than triazo-
lam and temazepam. Each drug—zolpidem, triazolam, and temazepam—produced orderly
dose-related impairment in learning and recall and psychomotor performance at their peak
concentrations (1).
The duration of the impairment relates to the duration of action mediated by both the
half-life and dose of a specific BzRA. In the study cited earlier, the functions relating impairment
to time since ingestion for the three drugs revealed a six-hour duration impairment relative to
placebo with 60 mg temazepam and a three-hour duration impairment with 20 mg zolpidem,
although these two drugs and doses had comparable impairing effects at their peak (1). Although
c33 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:45 Char Count=
zolpidem is considered more GABAA ␣1 -receptor selective than triazolam and temazepam, it
did not produce a differential pattern of impairment.
When the BzRA impairment extends to the morning following a nighttime administration,
this impairment is referred to as “residual effects.” Residual effects are merely the prolongation
of the therapeutic effect of the drug into next-day wakefulness. BzRAs with longer durations of
action, as determined by the half-life of the drug and secondarily by the dose of the drug (e.g.,
higher doses and longer half-lives extend duration of action), are more likely to produce residual
effects. Using performance and driving assessments and the Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT),
studies have found differences in residual effects between short- and long-acting drugs and
between doses of the same drug. For example, an early study in healthy elderly compared the
daytime residual effects of triazolam 0.25 mg and flurazepam 15 mg administered before sleep
(2). Both drugs produced a comparable one hour increase in total sleep time, but flurazepam, a
long-acting drug, produced increased daytime sleepiness on the MSLT the following day, while
triazolam, a short-acting drug, reduced sleepiness on the MSLT. In the same study, next-day
vigilance performance was impaired with flurazepam, but not with triazolam.
Given a desired sleep period of eight hours, middle-of-the-night administration of a short
half-life drug (i.e., three to five hours) is likely to produce residual effects. In other words, the
likelihood of residual effects is determined by the time of administration relative to the desired
time of arising and the pharmacokinetics of the given drug. A study comparing the residual
effects of the short half-life drug (2.5–4.5 hours), zolpidem (10 mg), with the ultra short half-life
drug (1 hour), zaleplon (10 mg), illustrates this point (3). The drugs were administered in the
middle of the night at either 3, 4, 5, or 6 AM before a desired 8 AM awakening, translating to an
awakening that occurred—two to five hours postadministration. Zolpidem produced residual
effects on digit symbol substitution and immediate and delayed memory recall after all middle-
of-the-night administrations, but zaleplon administration had no effects at 8 AM, even after the
6 AM (two hours postadministration). The FDA label for recently approved hypnotics, given
their distinct pharmacokinetics, now includes the caution that “x” hours be devoted to sleep
when using the medication. For example, the zaleplon label cautions four or more hours, while
the zolpidem-CR formulation cautions eight hours.
Falls in the elderly are often cited as a special case of psychomotor impairment associated
with BzRAs, either due to elevated peak plasma concentrations or residual effects. Several ques-
tions arise, including whether the reported association is unique to BzRAs, whether that risk is
greater than the untreated condition, and if truly associated, how it can be minimized, which
will be discussed later. Falls in the elderly are not unique to BzRAs, which are not independent
predictors of falls when controlling for comorbid diseases. A case–control study of community-
living persons aged 66 years and older who visited emergency departments for injurious falls
during one year in a Canadian health district identified seven medication classes, including
sedatives, that were associated with falls (4). After controlling for comorbid conditions, nar-
cotics, anticonvulsants, and antidepressants were independent predictors, but not hypnotics.
In a prospective study of elderly women and fractures, narcotics and antidepressants were
associated with increased risk for fractures, while benzodiazepines and anticonvulsants were
not independent predictors (5).
There are data to suggest that sleep problems and daytime sleepiness in elderly are
independent factors increasing risk of accidental injury and falls. A representative sample
of elderly adults from northern California was surveyed by telephone (6). Nighttime sleep
problems were a risk for fractures and remained so after controlling for demographic variables
and concurrent medical diseases. A more recent study assessing the risk of BzRA-associated falls
in elderly controlled for insomnia (7). This large study of nursing home residents in Southeastern
Michigan found that insomnia represents a significant risk for falls but BzRAs do not. Thus, the
risk of falls associated with BzRAs was actually less than untreated insomnia.
Cognitive Impairment
Another major side effect of BzRAs is cognitive impairment, most typically anterograde amne-
sia. Anterograde, in contrast to retrograde, amnesia is failure to recall information presented
after consumption of the drug. The degree of amnesia is determined by the level of plasma
c33 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:45 Char Count=
concentration at the time of information input. It can be because of attentional and/or consolida-
tion failures in the memory process. At peak plasma concentration, very orderly dose-dependent
amnesic effects have been demonstrated for BzRAs (1). The amnesia is, in part, related to the
sedative effects of the BzRAs, as the degree of the amnesia parallels the sedative effects of the
drug as measured by the MSLT (8). Failure to consolidate the newly acquired material is one
cause of the amnesia. This explanation is supported by a study in which the drug-induced rapid
return to sleep was delayed for 15 minutes (i.e., wakefulness was maintained for 15 minutes)
and memory was preserved (9). The extent to which the sedative effects mediate the amnesic
effects has been debated extensively. Several studies have attempted to dissociate these two
effects. The amnestic effects of drugs with differing sedative effects were compared, or the
antagonist, flumazenil, was used to dissociate sedative and amnestic effects, but the studies
had equivocal results (10,11). The problem in these studies was that sedation was self-reported
rather than objectively assessed. Another important complication is that sleep, even brief sleep
periods, produce retrograde amnesia.
Amnesia is associated with the receptor selectivity of the BzRAs. These act as allosteric
modulators of GABAA receptors and gene knockin studies have identified and characterized
various GABAA receptor subunits for their pharmacological profiles (12). The animal data
indicate that the ␣1 -receptor subtype mediates both the sleep and amnesic effects of the BzRAs
(12). When zolpidem, a nonbenzodiazepine hypnotic, was introduced, it was hypothesized that
because of the receptor selectivity of zolpidem, amnesia could be avoided. But, as noted above,
the ␣1 -receptor–selective agent zolpidem did not differ from nonselective benzodiazepines in
its amnesic effects (1). Zopiclone and its S-isomer, eszopiclone, are less selective for ␣1 -receptors
than zopidem and more selective for ␣3 -receptors, but we are unaware of studies that have
compared their amnestic effects, although adverse events characterized as amnesia have been
reported. It must be concluded that all BzRAs that act extensively at the ␣1 -receptor produce
dose-dependent anterograde amnesia with as yet no studies demonstrating differences between
the various drugs when sedative potency is controlled.
Finally, long-term BzRA use is purportedly associated with cognitive impairment, partic-
ularly in elderly. One has to distinguish time-limited impairment (i.e., the time over which drug
is present in plasma) in acute use from long-term impairment in chronic use. There have been
reports of “global amnesia” associated with BzRAs (13). The reported total amnesia lasted for
several hours after consuming triazolam, 0.5 mg, but the triazolam use was also accompanied
by variable amounts of reported alcohol consumption.
These reports of acute global amnesia contrast with the suggestion that chronic BzRA use
is associated with cognitive impairment. The results of studies assessing cognitive function in
elderly chronic BzRA users are equivocal, with some studies reporting impairment and others
finding minimal or no impairment (14–16). It is difficult to reach definitive conclusions because
these reports are cross-sectional and retrospective in nature with a number of confounds. Further,
determining the appropriate controls for these studies is problematic. Most of the information
on BzRA cognitive impairment is from patients with anxiety disorders, who are using long-
acting benzodiazepines. The relevance of these data to current best clinical practice for insomnia
pharmacotherapy (i.e., use of short-acting nonbenzodiazepine hypnotics) is questionable.
Discontinuation Effects
The most frequently reported discontinuation effect of the BzRAs in clinical use is rebound
insomnia (17). Rebound insomnia is defined as worsened sleep for one or two nights relative
to baseline. It can occur after even one to two nights of previous BzRA use (17). The rebound
insomnia does not appear to increase in severity with the duration of nightly use, at least in the
short term. Rebound insomnia was reported with the 0.5-mg dose, but not the 0.25-mg dose, of
the short-acting drug, triazolam (17). Proper multiple-dose studies that explore the threshold
dose for rebound with other BzRA hypnotics have not been done. Many studies report an
absence of rebound, but do not include a positive control that demonstrates that the study
design is robust enough to detect rebound. Rebound is likely to occur after high doses (i.e.,
beyond minimally effective doses) of all short- and intermediate-acting BzRAs. This prediction
is made based on the multiple-dose studies of daytime performance impairment that have
c33 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:45 Char Count=
compared various BzRAs to triazolam and find comparable impairment at triazolam doses
that produce rebound (1). Rebound is unlikely to occur with a long-acting drug because of the
gradual decline in plasma concentrations inherent to its pharmacology.
Rebound insomnia is an exacerbation of the original symptom (i.e., difficulty falling and or
staying asleep), and thus, differs from recrudescence, which is the return of the original symptom
at its original severity. It is not a withdrawal syndrome (i.e., expression of new symptoms), at
least in the available short-term studies (i.e., two weeks and less), which induced rebound but
no other new symptoms (17). Its time course differs from that of a withdrawal syndrome, lasting
for one or two nights only. Finally, there is no evidence to suggest that the benzodiazepine and
nonbenzodiazepine BzRAs differ in the likelihood of producing rebound (17,18).
The extent to which duration of use and dose might combine to increase the likelihood of
rebound, even at clinical doses, when used in the long-term is unknown. A recent study assessed
rebound after six months of the nightly use of eszopiclone 3 mg, its clinical dose (19). Over the
14 days studied after discontinuation of eszopiclone, no increase in self-reported sleep latency
or wake after sleep onset relative to baseline was observed. These data need to be considered
cautiously as no positive control was used in this study. It also has been suggested that the
experience of rebound insomnia leads to continued chronic use of the hypnotic. Importantly, a
study, which directly tested that notion, showed that the experience of rebound insomnia does
not alter the subsequent likelihood of self-administering triazolam 0.25 mg (20). In summary,
while rebound insomnia is a reliable effect associated with BzRAs, its clinical significance, if
any, is yet to be identified.
Abuse Liability
There has long been concern that behavioral or physical dependence develops with chronic
use of BzRAs. The concern is based on reports of physical and behavioral dependence with
long-term daytime anxiolytic use of therapeutic doses of BzRAs (21). Systematic information
regarding the dependence liability of long-term therapeutic use of BzRA hypnotics at clinical
doses is very limited. Majority of persons in population-based studies report using hypnotics
for two weeks or less (22,23). Two recent placebo-controlled, double-blind studies of eszopiclone
3 mg reported no evidence of physical or behavioral dependence after six months of nightly
use (19,24). But, neither of these studies directly tested for physical and behavioral dependence
liability.
Short-term studies directly testing the behavioral dependence liability of BzRA hyp-
notics suggest that they have a low behavioral dependence liability (25). Behavioral depen-
dence liability can be directly tested by assessing the self-administration of active drug versus
placebo administered as color-coded capsules. After sampling each color-coded capsule over 7
to 14 subsequent nights, patients choose the desired capsule based on its color. Hypnotic self-
administration by insomniacs is not associated with dose escalation with repeated use when
provided opportunity to self-administer multiple capsules (26), does not increase with rebound
insomnia (20), does not generalize to daytime use (27), and varies as a function of the nature and
severity of the patients’ sleep disturbance (25). These short-term studies lead to the conclusion
that insomnia patients’ hypnotic self-administration is a therapy-seeking behavior and not drug
seeking or abuse. It should be noted that these conclusions are true for insomniacs and normal
controls, but not for individuals with a drug abuse history.
One important question is the extent to which receptor subtype selectivity may influence
the abuse liability of the BzRAs. One assessment of the relative abuse liability of hypnotic
drugs failed to find differential receptor subtype selectivity in abuse liability among drugs used
as hypnotics (28). For example, the ␣1 selective drug, zolpidem, did not differ from various
nonselective BzRAs. However, there are very few studies comparing multiple doses of multiple
drugs and thus the rating had to be made across a variety of methodologies and data sources.
In addition, the rating also included drug toxicities and thus was not specific to what is more
narrowly defined as drug abuse liability.
and sleep-related eating disorder are problematic; they are not peer-reviewed, generally not
independently documented, are subject to confirmation bias, and likely overrepresent the real
risk. Further, they do not provide information that can lead to a scientific medical understanding
of the phenomena, and they raise unnecessary concern among patients and their physicians.
In the scientific medical literature peer-reviewed case reports of parasomnia-like BzRA-
associated side effects have also appeared. But, again one must be cautious. Case reports do
provide some information about contributing factors, but it is not placebo-controlled informa-
tion. Most importantly, the real risk of BzRA-associated parasomnia is unknown because the
rate of exposure is not known. The number of prescriptions written and doses consumed at the
time of the event is unknown and consequently, the incidence of the events cannot be deter-
mined. Finally, there have been several publications that suggest that the appearance of media
reports and medical articles distort the true prevalence of adverse events.
As noted earlier, transient global amnesia has been reported in association with the use of
triazolam by otherwise healthy individuals experiencing sleep disturbance (13,29). The memory
loss was for all autobiographical events transpiring over an 8- to 12-hour period. In some of these
cases in which clinical doses were used, prior stress, sleep deprivation, and a virus may have
contributed to produce the amnesia. In other cases, supraclinical doses and alcohol ingestion are
likely contributory factors. It is unlikely that this phenomenon is unique to triazolam as similar
kinds of amnesia are produced by the intravenous administration of other benzodiazepines.
Somnambulism has been reported with zolpidem and zaleplon (30,31). These episodes
of somnambulism have occurred with two to three times the clinical doses of the drug, in
individuals with a prior history of somnambulism and in individuals with prior traumatic
head injury. Zolpidem-associated somnambulism also has been reported in combination with
antidepressant treatment (32). Somnambulism is believed to be associated with partial arousals
from sleep, which alcohol and sleep deprivation also exacerbate. Not surprisingly, both alcohol
and sleep deprivation also exacerbate somnambulism.
Finally, there are case reports of sleep-related eating disorder associated with psychotropic
medications, including BzRAs (33–35). There is a dispute as to whether sleep-related eating
disorder is a disorder of partial arousal from sleep with altered levels of consciousness or is the
psychiatric disorder of nocturnal eating with awareness and recall (36,37). Sleep-related eating
disorder is hypothesized to share a common pathophysiology with somnambulism. Zolpidem
was reported to exacerbate sleep-related eating disorder and in several cases induce it de novo
(37). In some of these cases greater than 10 mg of zolpidem doses were being used and in other
cases there was use of sedating antidepressants. Sleep-related eating disorder has also been
reported with triazolam (38,39).
These case reports have a common thread running through them—excessive hypnotic
activity or sleep drive. The excessive hypnotic activity is produced by high doses, clinical doses
in vulnerable individuals (i.e., those with a past history of sleep disorders or brain injury), the
combination of clinical or high doses with prior sleep deprivation due to stress or illness, or
the combination of clinical or high doses with the prior consumption of alcohol or other CNS
drugs. The types of behaviors described in these case reports also share a commonality. They all
are symptoms of excessive hypnotic activity or excessive sleepiness. Amnesia and memory dif-
ficulties are reported by patients with excessive daytime sleepiness. Sleep deprivation produces
intense slow wave sleep and abrupt arousals from slow wave sleep after prior sleep deprivation
is known to be associated with sleep inertia–behaving individuals with little consciousness and
memory. Patients with excessive sleepiness are known to engage in automatic behavior, and
sleep deprivation is known to induce somnambulism in individuals with a previous history
of somnambulism. Taken together the data suggest that parasomnia reports associated with
BzRAs are the result of excessive hypnotic/sedative activity in vulnerable individuals.
Nontreatment
Untreated insomnia has well-documented morbidity. Insomniacs report reduced quality of life
on the Short-Form-16 Quality of Life Questionnaire across all of its domains, and they rate their
quality-of-life akin to that of patients with other chronic disorders such as congestive heart
failure and clinical depression (42,43). Compared to individuals without insomnia, insomniacs
self-report increased days of restricted activity due to illness and increased days spent in bed as a
result of illness (44). Higher rates of absenteeism are reported by those with insomnia compared
with controls, and rates of work-related accidents and traffic accidents are higher (42,45). Among
nursing home residents, the risk of falls and fractures was higher in the untreated insomniacs
than those treated with hypnotics (7). This finding contradicts the general perception based on
earlier studies that hypnotic treatment in elderly is associated with greater risk of falls and the
risk reversal likely relates to a wider current use of short-acting rather than long-acting hypnotics
as in the earlier studies. Importantly, those earlier studies did not control for insomnia, which
itself is a risk factor for falls.
Untreated insomnia is associated with increased risk of incident cases and relapse of psy-
chiatric disorders and with an exacerbation of medical diseases. A number of studies have now
demonstrated a heightened risk of future depression in persons reporting insomnia without a
current or previous history of depression, and a study has shown insomnia precedes incident
depression as well as relapse (46–48). In addition, there is an increased risk of anxiety disorders
and drug and alcohol abuse associated with insomnia (46). Whether treating insomnia would
reduce these risks is yet to be determined. However, one recent study did find that adjunctive
pharmacological treatment of insomnia coexisting with primary depression not only improved
the insomnia, but also improved depressive symptoms beyond that found with standard antide-
pressant monotherapy (49).
Insomnia is comorbid with various medical diseases and evidence indicates that disturbed
sleep exacerbates medical disease, specifically diseases with pain as a prominent symptom.
In a prospective study, self-ratings of sleep and pain in patients with fibromyalgia showed
that nights with poor sleep were followed by days with greater pain (50). Also, patients with
rheumatological disorders frequently report insomnia and disturbed sleep. In two small studies,
treatment of insomnia associated with rheumatoid arthritis with either triazolam or zopiclone
failed to show concurrent improvement in both sleep and pain (51,52). However, in a recent
large study of patients with rheumatoid arthritis and coexisting insomnia, eszopiclone 3 mg
improved both nighttime sleep and daytime joint pain (53). Studies in healthy normals have
suggested that poor sleep may exacerbate pain. Studies have found that total sleep deprivation
is hyperalgesic and a recent study in healthy normals has shown that only a four-hour reduction
of sleep time for a single night is hyperalgesic to a radiant heat stimulus (54).
Self-Medication
The pursuit of ineffective and potentially dangerous self-treatments is not fully appreciated as
a risk of not treating the patient with insomnia. In a population-based study in Southeastern
Michigan, respondents with insomnia reported poorer sleep hygiene practices than noninsom-
niacs (55). Among the compensatory self-help behaviors reported by insomniacs is napping and
sleeping on weekends, behaviors that can potentially exacerbate and perpetuate the insomnia.
Insomniacs use nonprescription substances to self-treat their insomnia, including OTC
medications, herbals, and alcohol (23). The active component of all OTC sleep aids is H1 antihis-
tamine, typically diphenhydramine 25 to 50 mg. Beyond there being no clear placebo-controlled
studies that show diphenhydramine has hypnotic efficacy, rapid tolerance development to its
sedative effects has been shown (56). Low-dose alcohol as a sleep aid is potentially dangerous for
two reasons. Low-dose alcohol initially improves the sleep of insomniacs, which is why they
self-administer it as a sleep aid (57). However, within six nights tolerance develops, sleep is
worsened beyond that of baseline, and larger alcohol doses are self-administered to achieve the
c33 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:45 Char Count=
sleep effect (57–59). Insomniacs who reported using alcohol as a sleep aid also reported greater
levels of daytime sleepiness than those who used prescription or OTC drugs for sleep (23).
Nonhypnotic Medications
The most commonly used drugs to treat insomnia according to the Physician Drug and Diagnosis
Audit (PDDA) database in 2002 (the most recently available published data) were sedating
antidepressants (60). They were 1.53 times more likely to be prescribed for insomnia than BzRAs
and the three most common were trazodone, amitriptyline, and mirtazapine. These prescribing
data may not represent current practice given the very recent introduction of a number of new
FDA-approved BzRA hypnotics. Nevertheless, the safety of sedating antidepressants used to
promote sleep, as opposed to treating depression, merits discussion.
Information on antidepressant safety is primarily derived from use in depressed patients
and the data are for higher doses (e.g., antidepressant doses) than typically used for insomnia.
As an example, the doses reported in the PDDA for trazodone were 50 mg in 53% of those men-
tioned, 100 mg in 31% and 150 mg in 14% (60). The 150-mg dose is the usual daily antidepressant
dose. However, on the basis of recent studies of low-dose doxepin as a hypnotic, antidepressants
at low doses are likely safer than the higher antidepressant doses (61).
Trazodone: Sedation is a widely reported side effect with trazodone use, which possibly
relates to its approximate 12-hour half-life in elderly patients, although in younger patients
the half-life is 6 hours (62). On an average across studies, 29% of depressed patients reported
daytime drowsiness at antidepressant doses. Even at the lower 50-mg dose, the only placebo-
controlled trial of its use as treatment for primary insomnia reported 23% of patients over the
two-week treatment had problems with daytime somnolence compared to 8% with placebo and
16% with zolpidem (63). Trazodone has significant cardiovascular risks including hypotension,
orthostatic hypotension, ventricular arrhythmias, conduction disturbances, and exacerbation of
ischemic attacks. Finally, trazodone is associated with priapism in many case reports and an
analysis found that most cases occurred with 50 to 150 mg daily doses, which are the doses
more typically used for insomnia (64). However, this case report information on priapism may
suggest a higher incidence than the estimated incidence of 1/5000.
Amitriptyline: It is known for its anticholinergic side effects, including blurred vision, dry
mouth, urinary retention, orthostatic hypotension, flushing, tachycardia, and confusion (65).
Cardiac toxicity in overdoses has been reported, and at clinical doses in patients with known
cardiovascular disease, there are increased cardiac risks. But it should be emphasized that this
information may not be relevant to the lower doses often used to treat insomnia.
Mirtazapine: The major side effects associated with mirtazapine are weight gain and
increased appetite. Increased daytime sleepiness and dizziness have also been reported (65).
Quetiapine and olanzapine: In the PDDA data for 2002, the antipsychotics quetiapine and
olanzapine and the antihistamines hydroxyzine and diphenhydramine were also frequently
used as hypnotics. As is true of the sedating antidepressants, there is no safety information for
these drugs at the doses used for hypnotic effects. These drugs are chosen for their reported
sedating effects, generally produced by their H1 antagonism. To the extent that a given drug has
a long half-life (i.e., olanzapine’s half-life is 20–50 hours), its duration of action will be extended
to the following day, producing residual impairment of function. But they also affect other
neurotransmitter systems, which produce other side effects such as dizziness and hypotension.
sedating drugs, and time-in-bed after drug ingestion should be carefully monitored. Secondly,
by most indications these are very rare side effects when the medications are used appropriately.
As with psychomotor impairment, the risk of cognitive impairment is reduced by
attention to duration of action and dose. Special caution should be taken for patients who are
elderly. Both duration of action and peak plasma concentration can be enhanced in elderly
with the result being a greater likelihood of psychomotor and cognitive impairment. The same
is true for patients with renal or hepatic problems. In addition, elderly patients are more likely
to be using multiple drugs, many of which have sedating effects, further enhancing risks of
cognitive impairment.
Rebound insomnia can be minimized with short- and intermediate-acting drugs by grad-
ually tapering the dose over several nights. Its impact can be reduced with patient instructions
that include the caution that rebound can occur, but that rebound endures for one to two nights,
when it does occur.
Finally, identifying patients with an enhanced liability of dependence development,
whether physical or behavioral, is quite difficult. The most reliable predictor is a previous
history of drug or alcohol abuse. While treating patients, possible dose escalation should be
closely monitored and the use of the medication outside of the therapeutic context noted. Thus,
any daytime use of a hypnotic, except for night workers who are day sleepers, should be
discouraged and when observed should be a sign of concern.
REFERENCES
1. Rush CR, Griffiths RR. Zolpidem, triazolam, and temazepam: Behavioral and subject-rated effects in
normal volunteers. J Clin Psychopharmacol 1996; 16:146–157.
2. Carskadon MA, Seidel WF, Greenblatt DJ, et al. Daytime carryover of triazolam and flurazepam in
elderly insomniacs. Sleep 1982; 5:361–371.
3. Danjou P, Fruncillo PR, Worthington P, et al. A comparison of the residual effects of zaleplon and
zolpidem following administration 5 to 2 h before awakening. Br J Clin Pharmacol 1999; 48:367–374.
4. Kelly KD, Pickett W, Yiannakaoulias N, et al. Medication use and falls in community-dwelling older
persons. Age Ageing 2003; 32:503–509.
5. Ensrud KE, Blackwell T, Mangione CM, et al. Central nervous system active medications and risk for
fractures in older women. Arch Intern Med 2003; 163:949–957.
6. Brassington GS, King AC, Bliwise DL. Sleep problems as a risk factor for falls in a sample of
community-dwelling adults aged 64–99 years. J Am Geriatr Soc 2000; 48:1234–1240.
7. Avidan AY, Fries BE, James ML, et al. Insomnia and hypnotic use recorded in the minimum data set
as predictors of fall and hip fractures in Michigan nursing homes. J Am Geriatr Soc 2005; 53:955–962.
8. Roehrs T, Merlotti L, Zorick F, et al. Sedative, memory and performance effects of hypnotics. Psy-
chopharmacology 1994; 116:130–134.
9. Roehrs T, Zorick F, Sicklesteel J, et al. Effects of hypnotics on memory. J Clin Psychopharmacol 1983;
3:310–313.
10. Green JF, McElholm A, King DJ. A comparison of the sedative and amnestic effects of chlorpromazine
and lorazepam. Psychopharmacology 1996; 128:67–73.
11. Curran HV, Birch B. Differentiating the sedative, psychomotor and amnesic effects of benzodiazepines:
A study with midazolam and the benzodiazepine antagonist, flumazenil. Psychopharmacology 1991;
103:519–523.
12. Mohler H, Crestani F, Rudolph. GABA-A receptor subtypes: A new pharmacology. Curr Opin Phar-
macol 2001; 1:22–25.
13. Shader RI, Greenblatt DJ. Triazolam and anterograde amnesia: All is not well in the z-zone. J Clin
Psychopharmacol 1983; 3:273.
14. Paterniti S, Dufouil C, Alperovitch A. Long-term benzodiazepine use and cognitive decline in the
elderly: The Epidemiology of Vascular Aging Study. J Clin Psychopharmacol 2002; 22:285–293.
15. Allard J, Artero S, Ritchie K. Consumption of psychotropic medication in the elderly: A re-evaluation
of its effect on cognitive performance. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2003; 18:874–878.
16. McAndrews MP, Weiss RT, Sandor P, et al. Cognitive effects of long-term benzodiazepine use in older
adults. Hum Psychopharmacol Clin Exp 2003; 18:51–57.
17. Roehrs TA, Vogel G, Roth T. Rebound insomnia: Its determinants and significance. Am J Med 1990;
88:43S–46S.
18. Roth T, Soubrane C, Titeux L, et al. Efficacy and safety of zolpidem-MR: A double-blind, placebo-
controlled study in adults with primary insomnia. Sleep Med 2006;7:397–406.
c33 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:45 Char Count=
19. Walsh J, Krystal A, Amato DA, et al. Nightly treatment of primary insomnia with eszopiclone for six
months: Effect on sleep quality of life, and work limitations. Sleep 2007; 30:959–968.
20. Roehrs T, Merlotti L, Zorick F, et al. Rebound insomnia and hypnotic self administration. Psychophar-
macology 1992; 107:480–484.
21. Woods JH, Winger G. Current benzodiazepine issues. Psychopharmacology 1995; 118:107–103.
22. Mellinger GD, Balter MB, Uhlenhuth EH. Insomnia and its treatment. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1985;
42:225–232.
23. Roehrs T, Hollebeek E, Drake C, et al. Substance use for insomnia in Metropolitan Detroit. J Psychosom
Res 2002; 53:571–576.
24. Krystal AD, Walsh JK, Laska E, et al. Sustained efficacy of eszopiclone over 6 months of nightly
treatment: Results of a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study in adults with chronic
insomnia. Sleep 2003; 26:793–799.
25. Roehrs T, Bonahoom A, Pedrosi B, et al. Disturbed sleep predicts hypnotic self administration. Sleep
Med 2002; 3:61–66.
26. Roehrs T, Pedrosi B, Rosenthal L, et al. Hypnotic self administration and dose escalation. Psychophar-
macology 1996; 127:150–154.
27. Roehrs T, Bonahoom A, Pedrosi B, et al. Nighttime versus daytime hypnotic self-administration.
Psychopharmacology 2002; 161:137–142.
28. Griffiths RR, Johnson MW. Relative abuse liability of hypnotic drugs: A conceptual frame-
work and algorithm for differentiating among compounds. J Clin Psychiatry 2005; 66(suppl 9):
31–41.
29. Morris HH, Estes ML. Traveler’s amnesia. Transient global amnesia secondary to triazolam. JAMA
1987; 258:945–946.
30. Yang W, Dollear M, Muthukrishnan SR. One rare side effect of zolpidem—sleepwalking: A case
report. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2005; 86:1265–1266.
31. Liskow B, Pikalov A. Zaleplon overdose associated with sleepwalking and complex behavior. J Am
Acad Child Adoles Psychiatry. 2004; 43:927–928.
32. Lange CL. Medication-associated somnambulism. J Am Acad Child Adoles Psychiatry 2005; 44:
211–212.
33. Paquet V, Strul J, Servais L, et al. Sleep-related eating disorder induced by olanzapine. J Clin Psychiatry
2002; 63:7.
34. Lu ML, Shen WW. Sleep-related eating disorder induced by risperidone. J Clin Psychiatry 2004;
65:273–274.
35. Morgenthaler TI, Silber MH. Amnestic sleep-related eating disorder associated with zolpidem. Sleep
Med 2002; 3:323–327.
36. Schenk CH, Mahowald MW. Review of nocturnal sleep-related eating disorders. Int J Eat Disord 1994;
15:343–356.
37. Vetrugno R, Manconi M, Strembi LF, et al. Nocturnal eating: Sleep related eating disorder or nocturnal
eating syndrome? A videopolysomnographic study. Sleep 2006; 29:876–877.
38. Menkes DB. Triazolam-induced nocturnal bingeing with amnesia. Aust N Z J Psychaitry 1992; 26:
320–321.
39. Lauerma H. Nocturnal wandering caused by restless legs and short-acting benzodiazepines. Acta
Psychiatr Scand 1991; 83:492–493.
40. Ancoli-Israel S, Roth T. Characteristics of insomnia in the United States: Results of the 1991 National
Sleep Foundation Survey. Sleep 1999; 22:S347–S353.
41. Johnson EO, Roehrs T, Roth T, et al. Epidemiology of alcohol and medication as aids to sleep in early
adulthood. Sleep 1998; 21:178–186.
42. Zammit GK, Weiner J, Damato N, et al. Quality of life in people with insomnia. Sleep 1999; 22
(suppl 2):S379–S385.
43. Katz DA, McHorney CA. The relationship between insomnia and health-related quality of life in
patients with chronic illness. J Fam Pract 2002; 51:229–235.
44. Simon GE, Von Korff M. Prevalence, burden, and treatment of insomnia in primary care. Am J
Psychiatry 1997; 154:1417–1423.
45. Leger D, Guilleminault C, Bader G, et al. Medical and socio-professional impact of insomnia. Sleep
2002; 25:625–629.
46. Breslau N, Roth T, Rosenthal L, et al. Sleep disturbance and psychiatric disorders: A longitudinal
epidemiological study of young adults. Biol Psychiatry 1996; 39:411–418.
47. Chang PP, Ford DE, Mead LA, et al. Insomnia in young men and subsequent depression. The Hohns
Hopkins Precursor Study. Am J Epidemiol 1997; 146:105–114.
48. Perlis ML, Giles DE, Buysse DJ. Self-reported sleep disturbance as a prodromal symptom in recurrent
depression. J Affect Disord 1997; 42:209–212.
c33 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:45 Char Count=
49. Fava M, McCall WV, Krystal A, et al. Eszopiclone co-administered with fluoxetine in patients with
insomnia coexisting with major depressive disorder. Bio Psychiatry 2006.
50. Affleck G, Urrows S, Tennen H, et al. Sequential daily relations of sleep, pain intensity, and attention
to pain among women with fibromyalgia. Pain 1996; 68:363–368.
51. Walsh JK, Muehlbach MJ, Lauter SA, et al. Effect of triazolam on sleep, daytime sleepiness and
morning stiffness in patients with rheumatoid arthritis. J Rheumatol 1996; 23:245–252.
52. Drewes AM, Bjerregard K, Jorgensen T, et al. Zopiclone as night medication in rheumatoid arthritis.
Scand J Rheumatol 1998; 27:180–187.
53. Schnitzer T, Rubens R, Price J, et al. The effect of eszopiclone 3 mg compared with placebo in patients
with rheumatoid arthritis and co-existing insomnia. Poster presented at: Sleep; 2006; Salt Lake City,
UT. Abstract.
54. Roehrs T, Hyde M, Blaisdell B, et al. Sleep loss and REM sleep loss are hyperalgesic. Sleep 2006;
29:145–151.
55. Jefferson CD, Drake Cl, Scofield HM, et al. Sleep hygiene practices in a population-based sample of
insomniacs. Sleep 2005; 28:611–615.
56. Richardson G, Roehrs T, Rosenthal L, et al. Tolerance to daytime sedative effects of H1 antihistamines.
J Clin Psychopharmacol 2002; 22:511–515.
57. Roehrs T, Papineau K, Rosenthal L, et al. Ethanol as a hypnotic in insomniacs: Self administration and
effects of sleep and mood. Neuropsychopharmacology 1999; 20:279–286.
58. Roehrs, Blaisdell B, Cruz N, et al. Tolerance to hypnotic effects of ethanol in insomnias. Sleep 2004;
27(abstract suppl):A52.
59. Roehrs TA, Blaisdell B, Richardson GS, et al. Insomnia as a path to alcoholism: Dose escalation. Sleep
2003; 26(abstract suppl):A307.
60. Compton-McBride S, Schweitzer, Walsh JK. Most commonly used drugs to treat insomnia in 2002.
Sleep 2004; 27(abstract suppl):A255.
61. Roth T, Durrence H, Gotfried M, et al. Efficacy and safety of doxepin 1 and 3 mg in a 3-month trial of
elderly adults with chronic primary insomnia. Sleep 2008; 31(abstract suppl):A230.
62. Mendelson WB. A review of the evidence for the efficacy and safety of trazodone in insomnia. J Clin
Psychiatry 2005; 66:469–476.
63. Walsh JK, Erman M, Erwin CW, et al. Subjective hypnotic efficacy of trazodone and zolpidem in
DSM-III-R primary insomnia. Hum Psychopharmacol 1998;13:191–198.
64. Thompson JW, Ware MR, Blashfield RK. Psychotropic medication and priapism: A comprehensive
review. J Clin Psychiatry 1990; 51:430–433.
65. Flores BH, Schatzberg AF. Mirtazepine. In: Schatzberg A, Nemeroff C, eds. The American Psychiatric
Publishing Textbook of Psychopharmacology. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing,
Inc., 2004:341–347.
66. Roth T, Roehrs TA, Vogel GW, et al. Evaluation of hypnotic medications. In: Prien RF, Robinson DS,
eds. Clinical Evaluation of Psychotropic Drugs: Principles and Guidelines. New York: Raven Press,
1995:579–592.
67. Mendelson WB. The use of sedative/hypnotic medication and its correlation with falling down in the
hospital. Sleep 1996; 19:698–701.
68. Ensrud KE, Blackwell TL, Mangione CM, et al. Central nervous system-active medications and risk
for falls in older women. J Am Geriatr Soc 2002; 50:1629–1637.
69. Kallin K, Lundin-Olsson L, Jensen J, et al. Predisposing and precipitating factors for falls among older
people in residential care. Public Health 2002; 116.
70. Thapa PB, Gideon P, Cost TW, et al. Antidepressants and the risk of falls among nursing home
residents 263–271. N Engl J Med 1998; 339:875–882.
71. Mendelson WB, Thompson C, Franko T. Adverse reactions to sedative hypnotics: Three years’ expe-
rience. Sleep 1996; 19:702–706.
72. Kato K, Hirai K, Nichiyama K, et al. Neurochemical properties of ramelteon (TAK-375), a selective
MT1 / MT2 receptor agonist. Neuropharmacology 2005; 48:301–310.
73. Karin A, Tolbert D, Cao C. Disposition kinetics and tolerance of escalating single doses of remelteon,
a high-affinity MT1 and MT2 melatonin receptor agonist indicated for treatment of insomnia. J Clin
Pharmacol 2006; 46:140–148.
74. National Institutes of Health State of the Science Conference. Manifestations and management of
chronic insomnia in adults. June 13–15, 2005. Sleep 2005; 28:1049–1057.
75. Bunney WE, Azarnoff DL, Brown BW, et al. Report of the Institute of Medicine Committee on the
efficacy and safety of halcyon. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:349–352.
c34 IHBK059-Sateia March 25, 2010 9:49 Char Count=
The inclusion of a separate chapter regarding the off-label use of prescription medications
for insomnia in this book is a reflection of the peculiarities of the treatment of insomnia in
the United States. The idiosyncratic nature of off-label prescribing in the field of insomnia
is underlined by the absence of similar chapters devoted to off-label prescribing in standard
textbooks on cardiac pharmacology, pulmonary pharmacology, etc. The need for such a chapter
in a textbook on insomnia treatment is derived from statistics on physician prescribing that, until
recently, revealed that off-label medications were prescribed preferentially for insomnia, in lieu
of medications that are FDA approved for insomnia. As recently as 2002, the most-prescribed
medication for insomnia in the United States was trazodone, which is approved by the FDA
as an antidepressant, not as a hypnotic (1) (Table 1). It had not always been this way, as in
1986 the FDA-approved hypnotics triazolam, flurazepam, and temazepam were the leading
choices among physicians (2). Somehow during the late 1980s and early 1990s, FDA-approved
hypnotics were surpassed by trazodone. It is not entirely clear how and why this happened,
but we can hypothesize the action of several factors including
r a belief among prescribers that trazodone, other antidepressants, and later, some antipsy-
chotics were reliably effective for treating insomnia;
r a belief among prescribers that trazodone, other antidepressants, and later, some antipsy-
chotics may be safer for patients than FDA-approved hypnotics;
r a belief among prescribers that FDA-approved hypnotics would be needed or asked for by
patients for a duration of time that outstripped their FDA-approved indication of use, thus
exposing the prescriber to potential liability if there was an untoward event.
The beliefs contained in the three hypotheses are for the most part unsubstantiated, as
there are few placebo-controlled trials to show that any non-FDA approved sleep aid is effective
in insomnia, or has fewer side effects than approved medications. However, it is true that until
recently, the use of FDA-approved hypnotics in the United States appeared to be constrained
by package labeling that recommended that schedule IV hypnotics not be given for more
than two consecutive weeks, unless the patient was reevaluated. FDA-approved treatments for
insomnia generally require (1) two randomized, placebo-controlled clinical trials in insomnia
patients showing a superior induction/maintenance of sleep for the investigational medication
as compared with placebo and (2) evidence of superiority in both patient report and an objective
measurement of sleep [i.e., polysomnography (PSG)]. The data supporting trazodone and other
non–FDA-approved medicines prescribed for sleep generally lack one or more of these elements.
The importance of each of these elements is defined below:
398 McCALL
Antidepressants
Amitriptyline 0.8
Doxepin 0.2
Mirtazapine 0.7
Trazodone 2.7
FDA-approved hypnotics
Flurazepam 0.2
Temazepam 0.6
Zaleplon 0.4
Zolpidem 2.0
Sedatives
Alprazolam 0.3
Clonazepam 0.4
Lorazepam 0.3
Antipsychotics
Olanzapine 0.2
Quetiapine 0.5
Antihistamines/sedative
Hydoxyzine 0.3
(SL) and increases in total sleep time (TST) are routinely seen with administration of
placebo alone, making it impossible to assume improvement in insomnia seen with
an investigational drug and is attributable to the drug and not the placebo effect, in
the absence of a placebo comparator (3,4).
2. The importance of testing in insomnia patients
Needless to say, medications may have different effects in persons who are ill than in persons
who are well. A classic example in insomnia research is the contrast of the effect of the
beta-blocker propranolol in good sleepers versus anxious sleepers. Placebo-controlled
comparisons of propranolol in good sleepers have shown a deterioration of sleep
(5,6), while a study in sleepers who were anticipating surgery the next day showed
better sleep with propranolol than with placebo (7). The take-home message is “in the
absence of testing done directly in the population of interest, it is hazardous to predict
how a medication might work in an ill-population”
3. The importance of measuring both patient-report as well as objective testing
Many non-FDA approaches to insomnia treatment have relied upon patient-report of improve-
ment without confirmation with an independent objective method (i.e., PSG), or occa-
sionally have relied upon PSG improvement, absent patient-reported improvement. The
hazards of both approaches are illustrated in the following two studies. The first study
examined the serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) fluoxetine in depressed insomniacs,
showing a perception of benefit, but concurrent PSG testing showed some worsening
of EEG-defined sleep in the same patients (8). In the second study, the anticonvulsant
tiagabine was found to enhance slow wave sleep (SWS), which is generally believed to
connote a benefit, but the same insomniac patients did not appreciate a meaningful subjec-
tive improvement in their sleep (9). These two studies show that patient’s impressions and
PSG data may dissociate in the direction of their effect. At the present time, it is impossible
to discern the meaning of patient benefit in the absence of PSG-benefit, or PSG-benefit in
the absence of patient-reported benefit. So, the present standard for showing anti-insomnia
efficacy is the demonstration of both patient-reported and PSG benefit.
The preferential use of non-FDA-approved approaches for insomnia entails its own unique
set of problems, over and above the risks inherent in the particular off-label medication pre-
scribed. In general, the FDA does not prohibit the use of approved medications for off-label
indications, as the FDA recognizes the judgment of the prescriber to best understand the needs
of the patient (10). However, the preferential avoidance of approved approaches and embrace
c34 IHBK059-Sateia March 25, 2010 9:49 Char Count=
Antidepressants
Amitriptyline 0.5
Mirtazapine 0.7
Trazodone 2.1
FDA-approved hypnotics
Eszopiclone 1.4
Ramelteon 0.9
Temazepam 0.6
Zaleplon 0.2
Zolpidem 3.6
Sedatives
Alprazolam 0.3
Clonazepam 0.5
Lorazepam 0.5
Antipsychotics
Quetiapine 0.9
Others
Clonidine 0.2
Cyclobenzaprine 0.2
Tizanidine 0.2
ANTIDEPRESSANTS
Trazodone
Pharmacology
Trazodone is a triazolopyridine antidepressant that was introduced in the United States in
1982 under the brand name Desyrel. It has relatively weak SSRI properties, and is a blocker
of postsynaptic serotonin receptors 5-HT1A , 5-HT1C , and 5-HT2 , as well as postsynaptic ␣1 -
adrenergic receptors. It has an elimination half-life of about five to nine hours. When prescribed
as an antidepressant, the usual doses of trazodone are ≥ 150 mg daily.
Sleep Efficacy
Although the sleep effects of trazodone have been poorly documented in persons who do not
have psychiatric disorders, it is widely believed to have beneficial effects on sleep, as reflected
in its favored status in the rank order of prescribing rates (Tables 1 and 3). Furthermore, a
recent survey revealed that trazodone was the first-line choice of 78% of psychiatrists when
prescribing for SSRI-related insomnia (12). This favoritism is baffling given the sparse evidence
c34 IHBK059-Sateia March 25, 2010 9:49 Char Count=
400 McCALL
1987 1996
Antidepressants
Amitriptyline 421 748
Doxepin 263 269
Trazodone 222 1328
FDA-approved hypnotics
Flurazepam 1677 373
Temazepam 1201 904
Triazolam 3199 209
Zolpidem — 1218
Sedatives
Alprazolam 397 250
Clonazepam 28 330
Lorazepam 346 398
of sleep effect in psychiatric patients. In 17 patients with insomnia associated with fluoxetine
or bupropion, the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index improved after one week of trazodone 50 mg,
compared with placebo (13). In seven patients with insomnia associated with the monoamine
oxidase inhibitor brofaromine, SWS increased and the number of polysomnographic awaken-
ings decreased after one week of trazodone 50 mg administration, compared with placebo (14).
The best evidence for a beneficial sleep effect comes from a single, large study in primary
insomniacs, albeit the period of study was for only two weeks. Walsh et al. conducted a three-
armed randomized comparison of bedtime doses of trazodone 50 mg, versus zolpidem 10 mg
and versus placebo in 278 primary insomniacs. Trazodone and zolpidem were both superior
to placebo in patient-reported reduction in SL for week 1, but trazodone did not separate from
placebo by week 2, while zolpidem maintained its efficacy in week 2. TST effects were mixed,
with both trazodone and zolpidem showing an advantage over placebo at week 1, but not week
2 (15).
In normal sleepers, trazodone 50 to 200 mg increases SWS over short periods of time (≤2
weeks) compared to placebo in 9, 8, and 6 subjects, respectively (16–18). As stated above, the
clinical significance of an increase in SWS is not always clear, although it is usually inferred to
be a sign of potential benefit.
Side Effects
The most common side effects of small bedtime doses of trazodone may be residual morning
sedation. Daytime doses of trazodone (TRZ) 100 mg impairs critical flicker fusion and choice
reaction time one to four hours later (19,20). Bedtime doses of TRZ 100 mg lowers BP and
impairs critical flicker fusion the next morning (21). Less common side effects include orthostatic
hypotension (from peripheral adrenergic blockade) and priapism (22). Priapism has an incidence
of about 1/6000 persons and can occur at low doses, early in treatment.
Amitriptyline
Pharmacology
Prior to the introduction of fluoxetine in the United States in 1987, tricyclic antidepressants
(TCAs) were first-line somatic treatment of major depressive episode (MDE), and TCA therapy
could be counted upon to produce reliable, early improvement in the sleep of persons with
insomnia and MDE, as compared to the relative lack of effect of psychotherapy on insomnia
(23). Amitriptyline is a TCA with a half-life of 20 to 30 hours. Its synaptic effects include reup-
take blockade of 5-HT, as well as anticholinergic, antihistaminergic, and ␣1 -blockade. Typical
antidepressant dosages are ≥ 75 mg.
c34 IHBK059-Sateia March 25, 2010 9:49 Char Count=
Sleep Efficacy
There are no data on the effect of amitriptyline on sleep in primary insomnia. However, in
depressed inpatients (many of whom are presumed to have had insomnia) a four-week course
of amitriptyline, escalated from 50 mg at bedtime (qhs) to 50 mg four times per day (qid),
was associated with an increase in PSG-determined TST, and a reduction in SL, early morning
awakening, and a general suppression of Rapid Eye Movement Sleep (REM) as compared to
baseline (24). This study did not have a placebo comparison and did not provide patient-reports.
Doxepin
Pharmacology
Doxepin is a TCA with a half-life of 10 to 25 hours. Like amitriptyline, at standard antidepressant
doses, doxepin has 5-HT and noradrenergic (NE) reuptake blockade properties, as well as
blockade of cholinergic, histaminergic, and ␣-adrenergic activity. Typical antidepressant doses
of doxepin are ≥ 75 mg daily. Smaller doses have been tested for hypnotic potential in primary
insomniacs. It is likely that at very low doses (<10 mg) doxepin’s meaningful pharmacologic
activity is almost exclusively antihistaminergic, with little or no significant effect in serotonergic
or adrenergic systems. Theoretically, this low-dose profile would facilitate sleep without other
side effects.
Sleep Efficacy
Doxepin 25 to 50 mg was superior over 4 weeks in 47 primary insomniacs in improving PSG
total sleep time, sleep efficiency and sleep quality, and daytime ability to work (25). These
effects were confirmed in a study of 67 young adults with primary insomnia who underwent
a four-period crossover design comparison of placebo versus doxepin 1, 3, and 6 mg (26). Each
treatment period lasted for two days. All three doses of doxepin in increased PSG-defined
TST and reduced Wake Time after Sleep Onset (WASO), while only the 6-mg dose reduced SL
compared with placebo. REM% decreased with the 3- and 6-mg dose with no change in SWS.
Patient-report showed superiority for doxepin in reducing SL and increasing TST only for the
6-mg dose.
Side Effects
The side effects of low-dose doxepin in primary insomniacs were infrequent and numerically
similar to placebo with the two-day exposure, four-way crossover design (26).
Nortriptyline
Pharmacology
Nortriptyline is a TCA with a half-life of 22 to 39 hours, potent reuptake blockade of NE, and
weaker reuptake of 5-HT. It also has anticholinergic effects as well as weak ␣1 -adrenergic and
histaminergic blockade.
Sleep Efficacy
There is no data regarding the efficacy of nortriptyline in primary insomnia, but in a sample
of 20 depressed insomniacs, acute exposure to nortriptyline reduced PSG-determined SL and
suppressed most measures of REM sleep. Patient-report was not described in this study (27).
In a somewhat different design, a comparison was made in the sleep profiles of previously
depressed, now-remitted elderly patients who had received one year of maintenance treatment
for depression with either nortriptyline versus placebo. Interestingly, the nortriptyline patients
had longer PSG-determined SL and no advantages in sleep maintenance, but the nortriptyline
patients did exhibit more intense SWS (28).
Side Effects
The most frequent side effect is dry mouth (29).
c34 IHBK059-Sateia March 25, 2010 9:49 Char Count=
402 McCALL
Mirtazapine
Pharmacology
Mirtazapine is a tetracyclic piperazinoazepine with a half-life of 22 to 40 hours. It has minimal
effect on monoamine uptake, but is a potent inhibitor of 5-HT2 and 5-HT3 receptors, as well as
central ␣2 -adrenergic receptors (30). Typical antidepressant doses are ≥15 mg daily.
Sleep Efficacy
There are no placebo-RCT in primary insomniacs. However, mirtazapine 30 mg reduced PSG
sleep latency and increased SWS in six good sleepers on the first night in the sleep lab compare
to placebo in a crossover design (31). Mirtazapine 30 mg increased PSG sleep efficiency and
SWS compared to placebo on the third night in the laboratory in 20 good sleepers in parallel-
arm clinical trial (32). In 22 depressed insomniacs randomized to either mirtazapine 45 mg
or fluoxetine 40 mg, the conclusion of 8 weeks of treatment saw greater reductions in PSG-
determined SL and increases in TST for mirtazapine as compared with fluoxetine. Patient-report
was not described (33).
Side Effects
Bedtime doses of Mirtazapine were associated with prolonged motor reaction times the next
day (34) and impaired actual driving performance at 30 mg, compared with placebo with acute,
but not chronic dosing (30).
ANTIPSYCHOTICS
Quetiapine
Pharmacology
Quetiapine is an atypical antipsychotic with a half-life of two to three hours. It has high affinity
for 5-HT2A receptors and weak affinity for dopamine, muscarinic, and adrenergic receptors.
Typical antipsychotic doses are 150 to 800 mg daily.
Sleep Efficacy
There are no randomized controlled trials in primary insomnia. Quetiapine 25 or 100 mg at
bedtime was superior to placebo for one night in increasing PSG total sleep time and subjective
sleep quality in 14 good sleepers under noisy conditions. The authors speculate that the benefi-
cial sleep effect was mediated through blockade of histaminergic, dopaminergic, and adrenergic
receptors. Of note, the 100-mg dose induced periodic limb movements (PLMs) (35). Quetiapine
has been tested in an open study of doses of 25 to 75 mg at bedtime for 6 weeks in 18 patients
with primary insomnia, finding there were PSG-determined and patient-report improvements
in TST, but not SL, compared to baseline (36). Quetiapine has been tested against placebo in
demented patients with associated sleep disturbance. Doses of quetiapine started at 25 mg and
could be increased to 125 mg by the fifth week. Quetiapine was associated with increases in
actigraphically determined TST as early as the first week, and this advantage was still seen at
the end of the fifth week (37).
Side Effects
Quetiapine is associated with weight gain, with the potential for glucose intolerance. Similar to
other atypical antipsychotics, the FDA has required a black box warning for quetiapine noting
increased risk of cerebrovascular accident in elderly patients.
Olanzapine
Pharmacology
Olanzapine is an atypical antipsychotic and a derivative of thienobenzodiazepines. It has potent
antagonist properties for 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C receptors, with weak affinity for dopamine recep-
tors. Olanzapine also is active at muscarinic, ␣1 -adrenergic, and histaminergic receptors (38).
Olanzapine has a half-life of 36 hours. Typical antipsychotic dosages are ≥5 mg daily.
c34 IHBK059-Sateia March 25, 2010 9:49 Char Count=
Sleep Efficacy
There are no studies of Olanzapine in primary insomnia. The only placebo-controlled study of
Olanzapine was conducted in nine good sleepers, using a three-way crossover comparison of
three-nights exposure to placebo versus Olanzapine 5 or 10 mg. Both doses of Olanzapine were
associated with PSG-determined reductions in SL and WASO, increases in TST and SWS, and
suppression of REM. These volunteers also reported better sleep quality with olanzapine than
placebo (38). The same authors later examined the addition of a three-week course of olanzapine
2.5 to 10 mg to ongoing SSRI therapy in 12 depressed patients with an inadequate response
to SSRI monotherapy. There was no placebo control. The findings were similar to the earlier
study, with reductions in SL and WASO, and gains in TST and SWS. Unexpectedly, patients
reported difficulty getting up in the morning, with an approximate one-hour increase in time
in bed at home (39). Similar small, uncontrolled studies of olanzapine have been undertaken in
persons with schizophrenia, using doses up to 20 mg for up to four weeks, and again reported
PSG-determined increases in TST and SWS, and reductions in WASO (40,41).
Side Effects
Olanzapine is associated with weight gain, with the potential for glucose intolerance. Similar to
other atypical antipsychotics, the FDA has required a black box warning for olanzapine noting
increased risk of cerebrovascular accident in elderly patients.
Other Antipsychotics
Although the atypical antipsychotics risperidone and ziprasidone and the conventional antipsy-
chotics haloperidol and thiothixene are not among the most common medications prescribed
for insomnia (Tables 1–3), it is noteworthy that in general their sleep effects mirror to a greater
or lesser degree what is reported for quetiapine and olanzapine, namely increases in TST and
SWS (42–44).
Anxiolytics
Pharmacology
Lorazepam, clonazepam and alprazolam are all classic benzodiazepines with rapid absorption
and respective half-lives of 10 to 20, 24 to 56, and 10 to 15 hours. All three are indicated
for treatment of anxiety states, and clonazepam is also indicated for epilepsy. There are no
fundamental differences between those benzodiazepine approved for insomnia and those not
approved for insomnia, other than pharmacokinetics and route of administration.
Lorazepam
Sleep Efficacy
Lorazepam 2.5 mg was compared in a parallel design against midazolam 15 mg and placebo in
60 adults who were dosed the night before surgery. Outcomes were recorded by direct nursing
observation and found that while midazolam was superior for sleep induction, lorazepam was
significantly superior to placebo for sleep maintenance and patient-reported quality of sleep
(45). A similar study of sleep on the night before surgery was conducted in 60 adults allocated in
a randomized, balanced, parallel design to either lorazepam 2 mg, lorazepam 4 mg, nitrazepam
10 mg, lormetazepam 1 mg, lormetazepam 2 mg, or placebo. Both doses of lorazepam were
associated with longer sleep on the preoperative night as compared to placebo (46). A crossover
study of six good sleepers compared on night dosing of lorazepam 1 mg versus zolpidem 10 mg,
zopiclone 7.5 mg, triazolam 0.25 mg, and placebo under noisy conditions found that lorazepam
reduced SL and increased TST (47).
Lorazepam 0.5 mg tid and then 1.5 mg at bedtime were examined in a two-stage crossover
comparison with placebo in 12 adults with primary insomnia. Each treatment phase lasted for
four days. Both dosing strategies of lorazepam were superior to placebo in both subjective and
PSG sleep (48). A single-blind examination of lorazepam 3 mg was conducted for 7 nights in
6 adults with primary insomnia without the benefit of a placebo comparison. By both patient-
report and PSG, lorazepam was associated with decreased SL and increased TST (49).
c34 IHBK059-Sateia March 25, 2010 9:49 Char Count=
404 McCALL
Side Effects
In six adults with primary insomniacs, lorazepam 3 mg was associated with poorer perfor-
mance on the digit symbol substitution task (DSST) and timed card-sorting task as compared
with performance at baseline (49). In 30 adults with primary insomnia, lorazepam 2 mg was
associated with greater reported problems with dizziness, drowsiness, and poor coordination
than placebo (50). A single dose of lorazepam 4 mg at bedtime was associated with more com-
plaints of sleepiness, confusion, slurred speech, clumsiness, and blurred vision than placebo
presurgical patients (46). Morning alertness was reduced in a different study of presurgical
patients after a single bedtime dose of lorazepam 2.5 mg. A one-week exposure to lorazepam
1 mg at bedtime was compared against bedtime doses of alprazolam 0.25 mg and placebo in six
good sleepers using a blinded, randomized crossover design. Lorazepam, but not alprazolam,
was associated with slower reaction time the morning following the final dose in each treatment
phase (51). Lorazepam has also been associated with drug-withdrawal insomnia after as little
as seven nights of continuous use of either 2 mg (52) or 4 mg (53).
Clonazepam
There are no double-blind randomized placebo-controlled trials of clonazepam in primary
insomnia. However, a single-blind study found that clonazepam 0.5 mg reduced PSG-
determined total wake time over seven nights of dosing as compared with the pretreatment
baseline (54). However, clonazepam has been tested using double-blind randomized placebo-
controlled trials in patients with PLMs and depression associated with insomnia.
In 80 depressed patients receiving fluoxetine, the coadministration of clonazepam 0.5 to
1.0 at bedtime for 21 days was superior to placebo in improving scores on the Hamilton Rating
Scale for Depression (HRSD) sleep items over the entire three weeks (55). The success of this
study led the authors to repeat the study using a longer period of drug administration: 50
depressed patients received fluoxetine and the coadministration of clonazepam 0.5 to 1.0 versus
placebo at bedtime for 18 weeks. Again, clonazepam was superior to placebo in Hamilton Rating
Scale for Depression sleep items for the first three weeks, but clonazepam did not separate from
placebo after three weeks (56). Neither study reported an advantage for clonazepam for any
clinical parameter other than sleep.
In a mixed sample of 10 restless leg syndrome patients and 16 PLM patients, a single bed-
time dose of clonazepam 1 mg reduced PSG-determined SL and increased TST compared with
baseline, without impacting the rate of PLMs (57). In a sample of 20 PLM patients, including 8
with insomnia, one month of clonazepam 0.5–2.0 mg was superior to placebo in a parallel-design
comparison (58). In this study, clonazepam was associated with PSG-determined reductions in
SL, PLM frequency, PLM arousals, and increases in TST.
Alprazolam
There are no randomized, placebo-controlled trials of alprazolam for insomnia. Using a single-
blind design, alprazolam 1 mg was administered for one week at bedtime in six adults with
primary insomnia. Compared with baseline, alprazolam was associated with PSG-determined
reduction in SL and WASO, and increases in TST. However, a worsening compared with baseline
was seen during the first week of single-blind discontinuation of alprazolam (59).
ANTICONVULSANTS
Although no anticonvulsants appear on the list of most common medications prescribed for
sleep, several studies have shown potential clinical promise of anticonvulsants in selected
populations.
FDA-approved treatments for insomnia are controlled substances and convey some risk for
abuse in alcoholics. Gabapentin, 1-(aminomethyl) cyclohexane acetic acid, is a GABA analogue
excreted unchanged through the kidneys. Gabapentin is not a controlled substance, is not
thought to have potential for abuse, and can be used in alcoholics with reduced hepatic function.
It has also been shown to increase SWS in normals (60).
Gabapentin may also be helpful in treating insomnia during alcoholic abstinence. Fifty
alcohol-dependent outpatients with persisting insomnia were treated with either gabapentin
or trazodone for four to six weeks, according to the prescriber’s preference. Thirty-four were
treated with gabapentin (mean dose 888 mg) at bedtime and 16 were treated with trazodone
(105 mg) at bedtime. The gabapentin group improved significantly more than the trazodone
group in insomnia severity (61).
Finally, gabapentin has been shown to reduce alcohol use and cravings, as compared with
placebo (62).
Gabapentin has also been shown to reduce pain-related sleep disturbance in a variety
of pain syndromes, including fibromyalgia (63), peripheral neuropathy (64–66), and traumatic
nerve injury pain (67). Dosing of gabapentin for neuropathic pain is higher than what is used
for alcohol abstinence insomnia; doses of 2400 to 3600 mg/day would be common.
Pregabalin is closely related to gabapentin, and like gabapentin, it interferes with influx
of calcium into nerve terminals. Pregabalin’s half-life is five to six hours and it is exclusively
eliminated by the kidney. It is FDA approved for the treatment of fibromyalgia, neuropathic
pain, postherpetic neuralgia, and epilepsy. It has a robust literature showing that treatment
of these pain syndromes with pregabalin results in less patient-reported, pain-related sleep
disturbance at doses of 150 to 600 mg/day (68–71). Like gabapentin, pregabalin enhances SWS
in normal volunteers (72).
Valproic Acid
Sleep loss has been described as a risk factor for the precipitation or perpetuation of acute
mania. Therefore, it is likely that treatments for acute mania should have effects that sup-
port sleep induction or sleep maintenance. Valproic acid in the form of divalproex was
tested in 377 patients with acute mania, with patients randomly assigned in a 1:1 ratio to
21 days of double-blind treatment with divalproex ER (N = 192) or placebo (N = 185). Daily
dosage was increased to 500 mg daily by day 3. Treatment with divalproex was associated
with improvement on a mania rating scale, but specifically with reported improvement in
sleep (73).
Tiagabine
Tiagabine is a GABA uptake inhibitor with a half-life of seven to nine hours. Two hundred
and thirty-two men and women with primary insomnia were randomly assigned to receive
tiagabine 4, 6, 8, 10 mg or placebo in a randomized, double-blind, parallel-group study. Efficacy
was assessed using PSG and self-report measures. No significant differences were observed
between tiagabine and placebo in wake after sleep onset, latency to persistent sleep, or TST.
This study is important for testing the concept of whether induction of SWS is pathognomonic
for an effective insomnia agent, showing that SWS induction and anti-insomnia efficacy are
dissociable (9). Tiagabine is no longer under commercial development as a hypnotic.
CONCLUSIONS
Doxepin is the only medication with frequent off-label use for insomnia that is supported
by double-blind, placebo-controlled studies in primary insomnia, confirming superior activity
confirmed by both patient-report and PSG. Doxepin may receive FDA approval by the time this
chapter is published. Although none of the other medications described in this chapter meet
this same standard, there is still a suggestion of sleep efficacy for most of them. Intriguingly,
many of these medications enhance SWS, a feature not seen in the most recent FDA-approved
hypnotics (74–76). Although the lessons learned from tiagabine have shown that enhancement
of SWS can be divorced from the usual signs of sleep efficacy, enhancement of SWS seems to be
a near-universal feature of the off-label approaches to insomnia treatment.
c34 IHBK059-Sateia March 25, 2010 9:49 Char Count=
406 McCALL
If the use of off-label medications is driven in part by the belief that these medications
are in some way safer than FDA-approved hypnotics, then it is surprising how little safety
information is there about use of these off-label approaches in insomniacs. What data does exist
typically shows some degree of next-day impairment, bringing into question whether these
off-label approaches are really any safer than FDA-approved insomnia treatments.
RECOMMENDATIONS
None of the off-label approaches to treating insomnia can be recommended as first-line treatment
for primary insomnia, perhaps with the exception of low-dose doxepin. However, off-label
medications may be justified as monotherapy or as add-on therapy if FDA-approved treatments
fail, or if the patient has a strong preference for avoiding controlled substances. Also, off-label
approaches may be indicated in specific clinical circumstances, such as given in the following:
r Trazodone, tricyclic antidepressants, or mirtazapine for insomnia associated with depression
r Quetiapine or olanzapine for insomnia associated with psychosis
r Lorazepam, clonazepam, or alprazolam for insomnia associated with anxiety
r Clonazepam for insomnia associated with PLMs
r Valproic acid for insomnia associated with bipolar disorder
r Gabapentin or pregabalin for insomnia associated with alcohol abstinence or pain
REFERENCES
1. Walsh JK. Drugs used to treat insomnia in 2002: Regulatory-based rather than evidence-based
medicine. Sleep 2004; 27:1441–1442.
2. Walsh JK, Schweitzer PK. Ten-year trends in the pharmacological treatment of insomnia. Sleep 1999;
22:371–375.
3. McCall V, D’Agostino RB Jr, Dunn A. A meta-analysis of sleep changes associated with placebo in
hypnotic clinical trials. Sleep Med 2003; 4:57–62.
4. McCall V, Perlis M, Tu X, et al. A comparison of placebo and no-treatment during a hypnotic clinical
trial. Int J Clin Pharmacol Ther 2005; 43:355–359.
5. Betts TA, Alford C. Beta-blockers and sleep: A controlled trial. Eur J Clin Pharmacol 1985; 28:65–68.
6. Kostis JB, Rosen RC. Central nervous system effects of beta-adrenergic-blocking drugs: The role of
ancillary properties. Circulation 1987; 75:204–212.
7. Chierichetti SM, Moise G, Galeone M, et al. Beta-blockers and psychic stress: A double-blind, placebo-
controlled study of bopindolol vs lorazepam and butalbital in surgical patients. Int J Clin Pharmacol
Ther Toxicol 1985; 23:510–514.
8. Gillin JC, Rapaport M, Erman MK, et al. A comparison of nefazodone and fluoxetine on mood and
on objective, subjective, and clinician-rated measures of sleep in depressed patients: A double-blind,
8-week clinical trial. J Clin Psychiatry 1997; 58:185–192.
9. Walsh J, Perlis M, Rosenthal M, et al. Tiagabine increases slow-wave sleep in a dose-dependent fashion
without affecting traditional efficacy measures in adults with primary insomnia. J Clin Sleep Med 2006;
2:35–41.
10. Kuntz R. Off-label prescribing of antidepressants and anxiolytics: An attorney’s guide to psychoactive
drugs. J Psychiatry and the Law 1998; 26:519–532.
11. Kramer S, McCall V. Off-label prescribing: 7 steps for safer, more effective treatment. Curr Psychiatr
2006; 5:15–28.
12. Dording C, Mischoulon D, Petersen T, et al. The pharmacologic management of SSRI-induced side
effects: A survey of psychiatrists. Ann Clin Psychiatry 2002; 14:143–147.
13. Nierenberg A, Adler LA, Peselow E, et al. Trazodone for antidepressant-associated insomnia. Am J
Psychiatry 1994; 151:1069–1072.
14. Haffmans PMJ. The effects of trazodone on sleep disturbances included by brofaromine. Eur Psychiatry
1999; 14:167–171.
15. Walsh JK, Erman M, Erwin CW, et al. Subjective hypnotic efficacy of trazodone and zolpidem in
DSMIII-R primary insomnia. Hum Psychopharmacol 1998; 13:191–198.
16. Montgomery I, Oswald I, Morgan K, et al. Trazodone enhances sleep in subjective quality but not in
objective duration. Br J Clin Pharmacol 1983; 16:139–144.
17. Yamadera H, Nakamura S, Suzuki H, et al. Effects of trazodone hydrochloride and imipramine on
polysomnography in healthy subjects. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1998; 52:439–443.
18. Ware JC, Pittard JT. Increased deep sleep after trazodone use: A double-blind placebo-controlled study
in healthy young adults. J Clin Psychiatry 1990; 51:18–22.
c34 IHBK059-Sateia March 25, 2010 9:49 Char Count=
19. Burns M, Hoskowitz H, Jaffe J. A comparison of the effects of trazodone and amitriptyline on skills
performance by geriatric subjects. J Clin Psychiatry 1986; 47:252–254.
20. Warrington SJ, Ankier SI, Turner P. An evaluation of possible interactions between ethanol and tra-
zodone or amitriptyline. Br J Clin Pharmacol 1984; 18:549–557.
21. Saletu-Zyhlarz G, Abu-Bakr M, Anderer P, et al. Insomnia related to dysthymia: Polysomnographic
and psychometric comparison with normal controls and acute therapeutic trials with trazodone.
Neuropsychobiology 2001; 44:139–149.
22. James S, Mendelson W. The use of trazodone as a hypnotic: A critical review. J Clin Psychiatry 2004;
65:752–755.
23. DiMascio A, Weissman MM, Prusoff BA, et al. Differential symptom reduction by drugs and psy-
chotherapy in acute depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1979; 36:1450–1456.
24. Kupfer DJ, Spiker DG, Coble P, et al. Amitriptyline and EEG sleep in depressed patients: I. Drug effect.
Sleep 1978; 1:149–159.
25. Hajak G, Rodenbeck A, Voderholzer U, et al. Doxepin in the treatment of primary insomnia: A placebo-
controlled, double-blind, polysomnographic study. J Clin Psychiatry 2001; 62:453–463.
26. Roth T, Rogowski R, Hull S, et al. Efficacy and safety of doxepin 1 mg, 3 mg, and 6 mg in adults with
primary insomnia. Sleep 2007; 30:1555–1561.
27. Kupfer DJ, Spiker DG, Rossi A, et al. Nortriptyline and EEG sleep in depressed patients. Biol Psychiatry
1982; 17:535–546.
28. Reynolds CF III, Buysse D, Brunner D, et al. Maintenance nortriptyline effects on electroencephalo-
graphic sleep in elderly patients with recurrent major depression: Double-blind, placebo- and plasma-
level-controlled evaluation. Biol Psychiatry 1997; 42:560–567.
29. Haggstram FM, Chatkin JM, Sussenbach-Vaz E, et al. A controlled trial of nortriptyline, sustained-
release bupropion and placebo for smoking cessation: Preliminary results. Pulm Pharmacol Ther 2006;
19:205–209.
30. Wingen M, Bothmer J, Langer S, et al. Actual driving performance and psychomotor function in
healthy subjects after acute and subchronic treatment with escitalopram, mirtazapine, and placebo: A
crossover trial. J Clin Psychiatry 2005; 66:436–443.
31. Ruigt GSF, Kemp B, Groenhout CM, et al. Effect of the antidepressant Org 3770 on human sleep. Eur
J Clin Pharmacol 1990; 38:551–554.
32. Aslan S, Isik E, Cosar B. The effects of mirtazapine on sleep: A placebo controlled, double-blind study
in young healthy volunteers. Sleep 2002; 25:677–679.
33. Winokur A, DeMartinis NA III, McNally DP, et al. Comparative effects of mirtazapine and fluoxetine
on sleep physiology measures in patients with major depression and insomnia. J Clin Psychiatry 2003;
64:1224–1229.
34. Ridout F, Meadows R, Johnsen S, et al. A placebo controlled investigation into the effects of paroxetine
and mirtazapine on measures related to car driving performance. Hum Psychopharmacol 2003; 18:261–
269.
35. Cohrs S, Rodenbeck A, Guan Z, et al. Sleep-promoting properties of quetiapine in healthy subjects.
Psychopharmacology 2004; 174:421–429.
36. Wiegand M, Landry F, Bruckner T, et al. Quetiapine in primary insomnia: A pilot study. Psychophar-
macology 2008; 196:337–338.
37. Nanda F, Singer C. Placebo-controlled trial of quetiapine for sleep disturbance in dementia and MCI
[abstract]. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2008; 16(3)(suppl I):A126.
38. Sharpley AlL, Vassallo CM, Cowen PJ. Olanzapine increases slow-wave sleep: Evidence for blockade
of central 5-HT2 C receptors in vivo. Soc Biol Psychiatry 2000; 47:468–470.
39. Sharpley AL, Attenburrow EJ, Hafizi S, et al. Olanzapine increases slow wave sleep and sleep conti-
nuity in SSRI-resistant depressed patients. J Clin Psychiatry 2005; 66:450–454.
40. Salin-Pascual RJ, Herrera-Estrella M, Galicia-Polo L, et al. Olanzapine acute administration
in schizophrenic patients increases delta sleep and sleep efficiency. Biol Psychiatry 1999; 46:
141–143.
41. Muller MJ, Rossbach WMK, Roschke J, et al. Subchronic effects of olanzapine on sleep EEG
in schizophrenic patients with predominantly negative symptoms. Pharmacopsychiatry 2004; 37:
157–162.
42. Maixner S, Tandon R, Eiser A, et al. Effects of antipsychotic treatment on ploysomnographic measures
in schizophrenia: A replication and extension. Am J Psychiatry 1998; 155:1600–1602.
43. Cohrs S, Meier A, Neumann A, et al. Improved sleep continuity and increased slow wave sleep and
REM latency during ziprasidone treatment: A randomized, controlled, crossover trial of 12 healthy
male subjects. J Clin Psychiatry 2005; 66:989–996.
44. Sharpley AL, Bhagwager Z, Hafizi S, et al. Risperidone augmentation decreases rapid eye movement
sleep and decreases wake in treatment-resistant depressed patients. J Clin Psychiatry 2003; 64:192–196.
c34 IHBK059-Sateia March 25, 2010 9:49 Char Count=
408 McCALL
45. Mattila MAK, Salmela J, Vaananen A, et al. Midazolam vs lorazepam and placebo as hypnotic pre-
medication before surgery. A controlled, double-blind study. Drugs Exp Clin Res 1985; 12:841–844.
46. Sanders LD, Yeomans WA, Rees J, et al. A double-blind comparison between nitrazepam, lorazepam,
lormetazepam and placebo as preoperative night sedatives. Eur J Anaesthesiol 1988; 5:377–383.
47. Parrino L, Boselli M, Spaggiari MC, et al. Multidrug comparison (lorazepam, triazolam, zolpidem,
and zopiclone) in situational insomnia: Polysomnographic analysis by means of the cyclic alternating
pattern. Clin Neuropharmacol 1997; 20:253–263.
48. Bonnet MH, Arand DL. The use of lorazepam TID for chronic insomnia. Int Clin Psychopharmacol
1999; 14:81–89.
49. Walsh JK, Schweitzer PK, Parwatikar S. Effects of lorazepam and its withdrawal on sleep, performance,
and subjective state. Clin Pharmacol Ther 1983; 34:496–500.
50. Cohn JB. Double-blind crossover comparison of triazolam and lorazepam in the posthypnotic state. J
Clin Psychiatry 1984; 45:104–107.
51. Jurado JL, Fernandez-Mas R, Gernandez-Guardiola A. Effects of 1 week administration of two ben-
zodiazepines on the sleep and early daytime performance of normal subjects. Psychopharmacology
1989; 99:91–93.
52. Kales A, Bixler EO, Soldatos C, et al. Lorazepam: Effects on sleep and withdrawal phenomena.
Pharmacology 1986; 32:121–130.
53. Scharf MB, Kales A, Bixler EO, et al. Lorazepam—efficacy, side effects, and rebound phenomena. Clin
Pharmacol Ther 1982; 31:175–179.
54. Kales A, Manfredi RL, Vgontzas AN, et al. Clonazepam: Sleep laboratory study of efficacy and
withdrawal. J Clin Psychopharmacol 1991; 11:189–193.
55. Londborg PD, Smith WT, Glaudin V, et al. Short-term cotherapy with clonazepam and fluoxetine:
Anxiety, sleep disturbance and core symptoms of depression. J Affect Disord 2000; 61:73–79.
56. Smith WT, Londborg PD, Glaudin V, et al. Is extended clonazepam cotherapy of fluoxetine effective
for outpatients with major depression? J Affect Disord 2002; 70:251–259.
57. Saletu M, Anderer P, Saletu-Zyhlarz G, et al. Restless legs syndrome (RLS) and periodic limb move-
ment disorder (PLMD). Acute placebo-controlled sleep laboratory studies with clonazepam. Eur
Neuropsychopharmacol 2001; 11:153–161.
58. Peled R, Lavie P. Double-blind evaluation of clonazepam on periodic leg movements in sleep. J Neurol
Neurosurg Psychiatry 1987; 50:1679–1681.
59. Kales A, Bixler EO, Vela-Bueno A, et al. Alprazolam: Effects on sleep and withdrawal phenomena. J
Clin Pharmacol 1987; 27:508–515.
60. Foldvary-Schaefer N, De Leon Sanchez I, Karafa M, et al. Gabapentin increases slow-wave sleep in
normal adults. Epilepsia 2002; 43:1493–1497.
61. Karam-Hage M, Brower K. Open pilot study of gabapentin versus trazodone to treat insomnia in
alcoholic outpatients. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2003; 57:542–544.
62. Furieri F, Nakamura-Placios E. Gabapentin reduces alcohol consumption and craving: A randomized,
double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. J Clin Psychiatry 2007; 68:1691–1700.
63. Arnold LM, Goldenberg D, Stanford S, et al. Gabapentin in the treatment of fibromyalgia: A random-
ized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, multicenter trial. Arthritis Rheum 2007; 56:1336–1344.
64. Hahn K, Arendt G, Braun J, et al. A placebo-controlled trial of gabapentin for painful HIV-associated
sensory neuropathies. J Neurol 2004; 251:1260–1266.
65. Rice A, Maton S; Postherpetic Neuralgia Study Group. Gabapentin in postherpetic neuralgia: A
randomised, double blind, placebo controlled study. Pain 2001; 94:215–224.
66. Backonja M. Gabapentin monotherapy for the symptomatic treatment of painful neuropathy: A
multicenter, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial in patients with diabetes mellitus. Epilepsia
1990;40(suppl):S57–S59.
67. Gordh T, Stubhaug A, Jenson T, et al. Gabapentin in traumatic nerve injury pain: A randomized,
double-blind, placebo-controlled, cross-over, multi-center study. Pain 2008; 138:255–266.
68. Arnold LM, Russell I, Diri E, et al. A 14-week, randomized, double-blinded, placebo-controlled
monotherapy trial of pregabalin in pateints with fibromyalgia. J Pain 2008; 9:792–805.
69. Freeman R, Durso-Decruz E, Emir B. Efficacy, safety, and tolerability of pregabalin treatment for
painful diabetic peripheral neuropathy: Findings from seven randomized, controlled trials across a
range of doses. Diabetes Care 2008; 31:1448–1454.
70. Mease P, Russell I, Arnold LM, et al. A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, phase III trial
of pregabalin in the treatment of patients with fibromyalgia. J Rheumatol 2008; 35:502–514.
71. Siddall P, Cousins M, Otte A, et al. Pregabalin in central neuropathic pain associated with spinal cord
injury: A placebo-controlled trial. Neurology 2006; 67:1792–1800.
72. Hindmarch I, Dawson J, Stanley N. A double-blind study in healthy volunteers to assess the effects
on sleep of pregabalin compared with alprazolam and placebo. Sleep 2005; 28:187–193.
c34 IHBK059-Sateia March 25, 2010 9:49 Char Count=
73. Bowden CL, Swann A, Calabrese J, et al. A randomized, placebo-controlled, multicenter study of
divalproex sodium extended release in the treatment of acute mania. J Clin Pyshciatry 2006; 67:
1501–1510.
74. Zammit GK, McNabb LJ, Caron J, et al. Efficacy and safety of eszopiclone across 6-weeks of treatment
for primary insomnia. Curr Med Res Opin 2004; 20:1979–1991.
75. Roth T, Soubrane C, Titeux L, et al. Eficacy and safety of zolpidem-MR: A double-blind, placebo-
controlled study in adults with primary insomnia. Sleep Med 2006; 7:397–406.
76. Zammit GK, Erman M, Wang-Weigand S, et al. Evaluation of the efficacy and safety of ramelteon in
subjects with chronic insomnia. J Clin Sleep Med 2007; 3:495–504.
c35 IHBK059-Sateia March 25, 2010 9:53 Char Count=
INTRODUCTION
The circadian system plays an essential role in regulating the daily cycles of physiological and
behavioral functions, such as core body temperature, hormonal rhythms of cortisol and mela-
tonin secretion, and levels of sleepiness, alertness, and performance (1). Circadian rhythms are
endogenously generated cellular, physiologic or behavioral cycles with a genetically encoded
period of approximately 24 hours. In mammals, circadian rhythms are regulated by a central
circadian clock located in the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) of the anterior hypothalamus
(2–4). The endogenous circadian period in humans has been shown to be approximately
24.2 hours (5). Because under normal conditions, the circadian system is synchronized or
entrained to the 24-hour rotation of the earth, individuals with an endogenous period longer
or shorter than 24 hours require a daily net advance or delay of the SCN-driven rhythm (6).
Light, melatonin, and high-intensity physical activity have been shown to act as synchronizing
agents for the circadian clock. However, light is the primary circadian synchronizing agent in
humans (7).
In humans, the most apparent function of the circadian system is the regulation of the
sleep–wake cycle (8). Circadian propensity for sleep and wakefulness is regulated by multisy-
naptic pathways from the SCN to wake and sleep promoting areas of the brain (9–12). Circadian
synchronizing agents such as light and melatonin not only reset the timing of circadian rhythms
but also can influence the firing rate of SCN neurons and thus modulate levels of alertness
and sleepiness. Thus, both light and melatonin have been studied for the treatment of sleep
disorders, including insomnia. In this chapter, we will discuss the evidence for a role of endoge-
nous melatonin in sleep regulation and the use of exogenous melatonin and melatonin receptor
agonists as treatments for insomnia.
Melatonin
Pineal
SCN
MT2
MT1
VSPZ VLPO Sleep
PVN DMH
LHA Wakefulness
SCG
Figure 1 This is a schematic representation of the role of the circadian system and pineal melatonin in the
regulation of sleep and wakefulness. Light–dark information is received by the retina (retinal ganglion cells
that are most responsive to short wavelength (blue) light) and travels via the retinohypothalamic tract to the
SCN. Information travels from the SCN via the paraventricular nucleus (PVN) to the sympathetic preganglionic
thoracic spinal cord neurons, which in turn project to the superior cervical ganglia (SCG) to signal the pineal to
produce melatonin. Melatonin binding to the MT1 receptors primarily produces inhibition of SCN neuron firing, and
melatonin binding primarily to MT2 receptors phase shift neural firing rhythms in the SCN. The SCN influences
sleep and wakefulness via efferent projections to other brain sleep [ventrolateral preoptic nucleus (VLPO)] and
arousal [lateral hypothalamus (LHA)] centers. The SCN sends projections to the ventral subparaventricular zone
(vSPZ) which in turn sends projections to the dorsomedial nucleus of the hypothalamus (DMH) and finally to the
VLPO and LHA.
It is thought that exogenous melatonin promotes sleep by its ability to inhibit electrical activity
or wake promoting signal from the SCN. Activation of MT1 and MT2 SCN receptors (primarily
MT2) can also reset the timing of circadian rhythms (21–23). Although the function of the MT3
receptor is not well understood, it is not likely to play an important role in sleep regulation
(24). Under normally entrained conditions, the activity of the MT1 and MT2 receptors promotes
sleep and also synchronizes the regular timing of the sleep–wake cycle. These distinct roles
for the different melatonin receptors offer opportunities for receptor-specific drug therapies of
circadian-based sleep disorders and insomnia.
EXOGENOUS MELATONIN
Exogenous melatonin has been shown to be efficacious for the treatment of sleep disturbances
associated with circadian rhythm sleep disorders. Substantial evidence shows that when timed
appropriately, melatonin is able to realign and synchronize circadian rhythms. On the basis of
its phase–response curve, melatonin, when given in the late afternoon/evening (prior to the
nadir of the core body temperature rhythm), produces phase advances, whereas, in the late
night/early morning (after the nadir of the core body temperature rhythm), it produces phase
delays in physiological and behavioral rhythms, including that of the sleep–wake cycle (25,26).
Because melatonin possesses both chronobiotic as well as soporific effects, it has been studied
and indicated for the treatment of circadian rhythm sleep disorders, such as delayed sleep phase
disorder, free-running disorder in blind people, jet lag disorder, and shiftwork sleep disorder
(27–30). However, in this section, we will focus on the use of exogenous melatonin for the
treatment of insomnia.
When Dr. Aaron Lerner first identified the pineal hormone melatonin, he connected it to
previously described lightening effects of pineal extracts on amphibian skin. He gave melatonin
to patients with vitiligo, hoping to treat the skin condition, but instead noted that his patients
became sleepy (13,31). Since then, several other studies have shown that administration of
exogenous melatonin during the biological day (when endogenous melatonin levels are low)
can promote sleep (32,33). These findings, together with findings of an association between
decreased level of nocturnal melatonin and pineal calcification (34,35) with insomnia provide
further rationale for using melatonin to improve sleep quality in insomniacs. However, clinical
studies on the efficacy of melatonin for the treatment of insomnia have yielded inconsistent
results. Such inconsistencies may be due to differences in pharmacokinetic properties of the
melatonin preparation, purity of the formulations, doses, and timing of administration that
c35 IHBK059-Sateia March 25, 2010 9:53 Char Count=
were used in the various studies (36). Furthermore, clinical studies have not used well-defined
inclusion and exclusion criteria, and outcome measures to evaluate melatonin’s efficacy have
been inconsistent across studies.
This latter point is highlighted in recent meta-analyses and systematic reviews that eval-
uated the efficacy and safety of exogenous melatonin in the treatment of primary and sec-
ondary sleep disorders (37,38). One meta-analysis of 14 randomized controlled trials concluded
that with short-term use, melatonin was ineffective in treating most primary sleep disorders,
including insomnia. Although the authors did note that melatonin may be useful for the treat-
ment of sleep-onset insomnia associated with delayed sleep phase disorder (38). However, a
meta-analysis of 17 studies that included mixed populations of healthy volunteers, insomnia
patients, and individuals with psychiatric conditions concluded that melatonin is both effective
in improving sleep efficiency and decreasing sleep-onset latency (37).
Some of the more positive effects of melatonin treatment for insomnia have come from
studies in older adults (39–43), in particular those with low melatonin levels (34,39,41,44). Of
the various formulations of melatonin used, the prolonged-release (PR) formulations appear
to produce more consistent improvements in sleep maintenance (41), latency to persistent
sleep (LPS) (42), and subjective sleep quality (43). The effects of three-week prolonged-release
melatonin 2 mg was evaluated in a multicenter, randomized placebo-controlled study in 170
patients, aged 55 years and older with primary insomnia (43). In this large study, prolonged-
release melatonin improved measures of subjective sleep quality and morning alertness. There
were no withdrawal or rebound symptoms upon discontinuation and the adverse effects were
generally mild and not significantly different from placebo. Yet, it is important to note that
there are also studies in which melatonin did not improve sleep quality in older adults with
insomnia (45,46).
Melatonin has also been studied for the treatment of sleep disturbances in older adults
with dementia. Similar to the studies in general adults, studies in patients with dementia
have yielded inconsistent results (47,48). The largest multicenter randomized placebo-controlled
study of patients with Alzheimer Disease failed to show significant improvements in actigraphy
derived sleep measures with melatonin 5 mg, but there was a trend for improvement in the
group that received 10 mg (47,48). A recent randomized controlled trial in older adults with
dementia showed that melatonin 2.5 mg taken in the evening shortened sleep-onset latency
and increased average sleep duration by 27 minutes (47,48). However, melatonin had negative
effects on affect and also increased withdrawn behavior. Interestingly, these adverse effects were
not seen if melatonin was given in combination with daytime bright light, suggesting that in
this patient population, although low-dose melatonin can improve sleep, it is more effective
when combined with bright light exposure during the day (49).
While melatonin has been shown to be effective in treating insomnia in some studies, the
lack of large-scale, double-blind, placebo-controlled clinical trials in the general adult population
with insomnia, the variety of available preparations, differences in doses used, and concerns
about long-term effects have led the NIH State-of-the Science Consensus Statement on Chronic
Insomnia panel to conclude that “While melatonin appears to be effective for the treatment
of circadian rhythm disorders, little evidence exists for efficacy in the treatment of insomnia
or its appropriate dosage. In short-term use, melatonin is thought to be safe, but there is no
information about the safety of long-term use” (50).
asleep when compared to placebo (53,54). In patients with chronic primary insomnia, ramelteon
8 mg taken 30 minutes before bedtime improved LPS in the first week and for the duration of
five-week (55,60) and six-month studies (61). Subjective sleep latency improved in some, but
not in all studies, and was not maintained at all time points throughout the six-month period.
Improvements in other objective and self-reported sleep parameters, including total sleep time
were inconsistent among various studies.
Ramelteon has also been studied in special populations, such as older adults. Older adults
are particularly interesting because melatonin levels may decline with aging (62,63). In a large
study of older adults, 65 to 93 years with chronic insomnia ramelteon administration over a
five-week period significantly decreased self-reported sleep latency (56,57,64). However, there
were no significant differences between ramelteon and placebo on sleep quality and number of
awakenings.
In the clinical studies, ramelteon was generally well tolerated and was not associated
with withdrawal, abuse or rebound insomnia (55,57,65). The most common adverse events
were headache, somnolence, dizziness, and fatigue (55,56). Furthermore, ramelteon had no
significant effect on cognitive or psychomotor function compared to placebo (58,65) and no
impact on balance in the middle-of-the-night testing (61,66). Studies of long-term use (six
months) on endocrine function showed increases in prolactin in women, without measurable
effects on reproductive function (67). Other safety studies have included patients with mild to
moderate (68) and severe (69) chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (COPD) or obstructive
sleep apnea (70) in which administration of 8 mg of ramelteon showed no significant reductions
in oxygen saturation (68,69).
SUMMARY
Endogenous melatonin produced by the pineal gland plays an important role in sleep–wake
regulation. While exogenous melatonin has been shown to possess sleep-promoting properties,
studies on its effectiveness in the treatment of adults with chronic insomnia have yielded
inconsistent results. Some of these inconsistencies may be related to the differences in the study
populations, type of formulation, dose, and outcome measures of efficacy between studies.
Within the area of insomnia, the most promising results for melatonin come from studies in
late-middle age and older adults. Although melatonin has been shown to be effective in some
c35 IHBK059-Sateia March 25, 2010 9:53 Char Count=
short-term studies of insomnia and found to be generally safe in low doses of 1 to 5 mg, more
short-term and long-term multicenter randomized clinical trials are needed before evidence-
based clinical guidelines can be established for its use in the treatment of insomnia.
The development of melatonin receptor agonists with more specific and stronger affinities
for particular receptor subtypes within the SCN will likely lead to improved understanding of
the role of endogenous melatonin in sleep regulation and help develop more targeted therapies
for insomnia.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Funding for this work was provided by a National Institutes of Health grant R01HL069988.
REFERENCES
1. Zee PC, Manthena P. The brain’s master circadian clock: Implications and opportunities for therapy
of sleep disorders. Sleep Med Rev 2007; 11(1):59–70.
2. Inouye ST. Ventromedial hypothalamic lesions eliminate anticipatory activities of restricted daily
feeding schedules in the rat. Brain Res 1982; 250(1):183–187.
3. Meijer JH, Rietveld WJ. Neurophysiology of the suprachiasmatic circadian pacemaker in rodents.
Physiol Rev 1989; 69(3):671–707.
4. van den Pol, The hypothalamic suprachiasmatic nucleus of rat: Intrinsic anatomy. J Comp Neurol
1980; 191(4):661–702.
5. Czeisler CA, et al. Stability, precision, and near-24-hour period of the human circadian pacemaker.
Science 1999; 284(5423):2177–2181.
6. Moore-Ede MC, Czeisler CA, Richardson GS. Circadian timekeeping in health and disease. Part 1.
Basic properties of circadian pacemakers. N Engl J Med 1983; 309(8):469–476.
7. Czeisler CA, Wright KPJ. Influence of light on circadian rhythmicity in humans. In: FW Turck, PC Zee,
eds. Neurobiology of Sleep and Circadian Rhythms. New York: Marcel Dekker, 1999:149–180.
8. Dijk DJ, Lockley SW. Integration of human sleep-wake regulation and circadian rhythmicity. J Appl
Physiol 2002; 92(2):852–862.
9. Aston-Jones G. Brain structures and receptors involved in alertness. Sleep Med 2005; 6(suppl 1):S3–S7.
10. Chou TC et al. Afferents to the ventrolateral preoptic nucleus. J Neurosci 2002; 22(3):977–990.
11. Deurveilher S, Semba K. Indirect projections from the suprachiasmatic nucleus to major arousal-
promoting cell groups in rat: Implications for the circadian control of behavioural state. Neuroscience
2005; 130(1):165–183.
12. Zhang S et al. Lesions of the suprachiasmatic nucleus eliminate the daily rhythm of hypocretin-1
release. Sleep 2004; 27(4):619–627.
13. Lerner AB et al. Isolation of melatonin, the pineal gland factor that lightens melanocytes. J Am Chem
Soc 1958; 80:2587.
14. Moore RY. Suprachiasmatic nucleus in sleep-wake regulation. Sleep Med 2007; 8(suppl 3):27–33.
15. Moore RY. Neural control of the pineal gland. Behav Brain Res 1996; 73(1–2):125–130.
16. Burgess HJ et al. The relationship between the dim light melatonin onset and sleep on a regular
schedule in young healthy adults. Behav Sleep Med 2003; 1(2):102–114.
17. Monk TH et al. Circadian rhythms in human performance and mood under constant conditions. J
Sleep Res 1997; 6(1):9–18.
18. Nakamura TJ et al. Light response of the neuronal firing activity in the suprachiasmatic nucleus of
mice. Neurosci Lett 2004; 371(2–3):244–248.
19. Jin X et al. Targeted disruption of the mouse Mel(1b) melatonin receptor. Mol Cell Biol 2003; 23(3):1054–
1060.
20. Liu C et al. Molecular dissection of two distinct actions of melatonin on the suprachiasmatic circadian
clock. Neuron 1997; 19(1):91–102.
21. Dubocovich ML. Melatonin receptors: Role on sleep and circadian rhythm regulation. Sleep Med 2007;
8(suppl 3):34–42.
22. Hunt AE et al. Activation of MT(2) melatonin receptors in rat suprachiasmatic nucleus phase advances
the circadian clock. Am J Physiol Cell Physiol 2001; 280(1):C110–C118.
23. Dubocovich ML et al. Molecular pharmacology, regulation and function of mammalian melatonin
receptors. Front Biosci 2003; 8:d1093–d1108.
24. Nosjean O et al. Identification of the melatonin-binding site MT3 as the quinone reductase 2. J Biol
Chem 2000; 275(40):31311–31317.
25. Burgess HJ, Revell VL, Eastman CI. A three pulse phase response curve to three milligrams of melatonin
in humans. J Physiol 2008; 586(2):639–647.
c35 IHBK059-Sateia March 25, 2010 9:53 Char Count=
26. Lewy AJ et al. The human phase response curve (PRC) to melatonin is about 12 hours out of phase
with the PRC to light. Chronobiol Int 1998; 15(1):71–83.
27. Mundey K et al. Phase-dependent treatment of delayed sleep phase syndrome with melatonin. Sleep
2005; 28(10):1271–1278.
28. Nagtegaal JE et al. Delayed sleep phase syndrome: A placebo-controlled cross-over study on the
effects of melatonin administered five hours before the individual dim light melatonin onset. J Sleep
Res 1998; 7(2):135–143.
29. Lewy AJ et al. Capturing the circadian rhythms of free-running blind people with 0.5 mg melatonin.
Brain Res 2001; 918(1–2):96–100.
30. Sack RL et al. Entrainment of free-running circadian rhythms by melatonin in blind people. N Engl J
Med 2000; 343(15):1070–1077.
31. Nordlund JJ, Lerner AB. The effects of oral melatonin on skin color and on the release of pituitary
hormones. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1977; 45(4):768–774.
32. Reid K, van den Heuvel C, Dawson D. Day-time melatonin administration: Effects on core temperature
and sleep onset latency. J Sleep Res 1996; 5(3):150–154.
33. Wyatt JK et al. Sleep-facilitating effect of exogenous melatonin in healthy young men and women is
circadian-phase dependent. Sleep 2006; 29(5):609–618.
34. Leger D, Laudon M, Zisapel N. Nocturnal 6-sulfatoxymelatonin excretion in insomnia and its relation
to the response to melatonin replacement therapy. Am J Med 2004; 116(2):91–95.
35. Mahlberg R et al. Degree of pineal calcification (DOC) is associated with polysomnographic sleep
measures in primary insomnia patients. Sleep Med 2008; 10(4):439–445.
36. Doghramji K. Melatonin and its receptors: A new class of sleep-promoting agents. J Clin Sleep Med
2007; 3(5)(suppl):S17–S23.
37. Brzezinski A et al. Effects of exogenous melatonin on sleep: A meta-analysis. Sleep Med Rev 2005;
9(1):41–50.
38. Buscemi N et al. The efficacy and safety of exogenous melatonin for primary sleep disorders. A
meta-analysis. J Gen Intern Med 2005; 20(12):1151–1158.
39. Garfinkel D et al. Improvement of sleep quality in elderly people by controlled-release melatonin.
Lancet 1995; 346(8974):541–544.
40. Wurtman RJ, Zhdanova I. Improvement of sleep quality by melatonin. Lancet 1995; 346(8988):1491.
41. Haimov I et al. Melatonin replacement therapy of elderly insomniacs. Sleep 1995; 18(7):
598–603.
42. Hughes RJ, Sack RL, Lewy AJ. The role of melatonin and circadian phase in age-related sleep-
maintenance insomnia: Assessment in a clinical trial of melatonin replacement. Sleep 1998; 21(1):
52–68.
43. Lemoine P et al. Prolonged-release melatonin improves sleep quality and morning alertness in
insomnia patients aged 55 years and older and has no withdrawal effects. J Sleep Res 2007; 16(4):
372–380.
44. Zhdanova IV et al. Melatonin treatment for age-related insomnia. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2001;
86(10):4727–4230.
45. Dawson D et al. Effect of sustained nocturnal transbuccal melatonin administration on sleep and
temperature in elderly insomniacs. J Biol Rhythms 1998; 13(6):532–538.
46. Baskett JJ et al. Does melatonin improve sleep in older people? A randomised crossover trial. Age
Ageing 2003; 32(2):164–170.
47. Asayama K et al. Double blind study of melatonin effects on the sleep-wake rhythm, cognitive and
non-cognitive functions in Alzheimer type dementia. J Nippon Med Sch 2003; 70(4):334–341.
48. Singer C et al. A multicenter, placebo-controlled trial of melatonin for sleep disturbance in Alzheimer’s
disease. Sleep 2003; 26(7):893–901.
49. Riemersma-van der Lek RF et al. Effect of bright light and melatonin on cognitive and noncognitive
function in elderly residents of group care facilities: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 2008;
299(22):2642–2655.
50. National Institutes of Health. NIH State-of-the-Science Conference Statement: Manifestations and
Management of Chronic Insomnia in Adults. NIH Consensus and State-of-the-Science Statements.
Bethesda: National Institutes of Health 2005; 22(2): 1–30.
51. Simpson D, Curran MP. Ramelteon: A review of its use in insomnia. Drugs 2008; 68(13):1901–1919.
52. Kato K et al. Neurochemical properties of ramelteon (TAK-375), a selective MT1/MT2 receptor agonist.
Neuropharmacology 2005; 48(2):301–310.
53. Roth T, Stubbs C, Walsh JK. Ramelteon (TAK-375), a selective MT1/MT2-receptor agonist, reduces
latency to persistent sleep in a model of transient insomnia related to a novel sleep environment. Sleep
2005; 28(3):303–307.
54. Zammit G et al. The effects of ramelteon in a first-night model of transient insomnia. Sleep Med 2009;
10(1):55–59.
c35 IHBK059-Sateia March 25, 2010 9:53 Char Count=
55. Mini L, Wang-Weigand S, Zhang J. Ramelteon 8 mg/d versus placebo in patients with chronic insom-
nia: Post hoc analysis of a 5-week trial using 50% or greater reduction in latency to persistent sleep as
a measure of treatment effect. Clin Ther 2008; 30(7):1316–1323.
56. Mini LJ, Wang-Weigand S, Zhang J. Self-reported efficacy and tolerability of ramelteon 8 mg in older
adults experiencing severe sleep-onset difficulty. Am J Geriatr Pharmacother 2007; 5(3):177–184.
57. Roth T et al. Effects of ramelteon on patient-reported sleep latency in older adults with chronic
insomnia. Sleep Med 2006; 7(4):312–318.
58. Roth T et al. A 2-night, 3-period, crossover study of ramelteon’s efficacy and safety in older adults
with chronic insomnia. Curr Med Res Opin 2007; 23(5):1005–1014.
59. Stigler KA, Posey DJ, McDougle CJ. Ramelteon for insomnia in two youths with autistic disorder. J
Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol 2006; 16(5):631–636.
60. Zammit G et al. Evaluation of the efficacy and safety of ramelteon in subjects with chronic insomnia.
J Clin Sleep Med 2007; 3(5):495–504.
61. Wang-Weigand S, Mayer G, Roth-Schechter B. Long Term Efficacy and Safety of Ramelteon 8 mg
Treatment in Adults with Chronic Insomnia: Results of a Six Month, Double Blind, Placebo Controlled,
Polysomnography Trial in World Congress of the World Federation of Sleep Research and Medicine
Societies. Cairns, Queensland, Australia; 2007: A525.
62. Iguchi H. Age dependent changes in the serum melatonin concentrations in healthy human subjects
and in patients with endocrine and hepatic disorders and renal failure (author’s transl). Fukuoka
Igaku Zasshi 1981; 72(7):423–430.
63. Sack RL et al. Human melatonin production decreases with age. J Pineal Res 1986; 3(4):379–388.
64. Turek FW, Gillette MU. Melatonin, sleep, and circadian rhythms: Rationale for development of specific
melatonin agonists. Sleep Med 2004; 5(6):523–532.
65. Johnson MW, Suess PE, Griffiths RR. Ramelteon: A novel hypnotic lacking abuse liability and sedative
adverse effects. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006; 63(10):1149–1157.
66. Hajak G, Ebrahim I, M H. Effect of ramelteon and zopiclone on body sway at peak plasma levels
in chronic insomnia [abstract NR611]. In: Conference Proceedings of the160thAnnual Meeting of the
American Psychiatric Association; 2007; San Diego, CA.
67. Richardson G, Wang-Weigand S. Effects of long-term exposure to ramelteon, a melatonin recep-
tor agonist, on endocrine function in adults with chronic insomnia. Hum Psychopharmacol 2008;
24(2):103–111.
68. Kryger M et al. Effect of ramelteon, a selective MT(1)/MT (2)-receptor agonist, on respiration during
sleep in mild to moderate COPD. Sleep Breath 2008; 12(3):243–250.
69. Kryger M et al. The effects of ramelteon on respiration during sleep in subjects with moderate to
severe chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Sleep Breath 2009; 13(1):79–84.
70. Kryger M, Wang-Weigand S, Roth T. Safety of ramelteon in individuals with mild to moderate obstruc-
tive sleep apnea. Sleep Breath 2007; 11(3):159–164.
71. Zemlan FP et al. The efficacy and safety of the melatonin agonist beta-methyl-6-chloromelatonin in
primary insomnia: A randomized, placebo-controlled, crossover clinical trial. J Clin Psychiatry 2005;
66(3):384–390.
72. Yous S et al. Novel naphthalenic ligands with high affinity for the melatonin receptor. J Med Chem
1992; 35(8):1484–1486.
73. Millan MJ et al. The novel melatonin agonist agomelatine (S20098) is an antagonist at
5-hydroxytryptamine2 C receptors, blockade of which enhances the activity of frontocortical dopamin-
ergic and adrenergic pathways. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 2003; 306(3):954–964.
74. Ying SW et al. Melatonin analogues as agonists and antagonists in the circadian system and other
brain areas. Eur J Pharmacol 1996; 296(1):33–42.
75. Stein DJ, Ahokas AA, de Bodinat C. Efficacy of agomelatine in generalized anxiety disorder: A ran-
domized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. J Clin Psychopharmacol 2008; 28(5):561–566.
76. Bourin M, Mocaer E, Porsolt R. Antidepressant-like activity of S 20098 (agomelatine) in the forced
swimming test in rodents: Involvement of melatonin and serotonin receptors. J Psychiatry Neurosci
2004; 29(2):126–133.
77. Montgomery SA, Kasper S. Severe depression and antidepressants: Focus on a pooled analysis of
placebo-controlled studies on agomelatine. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 2007; 22(5):283–291.
78. Loo H, Hale A, D’Haenen H. Determination of the dose of agomelatine, a melatoninergic agonist and
selective 5-HT(2C) antagonist, in the treatment of major depressive disorder: A placebo-controlled
dose range study. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 2002; 17(5):239–247.
79. Dolder CR, Nelson M, Snider M. Agomelatine treatment of major depressive disorder. Ann Pharma-
cother 2008; 42(12):1822–1831.
80. Rajaratnam SM et al. Melatonin agonist tasimelteon (VEC-162) for transient insomnia after sleep-time
shift: Two randomised controlled multicentre trials. Lancet 2008; 373(9662):482–491.
c36 IHBK059-Sateia March 25, 2010 12:27 Char Count=
Kelleen N. Flaherty
Department of Biomedical Writing, University of the Sciences in Philadelphia, Philadelphia,
Pennsylvania, U.S.A.
INTRODUCTION
A large percentage of individuals with insomnia self-medicate with alcohol, over-the-counter
(OTC) antihistamines, or complementary and alternative preparations (1,2). In the United
States, the formulation and manufacture of OTC products are regulated by the U.S. Food
and Drug Administration (FDA). Other products not requiring a prescription, such as dietary
supplements, are unregulated. The National Center for Complementary Health and Alternative
Medicine (NCCAM) at the National Institutes of Health (NIH) defines “complementary and
alternative medicine (CAM)” as “practices that are unproven by science and not presently con-
sidered an integral part of conventional medicine” (3). CAM encompasses a variety of dietary
supplements, herbal compounds, or formulations containing a mixture of these (4,5). Individ-
uals seeking relief from insomnia may also combine two or more of any agents for insomnia
(including prescription medications, OTC agents, alcohol, and CAM preparations) (6). Alcohol
is the substance most commonly used to self-medicate for insomnia. Some 1.6 million non-
institutionalized adults with insomnia are estimated to use CAM to treat their insomnia or
difficulties in sleeping (7); 20% of the population using herbals alone (8). The popularity of
CAM in general is increasing; a 2007 national survey of adults and children found that 38.3%
of U.S. adults using any CAM increased from 36% in 2002. Of those individuals using CAM in
2007, 1.4% of adults and 1.8% of children were doing so for insomnia. Nonvitamin, nonmineral
products accounted for most (17%) CAM use (9).
Despite the perceived efficacy of these agents by the individuals who use them, there
are several concerns associated with their use. There have been very few controlled studies
conducted on most of these substances, and the available evidence has demonstrated little or no
clinical efficacy. Safety data are similarly scant or lacking (10,11). Since CAM preparations are
not regulated by the FDA, the consistent potency and product purity are not assured (10,12–14).
Compounding this, many herbal products are mixtures of more than one type of herb. The FDA
may intervene to remove a product from the market or issue advisories or warnings only when
significant safety concerns are evident, as was the case with tryptophan in 1990 (10,13,15,16)
and kava kava in 2002 (17). Although generally regarded as benign, the use of CAM substances
may be associated with clinically important and sometimes lethal adverse effects, such as the
liver toxicity reported for kava kava, as well as the myriad problems associated with exces-
sive alcohol use (10,12,18). CAM tends to be used more by individuals experiencing insomnia
comorbid with other chronic conditions, particularly psychiatric illness (e.g., anxiety and mood
disorders), congestive heart failure, hypertension, and obesity (7,19). Apart from the fact that
comorbid conditions themselves may lower the risk threshold associated with use of certain
OTC and CAM products (such as CNS and GI effects with antihistamine use (20) or hepatotox-
icity with kava kava) (8,10,18), the potential for pharmacologic interactions is also significant
(10,13). St. John’s wort, for example, is an inducer of cytochrome P450 isoenzymes (8,10) and
has the potential to interfere with protease inhibitors, chemotherapeutic agents, and oral con-
traceptives (21). Kava kava, valerian, and St. John’s wort may interact with anesthetics (22),
c36 IHBK059-Sateia March 25, 2010 12:27 Char Count=
and tryptophan may have toxic effects when used concomitantly with certain psychiatric med-
ications (11). Further complicating these issues, most of the OTC and CAM products are per-
ceived by patients to be safe, as they often are touted to be “natural” (8), or are not considered
dangerous since they do not require a prescription. Frequently patients do not report their use
of these agents to their physicians when listing their “real” medications and physicians are not
necessarily aware that they should inquire about them (12,19).
ALCOHOL
Large-scale studies have estimated that 13% to 28% of individuals with insomnia use alcohol
specifically to induce sleep (6,23). One Japanese study reported the use of alcohol as a sleep-
promoting agent in 48% of male survey respondents (24). A National Sleep Foundation (NSF)
survey conducted in 1991 found that some 30% of their respondents with chronic insomnia used
alcohol as a sleep aid, and that 67% of them found it to be effective in reducing sleep latency
(25). Alcohol is a CNS depressant; its hypnotic properties may be a consequence of multiple
pharmacologic effects, including its interaction with gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) and
glutamate (26,27). Its effects on sleep have been investigated for decades, often in controlled
trials, but frequently with conflicting results. Some studies have shown that alcohol does, in fact,
decrease sleep latency (28–31), while others have shown that it does not (23,32,33). The effects of
alcohol on sleep typically are dose dependent (10,23) and may also depend upon the chronicity
of the insomnia as compared with individuals without insomnia (23). While total sleep time
(TST) is not usually affected by alcohol intake, a dose-dependent redistribution of REM sleep
and NREM sleep stages is common, although this has not been demonstrated consistently. A
common finding is REM suppression during the first half of the night, followed by REM rebound
(or “mini withdrawal”) during the latter half of the night as the alcohol is metabolized and the
serum level decreases (23,26,30,31). An increase in NREM stages 3 and 4 sleep during the first
half of the night may be associated with alcohol ingestion (31), as may an increase in NREM
stage 1 and waking in the latter half of the night (30). Low doses of alcohol have been shown
to increase TST and therefore, may result in mildly beneficial effects (23,32). Development of
tolerance to alcohol develops quickly, often within three nights in healthy volunteers (31), and
is associated with a concomitant return to normal sleep architecture (26). Use of alcohol as
a sleep aid has also been shown to impair secretion of growth hormone (GH), independent
of slow wave sleep (SWS) alterations (26,34); exacerbate snoring and sleep apnea (23,35); and
worsen restless legs syndrome (RLS) (2). Nightly use of alcohol can cause or worsen insomnia
or exacerbate other psychiatric conditions such as depression or anxiety (36). Chronic use of
alcohol for insomnia may lead to the development of alcohol dependence (36,37). Finally, it has
been argued that an improved mood associated with the use of alcohol may be contributing to
beneficial effects on sleep (23). Given the potentially significant problems associated with the
regular use of alcohol as a sleep-inducing agent, its use should be discouraged.
ANTIHISTAMINES
First-generation antihistamines, such as diphenhydramine, doxylamine, chlorpheniramine (as
well as the prescription drugs hydroxyzine and doxepin), are pharmacodynamic antagonists
at central histamine H1 receptors. The first-generation antihistamines are liposoluble and read-
ily cross the blood-brain barrier, and may be associated with significant anticholinergic and
sedative effects, hence their popularity as OTC sleep aids (38–40). Diphenhydramine is by far
the most common antihistamine encountered in OTC sleep aids, either by itself (e.g., Nytol R
,
R
Sominex ) or in combination with pain relievers in “nighttime” or “PM” formulations (e.g.,
Tylenol PM R
). Diphenhydramine has a tmax of 1.7 (±1) hours, a t1/2 of 9.2 (±2.5) hours, and a
duration of action of approximately 12 hours (39). Subjective improvement of sleep onset and
sleep quality are common in subjects taking diphenhydramine (typically at a dose of 25–50 mg)
(2,10,41–45), but few randomized, placebo-controlled studies assessing the safety and efficacy
of diphenhydramine in the treatment of insomnia have been conducted. Efficacy in at least one
sleep parameter has been demonstrated in populations of adults (41), geriatric subjects (includ-
ing those in long-term care) (44,45), pediatric subjects (42), and psychiatric patients (43). A
1983 double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover study evaluated the use of diphenhydramine
50 mg in adult subjects complaining of difficulty with sleep onset. On the basis of subjective
c36 IHBK059-Sateia March 25, 2010 12:27 Char Count=
assessments (daily sleep logs), diphenhydramine was significantly more effective than placebo
at decreasing sleep latency, decreasing frequency of awakenings, decreasing wake time after
sleep onset (WASO), increasing sleep duration, and increasing quality of sleep. Subjective physi-
cian assessment of efficacy was similar (41).
While diphenhydramine has been shown to be associated with sedation and improvement
in sleep parameters such as sleep onset, duration, and quality, it also is associated with signif-
icant safety concerns regarding its relatively long half-life and its anticholinergic properties
resulting from postsynaptic muscarinic receptor antagonism. Complaints of residual sedation,
drowsiness, or grogginess during the morning or daytime following bedtime use are common
(11,38,41). The anticholinergic activity has the potential to cause other side effects, such as
diminished cognitive function and delirium, dry mouth, blurred vision, urinary retention, con-
stipation, and risk of increased intraocular pressure in individuals with narrow-angle glaucoma
(11). Undesired sedation, delirium, and anticholinergic effects are of particular concern in geri-
atric patients (11,46). Additionally, there are potential pharmacokinetic and pharmacodynamic
interactions with concomitant medications, CAM substances, and alcohol. Caution is advised
when patients are taking other medications with anticholinergic effects. Diphenhydramine also
exacerbates the adverse effects of ethanol upon oculomotor coordination, cognitive function,
and driving ability (40). An additional concern with the use of diphenhydramine as a sleep
aid is the possible development of tolerance to its sedating effects. In one study in healthy
volunteers, diphenhydramine was administered 50 mg twice daily and sleepiness was assessed
both objectively and subjectively throughout the day. Development of tolerance to the sedating
effects of diphenhydramine was observed after administration of the medication for four con-
secutive days (47). The development of tolerance results in reduced efficacy and may contribute
to the development of dependence or abuse. Tolerance to the antihistamine sedating effects
may lead to dose escalation and other possible reinforcing effects, such as euphoria. Antihis-
tamine abuse has been documented; case reports exist for doses of diphenhydramine as high
as 3000 mg per day (48). The position of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) on
diphenhydramine is as follows: “Sufficient evidence does not exist to support over-the-counter
(OTC) sleep aids . . . OTC sleep aids that contain antihistamine may provide modest, short-term
benefits for adults with mild cases of insomnia. It is important to be aware, however, that the
use of antihistamines may produce a variety of side effects” (49).
MELATONIN
Melatonin is a pineal hormone involved in the regulation of circadian rhythms. Although it
has sleep-promoting effects on healthy volunteers and has been assessed in a small num-
ber of controlled clinical trials, its efficacy in the treatment of insomnia has not been well
established (50,51), and some controlled studies have shown it to be indistinguishable from
placebo (52). Melatonin generally is considered to be safe, but its safety in long-term use has not
been demonstrated (11,50). Melatonin is a very popular dietary supplement. The 2002 Alter-
native Health/Complementary and Alternative Medicine Supplement to the National Health
Interview Survey (NHIS) interviewed an age and socioeconomically representative population
of 31,044 individuals about use of 25 pharmacologic and nonpharmacologic CAM therapies,
including melatonin. The use of melatonin was reported by 5.2% of the respondents and of
those individuals, 27.5% identified insomnia as at least one reason for its use. Most melatonin
use occurred without physician consultation (52). A retrospective meta-analytic study was con-
ducted in 2005 by Buscemi and colleagues evaluating the safety and efficacy of melatonin in the
treatment of primary insomnia. Initially, 1884 melatonin studies were identified; however, only
14 met the randomized-controlled trial criteria for assessment of efficacy and only 10 met the
criteria for safety assessment (50). Mean improvement of sleep latency was 11.7 minutes. Two
studies in the insomnia meta-analysis also investigated the efficacy of melatonin in shortening
sleep onset in subjects with delayed sleep phase syndrome (DSPS). Subjects with DSPS taking
melatonin experienced an average improvement in sleep latency of 38.8 minutes, as compared
with 7.2 minutes in subjects with insomnia. The meta-analysis also assessed secondary out-
come measures such as sleep efficiency, sleep quality, wakefulness after sleep onset (WASO),
TST, and percentage of time spent in REM sleep. None of these secondary measures reached
statistical significance. Safety assessment (headache, nausea, dizziness, drowsiness) showed no
c36 IHBK059-Sateia March 25, 2010 12:27 Char Count=
significant difference in adverse events between melatonin and placebo. The study group for
the meta-analysis concluded that there was little evidence supporting the use of melatonin
as an exogenous sleep aid, although it might be useful for treating DSPS. The researchers
also concluded that melatonin appears to be safe, at least for short-term use (up to three
months) (50).
A second meta-analysis by Brzezinski and colleagues evaluated the effects of exogenous
melatonin on sleep in 17 randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trials using objective
measures of sleep evaluation and including at least six adult subjects with no severe disabling
systemic disease (53). Crossover and parallel group designs were included, but case studies
were not. This meta-analysis was a review of the trials investigating the effects of melatonin
on sleep and not on insomnia per se. However, nine of the trials in this meta-analysis involved
participants with insomnia (five of the insomnia studies were also included in the Buscemi meta-
analysis). In a subanalysis of healthy subjects with no relevant medical condition other than
insomnia, subjects administered exogenous melatonin had shorter sleep onset by an average of
3.9 minutes, higher sleep efficiency by an average of 3.1%, and a longer TST by an average of 13.7
minutes. The authors concluded that there was statistically significant evidence demonstrating
that the use of exogenous melatonin improves sleep latency, sleep efficiency, and TST (53).
However, it remains unclear whether these modest changes represent clinically significant
improvements. The meta-analysis authors also concluded that melatonin generally is safe, at
least for short-term use in adults.
The role of melatonin in sleep-wake regulation and the use of exogenous melatonin and
melatonin agonists as sleep aids is discussed further in chapter 35.
L-TRYPTOPHAN
L-tryptophan is an essential amino acid and dietary precursor of serotonin. The role it plays in
the biochemistry of sleep and its impact on sleep architecture have not been fully elucidated,
although it has been regarded as a “natural” sleep aid. Its purported positive effects on sleep
onset and maintenance are based on very few and mostly uncontrolled clinical studies that often
included noninsomniac subjects (10,19,54–57). L-tryptophan may be most beneficial in subjects
with mild insomnia or in normal subjects with situational insomnia accompanied by longer-
than-average sleep onset (58,59). Tryptophan generally is considered to be safe and reportedly
is not associated with visuomotor, cognitive, or memory impairment. However, L-tryptophan
as a supplement was removed from the market in 1989 because of the development of an often
serious and sometimes fatal (37 attributed deaths) eosinophilia myalgia syndrome (EMS) in
some individuals who had taken one of several tryptophan-containing products. The cause
of the eosinophilia myalgia syndrome was ultimately traced to a contaminant from a single
manufacturer, and L-tryptophan is once again available (2,15,16). Nevertheless, the NIH chronic
insomnia State-of-the-Science summary report cautions against the use L-tryptophan due to
possible adverse effects, particularly if used in conjunction with psychiatric medications (11).
When concern about L-tryptophan developed and its availability became limited, 5-hydroxy-
L-tryptophan (5-HTP, the immediate precursor to serotonin, or 5-HT) grew in popularity as a
“natural hypnotic.” After the L-tryptophan recall, scrutiny of 5-HTP for serious adverse events
was high, but no definitive toxicity with use of the agent has been confirmed (15). 5-HTP is
commercially produced by extraction from the seeds of Griffonia simplicifolia, a woody African
shrub. Effects of 5-HTP on insomnia are inconclusive (10,19).
St. John’s Wort (Amber Touch and Heal, Goat Weed, Hardhay, Hypericum, SJW,
Klamath Weed, Millepertuis, Rosin Rose, Tipton Weed)
Hypericum perforatum is a perennial herb indigenous to Europe, but found across the United
States. It has been used for medicinal purposes for millennia. The above-ground portion of the
plant is dried and a red liquid is extracted to create the dietary supplements. There are several
pharmacologically active compounds in St. John’s wort, but those most likely to be responsible
for sleep-promoting effects are hypericin and hyperforin. Hypericum may exert its effects through
impact on catecholamine neurotransmission, via inhibition of neurotransmitter metabolism,
c36 IHBK059-Sateia March 25, 2010 12:27 Char Count=
modulation of neurotransmitter receptor density and sensitivity, and synaptic reuptake inhi-
bition (14). Although the exact mechanism of action of Hypericum remains unknown, hyper-
forin has been demonstrated to inhibit the reuptake of serotonin, norepinephrine, dopamine,
GABA, and L-glutamate (10). Almost all of the safety and efficacy studies of St. John’s wort
have been for its utility in managing depression; none have been conducted for insomnia.
However, the effects of St. John’s wort on sleep architecture have been assessed in healthy
volunteers with varying results, including inconsistent reports of increased latency to REM
sleep and increased percentage of slow wave sleep (10,14). Adverse events associated with
the use of St. John’s wort include fatigue, gastrointestinal upset, dizziness, anxiety, headache,
photosensitivity, and phototoxicity. The significant concern associated with the use of St. John’s
wort, however, is its potential interactions with other drugs. Of all the popular herbal sup-
plements, St. John’s wort is the most problematic as far as pharmacokinetic interactions are
concerned. St. John’s wort is an inducer of several CYP450 isoenzymes, including CYP3A4.
Studies have shown that St. John’s wort decreases cyclosporine levels; may be associated
with breakthrough bleeding in women on oral contraceptives; lowers concentration levels of
statins, midazolam, nifedepine, protease inhibitors, theophylline, and tricyclic antidepressants;
decreases the effects of warfarin; can have significant interactions with SSRI antidepressants,
anesthetics, digoxin, loperamide, chemotherapeutic agents, and thyroid medications; and may
contribute to the development of the serotonin syndrome (14,21). Individuals taking any med-
ications that are metabolized by the CYP450 system should be cautioned against using St.
John’s wort.
SUMMARY
A variety of nonprescription pharmacologic agents are widely used as sleep aids (Table 1).
Although the most frequently used substance to induce sleep, alcohol has not been determined
to be safe or efficacious as a sleep aid or in the treatment of insomnia. OTC antihistamine sleep
aids may be popular because they are readily accessible, do not require a prescription, and are
perceived to be safe. Herbal and dietary supplements, such as melatonin and valerian, may be
appealing to consumers because they are viewed as “natural” products. Accordingly, it are not
surprising that the use of OTC and CAM compounds is widespread. However, data are scant or
lacking on the safety and efficacy of most of these agents, some can have serious side effects or
cause significant drug interactions, and consistent purity and concentration of the unregulated
marketed agents are not assured. Increased awareness of the potential safety issues associated
with OTC and CAM agents is required by both consumers and health care professionals alike.
REFERENCES
1. Roehrs T, Hollebeek E, Drake C, et al. Substance use for insomnia in Metropolitan Detroit. J Psychosom
Res 2002; 53 (1):571–576.
2. Curry DT, Eisenstein RD, Walsh JK. Pharmacologic management of insomnia: Past, present, and future.
Psychiatr Clin North Am 2006; 29 (4):871–893.
3. Expanding Horizons of Health Care: NCCAM Strategic Plan, 2005–2009. National Center for Com-
plementary Health and Alternative Medicine, National Institutes of Health; 2004.
4. Cvetanovic I, Golbin D. Polysomnographic evaluations of the effect of Nytex (natural supplements)
in insomniac patients. Sleep 2007; 30(abstract Suppl):A230–A231.
5. Morin CM, Koetter U, Bastien C, et al. Valerian-hops combination and diphenhydramine for treating
insomnia: A randomized placebo-controlled clinical trial. Sleep 2005; 28(11):1465–1471.
6. Johnson EO, Roehrs T, Roth T, et al. Epidemiology of alcohol and medication as aids to sleep in early
adulthood. Sleep 1998; 21(2):178–186.
7. Pearson NJ, Johnson LL, Nahin RL. Insomnia, trouble sleeping, and complementary and alterna-
tive medicine: Analysis of the 2002 National Health Interview Survey data. Arch Intern Med 2006;
166(16):1775–1782.
8. Bent S. Herbal medicine in the United States: Review of efficacy, safety, and regulation: Grand rounds
at University of California, San Francisco Medical Center. J Gen Intern Med 2008; 23(6):854–859.
9. Barnes PM, Bloom B, Nahin RL. Complementary and Alternative Medicine use Among Adults and
Children: United States, 2007. National Health Statistics Reports, No. 12. Hyattsville, MD: National
Center for Health Statistics, 2008.
10. Meoli AL, Rosen C, Kristo D, et al. Oral nonprescription treatment for insomnia: An evaluation of
products with limited evidence. J Clin Sleep Med 2005; 1(2):173–187.
11. National Institutes of Health. National Institutes of Health State of the Science Conference Statement
on Manifestations and Management of Chronic Insomnia in Adults, June 13–15, 2005. Sleep 2005;
28(9):1049–1057.
12. Huntley A. Over-the-counter herbals and drug interactions: Getting the (right) message across. Expert
Opin Drug Saf 2006; 5(1):5–6.
13. Cobert BL. Herbal remedies and drug interactions: An issue overlooked by health authorities. Expert
Opin Drug Saf 2006; 5(1):7.
14. Greeson JM, Sanford B, Monti DA. St. John’s wort (Hypericum perforatum): A review of the cur-
rent pharmacological, toxicological, and clinical literature. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2001; 153(4):
402–414.
15. Das YT, Bagchi M, Bagchi D, et al. Safety of 5-hydroxy-L-tryptophan. Toxicol Lett 2004; 150(1): 111–122.
16. Criswell LA, Sack KE. Tryptophan-induced eosinophilia-myalgia syndrome. West J Med 1990;
153(3):269–274.
17. U.S. Food and Drug Administration Center for Food Safety and Applied Nutrition. Kava-containing
Dietary Supplements May Be Associated with Severe Liver Injury. http://www.fda.gov/Food/
ResourcesForYou/Consumers/ucm085482.htm. Published March 25, 2002. Updated May 4, 2009.
Accessed June 1, 2009.
18. Clouatre DL. Kava kava: Examining new reports of toxicity. Toxicol Lett 2004; 150(1):85–96.
19. Fugh-Berman A, Cott JM. Dietary supplements and natural products as psychotherapeutic agents.
Psychosom Med 1999; 61(5):712–728.
20. Cirillo V, Tempero K. Pharmacology and therapeutic use of antihistamines. Am J Hosp Pharm 1976;
33(1200):1207.
21. Hammerness P, Basch E, Ulbricht C, et al. St John’s wort: A systematic review of adverse effects and
drug interactions for the consultation psychiatrist. Psychosomatics 2003; 44(4):271–282.
c36 IHBK059-Sateia March 25, 2010 12:27 Char Count=
22. Ang-Lee MK, Moss J, Yuan CS. Herbal medicines and perioperative care. JAMA 2001; 286(2):208–216.
23. Roehrs T, Papineau K, Rosenthal L, et al. Ethanol as a hypnotic in insomniacs: Self administration and
effects on sleep and mood. Neuropsychopharmacology 1999; 20(3):279–286.
24. Kaneita Y, Uchiyama M, Takemura S, et al. Use of alcohol and hypnotic medication as aids to sleep
among the Japanese general population. Sleep Med 2007; 8(7–8):723–732.
25. Ancoli-Israel S, Roth T. Characteristics of insomnia in the United States: Results of the 1991 National
Sleep Foundation Survey. I. Sleep 1999; 22(suppl 2):S347–S353.
26. Roehrs T, Roth T. Sleep, sleepiness, and alcohol use. Alcohol Res Health 2001; 25(2):101–109.
27. Koob GF. The neuropharmacology of ethanol’s behavioral action: New data, new paradigms, new
hope. In: Dietrick RA, Erwin VG, eds. Pharmacological Effects of Ethanol on the Nervous System.
New York: CRC Press, 1996:1–12.
28. Rundell OH, Lester BK, Griffiths WJ, et al. Alcohol and sleep in young adults. Psychopharmacologia
1972; 26(3):201–218.
29. MacLean AW, Cairns J. Dose-response effects of ethanol on the sleep of young men. J Stud Alcohol
1982; 43(5):434–444.
30. Williams DL, MacLean AW, Cairns J. Dose-response effects of ethanol on the sleep of young women.
J Stud Alcohol 1983; 44(3):515–523.
31. Williams H, Salamy A. Alcohol and sleep. In: Kissin B, Begleiter H, eds. The Biology of Alcoholism.
New York: Plenum Press, 1972:435–483.
32. Stone BM. Sleep and low doses of alcohol. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1980; 48(6):706–709.
33. Dijk DJ, Brunner DP, Aeschbach D, et al. The effects of ethanol on human sleep EEG power spectra
differ from those of benzodiazepine receptor agonists. Neuropsychopharmacology 1992; 7(3):225–232.
34. Ekman AC, Vakkuri O, Ekman M, et al. Ethanol decreases nocturnal plasma levels of thyrotropin and
growth hormone but not those of thyroid hormones or prolactin in man. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1996;
81(7):2627–2632.
35. Dawson A, Bigby BG, Poceta JS, et al. Effect of bedtime alcohol on inspiratory resistance and respiratory
drive in snoring and nonsnoring men. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1997; 21(2):183–190.
36. Weissman MM, Greenwald S, Nino-Murcia G, et al. The morbidity of insomnia uncomplicated by
psychiatric disorders. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 1997; 19(4):245–250.
37. Janson C, Lindberg E, Gislason T, et al. Insomnia in men—A 10-year prospective population based
study. Sleep 2001; 24(4):425–430.
38. Roth T, Roehrs T, Koshorek G, et al. Sedative effects of antihistamines. J Allergy Clin Immunol 1987;
80(1):94–98.
39. del Cuvillo A, Mullol J, Bartra J, et al. Comparative pharmacology of the H1 antihistamines. J Investig
Allergol Clin Immunol 2006; 16(suppl 1):3–12.
40. Montoro J, Sastre J, Bartra J, et al. Effect of H1 antihistamines upon the central nervous system. J
Investig Allergol Clin Immunol 2006; 16(suppl 1):24–28.
41. Rickels K, Morris RJ, Newman H, et al. Diphenhydramine in insomniac family practice patients: A
double-blind study. J Clin Pharmacol 1983; 23(5–6):234–242.
42. Russo RM, Gururaj VJ, Allen JE. The effectiveness of diphenhydramine HCl in pediatric sleep disor-
ders. J Clin Pharmacol 1976; 16(5–6):284–288.
43. Kudo Y, Kurihara M. Clinical evaluation of diphenhydramine hydrochloride for the treatment of
insomnia in psychiatric patients: A double-blind study. J Clin Pharmacol 1990; 30(11):1041–1048.
44. Meuleman JR, Nelson RC, Clark RL Jr. Evaluation of temazepam and diphenhydramine as hypnotics
in a nursing-home population. Drug Intell Clin Pharm 1987; 21(9):716–720.
45. Glass JR, Sproule BA, Herrmann N, et al. Effects of 2-week treatment with temazepam and diphenhy-
dramine in elderly insomniacs: A randomized, placebo-controlled trial. J Clin Psychopharmacol 2008;
28(2):182–188.
46. Conn DK, Madan R. Use of sleep-promoting medications in nursing home residents: Risks versus
benefits. Drugs Aging 2006; 23(4):271–287.
47. Richardson GS, Roehrs TA, Rosenthal L, et al. Tolerance to daytime sedative effects of H1 antihis-
tamines. J Clin Psychopharmacol 2002; 22(5):511–515.
48. Thomas A, Nallur DG, Jones N, et al. Diphenhydramine abuse and detoxification: A brief review and
case report. J Psychopharmacol 2009; 23(1):101–105.
49. American Academy of Sleep Medicine. AASM Position Statement: Treating Insomnia with Over-the-
Counter Sleep Aids. Westchester, IL: American Academy of Sleep Medicine, 2006.
50. Buscemi N, Vandermeer B, Hooton N, et al. The efficacy and safety of exogenous melatonin for primary
sleep disorders. A meta-analysis. J Gen Intern Med 2005; 20(12):1151–1158.
51. Reiter RJ. Melatonin: Clinical relevance. Best Pract Res Clin Endocrinol Metab 2003; 17(2):273–285.
52. Bliwise DL, Ansari FP. Insomnia associated with valerian and melatonin usage in the 2002 National
Health Interview Survey. Sleep 2007; 30(7):881–884.
c36 IHBK059-Sateia March 25, 2010 12:27 Char Count=
53. Brzezinski A, Vangel MG, Wurtman RJ, et al. Effects of exogenous melatonin on sleep: A meta-analysis.
Sleep Med Rev 2005; 9(1):41–50.
54. Lindsley JG, Hartmann EL, Mitchell W. Selectivity in response to L-tryptophan among insomniac
subjects: A preliminary report. Sleep 1983; 6(3):247–256.
55. Boman B. L-tryptophan: A rational anti-depressant and a natural hypnotic? Aust N Z J Psychiatry
1988; 22(1):83–97.
56. Hartmann E. L-tryptophan: A possible natural hypnotic substance [editorial]. JAMA 1974; 230(12):
1680–1681.
57. Hartmann E. L-tryptophan: A rational hypnotic with clinical potential. Am J Psychiatry 1977; 134(4):
366–370.
58. Hartmann E. Effects of L-tryptophan on sleepiness and on sleep. J Psychiatr Res 1982; 17(2):107–113.
59. Schneider-Helmert D, Spinweber CL. Evaluation of L-tryptophan for treatment of insomnia: A review.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1986; 89(1):1–7.
60. Dietz BM, Mahady GB, Pauli GF, et al. Valerian extract and valerenic acid are partial agonists of the
5-HT5a receptor in vitro. Brain Res Mol Brain Res 2005; 138(2):191–197.
61. Miller LG. Herbal medicinals: Selected clinical considerations focusing on known or potential drug-
herb interactions. Arch Intern Med 1998; 158(20):2200–2211.
62. Winslow LC, Kroll DJ. Herbs as medicines. Arch Intern Med 1998; 158(20):2192–2199.
63. Wheatley D. Medicinal plants for insomnia: a review of their pharmacology, efficacy and tolerability.
J Psychopharmacol 2005; 19(4):414–421.
64. Jacobs BP, Bent S, Tice JA, et al. An internet-based randomized, placebo-controlled trial of kava and
valerian for anxiety and insomnia. Medicine (Baltimore) 2005; 84(4):197–207.
65. Bressler R. Herb-drug interactions: Interactions between kava and prescription medications. Geriatrics
2005; 60(9):24–25.
66. American Academy of Sleep Medicine. AASM Position Statement: Treating Insomnia with Herbal
Supplements. Westchester, IL: American Academy of Sleep Medicine, 2006.
67. Lichstein KL, Payne KL, Soeffing JP, et al. Vitamins and sleep: An exploratory study. Sleep Med 2008;
9(1):27–32.
68. Robinson CR, Pegram GV, Hyde PR, et al. The effects of nicotinamide upon sleep in humans. Biol
Psychiatry 1977; 12(1):139–143.
69. Durlach J, Pages N, Bac P, Bara M, Gviet-Bara A. Biorhythms and possible central regulation of mag-
nesium status, phototherapy, darkness therapy, and chronopathologic forms of magnesium depletion.
Magnes Res 2002; 15(1–2):49–66.
c37 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:46 Char Count=
Medications used for the treatment of insomnia during the past century have largely employed
two basic mechanisms for sleep promotion. Prescription sleeping pills such as barbiturates, ben-
zodiazepines (BZDs), and other BZD receptor agonistic modulators (BzRAs) act at the GABAA
receptor complex to enhance GABA inhibitory activity and promote sleep. Over-the-counter
sleep medications have been and continue to be formulations of nonselective histamine antago-
nists such as diphenhydramine or doxylamine. Advances in the understanding of neurochem-
ical control of sleep–wake, coupled with identification of previously unknown transmitters,
such as hypocretin/orexin, have created new horizons in the pharmacotherapy of insomnia.
While some pipeline development continues to target activation of sleep-promoting GABAer-
gic mechanisms, many of the current pharmaceutical efforts are directed at the identification
of safe and efficacious medications that antagonize wake-active transmitter systems such as
orexin, 5-HT2A , and histamine. Other agents are targeted at the melatonin system, potentially
expanding the choice of such agents beyond ramelteon. Certain pipeline drugs offer multiple
potential mechanisms of sleep-promoting action.
The following chapter attempts to capture the cross section of compounds in development
as of 2009. However, a review of this literature underscores the unpredictability of pharmacolog-
ical research and development. Failures to replicate initially promising efficacy data, emergence
of unforeseen adverse reactions, shifts in the competitive environment, and numerous other
factors create an ever-changing field. In recent years, numerous promising drugs have been
derailed, sometimes in late stages of development. Therefore, in considering compounds dis-
cussed in this chapter, one should keep in mind that, in all likelihood, many, if not most, of
the drugs discussed will never achieve FDA approval. Despite that, the research engendered in
discovery and development of these agents often serves to advance our knowledge of not only
pharmacotherapy but also sleep–wake biology itself.
428 SATEIA
EVT 201 (Evotec AG), a partial positive allosteric GABA receptor modulator in pre-clinical
studies, has undergone initial assessments for the treatment of primary insomnia. Sleep was
evaluated in 75 subjects who received placebo, EVT 201 (1.5 or 2.5 mg), in a cross-over design
(6). Two nights of polysomnography (PSG) and subjective sleep ratings indicated significantly
shorter latency to persistent sleep, greater total sleep time (TST), fewer awakenings, and reduced
wake after sleep onset. Sleep maintenance improvements were observed into the third and
fourth quarters of the night. Quality ratings were improved and no major adverse events were
reported, although mild effects on morning digit symbol substitution test were noted. A phase II,
randomized, placebo-controlled study (7) of EVT 201 in 149 elderly primary insomnia subjects
demonstrated increased TST, reduced latency to persistent sleep (LPS) and wakefulness after
sleep onset (WASO), and objective improvements in daytime alertness by multiple sleep latency
test (MSLT).
Adipiplon (NG2-73; Neurogen) has also been demonstrated to have partial GABA agonist
activity selective for the BZD ␣3 -subunit in pre-clinical studies. The unique selectivity profile
would theoretically promote both hypnotic and antianxiety effect. Phase I/II trials have been
conducted. These demonstrated significant reductions in sleep latency, reduction in WASO, and
improvements in TST and sleep efficiency (8,9). However, in a more recent phase II/III trial,
with an Ambien CR comparison, an unexpectedly high rate of adverse effects was reported (10).
As a result, Neurogen announced in 2008 that they had no further plans to advance adipiplon
at that time.
Further modifications to existing BzRA agents are also in various stages of development.
A longer-acting preparation (SKP-1041) of zaleplon, a short-acting agent with an elimination
half-life of only about one hour, is currently being evaluated. Somnus Therapeutics, Inc., has
allied with SkyePharma (SKP) to develop a longer-acting preparation using SKP’s GeoLock
controlled release technology. To date, only phase I assessment data of pharmacodynamics,
using varied formulations, have been published (11,12). Results show differing time courses
of sleep promotion using the MSLT as a primary evaluation tool, suggesting potential efficacy
for sleep-maintenance problems. Further clinical studies are currently underway. Intec Pharma
has also reported on development of a gastro-retentive formulation of zaleplon (zaleplon GR),
which provides a sustained duodenal infusion of the drug through the night.
An oral spray form of zolpidem (ZolpiMist R
; NovaDel Pharma, Inc.) was approved in
late 2008 for short-term treatment of sleep-onset insomnia. At dosages of 5 and 10 mg, the
spray formulation produced therapeutic levels more rapidly than standard zolpidem tablets
(13). Commercialization efforts for the product are in progress.
A sublingual preparation of zolpidem (Edluar R
, formerly Sublinox
R
) was approved for
short-term treatment of sleep initiation difficulty in March, 2009, and is now marketed in the U.S.
Recent studies (14) have demonstrated that sleep latency with this formulation is significantly
more rapid compared to standard, orally administered zolpidem. Another zolpidem sublingual
preparation (Intermezzo R
; Transcept Pharmaceuticals, Inc.), in low-dose formulation (1.75 mg
and 3.5 mg) is being evaluated for treatment of middle-of-the-night (MOTN) awakening (15).
Eighty-two patients with primary sleep maintenance problems were administered sublingual
zolpidem (1.75 mg or 3.5 mg) or placebo following awakening. Latency to persistent sleep
after middle-of-the-night awakenings were significantly lower and TST increased compared
to placebo. Subjective ratings of sleep quality were improved at the higher dose. There was
no evidence of morning impairment by Digit Symbol Substitution or subjective assessment of
sleepiness. As of this writing, the FDA has requested additional safety data for MOTN dosing.
Other, novel agents targeted at the GABA receptor have also been explored. Gaboxadol,
a GABA analogue and select agonist, unlike BzRAs enhances slow wave activity and has been
shown to reduce sleep latency and improve efficiency in healthy subjects and insomniacs (16–
21). However, efforts to obtain FDA approval for gaboxadol for the treatment of insomnia have
been abandoned at the present time.
OREXIN ANTAGONISTS
The orexin (hypocretin) system was independently described by two groups in the late 1990s
(22,23). This system consists of a relatively small number of neurons located in the lat-
eral hypothalamus, with extensive projections to brain regions involved in the regulation of
c37 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:46 Char Count=
sleep–wake, among others. Subsequent determinations that disruptions of this system play
a key role in the pathogenesis of narcolepsy with cataplexy fueled further interest in the
role of orexin neurons in sleep–wake regulation (24,25). It has since been demonstrated that
orexin neurons serve a controller function for other wake–active systems of the brain, including
other hypothalamic regions [histaminergic neurons of the tubero-mamillary nucleus (TMN)],
cholinergic basal forebrain and lateral dorsal / pedunculopontine nuclei, noradrenergic locus
coeruleus, and serotonergic dorsal raphe neurons (26). Orexin receptors are widely distributed
in the brain. Activation of orexin neurons and their downstream targets promotes wakefulness.
Orexins are neuropeptides derived from a larger neuropeptide, prepro-orexin. Orexin A
(hypocretin-1) and orexin B (hypocretin-2) act on two receptors, OX1 and OX2 (27). OX1 receptors
are most heavily distributed in locus coeruleus while OX2 receptors are expressed particularly
in histaminergic hypothalamic regions (TMN). Current evidence suggests that these receptors
play a critical role in induction/maintenance of wakefulness. Thus, it is not surprising that
there is significant ongoing investigation of orexin antagonists as potential sleep-promoting
medications.
Although there are a number of orexin antagonists in various stages of evaluation, the most
extensively studied to date is almorexant (Actelion-078573), a dual, selective and competitive
antagonist at both OX1 and OX2 receptors. Almorexant has an elimination half-life that varies
from 6 to 19 hours. Its effects on sleep have been evaluated in rats, dogs and humans. Brisbare-
Roch et al. (28). reported that almorexant, administered during the active period in rats, increased
indices of both NREM and REM sleep. Increases in NREM efficiency were comparable to those
observed with zolpidem and significantly greater than placebo. The increased proclivity to
sleep was accompanied by reduced locomotor activity as well. Similar effects were observed
with repeated dosing.
In humans, doses from 1 to 1000 mg were administered during morning hours. Brief (25
minutes) quantitative electroencephalographic (EEG) recordings were conducted 90 minutes
following administration and showed dose-dependent reduction in latency to persistent sleep.
Increases in theta and delta power were observed in the almorexant group but not in the
zolpidem group. Sedating effects resolved in dose-dependent fashion with effects no longer
evident for all but the maximal (1000 mg) dosage by 6.5 hours.
In these studies, the agent was apparently well tolerated; the most common adverse events
included somnolence, fatigue, reduced attention, dizziness, and diplopia. Of note, no evidence
of cataplectic-type events was observed in any species.
Initial proof-of-concept trials (29) in humans involved a randomized, placebo-controlled
crossover study of 161 primary insomnia subjects who underwent two nights polysomnography.
Dose-dependent improvement of sleep efficiency (by PSG) was observed at 50, 100, 200, and
400 mg. A dose-dependent trend to decrease in latency to persistent sleep and WASO was also
reported. A reduction in REM latency was noted but there was overall maintenance or increase
of REM%. No evidence of waking REM-intrusion symptoms was seen. Subjective ratings of
sleep were improved and the drug was apparently well tolerated with an absence of significant
daytime impairments.
Currently, almorexant is in phase III trials (RESTORA), which began in 2008 with expected
enrollment of 670 primary insomnia patients (30). Primary endpoints of the randomized,
placebo-controlled study will include measures of sleep onset and maintenance. A zolpidem
reference arm is also included.
GSK 649868 (GlaxoSmithKline Pharmaceuticals Ltd.) is a second orexin antagonist cur-
rently in phase II trials. Preliminary data (31) in healthy subjects and 52 patients with primary
insomnia show dose-dependent improvement versus placebo of sleep latency, wake after sleep
time, and TST at doses of 30 and 60 mg. REM sleep was also increased at the higher dose.
Other orexin antagonists are also being investigated but are in earlier phases of devel-
opment. The OX2 antagonist, JNJ-10397049, is highly selective, with little evidence of binding
at OX1 receptors (32). Comparison studies between almorexant and JNJ-10397049 have been
performed in rats. This preliminary work demonstrates that both agents reduce latency to per-
sistent NREM sleep, although the selective OX2 demonstrated this effect at dosages 10 times
lower than the dual antagonist (almorexant). MK-4305 (Merck & Co., Inc.) is also currently in
phase II development (33).
c37 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:46 Char Count=
430 SATEIA
5-HT2A ANTAGONISTS
Serotonergic neurons of the dorsal raphe play an important role in regulation of sleep and
wakefulness. An understanding of the function of serotonin (5-HT) in control of sleep–wake
has continued to evolve and is complicated by the heterogeneity of numerous receptor subtypes.
Serotonin activity of the dorsal raphe is at its highest firing rate during wake. Firing rate of these
neurons decreases with onset of NREM sleep and becomes negligible during REM sleep. A great
deal of research in this area has focused on the 5-HT2 family of receptors. It has been shown
that activation of 5-HT2A receptors is associated with tonic inhibition of NREM sleep (34).
Correspondingly, antagonism at the 5-HT2A receptor results in promotion of increased slow
wave activity in sleep (35).
These observations have led to significant interest in 5-HT2 antagonists as potential ther-
apies in the management of insomnia. Ritanserin, a potent 5-HT2 antagonist, has been studied
with respect to its effects on sleep in animals and humans. Early work demonstrated that admin-
istration of ritanserin to young, poor sleepers resulted in a doubling of slow wave activity, with
a corresponding decrease in N2 (stage 2 sleep) (36). Subsequent work has revealed compara-
ble increases in slow wave sleep in healthy volunteers (37), middle-aged poor sleepers (38),
and dysthymic patients (39). Some of these data have also suggested a reduction in frequency
of awakening (38) and improvement in subjective sleep quality (36). Ritanserin has also been
evaluated as a treatment for patients with schizophrenia (40). However, its patent has expired
without the drug ever becoming FDA-approved. In recent years, atypical antipsychotic medi-
cations with potent 5-HT2A and H1 antagonistic properties such as quetiapine and olanzapine
have been utilized off-label as sleep aids, particularly in psychiatric populations.
Eplivanserin (Ciltyri; Sanofi-aventis) is a 5-HT2A antagonist currently in phase III trials
for treatment of insomnia. A randomized, double-blind study (41) of 351 adults with chronic
insomnia treated to 4 weeks with either 1 mg or 5 mg of eplivanserin or placebo in the evening
found that the 5-mg dose resulted in a mean 39-minute reduction in the baseline 84-minute
wake time after sleep onset. This was significantly greater than the mean 26-minute reduction
with placebo. Eplivanserin 5 mg/day resulted in a 64% reduction in the number of nocturnal
awakenings, compared with a 36% decrease with placebo. More eplivanserin-treated patients
reported a significant improvement in the refreshing quality of sleep.
In two phase III, 12-week randomized controlled trial (RCTs) of eplivanserin (42,43) with
open-label extension, sleep maintenance insomnia subjects were treated with eplivanserin 5 mg
or placebo. Significantly greater reductions in WASO (least square means (LSM) difference =
11.5 min and 13.5 min, respectively) and number of awakenings was observed at 12 weeks with
eplivanserin with associated reported improvement in subjective sleep quality and morning
concentration. Other studies have demonstrated very significant increase in N3 sleep (44) and
delta frequency power on quantitative electroencephalographic analysis (45). In September,
2009, the FDA requested further information from Sanofi-aventis concerning risk–benefit ratio
for eplivanserin.
Sanofi-aventis has a second 5-HT2A antagonist, volinanserin, in development, although
this agent has not progressed as far in the pipeline as eplivanserin.
Pruvanserin (LY2422347; Eli Lilly and Company) was originally developed by Hypnion,
Inc., and obtained by Eli Lilly and Company in 2007. A phase II trial of this drug, which examined
its efficacy in the management of sleep-maintenance insomnia, was reportedly commenced in
2006. Published results for this trial were not identified. Eli Lilly and Company does not currently
(as of July 1, 2009) list pruvanserin among its pipeline agents (46) and appears to be focused
primarily on LY2624803, which has both 5-HT2A antagonist and antihistaminergic properties
(discussed in the following section).
Pimavanserin (ACP-103) is a selective 5-HT2A inverse agonist being developed by Aca-
dia Pharmaceuticals, Inc. Although the primary indication in development is for treatment of
Parkinson disease (PD) psychosis, a secondary evaluation of its effects on sleep maintenance
has also been pursued. In trials with older PD patients, pimavanserin improved psychosis and
also resulted in significant increase in slow wave sleep (47).
Another 5-HT2A inverse agonist, ADP-125 (Arena) showed promise in initial smaller trials
for treatment of insomnia. However, in late 2008 it was announced that the compound had failed
to meet primary or secondary endpoints in a larger phase II study. Indications at that time were
that the company no longer planned to pursue development.
c37 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:46 Char Count=
In summary, these agents have theoretical promise as a treatment for sleep maintenance
insomnia and largely unexplored potential for improving sleep quality in other conditions
by virtue of their enhancement of slow wave sleep activity. However, development has been
fraught with setbacks and it appears that several of the drugs are no longer in the active
pipeline. Eplivanserin is closest to market and its future currently rests on its developer’s ability
to respond adequately to FDA concerns.
MELATONIN AGONISTS
Melatonin is an endogenous peptide that is synthesized and released by the pineal gland. It
represents the key “darkness” signal in mammalian CNS. Exogenously administered melatonin,
available as an over-the-counter nutritional supplement, has been used by millions in the treat-
ment of insomnia, particularly over the past two decades. Distinct from its usage as an effective
chronobiological agent, its efficacy for chronic insomnia is not especially well established. Meta-
analyses (48,49) show mixed results with pooled data suggesting very modest improvement in
sleep parameters of uncertain clinical significance. Against this background, the only currently
approved melatonin agonist, ramelteon, was launched in 2005. This agent produces modest
reductions of sleep latency in patients with chronic insomnia, but its very short half-life limits
its application primarily to sleep-onset difficulties.
Other melatonin agonists are presently in testing. Tasimelteon (VEC-162; Vanda Pharma-
ceuticals, Inc.), a potent MT1/MT2 receptor agonist has been studied in phase II and III trials.
The phase II trial (50) involved 39 healthy subjects who were studied at baseline for 3 nights,
for 3 treatment nights with tasimelteon (at dosages of 20 mg, 50 mg, or 100 mg) or placebo,
with a 5-hour phase advance of schedule, and for one night following treatment. Reductions of
PSG sleep latency and improved sleep efficiency were observed, along with a dose-dependent
advance in melatonin rhythm. In the phase III trial (50) of 411 healthy subjects using the same
5-hour phase advance model of transient insomnia, latency to persistent sleep was significantly
reduced and sleep efficiency increased. Wake after sleep onset was reduced at the two lower
(20 and 50 mg) dosages. The subjects appeared to tolerate the drug well and there were no
indications of adverse events in excess of those seen with placebo.
A subsequent phase III trial (51) of 322 subjects with chronic insomnia characterized by
sleep initiation difficulty showed significant reductions of latency to persistent sleep, which
persisted throughout the five-week study period. No significant improvements were observed
in standard measures for wake after sleep onset. Subjects did not show evidence of daytime
impairment following use of tasimelteon. Submission of a marketing application for tasimelteon
is expected by mid-2011.
Preliminary phase II results of another melatonin agonist, PD-6735, were announced in
2004 (52). Significant reductions in sleep latency were seen in 40 insomnia subjects. However,
further development data has not been released on this compound.
432 SATEIA
evaluated in phase III trials involving adult (54) and elderly adult (55) primary insomnia sub-
jects. Each subject underwent two nights of PSG during each of five trials separated by 5 to 12
days in a multiple crossover design. An initial single-blind placebo assessment was followed by
randomized assignment to one of four sequences with every subject receiving all three doses of
doxepin plus placebo. The primary endpoint of wake during sleep showed significant reduc-
tions at the 3- and 6-mg dosage. TST, sleep efficiency, and WASO were improved at all three
doses. The 6-mg dose produced reduction of subjective time to fall asleep. The efficacy assess-
ment in elderly insomniacs was conducted with identical design and demonstrated comparable
results. All dosages were generally well tolerated without indication of significant anticholin-
ergic or other side effects. There did not appear to be significant daytime residual effects. In late
2009 Somaxon received an FDA response to its second submission for approval which indicated
continued efficacy concerns and denied approval of the drug in its current form.
Several current pipeline agents demonstrate dual or multiple mechanisms of action, which
may prove effective in management of insomnia. One of the most widely studied agents,
agomelatine (Servier; as Valdoxan in the European Union), is in development primarily as an
antidepressant drug and has already received approval in the European Union. It is in phase III
trials in the United States, licensed to Novartis International AG, which expects submission of
FDA application no sooner than 2012. The agent has a combined action of 5-HT2C antagonism,
coupled with potent melatonin agonistic properties. In addition to its proven antidepressant
properties, it demonstrates significant sedative properties.
Lemoine et al. (56) reported significantly greater subjective improvement in “getting to
sleep” and sleep quality in 332 patients with major depressive disorder treated with agomelatine
25 to 50 mg qd compared to venlafaxine. Other studies have demonstrated increased TST and
sleep efficiency and reduced WASO without significant change in REM% or latency in patients
with major depressive disorder (MDD) (57). Support for the contention that agomelatine sta-
bilized NREM sleep is derived from studies indicating that NREM cyclic alternating pattern
(CAP) is reduced by this agent (58). Additional reports (59) have found significant improvement
in subjective sleep latency in generalized anxiety disorder patients treated with agomelatine.
In addition to its direct effects on sleep continuity and architecture, agomelatine has also been
shown to have significant phase-shifting effects that may play a role in its therapeutic effects.
Older adults treated with early evening agomelatine experienced a phase-advance of approxi-
mately two hours (60). The drug has generally been well tolerated in trials with favorable side
effect profiles, including minimal sexual side effects.
LY2624803 (formerly HY10275; Eli Lilly and Company) is a dual acting H1 /5-HT2A antag-
onist originally developed by Hypnion, Inc., and sold to Eli Lilly and Company in 2007. Phase II
data released in 2007 (61) on HY10275/ LY2624803 describes 52 patients with primary insomnia
who were administered HY10275, 1 to 3 mg, or placebo in a randomized clinical trial. Signifi-
cant reductions were observed in wake after sleep onset, which decreased in a dose-dependent
manner by 62 minutes for 3 mg (p < 0.001) and 35 minutes for 1 mg (p < 0.002). Subjects with
moderate to severe transient insomnia responded as well as or better than subjects with mild to
moderate transient insomnia. The drug also met secondary efficacy endpoints including signifi-
cant reduction in latency to persistent sleep. No adverse events were reported during the course
of the trial and no evidence of next-day impairment or residual fatigue was noted. A phase II
RCT of LY2624803(SLUMBER) (62), with a zolpidem comparison arm, is currently underway.
Subjects include both primary and secondary insomniacs. Primary endpoint is TST.
Esmirtazapine (ORG 50081), a derivative of the widely used antidepressant, mirtazapine,
is currently under development for the management of insomnia and hot flashes. Esmirtazapine
is a potent 5-HT2A antagonist with H1 and ␣-adrenergic properties.
Initial proof-of-concept trials in primary insomnia at three dosages of the drug showed
significant improvement in TST and WASO (63). A long-term safety study of two doses (1.5
and 3.0 mg) in elderly primary insomniacs is currently underway. Merck acquired Organon as
part of its recent buyout of Schering-Plough and currently lists esmirtazapine in its Phase III
pipeline.
ITI-007 (Intra-Cellular Therapies, Inc.) is a dual action 5-HT2A /dopamine receptor phos-
phoprotein modulator (DPPM) agent developed for the treatment of schizophrenia. Phase
2 studies have also been conducted for sleep maintenance insomnia. ITI-007 has variable
c37 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:46 Char Count=
CONCLUSIONS
Current investigations of novel compounds for the treatment of insomnia hold the promise of
introducing an array of new approaches to the management of this often chronic and highly
prevalent condition. Many of these agents would appear to have the potential to improve
sleep as defined by the conventional metrics of latency, WASO, TST, efficiency, and the like,
and by virtue of their unique mechanisms, offer additional advantages not seen with BzRAs,
such as enhancement of slow wave activity. Moreover, a number of these agents may offer dual
action in the management of insomnia comorbid with depression, attention deficit hyperactivity
disorder, hot flashes, or psychosis. However, as stated previously, the road to new drug appli-
cation is fraught with many obstacles and only time will tell which of these compounds reach
market.
REFERENCES
1. Roth T, Zammit GK, Scharf MB, et al. Efficacy and safety of as-needed, post bedtime dosing with
indiplon in insomnia patients with chronic difficulty maintaining sleep. Sleep 2007; 30(12):1731–1738.
2. Walsh JK, Moscovitch A, Burke J, et al. Efficacy and tolerability of indiplon in older adults with primary
insomnia. Sleep Med 2007; 8(7–8):753–759.
3. Rosenberg R, Roth T, Scharf MB, et al. Efficacy and tolerability of indiplon in transient insomnia. J Clin
Sleep Med 2007; 3(4):374–379.
4. Scharf MB, Black J, Hull S, et al. Long-term nightly treatment with indiplon in adults with pri-
mary insomnia: Results of a double-blind, placebo-controlled, 3-month study. Sleep 2007; 30(6):743–
752.
5. Lydiard RB, Lankford DA, Seiden DJ, et al. Efficacy and tolerability of modified-release indiplon in
elderly patients with chronic insomnia: Results of a 2-week double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. J
Clin Sleep Med 2006; 2(3):309–315.
6. Walsh JK, Thacker S, Knowles LJ, et al. The partial positive allosteric GABA(A) receptor modulator
EVT 201 is efficacious and safe in the treatment of adult primary insomnia patients. Sleep Med 2009;
10(8):859–864.
7. Walsh J, Knowles LJ, Tasker T, et al. The treatment of elderly patients with primary insomnia and
daytime sleepiness with EVT 201 improves sleep initiation, sleep maintenance, and daytime alertness.
Sleep 2008, 31(abstract suppl):A224.
8. Zammit G, Rice K, Accomando W, et al. Effects of NG2–73 on sleep onset, quality, and next day
function in a transient insomnia study. Sleep 2007; 30(abstract suppl):A239.
9. Roth T, Lankford A, Accomando WP, et al. Sleep onset and maintenance in patients with chronic
insomnia treated with adipiplon in a cross-over study. Sleep 2008; 31(abstract suppl):A40–A41.
10. Neurogen announces suspension of insomnia study with adipiplon. http://www.medicalnewstoday.
com/articles/114919.php. Published July 15, 2008. Accessed February 28, 2010.
11. Walsh JK, Cornette F, Staner C, et al. Pharmacodynamic profile of three novel formulations of zale-
plon in normal volunteers as evaluated by the multiple sleep latency test. Sleep 2009; 32(abstract
suppl):A267.
12. Gassen M, Kress A, Nedelec J, et al. Pharmacokinetic profile of single doses of zaleplon in three novel
formulations in normal volunteers. Sleep 2009; 32(abstract suppl):A267.
13. http://www.novadel. com/pipeline/zolpimist.htm. Accessed October 15, 2009.
14. Staner L, Eriksson M, Cornette F, et al. Sublingual zolpidem is more effective than oral zolpidem in
initiating early onset of sleep in the post-nap model of transient insomnia: A polysomnographic study.
Sleep Med 2009; 10(6):616–620.
15. Roth T, Hull SG, Lankford DA, et al. Low-dose sublingual zolpidem tartrate is associated with dose-
related improvement in sleep onset and duration in insomnia characterized by middle-of-the-night
(MOTN) awakenings. Sleep 2008; 31(9):1277–1284.
16. Lankford DA, Corser BC, Zheng YP, et al. Effect of gaboxadol on sleep in adult and elderly patients
with primary insomnia: Results from two randomized, placebo-controlled, 30-night polysomnography
studies. Sleep 2008; 31(10):1359–1370.
c37 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:46 Char Count=
434 SATEIA
17. Walsh JK, Mayleben D, Guico-Pabia C, et al. Efficacy of the selective extrasynaptic GABA A agonist,
gaboxadol, in a model of transient insomnia: A randomized, controlled clinical trial. Sleep Med 2008;
9(4):393–402.
18. Lundahl J, Staner L, Staner C, et al. Short-term treatment with gaboxadol improves sleep maintenance
and enhances slow wave sleep in adult patients with primary insomnia. Psychopharmacology (Berl)
2007; 195(1):139–146.
19. Walsh JK, Deacon S, Dijk DJ, et al. The selective extrasynaptic GABAA agonist, gaboxadol, improves
traditional hypnotic efficacy measures and enhances slow wave activity in a model of transient
insomnia. Sleep 2007; 30(5):593–602.
20. Deacon S, Staner L, Staner C, et al. Effect of short-term treatment with gaboxadol on sleep maintenance
and initiation in patients with primary insomnia. Sleep 2007; 30(3):281–287.
21. Mathias S, Zihl J, Steiger A, et al. Effect of repeated gaboxadol administration on night sleep
and next-day performance in healthy elderly subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology 2005; 30(4):833–
841.
22. de Lecea L, Kilduff TS, Peyron C, et al. The hypocretins: Hypothalamus-specific peptides with neu-
roexcitatory activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1998; 95(1): 322–327.
23. Sakurai T, Amemiya A, Ishii M, et al. Orexins and orexin receptors: A family of hypothalamic neu-
ropeptides and G protein-coupled receptors that regulate feeding behavior. Cell 1998; 92(4):573–585.
24. Lin L, Faraco J, Li R, et al. The sleep disorder canine narcolepsy is caused by a mutation in the
hypocretin (orexin) receptor 2 gene. Cell 1999; 98(3):365–376.
25. Chemelli RM, Willie JT, Sinton CM, et al. Narcolepsy in orexin knockout mice: Molecular genetics of
sleep regulation. Cell 1999; 98(4):437–451.
26. Saper CB, Chou TC, Scammell TE. The sleep switch: Hypothalamic control of sleep and wakefulness.
Trends Neurosci 2001; 24(12):726–731.
27. Marcus JN, Aschkenasi CJ, Lee CE, et al. Differential expression of orexin receptors 1 and 2 in the rat
brain. J Comp Neurol 2001; 435(1):6–25.
28. Brisbare-Roch C, Dingemanse J, Koberstein R, et al. Promotion of sleep by targeting the orexin system
in rats, dogs and humans. Nat Med 2007; 13(2):150–155.
29. Dingemanse J et al. Proof-of-Concept Study in Primary Insomnia Patients with Almorexant (ACT-
078573): A Dual Orexin Receptor Antagonist. In: Fifth World Congress of the World Federation of
Sleep Research and Sleep Medicine Societies; 2007; Cairns, Australia.
30. Almorexant. http://www1.actelion.com/en/scientists/development-pipeline/phase-3/almorexant
.page. Accessed October 17, 2009.
31. Bettica P, Lichtenfeld U, Squassante L, et al. The orexin antagonist SB-649868 promotes and main-
tains sleep in healthy volunteers and in patients with primary insomnia. Sleep 2009; 32(abstract
suppl):A252–A253.
32. Dugovic C, Shelton JE, Aluisio LE, et al. Blockade of orexin-1 receptors attenuates orexin-2 receptor
antagonism-induced sleep promotion in the rat. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 2009; 330(1):142–151.
33. http://www.merck.com/research/pipeline/home.html?WT.svl=mainnav. Accessed October 20,
2009.
34. Adrien J. The serotoninergic system and sleepwakefulness regulations. In: Kales A, ed. Pharma-
cology of Sleep, Handbook of Experimental Pharmacology. Berlin, Germany: Springer-Verlag, 1995:
91–116.
35. Landolt HP, Meier V, Burgess HJ, et al. Serotonin-2 receptors and human sleep: Effect of a selective
antagonist on EEG power spectra. Neuropsychopharmacology 1999; 21(3):455–466.
36. Viola AU, Brandenberger G, Toussaint M, et al. Ritanserin, a serotonin-2 receptor antagonist, improves
ultradian sleep rhythmicity in young poor sleepers. Clin Neurophysiol 2002; 113(3):429–434.
37. Sharpley AL, Solomon RA, Fernando AI, et al. Dose-related effects of selective 5-HT2 receptor antag-
onists on slow wave sleep in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1990; 101(4):568–569.
38. Adam K, Oswald I. Effects of repeated ritanserin on middle-aged poor sleepers. Psychopharmacology
(Berl) 1989; 99(2):219–221.
39. Paiva T, Arriaga F, Wauquier A, et al. Effects of ritanserin on sleep disturbances of dysthymic patients.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1988; 96(3):395–399.
40. Akhondzadeh S, Malek-Hosseini M, Ghoreishi A, et al. Effect of ritanserin, a 5HT2A/2C antagonist,
on negative symptoms of schizophrenia: A double-blind randomized placebo-controlled study. Prog
Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2008; 32(8):1879–1883.
41. Jancin B. Eplivanserin soothes insomnia without next-morning effects. Clin Psychiat News 2008;
36(11):41.
42. Erman M, Kryger M, Soubrane C, et al. A randomized double-blind placebo-controlled 12-week
trial of eplivanserin in insomniac patients with sleep maintenance difficulties. Sleep 2009; 32(abstract
suppl):A41.
c37 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:46 Char Count=
The development of new therapeutics for the treatment of insomnia involves the design and
execution of clinical trials. Clinical trials are research studies involving human subjects that
serve to characterize drugs under development; examine their safety, tolerability, and adverse
effects in healthy subjects; and document their efficacy and safety in patient populations. Clinical
trials are the most important and costly phase of the drug development process. According to
industry reports, the pharmaceutical industry spent $44.5 billion on research and development
(R&D) in 2007 (1) with clinical development costs representing approximately 60% of these
expenditures (2). The data derived from the conduct of clinical trials are critical to the delivery
of new therapeutics to the market. These data ultimately drive the drug development lifecycle
and provide the basis of all new drug applications (NDAs) in the United States and other
regulatory environments.
CLINICAL TRIALS AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF NEW THERAPEUTICS FOR INSOMNIA 437
While the Food, Drug, and Cosmetic Act compelled manufacturers to document the safety
of their drug products, it is remarkable that there still was no requirement that they document
efficacy. Claims could be made regarding a drug’s ability to treat or cure any kind of disease,
even when there was no evidentiary basis for such claims. To close this loophole, policy mak-
ers eventually introduced new legislation that expanded governmental control and regulatory
oversight of drug products, including requirements that certain products could be used only
under the supervision of a medical professional. In particular, the 1951 Durham–Humphrey
Amendment to the Food, Drug, and Cosmetic Act made certain medications available by pre-
scription only (6). This act limited access to drug products more than ever before, but the control
of drug products remained insufficient. Gaps in regulatory oversight continued to persist due
to controversy over how much power the FDA should wield over private industry and health
care practices. Therefore, the next truly significant milestone did not occur until 1958, after yet
another catastrophe brought inadequate regulatory policies into the spotlight once again. At
that time, the W.S. Merrill Company began marketing thalidomide in Germany as a treatment for
morning sickness. Shortly thereafter, an application was filed for the approval of this drug in
the United States. Some concerns regarding the safety of this drug led an astute medical officer
at the FDA, Frances Kelsey, to delay thalidomide’s marketing approval. Kelsey’s actions are
likely to have spared thousands of unborn children, as it was later learned that thalidomide
had serious teratogenic adverse effects. The West German pediatrician Widukind Linz reported
an association between thalidomide and the birth of nearly 4000 children exhibiting abnormal
limb growth. Relatively few cases of these birth defects were reported in the United States
because of Kelsey’s actions, and the drug was completely removed from the market in 1961 (7).
Interestingly, thalidomide later returned to the market under the brand name Thalomid, and
currently is indicated for the treatment of multiple myeloma and cutaneous manifestations of
moderate to severe erythema nodosum leprosum (ENL) provided that safety precautions are
taken (8).
It was in a climate of concern for consumer safety that Senator Estes Kefauver held
congressional hearings to address pharmaceutical industry practices regarding the development
of new drug products. These hearings led to the 1962 Kefauver–Harris Amendment to the Food,
Drug, and Cosmetic Act. This landmark legislation required drug manufacturers to demonstrate
“substantial evidence” of a drug’s efficacy for its marketed indication, in addition to requiring
demonstration of its safety. Further, the Kefauver–Harris Amendment expanded the FDA’s
authority to regulate advertising, inspect drug manufacturing facilities, and withdraw untested
drugs from the market (9,10).
Since the 1960s, many new forms of legislation and regulatory oversight have been intro-
duced to govern the drug development and approval process. One guidance document partic-
ularly important to the field of sleep medicine was produced by the FDA in 1977, specifically
relating to the development of hypnotics (11). This document provided guidelines regarding
acceptable approaches to the study of investigational hypnotics in human subjects. It also
offered specific recommendations regarding the types of clinical trials that should be con-
ducted as part of a hypnotic development program, as well as the endpoints that should be
considered. The durability of this document is remarkable, given that its content regarding
hypnotic development remains applicable to this day. In addition to guidance on the conduct
of clinical trials, the FDA recently has issued requirements for the labeling of hypnotics. In
December 2006, the FDA sent letters to manufacturers of products approved for the treat-
ment of sleep disorders indicating that they would be required to revise product labeling to
include warnings about certain potential adverse events (12). Although the likelihood of the
occurrence of these events was not stated in the FDA’s notice, these AEs were considered
significant enough to warrant a revision of the product labeling. The adverse events (AEs) of
concern were
438 ZAMMIT
In conjunction with the labeling revisions required, the FDA required that each product
manufacturer send letters to health care providers highlighting these changes. Manufacturers
of sedative–hypnotic products also were requested to develop Patient Medication Guides for
sedative–hypnotic products to inform consumers of the potential risks associated with these
newly labeled adverse events.
As the FDA pursues its public health mission, it is clear that the regulatory oversight of
drug development continues to be based on requirements imposed by legislation. For example,
in 1981, the FDA and the Department of Health and Human Services (DHHS) revised regulations
for human subject protection based on the recommendations of the 1979 Belmont Report (13).
The most important revision undertaken at this time was the specification of the elements of
informed consent (14). Later, in 2002, the Best Pharmaceuticals for Children Act improved the
safety and efficacy of patented and off-patent medicines for children (14). These and other
current requirements for the development and testing of new drug products are now specified
in detail under several parts of the Code of Federal Regulations (CFR) title 21 (15). These
documents have relevance for the development of sleep therapeutics, and when combined with
specific guidance related to the development of hypnotics, provide science and industry with
sound foundations upon which to base the development of hypnotics.
PRECLINICAL STUDIES
An important step in the development of any new therapeutic is the submission of an investi-
gational new drug (IND) application to the FDA. This application contains information gained
from preclinical studies that are required prior to the initiation of testing in human subjects. Pre-
clinical studies fall into two categories—in vitro and in vivo. In vitro studies evaluate the effects
of a drug on human or animal tissue. In vivo studies are performed in live animals, usually rats,
dogs, and primates. Combined, these preclinical studies yield the pharmacology and toxicology
data that form the basic safety profile of a drug (16). These data are extremely important in that
they are used to select a safe starting dose for human studies, establish estimates of the toxic dose
in humans, and gain a fundamental understanding of the pharmacokinetic (PK) and pharmaco-
dynamic (PD) properties of a drug. The type of information that regulatory reviewers examine
in investigational new drugs includes data from carcinogenicity studies, genotoxicity studies,
toxicokinetics and pharmacokinetics, toxicity testing, reproductive toxicology, and pharmacol-
ogy studies (16,17). It is interesting to note that preclinical testing represents approximately 27%
of pharmaceutical R&D costs (2), and fewer than 10% of all new molecular entities (NMEs) will
find their way forward from preclinical evaluation to the submission of a NDA. This is because
drug manufacturers must study large numbers of drugs and apply rigid screening criteria to
determine which ones they will take further into development.
Specific to the development of hypnotics, the types of preclinical studies used to examine
products under consideration include tests of locomotion or myorelaxation, as these variables
provide information regarding a compound’s sedative properties. Examples include the rotarod
test (18,19), the loaded grid test (18,20), and tests of abuse liability (21). In the rotarod test,
rats are placed on a rotating surface (a rod) and faced with the challenge of remaining on
the surface as it rotates. Drug-free baseline performance is assessed to determine minimum
performance thresholds. Animals are then given active drug or placebo and tested to determine
the amount of time they are able to remain on the rotarod. Animals that remain on the rotarod
for shorter periods following drug administration may be showing evidence of myorelaxation.
In the loaded grid test, animals hold on to a weighted grid with their front paws and are
tested to determine the load required before they release. Again, this test is conducted so as
to yield results for the baseline, placebo and drug conditions. Preclinical tests also include
electroencephalographic (EEG) measures of sleep that provide indications of hypnotic activity
(19,22–29). Hypnotics such as zolpidem, zaleplon, and zopiclone all have been characterized by
these types of preclinical tests.
CLINICAL TRIALS AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF NEW THERAPEUTICS FOR INSOMNIA 439
1 FDA -
NDA Submitted
>5,000 250 5 Approved
IND Submitted
Prediscovery
Drug
Number of Volunteers
20–100 100–500 1000–5000
1.5–2
3–6 Years 6–7 Years Yr
information that constitutes the manufacturer’s evidence for carrying a drug forward, and
forms the basis of the NDA that is submitted to the FDA. Although these phases are repre-
sented in consecutive order, these phases may overlap or even run concurrently. For exam-
ple, a phase II study of hypnotic efficacy may be in progress when an important phase I
drug–drug interaction study is initiated.
Clinical drug development is costly. An analysis conducted in 2007 estimated that the
total combined cost of the preclinical and clinical trials required to bring a drug to the point of
submission of an NDA is more than $1.2 billion (30). This expense includes the costs of drugs
that fail to progress through the clinical development pipeline (31). For every 250 drugs that
complete preclinical testing, 5 will successfully complete phase I, and only one will ultimately
achieve regulatory approval (1). The four phases of clinical development are described in more
detail in the following section.
Phase I Trials
In these studies, researchers test an investigational drug or device in a small group of people
(20–100), usually normal health, volunteers (32). These studies are designed to determine the
metabolic and pharmacologic actions of a drug in humans, the side effects associated with
the drug at various doses, and if possible, to gain early evidence regarding drug effects using
biomarkers or tests in small cohorts of clinical populations. Common studies conducted in this
phase of drug development are first-in-human (FIH), single ascending dose (SAD), multiple
ascending dose (MAD), and drug–drug interaction (DDI) studies. During phase I, sufficient
information about the drug’s pharmacokinetics and PD effects should be obtained to permit
the design of well-controlled, scientifically valid, phase II studies. Phase I studies also evaluate
drug metabolism, structure–activity relationships, and the mechanism of action in humans (33).
There is a growing trend to include cohorts of patients in phase I studies, as researchers attempt
to learn more about drug effects early in development.
Phase II Trials
During phase II, an investigational drug or device is given to a larger group of people (100–500)
(32) to evaluate the efficacy and safety of a drug for a particular indication or indications in
patients with the disease or condition under study (34). Phase II studies also aim to determine the
common short-term side effects and risks associated with the drug or device under evaluation.
In clinical trials of hypnotics, it is common for phase II studies to employ experimental models in
healthy human subjects in order to assess efficacy, identify primary outcomes that will be used
in pivotal phase III studies, and determine the types of AEs that people report. However, in this
phase of drug development it is much more common to study small groups of patients, especially
when no experimental models exist. Phase II offers drug manufacturers an opportunity to
c38 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:47 Char Count=
440 ZAMMIT
determine the doses that will be carried forward into phase III by weighing the trade-offs
between efficacy and the risk of adverse events at each dose level evaluated.
Phase IV Trials
After a drug’s NDA has been approved by the FDA, phase IV trials may be conducted to further
clarify its efficacy and safety. Pharmacovigilence, pharmacoeconomic, and comparative efficacy
studies are most likely to be performed during this phase of the drug development lifecycle.
New and specific patient populations are likely to be studied. Elderly, pediatric, or other patient
groups could become a focus of research, depending on the potential use in those patient groups.
As an example, studies of therapeutics for insomnia have been conducted in special populations
of patients with comorbid depression and anxiety disorders, providing invaluable information
to the prescribing community. There are occasions when phase IV trials provide information
that is used to revise the labeling of a drug product (e.g., the inclusion of additional adverse
event information), and some phase IV trials may support a manufacturer’s decision to pursue
a secondary NDA (sNDA) that seeks approval for a new therapeutic indication for a drug.
CLINICAL TRIALS AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF NEW THERAPEUTICS FOR INSOMNIA 441
zolpidem that provides the immediate release of drug from an outer layer and a delayed
release of drug from an inner core) are different from one another (40,41). These products, in
turn, can be expected to be different from sublingual or other formulations of zolpidem that do
not require absorption in the intestinal tract (42,43).
PK studies often are combined with PD assessments, resulting in PK/PD modeling that
guides the early-phase development of a drug. In the study of sedative hypnotics, the most
elegant of these is the PK/PD model developed by Greenblatt and associates. In this model, the
drug being studied typically is administered to healthy adult subjects in the morning, following
a full night of sleep in a controlled setting. Blood samples for PK assessments are taken predose
and at several time points following dosing (e.g., before medication and at 0.5, 1, 1.5, 2, 2.5, 3, 4,
5, 6, 8, and 24 hours postdosing) (44). These PK samples may be coupled with other evaluations
of sedation [e.g., electroencephalographic assessments of beta () activity and the Digit Symbol
Substitution Test (DSST)] and various word recall instruments. The use of word recall tests
is important because of the known effects of many hypnotics on memory. The timing and
frequency of PK and PD sampling is determined by the experimental hypothesis appropriate
to the drug or formulation. For example, when testing a hypnotic with an extended-release
formulation, it may be important to assess the duration of sedative effects. The information
gleaned from this evaluation may guide dose selection or formulation, or provide information
regarding the residual effects investigators may observe in later-phase studies (45).
Drug–drug and food interaction studies are additional important studies conducted in
phase I. Phase I drug–drug interaction trials may include evaluations of a hypnotic’s interaction
with cytochrome P450 (CYP) inducers (e.g., rifampin), CYP inhibitors (e.g., azoles, ritonavir, and
erythromycin), histamine H2 receptor antagonists (e.g., cimetidine and ranitidine), antidepres-
sants, antipsychotics, antagonists of benzodiazepines, and other drugs known to cause sedation
(46).Food interactions also may be studied to examine the effect of certain foods or fasting on
drug absorption or metabolism (47–49). For example, grapefruit juice is a well-known inhibitor
of CYP3A4 and may be expected to increase exposure to hypnotics that are metabolized through
this pathway (50).
442 ZAMMIT
Phase-advance models have been used with success in assessing the efficacy of hypnotics
in treating transient insomnia. One of the advantages of these models is that they can be used in
either crossover or parallel group designs, whereas first-night models, by definition, are limited
to parallel group designs only. One of the earliest studies of the phase advance model used
a 180-degree shift of the sleep–wake cycle in 12 healthy subjects to determine the effects of
triazolam, flurazepam, and placebo in a parallel-group design. Subjects who received placebo
demonstrated significant sleep loss following the manipulation of their sleep, while the effect
was attenuated for those in the active medication groups (58). One of the interesting aspects of
the phase-advance model is that it may be used to assess sleep maintenance by demonstrating
separation between active drug and placebo in the later portions of the sleep period (59). While
the magnitude of the phase-shift has been thought to be of importance, several studies have
used short phase advances in order to detect meaningful effects. For example, one study has
shown that temazepam 7.5 mg and 15 mg administered to subjects in a 2-hour phase-advance
paradigm had similar effects on sleep architecture 60. Statistically significant effects on LPS,
TST, and other sleep variables relative to placebo have been found with other drugs, such as
indiplon and gaboxadol using this model (55,61,62), occasionally coupled with the first-night
model of transient insomnia (55).
There are a variety of noise models that have been used in phase II studies of insom-
nia. While the specific implementations may differ, these models offer a method of assessing
drug effects in the presence of experimental stimuli that perturb sleep, thereby demonstrating
sleep maintenance effects. One noise model involves the exposure of normal, healthy sub-
jects to continuous white noise during the sleep period at levels sufficient to disrupt sleep
[e.g., 45–75 decibels (db)] (63,64). This design may be able to detect differences between drug
conditions when cyclic alternating patterns (CAP) are used (65). Another common noise model
involves the use of traffic noise played during sleep, and has been shown to separate immediate-
release from modified-release formulations of hypnotics (66). Problematically however, the use
of the traffic noise model has not shown consistent results when separating active drug from
placebo (67).
Some phase II studies require the use of patient populations rather than healthy volunteers.
These studies might be used when it is desirable to test drug effects on a specific sleep outcome
variable that may not be reliably produced using an experimental model, when certain crossover
comparisons are of interest, or when repeated (e.g., nightly) dosing may be desired. Such
studies are also useful when the identification of well-defined patient groups may provide more
informative data than experimental models (68). Several double-blind, randomized, placebo-
controlled clinical trials of hypnotics have employed small samples of insomnia patients to
assess the efficacy and safety of hypnotics in PSG sleep laboratory studies (68–78).
CLINICAL TRIALS AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF NEW THERAPEUTICS FOR INSOMNIA 443
data are important because they provide impressions regarding efficacy and safety from the
patient’s perspective and offer information regarding the patient’s experience at home in their
natural sleep setting as opposed to the artificial sleep environment of a sleep laboratory where
most PSG recordings are performed.
One of the more interesting recent developments in hypnotic trials has been the use of
novel study designs that have employed the “as needed,” non-nightly, or non-bedtime use of
hypnotics. (95–99). One study assessed the effects of non-nightly use of zolpidem 10 mg or
placebo in 199 patients with primary insomnia. Patients were randomized in a parallel group
design, and were instructed to take no fewer than three pills and no more than five pills per
week for a period of 12 weeks. The data revealed that patients receiving zolpidem exhibited
(vs. baseline) a 42% decrease in sleep latency, a 52% reduction in number of awakenings, a 55%
decrease in wake time after sleep onset, and a 27% increase in total sleep time, without increases
in the amount of medication used during the study interval (100).
Some studies have involved non-bedtime dosing of hypnotics. This type of evaluation
is especially relevant for medications that may not have immediate sedative effects (enabling
administration earlier in the day), or for medications that have rapid onset of action, metabolism,
and elimination (enabling administration during nighttime awakenings). Studies employing
experimentally induced awakenings in subjects with primary insomnia have examined the
effects of treatment with short-acting hypnotics like zaleplon (101–104). One crossover design
PSG study examined the administration of zaleplon 10 mg, zolpidem 10 mg, and placebo
in 37 subjects with primary insomnia characterized by sleep maintenance difficulty. During
each treatment period, subjects were seen for an eight-hour PSG recording, during which they
were awakened and later given study drug or placebo. The study revealed that both zaleplon
and zolpidem reduced sleep latency in the middle of the night and increased total sleep time
following dosing, suggesting that after-bedtime treatment may be efficacious in treating sleep
maintenance insomnia (104). This approach is not risk free however. It is notable that zolpidem
was associated with poorer next-day performance and greater sleepiness on the multiple sleep
latency test (MSLT) than placebo; an indicator that PK/PD relationships are important when
selecting hypnotics for after-bedtime dosing. Additional studies have examined the use of
middle-of-the-night dosing with other drugs using PROs, including studies of indiplon and
sublingual zolpidem administered in lower doses than the oral formulation (42,105,106).
Subject Selection
The matter of subject selection is critically important to the success of clinical trials of hypnotics.
When healthy subjects are being considered for inclusion in phase II studies of hypnotics,
it is important that they are of the appropriate age (adult or elderly) and that they have no
confounding concurrent illness, concomitant medication use, abnormal findings on physical
examination, or abnormal clinical laboratory findings. Since recent FDA guidance now requires
manufacturers to assess the cardiac safety of all new drugs that have systemic exposure (107),
electrocardiographic (ECG) assessments are important in selecting subjects appropriate for
study.
Among the unique aspects of subject selection for hypnotic studies are those related to
subjects’ sleep habits. When selecting healthy subjects for clinical trials, one must consider
subjects’ usual bedtimes, rise times, napping, recent travel across multiple time zones, use of
stimulants such as caffeine or nicotine, and the use of alcohol or other substances that might
affect sleep. Some studies that have used experimental models of insomnia have been careful to
exclude otherwise healthy subjects who may be sleep deprived. Assessments used to identify
sleep deprivation include the Epworth Sleepiness Scale (108,109), Stanford Sleepiness Scale, and
the MSLT (110,111).
When identifying patients with primary insomnia to participate in phase II or III trials,
the selection criteria are of utmost importance. First and foremost, it is important that the
characteristics of the patient population be clearly specified. Most trials of hypnotics performed
for registration purposes have studied patients with primary insomnia, most often defined by
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, version IV (DSM-IV) criteria. There are
c38 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:47 Char Count=
444 ZAMMIT
relatively minor, but important, differences between DSM-IV criteria for primary insomnia and
other diagnostic nomenclatures also used to identify insomnia complaints. The International
Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, 10th Revision (ICD-10) specifies
criteria for “nonorganic insomnia” that stipulate that symptoms must be present at least three
times per week for at least one month, while the DSM-IV criteria specify a duration of one
month or longer but do not require a minimum number of times per week. In contrast to both
the DSM-IV and ICD-10, the Research Diagnostic Criteria (RDC) for insomnia disorder do not
specify a duration criterion. Therefore, the fundamental diagnostic definition of insomnia used
in a trial must be thoughtfully considered. The use of centralized interviews may be one way to
minimize variability in diagnostic ratings of insomnia made by clinicians, similar to those used
in clinical trials in other therapeutic indications (112).
Once the insomnia diagnosis is specified, it is important to consider the types of symptoms
that patients must have in order to participate in the trial. For example, if a clinical trial is
intended to assess the effects of a drug on sleep latency, the study protocol should require
subjects to report difficulty falling asleep. Similarly, if the clinical trial is intended to assess the
effects of a drug on sleep maintenance, the study protocol is likely to require subjects to report
wakefulness during the sleep period. The frequency and severity of the sleep problems required
for inclusion depends on the specific objectives of the protocol. Some recent studies assessing
drug effect in patients with sleep maintenance insomnia have required both a sleep latency
and wakefulness after sleep onset (WASO) of ≥20 minutes as measured by polysomnography
during the screening process (68,113).
The clinical trial protocol typically provides an exhaustive list of specific inclusion and
exclusion criteria for study subjects. All of these criteria must be met in order to enroll subjects
into a study. The determination that these criteria are met usually is based on an office visit
(“screening visit”) during which a clinician obtains all pertinent history and a physical exam-
ination, clinical laboratory tests, ECG, and other assessments are performed. The first office
visit is when written informed consent is obtained from the subject, prior to performing any
study-related activity. Failure to meet all inclusion and exclusion criteria may result in a protocol
violation, which may result in the exclusion of a subject from analysis.
It has become a common practice to include PSG screening nights in PSG trials of
hypnotics. These screening nights serve to confirm that subjects meet certain PSG inclu-
sion/exclusion criteria prior to randomization to active drug or placebo, and improve the
likelihood that trial participants mirror the insomnia diagnosis that investigators wish to eval-
uate. Studies designed to assess the effect of a hypnotic on reducing sleep latency commonly
require a minimum LPS criterion on PSG screening nights. For example, a mean LPS of ≥20
minutes on two or three PSG screening nights, with no night less than 15 minutes might be
employed. Studies designed to assess the effect of a hypnotic on sleep maintenance often employ
a minimum criterion for WASO. For example, a mean WASO of ≥60 minutes on two or three
PSG screening nights, with no night less than 45 minutes might be required for trial participa-
tion. Note that these criteria are dependent on the scientific hypothesis being tested and are not
hard-and-fast standards imposed by industry. In addition to such PSG inclusion criteria, there
are PSG exclusion criteria. The most common PSG exclusion criteria involve the exclusion of
subjects who have an apnea/hypopnea index (AHI) >10 or a periodic limb movement with
arousal index (PLMAI) >10. Some upward adjustments to these thresholds may be warranted
when working with elderly samples.
Multicenter Trials
Phase I studies often are conducted at single sites. However, as drugs enter phase II development,
clinical trials usually are assigned to a small number of specialized investigator sites. Hypnotic
studies typically might include six to eight investigator sites, all likely to have experienced,
trained staff and PSG recording capability. The use of multiple sites for phase II studies provides
geographic reach, enhances the accrual of study subjects from diverse populations, and enables
the study to progress more rapidly than those conducted at a single center. Further along this
continuum, phase III studies often involve such large samples that it is necessary to enlist
the assistance of a much larger number of investigator sites. At this point in the development
c38 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:47 Char Count=
CLINICAL TRIALS AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF NEW THERAPEUTICS FOR INSOMNIA 445
Outcome Variables
The goals of traditional hypnotic therapy primarily relate to reducing latency to sleep onset
or reducing wakefulness during the sleep period. The most commonly used primary outcome
variables in traditional PSG hypnotic studies have been latency to persistent sleep (LPS), total
sleep time (TST), sleep efficiency, and number of awakenings. These outcome variables have
been used in trials of benzodiazepines, nonbenzodiazepines (zolpidem, zaleplon, zopiclone),
and novel therapeutics such as ramelteon and doxepin. More recent focus on the prevalence and
impact of sleep-maintenance insomnia has led to the development of hypnotics that address this
type of sleep complaint. In PSG studies of drug effects on sleep maintenance, the most commonly
used outcome variable is WASO. WASO has been used as a primary outcome variable in pivotal
trials of both zolpidem ER and eszopiclone. Self-reported measures of sleep latency, total sleep
time, and sleep maintenance complement objective PSG outcome variables. These self-reported
measures may be used either in the context of PSG trials or, separately, in outpatient studies with
no PSG component. Traditionally, these variables have been collected using sleep diaries. More
recently it has become common to obtain patient-reported data using electronic or integrated
voice response system (IVRS) technology. Table 1 provides a summary of common primary and
secondary outcome variables used in PSG and outpatient studies of hypnotics.
Within recent years there has been increasing recognition that insomnia complaints regard-
ing sleep “quantity” (e.g., LPS, WASO) may include disruptions of sleep not detected by
traditional sleep stage scoring alone. These disruptions may be concurrent with, or independent
of, complaints of poor quality or nonrestorative sleep. Preliminary evidence of these disruptions
has sparked an interest in examining sleep microarchitecture. Such trials might involve careful
assessment of slow wave sleep activity measured during stages 3 and 4 sleep or throughout
the entire sleep period, or may focus on other measurable aspects of sleep through the use
of power spectral analysis (114) or scoring of cyclic alternating patterns (CAPs) (64,115–117).
At present, these newer outcome variables are not commonly used clinical trial endpoints,
but further exploration may lead to their consideration as outcomes for insomnia treatment
studies.
Other potential treatment outcomes for insomnia may include measures of daytime func-
tioning or health. Functional outcomes might assess next-day well-being, sleepiness, fatigue, or
performance on various psychomotor or memory assessments—all of which may be important
c38 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:47 Char Count=
446 ZAMMIT
to understand the clinical utility and proper labeling of hypnotics. For example, recent clinical
studies of hypnotics have begun to demonstrate that improvements following hypnotic treat-
ment may be associated with less napping and significantly higher ratings of daytime alertness,
sense of physical well-being, and quality-of-life (118). Finally, health outcomes might represent
an important new focus of hypnotic treatment. Recent data suggesting that sleep deprivation
is related to impairments in glucose metabolism (119–121), obesity and diabetes risk (122,123),
hypertension (124,125), cardiac arrythmias (126,127), and mortality (128) may increase the fre-
quency with which health outcomes are included in hypnotic trials.
While one primary objective of clinical trials is to assess efficacy, the other is to assess
safety. Safety outcomes in trials of hypnotics frequently include assessments of adverse events
and serious adverse events, and involve specific measures of sedation, cognitive functioning,
psychomotor functioning, and withdrawal. Adverse events are any untoward experience in a
clinical trial participant, whether or not the experience is believed to be related to the investi-
gational drug (129). Serious adverse events (SAEs) are adverse events that result in death or are
life-threatening, result in hospitalization or prolongation of an existing hospitalization, result
in significant disability or incapacity, or result in congenital abnormalities or birth defects (129).
These AEs and SAEs are reported by investigators and are carefully considered by regulatory
authorities when reviewing NDAs. In hypnotic trials, assessments of self-reported sedation
might be captured as AEs, especially if it occurs during the day. Objective measures of sedation
might further include the Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST), Symbol Copying Test (SCT), or
word recall tests. These tests often are performed at baseline, immediately after drug administra-
tion, and in the morning following awakening in order to assess next-day, residual impairment
(8.5 hours or more after dosing). Some studies have used the multiple sleep latency test to assess
residual sleepiness. Safety assessments also might be performed during the typical nighttime
sleep period in order to determine a drug’s effect on cognitive or psychomotor performance
during awakenings. As an example, some recent studies have used computerized dynamic pos-
turography to assess balance and postural control following middle-of-the-night awakenings
of healthy elderly and those with insomnia following bedtime dosing with hypnotic agents.
Specialty Populations
There are a number of health conditions that commonly are comorbid with insomnia. They
include obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) (130), chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (COPD)
(131), pain, menopause (132,133), and psychiatric illness such as depression (134) or anxiety
(135). Therefore, the assessment of hypnotic efficacy and safety in insomnia comorbid with
these other conditions is an important aspect of hypnotic life cycle management. Depression is
among the most commonly associated conditions comorbid with insomnia, giving rise to studies
of the combination use of antidepressants and hypnotics. The use of zolpidem was examined
in SSRI-treated patients with persistent comorbid insomnia (134). Patients who participated in
this study were diagnosed with depression, treated stably with the SSRIs fluoxetine, sertraline,
or paroxetine, and complained of sleep-onset difficulty or inadequate sleep time at least three
nights a week with daytime impairment. Over a four-week period, treatment with zolpidem
10 mg lengthened sleep time, improved sleep quality, reduced the number of awakenings, and
improved multiple measures of daytime functioning as compared to placebo.
A recent study evaluated the coadministration of eszopiclone 3 mg with the SSRI fluoxetine
in major depressive disorder (MDD) patients over an 8-week period (136). Compared to flu-
oxetine alone, the fluoxetine–eszopiclone group demonstrated statistically significant improve-
ments in all sleep parameters evaluated at all time points. Measures included sleep latency,
wake time after sleep onset, total sleep time, sleep quality, and depth of sleep. Importantly,
eszopiclone also resulted in a greater treatment response to fluoxetine as measured by improve-
ments on the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAM-D-17) and two clinical global impression
scales (CGI-I and CGI-S). Furthermore, a significantly greater percentage of individuals in the
cotherapy group were classified as responders (59% vs. 48%) and remitters (42% vs. 33%) at the
end of the study.
There are a limited number of clinical trials of subjects with insomnia and comorbid
anxiety. One double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled, parallel-group study examined
the efficacy of eszopiclone combined with escitalopram in adult patients with insomnia and
c38 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:47 Char Count=
CLINICAL TRIALS AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF NEW THERAPEUTICS FOR INSOMNIA 447
generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) (135). Patients received 10 mg of escitalopram oxolate for
10 weeks and were randomized to also receive either 3 mg of eszopiclone (n = 294) or placebo
(n = 301) nightly for the first 8 weeks. For the last two weeks, eszopiclone was replaced with a
single-blind placebo. Sleep, daytime functioning, psychiatric measures, and adverse events were
assessed throughout the 10-week period. The results showed that, compared with treatment
with placebo and escitalopram, coadministration of eszopiclone and escitalopram resulted in
significantly improved sleep and daytime functioning (P < 0.05), greater improvements in total
Hamilton Anxiety Scale (HAM-A) scores at each week (P < 0.05) and at weeks 4 through 10,
and greater improvements on the Clinical Global Impressions (CGI) of Improvement (P < 0.02).
Overall, the data obtained for the treatment of insomnia and comorbid psychiatrics conditions
suggest that study designs like these will become increasingly valuable during phases IIIb and
IV development, and may be used to document the efficacy and safety of hypnotics in insomnia
comorbid with other conditions. Such documentation adds to the clinical understanding of the
potential uses of hypnotics and also helps to differentiate hypnotics from each other.
In addition to the comorbidities presented here, the clinical trials of insomnia in special
populations may represent an important area of focus for future clinical trials. In the future,
subjects with histories of alcohol or drug problems, or those with Alzheimer’s disease or other
forms of dementia, may become routinely included in clinical trials. In addition, children with
insomnia might be considered a population of interest for clinical trials, given that no hypnotics
have been specifically indicated for the treatment of pediatric insomnia.
CONCLUSION
Clinical trials for hypnotics occur in a well-controlled regulatory environment. These trials
represent the method by which the efficacy and safety of new drug treatments for insomnia are
determined. The development of new hypnotics includes both preclinical (animal) and clinical
(human) studies, the latter being grouped into three phases that occur prior to the submission of
an NDA, and a fourth phase that occurs following the approval of a drug product. The successful
completion of a hypnotic development program involves the careful consideration of subject
and patient populations, study designs, methodology, and study endpoints. While regulatory
guidance and precedents exist, our expanding knowledge of insomnia, its morbidities, and its
pathophysiology will undoubtedly lead to new clinical development strategies for hypnotics
and the development of new therapeutics that treat insomnia and its morbidities.
REFERENCES
1. Pharmaceutical Research and Manufacturers of America (PhRMA). Washington, DC: PhRMA Annual
Survey, 2008.
2. Pharmaceutical Research and Manufacturers of America (PhRMA). Washington, DC: PhRMA Report,
2006.
3. U.S. Food and Drug Administration (FDA). Federal Food and Drugs Act of 1906 (The “Wiley Act”),
Public Law Number 59–384, 34 Stat. 768 (1906), 1906.
4. U.S. Food and Drug Administration (FDA). History of the FDA. 2008. http://www.fda.gov/oc/
history/historyoffda/default.htm.
5. Ballentine C. Taste of Raspberries, Taste of Death: The 1937 Elixir Sulfanilamide Incident. 1981.
6. U.S. Food and Drug Administration (FDA). History of the FDA: The 1938 Food, Drug, and Cosmetic
Act. 2008.
7. Daemmrich A. A tale of two experts: Thalidomide and political engagement in the United States and
West Germany. Soc Hist Med 2002; 15(1):137–158.
8. Celgene Corporation. Thalomid (thalidomide) Capsules. 2009.
9. Krantz JC Jr. New drugs and the Kefauver-Harris amendment. J New Drugs 1966; 6(2):77–79.
10. Krantz JC Jr. The Kefauver-Harris amendment after sixteen years. Mil Med 1978; 143(12):883.
11. Center for Drug Evaluation and Research (CDER). Guidelines for the clinical evaluation of hypnotic
drugs. In: U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, ed. 1977.
12. U.S. Food and Drug Administration (FDA). FDA Requests Label Change for All Sleep Disorder Drug
Products. FDA News 2007.
13. National Institutes of Health. Research TNCftPoHSoBaB. The Belmont Report: Ethical Principles and
Guidelines for the Protection of Human Subjects of Research, the National Research Act (Pub. L.
93–348), 1979.
c38 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:47 Char Count=
448 ZAMMIT
14. U.S. Food and Drug Administration (FDA). Milestones in U.S. Food and Drug Law History. 2005.
15. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (DHHS). Code of Federal Regulations (CFR),
Title 21.
16. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Guidance for Industry, Investigators, and Reviewers;
Exploratory IND Studies. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2006. Accessed 23 May,
2008.
17. Ng R. Drugs, From Discovery to Approval. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, 2004.
18. Sanger DJ, Morel E, Perrault G. Comparison of the pharmacological profiles of the hypnotic drugs,
zaleplon and zolpidem. Eur J Pharmacol 1996; 313(1–2):35–42.
19. Ebert B, Anderson NJ, Cremers TI, et al. Gaboxadol—a different hypnotic profile with no tolerance
to sleep EEG and sedative effects after repeated daily dosing. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2008;
90(1):113–122.
20. Griebel G, Perrault G, Tan S, et al. Comparison of the pharmacological properties of classical and
novel BZ-omega receptor ligands. Behav Pharmacol 1999; 10(5):483–495.
21. Dhawan K, Dhawan S, Chhabra S. Attenuation of benzodiazepine dependence in mice by a tri-
substituted benzoflavone moiety of Passiflora incarnata Linneaus: A non-habit forming anxiolytic. J
Pharm Pharm Sci 2003; 6(2):215–222.
22. Alexandre C, Dordal A, Aixendri R, et al. Sleep-stabilizing effects of E-6199, compared to zopiclone,
zolpidem and THIP in mice. Sleep 2008; 31(2):259–270.
23. Saitou K, Kaneko Y, Sugimoto Y, et al. Slow wave sleep-inducing effects of first generation H1-
antagonists. Biol Pharm Bull 1999; 22(10):1079–1082.
24. Shinomiya K, Shigemoto Y, Okuma C, et al. Effects of short-acting hypnotics on sleep latency in rats
placed on grid suspended over water. Eur J Pharmacol 2003; 460(2–3):139–144.
25. Shinomiya K, Shigemoto Y, Omichi J, et al. Effects of three hypnotics on the sleep-wakefulness cycle
in sleep-disturbed rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2004; 173(1–2):203–209.
26. Visser SA, Wolters FL, van der Graaf PH, et al. Dose-dependent EEG effects of zolpidem provide
evidence for GABA(A) receptor subtype selectivity in vivo. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 2003; 304(3):1251–
1257.
27. Winsky-Sommerer R, Vyazovskiy VV, Homanics GE, et al. The EEG effects of THIP (gaboxadol) on
sleep and waking are mediated by the GABA(A)delta-subunit-containing receptors. Eur J Neurosci
2007; 25(6):1893–1899.
28. Michaud JC, Muyard JP, Capdevielle G, et al. Mild insomnia induced by environmental perturbations
in the rat: A study of this new model and of its possible applications in pharmacological research.
Arch Int Pharmacodyn Ther 1982; 259(1):93–105.
29. Noguchi H, Kitazumi K, Mori M, et al. Electroencephalographic properties of zaleplon, a non-
benzodiazepine sedative/hypnotic, in rats. J Pharmacol Sci 2004; 94(3):246–251.
30. Di Masi JA, Grabowski HG. The cost of biopharmaceutical R&D: is biotech different? Managerial
and Dec Econ 2007; 28(4–5):469–479.
31. Sachs G. United States: Healthcare Services: CROs, 2007.
32. Pharma. Pharma Annual Report. Washington, DC: 2008.
33. Center for Drug Evaluation and Research (CDER).
34. National Institutes of Health (NIH). Glossary of Clinical Trial Terms. 2008.
35. Center for Drug Evaluation and Research (CDER). Guideline for the Format and Content of Human
Pharmacokinetics and Bioavailability Section of an Application. 1987.
36. Center for Drug Evaluation and Research (CDER). Guidance for Industry. Pharmacokinetics in
Patients with Impaired Hepatic Function: Study Design, Data Analysis, and Impact on Dosing and
Labeling. 2003.
37. Center for Drug Evaluation and Research (CDER). Guidance for Industry: Pharmacokinetics in
Patients with Impaired Renal Function: Study Design, Data Analysis, and Impact on Dosing and
Labeling. 1998.
38. Center for Drug Evaluation and Research (CDER). Guidance for Industry: Pharmacokinetics in
Pregnancy: Study Design, Data Analysis, and Impact on Dosing and Labeling. 2004.
39. Center for Drug Evaluation and Research (CDER). Draft Guidance for Industry General Considera-
tions for Pediatric Pharmacokinetic Studies for Drugs and Biological Products. 2003.
40. Zammit G. Zolpidem extended-release: Therapy for sleep induction and sleep maintenance difficul-
ties. Expert Opin Drug Metab Toxicol 2008; 4(3):325–331.
41. Greenblatt DJ, Legangneux E, Harmatz JS, et al. Dynamics and kinetics of a modified-release for-
mulation of zolpidem: Comparison with immediate-release standard zolpidem and placebo. J Clin
Pharmacol 2006; 46(12):1469–1480.
42. Roth T, Hull SG, Lankford DA, et al. Low-dose sublingual zolpidem tartrate is associated with dose-
related improvement in sleep onset and duration in insomnia characterized by middle-of-the-night
(MOTN) awakenings. Sleep 2008; 31(9):1277–1284.
c38 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:47 Char Count=
CLINICAL TRIALS AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF NEW THERAPEUTICS FOR INSOMNIA 449
43. Staner L, Eriksson M, Cornette F, et al. Sublingual zolpidem is more effective than oral zolpidem
in initiating early onset of sleep in the post-nap model of transient insomnia: A polysomnographic
study. Sleep Med 2008.
44. Greenblatt DJ, Harmatz JS, von Moltke LL, et al. Comparative kinetics and dynamics of zaleplon,
zolpidem, and placebo. Clin Pharmacol Ther 1998; 64(5):553–561.
45. Hindmarch I, Legangneux E, Stanley N, et al. A double-blind, placebo-controlled investigation of the
residual psychomotor and cognitive effects of zolpidem-MR in healthy elderly volunteers. Br J Clin
Pharmacol 2006; 62(5):538–545.
46. Hesse LM, von Moltke LL, Greenblatt DJ. Clinically important drug interactions with zopiclone,
zolpidem and zaleplon. CNS Drugs 2003; 17(7):513–532.
47. Greenblatt DJ, Patki KC, von Moltke LL, et al. Drug interactions with grapefruit juice: An update. J
Clin Psychopharmacol 2001; 21(4):357–359.
48. Shader RI, Greenblatt DJ. Fruit juices and pharmacology. J Clin Psychopharmacol 1997; 17(4):245–
246.
49. Yamazaki A, Kumagai Y, Fujita T, et al. Different effects of light food on pharmacokinetics and
pharmacodynamics of three benzodiazepines, quazepam, nitrazepam and diazepam. J Clin Pharm
Ther 2007; 32(1):31–39.
50. Sugimoto K, Araki N, Ohmori M, et al. Interaction between grapefruit juice and hypnotic drugs:
Comparison of triazolam and quazepam. Eur J Clin Pharmacol 2006; 62(3):209–215.
51. Roehrs T, Vogel G, Sterling W, et al. Dose effects of temazepam in transient insomnia. Arzneimit-
telforschung 1990; 40(8):859–862.
52. Roth T, Roehrs T, Vogel G. Zolpidem in the treatment of transient insomnia: A double-blind, ran-
domized comparison with placebo. Sleep 1995; 18(4):246–251.
53. Erman MK, Erwin CW, Gengo FM, et al. Comparative efficacy of zolpidem and temazepam in
transient insomnia. Hum Psychopharmacol 2001; 16(2):169–176.
54. Rosenberg R, Caron J, Roth T, et al. An assessment of the efficacy and safety of eszopiclone in the
treatment of transient insomnia in healthy adults. Sleep Med 2005; 6(1):15–22.
55. Rosenberg R, Roth T, Scharf MB, et al. Efficacy and tolerability of indiplon in transient insomnia. J
Clin Sleep Med 2007; 3(4):374–379.
56. Tietz EI, Roth T, Zorick FJ, et al. The acute effect of quazepam on the sleep of chronic insomniacs. A
dose-response study. Arzneimittelforschung 1981; 31(11):1963–1966.
57. Zammit G, Schwartz H, Roth T, et al. The effects of ramelteon in a first-night model of transient
insomnia. Sleep Med 2008.
58. Seidel WF, Roth T, Roehrs T, et al. Treatment of a 12-hour shift of sleep schedule with benzodiazepines.
Science 1984; 224(4654):1262–1264.
59. Seidel WF, Cohen SA, Bliwise NG, et al. Dose-related effects of triazolam and flurazepam on a
circadian rhythm insomnia. Clin Pharmacol Ther 1986; 40(3):314–320.
60. Erman MK, Loewy DB, Scharf MB. Effects of temazepam 7.5 mg and temazepam 15 mg on sleep
maintenance and sleep architecture in a model of transient insomnia. Curr Med Res Opin 2005;
21(2):223–230.
61. Walsh JK, Mayleben D, Guico-Pabia C, et al. Efficacy of the selective extrasynaptic GABA A agonist,
gaboxadol, in a model of transient insomnia: A randomized, controlled clinical trial. Sleep Med 2008;
9(4):393–402.
62. Walsh JK, Schweitzer PK, Sugerman JL, et al. Transient insomnia associated with a 3-hour phase
advance of sleep time and treatment with zolpidem. J Clin Psychopharmacol 1990; 10(3):184–189.
63. Terzano MG, Parrino L, Fioriti G, et al. Modifications of sleep structure induced by increasing levels
of acoustic perturbation in normal subjects. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1990; 76(1):29–38.
64. Terzano MG, Parrino L, Boselli M, et al. Changes of cyclic alternating pattern (CAP) parameters
in situational insomnia under brotizolam and triazolam. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1995; 120(3):
237–243.
65. Parrino L, Boselli M, Spaggiari MC, et al. Multidrug comparison (lorazepam, triazolam, zolpidem,
and zopiclone) in situational insomnia: Polysomnographic analysis by means of the cyclic alternating
pattern. Clin Neuropharmacol 1997; 20(3):253–263.
66. Hindmarch I, Legangneux, E, Stanley N. A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, 10-way
crossover study shows that a new zolpidem modified-release formulation improves sleep mainte-
nance compared to standard zolpidem. Sleep 2004; 27:A55.
67. Cluydts R, De Roeck J, Cosyns P, et al. Antagonizing the effects of experimentally induced sleep
disturbance in healthy volunteers by lormetazepam and zolpidem. J Clin Psychopharmacol 1995;
15(2):132–137.
68. Erman MK, Zammit G, Rubens R, et al. A polysomnographic placebo-controlled evaluation of the
efficacy and safety of eszopiclone relative to placebo and zolpidem in the treatment of primary
insomnia. J Clin Sleep Med 2008; 4(3):229–234.
c38 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:47 Char Count=
450 ZAMMIT
69. Monti JM, Attali P, Monti D, et al. Zolpidem and rebound insomnia—a double-blind, controlled
polysomnographic study in chronic insomniac patients. Pharmacopsychiatry 1994; 27(4):166–175.
70. Herrmann WM, Kubicki ST, Boden S, et al. Pilot controlled double-blind study of the hypnotic effects
of zolpidem in patients with chronic “learned” insomnia: Psychometric and polysomnographic
evaluation. J Int Med Res 1993; 21(6):306–322.
71. Declerck AC, Ruwe F, O’Hanlon JF, et al. Effects of zolpidem and flunitrazepam on nocturnal sleep
of women subjectively complaining of insomnia. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1992; 106(4):497–501.
72. Cohn JB, Wilcox CS, Bremner J, et al. Hypnotic efficacy of estazolam compared with flurazepam in
outpatients with insomnia. J Clin Pharmacol 1991; 31(8):747–750.
73. Elie R, Lavoie G, Bourgouin J, et al. Zopiclone versus flurazepam in insomnia: Prolonged adminis-
tration and withdrawal. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 1990; 5(4):279–286.
74. Nair NP, Schwartz G, Dimitri R, et al. A dose-range finding study of zopiclone in insomniac patients.
Int Clin Psychopharmacol 1990; 5(suppl 2):1–10.
75. Ponciano E, Freitas F, Camara J, et al. A comparison of the efficacy, tolerance and residual effects of
zopiclone, flurazepam and placebo in insomniac outpatients. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 1990; 5(suppl
2):69–77.
76. Cohn JB. Double-blind crossover comparison of triazolam and lorazepam in the posthypnotic state.
J Clin Psychiatry 1984; 45(3):104–107.
77. Roth T, Rogowski R, Hull S, et al. Efficacy and safety of doxepin 1 mg, 3 g, and 6 mg in adults with
primary insomnia. Sleep 2007; 30(11):1555–1561.
78. Roth T, Seiden D, Wang-Weigand S, et al. A 2-night, 3-period, crossover study of ramelteon’s efficacy
and safety in older adults with chronic insomnia. Curr Med Res Opin 2007; 23(5):1005–1014.
79. Scharf MB, Roth T, Vogel GW, et al. A multicenter, placebo-controlled study evaluating zolpidem in
the treatment of chronic insomnia. J Clin Psychiatry 1994; 55(5):192–199.
80. Fabre LF Jr, Brachfeld J, Meyer LR, Slowe IA, et al. Multi-clinic double-blind comparison of triazolam
(Halcion) and placebo administered for 14 consecutive nights in outpatients with insomnia. J Clin
Psychiatry 1978; 39:679–682.
81. Walsh JK, Targum SD, Pegram V, et al. A multi-center clinical investigation of estazolam: Short-term
efficacy. Curr Ther Res 1984; 36:866–874.
82. Walsh JK, Soubrane C, Roth T. Efficacy and safety of zolpidem extended release in elderly primary
insomnia patients. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2008; 16(1):44–57.
83. Zammit G, Erman M, Wang-Weigand S, et al. Evaluation of the efficacy and safety of ramelteon in
subjects with chronic insomnia. J Clin Sleep Med 2007; 3(5):495–504.
84. Nowell PD, Mazumdar S, Buysse DJ, et al. Benzodiazepines and zolpidem for chronic insomnia: A
meta-analysis of treatment efficacy. JAMA 1997; 278(24):2170–2177.
85. Krystal AD, Erman M, Zammit GK, et al. Long-term efficacy and safety of zolpidem extended-release
12.5 mg, administered 3 to 7 nights per week for 24 weeks, in patients with chronic primary insomnia:
A 6-month, randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel-group, multicenter study. Sleep
2008; 31(1):79–90.
86. Walsh JK, Krystal AD, Amato DA, et al. Nightly treatment of primary insomnia with eszopiclone for
six months: Effect on sleep, quality of life, and work limitations. Sleep 2007; 30(8):959–968.
87. Scharf MB, Black J, Hull S, et al. Long-term nightly treatment with indiplon in adults with primary
insomnia: Results of a double-blind, placebo-controlled, 3-month study. Sleep 2007; 30(6):743–752.
88. Krystal AD, Walsh JK, Laska E, et al. Sustained efficacy of eszopiclone over 6 months of nightly
treatment: Results of a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study in adults with chronic
insomnia. Sleep 2003; 26(7):793–799.
89. Zammit GK, McNabb LJ, Caron J, et al. Efficacy and safety of eszopiclone across 6-weeks of treatment
for primary insomnia. Curr Med Res Opin 2004; 20(12):1979–1991.
90. Roth T, Soubrane C, Titeux L, et al. Efficacy and safety of zolpidem-MR: A double-blind, placebo-
controlled study in adults with primary insomnia. Sleep Med 2006; 7(5):397–406.
91. Hajak G, Clarenbach P, Fischer W, et al. Zopiclone improves sleep quality and daytime well-being
in insomniac patients: Comparison with triazolam, flunitrazepam and placebo. Int Clin Psychophar-
macol 1994; 9(4):251–261.
92. Dominguez RA, Goldstein BJ, Jacobson AF, et al. Comparative efficacy of estazolam, flurazepam,
and placebo in outpatients with insomnia. J Clin Psychiatry 1986; 47(7):362–365.
93. Scharf MB, Roth PB, Dominguez RA, et al. Estazolam and flurazepam: A multicenter, placebo-
controlled comparative study in outpatients with insomnia. J Clin Pharmacol 1990; 30(5):
461–467.
94. Ancoli-Israel S, Walsh JK, Mangano RM, et al. Zaleplon: A novel nonbenzodiazepine hypnotic,
effectively treats insomnia in elderly patients without causing rebound effects. Prim Care Companion
J Clin Psychiatry 1999; 1(4):114–120.
c38 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:47 Char Count=
CLINICAL TRIALS AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF NEW THERAPEUTICS FOR INSOMNIA 451
95. Cluydts R. Zolpidem ‘as needed’: Methodological issues and clinical findings. CNS Drugs 2004;
18(suppl 1):25–33; discussion 43–45.
96. Hajak G, Geisler P. Experience with zolpidem ‘as needed’ in primary care settings. CNS Drugs 2004;
18(suppl 1):35–40; discussion 1, 3–5.
97. Walsh JK, Roth T, Randazzo A, et al. Eight weeks of non-nightly use of zolpidem for primary insomnia.
Sleep 2000; 23(8):1087–1096.
98. Hajak G, Cluydts R, Declerck A, et al. Continuous versus non-nightly use of zolpidem in chronic
insomnia: Results of a large-scale, double-blind, randomized, outpatient study. Int Clin Psychophar-
macol 2002; 17(1):9–17.
99. Roth T, Hauri P, Zorick F, et al. The effects of midazolam and temazepam on sleep and performance
when administered in the middle of the night. J Clin Psychopharmacol 1985; 5(2):66–69.
100. Perlis ML, McCall WV, Krystal AD, et al. Long-term, non-nightly administration of zolpidem in the
treatment of patients with primary insomnia. J Clin Psychiatry 2004; 65(8):1128–1137.
101. Danjou P, Paty I, Fruncillo R, et al. A comparison of the residual effects of zaleplon and zolpidem
following administration 5 to 2 h before awakening. Br J Clin Pharmacol 1999; 48(3):367–374.
102. Walsh JK, Pollak CP, Scharf MB, et al. Lack of residual sedation following middle-of-the-night zaleplon
administration in sleep maintenance insomnia. Clin Neuropharmacol 2000; 23(1):17–21.
103. Hindmarch I, Patat A, Stanley N, et al. Residual effects of zaleplon and zolpidem following middle
of the night administration five hours to one hour before awakening. Hum Psychopharmacol 2001;
16(2):159–167.
104. Zammit GK, Corser B, Doghramji K, et al. Sleep and residual sedation after administration of zaleplon,
zolpidem, and placebo during experimental middle-of-the-night awakening. J Clin Sleep Med 2006;
2(4):417–423.
105. Farber RH, Burke PJ. Post-bedtime dosing with indiplon in adults and the elderly: Results from two
placebo-controlled, active comparator crossover studies in healthy volunteers. Curr Med Res Opin
2008; 24(3):837–846.
106. Roth T, Zammit GK, Scharf MB, et al. Efficacy and safety of as-needed, post bedtime dosing
with indiplon in insomnia patients with chronic difficulty maintaining sleep. Sleep 2007; 30(12):
1731–1738.
107. Center for Drug Evaluation and Research (CDER). Guidance for Industry: E14 Clinical Evaluation of
QT/Qtc Interval Prolongation and Proarrhythmic Potential for Non-Antiarrhythmic Drugs. In: U.S.
Department of Health and Human Services, FADA, ed.; 2005.
108. Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: The Epworth Sleepiness Scale. Sleep
1991; 14(6):540–545.
109. Johns MW. Reliability and factor analysis of the Epworth Sleepiness Scale. Sleep 1992; 15(4):376–381.
110. Carskadon MA, Dement WC, Mitler MM, et al. Guidelines for the multiple sleep latency test (MSLT):
A standard measure of sleepiness. Sleep 1986; 9(4):519–524.
111. Thorpy MJ. The clinical use of the Multiple Sleep Latency Test. The Standards of Practice Committee
of the American Sleep Disorders Association. Sleep 1992; 15(3):268–276.
112. Kobak KA, DeBrota D, Engelhardt N, et al. Site vs. centralized raters in a clinical depression trial. In:
46th Annual Meeting of the National Institutes of Health, New Clinical Drug Evaluation Unit,; 2006;
Boca Raton, FL.
113. McCall WV, Erman M, Krystal AD, et al. A polysomnography study of eszopiclone in elderly patients
with insomnia. Curr Med Res Opin 2006; 22(9):1633–1642.
114. Monti JM, Alvarino F, Monti D. Conventional and power spectrum analysis of the effects of zolpidem
on sleep EEG in patients with chronic primary insomnia. Sleep 2000; 23(8):1075–1084.
115. Ozone M, Yagi T, Itoh H, et al. Effects of zolpidem on cyclic alternating pattern, an objective marker of
sleep instability, in Japanese patients with psychophysiological insomnia: A randomized crossover
comparative study with placebo. Pharmacopsychiatry 2008; 41(3):106–114.
116. Terzano MG, Parrino L. Clinical applications of cyclic alternating pattern. Physiol Behav 1993;
54(4):807–813.
117. Terzano MG, Parrino L, Spaggiari MC, et al. CAP variables and arousals as sleep electroencephalo-
gram markers for primary insomnia. Clin Neurophysiol 2003; 114(9):1715–1723.
118. Scharf M, Erman M, Rosenberg R, et al. A 2-week efficacy and safety study of eszopiclone in elderly
patients with primary insomnia. Sleep 2005; 28(6):720–727.
119. Knutson KL, Spiegel K, Penev P, et al. The metabolic consequences of sleep deprivation. Sleep Med
Rev 2007; 11(3):163–178.
120. Knutson KL, Van Cauter E. Associations between sleep loss and increased risk of obesity and diabetes.
Ann N Y Acad Sci 2008; 1129:287–304.
121. Van Cauter E, Holmback U, Knutson K, et al. Impact of sleep and sleep loss on neuroendocrine and
metabolic function. Horm Res 2007; 67(suppl 1):2–9.
c38 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:47 Char Count=
452 ZAMMIT
122. Gangwisch JE, Heymsfield SB, Boden-Albala B, et al. Sleep duration as a risk factor for diabetes
incidence in a large U.S. sample. Sleep 2007; 30(12):1667–1673.
123. Gangwisch JE, Malaspina D, Boden-Albala B, et al. Inadequate sleep as a risk factor for obesity:
Analyses of the NHANES I. Sleep 2005; 28(10):1289–1296.
124. Gangwisch JE, Heymsfield SB, Boden-Albala B, et al. Short sleep duration as a risk factor for hyper-
tension: Analyses of the first National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. Hypertension 2006;
47(5):833–839.
125. Gottlieb DJ, Redline S, Nieto FJ, et al. Association of usual sleep duration with hypertension: The
Sleep Heart Health Study. Sleep 2006; 29(8):1009–1014.
126. Ozer O, Ozbala B, Sari I, et al. Acute sleep deprivation is associated with increased QT dispersion in
healthy young adults. Pacing Clin Electrophysiol 2008; 31(8):979–984.
127. Sari I, Davutoglu V, Ozbala B, et al. Acute sleep deprivation is associated with increased electrocar-
diographic P-wave dispersion in healthy young men and women. Pacing Clin Electrophysiol 2008;
31(4):438–442.
128. Gangwisch JE, Heymsfield SB, Boden-Albala B, et al. Sleep duration associated with mortality in
elderly, but not middle-aged, adults in a large US sample. Sleep 2008; 31(8):1087–1096.
129. Center for Drug Evaluation and Research (CDER). Guidance for Industry: E6 Good Clinical Practice:
Consolidated Guidance. In: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, FADA, ed., 1996.
130. Kryger M, Wang-Weigand S, Roth T. Safety of ramelteon in individuals with mild to moderate
obstructive sleep apnea. Sleep Breath 2007; 11(3):159–164.
131. Cohn MA, Morris DD, Juan D. Effects of estazolam and flurazepam on cardiopulmonary function in
patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Drug Saf 1992; 7(2):152–158.
132. Dorsey CM, Lee KA, Scharf MB. Effect of zolpidem on sleep in women with perimenopausal and
postmenopausal insomnia: A 4-week, randomized, multicenter, double-blind, placebo-controlled
study. Clin Ther 2004; 26(10):1578–1586.
133. Soares CN, Joffe H, Rubens R, et al. Eszopiclone in patients with insomnia during perimenopause
and early postmenopause: A randomized controlled trial. Obstet Gynecol 2006; 108(6):1402–1410.
134. Asnis GM, Chakraburtty A, DuBoff EA, et al. Zolpidem for persistent insomnia in SSRI-treated
depressed patients. J Clin Psychiatry 1999; 60(10):668–676.
135. Pollack M, Kinrys G, Krystal A, et al. Eszopiclone coadministered with escitalopram in patients with
insomnia and comorbid generalized anxiety disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2008; 65(5):551–562.
136. Fava M, McCall WV, Krystal A, et al. Eszopiclone co-administered with fluoxetine in patients with
insomnia coexisting with major depressive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2006; 59(11):1052–1060.
c39 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:49 Char Count=
39 Practice Models
Michael J. Sateia
Department of Psychiatry, Dartmouth Medical School, Lebanon, New Hampshire, U.S.A.
INTRODUCTION
Acute and chronic insomnia are prevalent in a multitude of medical settings. The degree of
relevance of this disorder to the particular medical care being delivered will vary substantially
from one setting to another. Nevertheless, it is incumbent on health care providers of all types
to recognize the potential importance of this problem and the influence it may have on care
delivery and outcome, and to be familiar with at least the basics of assessment and management.
Chronic insomnia is associated with increased health care utilization, chronic illness, and high
rates of disability and functional impairment (1,2) (see also chapter 3). As outlined in previous
chapters, the condition may also predispose to psychiatric disorders (3,4), alter pain thresholds
(5), and perhaps, adversely affect cardiovascular function (6), to name but a few of the possible
consequences.
Recognizing the presence of an insomnia problem is a relatively straightforward process,
in that it requires only a complaint of sleep disturbance, despite adequate opportunity to sleep,
that is associated with daytime consequences (7). Despite the relative simplicity of the general
diagnosis, we know that most individuals with the problem are not identified. Health care
providers are constantly assailed with the need to do more and more in a health care environment
that allows them less and less time per patient. In this climate of competition for the attention
of providers (and patients), it is necessary to make the case for why this particular problem
warrants precious time. Ultimately, this requires demonstration that intervention enhances
outcome in one or more spheres. At present, the field of sleep medicine is still some distance
from making that case in a convincing manner. The same, of course, may be said for a host
of other common health care practices. Nevertheless, data on outcomes in current therapeutic
research, though spotty, suggest that identification and treatment improves health and function
(8–10).
The extent to which insomnia is identified and addressed can be expected to vary widely
from one medical discipline to another. Although the greatest management expertise presum-
ably resides within sleep medicine, there can be little doubt that the great majority of insomnia
is encountered in primary care and mental health practices. Therefore, it is necessary to consider
what levels of intervention can reasonably be expected in these varied settings. A simple goal
would be mere identification of a chronic sleep problem, but the extant data suggest that cur-
rent practice is far from achieving this goal. Neither physicians nor patients seem particularly
inclined to bring this issue to medical attention, despite the fact that some patients, when asked,
report insomnia as a very significant symptom (11).
What, then, are realistic expectations at a primary care level? Routine health screening
should include, at a minimum, an inquiry regarding the patient’s sleep. This brief query, coupled
with determination of daytime consequences and adequacy of opportunity to sleep (i.e., time
in bed under conducive circumstances), is sufficient to establish the presence of an insomnia
problem. Primary care providers can also be expected to ascertain the duration of the problem.
Management of an acute problem is likely to be quite different from that of one that has been
present for years or decades.
454 SATEIA
recommendations are quite often for over-the-counter (OTC) products (antihistamine or mela-
tonin) or off-label use of sedating antidepressants (12). Patients may have initiated self-
medication with alcohol or OTC products, including herbal supplements.
Optimal management of short-term insomnia is reasonably straightforward. It is neces-
sary to consider what current stresses or conditions may have precipitated the sleep problem.
The presence of other common and readily treatable illness such as major depression must
be considered. If the insomnia is, indeed, an adjustment disturbance, several straightforward
approaches are indicated. First, consideration of whether the patient requires further assistance
in coping effectively with the current stress. Brief stress management counseling in the office
or referral for more specialized mental health counseling are options. On occasion, daytime
pharmacotherapy (antianxiety medication) may be indicated. Second, sleep education is likely
to reduce the likelihood of an acute disorder evolving into chronic insomnia. These principles
are discussed in detail in chapter 23. Preventing the development of factors that are likely to
perpetuate the insomnia by encouraging maintenance of a regular schedule and stimulus con-
trol measures is particularly important. Finally, consideration must be given to pharmacological
aid. As noted, patients may already be self-medicating. Although specific practice parameters
regarding pharmacological management of adjustment insomnia do not exist, most sources
suggest that self-medicating is generally not advisable. Alcohol, the most common form of self-
medication for insomnia, clearly is contraindicated (chapters 16 and 36) and efficacy of OTC
products is not well established (chapter 36). When patient and physician agree that the degree
of distress or dysfunction warrants use of a sleep aid, a benzodiazepine receptor agonist (BzRA)
or melatonin agonist hypnotic is indicated (13). The efficacy of these drugs is well established
and, when used and monitored properly, they carry limited clinical risk. The most critical aspect
of choosing a specific hypnotic medication is matching the duration of the clinical action of the
drug to the sleep complaint. This issue is discussed at greater length in preceding chapters. Per-
haps most importantly, follow-up in the primary care setting is critical. This should include (1)
review of the current status of the insomnia; (2) inquiry regarding precipitating conditions; (3)
continued sleep hygiene and stimulus control education; (4) determination of effectiveness and
side effects of medication; (5) tapering and discontinuation of hypnotic medication as exacerbat-
ing events and sleep disturbance resolve, with education regarding possible transient rebound
insomnia.
Comorbidities
Certainly, primary care physicians should address those common comorbidities that are within
the realm of their expertise—conditions such as major depression, substance abuse, pain,
gastro-esophageal reflux disorder (GERD), nocturia, nocturnal respiratory distress (e.g.,
asthma), or endocrine disease. A review of medications with consideration of possible offend-
ing substances is likewise appropriate. When comorbidities such as this are identified and a
suspicion of causal linkage to the insomnia exists, primary care providers have traditionally
been inclined to focus primarily on treating the comorbidity, with the expectation that the sleep
problem, as a “secondary” symptom, would resolve. More recent evidence indicates the need
for a paradigm shift in this respect (15). Studies in several key areas, particularly depression
and pain, suggest that optimal outcome may be achieved by concurrent treatment of not only
the comorbid condition, but the insomnia as well (16,17). Treatment for the insomnia may be
psychological/behavioral, pharmacological, or both.
Prescribing Hypnotics
The decision regarding prescription of medication for chronic insomnia patients is a complex
one. Numerous factors influence this decision. These include (1) patient preference; (2) prior
experience with medication and results of this; (3) willingness to engage in CBT-I; (4) availabil-
ity of CBT clinicians; (5) contraindications to use of hypnotics or other sedating medications.
Many physicians are reluctant to prescribe medication for chronic insomnia, based on fears of
dependency and tolerance. The practice of recommending OTC medication (commonly anti-
histamine compounds) or sedating antidepressants, in all likelihood, reflects the belief that
c39 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:49 Char Count=
456 SATEIA
these agents are safer alternatives to approved BzRA medications. Unfortunately, the efficacy
of these alternatives is not well established in chronic insomnia, and it is not clear that they are
intrinsically “safer” than standard hypnotics (28). Therefore, current AASM guidelines recom-
mend that when medications are prescribed for chronic insomnia, the first-line choice should
be an appropriate shorter-acting BzRA (13). The more important issues, perhaps, for health
care providers with respect to prescribing these medications are (1) patient education regarding
goals, expectations, and side effects of medication; (2) use of minimal effective dosages of a
medication with a duration of action appropriate to the insomnia complaint; (3) implemen-
tation of effective psychological and behavioral therapies; (4) tapering and discontinuation of
medication in conjunction with (3); and (5) continued follow-up during and after medication
trials. Alternative pharmacotherapies are addressed briefly in the following section on mental
health models and more extensively elsewhere in this text.
Combined Treatment
Clinicians must recognize that when hypnotic medication is prescribed for chronic insomnia,
it should, whenever possible, be accompanied by CBT-I. The primary reason for this dictum is
that current data make clear that short-term trials of sleep medication, while effective during
the period of active administration, do not typically produce sustained improvement of the
sleep disturbance (18,29,30). Evidence is also clear that CBT-I does produce durable change.
Thus, in the absence of these nonpharmacological therapies, clinicians are frequently left in the
uncomfortable position of either discontinuing medication, with resulting recrudescence of the
problem, or maintaining chronic hypnotic use.
For some time, there has been debate over whether combining pharmacotherapy with
CBT-I is advantageous or, alternatively, counterproductive. Proponents of combined treatment
argue that medication may provide faster relief and enhance patient confidence in their ability to
sleep, thus reinforcing gains associated with CBT. Opponents suggest that patients may misat-
tribute gains to medication, rather than to CBT, be less motivated to implement skills effectively,
and thus, be more prone to relapse with discontinuation of the medication. The evidence on
this is mixed. The most widely cited study of combined treatment (18) found that combined
therapy was associated with a trend toward greater improvement immediately posttreatment
than either treatment alone, but the longer-term outcome with combined treatment was more
variable than with CBT alone. Another study found no advantage of combined treatment over
CBT alone (29).
long-term complications or dependency risks (i.e., discontinue the medication) and a patient
who is desperate for relief from insomnia. To further complicate this scenario, patients often
report that the current medication, and many prior ones, are ineffective, but will resist efforts
to discontinue them. In turn, this may prompt further and increasingly desperate efforts on
the part of the prescriber to find an effective medication or combination of medications. While
pharmacotherapy has a legitimate place in the treatment of chronic insomnia, as discussed, it is
seldom the case that rapid turnover polypharmacy, in its own right, is the solution for chronic
insomnia. Efforts to simplify and discontinue medications in this population may be the more
advantageous therapeutic strategy in many cases. Given the fact that the improvements seen
with short-term medication trials alone are not generally sustained in chronic insomnia patients,
it is hardly surprising that primary care providers would often meet resistance and be frustrated
in efforts to achieve medication tapering or discontinuation without providing the patient with
effective alternative therapies. A growing body of evidence clearly defines the effectiveness of
CBT-I in promoting successful tapering and discontinuation of chronic hypnotic medication (18).
One study (33) demonstrated a twofold higher rate of complete hypnotic discontinuation
in older chronic hypnotic users when CBT-I (small group format × 8 weeks) was utilized in
combination with medication tapering (77% discontinuation) versus tapering alone (38%). This
differential was even greater at 12-month follow-up (70% vs. 24%). These observations have
been extended in a separate study to assess the combination of self-help CBT with tapering
(34). These two conditions produced comparable rates of reduction in hypnotic usage, although
sleep efficiency increased and wake time decreased in the CBT group. However, the sleep-related
improvements in the CBT group may reflect primarily the effects of restricted time in bed. In a
similar investigation (35) of BZD discontinuation in patients with anxiety disorder or insomnia,
treatment as usual (tapering and physician counseling) was compared to tapering coupled with
either group support or group CBT). Both group support and CBT produced significantly higher
rates of discontinuation (85% and 83%, respectively) than treatment as usual (39%). Of note,
however, is the fact that rates for group support (in which any specific recommendations were
specifically prohibited) were equivalent to those for CBT. A more recent placebo-controlled
investigation (36) suggests that administration of melatonin in conjunction with tapering may
promote higher rates of hypnotic discontinuation in an elderly population than tapering alone.
In summary, while some patients may benefit from chronic use of sleep medications, many
others have not received adequate evaluation or nonpharmacological therapies. Ultimately, a
number of chronic insomnia sufferers will not respond adequately to the level of treatment
offered by primary care providers. In such cases, consideration should be given to referral to a
specialist in sleep medicine. This is discussed further in later sections.
458 SATEIA
seems quite clear that insomnia comorbid with psychiatric conditions has outcomes that are
quite comparable to those observed in primary insomnia. Widespread application of group
CBT-I by psychologists or psychiatric nurses in community mental health settings, inpatient
or partial hospitalizations, or rehabilitation programs might provide significant benefit, but
initiation of such endeavors is in its infancy, at best.
Patients with chronic psychiatric disorders often exhibit many complicating factors that
must be addressed if sleep disturbance is to improve. Substance abuse may be foremost among
these. The characteristics and management of insomnia comorbid with alcohol and drug abuse
is discussed in detail in chapter 16. As a result of isolation and lack of social zeitgebers, patients
with chronic psychiatric illness are prone to circadian disorders such as delayed or irregu-
lar sleep–wake rhythms. Special consideration must be given to the impact of psychotropic
medications on sleep–wake function. The contribution of certain symptoms associated with
specific psychiatric disorders to insomnia may also need to be addressed. One of the most com-
mon examples of this is recurrent nightmares in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder.
In recent years, two therapies have shown particular promise in managing nightmares. The
␣1 -adrenergic antagonist, prazosin, has been shown to not only reduce nightmare frequency,
but also to improve sleep in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) patients (43,44). Likewise, the
cognitive-behavioral therapy of imagery rehearsal and desensitization shows similar outcomes
(45,46). Other symptoms such as nocturnal panic attacks or hallucinations may also require
specific treatment focus.
Pharmacotherapy for insomnia in the setting of mental illness requires special consider-
ations. In many cases, the primary psychopharmacological treatment may be a sedating com-
pound, which can be administered at bedtime and may significantly aid sleep. Examples of this
would include sedating antidepressant medications such as mirtazapine, or antipsychotic med-
ications including many first-generation neuroleptics as well as second-generation drugs such
as quetiapine, olanzapine, or risperidone. Mood-stabilizing agents may also provide substantial
sedative effect, which may be used to good advantage at bedtime. Certainly, combined use of
these medications for psychiatric indications and sleep disturbance may be quite appropriate
and may simplify the medication regimen.
Beyond this combined indication, however, there are many patients who require the
addition of a sedating medication primarily for the indication of insomnia. Psychiatrists, like
many primary care physicians, often prescribe off-label use of sedating antidepressants such as
trazodone (47–50), or sedating antipsychotics, especially quetiapine (51–53). These agents and
others are discussed in detail in chapter 34. While these agents may have certain advantages in
this population, it is important for practitioners to recognize that there has been very limited
assessment of these drugs, often in uncontrolled studies, with regard to their efficacy in chronic
insomnia. For this reason, guidelines suggest that BzRAs or ramelteon are the treatments of
choice for chronic insomnia, including those cases comorbid with mental disorders. Having
said that, it must also be recognized that in patients with high potential for abuse, such drugs
may be contraindicated.
CONCLUSIONS
Modern medicine, despite its many advances, is not providing reliable detection or effective
treatment for the vast majority of people with chronic sleep disturbance. This probably reflects
some degree of indifference to the problem, stemming from lack of education and awareness, as
well as uncertainty regarding the appropriate assessment and management. The net result of this
is unnecessary suffering, increased health care utilization, and in all likelihood, increased risk
for significant medical and psychiatric disorders. Chronic insomnia is probably preventable
for many, although we lack the large-scale public health interventions necessary to test this
assertion. Likewise, the great majority of established chronic sleep disturbance does not come
to medical attention. The economic cost of this enormous gap in loss of productivity, accidents,
OTC and self-help therapies, and increased health care utilization numbers in the billions. Yet,
capturing the attention of those affected and their care providers remains a daunting task.
Unfortunately, while medicine struggles to manage numerous problems that have no
effective treatments, this readily treatable disorder is largely ignored. However, sleep medicine
must come to terms with the fact that, while we have made great strides in developing and
testing efficacious therapies that can produce long-term improvement, we lag far behind in
identifying or developing the resources necessary to effectively deliver these therapies to even
a small percentage of the affected population. It seems likely that health care providers would
be far more prepared to address the issue of chronic insomnia if a clear treatment pathway and
necessary resources were available to them. Absent this, we should expect little change in the
current dynamic.
REFERENCES
1. Simon GE, VonKorff M. Prevalence, burden, and treatment of insomnia in primary care. Am J Psychi-
atry 1997; 154(10):1417–1423.
2. Terzano MG, Parrino L, Cirignotta F, et al. Studio Morfeo: insomnia in primary care, a survey conducted
on the Italian population. Sleep Med 2004; 5(1):67–75.
3. Breslau N, Roth T, Rosenthal L, et al. Sleep disturbance and psychiatric disorders: A longitudinal
epidemiological study of young adults. Biol Psychiatry 1996; 39(6):411–418.
4. Weissman MM, Greenwald S, Nino-Murcia G, et al. The morbidity of insomnia uncomplicated by
psychiatric disorders. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 1997; 19(4):245–250.
5. Roehrs T, Hyde M, Blaisdell B, et al. Sleep loss and REM sleep loss are hyperalgesic. Sleep 2006;
29(2):145–151.
c39 IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 10:49 Char Count=
460 SATEIA
32. Krystal AD, Walsh JK, Laska E, et al. Sustained efficacy of eszopiclone over 6 months of nightly
treatment: Results of a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study in adults with chronic
insomnia.Sleep 2003; 26(7):793–799.
33. Baillargeon L, Landreville P, Verreault R, et al. Discontinuation of benzodiazepines among older
insomniac adults treated with cognitive-behavioural therapy combined with gradual tapering: A
randomized trial. CMAJ 2003; 169(10):1015–1020.
34. Belleville G, Guay C, Guay B, et al. Hypnotic taper with or without self-help treatment of insomnia:
A randomized clinical trial. J Consult Clin Psychol 2007; 75(2):325–335.
35. O’Connor K, Marchand A, Brousseau L, et al. Cognitive-behavioural, pharmacological and psychoso-
cial predictors of outcome during tapered discontinuation of benzodiazepine. Clin Psychol Psychother
2008; 15(1):1–14.
36. Garzon C, Guerrero JM, Aramburu O, et al. Effect of melatonin administration on sleep, behavioral
disorders and hypnotic drug discontinuation in the elderly: A randomized, double-blind, placebo-
controlled study. Aging Clin Exp Res 2009; 21(1):38–42.
37. Dombrovski AY, Mulsant BH, Houck PR, et al. Residual symptoms and recurrence during maintenance
treatment of late-life depression. J Affect Disord 2007; 103(1–3):77–82.
38. Dombrovski AY, Cyranowski JM, Mulsant BH, et al. Which symptoms predict recurrence of depression
in women treated with maintenance interpersonal psychotherapy? Depress Anxiety 2008; 25(12):1060–
1066.
39. Pigeon WR, Hegel M, Unützer J, et al. Is Insomnia a perpetuating factor for late-life depression in the
IMPACT cohort? Sleep 2008; 31(4):482–488.
40. Edinger JD, Olsen MK, Stechuchak KM, et al. Cognitive behavioral therapy for patients with primary
insomnia or insomnia associated predominantly with mixed psychiatric disorders: A randomized
clinical trial. Sleep 2009; 32(4):499–510.
41. Espie CA, MacMahon KM, Kelly HL, et al. Randomized clinical effectiveness trial of nurse-
administered small-group cognitive behavior therapy for persistent insomnia in general practice.
Sleep 2007; 30(5):574–584.
42. Rybarczyk B, Lopez M, Benson R, et al. Efficacy of two behavioral treatment programs for comorbid
geriatric insomnia. Psychol Aging 2002; 17(2):288–298.
43. Taylor FB, Martin P, Thompson C, et al. Prazosin effects on objective sleep measures and clinical
symptoms in civilian trauma posttraumatic stress disorder: A placebo-controlled study. Biol Psychiatry
2008; 63(6):629–632.
44. Raskind MA, Peskind ER, Hoff DJ, et al. A parallel group placebo controlled study of prazosin for
trauma nightmares and sleep disturbance in combat veterans with post-traumatic stress disorder. Biol
Psychiatry 2007; 61(8):928–934.
45. Moore BA, Krakow B. Imagery rehearsal therapy for acute posttraumatic nightmares among combat
soldiers in Iraq. Am J Psychiatry 2007; 164(4):683–684.
46. Krakow B, Zadra A. Clinical management of chronic nightmares: Imagery rehearsal therapy. Behav
Sleep Med 2006; 4(1):45–70.
47. Schwartz T, Nihalani N, Virk S, et al. A comparison of the effectiveness of two hypnotic agents for the
treatment of insomnia. Int J Psychiatr Nurs Res 2004; 10(1):1146–1150.
48. Kaynak H, Kaynak D, Gozukirmizi E, et al. The effects of trazodone on sleep in patients treated with
stimulant antidepressants. Sleep Med 2004; 5(1):15–20.
49. Saletu-Zyhlarz GM, Anderer P, Arnold O, et al. Confirmation of the neurophysiologically predicted
therapeutic effects of trazodone on its target symptoms depression, anxiety and insomnia by post-
marketing clinical studies with a controlled-release formulation in depressed outpatients. Neuropsy-
chobiology 2003; 48(4):194–208.
50. Nierenberg AA, Adler LA, Peselow E, et al. Trazodone for antidepressant-associated insomnia. Am J
Psychiatry 1994; 151(7):1069–1072.
51. Philip NS, Mello K, Carpenter LL, et al. Patterns of quetiapine use in psychiatric inpatients: An
examination of off-label use. Ann Clin Psychiatry 2008; 20(1):15–20.
52. Wiegand MH, Landry F, Bruckner T, et al. Quetiapine in primary insomnia: A pilot study. Psychophar-
macology (Berl) 2008; 196(2):337–338.
53. Endicott J, Rajagopalan K, Minkwitz M, et al. A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study
of quetiapine in the treatment of bipolar I and II depression: Improvements in quality of life. Int Clin
Psychopharmacol 2007; 22(1):29–37.
54. Chesson AL Jr,Ferber RA, Fry JM, et al. The indications for polysomnography and related procedures.
Sleep 1997; 20(6):423–487.
app IHBK059-Sateia March 31, 2010 22:17 Char Count=
Appendix: Resources
WEBSITES
http://www.sleepeducation.com – the public education site of the American Academy of Sleep
Medicine (AASM) includes detailed information on the causes and treatments of insomnia and
other sleep disorders.
http://www.aasmnet.org – the American Academy of Sleep Medicine professional site includes
detailed information on accreditation of sleep centers, professional education programs and
resources, clinical standards and useful information for the health professional within and
outside the field.
http://www.sleepcenters.org – this AASM site provides a roster of accredited sleep centers
across the United States.
http://www.sleepresearchsociety.org/ – the Sleep Research Society is the major professional
organization of sleep researchers. The site includes educational opportunities and products
and additional resources for sleep researchers.
http://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/about/ncsdr/index.htm – the National Center on Sleep Disorders
Research (NCSDR) is a division within the National Heart, Lung and Blood Institute (NHLBI).
This site provides public and professional education and research information.
http://www.sleepfoundation.org – the National Sleep Foundation engages in public education
and information gathering through its annual national sleep survey.
http://www.americaninsomniaassociation.org – the American Insomnia Association is a
patient-based organization which is dedicated to advancing identification and treatment of
insomnia through public and professional education and research.
http://www.absm.org/BSM Specialists.htm – the American Board of Sleep Medicine (ABSM)
maintains a list of certified behavioral sleep medicine specialists nationwide.
http://www.absm.org/Diplomates/listing.htm – the ABSM also maintains a list of health pro-
fessionals certified by this board.∗
∗
Since 2007, the board certification examination in sleep medicine is offered by the American
Board of Internal Medicine, in conjunction with other primary specialty boards. Rosters of
physicians certified since 2007 by these respective boards can be found on the web sites of those
organizations.
Index
Note: Page numbers followed by f, n, and t indicate figures, notes, and tables, respectively.
466 Index
Index 467
468 Index
Index 469
470 Index
Index 471
472 Index
Index 473
474 Index
Number of awakenings, 50n, 332t, 335t OSA. See Obstructive sleep apnea
NWAK. See Number of awakenings Outcome variables, 445–446
Overall oxygen consumption, 66, 67f
Objective daytime consequences
balance, 14 Pain administration, 141
hand/eye coordination, 14 Pain, chronic, 36
logical reasoning, 14 Paired t-tests, 79
math, 14 Panic disorder, 130–131
memory, 13–14 Paradoxical insomnia, 66, 110, 114
sleepiness/alertness, 14 ICSD-2 insomnia disorders, 104t
vigilance, 14 pediatric, 238t
Obstructive sleep apnea, 93 sleep restriction therapy, 286
pediatric insomnia, 237 Paradoxical intention, 292, 304–305, 305t
psychiatric disorders, 133t, 135 Paralimbic system, 78–79
sleep-related breathing disorder, 210, 211 Parasomnias, 102, 106
symptoms, 86t Parkinson’s disease, 158
Off-label prescribing in insomnia Passive-paradoxical techniques, 299
anticonvulsants Patient global impressions, 381
gabapentin, 404–405 Patient history in pediatric insomnia, 241t
pregabalin, 405 Patient medication guides, 438
tiagabine, 405 Pediatric insomnia, 379
valproic acid, 405 classification, 237, 238t–240t
antidepressants definition, 235
amitriptyline, 400–401 differential diagnosis
doxepin, 401 delayed sleep phase syndrome, 237
mirtazapine, 402 disorders of arousal, 240
nortriptyline, 401 obstructive sleep apnea, 237
trazodone, 399–400 periodic limb movement disorders, 237
antipsychotics restless leg syndrome, 237
alprazolam, 404 disorders of arousal, 240
anxiolytics, 403 epidemiology
clonazepam, 404 family history, 236
lorazepam, 403–404 neurological and psychiatric disorders, 236
olanzapine, 402–403 evaluation, 240, 241t
quetiapine, 402 management, 240,
drug occurrences, 398t, 399t behavioral insomnia of childhood, 242–
patient-report evaluation, 398 244, 243t
pharmacological treatment, 400t pharmacologic interventions, 245–251
placebo comparison, 397 sleep hygiene for children, 242t
testing in insomnia patients, 398 medications
Olanzapine, 402–403 alpha2-adrenergic receptor agonists, 246
Older adults, insomnia in, 378, 378t antidepressants, 251
age-related sleep/wake pattern antihistamines, 245
circadian rhythms, 225–227 benzodiazepines, 250
homeostatic processes, 225 BzRA hypnotics, 250
sleep architecture, 224–225 melatonin, 250
cognitive behavioral therapy, 357f, 361 normal sleep, development of
correlates of insomnia adolescents (13–18 years), 236
dementia, 228 infants and toddlers (0–2 years), 235–236
medical factors, 227 school-aged children (7–12 years), 236
medications and substances, 229 young children (3–6 years), 236
psychiatric disorders, 227 pharmacotherapy, 246t–249t
sleep disorders, 228–229 Penn state worry questionnaire, 130
treatment Periodic limb movement disorder,.
cognitive-behavioral treatment, 230–231 pediatric insomnia, 237
light therapy, 231 psychiatric disorder, 134
pharmacologic, 229–230 See also under Movement disorders
Opioids, 166t, 170–171 Periodic limb movements, 135
Opponent process model, 182, 182f, 183f Periodic limb movements in sleep, 228
Orexin antagonists, 428–429 Personality, 46–47
Organic sleep disorders, 106 PET. See Positron emission tomography
ind IHBK059-Sateia March 28, 2010 14:3 Char Count=
Index 475
476 Index
Index 477
478 Index
Index 479