PEMBSADA PSY4 Eysenck
PEMBSADA PSY4 Eysenck
● Hans Eysenck was born in Germany but moved to England after turning 18 and spent
most of his working life there.
● He eventually went to school to earn a Ph.D. in Psychology from the University College
London in 1940 under the supervision of psychologist Cyril Burt, who is best-known for
his research on the heritability of intelligence.
● Eysenck was born on March 4, 1916
● He died on September 4, 1997
● His research interests were broad, but he is perhaps best known for his theories of
personality and intelligence.
● Eysenck's theory of personality focused on temperaments, which he believed were
largely controlled by genetic influences.1 He utilized a statistical technique known as
factor analysis to identify what he believed were the two primary dimensions of
personality: extraversion and neuroticism. He later added a third dimension known as
psychoticism.
● Eysenck was a hugely influential figure in psychology. At the time of his death in 1997,
he was the most-frequently cited psychologist in scientific journals.
● Despite his influence, Eysneck was also a controversial figure. His suggestion that
racial differences in intelligence were due to genetics rather than environment
generated a tremendous amount of pushback.
● During World War II, Eysenck worked as a research psychologist at Mill Hill Emergency
Hospital.
● He later founded the psychology department at the University of London Institute of
Psychiatry, where he worked until 1983. He served as Professor Emeritus at the school
until his death in 1997.
● He was also an extremely prolific writer.
● Over the course of his career, Eysenck published more than 75 books and over
1,600 journal articles. Prior to his death, he was the most frequently cited living
psychologist.2
Contributions to Psychology
● In addition to being one of the most famous psychologists, Eysenck was also one of the
most controversial.3 One of his earliest controversies revolved around a paper he wrote in
1952 on the effects of psychotherapy. In the paper, Eysenck reported that two-thirds of
therapy patients improved significantly or recovered within two years, regardless of
whether or not they received psychotherapy.4
● He was also a vocal critic of psychoanalysis, dismissing it as unscientific.
● The greatest controversy surrounding Eysenck was his view of the heritability
of intelligence, more specifically his view that racial differences in intelligence could
be partially attributed to genetic factors.5
● After one of his students was criticized for publishing a paper suggesting that genetics
were responsible for racial differences in intelligence, Eysenck defended him and later
published The IQ Argument: Race, Intelligence, and Education, which incited
considerable controversy and criticism. His 1990 autobiography took a more moderate
view that ascribed greater importance to the role of environment and experience in
shaping intelligence.
● He also played a major role in establishing approaches to clinical training and
psychotherapy that were firmly rooted in empirical research and science.
Dimensions of Temperament
Neuroticism refers to one’s emotional stability, or lack thereof. Neuroticism raises the intensity
of emotional reactions. Individuals who measure high in neuroticism are more likely to suffer
from neuroses, but high neuroticism is not necessarily less desirable than low levels of
neuroticism.
It incorporates:
● mood swings
● poor emotional adjustment
● feelings of inferiority
● lack of social responsibility
● lack of persistence
● issues of trust vs. suspiciousness
● social shyness, hypochondria, and the
● lack of relaxed composure.
For example, aesthetic appreciation and creativity can benefit from an individual being highly
emotional. On the clearly negative side, high levels of neuroticism have routinely been found
in criminals, perhaps because whenever an individual has antisocial tendencies, a high level of
neuroticism enhances their fear/anxiety responses and functions as a powerful, albeit
dysfunctional, drive (Eysenck, 1977, 1982; Kendrick, 1981).
In contrast, people high on stability tend to need more stimulation to activate their flight-or-
fight reaction and are therefore considered more emotionally stable. (Boundless, n.d.) Cattell
also studied neuroticism, and his findings were very similar to those of Eysenck (Cattell &
Scheier, 1961).
Psychoticism was added to Eysenck’s theory well after identifying extraversion and neuroticism,
and it is the least clearly defined or heritable of the three superfactors. It incorporates traits of
dominance-leadership, dominance-submission, sensation seeking, and the lack of a
superego.
The major strength of Eysenck’s model is that he was one of the first to make his approach
more quantifiable; it was therefore perceived to be more “legitimate,” as a common criticism
of psychological theories is that they are not empirically verifiable.
Eysenck proposed that extraversion was caused by variability in cortical arousal, with introverts
characteristically having a higher level of activity in this area than extroverts. He also
hypothesized that neuroticism was determined by individual differences in the limbic system, the
part of the human brain involved in emotion, motivation, and emotional association with
memory. Unlike Allport’s and Cattell’s models, however, Eysenck’s theory has been criticized
for being too narrow. (Boundless, n.d.)