Family Environment and Attachment in Relation To Complex Trauma A
Family Environment and Attachment in Relation To Complex Trauma A
2020
Recommended Citation
Ocean, Susan Elizabeth, "FAMILY ENVIRONMENT AND ATTACHMENT IN RELATION TO COMPLEX
TRAUMA AND C-PTSD" (2020). Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers. 11656.
https://scholarworks.umt.edu/etd/11656
This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at ScholarWorks at University
of Montana. It has been accepted for inclusion in Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers
by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks at University of Montana. For more information, please contact
[email protected].
FAMILY ENVIRONMENT AND ATTACHMENT
IN RELATION TO COMPLEX TRAUMA AND C-PTSD
By
Dissertation
Doctor of Philosophy
In Psychology
August 2020
Approved by:
Scott Whittenberg,
Graduate School Dean
Childhood traumatic experiences occurring during critical developmental stages are strongly
linked to poor mental health outcomes during adulthood, including PTSD. Yet, individuals who
have experienced multiple traumas (and across developmental stages) report a profile of
symptoms that is not well-represented by traditional PTSD diagnostic criteria. Recent research
suggests that resulting post-traumatic stress after the experience of complex trauma should be
considered a separate, yet related, disorder from the well-established PTSD. Since traumatic
experiences are commonplace and often detrimental, establishing which factors contribute to risk
and resilience is of great importance. Having secure attachment to a primary caregiver and
family cohesion are consistently supported in the literature as contributors to resilience. The
primary objective of the current research was to contribute to the ongoing development and
understanding of complex trauma and the proposed C-PTSD categorization. Additionally, this
study evaluated the relationship between experiencing trauma, developing trauma
symptomology, and familial factors of (1) a positive family environment in youth and (2) secure
attachment. Results provided supportive evidence of a significant relationship between a higher
number of potentially traumatic events (PTEs) and increased trauma symptomology, specifically
PTSD and C-PTSD. A positive family environment and a more secure attachment style were
found to be associated with less PTSD and C-PTSD symptomology. This study provides
preliminary support and suggests further exploration of factors that may strengthen resilience and
protect against trauma-related symptoms would be beneficial.
ii
Introduction
Childhood experiences of trauma are a significant public health concern in the United
States. In a general population sample of children and adolescents, Costello, Erkanli, Fairbank,
and Angold (2002) found that one in four had experienced at least one high-magnitude stressor
(such as the death of a caregiver) in their lifetime. Over half (57%) reported experiencing
victimization in youth aged 2 to 17 years old, only 29% had experienced no victimization,
concluding that childhood exposure to violence, crime, maltreatment, and other forms of
victimization are a “routine part of ordinary childhood in the United States” (Finkelhor, Ormrod,
Turner, & Hamby, 2005; p.18). In minority, refugee, and clinical populations, a single
experience of trauma is the exception rather than the rule (Kira, 2008) and victimized youth are
then at greater risk, as they are frequently re-victimized (Finkelhor, Ormrod, & Turner, 2007;
Herman, 1992). Childhood trauma literature suggests that an early history of maltreatment or
severe adversity significantly affects the mental health of these individuals (Briere & Jordan,
during critical developmental stages are thought to have considerable negative influence on adult
mental health (Briere & Jordan, 2009; Felitti et al., 1998; Rees et al., 2011). The Centers for
Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) reported information about adverse childhood
experiences collected from approximately 9,000 adult health maintenance organization (HMO)
members (Felitti, et al., 1998). Over 30% of participants reported being physically abused,
23.5% reported being exposed to family alcohol abuse, 19.9% reported being sexually abused,
18.8% reported experiencing mental illness in their family, 12.5% reported witnessing
1
interparental violence, 11% reported emotional abuse as a child, and almost 5% of participants
reported having experienced family drug abuse. The authors outlined relationships between these
experiences and depression, suicidality, domestic violence, alcohol and drug abuse, sexual
promiscuity and sexually transmitted diseases, as well as other serious health-related concerns in
adulthood. Further, the more cumulative and stressful the traumatic experiences, the more likely
individuals were to develop health problems later in life such as cancer, stroke, heart disease, and
diabetes.
The development of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is one of the most common
psychological sequelae of trauma in adults (Copeland, Gordon, Angold, & Costello, 2007;
Feeny, Foa, Treadwell, & March, 2004). Individuals who develop PTSD following trauma have
an even greater risk of developing life course impairments, including major depression,
substance dependence, unemployment, and marital instability (Breslau, Davis, Peterson, &
Schultz, 2000). Lifetime prevalence rate estimates for adults with PTSD range from 6.8%
Individuals who experience trauma at an early age or for a prolonged period of time, or
who experience trauma of an interpersonal nature, may show symptoms that fall outside the
range covered by PTSD (van der Kolk, 2005). These symptoms often give rise to “comorbid”
diagnoses, frequently thought of and treated separately, and as unrelated to the traumatic
experience. In fact, individuals who have experienced trauma across a variety of time spans and
developmental stages have reported numerous symptoms not represented by a PTSD diagnosis.
Reported symptoms have included depression, anxiety, dissociation, substance misuse, self-
(including parenting), as well as medical and somatic concerns (Courtois, 2008). These
2
symptoms are commonly categorized as comorbid diagnoses rather than identified as meaningful
identification of factors that promote both risk and resiliency. Family environment and secure
attachment have been identified as primary protective factors that influence resilience to trauma-
related disorders. Family unity and cohesion are associated with resilience (Liem et al., 1997;
Resnick et al, 1997). Further, children are thought to develop different patterns of attachment
organization based on their experiences with their primary caregivers (Ainsworth et al., 1978).
When these experiences are positive, and the caregiver is accessible and responsive, the social
development of the child will follow a “normal course” (Ainsworth et al.; p. 9). Attachment
styles impact the development of the internal working models applied to future relationships
(Paley, Cox, Burchinal, & Payne, 1999). They influence individuals’ beliefs regarding their own
self-worth and schemas of how others will respond to their needs. When a secure attachment
representation is developed, others are considered supportive and reliable, and self-worth
healthy. When an insecure model is established, others are believed to be unavailable, rejecting,
attachment styles within adult relationships is an important area to consider in relation to overall
health and wellness. In Bowlby’s words (as cited in Hazan & Shaver, 1994), attachment is an
essential consideration when studying human behavior “from the cradle to the grave.”
The result of traumatic experience during developmental stages is either stronger, more
resilient individuals who are better able to successfully maneuver life experiences, or more
vulnerable, less resistant individuals who may be limited in their ability to effectively navigate
3
by Sroufe (1997): “Disturbance is not a given; it is supported. Pathology is not something a child
‘has’; it is a pattern of adaptation reflecting the totality of the developmental context to that
point.” Those who move through traumatic experiences and manage to remain on a normal
which factors contribute to risk and to resilience is a topic of great importance for children, their
clinicians, their families, and the community. Although the field is vast and there are difficulties
defining a fluid construct such as resilience, research currently suggests that building resiliency
may not be the obscure and daunting task it was once thought to be (Bonanno, 2008; Masten,
2001: Yehuda, 2004). In order to protect individuals and communities from considerable mental
health difficulties, a better understanding of the complexity of trauma experiences and resulting
disruptions, factors that facilitate resilience, and treatment options that appropriately address
both of these will aid in fewer long-term mental and physical health problems (Jonkman et al.,
Review of Literature
A clinical diagnosis of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) includes these criteria: (1)
exposure to a traumatic event, (2) re-experiencing the event, (3) avoidance of the trauma-related
stimuli, (4) negative thoughts or feelings related to the traumatic event, (5) prolonged
physiological hyperarousal, (6) symptom duration of longer than one month, and (7) functional
impairment due to these symptoms (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013). Re-
experiencing symptoms of PTSD may include disturbing, intrusive thoughts and nightmares;
4
negative thoughts and feelings may include a negative affect, feelings of isolation, and
exaggerated blame of self or others; hyperarousal may include disruptive hypervigilance and a
PTSD is less frequently diagnosed in childhood than during adulthood (Feeny, et al.,
2004; Yule, 2001). A general population sample of 1,420 children (nine-, eleven-, and thirteen-
year-olds) were followed annually through sixteen years of age (Copeland, et al., 2007). Though
trauma experiences were common, clinical PTSD was rarely found in this age group. According
to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, 4th edition Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR; APA, 2000),
childhood symptoms may present differently than adult symptoms and may include disorganized
disturbance, and difficulties concentrating. The DSM-5 (APA, 2013) now includes updated
guidelines for a PTSD diagnosis in children less than six years of age: (1) exposure to a
traumatic event, (2) intrusive re-experiencing, (3) avoidance of trauma-related stimuli, and (4)
be at risk for symptoms and functional impairment well beyond a PTSD diagnosis (Ford, 2017).
Hodges et al. (2013) found that youth who experienced cumulative interpersonal trauma
developed symptom complexity rather quickly, supporting the proposed developmental trauma
disorder (DTD; van der Kolk, 2005). DTD would classify those individuals with a high number
of varying symptoms associated with repeated trauma exposure along the developmental
continuum. A DTD diagnosis continues to be debated and falls within the ongoing discussion
5
Complexity of Traumatic Experiences and Resulting Symptomology
What happens when an individual experiences complex trauma? Courtois (2008) has said
it quite simply, “complex trauma generates complex reactions” (p. 86). Van der Kolk (2005, p.2)
defines complex trauma in childhood as “the experience of multiple, chronic and prolonged,
developmentally adverse traumatic events, most often of an interpersonal nature, often within the
child’s caregiving system.” Herman (1992) found that there have been multiple independent
dating back to Niederland’s work with Holocaust survivors in the 1960’s. Niederland concluded
that the single concept of ‘traumatic neurosis’ was insufficient for the multitude of clinical
presentations he observed. Herman (1992) also indicated that Tanay, who worked with
Holocaust survivors at about the same time, described character changes in traumatized
individuals that fell outside a typical trauma response (Krystal, 1968). When childhood and
developmental considerations enter the traumatic response equation, Gelinas (1983) spoke of
traumatic syndrome.” As noted, van der Kolk (2005) has suggested “developmental trauma
disorder,” a term that seems to encapsulate the importance of both the complexity of the
Given the complicated nature of prolonged and interpersonal traumatic experiences and
the effects of these on individuals and on society, it is not surprising that the diagnosis of
complex-posttraumatic stress disorder (C-PTSD) is also complex, and even controversial (Ford,
2017). Despite Hermann’s proposal over two decades ago, that trauma should be evaluated with
more breadth, C-PTSD does not yet exist as a diagnostic category in the standard diagnostic
6
reference for the mental health field, the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM). Though C-
PTSD had not yet been formally defined and no standardized measurement had been agreed
upon, Hermann’s appeal resulted in numerous aspects of complex trauma being evaluated by
research. There was enough consensus to warrant the proposed C-PTSD diagnosis in the newest
2019). The current diagnosis consists of six symptoms clusters that include the three PTSD
negative self-concept.
In both the American Psychiatric Association’s DSM and the World Health
Organization’s ICD, the complexity of PTSD criteria has increased through the years. “Enduring
added to the ICD-10 (WHO, 1992) category of adult personality and behavior disorders.
EPCACE may be preceded by PTSD and must be chronic (at least two years). In addition to
PTSD symptoms, EPCACE also includes changes in beliefs about the world, self, and the future
that endure. In alignment with the ongoing categorical versus spectrum debate and discussion of
how diagnoses might be more accurately assessed and treated, the complex trauma construct has
begun to gain momentum in research. There are a number of proposed diagnoses being explored
along the trauma continuum that will likely expand our understanding of trauma-related
disorders beyond PTSD. These include: complex PTSD (C-PTSD), which labels those who have
extreme stress (DESNOS), a diagnostic category catching those that do not neatly fit the PTSD
criteria and most often indicating that the victim was in some way captive by the perpetrator of
7
the trauma (Herman, 1992); “cumulative trauma,” a term used to identify the number of
different types of interpersonal trauma an individual has experienced (Briere, Hodges, &
Godbout, 2010; Briere, Kaltman, & Green, 2008; Follette, Polusny, Bechtle, & Naugle, 1996);
continuous traumatic stress (CTS), which identifies those individuals living in realistic and
ongoing fear of continual victimization (Eagle, 2013); partial PTSD, for those meeting sub-
threshold PTSD symptoms (Friedman et al., 2011); and developmental trauma disorder (DTD;
van der Kolk, 2005), which classifies those individuals with a high number of varying symptoms
associated with repeated trauma exposure along the childhood developmental continuum. It
should be noted that complex trauma and complex PTSD are often thought to be associated with
childhood experiences. Although this relationship is certainly established in the literature, trauma
events that are sustained, repeated, or complex in nature do not only occur in childhood; hence,
‘complex trauma’ will be considered an umbrella term, under which DTD falls.
Classification of Diseases (ICD-11; 2019), there are separate and distinct definitions for
posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and complex PTSD (C-PTSD; Maercker et al., 2013).
Many of the symptoms that are currently associated with the C-PTSD diagnosis in the ICD-11
are included in DSM-5’s PTSD category. The difference is in how they are classified. In the
DSM-5 all symptoms are included under one umbrella category and in the ICD-11 there are two
separate, yet related, diagnoses. DSM-IV-TR included three major symptom clusters: re-
experiencing, avoidance and/or numbing, and arousal. DSM-5 has broken the avoidance and/or
numbing cluster into two distinct categories: avoidance and persistent negative alterations in
cognitions and mood. This new category, called “alterations in arousal and reactivity,” contains
most of DSM-IV-TR’s numbing symptoms and also now includes irritable or aggressive
8
behavior and reckless or self-destructive behavior as well. In the most recent DSM (DSM-5,
APA, 2013), PTSD has changed substantially in ways that are similar to the EPCACE
symptoms. Criterion A no longer requires the intense emotional reaction of fear, hopelessness, or
horror at the time of the traumatic event. Criterion D now includes negative alterations in
cognitions and mood: persistent negative beliefs about oneself, distorted blame of self or others,
and overwhelming emotional distress (i.e., anger, guilt, shame). These changes mean that
persistent difficulties with beliefs and emotions are now included as core elements of PTSD. The
hyperarousal symptom category now includes dysregulated behavior in the form of verbal or
Further, a new PTSD subtype was added to DSM-5 that is characterized by hypo-arousal
anxiety disorder category and is placed in a new section of trauma and stressor-related disorders.
As proposed, PTSD will continue to result from symptoms related to the experience of trauma
(re-experiencing, avoidance, and hyperarousal). The ICD-11 C-PTSD diagnosis requires that an
individual meet all criteria for PTSD and additional symptoms related to disturbances in self-
organization: (1) affective dysregulation, (2) a negative sense of self and identity, and (3)
difficulty in interpersonal relatedness (Hyland, 2017; Marinova & Maercker, 2015; WHO, 2019).
These categories propose to cover the array of difficulties experienced by those who have
undergone sustained, repeated, or complex trauma (Cloitre et al., 2015). DSM-5 also added
special criterion for those with dissociative symptoms or delayed expression of symptoms and
included verbiage for developmental considerations in youth. Again, it is noted that youth
symptomology may present differently than adult symptoms and often manifests in behavior.
9
The ICD-11 C-PTSD diagnosis does not specify particular traumatic experiences that are
required to result in the C-PTSD diagnosis. However, it does suggest that repeated or prolonged
traumas, from which escape is difficult or impossible are commonly associated with this
diagnosis (Cloitre, Garvert, Brewin, Bryant, & Maercker, 2013; Maercker et al., 2013; WHO,
2019). Identifying the most appropriate method of evaluating and measuring the complexity of
potentially traumatic events (PTEs) is a challenging task. Identifying frequency has been the
most commonly used method of determining severity of PTEs (Tolin & Foa, 2006). Whether this
the number of different types of interpersonal trauma experienced (Briere, Hodges, & Godbout,
2010; Cloitre et al.). This count has been shown to be a robust predictor of negative psychosocial
outcomes (Cloitre et al., 2009). However, there is disagreement about which is the best way to
account for symptom complexity – whether counting individual trauma types, experiences of
trauma regardless of type, or some combination of these two variables being the best method is
Cloitre et al. (2015) argue that the effects of exposure to trauma are heterogeneous and
that the current PTSD diagnosis and related available treatments do not adequately address this
exposure) are associated with an increase in number of symptoms that occur beyond those found
in PTSD (Cloitre et al., 2008; Briere, Kaltman, & Green, 2008; Karam et al., 2014). Commonly,
these include emotional dysregulation, difficulties with interpersonal relations, substance misuse,
anger, dissociation, and suicidality (Cloitre et al.). Complex trauma experiences both in
adulthood and during childhood predict symptom complexity; however, cumulative trauma
10
symptomology (Cloitre et al., 2009). Complex psychological trauma interferes with individual
adaptive growth, adversely affects numerous biopsychosocial outcomes, and interferes with
development of resilience (D’Andrea et al., 2012). Traumas identified as complex may include:
violence that occurs within relationships where the individual should be able to expect safety and
protection; sexual, physical, or emotional abuse and neglect of a youth; betrayal of caregiver or
authority trust; and intentional violation of physical boundaries and integrity. Ford (2017) links
these different forms of trauma with four descriptors: intentional, interpersonal, inescapable, and
and moral principles of beneficence, dignity, autonomy, and justice (Ford, 2017). Whether these
are the crucial aspects of traumatic experiences in determining outcomes is yet to be agreed
upon.
Hyland (2017) sought to contribute construct validity to this diagnosis and to assess
whether gender, trauma history, and psychological risk factors (anxiety and dysthymia) distinctly
and the number of instances of sexual abuse experienced in childhood showed greater effects on
PTSD symptoms than on DSO symptoms. Higher levels of anxiety were more predictive of
PTSD symptoms, where higher levels of dysthymia were strongly predictive of DSO symptoms.
There is a growing body of evidence that supports the construct validity of C-PTSD as a
distinct diagnosis (Hyland, 2017). According to Cloitre et al. (2015), six studies have shown
support for the ICD-11 formulation of PTSD and C-PTSD. Cloitre, Garvert, Brewin, Bryant, and
Maercker (2013) evaluated those who had experienced a range of interpersonal violence, while
Elklit, Hyland, and Shevlin (2014) looked at rape victims, domestic violence victims, and those
11
who had experienced traumatic bereavement. Community samples of both young adults
(Perkonigg, Hofler, Wittchen, Trautmann, & Maercker, 2015) and veterans (Wolf et al., 2015),
as well as a population of institutional abuse victims (e.g., foster care, religious organizations;
Knefel, Garvert, Cloitre, & Lueger-Schuster, 2015) have shown support. Preliminary data from a
clinical sample of trauma-exposed youth also found distinct PTSD and C-PTSD categories
(Stolbach, Garvert, & Cloitre, 2014). Further, analyses indicate that C-PTSD is more frequently
found among those who have suffered complex trauma histories and is correlated with more
There has also been disagreement surrounding the definitions of individual variables
included in the C-PTSD discussion. Some researchers have indicated that C-PTSD lacks
discriminant validity because there is a great deal of overlap between PTSD and C-PTSD
(Bryant, 2012). Cloitre et al. (2011) argues that the overlap is part of the definition and that C-
PTSD is a complex variation of PTSD. Bryant (2012) further justifies the construct by arguing
that while emotional dysregulation is in some way part of every diagnostic category, that it is the
requirement of emotional dysregulation that distinguishes C-PTSD from PTSD and other
diagnoses.
developmental years may considerably and negatively impact the more vulnerable individual
well into adulthood (Briere & Jordan, 2009; Felitti et al., 1998; Rees et al., 2011). Since
traumatic experiences are commonplace and often detrimental, establishing which factors
contribute to risk and resiliency is of great importance. “By examining the processes that
12
should be better able to devise ways of promoting positive outcomes in high-risk children and
adapting. Those who move through traumatic experiences and manage to remain on a normal
developmental trajectory are said to have resilience. In less favorable outcomes, negative
consequences may include pathological developments. How we think about these outcomes
drives what we do about them. Research now suggests that though resilience most certainly
exists in a complex system, building resilience is much easier, less elusive, and a far more
ordinary process than once believed (Bonanno, 2008; Masten, 2001; Yehuda, 2004).
Researchers typically categorize the building blocks of resiliency into three primary
groups: individual, familial, and community factors (Luthar et al., 2000; Punamäki, Qouta,
Miller, & El-Sarraj, 2011; Rutter, 1999; Werner, 2000). Individual protective factors may
include such strengths as an internal locus of control, the use of flexible coping strategies, an
easy temperament, higher intelligence, a positive self-concept, and sociability (Werner, 2000).
Dispositions are often thought to have a strong genetic base; however, they may certainly be
Scarr & McCartney, 1983). Familial protective factors may include a developed secure
attachment, well-adjusted and competent caregivers, low birth order, a small family size, and
strong religious beliefs (Werner & Smith, 1992). While the individual exists within the context
of family, the family also functions within the larger context of community. Protective factors in
this category may include close friendships, a positive educational environment and experience,
as well as positive role models (e.g., teachers; Luthar et al., 2000). The familial factors of a
13
positive family environment in youth and a securely developed attachment are the primary focus
Primary protective factors that fall within the family context and are consistently
supported in the current literature include: parents who are physically and psychologically
healthy, parental support, and family cohesion. Childhood is the period of time during which
self-regulation, self-soothing, identity formation, and the ability to be in relationship with others
is developed (Cook et al., 2005; Kinniburgh, Blaustein, Spinazzola, & van der Kolk, 2005). Not
only do caregiving relationships during this stage of development form the foundation for youth
representation of self, but also of others, and, of how to interact with their community at large
(Cook et al., 2005). When the environment is negative, the child has little support, and traumatic
2017). Maltreatment negatively influences secure attachment and other biological systems meant
to aid affect, behavior, and cognition throughout developmental stages (Cook et al., 2005;
Kinniburgh, et al., 2005). Further, McCormack and Thomson (2017) suggest that impaired
emotional, intellectual, and psychosocial development may prevent these individuals from later
Punamäki et al. (2011) analyzed the prevalence of resilience within a sample of 640
Palestinian children and adolescents living in conditions of armed conflict and military
occupation on the Gaza Strip. The resilient children had psychologically healthy parents, who
were supportive and practiced fewer punishing methods of parenting. Literature on resilience
generally suggests that parents who are able to regulate their own emotions are more likely able
to provide a safe family environment, despite potentially traumatic events. These parents are
14
successful in comforting their children and providing hope and safety, even in the terrifying
circumstances of war.
One of the most powerful supports for young children in the face of trauma is a positive
relationship with a primary caregiver (e.g., secure attachment; Scheeringa & Zeanah, 2001;
Zeanah, Boris, & Larrieu, 1997). Parenting quality may include structure, warmth, and
have been strongly related to less positive outcomes in young children (Deblinger, Steer, &
Lipmann, 1999; Laor et al., 1997; Scheeringa & Zeanah, 2001). Punamäki et al. (2011) suggest
that family support, as a protective factor, crosses cultural boundaries; all children benefit from
Not only in various cultures, but also with various types of trauma and health risks,
family unity and cohesion have been found to have associations with resilience. In a study of
adult survivors of childhood sexual abuse, individuals categorized as having resilience were
more likely to have experienced a less stressful family environment, fewer family disruptions
(e.g., death, divorce), and more stable and cohesive family relations (Liem et al., 1997). Further,
in the National Longitudinal Study on Adolescent Health, 12,118, seventh- through twelfth-grade
adolescents were interviewed to identify risk and protective factors in relation to their emotional
health, violence, substance use, and sexuality (Resnick et al., 1997). Parent and family
connectedness was found to be a primary protective factor in relation to almost every health risk
behavior studied, with the exception of teenage pregnancy. As previously reported, a primary
focus of the current study is the familial factor of a positive family environment in youth.
Parenting quality and the strength of the parent-child relationship has also consistently
been found to contribute to social competence (Masten et al., 1999). Further, positive role-
15
models and a safe and stable environment encourage children’s social competence and their
pursuit of additional skills (Brown, Kallivayalil, Mendelsohn, & Harvey, 2012). All individuals
showing resilience in the Kauai Longitudinal Study reported at least one person in their life that
emphasizes the “ordinary magic” of resilience (Masten, 2001, p. 227). Human growth and
adaptation normally include processes that strengthen it. However ordinary, these processes are
vulnerable to assaults from potentially traumatic events, which disrupt healthy regulation.
resilience” (Bonanno, 2008, p.110). Although it is difficult to quantify “normal,” much resilience
research suggests that recovery from stress and traumatic experiences indicate mental health and
resilience is the more typical path. In reviewing the state of resilience research to date, Luthar et
al., (2000) suggest that it is yet too early to merge the concepts of “resilience” and “positive
adjustment.” However, as research continues to identify and refine protective and risk factors
along developmental pathways, we may find less need to focus on and refine the construct of
resilience itself and simply understand that there are unlimited points along an individual’s
In the 1940’s, John Bowlby developed a theory of human protection and survival that is still
widely researched and supported. Since that time, its application and implications have continued
to be expanded upon in the literature (Bretherton, 1992). Bowlby’s attachment theory (1951)
described a set of innate behaviors of humans, mainly focused in infancy and childhood, which
operate to establish proximity to caregivers in order to assure protection from danger. This
16
biological aspects that interact in order to accomplish three main functions of an attachment
relationship: proximity maintenance, a feeling of having a safe haven, and establishing a secure
to the theory began in 1953 (Bretherton, 1992). Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, and Wall (1978)
described the attachment system as a set of stable behaviors that are concerned with
reproduction, care, and protection of young. A child’s attachment behavior focuses on achieving
and maintaining close proximity to other people. It is developmentally typical that by the child’s
sixth or seventh month of life, these behaviors are primarily directed toward one person, the
primary caregiver (Ainsworth et al., 1978; Hazan & Shaver, 1994). Complementary behavior
that has the same protection and survival function is activated in the caregiver (Ainsworth et al.).
When the caregiver is supportive and caring in response, the child is able to achieve a state of felt
security that then allows the activation and use of other behavioral systems (Hazan & Shaver). A
caregiver’s inconsistent, unavailable, and unreliable responses result in the child experiencing a
sense of insecurity.
Bowlby (1969; 1973) and Ainsworth et al. (1978) explained that the role of affect and
emotion within the attachment system is to evaluate, appraise, and interpret environmental
ability to respond to dangerous situations automatically and to recognize certain danger cues
without having to learn them. Bowlby (1973) indicated that humans’ innate danger cues include:
unusual or strange situations, sudden changes in environment, being alone, and having others
rapidly approach. The emotional appraisal of these cues then activates the attachment system,
causing behavior meant to bring the caregiver close. When more than one danger cue is present,
17
or when a person is tired or ill (Feeney & Collins, 2001), the individual is likely to respond with
Interference with the primary goal of proximity is likely to result in anxiety and protest.
Emotional reactions meant to bring a caregiver close are predictable and most often occur in a
particular sequence (Hazan & Shaver, 1994). Protest occurs first, which may include crying,
searching, and resistance to comfort from anyone other than the primary caregiver. If these
actions do not bring the caregiver closer, despair, passivity, and sadness are likely to follow, with
emotional detachment being the final stage if the child’s attempts result in failure. Bowlby
regarded these responses as highly adaptive (Hazan & Shaver). Other signaling behavior of
human infants and children includes calling, smiling, and when the child is old enough, crawling
The critical aspect of proximity is not just the simple presence of the caregiver, but their
availability and responsiveness, even when separation occurs (Ainsworth et al., 1978). When a
child develops a sense of security in the caregiver’s reliability, attachment behavior decreases
and is less likely to occur with short periods of separation. Ainsworth et al. refer to this low level
of attachment behavior activation as “using the mother as a secure base from which to explore”
(p. 22). Additional factors that may contribute to a child’s attachment security include his or her
own temperament and the sensitivity of the caregiver’s responsiveness (Hazan & Shaver, 1994;
Pederson & Moran, 1999). When a child is able to use the primary caregiver as a secure base, he
If a child experiences positive separation from and reunion with his caregiver, these
experiences help him to build a positive internal working model of his attachment figure
(Ainsworth et al., 1978). This representational model allows the attachment bond to be
18
maintained while the child is able to accept longer periods of absence without serious distress.
Although these inner representations cannot replace proximity or contact with the caregiver,
much research is based on the presumption that they are carried into adulthood and influence
relationships throughout the lifespan (Roisman, Madsen, Henninghausen, Sroufe, & Collins,
2001).
Narrowly defining Bowlby’s theory to include only the physical proximity of child and
al., 1978). In defense of broadly defining and applying attachment theory, Pederson and Moran
(1999) explain that the attachment system could also function to establish a stable relationship in
which to develop social skills. The authors interpret Bowlby’s statements that attachment
functions include comfort and assistance as support for expanding on the theory’s
conceptualization of child and caregiver relations. They argue that the socialization process of
the human child during its lengthy period of dependency would confer survival and reproductive
advantage.
Childhood Attachment
Children are thought to develop different patterns of attachment organization based on their
experiences with their primary caregivers (Ainsworth et al., 1978). When these experiences are
positive, and the caregiver is accessible and responsive, the social development of the child will
follow a “normal course” (Ainsworth et al.; p. 9). In their recommendation to use attachment
theory as an organizational framework for research on other important close relationships, Hazan
and Shaver (1994) boil the attachment system conclusion down to a single question: “Can I
count on my attachment figure to be available and responsive when needed?” (p. 5). The authors
indicate that there are three possible answers to this question: yes, no, and maybe.
19
These patterns of caregiver behavior lead to three primary attachment organization styles in
the child (Hazan & Shaver, 1994). Where the caregiver is consistently available and responsive,
the child’s attachment style is said to be secure. When the caregiver is consistently unavailable
disorganized/disoriented has been recognized when one pattern cannot be specifically identified
or the child’s attachment has become confused due to caregiver pathology or interruptions in
caregiving relationships.
These attachment styles affect the development of the internal working models applied to
future relationships (Paley, Cox, Burchinal, & Payne, 1999). They influence individuals’ beliefs
regarding their own self-worth and schemas of how others will respond to their needs. As would
reliable. Self-worth is high. When an insecure model develops, others are expected to be
unavailable, rejecting, or inconsistently available and self-worth is low. Although more research
is needed in this area, the impact of attachment styles within various aspects of adult
relationships is a diverse and growing field of research. Attachment as a construct has continued
to broaden since its inception and has grown to include different types of social relationships and
Adult Attachment
The impact of early attachment relationships and the resulting internal working models
have been shown to influence an individual’s functioning in close relationships and across the
lifespan (Barry & Lawrence, 2013; Paley et al., 1999). Furthermore, overall romantic
20
safety and security needs (Hazan & Shaver, 1994). For these reasons, attachment theory provides
a useful framework for researching and understanding social relationship processes, experiences,
and well-being.
comfort, care, support, and intimacy to those in the relationship (Barry & Lawrence, 2013).
These needs become more salient during times of illness, stress, or danger. For the human
animal, close relationships are of utmost importance and one of the primary indicators of
psychological health and well-being throughout all life stages (Hazan & Shaver, 1994). Parents,
or other primary caregivers, are our first social relationships. While their position as primary
attachment relations and models for our first internal representations of relationship are not
typically given up completely, their roles shift to more peer-like as we become adults (Hazan &
Shaver). Romantic relationships often take their place in meeting primary attachment needs.
The expansion of attachment theory to adulthood and extended social relationships allows
for the exploration of the processes that motivate humans to establish and sustain attachment
bonds with significant others. Within these reciprocal relationships, research indicates that
meaning and experience can be effectively evaluated within the attachment framework
(Birnbaum et al., 2006). Makinen and Johnson (2006) refer to the attachment bond as, “an active,
affectionate, reciprocal relationship in which partners mutually derive and provide closeness,
comfort, and security” (p. 1055). Adults typically rely primarily on their romantic partner as their
main, and often most important, source of comfort and care (Feeney & Collins, 2001). Even in
these most important and intimate relations, where individuals feel willing and able to express
their innermost thoughts and feelings, “expression of inner states” (p. 515) need to be monitored
and regulated to some degree in order to maintain equilibrium within the relationship (Ben-
21
Naim, Hirschberger, Ein-Dor, & Mikulincer, 2013). Intimate and close relations result in
much like in caregiver-child relationships, Hazan and Shaver’s (1994) bottom line attachment
question becomes, “Can I trust my partner to be available and responsive to my needs?” (p. 13).
attachment; the two dimensions being anxiety and avoidance (Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998:
Fraley & Waller, 1998). Attachment anxiety is characterized by patterns of intensive effort in
seeking close proximity, as well as hypersensitivity and focused attention around relational
as well as denial of attachment needs and vulnerability (Mikulincer & Florian, 1998). Lower
levels of both dimensions are indicative of more secure and healthier attachment (Schachner,
Shaver, & Mikulincer, 2005). Attachment styles are believed to remain relatively stable as
developed in childhood, into and through adulthood (Bowlby, 1988). There are, of course,
instances when this is not the case (e.g., earned security), however, generally speaking, adult
attachment orientations can be thought of as “chronic interpersonal styles” (Feeney & Collins,
2001, p. 973) reflecting patterns of expectations, emotions, and behaviors regarding the self and
relationships with others (Birnbaum, Reis, Mikulincer, Gillath, & Orpaz, 2006).
One of the most widely used and well-validated self-report measures of adult attachment, the
Experiences in Close Relationships-Revised scale (ECR-R; Fraley, Waller, & Brennan 2000),
measures individuals on two subscales of attachment: avoidance and anxiety. The measure
provides an average score for both attachment-related avoidance and for anxiety. In general,
22
avoidant individuals find discomfort with intimacy and seek independence, while anxious
individuals tend to fear rejection and abandonment (Fraley, Waller, & Brennan, 2000).
typically seen as the gold standard in the field (Roisman et al., 2007). However, the four
attachment orientations are achieved by measuring adults’ placement on the two fundamental
dimensions of anxiety and avoidance (Feeney & Collins, 2001). Securely attached adults are low
on both the anxiety and the avoidance dimensions. Insecurely attached adults are high on either
the anxiety or avoidance dimensions, or both. Dismissing (avoidant) individuals are low in
anxiety and high in avoidance. Those who are fearfully avoidant report high anxiety and high
avoidance. Finally, preoccupied (anxious) adults are high in anxiety and low in avoidance.
Researchers working to use dimensional models argue that statistical power and precision of
measurement are compromised when cut-points are arbitrarily determined (Roisman et al.,
stability longitudinally. Roisman et al. argue that the differentiations made by the well-known
and frequently used Adult Attachment Interview match more closely with a dimensional model
and that using the anxiety and avoidance dimensions may offer a better understanding of
attachment security than the categorizations that are currently used. The authors suggest that the
same research questions could be addressed with increased statistical power and greater insight,
23
A Note on Continuous-Secure versus Earned-Secure Attachment
Yet another topic of disagreement that is found within the attachment literature is the
individuals convey a generally positive state of mind regarding positive experiences throughout
their childhood (Paley et al., 1999). Earned-secure individuals communicate stories of difficult
childhoods in a generally positive and realistic way that indicates they are not likely controlled
by these experiences. Although Paley et al. indicate differences regarding the two divisions of
secure attachment, results of their study on marital functioning indicated that current state of
mind regarding childhood experiences was more likely related to future relationship problems
than the experiences themselves. This does not support the need for a distinction. Furthermore,
Roisman, Fortuna, and Holland (2006) conducted a study that manipulated mood in order to
compare earned and continuous attachment security. Their results indicated that categorization of
earned-secure versus continuous-secure was altered with a simple mood induction exercise,
while categorization of the over-arching categories of secure versus insecure was not. While this
research is needed.
Current Project
The purpose of the current research is to contribute knowledge regarding the experiences
of potentially traumatic events (PTEs), as well as factors that may insulate against complex
trauma symptomology. While it is generally accepted that a positive family environment in youth
and a securely developed attachment style contribute to resilience and are considered protective
factors, the literature has not yet evaluated these factors in relation to complex trauma
experiences and the development of C-PTSD. Therefore, this study aims to provide unique
24
information regarding the potentially moderating effects of a positive family environment in
youth and a securely developed attachment style on the relationship between PTEs and the
Further, C-PTSD is currently being considered a separate, yet related, disorder from the
well-established PTSD. The most recent research has supported a distinct display of symptoms
for those individuals who have experienced prolonged and interpersonal violence (e.g.,
childhood abuse, domestic violence, being a prisoner of war). Using the ICD-11 models of PTSD
and C-PTSD will be associated with differing profiles of those individuals who endorse
symptoms. Current review suggests that the ICD-11 model, using the ITQ measure, has only
begun to be evaluated and has not yet been used in a college sample. The current study
evaluating this categorization will also contribute knowledge to the growing literature on
complex trauma.
Youth with trauma histories are a difficult population with whom to conduct research due
to their dependent status, reliance on caregivers or other family members, their hesitancy to
report on illegal or unsafe traumatic experiences, and their overall ‘vulnerable’ status (Campbell,
Greeson, & Fehler-Cabral, 2014; McDonald, 2015). As such, an undergraduate college sample is
seemingly the ideal sample to query; the majority being near the end of their childhood
development, they are more likely of an age young enough to be competent reporters of recent
experiences and relations, yet less dependent on caregivers and therefore more likely to report on
negative traumas that may have occurred in or related to the family framework.
The primary objective of the current study is to contribute additional understanding to the
ongoing development and knowledge of complex trauma and resulting C-PTSD symptomology,
25
as well as to evaluate whether the factors of family environment and attachment account for
The current study uses a non-clinical sample of college students in order to determine
qualitatively different groups or classes of participants, using the only self-report symptom scale
currently available that is designed to measure C-PTSD classification as defined by the ICD-11
(International Trauma Questionnaire for ICD-11; ITQ; Cloitre, Roberts, Bisson, & Brewin,
2014). While identifying trauma symptoms in a non-clinical college population sample, this
study will contribute to the ongoing evaluation of the three-tier classification of the International
Trauma Questionnaire (ITQ). Research has demonstrated that participants would belong in one
of three categories that include (a) those with low or no symptom endorsement, (b) those who
endorse symptoms indicating they meet criteria for PTSD, and (c) those who endorse symptoms
indicating they meet criteria for C-PTSD. Because this is a community sample and not a clinical
population, it was important to assess the degree to which the population has clinical symptoms.
Those individuals who are thought to have built resilience have been identified by
research to be resistant to the development of mental health diagnoses (D’Andrea et al., 2012;
Sroufe, 1997). As described above, factors that contribute to the development of resilience
include individual, familial, and community factors (Luthar et al., 2000; Punamäki, Qouta,
Miller, & El-Sarraj, 2011; Rutter, 1999; Werner, 2000). The current study investigated whether
participants who reported having (1) a more positive family environment in youth and (2) secure
attachment were less likely related to those who developed trauma symptomology, and more
26
Specifically, it was predicted that:
1. Trauma Symptomology:
reported on the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) and the Life Events
Checklist for DSM-5 (LEC-5), and their ITQ-determined group belonging (non-
2. Family Environment:
the Family Environment Scale (FES) will result in and discriminate between three
group.
3. Attachment:
27
Experiences in Close Relationships – Revised (ECR-R) will result in significant
relational subscales of the Family Environment Scale (FES), and (2) more secure
Method
Participants
Participants are undergraduate students enrolled at The University of Montana during the
Fall 2018 and Spring 2019 semesters, who participated voluntarily for research credit in their
psychology or related courses. As expected, the study sample was predominantly freshman and
sophomore; there was no exclusion criteria for age or class standing. Current research into
various aspects of complex trauma has primarily evaluated participants seeking or participating
in treatment, or those known to have experienced trauma. This study aims to contribute to the
family environment in youth and a securely developed attachment style as potentially protective
factors in relation to C-PTSD symptomology. Further, current research does not include using
28
A total of 469 college students participated in the study. Data from 30 respondents was
discarded due to excessive missing data (more than 10%). In addition, 13.5% of participants had
one or more missing data points (but missing less than 5%) for one or more of the assessment
measures. Missing data points on individual measures were replaced with respondents’ mean
scores, based on completed items from that particular scale. Power analyses (Faul, Erdfelder,
Buchner, & Lang, 2009) conducted prior to data collection revealed a sample size of 336 would
provide sufficient power for the purposes of this study. Certain statistical analyses used in the
were made to the analyses and are specifically addressed in those sections. Participants in the
remaining sample (n = 439) ranged in age from 18 to 66 years old (mean age = 21.90 years, SD
= 6.48). The majority reported freshman class standing (46.7%), with a minority sophomore
(21.6%), junior (15.9%), senior (14.1%), and graduate (0.2%). Six participants (1.4%) were
unsure of their class standing. Participants consisted predominantly of persons who identified as
(5%), being Native American/Alaska Native (3.9%), Hispanic (3%), Asian/Pacific Islander
(2.7%), and Black (1.1%). More than 90% of participants reported having an adult attachment
figure who loved and supported them, and with whom they felt close. The majority of
respondents identified their mother or step-mother as that person (37%). Participants also
identified their father or step-father (30%) or both parents (12%) as primary attachment figures.
When comparing participants’ group belonging (non-clinical, PTSD, and C-PTSD), groups did
not differ significantly in age F(2) = 0.762, p > .05, class standing F(2) = 2.659, p > .05, or
29
racial/ethnic composition F(2) = .616, p > .05. There were significant differences between the
groups in whether or not they reported having a primary attachment figure F(2) = 0.494, p =
reported in Table 1.
Measures
assessment, diagnoses, and treatment. However, for the purpose of the current study, self-report
surveys were administered in consideration of both time and expense. Further, it has been
reported that individuals may be more comfortable and therefore more forthcoming, when
responding to a questionnaire than to another individual (Nader, 2008). All self-report measures
(see Appendix B). Participants were asked to provide their age, class standing, ethnicity, current
regarding the relationship between complex trauma and potentially traumatic events (PTEs)
experienced by participants, these events were measured by both a childhood and a lifetime
experience measure. Further, as explained previously, cumulative trauma has been most
has experienced (Briere, Hodges, & Godbout, 2010; Cloitre et al.). PTEs will be measured by
adding the total number of distinct types of traumatic experiences from the following two
30
Table 1
Ethnicity (%)
White 84 82 4.9 13.1
Class Standing
Freshman 46.7 80 5.9 14.1
Note. Mean age and standard deviation of total sample and participants by ITQ-categorized trauma group
belonging. Percentage of total sample and participants by ITQ-categorized trauma group belonging.
______________________________________________________________________________
31
The Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ: Bernstein and Fink, 1997; see Appendix C)
specific to childhood. The CTQ includes five subscales: Emotional Abuse, Physical Abuse,
Sexual Abuse, Emotional Neglect, and Physical Neglect. Participants are asked to report their
experiences “growing up as a child and as a teenager” within five domains using a 5-point scale
ranging from 1 (never true) to 5 (very often true) regarding the frequency the event was
experienced. Bernstein and Fink also provide interpretive guidelines to allow the identification of
likely cases of abuse and neglect for three levels of severity: low, moderate, and severe.
Bernstein et al. (2003) utilized four samples (clinical and nonclinical, N = 1978) to examine
internal consistency/reliability and found the following coefficient alpha ranges: .84-.89 for
emotional abuse, .81 to .86 for physical abuse, .92 to .95 for sexual abuse, .85 to .91 for
emotional neglect, and .61 to .78 for physical neglect. In addition, Bernstein et al. provided
results suggesting good validity, as evidenced by the measurement invariance of the scale across
four diverse populations, as well as the criterion-related validity of corroborative data between
therapists’ ratings of abuse and neglect and participants’ responses. The CTQ also has three
The Life Events Checklist for DSM-5 (LEC-5; Weathers et al., 2013; see Appendix D) is
a 17-item self-report measure designed to assess for exposure to potentially traumatic events
(PTEs) in a participants’ lifetime. The events referenced in the LEC-5 do not include childhood
abuse or neglect, but do include other traumatic events that may have occurred during childhood.
The measure prompts participants to rate their exposure to 16 events known to potentially result
32
substance, sexual assault, combat, captivity, illness/injury/human suffering, violent or accidental
death, and/or harm caused by respondent. One additional item assesses exposure to “Any other
very stressful event or experience,” which is used to capture experiences not listed. Participants
respond to each item (e.g., Serious accident at work, home, or during recreational activity), by
selecting one of six responses: “happened to me”, “witnessed it”, “learned about it”, “part of my
job”, “not sure”, and “does not apply.” The LEC-5 is a recent revision of the Life Events
Checklist for DSM-IV (Gray, Litz, Hsu, & Lombardo, 2004). Psychometric characteristics for
the LEC-5 are not yet available. Because there are minimal changes between the versions, LEC-5
(Weathers et al., 2013). The LEC has demonstrated strong convergence with measures of
psychopathology that are known to be associated with trauma exposure. Changes to the new
measure include addition of the “Part of my job” response option, and a wording change to one
of the items. The LEC does not produce a total score, rather it yields a total number of PTEs
Potentially protective factors. The current study evaluated both the experience of a
positive family environment in youth and a securely developed attachment style as potentially
protective factors in the development of trauma symptomology, and more specifically the
Relationships-Revised scale (ECR-R; Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998; Fraley, Waller, &
Brennan, 2000; see Appendix E). The ECR-R is a 36-item self-report measure containing two
dimensional subscales: attachment anxiety (defined as discomfort with relational closeness and
depending on others; 18 items) and attachment avoidance (fear of rejection and abandonment; 18
33
items). Participants are asked to indicate the extent to which they agree with each item on a 7-
point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree) and to respond
regarding how they feel in emotionally intimate relationships (i.e., how they “generally
experience relationships”). A third attachment variable was created by plotting the intersection of
the anxiety and avoidance dimensions on a four-quadrant graph resulting in an attachment style
Studies completed by Sibley, Fischer, and Liu (2005) demonstrated high internal
reliability (α=.93 for attachment anxiety and α=.94 for attachment avoidance), high test-retest
reliability over a 3-week period (r=.90 for attachment anxiety and r=.92 for attachment
avoidance), and an accurate fit for the hypothesized two-factor solution as examined through
confirmatory factor analysis. In addition, the ECR-R demonstrated good validity, as measured by
its association with interaction diary ratings by subjects, as well as the scales’ moderate
Horowitz, 1991; see Sibley et al., 2005). Cronbach’s Alpha for the current study was .95.
Family Environment. The Family Environment Scale (FES; Moos & Moos, 1981; see
measure the social and environmental characteristics of a family. The FES is useful for
understanding how family members perceive the family and how each member’s behavior
affects the family unit during a time of crisis or transition. For the current study, participants
were asked to respond to items in relation to their family of origin. There are three versions of
the FES: the ‘Real Form,’ which measures participants’ perceptions of their family environment;
the ‘Ideal Form,’ which measures how participants would conceptualize the ideal family
environment; and the ‘Expectations Form,’ which measures participants’ expectations of what
34
their future family will be like. For the purposes of the current research, only the ‘Real Form’
was used.
Scores load on three primary scales that include the Relationship, System Maintenance,
and Personal Growth Scales. The Relationship Scale is comprised of three subscales: (1)
expressiveness, (2) conflict, and (3) cohesion. Items include questions about the extent of help,
support, and commitment family members have for one another. These scales also assess the
degree that family members are able to express their feelings directly, act openly, and openly
express aggression, conflict, and anger within the family environment. The System Maintenance
Scale is comprised of two subscales: (4) control and (5) organization. These subscales measure
clear organization and structure in family planning and the degree to which set rules and
procedures are used by the family. The Personal Growth Scale includes: (6) achievement
cultural orientation; and the (10) independence subscales all assess the Personal Growth
dimensions. The degree that members of the family are self-sufficient, assertive, and make their
own decisions is assessed by these subscales, as is the extent that activities are placed into a
cultural, and intellectual activities; the degree of importance placed on religious and ethical
issues; and the degree of involvement in recreational and social activities. According to the
authors, the Relationship and System Maintenance dimensions primarily reflect internal
functioning and the Personal Growth dimension primarily reflects relations between the family
and social or community contexts. For this reason, and for the purposes of this study, only the
five subscales of the Relationship and System Maintenance dimensions will be evaluated: (1)
Cohesion, (2) Expressiveness, (3) Conflict, (4) Organization, and (5) Control.
35
Table 2
Percentage of Participants Who Reported Potentially Traumatic Events in the Total Sample and
______________________________________________________________________________
Childhood
36
Physical Assault 170 38.7 75.9 5.3 18.8
Note. Sample sizes and percentage of populations listed were determined by hand categorizing seventy-
nine participants who had reported traumatic experiences in the other category. Percentages of PTEs
categorized by ITQ group belonging were determined by SPSS with other category as self-reported.
37
The answer sheet used to score the questionnaire is arranged so that each column of
responses comprises an FES subscale. The subscale raw scores of each participant are
determined by summing the number of responses provided in each column. The total raw score
was determined by summing the total number of responses across the columns. Raw scores were
The FES normative sample for the ‘Real Form’ subscales was based on 1,125 non-
distressed and 500 distressed families. When compared to non-distressed families, distressed
families were lower on cohesion and expressiveness and higher on conflict and control (Moos &
Moos, 1981). For each of the five relevant FES subscales, Cronbach’s alpha fell within an
acceptable range (varying from a high of .78 for the cohesion subscale, to a low of .67 for the
control subscale), indicating an adequate amount of internal consistency for the subscales. Test-
retest reliability for all subscales was calculated using data from 47 individuals who responded to
the ‘Real Form’ twice, with an eight-week interval between pre- and post-test responses. Test-
retest reliability was found to be within an acceptable range, varying from a low of .73 for the
expressiveness subscale to a high of .86 for the cohesion subscale. Cronbach’s Alpha for the
current study ranged from a modest low of .62 for the cohesion subscale to a high of .82 for the
Bisson, & Brewin, 2017; Appendix G) is a self-report measure that was developed for the
assessment of ICD-11 PTSD and C-PTSD diagnoses. The ITQ results in a three-tier
classification that identifies participants’ group belonging in one of three categories that include
(1) those with low or no symptom endorsement (i.e., non-clinical), (2) those who endorse
symptoms indicating they meet criteria for PTSD, and (3) those who endorse symptoms
38
indicating they meet criteria for C-PTSD. As mentioned previously, there is overlap in these
emotional dysregulation distinguishes C-PTSD from PTSD. The ITQ categorization of group
Previously referred to as the ICD-11 Trauma Questionnaire (ICD-TQ), the ITQ is a 20-
item self-report measure with nine PTSD and nine Disturbances in Self-Organization (DSO)
items. Three items are used to measure Re-experiencing (RE; items P1–P3), two items to
measure avoidance (AV; items P4–P5), and two items to measure Sense of Threat (Th; items P6–
P7). CPTSD includes PTSD as well as three clusters reflecting disturbances in self-organization
(DSO). Nine items represent the three DSO clusters of Affective Dysregulation (AD; items C1–
C2), Negative Self-Concept (NSC; items C3–C4), and Disturbances in Relationships (DR; items
C5–C6). Symptom endorsement is scored on a Likert scale, indicating how much a symptom has
been bothersome in the past month, with scores ranging from 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely). The
PTSD items are answered in response to the question ‘how much have you been bothered by that
problem for the past month?’ and the DSO items are answered in terms of how one ‘typically
feels, thinks about themselves, or relates to others.’ A diagnosis of PTSD requires that: (i) an
individual has experienced a traumatic event, (ii) indicates the presence of at least one symptom
in each of its three clusters (as indicated by a score of ≥ 2 on the Likert scale – ‘Moderately’),
and (iii) indicates functional impairment associated with these symptoms. A diagnosis of C-
PTSD requires that: (i) PTSD criteria are met, (ii) indicates the presence of at least one symptom
in each of the three DSO clusters (as indicated by a score of ≥ 2 on the Likert scale –
‘Moderately’), and (iii) indicates functional impairment associated with these symptoms.
39
In an evaluation of this measure and its distinct symptom profiles, Karatzias et al. (2017)
found evidence to support the measure. Reported Cronbach’s alpha was high for the DSO
indicators (AD = .79, NSC = .91, and DR = .83) and was modest for the PTSD indicators (RE =
.55, AV = .63, and Th = .78). Cronbach’s Alpha for the current study was high for both the DSO
indicators (AD = .60, NSC = .88, and DR = .80) and the PTSD indicators (RE = .80, AV = .78,
and Th = .78).
Procedure
This study was conducted in accordance with the code of conduct of the American
Psychological Association and was submitted for approval from the Institutional Review Board
of the University of Montana prior to data collection. A description of the survey was posted on
an online psychology research board and participants completed an online anonymous survey.
When participants entered the online survey site, they were presented with a study description
that explained their participation and explicitly stated they were free to elect not to complete the
survey or to skip any question. The study description also included contact information for the
University of Montana’s Counseling Services in the event they wished to talk regarding any
stress due to study participation (see Appendix A). After completing the survey, participants
were directed to a separate website where they received extra credit for their participation;
Data from a total of 439 participants with acceptable levels of data was evaluated and is
reported on in the current study. Internal consistency was calculated for all the measures and, as
reported in the Method section, all alphas were found to be acceptable. All statistical analyses
40
were carried out with IBM’s Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) for Mac, version
Results
Psychological symptomology
The International Trauma Questionnaire (ITQ; Cloitre, Roberts, Bisson, & Brewin, 2017)
categorized participants into three groups based on trauma symptomology: (1) non-clinical (n =
357), (2) meeting criteria for PTSD (n = 23), and (3) meeting criteria for C-PTSD (n = 59). The
current C-PTSD diagnosis consists of six symptoms clusters that include the three PTSD criteria
negative self-concept.
These group categorizations were used in each of the following hypotheses. A priori
power analyses for proposed statistical methods indicated that overall, 336 participants were
needed and between 59-168 were needed per ITQ-determined group belonging for adequate
power. Although total sample size (N = 439) was sufficient, group belonging did not have
adequate sample size. Despite being under-powered, this study resulted in numerous significant
findings.
Participants reported childhood abuse experience through the CTQ (Bernstein & Fink,
1997), which measures emotional, physical, and sexual abuse, as well as emotional and physical
neglect. Bernstein and Fink offer guidelines for classifying varying abuse and neglect
experiences into four categories: (1) none to minimal, (2) low to moderate, (3) moderate to
severe, and (4) severe to extreme. For the purpose of this study, participants were dichotomized
41
into two groups for each of the five categories of abuse and neglect. For each category,
participants with scores in the none to moderate range were classified as not having endorsed
that PTE. Participants in the moderate to extreme range were classified as having endorsed that
those who did experience abuse/neglect in childhood, emotional abuse was the most commonly
endorsed (10.5%). Ten percent endorsed sexual abuse, 5.9% endorsed physical abuse, 4.3%
Participants reported other lifetime potentially traumatic events (PTEs) on the Life
Events Checklist for DSM-5 (LEC-5; Weathers et al., 2013). This measure did not include
childhood abuse. Every category of PTE listed on the LEC-5 was endorsed. The most commonly
experienced event was having been in a transportation accident (62.4%), the sudden and
unexpected death of someone close (61%), and a life-threatening illness or injury (41.7%). A
significant number of participants also reported an experience of physical assault (38.7%), and
accident (32.8%). Other categories frequently endorsed included having experienced a natural
When comparing potentially traumatic events in the overall sample, the Native American
population stands out. While making up only 3.9% of the total sample size, Native Americans
represented almost 12% of those who endorsed symptom criteria for PTSD and 47% of those
who endorsed C-PTSD symptomology. This is a salient illustration of the increased vulnerability
In order to examine lifetime reports of participants’ PTEs, scores for both the CTQ and
the LEC-5 were combined to obtain a total number of potential traumatic events experienced.
42
When these reports were totaled, only 6.6% of respondents had not experienced PTEs in their
lifetime. The most common number of PTEs experienced were two or three (12.3%,
respectively), with 11.8% of participants endorsing five PTEs, 11.2% endorsing four PTEs,
10.7% endorsing one, 10.3% endorsing six, 6.8% endorsing seven, and 5.7% endorsing eight
PTEs. Just over 12% of participants endorsed nine or more PTEs. See Table 2 for specific
information on PTEs.
Briere et al. (2008) warn that cumulative trauma and symptom complexity variables are
unlikely to be normally distributed, recommending that statistical analyses used to evaluate this
data is resistant to normality violations. Hayes and Rockwood (2017) debunk this as “myth” and
indicate that centering independent and dependent variables will not affect interaction tests, but
simply change the metric of measurement. Because there are conflicting ideas on how normalcy
of data distribution influences results in relation to trauma, this assumption was given particular
consideration. Non-parametric models were used to evaluate data that violated normality
assumptions.
experienced, as reported on the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) and the Life
Events Checklist for DSM-5 (LEC-5), and their ITQ-determined group belonging (non-
Due to the fact that the dependent variable is a positive integer that fits the Poisson
distribution, Poisson regression was used to test this association. Two separate regressions were
conducted due to complications resulting from combining childhood and lifetime experiences
(i.e., total number of PTEs). Thus, the dependent variables in these analyses were the number of
PTEs experienced by participants (1) related to abuse in childhood and (2) all other traumatic
43
events throughout their lifetime. The independent variables were participants’ group belonging
Both the likelihood ratio chi-square test (χ2 = 39.719, DF = 2, p < .0001) and the
deviance-based goodness of fit (D = 460.4361, DF = 436, D/DF = 1.06) indicated that the full
model using the CTQ childhood PTEs measure was a significant improvement in fit over a null
model (i.e., no predictors). Though belonging to the PTSD group was not a significant predictor
of the number of experienced PTEs, the incidence rate ratio (1.744) indicated that for those in the
PTSD group, the incidence rate for childhood PTEs was 1.74 times greater than that for the non-
clinical group. In other words, the incidence rate for those with PTSD was 74.4% greater than
that for the non-clinical group. Belonging to the C-PTSD group was a significant predictor of the
number of experienced PTEs (b = 1.203, S.E. = .1779, p < .0001). The incidence rate ratio
(3.331) indicated that for those in the C-PTSD group, the incidence rate for childhood PTEs was
3.33 times greater than that for the non-clinical group. In other words, the incidence rate for
those with C-PTSD was 233% greater than that for the non-clinical group.
Although the likelihood ratio chi-square test (χ2 = 38.796, DF = 2, p < .0001) indicated
that the full model using the LEC lifetime PTEs measure was a significant improvement in fit
over a null model (i.e., no predictors), the deviance-based goodness of fit (D = 982.892, DF =
436, D/DF = 2.254) did not. The deviance-based test provided a better result because it indicated
how well outcomes met assumptions that the outcome is a positive integer, and that the mean and
Belonging to the PTSD group was not a significant predictor of the number of
experienced PTEs (b = 0.185, S.E. = .0972, p = .057). The incidence rate ratio (1.203) indicated
that for those in the PTSD group, the incidence rate for lifetime PTEs was 1.203 times greater
44
than that for the non-clinical group. In other words, the incidence rate for those with PTSD was
only 20.3% greater than that for the non-clinical group. Belonging to the C-PTSD group was
found to be a significant predictor of the number of experienced PTEs (b = 0.376, S.E. = .0592, p
< .0001). Further, the incidence rate ratio (1.456) indicated that for those in the C-PTSD group,
the incidence rate for lifetime PTEs was 1.456 times greater than that for the non-clinical group.
In other words, the incidence rate for those with C-PTSD was 45.6% greater than that for the
Family Environment
Hypothesis 2: A more positive family environment, as reported on the FES, will result in
less trauma symptomology, as reported on the ITQ: non-clinical, PTSD, and C-PTSD.
In order to evaluate this relationship, it was proposed that five separate between-subjects
one-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) would be conducted. While meeting criteria for the
assumption of homogeneity of variance, all collected FES subscale data distributions violated
assumptions of normality. For this reason, the Kruskal-Wallis H test for non-parametric data was
used instead. The dependent variables in these analyses were participants’ scores on the five FES
variable was participants’ ITQ group belonging (0 = non-clinical, 1 = PTSD, 2 = C-PTSD). FES
subscales were examined for multicollinearity and found to be independent; tolerance scores
ranged between 0.524 – 0.790 and VIF scores ranged between 1.27 – 1.91. Family environment
45
Table 3
____________________________________________________________________________________________________________
Lifetime Trauma
Note. Hypothesis 1. Potentially traumatic event (childhood, lifetime) variables evaluating variance in relation to group belonging (non-
** p < .01.
___________________________________________________________________________________________________________
46
Cohesion Subscale
Cohesion subscale scores met criteria for homogeneity of variance according to Levene’s
test, F(2,436) = .212, p = .809. However, Shapiro-Wilk’s (W(439) = .914, p < .000) indicated
that data were not normally distributed. A Kruskal-Wallis H test showed that there was a
statistically significant difference among the three ITQ group belonging categorizations on FES
cohesion subscale scores, H (2) = 7.428, p = .024. The family cohesion mean rank was 224.93
for the non-clinical sample, 245.26 for those who met criteria for PTSD, and 180.32 for those
who met criteria for C-PTSD. Dunn’s pairwise tests with Bonferroni corrections were conducted
to make post hoc comparisons. The participants in the C-PTSD group (M = 37.64, SD = 14.77)
scored significantly lower than the non-clinical group (M = 42.66, SD = 13.79). There was no
evidence of a significant difference between other groups. Effect size was small (!2 = 0.017).
Expressiveness Subscale
Levene’s test, F(2,436) = .109, p = .897. However, Shapiro-Wilk’s (W(439) = .956, p < .000)
indicated that data were not normally distributed. A Kruskal-Wallis H test showed that there was
a statistically significant difference among the three ITQ group belonging categorizations on FES
expressiveness subscale scores, H (2) = 20.157, p < .000. The family expressiveness mean rank
for the non-clinical sample was 231.99, 204.85 for those who met criteria for PTSD, and 153.34
for those who met criteria for C-PTSD. Dunn’s pairwise tests with Bonferroni corrections were
conducted to make post hoc comparisons. The participants in the C-PTSD group (M = 40.34, SD
= 13.77) scored significantly lower than the non-clinical group (M = 49.18, SD = 13.58). There
was no evidence of a significant difference between other groups. Effect size was small (!2 =
0.046).
47
Table 4
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
Note. Hypothesis 2. Family Environment Scale subscale means, standard deviations, and
______________________________________________________________________________
48
Conflict Subscale
Conflict subscale scores met criteria for homogeneity of variance according to Levene’s
test, F(2,436) = .796, p = .452. However, Shapiro-Wilk’s (W(439) = .930, p < .000) indicated
that data were not normally distributed. A Kruskal-Wallis H test showed that there was a
statistically significant difference among the three ITQ group belonging categorizations on FES
conflict subscale scores, H (2) = 24.513, p < .000. The family conflict mean rank score was
207.44 for the non-clinical sample, 222.20 for those who met criteria for PTSD, and 295.12 for
those who met criteria for C-PTSD. Dunn’s pairwise tests with Bonferroni corrections were
conducted to make post hoc comparisons. The participants in the C-PTSD group (M = 61.29, SD
= 12.93) scored significantly higher than the non-clinical group (M = 51.36, SD = 13.90). There
was no evidence of a significant difference between other groups. Effect size was small (!2 =
0.056).
Organization Subscale
Levene’s test, F(2,436) = .622, p = .537. However, Shapiro-Wilk’s (W(439) = .957, p < .000)
indicated that data were not normally distributed. A Kruskal-Wallis H test showed that there was
not a statistically significant difference among the three ITQ group belonging categorizations on
FES Organization subscale scores, H (2) = 1.689, p = .430. The family organization mean rank
was 223.52 for the non-clinical sample, 214.48 for those who met criteria for PTSD, and 200.84
Control Subscale
Control subscale scores met criteria for homogeneity of variance according to Levene’s
test, F(2,436) = .155, p = .856. However, Shapiro-Wilk’s (W(439) = .958, p < .000) indicated
49
that data were not normally distributed. A Kruskal-Wallis H test showed that there was a
statistically significant difference among the three ITQ group belonging categorizations on FES
Control subscale scores, H (2) = 6.727, p = .035. The family conflict mean rank was 214.24 for
the non-clinical sample, 281.93 for those who met criteria for PTSD, and 230.70 for those who
met criteria for C-PTSD. Dunn’s pairwise tests with Bonferroni corrections were conducted to
make post hoc comparisons. Participants in the PTSD group (M = 58.17, SD = 12.55) scored
significantly higher than the non-clinical group (M = 51.01, SD = 13.37). There was no evidence
of a significant difference between other groups. Effect size was small (!2 = 0.015).
Attachment
the ECR-R, and their ITQ trauma symptomology categorizations: non-clinical, PTSD, and
C-PTSD.
distributions met criteria for the assumption of homogeneity of variance, while violating
Procedure) were conducted. It was also decided to explore these relationships further. For this
reason, the Kruskal-Wallis H test for non-parametric data was also used. The dependent
variables in these analyses were participants’ scores on the dimensional attachment subscale, and
the independent variable was participants’ ITQ group belonging (binary categorization for
examined for multicollinearity and found to be independent (Tolerance = 1.00, VIF = 1.00).
50
Table 5
Note. Hypotheses 3A and 3B. Experiences in Close Relationships – Revised (ECR-R) attachment
** p < .01.
______________________________________________________________________________
51
Hypothesis 3a: PTSD symptomology will be associated with higher scores on the anxiety
As previously reported, research has shown that higher levels of anxiety may be more
predictive of PTSD symptoms when compared with those of C-PTSD (Hyland, 2017). While
ECR-R anxiety scores met criteria for homogeneity of variance according to Levene’s test,
F(1,378) = 2.762, p = .097, Shapiro-Wilk’s, W(380) = .981, p < .000, indicated that data were not
normally distributed. In order to determine if the PTSD group had experienced less secure
attachment, as measured by higher mean ranks on attachment anxiety, than the non-clinical
group, a Mann-Whitney U test was conducted. The dependent variable was participants’ scores
on the anxiety dimension of the ECR-R. The independent variable was participants’ group
mean ranks for both the PTSD group and non-clinical group were similar. Results, (N = 380) U =
3361, Z = -1.458, p = .145, indicate that the participants in the PTSD group (Mean Rank =
222.87) did not have significantly different mean ranks on the ECR-R anxiety subscale than non-
The ECR-R anxiety subscale was evaluated beyond the hypotheses in order to identify
other significant relationships with group belonging. A Kruskal-Wallis H test showed that there
was a statistically significant difference among the three ITQ group belonging categorizations on
the ECR-R anxiety subscale scores, H (2) = 31.932, p < .0001. The ECR-R anxiety mean rank
was 204.66 for the non-clinical sample, 242.00 for those who met criteria for PTSD, and 304.25
for those who met criteria for C-PTSD. Dunn’s pairwise tests with Bonferroni corrections were
conducted to make post hoc comparisons. The participants in the C-PTSD group (Mean Score =
4.50, SD = 1.30) were ranked significantly higher than the non-clinical group (Mean Score =
52
3.43, SD = 1.25). There was no evidence of a significant difference between other groups. Effect
Hypothesis 3b: C-PTSD symptomology will be associated with higher scores on the
Levene’s test, F(1,414) = .012, p = .912 indicated that data met criteria for homogeneity
of variance. Shapiro-Wilk’s (W(416) = .978, p < .000) confirmed that data were not normally
distributed. In order to determine if the C-PTSD group had experienced less secure attachment,
as measured by higher scores on the avoidance dimension of the ECR-R, than the non-clinical
group, a Mann-Whitney U test was conducted. The dependent variable was participants’ scores
on the avoidance dimension of the ECR-R. The independent variable was participants’ group
avoidance scores of both the C-PTSD group and the non-clinical group differed. Results, (N =
416) U = 7412, Z = -3.647, p = .000, indicate that the participants in the C-PTSD group (Mean
Rank = 261.37) had significantly different mean ranks on the ECR-R avoidance subscale than
non-clinical participants (Mean Rank = 199.76). Effect size was small (η2 = 0.031).
The ECR-R avoidance subscale was evaluated further in order to identify other
significant relationships with group belonging. A Kruskal-Wallis H test showed that there was a
statistically significant difference among the three ITQ group belonging categorizations on the
ECR-R anxiety subscale scores, H (2) = 13.819, p = .001. The ECR-R avoidance mean rank was
209.91 for the non-clinical sample, 234.54 for those who met criteria for PTSD, and 275.41 for
those who met criteria for C-PTSD. Dunn’s pairwise tests with Bonferroni corrections were
conducted to make post hoc comparisons. The participants in the C-PTSD group (Mean Score =
3.61, SD = 1.17) scored significantly higher than the non-clinical group (Mean Score = 3.01, SD
53
= 1.17). There was no evidence of a significant difference between other groups. Effect size was
Hypothesis 3c: Overall attachment scores on the ECR-R, which combines the anxiety and
avoidance dimensions to create an ordinal quadrant score, will result in three distinct
Haenszel (CMH) test, using the ‘row mean scores differ’ statistic, with three degrees of freedom
was used. The CMH test determined if the distribution of data in the attachment groups was the
same or different than the ITQ-determined grouped belonging distribution. When the null
one or more of the two groups. A significant association between group belonging and
attachment groups was found (χ2 = 36.501, DF = 3, p <0.0001). When compared to both those
who were non-clinical and those with PTSD, there was a smaller proportion of participants with
C-PTSD in the securely attached group (1) and a higher proportion in the pre-occupied (3) and
fearfully avoidant (4) attachment groups. Further, proportionately almost half of the study
participants (46%) were both non-clinical and securely attached. The distribution of participants
across the four ECR-R groups was approximately the same for the non-clinical and the PTSD
Although the categorization portion of this analysis was conducted as a way to contribute
worth noting that the attachment measure used here did not allow for a category of disorganized
attachment. Disorganized attachment has been recognized when one pattern cannot be
specifically identified or the individual’s attachment has become confused due to pathology or
54
interruptions in caregiving relationships. The ability to account of these individuals in regard to
attachment style may significantly change the distribution of participants across group belonging.
Hypothesis 4: Protective factors of (1) a more positive family environment and (2) a more
secure attachment will account for significant variability in the relationship between
and secure attachment, the number of experienced PTEs, and trauma symptomology, a logistic
regression was conducted. It was originally proposed that this relationship would be evaluated
with moderation analyses; however, due to a lower sample size than anticipated, it was
determined that a binomial logistic regression would identify relationships between the data
more clearly. In this analysis, the PTSD and C-PTSD groups were combined in order to provide
appropriate power. Therefore, the dependent variable of the regression was participants’ ITQ
variables of the regression model included: (1) participants’ experienced PTEs, measured as
individual CTQ and LEC scores; (2) participants’ reported family environment, measured as FES
conflict, and control); and (3) participants’ attachment, measured as ECR-R avoidance and
anxiety dimensions. The logistic regression model was found to be a good fit, explaining 23%
(Nagelkerke R2) of the variance in trauma symptomology and correctly classifying 84.1% of the
cases. Results were statistically significant (χ2 = 68.373, DF = 8, p < 0.0001). Family cohesion,
expressiveness, and conflict, as well as attachment anxiety, and childhood trauma exposure were
each found to have significant relationships with reported trauma symptomology. There was no
55
evidence of significant relationships between family control, attachment avoidance, or lifetime
A one unit increase in an individual’s family cohesion score was associated with 1.05
times greater odds of having a clinically significant trauma symptomology (95% CI = 1.02 –
1.07). Although increased scores on family cohesion were associated with an increased
probability of belonging in the trauma symptomology group (χ2 = 12.14, DF = 1, p < 0.0001), the
increase in probability was quite small. Essentially, family cohesion has very little effect on the
overall outcome.
Family expressiveness was the only protective variable significantly associated with
individual’s family expressiveness score was associated with 0.969 times decreased odds of
having clinically significant trauma symptomology (95% CI = .95 - .99). Decreased scores on
trauma symptomology (χ2 = 6.50, DF = 1, p = .011). Again, the decrease in probability and the
clinically significant trauma symptomology (χ2 = 4.51, DF = 1, p = .034). A one unit increase in
an individual’s family conflict score was associated with 1.03 times greater odds of having
clinically significant trauma symptomology (95% CI = 1.00 – 1.05). As with cohesion and
expressiveness, the increase in probability and the effect of family conflict on the overall
56
Table 6
ECR-R
Attachment Non-Clinical PTSD CPTSD
Quadrants
Secure 200 10 9
Dismissing Avoidant 26 3 8
Pre-Occupied 95 9 26
Fearful Avoidant 35 2 16
______________________________________________________________________________
Note. Hypothesis 3C. Cochran-Mantel-Haenszel (CMH) ‘row mean scores differ’ statistic
graph.
______________________________________________________________________________
57
A one unit increase in an individual’s attachment anxiety score was associated with 1.65
times greater odds of having clinically significant trauma symptomology (95% CI = 1.29 - 2.12).
Increased scores on attachment anxiety were associated with an increased likelihood of clinically
significant trauma symptomology (χ2 = 15.566, DF = 1, p < .0001). An individual with a one unit
increase in attachment anxiety had a 65% greater probability of meeting criteria for trauma
symptomology.
A one unit increase in an individual’s childhood trauma exposure was associated with
1.73 times greater odds of having clinically significant trauma symptomology (95% CI = 1.21 –
2.47). Increased scores on childhood trauma exposure were associated with an increased
likelihood of clinically significant trauma symptomology (χ2 = 8.89, DF = 1, p = .003). For each
additional childhood trauma type an individual experienced, that individual had a 73% higher
probability of meeting criteria for trauma symptomology. Logistic regression results for
58
Table 7
Note. Hypothesis 4. Potentially traumatic events (childhood, lifetime), family environment (cohesion, expressiveness, conflict,
control), and attachment (anxiety, avoidance) variables in relation to group belonging (non-clinical, trauma symptomology).
___________________________________________________________________________________________________________
59
Discussion
The purpose of the current study was to examine relationships between family
environment and attachment factors that may have a protective relationship with potentially
traumatic events, and self-reported clinical categorization related to trauma. In this study,
surprisingly, and consistent with previous research (Goodwin, 1988; Hermann, 1992; van der
Kolk, 2005), the current study found significant relationships between experiencing potentially
traumatic events (PTEs) in childhood (e.g., childhood abuse) and also throughout one’s lifetime
protective in relation to experiencing PTEs were explored: experiencing a more positive family
environment and having a more secure attachment style. Significant relationships were found
between family cohesion, expressiveness, conflict, and control, and trauma symptomology.
Finally, an individual’s attachment style was also found to be significantly related to reported
trauma symptoms. While identification of significant relationships in the current research cannot
discern directionality and certainly does not imply causation, the importance of these variables in
Trauma Symptomology
experiences. This observation aligns with current data, which indicate that 1 in 4 youth in the
U.S. experience some form of child abuse and maltreatment in their lifetimes (Finkelhor, Turner,
Ormond, & Hamby, 2013). Of those who reported childhood abuse, emotional abuse was the
most commonly experienced, with more than 10% of the sample reporting this PTE. When
60
measuring across the lifetime, all 16 categories of PTEs were endorsed. Having been in a
transportation accident was the most common, with more than 62% reporting this PTE, and the
unexpected death of someone close (61%) was almost as common. Just over 12% of the sample
reported having experienced two or three PTEs in their lifetime, with the same percentage
endorsing that they had experienced nine or more. When both childhood and lifetime PTEs were
considered together, only 6.6% of participants reported having experienced none at all.
The first hypothesis in this study predicted that the number of potentially traumatic
events (PTEs) an individual experienced would be related to their trauma symptomology and
numbers of experienced PTEs were found to be related to higher incidence rates of both PTSD
and C-PTSD. Incidence of childhood PTEs were found to be 74% higher in those who reported
PTSD symptoms and 233% higher in those who reported C-PTSD symptoms. These results
aligned with current research in that the childhood PTE measure evaluated abuse experiences
and childhood abuse is known to result in trauma symptomology. Further, childhood abuse is
identified as one of the prolonged, chronic, and interpersonal experiences that are commonly
believed to contribute specifically to C-PTSD (Cloitre et al., 2013; Maercker et al., 2013; WHO,
2019).
to C-PTSD. There was a 46% greater incidence of lifetime PTEs for those with C-PTSD
symptomology. Though the lifetime measure of PTEs used in this study did not contribute as
significantly as the childhood measure, the cross-sectional nature of this study and the wide age
range of undergraduates (18-66) in the sample may have contributed to mixed results. Further,
61
classification based on PTE type-clusters has recently been found to strengthen correlational
results with trauma symptoms from this measure (Contractor, Weiss, Natesan Batley, & Elhai,
2020).
Family Environment
The second hypothesis predicted that a more positive family environment would be
related to less trauma symptoms. As hypothesized, when family environment was analyzed,
positive features were associated with less trauma symptomology. Results indicated that lower
scores on family cohesion, defined as the extent to which family members are concerned and
committed to the family and the degree to which family members are helpful and supportive of
each other, and expressiveness, the extent to which family members are allowed and encouraged
to act openly and express their feelings directly, were both associated with C-PTSD. Cohesive
and expressive families may have developed better communication styles, both within and
outside of familial relationships, that contribute to developing healthier coping skills and healing
following traumatic events. Families with these features may also offer more social closeness
and support that would further contribute to healing. Additionally, difficulties in emotional
complex trauma and in fact, differentiate C-PTSD from PTSD. That less family support and
more complex trauma symptoms are significantly associated, is perhaps not surprising and
Higher scores on family control, which measures rigidity of familial rules and
relationships, were associated with PTSD. Moreover, higher scores on family conflict were
associated with C-PTSD. This variable measured how commonly anger, aggression, and a
confrontational style is exhibited among family members. Controlling one’s emotions and using
62
more confrontational communication styles may not be as adaptive as expressive communication
and further, may not contribute to cohesion. These results support numerous current studies that
offer a positive family environment as protective of mental health and well-being. In a study that
evaluated effects of family environment on psychosis, results indicated that a more negative
family environment increased risk and a more positive environment was protective in the
2011). A more negative family environment was associated with youth diagnoses of conduct
disorder and oppositional defiant disorder, as well as predicted worse health outcomes (e.g.,
psychiatric hospitalization, substance misuse; Rey, Walter, Plapp, & Denshire, 2000).
Additionally, family conflict in youth has been linked to insomnia in adulthood (Gregory,
Avshalom, Moffitt, & Poulton, 2006). Having specific knowledge of familial protective factors is
beneficial to building both theory and therapeutic treatment models, as well as strengthening the
No matter the shared event, parents and the familial environment play an essential role in
youth response and healthy adjustment to traumatic events. Eisenberg and Silver (2011)
reviewed the literature regarding children’s coping and emotional regulation, and their parents’
roles in shaping their responses, in relation to the September 11th, 2001 terrorist attacks (9/11).
Findings indicated that parents played significant roles in determining their children’s well-
being. When parents encouraged emotional expression, positive reframing, and acceptance of
emotional reactions, their children experienced lower levels of distress (Gil-Rivas, Silver,
Holman, McIntosh, & Poulin, 2007). Further, parental rejection, avoidance of information-
sharing about the trauma, or distancing from their children’s emotional responses has been
63
shown to increase children’s negative responses to traumatic events (Charuvastra & Cloitre,
2008).
addressing these. Both individual and family therapy is of higher quality and more useful when
knowledge of these factors is incorporated into the work being addressed. Individuals may gain
confidence when they are knowledgeable regarding their abilities and competence. In the same
regard, when awareness increases in reference to particularly beneficial areas that need
Attachment
The third hypothesis predicted that an individual’s attachment style would be related to
trauma symptomology. When focusing on attachment and its relationship with trauma, it is worth
noting that more than 90% of the current study sample reported having an adult attachment
figure who loved and supported them, and with whom they felt close. The majority of
respondents identified their mother or step-mother as that person (37%). Participants also
identified their father or step-father (30%) or both parents (12%) as primary attachment figures.
While the current study did not determine these individuals’ attachment styles, consideration
should be given to secure attachment styles, as they are related to more positive functioning
within romantic and familial relationships (Paley et al., 1999). In a study evaluating attachment
in marital relationships, Paley et al. found that securely attached wives were effective problem-
solvers, and that they expressed more positive affect and less withdrawal than insecure wives. In
other research, securely attached men have been found to provide greater support to their
partners during a stressful situation than insecure men (Simpson, Rholes, & Nelligan, 1992).
Secure individuals are generally more able to regulate emotions effectively and thus, are more
64
likely to develop closeness and intimacy (Ben-Naim et al., 2013). They are also more likely to
symptomology. First, it was predicted that PTSD symptomology would be related to higher
scores on the anxiety dimension of attachment. Contrary to this prediction, these scores were not
found to be significantly related to PTSD. As previously mentioned, this study was under-
powered, and specifically, the ITQ-identified PTSD group contained only 23 participants.
Results may have been different if a larger sample had been obtained. Attachment anxiety scores
did, however, indicate a significant relationship with C-PTSD symptomology. The chronic,
prolonged, or repeated nature of traumatic events related to C-PTSD may interrupt healthy
Anxiously attached individuals have more difficulty maneuvering in close and romantic
relationships than those who are securely attached. In a study of the role of attachment in
emotion regulation during relationship conflict, Ben-Naim et al. (2013) found that these
individuals struggle to hold their negative thoughts and feelings in check. They appear to be less
able to discriminate when sharing their emotions with their partners. During stressful
interactions, their disclosure of negative emotions (e.g., contempt, sadness) increased and that of
positive emotions decreased. The authors indicated that this behavior was even more evident in
those individuals who were rated as high in anxious attachment. Anxious partners tend to regard
threatening situations in an exaggerated way and react prematurely to perceived danger (Ben-
Naim et al.).
Next, it was predicted that C-PTSD symptomology would be related to higher scores on
65
significant. Disorganized or mixed styles of attachment have been more frequently linked to
complex trauma (Ford & Courtois, 2009; Jacobs, Boyce, Ilan-Clarke, & Bifulco, 2019). Current
research supported this finding, with both anxious and avoidant attachment styles associated with
C-PTSD. Individuals with avoidant attachment styles tend to exhibit an inflated sense of self-
sufficiency and independence, less often seeking support from others (Ein-Dor, Mikulincer, &
Shaver, 2011). They may use suppression or distancing from negative emotions and experiences
as emotional coping methods (Ben-Naim et al., 2013). While suppression can result in negative
physical and psychological consequences for some, it appears to be less harmful for avoidant
attachment needs. These strategies may serve the function of shutting down both the attachment
system of the avoidant caregiver and the support seeking partner, forcing that individual to find
other ways to deal with their needs (Feeney & Collins, 2001).
Finally, it was predicted that the overall attachment classification would be related to
distinct categorization of trauma symptomology. Significantly, almost half of the study sample
(46%) reported secure attachment and also belonged to the non-clinical trauma symptom
category. As hypothesized, there were far fewer individuals with C-PTSD symptomology within
the group who identified as more securely attached. Further, more individuals who met criteria
for C-PTSD had less secure attachment, belonging to either the pre-occupied or fearfully
relationship found between any of the elements of attachment and a PTSD categorization.
reciprocally related behavioral system that increases our chance of survival early in life and
guides our feelings of security throughout adulthood (Hazan & Shaver, 1994). Insecure
66
attachment in adulthood is likely to contribute to relational issues that are most evident in close
relationships. According to Bowlby (1988), methods that may improve insecure attachment
include gaining an understanding of internal working models and having corrective emotional
within the family system in order to build psychological health and resilience. Emotionally
focused therapy (EFT) for couples offers an attachment-based treatment approach providing
experiences for couples. Each of these methods addresses bidirectional attachment and builds
emotional wellness.
The fourth and final hypothesis predicted that the factors of a more positive family
environment and a more secure attachment style would account for significant variability in the
relationship between experiencing trauma and the development of symptoms. While numerous
other factors not addressed in this study may account for trauma symptomology, the logistic
regression model explained 23% of the variance in trauma symptomology and correctly
classified more than 84% of cases. Cloitre et al. (2009) found that trauma experiences during
previous research, and in support of previous results from the current study, childhood abuse and
trauma exposure resulted in a 73% higher probability of endorsing trauma symptoms. Further,
less secure attachment, as measured by higher scores on attachment anxiety, resulted in a 65%
67
While lifetime PTE exposure and attachment avoidance did not explain significant
variance related to trauma categorization in this model, the family environment variables of
cohesion and conflict had significant relationships with trauma symptomology. Additionally,
symptomology. Even though the effect size was small for these family variables, results support
previously reported research that family unity and cohesion strongly contribute to resiliency.
The trauma experiences of the Native American population in this sample supports
previous research and strongly suggests further attention and investigation into the health
disparities of these individuals. While making up a small percentage (3.9%) of the total sample
size, Native Americans represented almost half (47%) of those who experienced complex trauma
symptomology. The vast health inequities of this population deserve considerable attention.
Given specific cultural differences and experiences, it is important to evaluate specific treatment
factors related to historical trauma, racism, and oppression, as well as building upon specific
factors related to increasing resiliency in this high-risk group is of utmost importance and long
over-due.
In addition to adding to the current research around complex trauma, these results have
important clinical implications. Frequently, primary focus is given to how to treat maladaptive
treatment models, attention should also be focused on variables that may provide protection and
family environment have been shown to reduce conduct problems in youth (Rey, Walter, Plapp,
68
& Denshire, 2000). The current study provided preliminary support for the potentially protective
elements of family environment (cohesion, expressiveness, conflict, and control) and attachment
(anxiety and avoidance) that were significantly related to reduced trauma symptomology and
Special consideration should be given in treatment to the significant role family plays in
building both risk and resiliency. While this variable is included by necessity when working with
youth who are still living within their family system, focus on family factors in work with adult
modalities is essential in the treatment of trauma. When addressing persistent and complex
trauma symptomology, understanding the negative contribution of weaknesses within the family
system and the importance of strengthening these relations may significantly increase positive
outcomes.
The current study also contributes to the vast literature suggesting that specific
consideration be given to complex trauma and C-PTSD. The pronounced differences in both the
experience of trauma and resulting symptomology between PTSD and C-PTSD clearly justify
further investigation. Hermann proposed more comprehensive evaluation in these areas more
than two decades ago. Despite increased research and findings, as well as the inclusion of C-
PTSD in the most recent ICD, C-PTSD is still not given sufficient consideration in the DSM. It
trauma-focused therapeutic methods that focus on complex trauma. Cloitre (2015) suggests that
consideration must be given to the heterogeneity of the trauma population. Profiles of complex
69
trauma throughout the lifespan will contribute to the understanding of proposed core symptoms
and to the development of interventions that match individual client needs (Cloitre, 2015). In
order to identify the optimal identification and treatment for a traumatized individual, reliable
Limitations
The present study has a number of methodological limitations. As with any cross-
sectional survey, causation cannot be implied. In addition, the measures used were self-report
questionnaires. While this method has valid strengths specific to trauma research as previously
described, it most certainly has well-known weaknesses as well (e.g., being subject to the biases
and limitations of retrospective reports). It has been suggested that symptomatology would be
more accurately determined if multiple informants (Lanktree et al., 2008) and multi-method
assessments, including interviews and/or observations were used (Courtois, 2004; Hoyle, Harris,
& Judd, 2002). As such, future studies may expand on current research by conducting clinician-
and categorization.
This research was limited by the relatively homogenous nature of the participants,
particularly in regard to race/ethnicity and educational status. The majority of participants (84%)
were White/non-Hispanic. Future research would expand on the current study by investigating a
more diverse sample, to include populations who experience higher levels of trauma (e.g.,
sexually- and gender-diverse individuals). Further, conducting research with a college population
the International Trauma Questionnaire (ITQ) for the ICD-11 has not yet been evaluated in
70
relation to a non-clinical population or a college sample. These results will contribute to growing
their contributions to risk and resiliency. An individual may have experienced caregivers, family
members, or a home environment that contributed both protective and injurious elements. While
these variables certainly overlap, each individual included in the current study reported on the
entirety of their traumatic experiences and on the same components of the specific factors
evaluated. An additional limitation is the relatively small n for some subgroups (e.g., 23
participants met criteria for PTSD, 59 met criteria for C-PTSD), offering less power than
anticipated.
Although, 84% of cases were correctly classified in the logistic regression, the effect
sizes were small. These results suggest that, as would be expected, numerous other factors not
accounted for in the present study may account for trauma symptomology. These limitations
notwithstanding, the present study has a number of strengths. These include well-validated
instruments that yielded good internal consistency in the present sample and results that support
Summary
In conclusion, the primary objective of the current study was to contribute knowledge to
the growing literature on complex trauma and possible protective factors that may contribute to
less trauma symptomology. Results indicate the high frequency of trauma exposure among
college undergraduates. That is, more than 93 percent of the respondents had experienced one or
more potentially traumatic events in their lifetime. Trauma is known to contribute to serious
71
psychological and physiological health concerns and the incredibly high incidence rate in a
college population underscores the seriousness of the problem. Identification of factors that
contribute to both risk and to resilience is critical in order to improve services and resources for
Results from the current study were consistent with previous research regarding the
relationship between potentially traumatic events (PTEs) and trauma symptomology and
categorization. Traumatic events that occur over extended periods of time during childhood and
hinder meeting developmental milestones (e.g., childhood abuse) are believed to significantly
contribute to complex trauma; this relationship was supported. The current study also found
evidence for associations between the number of PTEs experienced and trauma symptoms of
between experiencing PTEs, developing trauma symptomology, and the potentially protective
factors of positive family environment and secure attachment. Of the seven elements evaluated in
this study, the family environment subscales (1) cohesion, (2) expressiveness, (3) conflict, and
(4) control, as well as the attachment variables (1) anxiety and (2) avoidance, were all found to
be associated with trauma symptomology. Only family organization had no supporting evidence
of a significant relationship.
The findings of this study support further exploration of factors that promote resilience in
relation to both PTSD and C-PTSD. By developing knowledge around those factors that build
resilience and contribute to mental health and wellness in the face of adversity and trauma, both
childhood and lifetime developmental pathways may be strengthened. Understanding risk and
72
protective variables is essential in being better equipped to promote healthy development in the
73
Appendix A
Informed Consent
Thank you for agreeing to participate in this survey. The purpose of this study is to learn about
ways in which our past experiences are related to how we currently think, feel, and relate to
others. Most of these questions will ask you to choose from a set selection of options.
Sometimes, this will feel really easy to do and one of the options will feel like it accurately fits
what you think or feel. Other times, you may be torn between one or two options, and that is
okay! Just choose the option that best describes your experiences. There are no right or wrong
Undergraduate college students who are 18 years or older are eligible to participate in this
survey.
The survey contains two types of questions: Questions that require you to check a box associated
with the response that best describes your experiences, and questions where you are asked to
type your answers in a text box presented beneath the question. Most questions will ask you to
Answering the survey should take approximately 45 minutes to one hour to complete.
We believe that the likely risks of completing this survey are minimal. However, some of the
questions are about experiences you may have had – or are currently having – in regard to being
hurt physically, sexually, or emotionally. Because of this, some of the questions may make you
74
uncomfortable or be distressing to you. If you become distressed or desire assistance during or
after taking the survey, you should contact either or both of the following numbers:
Counseling Services…………………………………….243-4711
Please also note that you may exit out of the survey at any time. There will be an option at the
There are no direct benefits anticipated for you from answering questions on this survey.
However, this survey will provide valuable information about how past experiences influence
current experiences. This information may help with the development of effective treatments for
You may also be compensated for your time by receiving research credit in your psychology
course. If you are interested in receiving research credit, please follow the link at the end of this
survey. This link will take you to a separate page where you can enter your contact information.
To request more information about this survey or the study, please email Susan Ocean, M.A. at
Clicking below and continuing this survey indicates that I am female, I am 18 years or older, I
have read the description of the study, and I agree to participate in this study.
-I agree -I disagree
75
Appendix B
Demographic Information
1. How old are you? (please answer in years, and in number format only. e.g.: 21 years, 7
months = “21”)
5. How long have you been in this relationship? (Please answer in months, and in number
format only. E.g.: 5 years = “60”)
6. When you were growing up, did you have an adult who loved and supported you and that
you felt close with?
76
c. Good
d. Fair
e. Poor
77
Appendix C
Instructions: These questions ask about some of your experiences growing up as a child and as
a teenager. Although these questions are of a personal nature, please try to answer as honestly as
you can. For each question, select the response that best describes how you feel.
2. I knew that there was someone to take care of me and protect me.
5. There was someone in my family who helped me feel that I was important or special.
7. I felt loved.
9. I got hit so hard by someone in my family that I had to see a doctor or go to the hospital.
11. People in my family hit me so hard that it left me with bruises or marks.
12. I was punished with a belt, a board, a cord, or some other hard object.
76
16. I had the perfect childhood.
17. I got hit or beaten so badly that it was noticed by someone like a teacher, neighbor, or
doctor.
20. Someone tried to touch me in a sexual way, or tried to make me touch them.
21. Someone threatened to hurt me or tell lies about me unless I did something sexual with
them.
77
Appendix D
Instructions: Below are a number of difficult or stressful things that sometimes happen to
people. For each event check one or more of the boxes to the right to indicate that (a) it happened
to you personally, (b) you witnessed it happen to someone else, (c) you learned about it, (d) you
2. Fire or explosion
3. Transportation accident (for example, car accident, boat accident, train wreck, plane
crash)
6. Physical assault (for example, being attacked, hit, slapped, kicked, beaten up)
7. Assault with a weapon (for example, being shot, stabbed, threatened with a knife, gun,
bomb)
8. Sexual assault (rape, attempted rape, made to perform any type of sexual act through
11. Captivity (for example, being kidnapped, abducted, held hostage, prisoner of war)
78
16. Serious injury, harm, or death you caused to someone else
79
Appendix E
Instructions: The statements below concern how you feel in emotionally intimate relationships.
We are interested in how you generally experience relationships, not just in what is happening in
a current relationship. Respond to each statement by clicking a circle to indicate how much you
2. I often worry that my partner will not want to stay with me.
4. I worry that romantic partners won’t care about me as much as I care about them.
5. I often wish that my partner's feelings for me were as strong as my feelings for him or her.
7. When my partner is out of sight, I worry that he or she might become interested in someone
else.
8. When I show my feelings for romantic partners, I'm afraid they will not feel the same about
me.
12. I find that my partner(s) don't want to get as close as I would like.
13. Sometimes romantic partners change their feelings about me for no apparent reason.
80
14. My desire to be very close sometimes scares people away.
15. I'm afraid that once a romantic partner gets to know me, he or she won't like who I really am.
16. It makes me mad that I don't get the affection and support I need from my partner.
20. I feel comfortable sharing my private thoughts and feelings with my partner.
81
Appendix F
There are 90 statements in this booklet. They are statements about families. You are to decide
which of these statements are true of your family of origin and which are false. If you think the
statement is “true” or mostly “true” of the family you were raised in, make a “T” next to the
statement. If you think the statement is “false” or mostly “false” of your family, make an “F”
You may feel that some of the statements are true for some family members and false for others.
Mark “T” if the statement is true for most members. Mark “F” if the statement is false for most
members. If the members are evenly divided, decide which the overall stronger impression is and
answer accordingly.
Remember, we would like to know what your family seems like to you. So do not try to figure
out how other members see your family, but do give us your general impression of your family
8. Family members attend church, synagogue, Sunday school (or similar) fairly often.
82
10. Family members are rarely ordered around.
22. It’s hard to “blow off steam” at home without upsetting somebody.
25. How much money a person makes is not very important to us.
26. Learning about new and different things is very important in our family.
27. Nobody in our family is active in sports, little league, bowling, etc.
28. We often talk about the religious meaning of Christmas, Passover, or other holidays.
29. It’s often hard to find things when you need them in our household.
30. There is one family member who makes most of the decisions.
83
33. Family members hardly ever lose their tempers.
42. If we feel like doing something on the spur of the moment, we often just pick up and go.
45. We always strive to do things just a little better the next time.
48. Family members have strict ideas about what is right and wrong.
54. Family members almost always rely on themselves when a problem comes up.
55. Family members rarely worry about job promotions, school grades, etc.
84
56. Someone in our family plays a musical instrument.
57. Family members are not very involved in recreational activities outside work or school.
58. We believe there are some things you just have to take on faith.
62. Money and paying bills is openly talked about in our family.
63. If there’s a disagreement in our family, we try hard to smooth things over and keep the
peace.
64. Family members strongly encourage each other to stand up for their rights.
67. Family members sometimes attend courses or take lessons for some hobby or interest
(outside of school).
68. In our family each person has different ideas about what is right and wrong.
74. It’s hard to be by yourself without hurting someone’s feelings in our household.
85
77. Family members go out a lot.
78. The Bible, the Quran, or another religious doctrine, is a very important book/concept in
our home.
81. There is plenty of time and attention for everyone in our family.
83. In our family, we believe you don’t ever get anywhere by raising your voice.
84. We are not really encouraged to speak up for ourselves in our family.
85. Family members are often compared with others as to how well they are doing at work or
school.
88. Family members believe that if you sin you will be punished.
86
Appendix G
The International Trauma Questionnaire (ITQ; Cloitre, Roberts, Bisson, & Brewin, 2018)
Instructions: Please identify the experience that troubles you most and answer the questions in relation
to this experience.
Below are a number of problems that people sometimes report in response to traumatic or stressful life
events. Please read each item carefully, then circle one of the numbers to the right to indicate how much
you have been bothered by that problem in the past month.
Not A little Moderately Quite Extremely
at all bit a bit
1. Having upsetting dreams that replay part of the
experience or are clearly related to the experience? 0 1 2 3 4
87
9. Affected any other important part of your life such
as parenting, or school or college work, or other 0 1 2 3 4
important activities?
Below are problems that people who have had stressful or traumatic events sometimes experience. The
questions refer to ways you typically feel, ways you typically think about yourself and ways you typically
relate to others. Answer the following thinking about how true each statement is of you.
4. I feel worthless. 0 1 2 3 4
In the past month, have the above problems in emotions, in beliefs about yourself and in
relationships:
88
References
Ainsworth, M.D.S., Blehar, M.C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Theoretical background. In
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders
Barry, R. A., & Lawrence, E. (2013). “Don’t stand so close to me”: An attachment perspective of
484-494.
Ben-Naim, S., Hirschberger, G., Ein-Dor, T., & Mikulincer, M. (2013). An experimental study
questionnaire-ctq/.
Birnbaum, G. E., Reis, H. T., Mikulincer, M., Gillath, O., & Orpaz, A. (2006). When sex is more
than just sex: Attachment orientations, sexual experience, and relationship quality.
Bonanno, G. (2008). Loss, trauma, and human resilience: Have we underestimated the human
89
Bowlby, J. (1951). Maternal care and mental health. Geneva: World Health Organization.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and anger. New York:
Basic Books.
Routledge.
Brennan, N., Clark, C., & Shaver, P. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult attachment: An
integrative overview. In J. Simpson & W. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close
Breslau, N., Davis, G. C., Peterson, E. L., & Schultz, L. R. (2000). A second look at comorbidity
Bretherton, I. (1992). The origins of attachment theory: John Bowlby and Mary Ainsworth.
Briere, J. & Jordan, C. E. (2009). Childhood maltreatment, intervening variables, and adult
375-388.
Briere, J., Hodges, M., & Godbout, N. (2010). Traumatic stress, affect dysregulation, and
767-774.
Briere, J., Kaltman, S., & Green, B. L. (2008). Accumulated childhoot trauma and symptom
90
Brown, N. R., Kallivayalil, D., Mendelsohn, M., & Harvey, M. R. (2012). Working the double
Bryant, R. A. (2012). Simplifying complex PTSD: Comment on Resick et al. (2012). Journal of
Campbell, R., Greeson, M. R., & Fehler-Cabral, G. (2014). Developing Recruitment Methods for
Cloitre, M. (2015). The “one size fits all” approach to trauma treatment: Should we be satisfied?
Cloitre, M., Courtois, C. A., Charuvastra, A., Carapezza, R., Stolbach, B. C., and Green, B. L.
(2011). Treatment of complex PTSD: Results of the ISTSS expert clinician survey on best
Cloitre, M., Garvert, D. W., Brewin, C. R., Bryant, R. A., and Maercker, A. (2013). Evidence for
proposed ICD-11 PTSD and complex PTSD: A latent profile analysis. European Journal of
Cloitre, M., Roberts, N. P., Bisson, J. I., & Brewin, C. R. (2017). The International Trauma
Cloitre, M., Stolbach, B. C., Herman, J. L., van der Kolk, B., Pynoos, R., Wang, J., & Petkova,
91
Cloitre, M., Stovall-McClough, K. C., Nooner, K., Zorbas, P., Cherry, S., Jackson, C. L., Gan,
W., & Petkova, E. (2010). Treatment for PTSD related to childhood abuse: A randomized
Cloitre, M., Stovall-McClough, C., Ferssizidis, P., & Charuvastra, A. (2008). Attachment
women with childhood abuse histories. Journal of traumatic stress, 21, 282-289.
Contractor, A. A., Weiss, N. H., Natesan Batley, P., & Elhai, J. D. (2020). Clusters of trauma
types as measured by the Life Events Checklist for DSM-5. International Journal of
Cook, A., Spinazzola, J., Ford, J., Lanktree, C., Blaustein, M., Cloitre, M., & van der Kolk, B.
(2005). Complex trauma in children and adolescents. Psychiatric Annals, 35, 105-118.
Copeland, W. E., Gordon, K., Angold, A., & Costello, E. J. (2007). Traumatic events and
Costello, E. J., Erkanli, A., Fairbank, J. A., & Angold, A. (2002). The prevalence of potentially
traumatic events in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 15(2), 99-
112
D’Andrea, W., Ford, J., Stolbach, B., Spinazzola, J., & van der Kolk, B. A. (2012).
92
Deblinger, E., Steer, R. A., & Lippman, J. (1999). Sexually abused children suffering
1, 310-321.
Ein-Dor, T., Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2011). Attachment insecurities and the processing
Elklit, A., Hyland, P., and Shevlin, M. (2014). Evidence of symptom profiles consistent with
posttraumatic stress disorder and complex posttraumatic stress disorder in different trauma
https://doi.org/10.3402/ejpt.v5.24221.
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical
power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior
Feeney, B. C., & Collins, N. L. (2001). Predictors of caregiving in adult intimate relationships:
80(6), 972-994.
Feeny, N. C., Foa, E. B., Treadwell, K. R. H., & March, J. (2004). Posttraumatic stress disorder
in youth: A critical review of the cognitive and behavioral treatment outcome literature.
Felitti, V. J., Anda, R. F., Nordenberg, D., Williamson, D. F., Spitz, A. M., Edwards, V., …
of the leading causes of death in adults: The adverse childhood experiences (ACE) study.
93
Finkelhor, D., Ormrod, R. K., & Turner, H. A. (2007). Poly-victimization: A neglected
Finkelhor, D., Ormrod, R., Turner, H., & Hamby, S. (2005). The victimization of children and
Finkelhor, D., Turner, H. A., Ormond, R., & Hamby, S. L. (2013). Violence, crime, and abuse
Follette, V. M., Polusny, M. A., Bechtle, A. E., & Naugle, A. E. (1996). Cumulative trauma: The
impact of child sexual abuse, adult sexual assault, and spouse abuse. Journal of Traumatic
Stress, 9, 25-35.
Ford, J. D. (2017). Complex trauma and complex posttraumatic stress disorder. In APA
Gold, Vol. 1:281–305, Chapter xxii, 624 Pages. American Psychological Association; DC,
2017.
https://search.proquest.com/psycinfo/docview/1933894051/abstract/6422F7D62C7448EEP
Q/3.
Ford, J. D. & Courtois, C. A. (2009). Defining and understanding complex trauma and complex
traumatic stress disorders in C. Courtois & J. Ford (Eds.), Treating complex traumatic stress
132-154. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.4.2.132
94
Fraley, R. C. & Waller, N. G. (1998). Adult attachment patterns: A test of the typological model
in J. Simpson & W. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 77-
Fraley, R. C., Waller, N. G., & Brennan, K. A. (2000). An item-response theory analysis of self-
report measures of adult attachment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 350-
365.
Friedman, M. J., Resick, P. A., Bryant, R. A., and Brewin, C. R. (2011). Considering PTSD for
Gelinas, D. (1983). The persisting negative effects of incest. Psychiatry, 46, 312-332.
Ghasemi, A. & Zahediasl, S. (2012). Normality tests for statistical analysis: A guide for non-
https:// DOI:10.5812/ijem.3505.
González-Pinto, A., de Azúa, S, Ibáñez, B., Otero-Cuesta, S., Castro-Fornieles, J., et al. (2011).
475-488.
Gregory, A. M., Avshalom, C., Moffitt, T. E., and Poulton, R. (2006). Family conflict in
Herman, J.L. (1992). Complex PTSD: A syndrome in survivors of prolonged and repeated
95
Hidalgo, R. B., & Davidson, J. R. T. (2000). Posttraumatic stress disorder: Epidemiology and
Hodges, M., Godbout, N., Briere, J., Lanktree, C., Gilbert, A., & Kletzka, N. T. (2013).
Cumulative trauma and symptom complexity in children: A path analysis. Child Abuse &
Hyland, P., Shevlin, M., Brewin, C. R., Cloitre, M., Downes, A. J., Jumbe, S., Karatzias, T.,
Bisson, J. I., and Roberts, N. P. (2017). Validation of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD)
and complex PTSD using the international trauma questionnaire. Acta Psychiatrica
Jacobs, C., Boyce, N., Ilan-Clarke, Y., & Bifulco, A. (2019). Assessing attachment style in
Jonkman, C. S., Verlinden, E., Bolle, E. A., Boer, F., and Lindauer, R. J. L. (2013). Traumatic
stress symptomatology after child maltreatment and single traumatic events: Different
Karam, E. G., Friedman, M. J., Hill, E. D., Kessler, R. C., McLaughlin, K. A., Petukhova, M. …
Koenen, K. C. (2014). Cumulative traumas and risk thresholds: 12-month PTSD in the
World Mental Health (WMH) Surveys. Depression and Anxiety, 31, 130–42.
Kinniburgh, K. J., Blaustein, M., Spinazzola, J., van der Kolk, B. A. (2005). Attachment, self-
96
Kira, I. A., Lewandowski, L., Templin, T., Ramaswamy, V., Ozkan, B., & Mohanesh, J. (2008).
Knefel, M., Garvert, D. W., Cloitre, M., & Lueger-Schuster, B. (2015). Update to an evaluation
of ICD-11 PTSD and complex PTSD criteria in a sample of adult survivors of childhood
Laor, N., Wolmer, L., Mayes, L. C., Gershon, A., Weizman, R., & Cohen, D. J. (1997). Israeli
Liem, J. H., James, J. B., O’Toole, J. G., & Boudewyn, A. C. (1997). Assessing resilience in
67(4), 594-606.
Lindauer, R. J. L. (2012). Child maltreatment – clinical PTSD diagnosis not enough?!: Comment
Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (2000). The construct of resilience: A critical
evaluation and guidelines for future work. Child Development 71(3), 543-562.
Maercker, A., Brewin, C. R., Bryant, R. A., Cloitre, M., van Ommeren, M., Jones, L. M.,
Humayan, A., et al. (2013). Diagnosis and classification of disorders specifically associated
https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20057.
97
Makinen, J. A., & Johnson, S. M. (2006). Resolving attachment injuries in couples using
Psychotraumatology, 6, 1-10.
Masten, A. S., Hubbard, J. J., Gest, S. D., Tellegen, A., Garmezy, N., & Ramirez, M. (1999).
McDonald, J. H. (2014). Handbook of Biological Statistics (3rd ed.). Sparky House Publishing,
Baltimore, Maryland.
McDonald, M. K. (2015). A measure development study for youth trauma exposure and
Mikulincer, M. & Florian, V. (1998). The relationship between adult attachment styles and
(Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 143-165). New York:
Guildford Press.
98
Moos, R. (1981). Family Environment Scale. Palo Alto, California: Consulting Psychologists
Press.
Moos, R. H., & Moos, B. S. (1994). Family environment scale manual. Consulting Psychologists
Press.
Morgan, G.A., Leech, N. L., Gloeckner, G. W., & Barrett, K. C. (2011). IBM SPSS for
Introductory Statistics: Use and Interpretation (4th ed.). New York: Routledge Academic.
Paley, B., Cox, M. J., Burchinal, M. R., & Payne, C. C. (1999). Attachment and marital
13(4), 580-597.
Pederson, D. R., & Moran, G. (1999). The relationship imperative: Arguments for a broad
Perkonigg, A., Höfler, M., Cloitre, M., Wittchen, H. U., Trautmann, S., & Maercker, A. (2015).
Evidence for two different ICD-11 posttraumatic stress disorders in a community sample
Punamäki, R. L., Qouta, S., Miller, T., & El-Sarraj, E. (2011). Who are the resilient children in
Qouta, S., El-Sarraj, E., & Punamäki, R.L. (2001). Mental flexibility as resiliency factor among
R Core Team (2017). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for
99
Rees, S., Silove, D., Chey, T., Ivancic, L., Steel, Z., Creamer, M., Teesson, M., Bryant, R.,
McFarlane, A. C., Mills, K. L., Slade, T., Carragher, N., O’Donnell, M., & Forbes, D.
(2011). Lifetime prevalence of gender-based violence in women and the relationship with
Resnick, M. D., Bearman, P. S., Blum, R. W., Bauman, K. E., Harris, K. M., Jones, J., . . . Udry,
J. R. (1997). Protecting adolescents from harm. Findings from the National Longitudinal
Rey, J., Walter, G., Plapp, J., & Denshire, E. (2000). Family environment in attention deficit
Roisman, G. I., Fraley, R. C., & Belsky, J. (2007). A taxometric study of the Adult Attachment
Roisman, G.I., Madsen, S. D., Henninghausen, K. H., Sroufe, L. A., & Collins, W. A. (2001).
Development, 3, 156-172.
Rutter, M. (1999). Resilience concepts and findings: Implications for family therapy. Journal of
100
Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own enviroments: A theory of
Schachner, D., Shaver, P., & Mikulincer, M. (2005). Patterns of nonverbal behavior and
141-169.
Scheeringa, M. S., & Zeenah, C. H. (2001). A relational perspective on PTSD in early childhood.
Simpson, J. A., Rholes, W. S., & Nelligan, J. S. (1992). Support seeking and support giving
Psychopathology 9, 251-268.
Stolbach, B. C., Garvert, D., Cloitre, M. (2014). A latent class analysis of PTSD and complex
exposure and symptoms in children: Implications for DSM and ICD diagnostic
classification; Miami, FL Nov, The 30th annual conference of The International Society
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Computer assisted research design and analysis.
Tolin, D. F., & Foa, E. B. (2006). Sex differences in trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder: A
http://dx.doi.org.weblib.lib.umt.edu:8080/10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.959.
101
van der Kolk, B. A. (2005). Developmental Trauma Disorder. Psychiatric Annals, 35(5),401-
408.
Weathers, F. W., Blake, D. D., Schurr, P. P., Kaloupek, D. G., Marx, B. P., & Keane, T. M.
(2013). The Life Events Checklist for DSM-5 (LEC-5). Instrument available from the
Werner, E. E. (1993). Risk, resilience, and recovery: Perspectives from the Kauai Longitudinal
Werner, Emmy E., 115-132. New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press.
Werner, E. E., & Smith, R. S. (1992). Overcoming the odds: High risk children from birth to
Wolf, E. J., Miller, M. W., Kilpatrick, D., Resnick, H. S., Badour, C. L., Marx, B. P. …
World Health Organization. (1992). The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural
Organization.
Yehuda, R. (2004). Risk and resilience in posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Clinical
Yule, W. (2001). Post-traumatic stress disorder in children and adolescents. International Review
102
Zeanah, C. H., Boris, N. W., & Larrieu, J. A. (1997). Infant development and developmental
risk: A review of the past 10 years [Abstract]. Journal of the American Academy of Child
103