RCFT Writeup
RCFT Writeup
Introduction
The Rey Complex Figure Test (RCFT) stands as a pivotal tool within the realm of
neuropsychological assessment, offering clinicians a window into the intricate workings of
cognitive functioning. Developed by Swiss psychologist André Rey in the early 20th century,
this test presents participants with a deceptively simple yet intricately detailed line drawing,
challenging them to reproduce it from memory after a delay. Through its multi-faceted approach,
the RCFT evaluates an array of cognitive domains, including visuospatial abilities, memory
encoding and retrieval, and executive functions. Its standardized administration and scoring
procedures, coupled with its sensitivity to subtle cognitive impairments, have positioned the
RCFT as a cornerstone of psychological evaluations across diverse clinical populations.
Figure:
Equivalent and simplified versions of Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure (ROCF). In 1941, Rey
developed the classic ROCF (A). To avoid the learning effect by using the same figure twice,
some studies have developed multiple equivalent versions of the ROCF, such as the Taylor
figure (B), the modified Taylor figure (C), the Mark figure (D), and various versions of the
Medical College of Georgia Complex Figures (E–H). Subsequently, multiple simplified versions
of ROCF (I, K, L) were developed. The Benson figure (I) is less affected by executive
function. (K) has been proven to be suitable for the elderly. Kim developed (L) for digital
assessment tools. Similarly, the simplified Taylor figure (J) has also been confirmed to suit the
low-educated elderly.
1. Copying Phase:
Description: During the copying phase, participants are presented with the Rey Complex Figure
stimulus card and instructed to reproduce the figure as accurately as possible onto a blank sheet
of paper.
Assessment: This phase primarily evaluates participants' visuospatial abilities, visual perception,
and motor skills. It assesses their ability to perceive the complex visual stimuli, analyze its
spatial relationships, and accurately reproduce it using fine motor coordination.
Importance: The copying phase provides valuable insights into participants' immediate
visuospatial processing abilities. It serves as a baseline assessment of their perceptual and motor
skills, allowing clinicians to observe how they approach and execute a complex visual task in
real-time.
2. Immediate Recall Phase:
Description: Following the copying phase, participants are asked to reproduce the Rey Complex
Figure from memory onto a new sheet of paper immediately after completing the copying task.
Assessment: The immediate recall phase assesses participants' short-term memory, working
memory, and cognitive organization. It evaluates their ability to retain and recall visuospatial
information over a brief delay, without access to the original stimulus.
Importance: This phase provides insights into participants' memory encoding, retention, and
retrieval processes. It allows clinicians to assess the fidelity of their memory representation of
the figure and their ability to organize and reproduce spatial information from memory
accurately.
3. Delayed Recall Phase:
Description: After a delay period (typically ranging from 10 to 30 minutes), participants are
asked to reproduce the Rey Complex Figure from memory once again onto a new sheet of paper.
Assessment: The delayed recall phase evaluates participants' long-term memory retention and
retrieval processes. It assesses their ability to retain and recall visuospatial information over an
extended period, providing insights into the durability of their memory traces.
Importance: This phase offers valuable information about the persistence of participants'
memory representations over time. It allows clinicians to assess the stability of their memory
recall and identify any decay or degradation in memory performance following a delay, which
can be indicative of underlying cognitive impairments or neurological conditions.
5. Diagnostic Utility:
Beyond its role in assessing specific cognitive functions, the RCFT holds significant diagnostic
utility across a spectrum of neurological and psychiatric conditions. It is commonly employed in
the diagnosis and differential diagnosis of conditions such as traumatic brain injury, dementia
(including Alzheimer's disease), stroke, multiple sclerosis, and developmental disorders such as
autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). By
identifying patterns of cognitive strengths and weaknesses, the RCFT aids clinicians in
formulating accurate diagnoses, prognosticating outcomes, and developing targeted intervention
plans tailored to individuals' cognitive profiles.
Alzheimer's Disease (AD):
Alzheimer's disease is characterized by progressive cognitive decline, particularly in memory,
executive functions, and visuospatial abilities. The RCFT is instrumental in detecting early signs
of cognitive impairment associated with AD and monitoring disease progression over time.
Individuals with AD typically demonstrate deficits in memory and visuospatial processing,
reflected in their performance on the RCFT. During the copying phase, they may struggle to
accurately reproduce the complex figure, omitting details or misplacing elements due to impaired
visual perception and spatial orientation. In the recall phases, they may exhibit significant
difficulties in remembering and accurately reproducing the figure from memory, indicative of
deficits in encoding, retention, and retrieval processes. By analyzing patterns of performance on
the RCFT, clinicians can differentiate between normal age-related cognitive changes and
pathological decline associated with AD, facilitating timely diagnosis and intervention.
Scoring Methodology:
The scoring of the RCFT, based on the scoring criteria developed by Rey (1941; Corwin &
Bylsma, 1993) and presented in Lezak (1995). Rey’s scoring system divides the complex figure
into 18 units; each unit is scored separately for both accuracy and placement. The Rey Complex
Figure, with the 18 scoring units numbered, is illustrated in Figure 1. Specific criteria were
developed for scoring the accuracy and placement of units of drawings.
The scoring methodology for the Rey Complex Figure Test (RCFT) involves evaluating
participants' performance across multiple dimensions, including accuracy, organization, and
detail. Deviations from the original figure are carefully assessed to glean insights into
participants' cognitive functioning. Let's delve into each aspect in detail with suitable examples:
Accuracy:
Description: Accuracy refers to the extent to which participants' reproductions of the Rey
Complex Figure correspond to the original stimulus in terms of overall shape, proportions, and
spatial relationships.
Example: If the original figure contains a specific geometric shape or pattern, such as a circle or
a series of intersecting lines, accurate reproduction would entail participants correctly replicating
these elements in their drawings. Deviations from the original figure, such as misshapen or
omitted components, would indicate reduced accuracy in reproduction.
Organization:
Description: Organization assesses the structural coherence and spatial arrangement of
participants' drawings, including the logical sequencing and grouping of elements within the
figure.
Example: A well-organized reproduction of the Rey Complex Figure would exhibit clear
delineation of component parts, with elements grouped and positioned in a logical manner. For
instance, participants may systematically reproduce the figure by first outlining the perimeter,
then filling in internal details, and finally adding finer features. Disorganized drawings, on the
other hand, may lack cohesive structure, with elements scattered or haphazardly arranged.
Detail:
Description: Detail refers to the level of granularity and completeness in participants'
reproductions of the Rey Complex Figure, encompassing the inclusion of specific features,
intricacies, and embellishments present in the original stimulus.
Example: A highly detailed reproduction of the figure would capture fine-grained elements such
as intersecting lines, internal patterns, and small geometric shapes with precision and accuracy.
Participants may demonstrate attention to detail by faithfully replicating intricate components
and subtle nuances present in the original stimulus. Conversely, drawings lacking detail may
appear sparse or incomplete, with significant omissions or simplifications of complex features.
Scoring:
A score of 0, 0.5, 1, or 2 is assigned to each unit of the figure based on accuracy and placement
criteria. Unit scores are then summed to obtain the raw score for that drawing. The same scoring
criteria apply to all three drawing trials. Thus, raw scores ranging from 0.0 to 36.0 may be
obtained for the Copy, Immediate Recall, and Delayed Recall trials. For each unit of the figure, a
score of 2 is assigned if the unit was drawn accurately and placed correctly. A score of 1 is
assigned if the unit was drawn accurately, but was placed incorrectly. A score of 1 is assigned if
the unit was drawn inaccurately, but was placed correctly. A score of 1 is also assigned if the unit
was duplicated (i.e., drawn twice) in the drawing, but at least one of the duplicates was drawn
accurately and placed correctly. A score of 0.5 is assigned if the unit was drawn inaccurately and
was placed incorrectly, but is still recognizable. A score of 0.5 is assigned if the unit was
duplicated in the drawing, and both units were drawn inaccurately and were placed incorrectly,
but both were still recognizable. A score of 0 is assigned if the unit was omitted altogether or
was drawn inaccurately and placed incorrectly, and was not recognizable.
For each scoring unit, both a general scoring principle and specific scoring criteria are presented.
The specific scoring criteria specify accuracy and placement ratings in terms of precise distance
measurements
Scoring the recognition trial :
A true positive response on the Recognition trial occurs when a respondent indicates that he or
she recognizes an element that is part of the Rey Complex Figure (i.e., circles a target item).
Recognition trial Items 2, 5, 7, 8, 9, 12, 13, 15, 19, 20, 22, and 24 are target items. Sum the
number of these items that were circled, and enter this value as the raw score for Recognition
True Positives. A false positive response on the Recognition trial occurs when a respondent
indicates that he or she recognizes an element that is not part of the figure (i.e., circles a
distractor item). Recognition trial Items 1, 3, 4, 6, 10, 11, 14, 16, 17, 18, 21, and 23 are distractor
items. Sum the number of these items that were circled, and enter this value as the raw score for
Recognition False Positives.
A true negative response on the Recognition trial occurs when a respondent indicates that he or
she does not recognize an item that is not part of the figure (i.e., does not circle a distractor item).
The Recognition True Negatives raw score is the inverse of the Recognition False Positives raw
score. Therefore, subtract the Recognition False Positives raw score from 12, and record this
value as the raw score for Recognition True Negatives. A false negative response on the
Recognition trial occurs when a respondent indicates that he or she does not recognize an item
that is part of the figure. The Recognition False N negatives raw score is the inverse of the
Recognition True Positives raw score. Therefore, subtract the Recognition True Positives raw
score from 12, and record this value as the raw score for Recognition False Negatives.
The Recognition Total Correct raw score is the sum of the Recognition True Positives and
Recognition True Negatives raw scores. Sum these two scores and record this value as the
Recognition Total Correct raw score. After the raw scores for the RCFT have been calculated
and entered their respective spaces on page 1 of the test booklet, corresponding normative scores
may be recorded.
Interpretation of Deviations:
When interpreting deviations from the original figure in participants' drawings, clinicians
consider the nature and pattern of errors across dimensions such as accuracy, organization, and
detail. Significant discrepancies may suggest underlying cognitive impairments or difficulties in
specific cognitive domains:
Inaccuracies: Substantial errors or distortions in reproducing key elements of the figure may
indicate deficits in visuospatial perception or motor coordination.
Disorganization: Chaotic or disjointed drawings lacking coherent structure may suggest
difficulties in planning, executive functioning, or cognitive organization.
Lack of Detail: Sparse or rudimentary drawings with minimal detail may reflect deficits in
attention, visual processing, or memory encoding.
Reliability:
Test-Retest Reliability: Studies have reported test-retest reliability coefficients for the RCFT
ranging from around 0.60 to 0.90, indicating moderate to high levels of consistency over time.
Inter-Rater Reliability: Inter-rater reliability coefficients for the RCFT have been reported in the
range of 0.70 to 0.90, suggesting generally good agreement among raters in scoring the test.
Validity:
Content Validity: Content validity for the RCFT is supported by the fact that the test assesses key
cognitive functions related to visuospatial constructional ability, memory, and organizational
skills, which are relevant to its intended purpose.
Construct Validity: Construct validity has been demonstrated through correlations between
RCFT scores and performance on other neuropsychological measures assessing similar
constructs. For example, studies have shown significant correlations between RCFT scores and
measures of visuospatial ability, memory function, and executive functioning.