BGSU Dissertations 2119 Fisher
BGSU Dissertations 2119 Fisher
A Dissertation
DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY
August 2001
Committee:
Vincent Kantorski,
Graduate Faculty Representative
Milton D. Hakel -
Dara Musher-Eizenman 11 9
ii
ABSTRACT
Many changes in the workplace and in employees’ lives have taken place over the
past couple decades and have led to an increase in the attention paid to the boundary
between work and non-work issues among employees. However, much of this research
has been limited by its emphasis on family in the domain of non-work. The present study
work/personal life balance. Based on the conservation of resources theory and qualitative
perceptions of work/life balance, and it was expected that this scale would be related to a
variety of stressors and strains. Data were collected from a heterogeneous sample of 540
managers employed in a variety of organizations across the U.S. Responses from 273 of
these managers were retained for use as a holdout sample for cross-validation purposes.
Across both samples, results indicated three dimensions of work/life balance: work
interference with personal life, personal life interference with work, and work/personal
life enhancement.
pressure, time management behavior, overall feelings of job stress, job satisfaction, life
balance and work/family conflict. Overall, this study provides initial evidence of
Ill
warranted.
iv
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
I would like to thank the people who assisted me in achieving this educational
milestone. I am most grateful to my advisor. Dr. Carlla Smith, for her continued support,
dedication, open door, and confidence in me throughout this process. Many thanks to Dr.
Milt Hakel, not only for his feedback and service on this committee, but for much
wisdom and professional mentorship during my graduate school career. I would also like
Kantorski for their time and helpful suggestions. Sincere appreciation is also extended to
Drs. Mike Zickar and Jeff Stanton for their contributions to this project.
This research would not have been possible without the support of the American
expending the financial resources to make it all happen. Many thanks to Dr. Monica
Hemingway, Jody Hoffman, Julie Fuller, Maggie Laber. and Lilly Lin for assisting me
with the qualitative research conducted as part of this investigation. I also extend deep
gratitude to Dr. Steven Rogelberg for his mentoring earlier in my graduate career— for
stimulating my interest in quality of work life initiatives and survey measurement issues,
being an excellent statistics teacher, and always demonstrating enthusiasm for research.
successful graduate program at BG. Many thanks to the Penn State folks who gave me
knowledge and experience that opened windows of curiosity and doors of opportunity for
me— Drs. John Mathieu, Dave Day, Jim Farr, Tonia Heffner, and Jay Goodwin. I also
give thanks to Armand “Puck" Spoto for making those PSU days and many since then
encouragement from my parents, Loring Fisher and Janet Dimotsis. Thanks, mom and
also grateful for the love and support of my other “family,” Mickey Duggan, Chrisy
Duggan, and Holly Mack, who have always been there for me. I am also thankful for the
friends who greatly improved the quality of my graduate school life: Emily, Stephanie,
Jen, Paul, Stefoni, Brenda, Ana, Michelle, Heath, Jody, and Laurie.
I appreciate having had the opportunity to work with Drs. Andrea Goldberg and
Ellie McCreery. They have been great mentors and sources of support, both personally
and professionally. 1 thank each of them for teaching me the importance of and
satisfaction in having a life beyond work, and for showing me that achieving a
I give many thanks to Savanna for her daily reminders to strive for this balance in
my own life. Most importantly, I am grateful to my partner, Devin, for his love, support,
and patience during this process, for many helpful suggestions on earlier drafts, and for
adding so much to my life. I look forward to charting the course together through the
TABLE OF CONTENTS
INTRODUCTION.................................................................................................................1
Organizational Stress................................................................................................ 4
Definition of Stress....................................................................................... 4
Role Theory.................................................................................................. 6
Congruence Model....................................................................................... 9
Summary......................................................................................................13
Work/Family Conflict.................................................................................. 14
Summary..................................................................................................... 24
Hypotheses.............................................................................................................. 32
VIH
Hypothesis 1................................................................................................. 32
Hypothesis 2................................................................................................. 32
Hypothesis 3................................................................................................. 33
Hypothesis 4................................................................................................. 33
Hypothesis 5................................................................................................. 33
Hypothesis 6................................................................................................. 34
Hypothesis 7................................................................................................. 34
Hypothesis 8................................................................................................. 34
Hypothesis 9................................................................................................. 35
Hypothesis 10............................................................................................... 35
Participants................................................................................................... 37
Procedure..................................................................................................... 38
Measures...................................................................................................... 38
Results.......................................................................................................... 39
Study Two................................................................................................................ 41
Measures......................................................................................................46
Data Analyses.............................................................................................. 54
IX
Initial Sample........................................................................................................... 60
Demographic Information........................................................................... 60
Scale Assessment.........................................................................................60
Summary...................................................................................................... 88
Validity Evidence......................................................................................... 88
Hypothesis Tests.......................................................................................... 91
Hypothesis 1.....................................................................................91
Hypothesis 2.....................................................................................92
Hypothesis 3.....................................................................................92
Hypothesis 4.....................................................................................92
Hypothesis 5.....................................................................................93
Hypothesis 6.....................................................................................93
Hypothesis 7.....................................................................................93
Hypothesis 8.....................................................................................93
Hypothesis 9.....................................................................................93
Hypothesis 10...................................................................................94
Hypothesis 1...................................................................................125
Hypothesis 2...................................................................................128
Hypothesis 6...................................................................................129
Hypothesis 8...................................................................................129
Hypothesis 9...................................................................................130
Limitations.............................................................................................................166
REFERENCES.................................................................................................................. 170
LIST OF TABLES
Table
4 Work/Life Balance Scale Item Means and Standard Deviations - Initial Sample ....63
31 IRT a Parameters for Each Work/Life Balance Scale Item - Total Study
Sample....................................................................................................................... 115
XIV
LIST OF FIGURES
Figure
PREFACE
decided to pursue graduate study in a field in which I could devote my career to applying
development during graduate school, I opted to spend my third year on leave for a 12-
Armonk, NY. During my tenure at IBM, I often experienced difficulty in finding time to
pursue my personal interests (e.g., spending time with friends, playing tennis, bicycling,
running, spending time with my dog, and traveling) and think about trying to get enough
sleep while try ing to perform well on a job that involved working at least 50 hours a
Even as a single, childless worker I encountered this struggle, and it was then that
I learned two important things. First, I found that having a balance between my work life
and my life away from work was important for my overall health and well-being. (In
retrospect. I find it hard to believe that it took years of part-time jobs to get through
college, long hours in graduate school, and almost six months of full-time employment
before I realized that, not only did I live, eat, & breathe my job, but that sacrificing work
accomplishments in order to have a satisfying personal life never really occurred to me. I
think many of my friends and family would have agreed I was highly involved in my
It was not long after this epiphany during the winter of 1997/1998 that I learned
that being able to exercise regularly actually helped me cope with stress on the job, and
gave me more energy to get through a long work day. Having more energy helped me to
XVII
be more productive, which certainly was of benefit to my clients within the company.
Secondly, I realized that having a significant other, children, or having to care for aging
family members is not a prerequisite for having this struggle; the difficulty I experienced
was legitimate, despite others’ expectations that I would be available for lengthy travel
assignments overseas or could work overtime because I did not have anyone waiting for
me at home.
study to take a more empirical approach to learning about work/life balance among other
employees. I am grateful for the experience that taught me a valuable lesson, and the
many people at IBM and elsewhere who helped get me to where I am today, striving for
example, the estimated industry expense of stress-related disorders is $150 billion per
year, and more than 14% of insurance compensation claims are based on stress-related
disorders (Pelletier & Lutz, 1989; 1991). Stress at work is related to a number of
and turnover; Ganster & Schaubroeck, 1991; Gupta & Beehr, 1979; Ironson, 1992;
Murphy, Hurrell, Sauter, & Keita, 1995; Quick, Horn, & Quick; 1987, Sauter & Murphy,
One particular source of stress (i.e., a stressor) that has been the focus of much
challenge many of us face trying to juggle work responsibilities with aspects of one’s
personal life, such as maintaining friendships, exercising, spending time with a romantic
partner, and/or caring for one’s aging parents or young children. Previous work/non-
work conflict research has primarily focused on family as the salient aspect of life in the
non-work domain. For example, many studies have focused on developing and testing
models regarding the antecedents and outcomes of work/family conflict (e.g., Frone,
Russell, & Cooper, 1992; Frone, Yardley, & Markel, 1997; Grandey & Cropanzano,
1999).
2
understanding of stress that results from work/family conflict, this research can be
criticized for having both conceptual and methodological limitations. For example, this
addition to family. Most studies only include employees who are married, have children,
and/or have elder care responsibilities. As a result, employees who are not married, do
not have children, or may not have the responsibility of caring for aging parents are
omitted from study, yet these employees may still perceive stress resulting from other
types of work/non-work conflict. The purpose of the present study is to address some of
results from the process of trying to juggle one’s work and personal life.
The remainder of this paper will describe the present study undertaken to develop
the construct of work/personal life balance (also referred to as work/life balance). The
a description of the present study, the hypotheses to be tested in this study, and a
description of the methodology employed to test those hypotheses. Finally, the statistical
Many changes in the workplace and in employees' lives have taken place over the
past couple decades and have led to an increase in the attention paid to work/non-work
conflict (Parcel, 1999). In the workplace, an increasing number of women are joining the
workforce, with the largest segment including single mothers (Lerner 1994; Zedeck,
increasing number of hours that employees work, compared to the traditional 40-hour
work week (Casner-Lotto & Hickey, 1999; Figart & Golden. 2000). In addition, workers
often telecommute (i.e., work from home) or bring work home, thus blurring the
boundaries between work and home (Hill, Miller, Weiner, & Colihan, 1998; Shamir,
support employees' needs, such as on-site childcare (Rothausen, Gonzalez, Clarke, &
O'Dell, 1998) and flexible work arrangements (including flexibility in time and location
women are delaying childbirth until their late thirties or early forties because of career
concerns (Ventura, Martin, Curtin, & Mathews, 1998). An increasing number of men
and women are adopting a more egalitarian approach to work and family issues, breaking
down traditional gender roles in work and family (Barnett & Rivers, 1996; Parcel, 1999).
paramount (Katz & Kahn, 1978). One’s work is one aspect of one’s life, and therefore
the examination of one’s work life should take into account the context in which that
4
work takes place, including family and other personal concerns (Kopelman, Greenhaus,
Connolly, 1983; Schein, 1976). When one’s work life interferes with his/her life outside
of work or one’s personal life negatively impacts his/her work, a particular source of
understand how work/non-work conflict may be considered a stressor, the next section
provides a definition and overview of the organizational stress process. Following the
Organizational Stress
Definition of Stress
term “stress.” For example, a stimulus definition focuses on stressors, such as events or
experience” (Smith & Sulsky, in press). In the context of work stress, the stressor may be
work. In addition, work stress is also concerned with non-work related events or
the stimulus or stressor, but rather with how an individual reacts to the stressor (Selye,
1965). This definition of stress is also referred to as the medical definition or approach.
long-term or sometimes chronic changes called strains (Smith & Sulsky, in press).
5
Examples of strains include long-term changes in behavior, attitudes, and health (e.g.,
A third definition of stress combines the first two approaches and is referred to as
a stimulus-response definition. This approach takes into account the interaction between
a stimulus (i.e., a stressor) and an individual's response to that stimulus. For the purpose
of the present study, stress is defined as “any circumstance (stressor) that places special
underscores the role of cognitive appraisal in the stress process. Specifically, Lazarus
purports that a particular event or situation is not a stressor unless it is appraised as such
A number of heuristic models of stress have been proposed (e.g., Beehr &
Newman, 1978; Edwards, 1992; Katz & Kahn, 1978). Although these models vary with
regard to specific elements, each acknowledges the dynamic nature of the stressor-strain
relationship such that stress is a process rather than a discrete event. Examples of these
models include the social environmental model of stress (French & Kahn. 1962; Katz &
Kahn, 1978), Beehr & Newman’s (1978) conceptual model of work stress, and
In total, these heuristic models indicate that stress is a dynamic process that takes
well as characteristics of his/her work and non-work environment (e.g., role demands at
work, role demands outside of work) and may lead to a number of potential outcomes for
both the individual and the organization for which he/she works.
Such heuristic models are useful for understanding stress as a process in general.
However, these models are not specific enough to yield thorough understanding of the
stress process, especially with regard to understanding the impact of any particular
employees’ work and non-work lives as a potential stressor. In order to better understand
conceptualizations. Among the theories or models described in this research are role
follows.
Role Theory
In the late 1970’s, work/non-work conflict research began with the application of
conflict (Kopelman et al., 1983; Quinn & Staines, 1979). Role theory was first
introduced in the 1950s by sociologists such as Parsons (1951) and Merton (1957),
7
although Kahn, Wolfe, Quinn, Snoek, and Rosenthal (1964) were the first to discuss role
theory in the context of work organizations. Role theory is based on the premise that an
roles outside of work. Role conflict occurs when two (or more) sets of pressures occur at
the same time such that compliance with one set makes it more difficult to comply with
the other set (Kahn et al., 1964). Objective role conflict is a product of the environment,
et al. (1964) suggest that interrole conflict will be greater if employees have multiple
roles. Multiple roles may lead to psychological conflict if each role cannot be adequately
fulfilled.
stress research (King & King, 1990; Smith & Sulsky, in press). This approach has been
related to work/non-work conflict research based on the notion that conflict experienced
between one’s work role and one’s role outside of work is a salient stressor. However,
reliance on role theory in work-family research has been criticized for a few reasons
(Grandey & Cropanzano, 1999; Zedeck & Mosier, 1990). First, Kahn et al.'s (1964) role
theory is primarily focused on roles in the context of the work environment and does not
address family or other non-work roles. Secondly, this theory fails to articulate variables
that may moderate the relationship between work and family stressors and stress
Spillover Model
In addition to the role theory approach to studying work and non-work aspects of
employees' lives, a number of other models appear widely throughout the work/non-work
8
conflict literature. These include the spillover model (Staines, 1980), the congruence
model (Morf, 1989), the compensation model (Staines, 1980), and the segmentation
The spillover model purports that there is a correspondence between what occurs
at work and what occurs in one’s life outside of work (Staines, 1980). That is, what
happens at work can “spill over” and affect one’s non-work life, or vice versa in which
aspects of one’s personal life can spill over and affect his/her work life. For example,
this approach suggests that satisfaction with one’s work leads to satisfaction at home.
Spillover has been conceptualized in both a positive and a negative way (e.g.,
Grzywacz, 2000; Grzywacz & Marks, 2000). Positive spillover refers to enhancement
that results from one's work life affecting his/her family life or vice-versa. An example
of positive spillover was previously mentioned (i.e., satisfaction in one domain leads to
satisfaction in the other domain). Negative spillover, on the other hand, refers to conflict
between work and non-work, such that negative aspects of one’s work affect non-work in
a negative manner. For example, long hours and psychological stress at work could lead
Most of the extant work-family research has relied upon a spillover explanation of
the relationships between aspects of work and family (e.g., Williams & Alliger, 1994;
Zedeck & Mosier, 1990). One of the problems with the spillover model, however, is this
notion of positive and negative spillover: One may experience both positive and negative
spillover at the same time (Gryzwacz & Marks, 2000). Research applying the spillover
theory to work-family conflict has yet to address this complication. For example, a job
with long hours or a great deal of travel may take away from time one spends doing non
9
work activities. However, the same job may also be financially rewarding and personally
satisfying to the employee, which can have a positive impact on one’s family.
Congruence Model
The congruence model described by Morf (1989) is similar to the spillover model
in that this model also postulates a positive relationship between work and non-work
roles. However, the congruence model attributes the cause for the relationship to a third
variable, whereas spillover is based on the notion that one domain directly influences
another (Morf, 1989; Staines, 1980; Zedeck & Mosier, 1990). In other words, this model
purports a positive relation between one's work and family life based on a third variable
underlying each domain (e.g., personality variables, genetic factors, and social or cultural
interpret situations in a negative way (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988). For example,
an individual may seem dissatisfied with his/her job as well as with his/her personal life,
and this simultaneous dissatisfaction in both domains could be due to the individual
having a strong negative affective disposition that impacts his/her perceptions in both
domains.
Compensation Model
understanding the relations between one’s work life and his/her life outside of work is the
compensation model (Staines, 1980). This approach suggests that what an individual is
missing in one domain, she will seek to find (i.e., compensate for) in another domain.
10
For example, an employee who finds her job to be boring may seek opportunities to do
something she finds exciting when not at work. In other words, the compensation model
Segmentation Model
A fourth model articulating the relations between work and non-work variables is
the segmentation model (Evans & Bartolomé, 1984). According to this model, work and
non-work domains are separate from one another, and as a result, there is no relationship
between what an employee does at work and what he does when not at work. In other
words, this model suggests that employees can compartmentalize their lives such that
work is separated from non-work in time, space, and function (Zedeck, 1992). As a
result, one's work remains at work, and their non-work life does not affect work or is not
affected by work.
Although previous research has found empirical support for each of the models
just described (spillover, congruence, compensation, and segmentation; e.g., Frone et al.,
1992; Hart, 1999; Williams & Alliger, 1994), each model has been criticized for a
number of reasons (Grandey & Cropanzano, 1999; Zedeck, 1992; Zedeck & Mosier,
1990). One criticism is that these models are “atheoretical,” not specifying boundary
by each of the models. For example, Zedeck and Mosier (1990) raised the question
regarding whether more than one model may be applicable to an individual and whether
spillover and compensation can occur simultaneously or in a sequential order (e.g., can
11
one experience spillover for some factors, and seek to compensate for others?). Some
empirical support has been found for all four models, although among them, most
research has been based on the spillover model (Small & Riley, 1990; Zedeck & Mosier,
1990). Grandey & Cropanzano (1999) pointed out that these models are limited in that
they have not been integrated into a single, comprehensive theory to guide work/life
research.
research examining employees' work and non-work lives (Grandey & Cropanzano,
1999). In 1988, Hobfoll proposed the conservation of resources theory as a new theory
of stress. This theory is based on the notion that “people have an innate as well as
learned desire to conserve the quality and quantity of their resources and to limit any state
gain expected resources (Hobfoll 1988; 1989). “Resources” are supplies that can help or
possessions, and examples include food, water, shelter, and a vehicle. Personal
resiliency that serves as a source of resistance to stress (Rush, Schoel, & Barnard, 1995).
Conditions refer to states that are considered desirable or that one seeks. Social support
12
is an example of a condition that is sought and can buffer one against stress
(Viswesvaran, Sanchez, & Fisher, 1999). Various types of social support exist, including
support from one’s supervisor, coworker(s), or spouse. The fourth type of resource is
energies, which refers to the means for attaining such resources. Examples of energies
include time, knowledge, or physical energy. This latter category includes particularly
salient resources with regard to meeting the multiple demands of one’s work and non
The conservation of resources model takes into account that stress is a process
(Edwards, 1992; Jex & Beehr, 1991; Smith & Sulsky, in press). Specifically, it is an
experienced loss of resources, or the threat of a loss of resources, that initiates the stress
process. The cognitive evaluation of resources here is similar to the cognitive appraisal
process described by Lazarus (1966) and his colleagues (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984), such
results from the process of balancing one's work and non-work life. As Grandey and
Cropanzano (1999) have indicated, the conservation of resources model is useful for
theory', interrole conflict between one's work role and his/her non-work role leads to
strains because one loses resources when try ing to perform both work and non-work
roles. This actual or potential loss of resources can lead to what Hobfoll (1989) terms a
strains.
13
When comparing work and non-work roles. Edwards and Rothbard (2000) have
indicated that there is a negative relationship between one’s work and non-work
resources such that resources used in one domain limit their use in another domain. Time
attempting to meet both work and non-work role demands. For example, the more time
one spends doing work, the less time is available for non-work activities. Edwards and
Rothbard (2000) point out that this negative relationship may be difficult to predict
because one’s non-work life includes multiple domains (e.g., family, community
activities, and personal hobbies/activities). In order to allocate more time to work, one
may spend less time sleeping or attending to one’s own, non-work activities.
How one allocates his/her resources may or may not be a conscious choice. For
example, one may choose to spend more time at work to complete a particular project in
time for a deadline. Or, one may be dealing with unforeseen non-work issues (e.g.,
family problems or the death of a close friend) that prevents one from focusing on work
Summary
These include role theory, spillover, congruence, compensation, segmentation, and the
experience interrole conflict between his or her work and non-work roles as a stressor.
Although many work/non-work conflict studies have been conceptualized based on role
theory, this approach has been criticized for its focus on roles within the work
environment and failure to lead to specific predictions about role conflict. The spillover,
14
congruence, compensation, and segmentation models have each served as a basis for
some previous research, but have been criticized for making competing predictions that
are not falsifiable. In addition, these models are more descriptive than prescriptive. In
other words, they describe relationships between work and non-work variables, but do
1988; 1989) suggests that an individual experiences stress in the event of resource loss.
Although this model is a descriptive theory not unlike the spillover, segmentation, and
stress reactions that result from work/non-work conflict because time and energy are
salient, limited resources necessary for achieving one's work and non-work goals.
Work/Eamily Conflict
Over the past two decades, an increased number of empirical studies have
examined work/non-work conflict (Allen, Herst, Bruck. & Sutton, 2000; Kossek &
Ozeki, 1998; Zedeck, 1992). The vast majority of this research has focused on “family”
work/family conflict. Work/family conflict occurs when work pressures and family
pressures occur at the same time such that compliance with pressures in one domain (e.g.,
work) makes it more difficult to comply with pressures in another domain (e.g., family).
Most work/family conflict research to date developed and tested antecedents and
outcomes of work/family conflict ( Frone et al., 1992; Frone, et al., 1997; Grandey &
Kopelman et al. (1983) were among the first to systematically examine the
construct of work-family conflict. They began by defining three role conflict variables:
work conflict, family conflict, and interrole conflict. Work conflict is the extent to which
one experiences incompatible role pressures within the work domain. Similarly, family
conflict is the extent to which one experiences incompatible role pressures with regard to
family. Interrole conflict occurs when one experiences pressures of one role that are
discordant with pressures in another role. Kopelman et al. (1983) developed three scales
to measure each of these constructs and assessed the scale validity with two data samples.
Kopelman et al.’s (1983) results did provide evidence of construct validity for
three dimensions of conflict. However, the only outcomes they examined were job
satisfaction, family satisfaction, and life satisfaction. Specifically, results of their path
analyses indicated a negative relationship between work conflict and job satisfaction, and
a negative relationship between family conflict and family satisfaction. Work conflict
and family conflict were modestly correlated. Work conflict predicted interrole conflict in
both samples, although family conflict predicted interrole conflict in only one of two
samples.
A number of other researchers (e.g., Grandey & Cropanzano, 1999; Greenhaus &
Beutell, 1985; Greenhaus & Parasuraman, 1986; Frone et al., 1992) built upon Kopelman
et al.’s work by developing and testing additional models of work-family conflict. For
articulated three potential sources of work-family conflict: time based conflict, strain-
based conflict, and behavior-based conflict. T ime-based conflict occurs when time spent
in one role (based on the number of hours worked, inflexibility in work scheduling, and
16
shiftwork) leads to difficulty in fulfilling another role. Strain-based conflict occurs when
strain such as tension, anxiety, irritability, etc. from one role make it difficult to perform
a second role. Behavior-based conflict takes place when behavior (e.g., expectations) in
one role lead to difficulty in meeting the requirements of another role. Greenhaus and
Beutell (1985) focused on potential sources of interrole conflict between work and non
work only to the extent that aspects of the work role are likely to be directly related to
interrole conflict. In addition, they only focused on interrole conflict with family, not
other non-work roles (e.g., leisure). These researchers proposed a new model of work-
family conflict based on their review, though they did not empirically test the model.
demonstrating that the work-family interface is bi-directional, such that work interferes
with family, and family interferes with work. They found that job stressors (e.g., lack of
autonomy, work pressure, and role ambiguity) predicted work-to-family conflict and
family stressors (including both parental stressors and marital stressors) predicted family-
distress and depression, although work-to-family conflict did not predict depression or
family distress.
Their model identified aspects of employees’ work and home situations that led to work
and family role stress, and work-to-family conflict and family-to-work conflict. Based on
a survey among university professors, they found that age and tenure predicted work-to-
family conflict such that younger individuals report more work-to-family conflict. In
17
addition, marital status, number of children at home, and gender explained 22% of the
variance in family role stress, and 26% of the variance in family-to-work conflict such
that those with more children, unmarried individuals, and women reported more family
that work-to-family conflict predicted job distress, and family-to-work conflict predicted
family distress. Job distress, but not family distress, significantly predicted turnover
intentions, life distress, and poor physical health. Although Grandey and Cropanzano
other studies have found that the children's age is an important determinant regarding
one’s ability to meet work and family demands (Bedeian, Burke, & Moffett, 1988;
work/family conflict. Two recent meta-analyses integrated and summarized the findings
regarding work/family conflict outcomes. First, Kossek and Ozeki (1998) published a
meta-analysis of two of the most frequently studied outcomes: job satisfaction and life
satisfaction. Their study found that higher levels of work/family conflict are associated
with decreases in both job and life satisfaction. Secondly, Allen et al. (2000) reviewed
previous research regarding outcomes of work/family conflict, and included many more
Allen et al. (2000) categorized work/family conflict outcomes into three groups:
outcomes, higher levels of work/family conflict are associated with decreased job
increased absenteeism and turnover intentions, and decreased job performance. Non
Kossek & Ozeki, 1998). Stress-related outcomes include general psychological strain,
somatic and physical symptom complaints, depression, substance abuse, burnout, work-
related stress, and family-related stress (Adams, King & King, 1996; Allen et al., 2000;
Burke, 1988; Frone et al., 1992; Greenhaus & Beutell, 1985; Netemeyer. Boles, &
A few studies to date have examined whether there are gender differences in
path of the model of work-family conflict articulated by Kopelman et al. (1983). In this
model, work conflict and family conflict are related constructs that each lead to interrole
conflict, and in turn lead to job satisfaction and family satisfaction, which each lead to
life satisfaction. Work conflict is the extent to which one experiences incompatible role
pressures within the work domain. Similarly, family conflict is the extent to which one
type of interrole conflict that occurs when one experiences pressures of one role (e.g.,
work) that are discordant with pressures in another role (e.g., family; Kopelman et al.,
1983).
Duxbury & Higgins (1991) tested for differences between men and women in
perceptions of work involvement and work conflict, work involvement with work-family
conflict, family involvement and family conflict, family involvement with work-family
conflict, work expectation and work-family conflict, work expectations with work
conflict, family expectations with family conflict, each type of expectations with work-
Their results were mixed, such that there were statistically significant differences
between men and women in 11 of 17 gender comparisons. Little support was found for
their hypotheses of gender differences in predictors of work and family conflict, and the
authors attributed the lack of differences to societal expectations and behavioral norms.
However, they found support for most of their hypotheses regarding predictors of work-
family conflict. As predicted, the relationship between work involvement and work-
family conflict was stronger for women than for men, and similarly the relationship
between family involvement and work-family conflict was stronger for men than for
women. With regard to predictors of quality of work life and quality of family life, all
but one gender hypothesis was supported. For example, the relationship between work
conflict and quality of work life, and the relationship between work-family conflict and
quality of family life was much stronger for men than for women. Among women, the
relationship between work-family conflict and quality of work life is stronger than for
men. However, there were no significant gender differences in the relationship between
Although this study was prompted by the belief that gender roles are changing as
more women enter the workforce and more men assist with family responsibilities,
Duxbury and Higgins (1991) concluded that societal expectations regarding gender role
expectations have not changed as much as expected over the past few decades. However,
this study is now over a decade old, and it is possible that one might find slightly
different results as societal expectations and behavioral norms regarding gender roles
continue to change. For example, the trend toward increases in the number of on-site day
care programs (Rothausen et al., 1998) and use of flexible work arrangements (e.g.,
alternative work schedules and telecommuting) during the 1990s suggest that additional
research is warranted.
More recently. Eagle, Miles, & Icenogle (1997) sought to determine if there are
gender differences in the extent to which employees’ work demands are permitted to
intrude into the family role and vice-versa. These researchers found that work-to-family
conflict was more prevalent than family-to-work conflict, and that there were no
the work-family interface, it seems that perceptions of work/family issues may be related
variables, a review of empirical research would not be complete without discussing the
measurement of the construct. Measures of work-family conflict have varied from study
21
to study. For example, some have employed single item measures (e.g., asking the extent
to which work and family interfere with one another; Quinn & Staines, 1979). Other
work/family researchers (e.g.. Burke, 1988) have used lengthy scales. A number of
work/family conflict measures (e.g.. Kopelman et al., 1983; Quinn & Staines, 1979) have
not accounted for the bi-directionality of work/family conflict (i.e., work interferes w ith
family and family interferes with work; Netemeyer et al. 1996) More recently,
researchers (e.g., Carlson, Kacmar, & Williams, 2000; Netemeyer et al., 1996) have
developed scales of work/family conflict that take into account the bi-directional nature
work/family conflict scale items refer to “home life” or “things I want to do at home,”
whereas others include the words “family life,” “family responsibilities,” and “family or
household chores or time for one’s self, rather than as specific to family. Secondly, it is
difficult to discern what, exactly, is meant by family. Does family refer to one’s spouse,
one’s children, or perhaps one’s parents? Use of the word “family” may pose problems
for survey respondents who must interpret survey questions, given increasing demands on
employees to deal with issues such as elder care (Hammer, Allen, & Grigsby, 1999;
(Sweet, 1989).
religious/spiritual or activities that interfere with work or with which work interferes. As
a result, employees may experience work/non-work conflict even without having family
responsibilities.
personal life interfere with work, we cannot say that we understand the complete
of work/non-work conflict that employees face by only examining “family” in the non
work domain. This also suggests that a more generic measure, one that assesses the
Recently a broader term has emerged in organizations and the popular press to
refer to work/non-work conflict. This new term is work/life balance, and holds promise
work/family conflict.
trade journals (e.g., Casner-Lotto & Hickey, 1999; Messmer, 1999) rather than empirical
research journals. In a recent article in the trade journal. Business Credit. Messmer
(1999) highlighted work/life balance as a tool that organizations can use to attract quality
work/life balance” (p. 55) because “employees are interested in making their careers
compatible with what is important in their personal lives” (p. 55). Shellenbarger (1997)
found that job candidates now ask questions about work/life balance in first round talks
23
with employers, as opposed to years past when these questions were saved for the final
such that workers have an increased desire for a better quality of life. Specifically, they
found that in the past few years, employees have indicated that they want more time to do
volunteer work and pursue hobbies and leisure activities. In addition, these researchers
reported that workers desire more time to spend with family, and that fewer employees
seem willing to sacrifice their family or personal time in order to advance in their careers.
Jeff Hill and his colleagues (Hill et al., 1998) are among the only researchers to
publish their findings on work/life balance in an empirical research journal. Hill et al.
(1998) sought to examine the influence of a virtual office environment (i.e., telework, or
working from home) on aspects of work and work/life balance at IBM. They conducted a
results based on thematic content coding of written survey comments were somewhat
mixed. More employees indicated that working in a virtual office blurred the boundary
between home and work than the number of employees who indicated a virtual office was
main effect in terms of a difference between the virtual office and traditional office
Although Hill et al. (1998) appear to take a broader approach to the study of
for work/life balance. The specific measure of work/life balance used in the Hill et al.
(1998) was not identified. Other articles on work/life balance mention the term, but fail
work/family conflict research are married or have children, and as a result, employees
who are not married and do not have children are usually omitted. Although work/life
needs to be conducted to understand what work/life balance is, identify what other
variables are related to work/life balance, and what inferences can be drawn as a result of
purpose of the present research is to fill this gap by first defining work/life balance, and
measure.
Summary
conflict. Previous empirical work/family conflict research developed and tested models
that have identified a number of work- and non-work related antecedents and outcomes
work involvement, family involvement, amount of time spent at work, amount of time
25
spent with family, role overload, work stressors, family stressors, age, marital status,
absenteeism and turnover intentions, and decreased job performance. Non-work related
stress. Measures of work/family conflict have varied from study to study, including
single- and multi-item measures. More recently researchers have developed multi
understanding of what leads to and results from work/family conflict, our knowledge of
the non-work domain. More recently, the term work/life balance has emerged in a few
journals and in the popular press. This term seems more inclusive of various activities in
one's personal life and is positively-worded (i.e., balance) rather than negatively-worded
(e.g., conflict).
an appropriate measure of the construct. The purpose of the present study is to fill this
The primary purpose of the present study is to define and measure the construct of
because it encompasses more than just family in the non-work domain. A broader
construct to examine work/non-work conflict will help to close the gap in the existing
literature and provide organizational researchers with a useful, valid measure of work/life
domain of interest, identifying a way to assess the domain, and understanding how the
construct is related to other constructs (Cook & Campbell, 1976; Cronbach & Meehl,
1955). This process is undertaken to assess construct validity (i.e., understanding what is
being measured).
definition (Cook & Campbell, 1976; Kopelman et al., 1983). In order to effectively do
this, the author undertook an exploratory, preliminary study to better understand the term
about their perceptions of work/life balance by one of three trained researchers. All of
these participants worked in a variety of occupations and types of organizations, and all
but two were employed full-time. The sample of participants also included both
individuals who were married and/or had children, as well as many who were single and
employees with immediate family (e.g., spouse and/or children), it was important to
questions pertaining to work/life balance (See Appendix A for a complete list of the
open-ended questions). For example, individuals were asked to indicate what the term,
“work/life balance,” means to them, and how they can tell if they have a balance between
inductive approach, generated a list of content themes that emerged from the data. The
panel members subsequently discussed the results, agreed upon content coding
consistency in coding responses), results were averaged across raters for interpretation.
Based on data obtained in this study, work/life balance was found to mean
different things to different employees. For example, many employees saw balance in
terms of how they spend or allocate their time, such as the amount of time they spend at
work versus doing things outside of work. Other employees said balance referred to their
ability to accomplish both their work and personal goals. In addition to time, many
goals) and/or non-work tasks (e.g.. goals for accomplishing things not related to work).
For example, one respondent defined work/life balance as “efforts directed toward
28
maintaining a happy and healthy life while pursuing a challenging and satisfying career.”
Other employees defined this balance as “successfully coordinating the time requirements
either,” “when you are able to leave your job at the office and come home to spend time
doing the things that you enjoy (e.g., hobbies, working out), find time to socialize, and
keep your life in order (e.g., have groceries in the fridge, bills paid, obligations met).”
This preliminary study suggested two dimensions of work/life balance. The first
dimension of work/life balance is time, such as how much time is spent at work,
compared to how much time is spent engaged in other activities. The second dimension
refers to behavior, such as work goal accomplishment, as work/life balance is based one’s
belief that he is able to accomplish what he would like at work and in his personal life.
conflict: time, strain, and behavior (Greenhaus & Beutell, 1985; Carlson et al., 2000).
expectations for how one should act in one role compared to another (Greenhaus &
behavior that was salient to employees in the qualitative work/life balance investigation.
identified in previous research and findings among interviews with employees in the
preliminary study.
In addition to time and goal accomplishment, there are two additional issues that
seem relevant to defining work/life balance. One of these is strain, based on previous
29
work/non-work conflict research that defined strain as the third source of interrole
conflict (i.e., in addition to time and behavior; Greenhaus & Beutell, 1985). Examples of
strains include anxiety, tension, having to miss important personal activities, and
considered when defining work/life balance. Although previous research has not
explicitly cited energy (or lack thereof) as a source of interrole conflict, the conservation
of resources model suggests that energy is a salient resource, and therefore a lack of
energy due to work or non-work demands may lead to an increase in perceived stress.
The rationale for including energy in the definition of work/life balance is consistent with
the notion of time; energy is a limited resource and relevant to employees being able to
with employees about work/life balance, there are four components relevant to defining
work/life balance: time, behavior (e.g., goal accomplishment), strain, and energy. Each
1988; 1989). Again, this model purports that stress is a reaction to an environment in
model, time and energy are examples of salient and limited resources. In addition, strain
experienced as a direct result of a loss of time or energy may lead to a loss of resources or
This model suggests that stress will result if individuals perceive that they lose
resources or devote resources to a task or activity without getting what they expect in
return. It follows that perceived stress is likely to result from a lack of work/life balance,
desired goals at work and in her life outside of work but does not reach both those goals,
she may perceive a lack of work/life balance. To the extent that she strives to regain or
attain balance, she is likely to expend rather than conserve resources. If she actually
loses time or energy, faces a potential loss of time or energy, or fails to gain what she
expects given the resources that she previously expended, then this stress may lead to any
satisfaction, etc.).
“balance” for any one individual is in the eye of the beholder (Lazarus, 1966; Lazarus &
Folkman, 1984). For example, spending fifty hours per week at work and the remainder
employee, but not for another. The qualitative research finding that work/life balance
The first step in developing a construct is to define the domain of interest. The
second step is to identify a way to assess this domain. As part of the present study, 31
self-report survey items were written incorporating time, energy, strain, and goal
review of existing measures of work/family conflict and using responses gathered from
the preliminary study. Examples of items including time, energy, strain, and goal
doing activities not related to work,” “My personal life drains me of the energy I need to
and “I am able to accomplish what I would like in both my personal and work lives.”
The present study involved two stages of scale development to assess the internal
consistency reliability and validity of these new items. In the first step, the items were
determine the extent to which these items are consistent with the conceptual definition of
the work/life balance construct (i.e., content validity). Based on subject matter expert
ratings of each item, scale items that were perceived to be relevant to the work/life
balance construct were administered during the second step: a mail survey including the
employees. Data obtained from this survey were used to assess validity for this new
measure, specifically by examining the psychometric properties of the scale and its
balance and other constructs. These hypotheses were tested to assess the construct
validity of the work/life balance measure. Construct validity assesses the extent to which
the scale measures what it purports to measure (i.e., the extent to which the measure
Cronbach & Meehl, 1955). Evidence for convergent validity was evaluated by
constructs, such as work/family conflict and other stressors (e.g., time pressure and role
extent to which work/life balance predicts a number of related outcomes or strains (e.g.,
Hypotheses
process (Cook & Campbell, 1976; Guion. 1998). Convergent validity refers to finding
work/family conflict are both defined as stressors that employees may face when trying
to manage their work and non-work responsibilities, measures of the two constructs
According to role theory, each role that an individual has includes a set of
behaviors to be performed (Kahn et al., 1964). The more roles one takes on or is
responsible for, the more role-related tasks or goals exist that need to be accomplished.
Role overload refers to too many behaviors that need to be performed as part of one’s
role(s). Work/life balance is the perception that one can accomplish both his work and
non-work goals, and these goals likely include the behavior in each role set. The more
33
tasks one must complete as part of a role (role overload), the less likely he is to perceive a
Time pressure is a specific stressor in which individuals perceive that they have a
limited amount of time to accomplish a task or set of tasks. Based on the conservation of
resources model, employees who perceive more time pressure will perceive a lower
Employees who are better at managing their time will be better at allocating their
time in accordance with their priorities, and therefore more effective in terms of
conserv ing time as a limited resource. This is consistent with Adams and Jex’s (1999)
finding that time management behaviors were directly and indirectly related to
work and non-work goals, and effective time management behavior is positively related
to time and task accomplishment, work/life balance should be positively correlated with
work/life balance should correspond with greater feelings of overall job stress.
34
Job satisfaction refers to the affective evaluation one has about his/her job
(Cranny, Smith, & Stone, 1992). Based on the definition of work/life balance, part of the
construct refers to accomplishing one’s goals at work. To the extent that one is able to do
so, and/or their job does not interfere with being able to accomplish non-work goals, an
employee is more likely to be satisfied with his/her job. Based on previous work/family
conflict research that has demonstrated a relationship between work/family conflict and
job satisfaction (e.g., Allen et al., 2000; Kossek & Ozeki, 1998), a similar relationship is
evaluation one holds about his/her life in general. Previous research on life satisfaction
considers this construct to be an indicator of overall quality of life (Duxbury & Higgins,
1991; Sekaran, 1985). Work/life balance literature has indicated that many employees
want both a fulfilling career and fulfilling personal life (Shellenbarger, 1997). Therefore,
for employees who desire both a career and a satisfying personal life, employees who are
able to achieve a work/life balance are more likely to be satisfied with their lives in
general.
Previous research has indicated that the desire for both a fulfilling career and
(Mathieu & Farr, 1991). Employees who can obtain the desired balance between work
35
and their personal life will be more committed to the organization that employs them
compared to employees who do not perceive that they have a work/life balance. In
turnover, many steps are involved in an employee's decision to leave an organization and
follow through (i.e., actually carry out the behavior). An employee's intention to quit is
the last step in this process before actually leaving, and therefore a better indicator of
turnover than other variables (e.g., job satisfaction, organizational commitment). This
prediction is also consistent with the rationale for the expected relationship between
personal life is more inclusive of non-work activities than work/family conflict, which
only focuses on the negative impact of work on family and vice-versa. To the extent that
other aspects of one’s personal life (i.e., salient activities or commitments in addition to
family) can affect one's work life or be affected by work, a more inclusive measure of the
interface between one's work and personal life will be empirically distinct from a more
work/life balance and work/family conflict are expected to be empirically similar, I also
36
hypothesize that work/life balance will account for some variance in other stressors and
The purpose of the present study was to develop and validate a measure of
work/life balance for use in organizational research. Work/life balance items were
written based on data gathered from interviews with employees, suggestions by a panel of
work family researchers, a review of existing measures of work/family conflict, and the
construct definition. First, these items were assessed with regard to content validity by a
panel of subject matter experts (SMEs). That is, these raters assessed the extent to which
each item seemed relevant to the construct of work/life balance, based on the conceptual
involving time, energy, strain, and goal accomplishment). In addition, SMEs were given
an opportunity to generate additional items that they perceive as relevant to the construct.
Items that were judged by multiple raters as relevant to the construct domain were
Data collected using the survey were used to assess the psychometric properties of the
scale using two samples: an initial sample, and a holdout sample. The hypotheses
presented earlier were tested to establish evidence of construct validity for the new'
Participants
graduate students, faculty, and practitioners, all of which have experience developing
Procedure
Netemeyer et al., 1996), the author wrote a total of 31 work/life balance scale items.
positive aspects of the work/personal life interface (e.g., Duxbury & Higgins, 1991;
MacDermid et al., 2000), these items sought to measure employees' perceptions of the
extent to which work interferes with personal life, personal life interferes with work, as
well as to what extent work enhances personal life, and personal life enhances work.
Items incorporated aspects of time, energy, strain, and behavior based on the conceptual
Each SME was given a paper and pencil survey listing the conceptual definition
of work/life balance, the 31 items, and a rating scale. Using the same procedure as
Netemeyer et al. (1996), SMEs rated each item based on the extent to which it is “2=Very
work/life balance content domain. The SMEs were also given an opportunity to generate
Measures
included 31 items written to assess perceptions of work/life balance as they relate to time,
behavior, strain, and energy. A complete list of these items is presented in Appendix B.
39
Item discrimination ability. Four items from Paulhus’s (1984; 1988) Balanced
Inventory of Desirable Responding were included among the work/life balance items to
verify the SMEs’ ability to identify and rate the new scale items appropriately. The
faster than the speed limit,” “I dropped litter on the street at least once in my life,” and “I
sometimes tell lies if I have to.” Item rating responses from any raters who indicated that
at least one of these four items was “somewhat relevant” or “very relevant" to the
construct of work/life balance were removed from further analyses. In other words,
ratings were only retained from subject matter experts who were able to discriminate
between items purported to be relevant to work/life balance and those that were not.
Results
I discarded responses from three SMEs who identified at least one of the social
Items for which all of the twelve remaining raters indicated that the items were somewhat
or very representative were retained for use in the second study; 7 items (Item 1,3, 14,
15, 17, 25, and 31 as listed in Appendix B) were discarded. All items retained for
subsequent scale validation had mean item ratings of M = 2.6 or higher on the 3-point
item rating scale. Two raters suggested slight wording changes to two of the items. The
24 items that remained as a result of the content rating exercise were incorporated in
Study Two. A list of all items generated and retained for inclusion in Study Two appears
in Table 1.
40
Table 1
Item
1. I struggle with trying to juggle both my work and non-work responsibilities.
2. My personal life drains me of the energy I need to do my job.
3. I feel overwhelmed when I try to balance my work and personal life.
4. My work suffers because of everything going on in my personal life.
5. I am too tired to be effective at work because of things I have going on in my personal
life.
6. My job gives me energy to pursue activities outside of work that are important to me.
7. I have difficulty scheduling vacation time because of my workload.
8. I am unable to relax at home because I am preoccupied with my work.
9. I am happy with the amount of time I spend doing activities not related to work.
10. Because of my job, I am in a better mood at home.
11.1 often have to make difficult choices between my work and my personal life
12.1 have to put aspects of my personal life “on hold” because of my work.
13.1 am able to accomplish what I would like in both my personal and work lives.
14. I am in a better mood at work because of everything I have going for me in my
personal life.
15.1 often neglect my personal needs because of the demands of my work.
16. When I am at work. I worry about things I need to do outside of work.
17. I have difficulty getting my work done because I am preoccupied with personal
matters at work.
18. My personal life suffers because of my work.
19.1 have to miss out on important personal activities due to the amount of time I spend
doing work.
20. My personal life gives me the energy to do my job.
21.1 feel that I allocate appropriate amounts of time to both work and non-work activities.
22.1 make personal sacrifices to get work done.
23.1 come home from work too tired to do things I would like to do.
24. My job makes it difficult to maintain the kind of personal life I would like.
41
Study Two
An eight-page survey was sent via mail to 10,206 members of the American
sponsor this study to learn more about work/life balance perceptions among their
membership. Accompanying the survey was a cover letter written by the AMA Director
of Research (see Appendix C). No incentive, other than availability of survey results,
employees. Although the majority (i.e., 70%) of AMA members are male, females were
disproportionately sampled for inclusion in the present study. Of the 10,206 managers
sampled, 50% were female and 50% were male. An equal sample of males and females
was sought in the present study because previous work/non-work conflict research has
more females included in the final sample, thus providing enough statistical power to
A total of 545 questionnaires were completed and returned for inclusion in this
next phase of scale development. 482 surveys were returned upon the initial survey
mailing (4% response rate). To increase the response rate, a second copy of the survey
was sent to 1,000 managers. This group of n=l ,000 was randomly selected among the
42
second mailing, yielding a total sample size of N=545, and an overall survey response
rate of 5%. Due to missing data, 540 questionnaires could be used for analysis.
The sample of 540 usable surveys was divided approximately in half in order to
create a holdout sample for cross-validation of the work/life balance scale. As a result,
267 (49.4% of total sample) respondents were included in an initial sample, and the
remaining 273 respondents (50.6% of total sample) were included in the holdout sample.
The sample of respondents consisted of 55% males and 45% females, which is
statistically different from the expected frequencies based on the survey mailing to a
sample of 50% males and 50% females (x“ (1) = 5.81, p < .05); slightly more males
responded than females. On average, respondents were between 45 and 49 years old.
Forty-nine percent of respondents have been employed by the same organization for at
least 10 years; 30% employed for 5-9 years; and 22% for less than 5 years. On average,
5, median = 50). The majority of respondents were married (77%). In addition, the
majority of respondents (79%) had obtained at least a bachelor’s degree, and had been
working for the same organization for at least five years (78.1%). Fifty-five percent of
respondents indicated that they have at least one child living in their household at least
50% of the time; 17% reported having no children living with them, and 28% left this
item blank. Among those who answered the question, 76% of the sample had at least one
presented in Table 2.
43
Table 2
Number of children
living in household at least
50% of the time 0 23.6
1 29.0
2 31.0
3 13.6
4 or more 2.8
Primary responsibility
for elder care Yes 12.0
No 88.0
44
Table 2 (continued)
Table 2 (continued)
Primary Area of
Responsibility Communications 3.4
Finance 12.7
General and Administrative Services 8.9
General Management 15.8
Human Resources 16.0
Information Systems & Technology 7.0
Insurance & Risk Management 3.0
International 0.2
Manufacturing 9.3
Marketing 8.0
Purchasing 3.4
Research & Development 5.9
Sales 4.6
Transportation & Distribution 1.7
With the exception of gender (as females were intentionally oversampled in the
membership population as a whole, as well as the sample of 10,206 to whom the survey
was mailed.
Measures
preliminary, exploratory study. Work/life balance items included in this survey consisted
of the items retained from Study One; a complete list of these items is presented in
Appendix D.
recommended the use of a time-based stem so that all respondents have the same frame
of reference for responding to the items. In the survey, respondents were asked to
indicate the frequency with which they have felt a particular way during the last three
months using a five-point scale: “Not at all," “Rarely," “Sometimes,” “Often,” and
“Almost All the Time." Respondents had the option of choosing a “Not Applicable"
response if the item did not pertain to them. “Not Applicable" responses were coded as
survey to serve as an initial validity check for the work/life balance scale items.
Respondents indicated the extent to which they agreed or disagreed with the statement,
“In general I feel that I have an adequate balance between my work and personal life,”
47
using a 5-point Likert-type rating scale ranging from “Strongly Agree” to “Strongly
Disagree.”
conflict. The work-to-family conflict scale consisted of 5 items assessing the negative
impact of work on family. Respondents indicated the extent to which they agreed or
disagreed with each item using a 5-point Likert-type rating scale, ranging from “Strongly
Agree” to “Strongly Disagree.” The internal consistency reliability (coefficient alpha) for
this scale was a = .90 in both the initial and holdout sample. A work-to-family conflict
scale score was computed for each respondent by taking the mean across all 5 items.
The family-to-work conflict scale assessed the extent to which one’s family
responsibilities interfere with work, using a separate five-item scale and the same
response format as the work-to-family items. The coefficient alphas for family-to-work
conflict were a = .88 and a = .89 in the initial and holdout samples, respectively.
Family-to-work scale scores were computed for each respondent by taking the mean
across all 5 items. See Appendix E for the work-to-family conflict and family-to-work
conflict measures.
Role overload. Role overload refers to having too much to do and was assessed
using a 3-item scale, adapted from Beehr, Walsh, & Taylor (1976). These items include
“I am given enough time to do what is expected of me on my job,” “It often seems like 1
have too much work for one person to do,” and “There is always more work to be done.”
Participants were asked to respond to these items using a 5-point Likert scale, ranging
from “Strongly Agree” to “Strongly Disagree.” An overall scale score for each
48
individual was computed by taking the average score across al, three items after reverse
coding the negatively-worded item. Coefficient alphas were a = .70 for the initial
Overall job stress. Overall job stress was assessed using the 15-item Stress-in-
General (S1G) scale (Stanton, et al., in press; Fisher et al., 2000). This measure is an
overall rather than a facet measure of job stress, and a measure that is widely applicable
rather than tied to specific stressors or strains. The SIG assesses two dimensions of
overall job stress. The first factor, entitled "Pressure," contains 7 items, such as
“Demanding,” “Hectic,” and “Many Things Stressful." The second factor, entitled
Previous research (e.g., Stanton et al., in press) found that when the SIG scale is
scored as two separate dimensions, both factors were correlated with a one-item measure
of general stress as well as specific facets of stress as measured by the Job Stress Index
(Sandman, 1992; Sandman & Smith, 1988). In a subsequent study, Stanton et al. (in
press) examined the relation between the SIG and physiological measures, and found
evidence to support the general or overall nature of the Pressure subscale and its
connection to chronic strains. In a follow-up study, Fisher et al. (2000) demonstrated that
both dimensions were measurably distinct from negative affectivity. Responses to each
of the 15-items on the SIG measure were recorded as “Y” for “Yes,” “N” for “No” and
“?” for “Cannot decide.” Scoring for the SIG is as follows: “N” = 0, “?” = 1, “Y” = 2 for
the positively worded items, and “N”=2, “?”=1 and “Y”=0 for the negatively-worded
items. A separate sum for each dimension of the scale was calculated. Higher scores
The SIG has demonstrated adequate internal consistency reliability for each
dimension in previous research. Coefficient alphas obtained for the Pressure and Threat
dimensions in the present study were consistent with previous findings (aprcssurc = .82, .78
and a,hreat = -82, .80 in the initial and holdout samples, respectively). See Appendix F for
Macan, Shahani, Dipboye, and Phillips’ (1990) 33-item self-report inventory of time
behaviors, including “goal setting and prioritizing,” “planning and scheduling," and
Goal setting and prioritizing is defined by Macan (1994) as “the setting of goals
concerning what the person wants or needs to accomplish and the prioritizing of tasks
necessary to achieve these goals.” (p. 391.) Examples of goal setting and prioritizing
items include “I set short-term goals for what 1 want to accomplish in a few' days or
weeks" and “I finish top priority tasks before going on to less important ones." She
defined the second dimension, planning and scheduling, as “the behaviors typically
associated with managing time, such as making lists, scheduling, and planning.” (p. 391).
activities at least a week in advance" and “1 make a list of things to do each day and
check off each task as it is completed.” The third aspect of time management is
preference for organization, which refers to “both a general organized approach to work
391 ). A few of the preference for organization items are “At the end of the workday I
50
leave a clear, we 11-organized work space,” and “I can find the things I need for my work
more easily when my work space is messy and disorganized than when it is neat and
organized.”
(Adams & Jex, 1999; Macan, 1994; Macan et al., 1990) of each of these three
dimensions. Coefficient alphas obtained in the initial and holdout samples, respectively,
of the present study were a=.84 and a=.81 for setting goals and priorities, a = .73 and
a=,75 for planning and scheduling, and a =.72 and a = .76 for preference for
using this scale have correlated highly with supervisors, coworkers, and friends’ reports
process model of time management found that engaging in time management behaviors is
which employees are pressured to complete their work within a fixed timeframe. Time
pressure was assessed using a 5-item scale developed for use in the present study in
which respondents indicated the extent to which they agreed of disagreed with each item
using a 5-point Likert-type rating scale. The five time pressure items were “I am often
pressured for time at work,” “There are too many things to be done at once,” “I often face
responded to each of the five time pressure items using a five-point scale ranging from
51
“Strongly Agree” to “Strongly Disagree." Internal consistency reliability for these five
Job satisfaction. Employees’ overall level of job satisfaction was assessed using
the Job-in-General (JIG) scale. The JIG scale is a global rather than a facet measure of
job satisfaction. An overall rather than a facet measure was chosen for use in this study
because I was interested in how work/life balance relates to job satisfaction as a whole,
rather than how work/life balance relates to specific aspects of one's job.
This scale consists of eighteen items to which respondents indicate the whether
each word or phrase describes their current job situation. The response scale for the JIG
scale is identical to the SIG: “Y” for “Yes,” “N” for “No,” and “?” for “Cannot decide.”
Scoring for the JIG is as follows: “N” = 0, “?” = 1, “Y” = 3 for the positively worded
items, and “N”=3, “?”=1 and “Y”=0 for the negatively-worded items. An overall score is
calculated for each individual by summing one's responses across the eighteen items.
JIG scores may range of 0 to 54, with higher scores indicating higher levels of overall job
satisfaction. In order to reduce the impact of missing data on summed scale scores,
listwise deletion of cases was employed such that no job satisfaction score was calculated
for any individual who left more than three items blank (Balzer et al., 1997).
Coefficient alphas measuring the internal consistency of the job satisfaction items
in the present study were a = .92 for the initial sample and a = .90 for the holdout
obtained in previous research, in which alpha has ranged from .91 to .95 (Ironson et al.,
1989). As evidence for construct validity for the JIG, this measure is highly correlated
with other measures of job satisfaction (r ranges from .67 to .80, median = .76; Ironson et
52
al., 1989) and moderately correlated with overall job stress as measured by the SIG scale
Life satisfaction. Life satisfaction is the degree to which individuals judge the
overall quality of their life favorably (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985; Pavot &
Diener, 1993). Overall life satisfaction was assessed using the 5-item Satisfaction with
Life Scale (Diener et al., 1985). This measure has been used frequently in quality-of-life
research to assess individuals’ overall satisfaction with life, and has demonstrated
appropriate levels of reliability and validity (Hart, 1999; Pavot & Diener, 1993). For
example, the estimate of internal consistency reliability (coefficient alpha) obtained for
this measure in previous research was a = .88, and the measure was correlated r = .49
with job satisfaction, r = .56 with non-work satisfaction, r = -.34 with work hassles, and r
Participants responded to the five items of life satisfaction using a 5-point Likert-
type scale, ranging from “Strongly Agree" to “Strongly Disagree." Life satisfaction
scores for each individual were computed by taking the arithmetic mean across the five
items. Higher scores indicate higher levels of life satisfaction. Coefficient alphas
(internal consistency) in the present study were a = .85 for both the initial and holdout
extent to which employees like and feel a part of the organization for which they work.
developed by Mowday, Steers, and Porter (1979). Participants were asked to indicate the
extent to which they agree or disagree with each organizational commitment item using a
53
organizational commitment score was computed for each individual by computing the
arithmetic mean of the six items. Higher scores indicate higher levels of organizational
commitment.
Bachiochi (1998). This measure has demonstrated acceptable reliability and validity. For
.80 to a = .86, and the measure is highly correlated with job satisfaction (r = .63) and
estimates obtained in the present study (coefficient alpha) were a = .83 and a = .82 for
the initial and holdout samples, respectively. See Appendix I for the complete
by Seashore, Lawler, Mirvis, and Cammann (1982). These items include “I often think
of quitting my job,” “1 will probably look for a new job within the next 12 months,” and
“I would like to leave my present job.” Participants were asked to respond to these items
using a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from “Strongly Agree” to “Strongly Disagree.” An
overall intention to quit scale score was computed by taking the average across
individuals’ responses to all three items. Previous research has used two of these three
items to measure intentions to quit, with a coefficient alpha of a = .91 (McClough et al.,
1998). Empirical research using the 2-item measure has found a negative relation with
53
job satisfaction (r = -.56 to -.64) and organizational commitment (r = -.52 to -.62; Fisher
et al., 2000; McClough et al., 1998). To improve the internal scale properties (e.g.,
internal consistency reliability), a third item (“I would like to leave my present job.”) was
added to the scale. Internal consistency reliability estimates (coefficient alpha) obtained
a = .91 and a = .90 for the initial and holdout samples, respectively.
management level, average number of total work hours per week, average number of
hours spent working per week at home, number and ages of children, and responsibility
for elder care. See Appendix J for the exact demographic measures.
Data Analyses
Data analyses for this study were conducted in three phases. The first two phases
involved using data from the initial sample. The third phase involved using data from the
holdout sample. The first phase focused on item and scale analysis to gather empirical
evidence to assess the work/life balance scale. The second phase of data analysis was
conducted to assess construct validity evidence by testing the hypotheses made about the
relations between work/life balance and other constructs using data gathered in the initial
sample. The third phase employed a hold-out sample to cross-validate the scale
psychometric properties (e.g., factor structure, reliability, and validity of the scale) with
(i.e., approximately half the sample) were randomly selected from the entire sample.
This group of respondents was set aside used as the holdout sample for cross-validation
purposes. The remaining 267 respondents were used in Phase One (scale item analysis)
and Phase Two (initial scale validity assessment). Consistent findings obtained in a
holdout sample would increase the generalizability of results by adjusting for random
sampling error.
The sample size for the holdout sample is considered more than sufficient for
conducting factor analyses and tests of the hypotheses. A minimum of 200 participants
and a participant-to-item ratio of 5 have been suggested for obtaining reliable factor
loading estimates in exploratory factor analysis (Cattell, 1978; Ford, MacCallum. & Tait,
1986; Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996). The participant-to-item ratio in each of these two
samples exceeded ten to one, and was considered sufficient for obtaining reliable
parameter estimates.
Phase one. For the first phase of the data analysis, item means and standard
likelihood extraction and an oblique rotation of the factors was conducted. The
After completing the exploratory factor analysis, I computed the corrected item-
total correlations (which are the bivariate correlations of each scale item compared to the
composite of all scale items that is corrected for autocorrelation; Nunnally, 1978), and
scale development recommendations by Stanton et al. (1999) and Clark & Watson
56
work/life balance scale item and a one-item overall work/life balance item (i.e., “In
general I feel that I have an adequate balance between my work and personal life”).
Using the results obtained across all of those analyses, I tested the latent factor structure
of the scale using partial confirmatory factor analysis (PCFA; Bollen, 1989). In a partial
confirmatory factor analysis, the latent factor structure (i.e., which items are to load on
which factors) is specified a priori, but the item loadings are freed for estimation. This
step enabled me to determine the best-fitting model with regard to the number of latent
factors underlying the observed variables (i.e., the scale items). The results I obtained in
each of these steps to evaluate the work/life balance scale items are presented in the next
chapter.
obtained from the initial sample (N=267) provided initial evidence for construct validity
statistically significant correlations (e.g., above r = .60) would provide evidence that the
two scales are measuring similar constructs. Additional evidence of convergent validity
time pressure and role overload. Criterion-related validity was evaluated by testing the
research hypotheses regarding the extent to which work/life balance was related to other,
overall job stress, life satisfaction, job satisfaction, organizational commitment, and
turnover intentions.
Finally, the work/life balance scale was compared to the work/family conflict
balance and work/family conflict in terms of the amount of variance shared with each
variable and related measures. This was accomplished by determining the extent to
which work/life balance accounted for a significant portion of unique variance in other
variables after controlling for variance in common with work/family conflict. This was
tested using hierarchical regression analysis in which work/family conflict was entered
into the regression equation as a predictor variable, and each outcome (e.g., life
satisfaction, job satisfaction, overall job stress) was used as a separate criterion or
dependent variable. Work/life balance was entered into the equation as a predictor in the
comparing the difference or change between the multiple R-squared obtained from the
first step and the multiple R-squared found in the second step would indicate that
work/life balance accounts for variance in the criterion variable that work/family conflict
does not.
Phase three. The data analysis in the third phase was conducted to cross-validate
the scale development process undertaken in Phase One and the initial scale validity
alpha), corrected item-total correlations, and correlations with a one-item overall measure
of work/life balance were computed for each of the work/life balance scale items using
classical test theory item analyses already conducted to gather additional information to
assess the psychometric properties of the work/life balance scale items. For example, I
computed the a parameter and test information function (TIF) for each scale item
obtained in an item response theory (IRT) analysis. I he a parameter measures how well
an item discriminates on the latent trait being assessed. The TIF provides evidence of the
sensitivity of the scale, which refers to how much information is provided by the scale at
all levels of the trait being measured. The purpose of examining the TIF is to determine
the extent to which the scale items measure the work/life balance content domain across
all levels of work/life balance. The IRT analysis requires a large sample size in order to
obtain reliable item response parameter estimates. Therefore, data from the initial and
Data analyses conducted in Phase Two were repeated in Phase Three to further
assess the construct validity of the work/life balance measure using the holdout sample.
Consistent findings in Phase Three compared to results obtained in Phase Two would
analyses were conducted using demographic variables to determine if there were any
demographic differences. These data analyses involved computing t-tests and using
across different demographic subgroups. The full sample (N=540), rather than the initial
59
and holdout samples individually, was used for the exploratory analyses in order to
increase statistical power (i.e., the probability of finding significant effects if, in fact, they
exist).
60
Initial Sample
Demographic Information
Among these 267 respondents, 57% were male and 43% were female. The proportion of
males and females in this sample was not significantly different from the proportion
obtained in the overall sample (yw2 (1) = .40, p > .05). The majority of respondents were
between 35 and 54 years old. The majority of respondents were married (79%). In
addition, the majority of respondents (81%) had obtained at least a bachelor’s degree, and
had been working for the same organization for at least five years (82%).
Scale Assessment
Exploratory factor analysis. Item means and standard deviations for the initial
item pool of the work/life balance scale are presented in Table 4. Higher means indicate
that respondents report having experienced that situation more frequently. In most cases,
items with higher means are purported to indicate lower levels of work/life balance. The
conducted using the correlation matrix as input to assess the dimensionality among the 24
work/life balance scale items (Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996). The purpose of PCA is to
extract the maximum amount of variance with each component from the data, with the
assumption that the observed variables (i.e., items) are a linear combination of some
61
Table 3
Primary responsibility
for elder care Yes 9.5
No 90.5
62
Table 3 (continued)
Work/Life Balance Scale Item Means and Standard Deviations - Initial Sample
Item Mean SD
1. I struggle with trying to juggle both my work and non-work responsibilities. 3.32 1.00
2. My personal life drains me of the energy I need to do my job. 2.03 0.84
3. I feel overwhelmed when I try to balance my work and personal life. 2.60 0.93
4. My work suffers because of everything going on in my personal life. 1.88 0.77
5. I am too tired to be effective at work because of things I have going on in my personal life. 1.76 0.70
6. My job gives me energy to pursue activities outside of work that are important to me. 2.71 1.12
7. I have difficulty scheduling vacation time because of my workload. 2.94 1.16
8. I am unable to relax at home because I am preoccupied with my work. 2.73 1.00
9. I am happy with the amount of time I spend doing activities not related to work. 3.14 1.10
10. Because of my job, I am in a better mood at home. 2.77 1.04
11.1 often have to make difficult choices between my work and my personal life 2.70 0.94
12.1 have to put aspects of my personal life “on hold" because of my work. 2.59 0.99
13. I am able to accomplish what I would like in both my personal and work lives. 3.22 1.03
14. 1 am in a better mood at work because of every thing 1 have going for me in my personal life. 3.53 1.01
15.1 often neglect my personal needs because of the demands of my work. 2.84 1.02
16. When I am at work, I worry' about things I need to do outside of work. 2.55 0.86
17. I have difficulty getting my work done because I am preoccupied with personal matters at work. 1.93 0.70
18. My personal life suffers because of my work. 2.54 1.03
19.1 have to miss out on important personal activities due to the amount of time I spend doing work. 2.41 0.95
20. My personal life gives me the energy to do my job. 3.37 1.00
21.1 feel that I allocate appropriate amounts of time to both work and non-work activities. 3.30 1.17
22.1 make personal sacrifices to get work done. 3.14 0.91
23.1 come home from work too tired to do things I would like to do. 3.25 0.94
24. Mv iob makes it difficult to maintain the kind of personal life I would like. 2.82 1.16
O\
LU
Table 5
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13
1 1.00
2 .37 1.00
3 .63 .40 1.00
4 .36 .58 .42 1.00
5 .29 .59 .36 .65 1.00
6 -.19 -.25 -.29 -.18 -.22 1.00
7 .35 .07 .34 .15 .07 -.16 1.00
8 .45 .24 .53 .23 .23 -.30 .56 1.00
9 -.46 -.25 -.46 -.18 -.26 .34 -.42 -.51 1.00
10 -.22 -.18 -.33 -.23 -.19 .53 -.27 -.35 .37 1.00
11 .56 .27 .50 .35 .21 -.19 .52 .53 -.43 -.28 1.00
12 .50 .24 .43 .28 .17 -.27 .54 .55 -.50 -.34 .64 1.00
13 -.46 -.38 -.50 -.38 -.34 .43 -.38 -.51 .61 .44 -.48 -.57 1.00
14 -.27 -.28 -.33 -.24 -.26 .34 -.19 -.31 .31 .37 -.28 -.27 .48
Note. Row and column labels refer to the numbering of Work/Life Balance scale items. (N=267)
2
Table 5 (continued)
I 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 H 12 13 14
15 .51 .22 .52 .24 .16 -.29 .53 .64 -.56 -.37 .55 .71 -.56 -.32
16 .28 .32 .26 .34 .34 -.27 .02 .10 -.18 -.18 .26 .25 -.26 -.19
17 .23 .44 .25 .50 .53 -.27 .05 .13 -.22 -.22 .28 .25 -.35 -.28
18 .53 .26 .54 .28 .21 -.31 .53 .59 -.55 -.38 .59 .72 -.59 -.23
19 .48 .23 .48 .24 .13 -.26 .51 .53 -.49 -.37 .64 .69 -.49 -.22
20 -.30 -.34 -.39 -.28 -.35 .39 -.24 -.32 .37 .40 -.34 -.35 .51 .71
21 -.47 -.21 -.51 -.26 -.24 .37 -.42 -.54 .62 .39 -.47 -.58 .65 .38
22 .39 .19 .40 .22 .13 -.15 .48 .53 -.45 -.31 .50 .65 -.50 -.28
23 .44 .33 .47 .28 .31 -.39 .37 .50 -.50 -.43 .41 .53 -.58 -.38
24 .50 .34 .51 .31 .27 -.38 .47 .57 -.57 -.44 .56 .73 -.60 -.37
Note. Row and column labels refer to the numbering of Work/Life Balance scale items. (N=267)
O'
Table 5 (continued)
15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24
15 1.00
16 .19 1.00
17 .19 .50 1.00
18 .74 .16 .23 1.00
19 .67 .16 .21 .76 1.00
20 -.35 -.18 -.37 -.35 -.29 1.00
21 -.59 -.26 -.28 -.64 -.55 .43 1.00
22 .65 .06 .11 .65 .59 -.25 -.50 1.00
23 .61 .29 .30 .57 .48 -.39 -.55 .48 1.00
24 .68 .26 .30 .71 .70 -.42 -.60 .58 .63 1.00
Note. Row and column labels refer to the numbering of Work/Life Balance scale items. (N=267)
67
underlying construct (Ford et al., 1986). The first principal component is the linear
combination of observed variables that accounts for the most variance among the items.
The second component is computed from the residual correlations after forming the first
component, and is the linear combination of observed variables that accounts for the most
variance uncorrelated with the first component. The process of extracting the maximum
variance from the residual correlations that is uncorrelated with previously computed
components is repeated until accounting for all unique variance. Mathematically, if all
components are retained from a PC A, the observed correlation matrix can be reproduced
from the PCA. As a result, PCA is an exploratory factor analytic tool that is useful for
providing an empirical summary of the data. This method is slightly different from a
factor analysis (i.e., using a principal factor or maximum likelihood factor extraction
eliminate unique and error variance from the factors (Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996).
However, results obtained from a principal components analysis and principal factor
analysis are very similar and do not usually yield different results upon interpretation.
Prior to conducting the PCA, many of the work/life balance scale items were
recoded so that for all items, higher scores indicated higher levels of work/life balance.
Because the pattern of missing data among work/life balance scale items appeared to be
random rather than systematic, listwise deletion was performed to handle missing data.
Listwise deletion refers to omitting any cases with any missing values from the analysis,
and is a conservative approach to handling missing data. Data from N=242 respondents
were retained for use in the principal components analysis after listwise deletion. This
68
sample size exceeds the minimum requirement of 200 cases or 5 respondents per item as
Two criteria were used to assess the number of components that describe the
correlations among the scale items: eigenvalues greater than one and the scree test (Cattell,
1978). The first step was to examine the eigenvalues obtained for each component.
Eigenvalues are related to the amount of variance accounted for by each component. A
rule of thumb is to retain components with eigenvalues greater than one (Tabachnick &
Fidell, 1996). There were three components with eigenvalues greater than one. These
The second step was to conduct a scree test. A scree test involves plotting
eigenvalues on the y-axis against the number of components on the x-axis, drawing a line
through each point on the graph, and determining where the line changes direction, or
levels off. The number of components to be retained for interpretation should be equal to
the number of components on the x-axis before the line changes direction. I he scree plot
a result, both criteria suggest that three components should be retained for interpretation.
As indicated earlier, the amount of variance that each observed variable (i.e., item)
contributes is one (Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996; Cattell, 1978). Because there were twenty-
four work/life balance scale items, the total variance is 24. Total variance accounted for by
each component includes shared (common) variance and unique variance. The three
69
Figure 1
Component Number
70
components that emerged from the PCA accounted for 60.6% of the total variance. The
first component accounted for 43.4%, the second accounted for 10.8%, and the third
component accounted for 6.4% of the total variance. Common or shared variance across
the set of items is computed by summing the communality estimates obtained for each
item, and is 14.55. The proportion of total variance accounted for by each component is
computed by dividing the common variance (sum of communality estimates, 14.55) by the
Because 1 predicted that the factors would correlate with one another, I performed
an oblique (direct oblimin) rotation of the components. An oblique rotation assumes that
the components are correlated. However, it is important to note that the components are
not necessarily correlated with one another when an oblique rotation is used. If results
indicate that the components are uncorrelated, then a more simple, orthogonal rotation can
Results of the PCA with the oblique rotation yielded three interpretable and
correlated components that converged in six iterations. The PCA pattern matrix (A) and
item communalities are presented in Table 6, and the component correlation matrix ((p) is
shown in Table 7. Because the pattern matrix was easily interpretable (only one item
cross-loaded on two components) and the three components were correlated with one
another (r range from .31 to .44), the oblique rotation seemed appropriate, and there was no
rotation.
71
Table 6
Note. Principal components extraction method with oblique rotation method. (N=242)
1 Refers to the work/life balance scale item number as listed in Table 4.
72
Table 7
Component
I 2 3
1 1.00
2 .31 1.00
Note. Principal components extraction method with oblique rotation method. (N=242)
73
solution. Component loadings obtained based on an oblique rotation are measures of the
unique relationship between the component (i.e., latent variable) and the observed variable
(i.e., item; Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996). Higher loadings indicate stronger relationships,
and the higher the loading, the more the observed variable is a good indicator of the latent
factor. A minimum loading of .40 was interpreted as evidence that the item loaded on a
particular component.
The item communality estimates specified the proportion of variance accounted for
by the components. Communalities for items in the first component ranged from .52 to
.76. Communalities for items in the second component range from .38 to .66.
Communalities for items in the third component range from .51 to .66. These
communalities indicated that a large proportion of the variance is explained by these three
components.
The component pattern matrix was easy to interpret because the component
loadings for each item exceeded .40 on one component and were much less than .40 on
each of the remaining components. The only exception to this was Item 13 (i.e., “1 am able
to accomplish what I would like in both my personal and work lives"), which had a loading
of .47 on the first component and .40 on the third component. These results indicated that
this item cross-loaded on two components, which made it difficult to discern whether Item
13 was more similar to items on the first component or items on the third component.
conducted a second PCA using an oblique rotation, but omitted Item 13 from the analysis.
The resulting component pattern matrix and communalities are presented in Table 8, and
74
Table 8
Pattern Matrix of Work/Life Balance Items after Removing Item 13 - Initial Sample
Note. Principal components extraction method with oblique rotation method. (N=242)
1 Refers to the work/life balance scale item number as listed in Table 4.
75
Table 9
Component
I 2 3
1 1.00
2 .30 1.00
Note. Principal components extraction method with oblique rotation method. (N=242)
76
the component correlation matrix appears in Table 9. These results were consistent with
the results obtained in the initial PCA, although none of the remaining items cross-loaded
on multiple components, and this factor solution was preferable to the initial results
After removing the cross-loading item, fourteen scale items remained in the first
component. Examples of items on this component with high factor loadings included “My
personal life suffers because of my work,’* and “1 have to miss out on important personal
activities because of my work.” The content of these items reflected the extent to which
work interferes with personal life, and therefore 1 labeled the component worA: interference
with personal life (WIPL). The five items in the second component indicated the opposite
Examples of these items included “My work suffers because of everything going on in my
personal life,” and “I have difficulty getting my work done because 1 am preoccupied with
personal matters.” Specifically, these items depicted the extent to which one's personal
life interferes with work. As a result, 1 labeled this component personal life interference
with work (PLIW). The four items in the third component involved positive effects of
one's work on personal life or vice versa, the extent to which one's personal life enhances
work. Therefore, this I labeled the third component work/personal life enhancement
(WPLE). Lower levels of interference (WIPL and PLIW) were interpreted as higher levels
Upon examination of the content of the fourteen items in the first component, some
items specifically indicated a directional relationship between work and personal life such
77
that work interferes with personal life. For example, the item with the highest loading on
this factor was “My personal life suffers because of my work." However, this directional
influence was less apparent in a few of the items (e.g., “I struggle with trying to juggle my
work and non-work responsibilities.”) To explore whether these fourteen items reflected
analysis among these fourteen items. Results yielded a clear one-component solution that
accounted for 58% of the variance among the items. The eigenvalue for this component
was 8.17, and the scree plot also indicated a one-component solution. The component
matrix and communalities for these fifteen items are presented in Table 10.
Additional item and scale analyses. After performing the exploratory factor
analysis to understand the underlying dimensions among the work/life balance scale items,
the next step was to compute additional item and scale statistical analyses to evaluate the
work/life balance scale items. First, 1 computed corrected item-total correlations among
work/life balance scale items on each dimension identified by the exploratory factor
analysis. Corrected item-total correlations measure the bivariate relationship between each
scale item and the total of all items in the scale and are corrected for autocorrelation
Three sets of corrected item-total correlations (one for each of the three scale
dimensions) were computed and the results are presented in Table 11. Previous research
has suggested that corrected item-total correlations above r = .45 indicate that the
78
Table 10
Component Matrix and Communalities Among Fourteen Work Interference with Personal
15 .85 .71
24 .83 .69
12 .83 .69
19 .81 .66
8 .77 .59
21 .76 .57
22 .75 .56
11 .74 .55
23 .71 .50
9 .71 .50
3 .68 .47
1 .68 .46
7 .67 .45
Table 11
Corrected Item-Total Correlations for Work/Life Balance Scale Items - Initial Sample
Note. WIPL = Work Interference with Personal Life; PLIW = b Personal Life
Interference with Work; WPLE = Work/Personal Life Enhancement
80
relationship between any one scale item and the whole scale is acceptable for scale
development (Ironson et al., 1989). All work/life balance items exceeded the r = .45
criterion, except Item 16 (on the PLIW dimension). The corrected item-total correlation
for Item 16 was r = .44, which was much lower than the corrected item-total correlations
each of the three dimensions of work/life balance. Coefficient alpha is a statistic that
measures the extent to which items are interrelated and have high communalities (Cortina,
note that the number of items in a scale affects the size of the alpha coefficient such that
scales with more items are more likely to the have higher coefficient alphas (Cortina,
1993). Therefore the level of coefficient alpha should be interpreted with the number of
scale items in mind. For example, a four-item scale w ith a coefficient alpha of .80 would
indicate more internal consistency among the items than a fifteen-item scale with the same
reliability estimate.
Among the work/life balance items, coefficient alphas obtained in the initial sample
were a = .94 for Work Interference with Personal Life (WIPL; 14 items), a = .81 for
Personal Life Interference with Work (PLIW; 5 items), and a = .77 for Work/Personal Life
Enhancement (WPLE; 4 items). The reliability analysis also indicated that removing Item
increase the reliability estimate from a = .81 to a = .83. The low corrected item-total
81
correlation for Item 16 as well as the increase in scale reliability after removing Item 16
from the PLIW scale suggest that Item 16 should be removed from the PLIW scale prior to
balance scale item with a one-item measure of overall work/life balance. That is,
respondents rated a single item, “In general I feel that I have an adequate balance between
my work and personal life.” Table 12 presents the bivariate correlations between each of
the work/life balance scale items and the one-item measure. Correlations between items on
the WIPL dimension and overall work/life balance were moderate to high in magnitude.
Correlations between WIPL items were also moderately correlated with overall work/life
balance. However, personal life to work interference (PLIW) correlations with overall
work/life balance were much lower than any of the other items, although still significantly
related with r’s between .20 and .26. This suggested that work/life balance was more
similar to work interference with personal life than any other dimension.
Confirmatory factor analysis. Using the results of the exploratory factor analysis
obtained in the previous step, I tested a measurement model of the work/life balance scale
item factor structure using partial confirmatory' factor analysis (PCFA). In PCFA, the
latent factor structure is specified a priori, but the item loadings are freed for estimation
(Bollen, 1989). The purpose of this analysis was to apply a confirmatory approach to
testing the factor structure of the work/life balance scale and to obtain fit statistics
useful for assessing how well the model fit the data. However, due to the exploratory
Table 12
Correlations Between Each Work/Life Balance Scale Item and One-Item Work/Life
Results of the exploratory factor analysis suggested that a three-factor model would fit the
data best. I tested a three-factor model using a maximum likelihood procedure and the
covariance matrix as input using AMOS Graphics 4.0 software (Arbuckle, 1999).
Although the initial item pool contained 24 items, two items (13 and 16) were removed
based on results obtained from the exploratory factor analysis and additional item and scale
analyses. Therefore, the initial model 1 tested was a three-factor model containing a total of
22 items: 14 items on the WIPL dimension (Items 1,3, 7, 8, 9, 11, 12, 15, 18, 19, 21, 22,
23. and 24), 4 items on the PLIW dimension (Items 2, 4, 5, 17), and 4 items on the WPLE
dimension (Items 6, 10. 14, and 20). No items were allowed to cross-load on any other
dimension.
Four model fit statistics were examined to assess the fit of the model: the Tucker-
Lewis Index (TLI; Tucker & Lewis, 1973: Bentler & Bonett, 1980), the comparative fit
index (CFI; Bentler, 1990), the normed fit index (NFL McDonald and Marsh, 1990), and
the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA; Steiger, 1990) based on the
recommendations of Hu and Bentler (1995), Medsker, Williams, and Holahan (1994), and
Hu & Bentler (1995). The TLI, CFI, and NFI are relative indices that are used to compare
alternative models using the same data. TLI, CFI, and NFI values below .90 indicate poor
model fit (Bentler & Bonett, 1980). Compared to widely used fit indices such as the NFI,
the TLI and CFI are robust across sample sizes (Hu & Bentler, 1995; Marsh. Balia, &
McDonald. 1988).
The fourth model fit index examined in this study was the RMSEA (Steiger, 1990.
The RMSEA is a fit index for comparing non-nested models that permits the relative
ordering of models from best fitting to worst fitting within a single data set, and represents
84
a discrepancy per degree of freedom test (Maruyama, 1998). MacCallum (1998) and Hu
and Bentler (1995) recommend the use of the RMSEA in addition to other fit statistics
when evaluating model fit. According to Steiger, (1990) RMSEA values below .10
indicate good fit and values below .05 indicate very good fit. More recently, however,
Browne & Cudeck (1992) claimed that an RMSEA of .08 represents the upper bound of
good fit, and Hu and Bentler (1995) claimed that .06 indicates good fit.
freedom for a PCFA model (Maruyama, 1998). However, the chi-square as a test of model
fit is not informative because the chi-square value is greatly affected by sample size. In
other words, the chi-square statistic is usually statistically significant because the sample
size required to conduct the analysis is typically large. Fit indices obtained from test of the
Although the TLI, CFI, and NFI results obtained from the PCFA indicate good
model fit, the RMSEA was higher than desirable to indicate good model fit according to
Hu and Bentler (1995). To improve the fit of the model to the data, two steps were taken.
First, I removed Item 3 from the WIPL dimension. Because Items 1 and 3 were strongly
analysis indicated that the error terms for Items 1 and 3 were highly correlated, and the
item content was very similar (i.e., “I struggle with trying to juggle both my work and non
work responsibilities" and “I feel overwhelmed when I try to balance my work and
personal life," respectively), it seemed appropriate to remove one of these items to improve
model fit. I chose item 3 for omission because the PCFA factor loading for Item 3 (.60)
was lower than the factor loading for Item 1 (.63). The fit indices for the three-factor
85
model having removed Item 3 are presented in Table 13. These results indicate that the
The second step taken to improve model fit was to remove the constraint that errors
among a pair of items are uncorrelated. In other words, the errors between Items 6 and
Item 10 were allowed to covary. These changes were made not only because the items
were empirically related, but also because they were semantically similar and therefore
theoretically related to one another. A third PCFA was conducted to test the fit of the
model to the data once this constraint had been removed. The fit indices for this model are
also presented in Table 13. Although allowing the errors from items 6 and 10 to
covary improved mode, fit, I removed the same constraint from items 9 and 21. The fit
indices found by testing this model appear in Table 13. With relative fit indices well
above .90 and an RMSEA of .06, these results suggest that this 21-item, three-factor model
with two pairs of errors allowed to covary fits the data well.
In order to rule out the alternative that a one- or two-factor mode, fit the data better
than the three-factor model, two additional PCFAs were conducted for comparative
purposes, and results obtained from these analyses are also presented in Table 13. As
expected, examination of these fit indices suggested that the three-factor model was
superior to the one-or two-factor model. A figure depicting the best-fitting model and
including the path coefficients (i.e., factor loadings) for each item is presented in Figure 2.
86
Table 13
Figure 2
Final Work/Life Balance Scale Confirmatory Factor Analysis Model - Initial Sample
Item 1 L
Item 7 L
Item 8—-
Item 9j
item 11 •
Item 12
WIPL
Item 15
Item 18
Item 19
Item 21
Item 22
Item 23
Item 24
-0.5:
Item 2
PLIW Item 4
Item 5
Item 17
Item 6~
Item 10
WPLE
Item 14
Item 20
88
Summary
The initial pool of 24 work/life balance items was reduced to a final set of 21 items
based on the results of the item and scale analyses conducted among data from the initial
sample. The resulting work/life balance scale was multi-dimensional, with 13 items
measuring work interference with personal life (WIPL), 4 items assessing personal life
interference with work (PLIW), and four items assessing work/personal life enhancement
although the convergent and criterion-related validity evidence regarding the relation
between each work/life balance dimension and other variables will be assessed based on
Validity Evidence
Scale descriptive statistics. Scale means and standard deviations for all study
variables are presented in Table 14. Scale mean distributions were normal for both WIPL
and WPLE, and somewhat positively skewed for PLIW. indicating that more managers
indicated low levels of personal life interference with work. I tested for mean differences
between the work/life balance scale dimensions using paired-samples t-tests. Results
indicated that managers perceived that their work interfered with their personal life (WIPL)
more than their personal life interfered with their work (PLIW), t (266) = 19.84, p < .01.
higher than their beliefs that their personal life interfered with their work, even after taking
into account the opposite direction of scaling for the two variables (t (266) = 20.84,
p < .01. There were no mean differences found between WIPL and WPLE.
Table 14
Standard
Scale Mean Deviation
1. WIPL - Work Interference with Personal Life 2.83 0.76
2. PLIW - Personal Life Interference with Work 1.90 0.61
3. WPLE - Work/Personal Life Enhancement 3.10 0.80
4. Overall Work/Personal Life Balance 3.32 1.05
5. WFC - Work-to-Family Conflict 2.84 0.93
6. FWC - Family-to-Work Conflict 2.03 0.74
7. Role Overload 3.61 0.78
8. Stress - Pressure 17.78 3.52
9. Stress - Threat 17.55 4.45
10. Time Pressure 3.45 0.76
11. Time Management Behavior (TMB)- Setting Goals and Priorities 3.64 0.58
12. Time Management Behavior (TMB)- Planning and Organizing 3.53 0.65
13. Time Management Behavior (TMB)- Preference for Organization 3.77 0.54
14. Job Satisfaction 41.90 10.91
15. Life Satisfaction 3.36 0.82
16. Organizational Commitment 3.80 0.74
17. Turnover Intentions 2.45 1.19
oo
o
Table 15
I 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 £6 12
1. WIPE .94
2. PLIW .39** .83
3. WPLE -.57** -.41** .77
4. Overall W/L Bal. -.67** -.29** .54** -
5. WFC .87** .35** -.53** -.60** .90
6. FWC .28** .58** -.29** -.27** .35** .88
7. Role Overload .45** .08 -.27** -.31** .48** .10 .70
8. Stress - Pressure .47** .12* -.30** -.26* ♦ .47** .11 .54** .82
9. Stress - Threat .52** .20** -.44** -.40** .49** .17** .50** .58** .82
10. Time Pressure .55** .22** -.34** -.35** .56** .18** .79* ♦ .61** .57** .84
11. TMB-Goals -.05 -.31** .29** .14* -.07 -.27** -.05 .00 -.20** -.09 .84
12. TMB-Planning -.09 -.24** .24** .12* -.08 -.18** -.02 .02 -.02 -.01 .62** .73
13. TMB-Org. -.41** -.37** .29* * .27** -.36** -.32** -.29** -.16** -.28** -.32** .36** .28** .72
14. Job Sat. -.31** -.28** .52** .31** -.32** -.13* -.14* -.17** -.43** -.23** .25** .15* .10 .91
15. Life Sat. -.55** -.42** .61** .63** -.53** -.35** -.24* -.24* -.42** -.32** .26** .19** .30** .49** .85
16. Org. Comm. -.16** -.21** .44** .22** -.21** -.16** -.10 -.05 -.31** -.14* .23** .12* .11 .57 .36** .83
17. Turnover .25** .21** -.38** -.28** .31** .19** .21** .13* .41** .23** -.14* -.07 -.14* -.61** -.39** -.64* • .90
Note. Coefficient alpha for each scale appears on diagonal. (N=267)
** p<.01
* p < .05
©
©
91
After gathering initial construct validity evidence for the work/life balance scale
by examining the psychometric properties of the scale and examining the scale mean
distributions, the next step was to examine the extent to which the work/life balance scale
was related to other variables. I tested a series of hypotheses regarding the relations
among the work/life balance dimensions and other variables to assess the convergent and
role overload, time pressure, the pressure and threat subscales of overall feelings of job
stress, time management behavior (setting goals and priorities, planning and scheduling,
and preference for organization), overall job satisfaction, life satisfaction, organizational
commitment, and turnover intentions. The bivariate correlations obtained during this
analysis and interpreted to test the hypotheses are presented in Table 15.
Hypothesis tests
Each of the hypotheses tested in this study involved assessing the relations
between work/life balance and other variables. In order to test these hypotheses. Lower
levels of WIPL and PLIW (both directions of interference) were interpreted as higher
balance and work/family conflict were strongly related to one another. In particular,
WIPL and WFC were very highly correlated, and PLIW was fairly strongly related to
92
FWC. These results indicate evidence for convergent validity between work/life balance
related to role overload. Hypothesis 2 was supported for two of the three work/life
relationship was found between personal life-to-work interference (PLIW) and role
overload.
related to perceptions of time pressure. Results indicated that WIPL was strongly related
to perceptions of time pressure. The other two dimensions of work/life balance were also
significantly related to time pressure in the expected direction, although the magnitude of
the correlations between time pressure and both PLIW and WPLE was not as high as the
employees' perceptions of work/life balance and time management behavior. All three
dimensions of time management behavior (setting goals and priorities, time management
planning and scheduling, and preference for organization) were correlated with PLIW
and WPLE in the predicted direction. However, WIPL was only related to having a
preference for organization and was not related at all to setting goals and priorities or
planning and scheduling. Although results generally support Hypothesis 3, the lack of a
relationship between WIPL and setting goals and prioritizing was contrary to what I
expected.
93
balance and feelings of overall job stress, or a positive relationship between stress and
interference. WIPL and WPLE were significantly related to both dimensions of overall
job stress. Pressure and Threat. However, PLIW was only related to the threat
dimension, not pressure. The magnitude of the obtained correlations was higher for
threat than pressure, suggesting a stronger relationship between work/life balance and
was much more strongly related to job satisfaction (r = .52) than either of the interference
Hypothesis 7. Similar to the results for the previous hypothesis, all three
dimensions of work/life balance were related to perceptions of life satisfaction. All three
dimensions were correlated relatively strongly with life satisfaction, although consistent
with the results for overall job satisfaction, WPLE was more strongly related to life
balance would be negatively related to their intentions to leave their work organization.
Although the distribution of turnover intentions was positively skewed (i.e., the majority
94
of employees were not planning to leave their jobs), results were obtained in the
predicted direction for all three work/life balance dimensions. Work/personal life
enhancement was more strongly related to turnover intentions than the other two
dimensions.
Hypothesis 10. The next step was to discern whether there was any evidence for
discriminant validity between work/life balance and work/family conflict. That is, were
they each really measuring two, albeit related, but distinct constructs? To test the
hypothesis that indeed they are empirically distinct constructs (Hypothesis 10), I
variance in each of the criterion variables that work/life balance accounted for above and
The results of this analysis are presented in Tables 16 to 26. R2 is the coefficient of
determination, which indicates the proportion of variance in the criterion variable that is
accounted for by the linear combination of the predictor variables. The change in R2
indicates the incremental variance accounted for by additional variables when they are
indicated that work/life balance accounted for a significant proportion of the variance in
each of the criterion variables after accounting for work/family conflict, with the
exception of role overload and time pressure. This suggests that, although similar, the
construct.
95
Table 16
Variable B SE B p
Step 1
Step 2
* p<.05
96
Table 17
Variable B SEB P
Step 1
Step 2
Work-to-Family Conflict .97 .44 .26**
Table 18
Variable B SEB ß
Step 1
Step 2
Work-to-Family Conflict .56 .53 .12
Table 19
Variable B SEB P
Step 1
Table 20
Variable B SEB ß
Step 1
Step 2
Work-to-Family Conflict .01 .07 .02
Table 21
Variable B SEB ß
Step 1
Table 22
Variable B SEB ß
Step 1
Step 2
Work-to-Family Conflict .03 .07 .05
**p<.01
* p < .05
102
Table 23
Variable B SEB P
Step 1
Step 2
Work-to-Family Conflict -2.55 1.29 -.22*
Table 24
Variable B SEB ß
Step 1
Step 2
Table 25
Variable B SEB P
Step 1
Step 2
Table 26
Variable B SEB ß
Step 1
Step 2
After completing the aforementioned analyses using the initial sample. I applied
the same statistical analyses to the holdout sample (N=273) as a method of cross-
Demographic Information
The demographic characteristics of the holdout sample were similar to the initial
Scale Assessment
Work/life balance scale item descriptive statistics are presented in Table 28.
Item-total correlations for each of the items were consistent with the results obtained
using the initial sample, and are presented in Table 29. Overall, the item total
correlations in the holdout sample were slightly lower on the WIPL and WPLE
dimensions and higher on the PLIW dimension compared to the corrected item-total
correlations obtained in the initial sample. Despite these general differences in trend, all
corrected item-total correlations in the holdout sample were above .45 (lowest was
r = .53), suggesting that each item was sufficiently related to the other items on the scale.
work/life balance in the holdout sample indicated that the scale dimensions demonstrated
acceptable levels of reliability (ex's = .93, .76, and .76, respectively). The coefficient
alpha for the PLIW dimension was slightly lower in this sample compared to the initial
sample (a=.83 vs. a = .76; consistent with the trend of lower corrected item-total
Table 27
Age 18 to 24 years .4
25 to 29 years 2.2
30 to 34 years 11.0
35 to 39 years 18.3
40 to 44 years 17.6
45 to 49 years 21.2
50 to 54 years 17.6
55 to 59 years 8.4
60 to 64 years 3.3
65 years or older 0
Number of children
living in household at least
50% of the time 0 23.1
1 29.2
2 30.3
3 14.9
4 or more 2.6
Table 27 (continued)
Work/Life Balance Scale Item Means and Standard Deviations - Holdout Sample
Item Mean SD
1. I struggle with trying to juggle both my work and non-work responsibilities. 3.28 1.07
2. My personal life drains me of the energy I need to do my job. 2.03 0.79
4. My work suffers because of everything going on in my personal life. 1.85 0.70
5. Iam too tired to be effective at work because of things I have going on in my personal life. 1.82 0.71
6. My job gives me energy to pursue activities outside of work that are important to me. 2.74 1.07
7. I have difficulty scheduling vacation time because of my workload. 2.99 1.14
8. Iam unable to relax at home because I am preoccupied with my work. 2.77 1.03
9. I am happy with the amount of time I spend doing activities not related to work. 3.14 1.16
10. Because of my job, I am in a better mood at home. 2.76 1.00
11.1 often have to make difficult choices between my work and my personal life 2.79 0.93
12.1 have to put aspects of my personal life “on hold" because of my work. 2.73 1.03
14.1 am in a better mood at work because of everything I have going for me in my personal life. 3.64 0.95
15. I often neglect my personal needs because of the demands of my work. 2.88 1.02
17. I have difficulty getting my work done because 1 am preoccupied with personal matters at work. 1.94 0.72
18. My personal life suffers because of my work. 2.62 1.05
19. I have to miss out on important personal activities due to the amount of time I spend doing work. 2.44 0.95
20. My personal life gives me the energy to do my job. 3.39 1.03
21.1 feel that I allocate appropriate amounts of time to both work and non-work activities. 3.39 1.14
22.1 make personal sacrifices to get work done. 3.13 0.92
23.1 come home from work too tired to do things I would like to do. 3.21 1.03
24. My job makes it difficult to maintain the kind of personal life I would like. 2.77 1.17
©
©
110
Table 29
Corrected Item-Total Correlations for Work/Life Balance Scale Items - Holdout Sample
Note. WIPL = Work Interference with Personal Life’ PLIW = Personal Life
Interference with Work; WPLE = Work/Personal Life Enhancement
Ill
Next, I computed the bivariate correlations between each scale item and the one-
item measure of overall work/life balance. These correlations are presented in Table 30.
In general, these correlations are similar to those obtained in the initial sample. However,
there are two differences worthy of noting. First, the correlations between PLIW items
and overall work/life balance were lower in the holdout sample compared to the initial
sample (r’s ranged from -.15 to -.18 in the holdout sample, whereas they ranged from -.20
to -.26 in the initial sample). This suggested that the personal life interference with work
items were less strongly related to perceptions of overall work/life balance in the second
The factor structure of the holdout sample was assessed using partial confirmatory
factor analysis to test the fit of the PCFA model obtained with the initial sample to the
data in the holdout sample. The three-factor model with two errors allowed to covary fit
the data reasonably well (x2 (184) = 466.07, TLI = .977, CFI = .982, NFI = .971, and
RMSEA = .075. The RMSEA was below Browne and Cudeck's (1992) recommendation
of .08 as an upper bound of good fit, and overall, these results suggest that the 3-factor
correlations, and scale reliability estimates summarized in the previous section are
approaches to item and scale analysis based on classical test theory’. Classical test theory
(CTT) assumes that any obtained score on a variable of interest is equal to the sum of the
true score and measurement error (i.e., random error; Guion, 1998). Another approach to
scale assessment is an item analysis based on item response theory (IRT). Item response
112
Table 30
Correlations Between Each Work/Life Balance Scale Item and One-Item Work/Life
theory links the probability of a particular item response (i.e., the likelihood of
“almost all the time” ) to the characteristic or latent trait that is being assessed by the test
(e.g., work interference with personal life; Drasgow & Hulin, 1990). One advantage of
IRT over CTT is that IRT parameter estimates do not vary based on the distribution of the
trait (Hambleton, Swaminathan, & Rogers, 1991). In other words, CTT takes into
account the measure as a whole, whereas IRT permits researchers to use any individual
approach" to scale development that combines CTT and IRT approaches to seek
applying Samejima's (1989) graded response model to the data using Multilog software
(Thissen, 1991). The graded response model has been recommended for use with
polytomous data (i.e., when there are more than two response options for an item; Zickar,
conducted three IRT analyses: one for each of the three work/life balance dimensions.
The first step in interpreting the IRT results was to examine the a parameter for
each of the initial work/life balance scale items. The a parameter measures item
discrimination ability (Zickar, personal communication; Reeve and Smith, 2001). IRT a
parameter estimates obtained in the present study and the standard error of measurement
evidence of the sensitivity of the scale, which refers to how much information is provided
by the scale at all levels of the trait being measured (e.g., WIPL, PLIW, or WPLE). The
purpose of examining the TIF is to determine the extent to which the scale items measure
the work/life balance content domain across all levels of work/life balance. In other
words, for the scale to be most useful and widely applicable, it is important for scale
items to measure work/life balance at low, moderate, and high levels of work/life
balance. The test information function is determined by plotting test information (0)
obtained for the test (i.e., each dimension of work/life balance) along the y-axis, and the
levels of theta (0), which range from -2.0 to +2.0 along the x-axis. Test information
functions for WIPL, PLIW, and WPLE are shown in Figures 3-5, respectively. These
figures are useful for identifying the theta levels (i.e., 0; levels of the latent trait) for
The test information function for the first dimension, WIPL, indicated that this
13-item scale provided information about the latent trait at most levels, although it was
less sensitive toward higher levels of work interference with personal life. (This was
indicated by the line on the graph being lower for high levels of theta compared to low or
moderate levels.) In other words, this thirteen-item scale was better at measuring work
interference with personal life among respondents who had little interference or a
moderate amount compared to those who perceived that their work interferes with their
With regard to personal life interference with work (PLIW) and work/personal
life enhancement (WPLE), each four-item scale was most sensitive at middle levels of the
115
Table 31
IRT a Parameters for Each Work/Life Balance Scale Item - Total Study Sample
11 1.59 .13
12 2.52 .20
15 2.41 .17
18 3.32 .24
19 2.28 .19
21 1.75 .13
22 1.78 .13
23 1.71 .15
24 2.70 .20
Note. N = 540
116
Figure 3
IRT Test Information Function for WIPL dimension - Total Study Sample (N=540)
Information
Figure 4
IRT Test Information Function for PLIW dimension - Total Study Sample (N=540)
Theta
118
Figure 5
IRT Test Information Function for WPLE dimension - Total Study Sample (N=540)
Theta
119
construct (thetas of-1.0 and +0.5) and much less sensitive at the high end (e.g., for
individuals with a high degree of personal life interference with work, etc.). For all three
scales, none was particularly useful for assessing high levels of each dimension, but
enhancement.
second phase of scale development. Exploratory factor analysis results, corrected item-
total correlation coefficients, and estimates of scale reliability (coefficient alpha) in the
initial sample suggested that two of these items be removed from the scale: Items 13 and
16 (as listed in Appendix C). Item 13 (“I am able to accomplish what I would like in
both my personal and work lives”) cross-loaded on two factors in the initial exploratory
factor analysis, making it difficult to determine how to interpret the item. Item 16
(“When I am at work I worry about things I have to do outside of work”) was removed
because the corrected item-total correlations and coefficient alphas computed with and
without the item indicated that Item 16 was not strongly related to the other items on the
same dimension, and the scale demonstrated higher reliability without Item 16. A third
scale item. Item 3 (“I feel overwhelmed when I try to balance my work and personal
life”), was removed from the scale based on the results obtained from a partial
Removal of Items 3, 13, and 16 improved the measurement properties of the scale
and resulted in a 21-item scale assessing three dimensions: work interference with
personal life (WIPL), personal life interference with work (PLIW), and work/personal
120
life enhancement (WPLE). A partial confirmatory factor analysis of the scale yielded a
21-item, three-factor model that indicated good fit to the data across both samples.
Work interference with personal life. WIPL was assessed with 13 items, and
demonstrated very high internal consistency reliability across both samples (a = .94 and
a = .93 for the initial and holdout samples, respectively). The coefficient alphas obtained
for this scale were high due to similarity in item content as well as a large number of
The content of most of the items on this scale specifically assessed the extent to
which work has a negative impact on one’s personal life. The item with the highest
factor loading and best ability to discriminate on the latent construct was Item 18 (“My
personal life suffers because of my work”). Results also indicated that Items 12, 15, and
among the items and high reliability is that there is semantic similarity in item content,
although the items did not seem to overlap completely with one another.
Two of the items on the WIPL scale (Items 9 and 21) were worded positively,
such that higher scores on these items indicated a lack of interference. These items were
recoded to be consistent with the other 11 items. Lower levels of work interference with
personal life were interpreted as indicative of higher levels of work/life balance. This
interpretation is consistent with the finding that WIPL items were strongly correlated
In addition to computing the scale item statistics based on classical test theory
(e.g., factor analysis, corrected item-total correlations, and reliability), I conducted item
121
response theory analyses to gather additional psychometric information about the scale.
First, these IRT results described which items were more useful for discriminating among
individuals with high and low levels of WIPL. All thirteen items discriminated
discriminated best, followed by Items 24, 12, 15, and 19, respectively. These results
converged with findings from the classical test theory analyses such that the items with
the highest a parameters also had the highest factor loadings and corrected item-total
correlations.
With regard to item content, nine of the WIPL items (Items 7, 8, 12, 15, 18, 19,
22. 23, and 24) implied a directional effect of work on personal life, such that work
affects or spills over into one's personal life. The remaining four items on this dimension
(Items 1,9, 11, and 21) did not imply this same directional effect, but were still more
strongly related to the items on this dimension than to items on either of the other two
dimensions in both samples. This finding suggested that these items were more
indicative of the extent to w hich work affects one's personal life than vice-versa (i.e., the
strain, and goal accomplishment. Review of the content of these items suggested that
four of the WIPL items (Items 7, 9, 19, and 21) incorporated issues of time, four items
(Items 8, 15, 18, and 24) described strain experienced in one’s personal life due to work,
one item (Item 23) pertained to energy, and the remaining items referred to
components of the work/life balance definition were included in items on the WIPL
dimension.
Personal life interference with work. The second dimension of the work/life
balance scale included four items that assessed the extent to which one’s personal life
interferes with one’s work (PLIW). Internal consistency reliability coefficients for this
scale obtained in both samples (a = .83 and a = .76) indicated that the scale had
acceptable levels of reliability. The lower alphas obtained for this dimension compared
to the WIPL scale could be attributed to less overlap in item content and fewer items in
All four of the PLIW items demonstrated acceptable levels of item discrimination
based on results of the IRT analysis. Among the four items. Items 4 (“My work suffers
because of everything going on in my personal life’’ and 5 (“I am too tired to be effective
at work because of things I have going on in my personal life’’) were the most
discriminating items. With regard to scale sensitivity, the PLIW scale provided more
information for individuals with low or moderate levels of PLIW than for high levels of
PLIW. In other words, these four items indicated very little about individuals whose
personal life interferes a great deal with their work. Additional items would have to be
written and assessed before being able to gather information about individuals with high
PLIW.
Compared to WIPL, items on the PLIW scale assessed the opposite direction of
the work/personal interface: the extent to which aspects of one’s personal life has a
negative impact on work. Examples of the items that loaded highly on this dimension
were Item 5 and Item 2 (“My personal life drains me of the energy I need to do my job.”).
123
With regard to the four components of work/life balance, Items 2 and 5 dealt with having
energy for work, Item 4 referred to strain experienced as a result of one's personal life
interfering with work, and Item 16 pertained to goal accomplishment (e.g., getting work
done). None of the items on this dimension incorporated the other issue identified as a
salient aspect of work/life balance: time. Future scale development efforts should be
directed toward writing new items to incorporate personal life interference with work as
it relates to time. An example of this type of item would be “My personal life takes away
The four items on this scale demonstrated high reliability and seemed to assess
personal life interference with work. However, unlike items on the WIPL scale, none of
the PLIW items correlated very highly with the one-item measure of overall work/life
balance. This finding suggested that perceptions of overall work/life balance were more
strongly related to the effect of work on personal life than the effect of personal life on
work.
assessed the extent to which one's personal life is enhanced by work or vice versa. The
four items on this scale were more highly related with one another than with items on
either of the other two dimensions, suggesting that measurement of the work/personal life
interface involved more than just directional differences in which work affects personal
life and vice versa. These four items demonstrated acceptable levels of reliability across
two samples of data (a = .77 and a = .76). The internal consistency reliability estimates
for this dimension were lower in the first sample compared to WIPL and PLIW, and
about the same as PLIW in the second sample. The corrected item total correlations
124
obtained for these four items were high, ranging from .68 to .75, suggesting that each
Two of the WPLE scale items (Items 14 and 20) seemed to discriminate much
better than Items 6 and 10. Item 14 was the most discriminating item, followed by Item
20, and then Items 10 and 6. The test item function for the WPLE scale was similar to
the TIF obtained for the PLIW scale; these four items as a whole provided more
information at low and moderate levels of the construct than at high levels. In other
words, these items assessed the extent to which work and personal life enhance one
another for those who reported low and moderate levels of enhancement, but is not useful
for identifying or describing individuals who perceive their personal life adds a great deal
In terms of item content, two of the WPLE items assessed the extent to which
work enhances personal life (Items 6 and 10), and two items referred to the positive
impact of one’s personal life on work. Unlike the previous two scale dimensions, in
which items were related with one another based on the direction of influence across the
work/personal life interface, these four items were related to one another regardless of the
direction of enhancement. These results can be explained in two ways. First, there were
not enough items to be able to detect this different response pattern. In other words, more
items assessing the positive contributions of work to one's personal life and vice versa
may have resulted in finding two separate dimensions. Secondly, the direction of
influence of one domain on another (work on personal life, or personal life on work) may
Validity Evidence
Results obtained in the initial sample (N = 267) supported the predictions made
regarding the relations among work/life balance and other variables. Further evidence for
validity of the scale is presented in this section, which describes the results obtained in
Scale descriptive statistics. Scale means and standard deviations computed for all
study variables in the holdout sample are presented in Table 32. The mean distributions
and pattern of work/life balance scale means compared to one another were the same as
Hypothesis Tests
balance scale, the next step was to examine the relations between each of the work/life
balance scale dimensions and the other variables measured in this study to test the
hypotheses in a second sample. Scale means and standard deviations for all study
variables are presented in Table 33, and the correlation matrix appears in Table 34. I
conducted a series of independent sample t-tests to examine whether any of the scale
means differed between the initial sample and the holdout sample. There were no
work/family conflict. Consistent with the initial sample, correlations obtained from the
holdout sample indicated a very strong relationship between WIPL and WFC, and a fairly
strong relationship between PLIW and FWC. WIPL was related to both WFC and FWC.
Table 32
Standard
Scale Mean Deviation
1. WIPL - Work Interference with Personal Life 2.85 0.75
2. PLIW - Personal Life Interference with Work 1.91 0.59
3. WPLE - Work/Personal Life Enhancement 3.13 0.78
4. Overall Work/Personal Life Balance 3.38 1.06
5. WFC - Work-to-Family Conflict 2.78 0.91
6. FWC - Family-to-Work Conflict 2.01 0.71
7. Role Overload 3.62 0.77
8. Stress - Pressure 17.98 3.17
9. Stress - Threat 17.92 4.39
10. Time Pressure 3.42 0.79
11. Time Management Behavior (TMB)- Setting Goals and Priorities 3.64 0.58
12. Time Management Behavior (TMB)- Planning and Organizing 3.55 0.67
13. Time Management Behavior (TMB)- Preference for Organization 3.75 0.61
14. Job Satisfaction 42.38 10.14
15. Life Satisfaction 3.31 0.85
16. Organizational Commitment 3.81 0.71
17. Turnover Intentions 2.46 1.16
K)
O
Table 33
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 IP 11 12 13 ]4 J5 IP 12
1. WIPL .93
2. PLIW .31** .76
3. WPLE -.64** -.40** .76
4. Overall W/L Bal. -.70** -.21»* .57** --
5. WFC .84** .31** -.54** -.69** .90
6. FWC .22** .51** -.30** -.11 .27** .89
7. Role Overload .55** .16** -.34** -.44** .51** .02 .67
8. Stress - Pressure .51** .17** -.36** -.43** .45** .07 .53** .78
9. Stress - Threat .56** .19** -.45** -.53** .52** .10 .53** .63** .80
10. Time Pressure .62** .30** -.45** -.47** .58** .19** .74** .56** .64** .84
11. TMB-Goals -.20** -.29** .35** .24** -.20** -.26** -.07 -.05 -.17** -.15* .81
12. TMB - Planning -.16** -.18** .24** .19** -.16** -.16** -.06 -.10 -.17** - 15* .63 *♦ .75
13. TMB-Org. -.44** -.38** .38** .31** -.38** -.31** -.35** -.34** -.41»* -.50** .39** .35** .76
14. Job Sat. -.41** -.21** .45** .43** -.42** -.13* -.29** -.17** -.45** -.35** .21** .12* .18** .90
15. Life Sat. -.56** -.34** .63** .64** -.55** -.26** -.32** -.27** -.43** -.45** .34** .25** .31** .52** .86
16. Org. Comm. -.22** -.30** .32** .26** -.27** -.11 -.16** -.11 -.20** -.25** .23** .14* .11 .54** .37** .80
17. Turnover .34** .26** -.37** -.38** .36** .10 .29** .24** .40** .36** -.19** -.09 -.24** -.60** -.39** -.70** .91
Note. Coefficient alpha for each scale appears on diagonal. (N=273)
** p<.01
* p < .05
t-j
"si
128
related to role overload. In the initial sample, Hypothesis 2 was supported for two of the
work/life balance dimensions: WIPL and WPLE, but not PLIW. The same pattern of
results was found in the holdout sample, such that no significant relationship was found
related to perceptions of time pressure. Results in the second sample indicated that all
three dimensions of work/life balance were related to perceptions of time pressure. The
relationship between time pressure and WIPL was strong, as indicated by a high
correlation of r = .62. These results were consistent with the correlations obtained in the
first sample. In other words, individuals with more interference and less enhancement
across the work/personal interface experienced more time pressure those with less
the initial sample, all three dimensions of time management behavior (setting goals and
priorities, planning and scheduling, and preference for organization) were significantly
correlated with WPLE in the predicted direction, and two dimensions (setting goals and
priorities and preference for organization) were related to PLIW. In addition, WIPL was
also related to preference for organization. These results were consistent with findings
from the first sample, except that the correlation between PLIW and planning and
balance and feelings of overall job stress, or a positive relationship between stress and
interference. Correlations among all three dimensions of work/life balance and stress
were statistically significant. However, although the general pattern of results obtained in
the second sample was consistent with findings from the first sample, the correlations
between PLIW and stress were low (r = . 17 and r = . 19), and therefore not indicative of a
very strong relationship between personal life interference with work and feelings of
positively related to overall job satisfaction. The results obtained in the second sample
were consistent with the first sample, indicating that WIPL, PLIW, and WPLE were
balance were related to perceptions of life satisfaction. The same pattern of correlations
was obtained in the second sample, such that WPLE was more strongly related to life
satisfaction than either work interference with personal life or personal life interference
with work.
balance and organizational commitment. In the second sample, all three work/life
indicated more support for Hypothesis 7 than I obtained in the initial sample because the
sample (r = -.22) was larger than the correlation between the same two variables in the
balance would be negatively related to their intentions to leave their work organization.
Similar to results obtained in the initial sample, all three dimensions of work/life balance
enhancement was more strongly related to turnover intentions than either of the other
Hypothesis 10. Next I tested Hypothesis 10. which stated that work/life balance
would be empirically distinct from WFC. To accomplish this, I repeated the hierarchical
regression analyses conducted earlier to test for incremental validity of work/life balance
Although results in the initial sample failed to indicate that work/life balance
accounted for incremental variance over work/family conflict in role overload and time
pressure, results in the second sample did find this effect (AR2 = .05, F (2,3) = 6.69,
p < .01 for role overload, and AR2 = .07, F (2,3) = 10.15, p < .01 for time pressure). For
the pressure dimension of overall work stress and time pressure, results in the holdout
sample indicated an even larger effect size compared to the initial sample (AR2 = .06,
F (2,3) = 7.14, p < .01 for overall stress in terms of pressure, compared to AR2 = .02 for
pressure in the prior sample). Although the incremental validity of work/life balance
with regard to job satisfaction was significant in the second sample (AR = .07,
131
F (2,3) = 8.42, p < .01), the effect size of .07 was smaller than the effect size of .18
obtained in the initial sample. The effect sizes obtained for other criterion variables in
the holdout sample were comparable to those found in the initial sample.
In summary, results of these analyses were consistent with prior findings such that
work/life balance (WIPL, PLIW, and WPLE) accounted for significant variance in each
of the other variables beyond work/family conflict. Hence, the results of these
that, although similar, work/life balance and work/family conflict are empirically distinct
constructs.
Exploratory Analyses
Demographic differences
analyses were conducted to determine whether the managers who participated in this
The full sample (N=540) was used for these analyses in order to have a large enough
sample size in each of the subgroups, which was necessary to increase statistical power.
perceptions of work interference with personal life, personal life interference with work,
or work/personal life enhancement; nor were there any differences based on education
level, p > .05. However, managers' perceptions did differ with respect to age on all three
dimensions of work/life balance (F (3,536) = 4.12, p < .01 for WIPL; F (3, 536) = 4.68,
p < .01 for PLIW, and F (3, 536) = 4.96, p < .01 for WPLE). Specifically, managers age
sixty or above reported significantly less work interference with personal life compared
132
to managers between the ages of 50 and 59 or younger. Personal life interference with
work appeared to decrease with age such that mean levels of PLIW were lower among
older managers (60 and older) compared to managers younger than 50 years of age.
Similarly, work/personal life enhancement increased with age; managers above age 60
Figures 6 to 8.
The total number of hours employees worked per week was significantly related
to work interference with personal life (r = .25, g < .01), but was unrelated to either of the
other two dimensions of work/life balance. Although the correlation between average
total work hours per week was significantly related to WIPL, the magnitude of the
correlation may have been attenuated by the low variability in work hours; more than half
the sample (54%) reported working an average of 45 to 55 hours per week, and 21%
The sixty-three managers who reported having elder care responsibilities (i.e.,
primary responsibility for caring for one or more of their parents, in-laws, or other
relatives) had significantly lower levels of work/personal life enhancement than the
Figure 6
Age Differences in Work Interference with Personal Life - Total Study Sample (N=540)
Mean of WIP
Age
134
Figure 7
Age Differences in Personal Life Interference with Work - Total Study Sample (N=540)
Mean of PLIW
Age
135
Figure 8
Age
136
In terms of the number of children who live at home at least 50% of the time,
those who have at least one child reported higher levels of WPLE compared to employees
without children at home (t(388) = -2.16, p < .05). Consistent with other findings, no
effects were found for WIPL or PLIW, p > .05. No significant differences were found
regarding the relationship between work/life balance and the age of children, p > .05.
asking respondents whether they perceived their personal life as a priority over work,
work as a priority over personal life, or both work and personal life as equally important.
Fourteen percent indicated that work was their priority, 29% indicated that personal life
was their priority, and the majority (i.e., 55%) indicated that they were both equally
important. A comparison of mean differences with regard to priority indicated that those
who prioritize their work over their personal life experience significantly more work
interference with their personal life (t (234) = -8.54, p < .05), less personal life
interference with work (t (234) = 2.32, p < .05), and less work/personal life enhancement
(t (234) = 3.47, p < .05) compared to those who indicated that their personal life is a
jobs and organizations. No differences were found regarding length of time in current
position, length of tenure within one's organization, or management level within the
p > .05. However, 1 found a clear difference with regard to salary level. Specifically,
managers who earn more money reported significantly more interference of their work
137
with their personal life (F (5,523) = 4.01, p < .01. A graph depicting mean levels of
WIPL for each salary level is presented in Figure 9. There were no differences in mean
work/personal life enhancement dimension accounted for unique variance in the criterion
measures after accounting for work/personal life interference (i.e., WIPL and PLIW).
interference. Results for variables in which WPLE accounted for a significant portion of
Work Interference with Personal Life by Annual Salary - Total Study Sample (N=540)
00
139
Table 34
Variable B SEB P
Step 1
Step 2
Work Interference with Personal Life .11 .04 .14**
Table 35
Variable B SEB ß
Step 1
Step 2
Work Interference with Personal Life .04 .05 .05
Table 36
Variable B SEB ß
Step 1
Step 2
Work Interference with Personal Life 2.52 .27 .43**
Table 37
Variable B SEB P
Step 1
Step 2
Work Interference with Personal Life -1.26 .67 -.09
Table 38
Variable B SEB ß
Step 1
Step 2
Work Interference with Personal Life -.30 .05 -.27**
Table 39
Variable B SEB ß
Step 1
Step 2
Work Interference with Personal Life .08 .05 .08
Table 40
Variable B SEB P
Step 1
Step 2
Work Interference with Personal Life .13 .08 .08
CHAPTER V. DISCUSSION
The purpose of the present study was to extend previous work/non-work conflict
the construct and developed a 31-item scale and reduced it to 21 items through empirical
population of managers, responses to these items fit a three-factor model in both samples.
The three dimensions that emerged were work interference with personal life (WIPL),
personal life interference with work (PLIW), and work/personal life enhancement
(WPLE). In this final chapter, I discuss the present study with regard to scale item
generation, scale item assessment, and initial validity evidence for scale. Next, I compare
implications of the results. Finally, I present the limitations of this study and overall
conclusions.
literature, and the conservation of resources theory (Hobfoll, 1988; 1989). This
occupational stressor. Four potential sources of imbalance were identified: time (e.g., the
amount of time spent at work compared to time spent pursuing non-work activities),
behavior (e.g., accomplishing one's work and non-work goals), strain (e.g., tension or
anxiety resulting from the process of trying to meet work and non-work demands), and
147
energy (e.g., having energy available to pursue non-work activities after a full-day’s
work).
Items included in the work/life balance measure were derived from the conceptual
as part of the construct definition process, existing measures of work/family conflict (e.g.,
Carlson et al., 2000; Netemeyer et al., 1996), and recommendations made by work/family
researchers (e.g., MacDermid et al., 2000). A total of 31 items were written and
administered to a panel of subject matter experts for content validation. Panelist ratings
of item relevance to the work/life balance construct were useful for reducing the item
pool to a smaller group of appropriate items; seven items were discarded from the initial
item pool because they did not seem to adequately assess the construct domain.
The twenty-four work/life balance scale items remaining after the initial content
part of a larger survey. Responses gathered from managers were randomly divided into
two groups; the second group was retained as a holdout sample for cross-validation
purposes.
Three of the twenty-four work/life balance items were removed from the scale
of the scale and resulted in a 21-item measure with acceptable psychometric properties
assessing three dimensions: work interference with personal life (WIPL), personal life
WIPL was assessed with 13 items, and demonstrated very high internal
consistency reliability across both samples. The content of most of the items on this scale
specifically assessed the extent to which work has a negative impact on one's personal
life. Content examination of the WIPL items suggested all four components of the
work/life balance definition (e.g. time, energy, goal accomplishment, and strain) were
The second dimension of the work/life balance scale included four items that
assessed the extent to which one's personal life interferes with work (PLIW). Internal
consistency reliability coefficients obtained in both samples indicated that the scale had
Compared to WIPL, items on the PLIW scale assessed the opposite direction of
the work/personal interface: the extent to which aspects of one's personal life have a
negative impact on work. Although these four items assessed work/life balance in terms
of goal accomplishment, energy, and strain, none of the items on this dimension
incorporated the other issue identified as a salient aspect of work/life balance: time.
Future scale development efforts should be directed toward writing new items to
incorporate personal life interference with work as it relates to time. Examples of this
type of item would be “My personal life takes away time that I would otherwise spend
work time.”
149
The third work/life balance scale dimension assessed the extent to which one's
personal life is enhanced by work or vice versa. These four items demonstrated
acceptable levels of reliability across two samples of data (a = .77 and a = .76).
the negative impact of work on family and vice versa; only recently have other
researchers (e.g., Gryzwacz, 2000; Gryzwacz & Marks, 2000) quantitatively assessed
positive spillover with regard to work and family. The present study extended beyond
this work by examining negative as well as positive influences in more than just family in
the non-work domain. Future research should examine whether there arc directional
differences regarding the positive influence of work on personal life and vice-versa.
Results obtained in both samples regarding the factor structure of the scales
indicated that the three-factor model, with WIPL, PLIW, and WPLE as three separate
dimensions, fit the data well. Although this factor structure was cross-validated in the
managerial jobs. Future research should examine the extent to which this factor structure
Item response theory findings indicated that each item in the 21-item scale
discriminated well, although only the WIPL dimension was able to provide information
at high levels of the underlying construct. New items should be developed to increase the
sensitivity of the scale at higher levels of personal life interference with work and
work/personal life enhancement. Increasing the sensitivity of the scale across all levels
150
of all three dimensions would serve to make the scale more useful for developing models
of work/life balance to understand the relationship work/life balance in the stress process
Scale Validity
The three subscales of the work/life balance scale were significantly related to one
another. Specifically, WIPE and WPLE were more highly correlated than WIPL and
PLIW, and PLIW and WPLE. Individuals who experienced less work interference with
personal life or personal life interference with work were more likely to perceive that one
domain enhanced the other. These correlations were not sufficiently large enough to
suggest that each dimension was in fact measuring the same thing (r’s ranged from .31 to
.64 across both samples); the empirical evidence suggested that the three scales seemed to
Convergent Validity
Evidence of convergent validity was assessed in two ways. First, I examined the
correlation between the three dimensions of work/life balance and a one-item overall
measure of the construct (WEB). All three dimensions were significantly related to the
overall measure. However, the strength of the relationship varied significantly across the
three dimensions. WIPL was strongly related to overall work/life balance, suggesting
that they are measuring very similar constructs. The correlation between WPLE and the
one-item measure was also fairly strong, though not as high as the correlation between
WIPL and WLB. Although PLIW was also significantly related to WLB, the relationship
they have a balance between their work and personal life are closely aligned with whether
they perceive their work as conflicting with their personal life and whether their work and
personal lives have positive effects on one another. The extent to which one’s personal
life interferes with work was related to overall perceptions of balance, but not nearly to
the same extent as the other two dimensions. This implies an asymmetry in the
importance of work versus personal life, which is consistent with the finding that more
role overload, and time pressure. Because two of the work/life balance scale dimensions
implied directional effects from one-domain to another (i.e., WIPL and PLIW) similar to
WFC and FWC, the respective scales were compared in two ways. First I compared
WIPL to WFC, and then compared PLIW to FWC. The correlations between WIPL and
WFC were very high in both samples (r = .87 and r =.84), which indicated very high
WIPL were very similar to their experience of work-to-family conflict. The correlation
between PLIW and FWC was also fairly high (above .50 in both samples), indicating that
the extent to which one's personal life interfered with work was similar to whether they
Role overload. According to role theory, the more roles one takes on or is
responsible for, the more role-related tasks or goals exist that need to be accomplished
152
(Kahn et al., 1964). Role overload refers to having too many behaviors to perform as part
of one’s role. Work/life balance was negatively related to role overload as expected for
both WIPL and WPLE. These results suggest that the more one is expected to do as part
of their job, the more likely their work would interfere with their personal life, and the
less their work would have a positive effect on one’s personal life.
Unlike WIPL and WPLE, however, PLIW and role overload were unrelated. The
role overload measure employed in the present study was specific to managers' work
role, and did not assess the extent to which managers' perceived they had too much to do
in any non-work roles. Therefore, having too much to do at work does not seem to be
related to whether an employee's personal life interferes with work. However, having
too much to do outside of work (i.e., non-work role overload) may be more closely
related to the extent to which one's personal life interferes with work. Future research
In addition, managers who responded to this survey did not report a great deal of
personal life interference with work (i.e., M = 1.90, SD = .60 across both samples).
Perhaps PLIW would be more strongly related to work role overload in samples with
perceive that they have a limited amount of time to accomplish a task or set of tasks.
Work/life balance was negatively related to time pressure as expected for all three
individuals will experience stress if they are threatened by a potential loss in resources,
experience an actual loss, or fail to gain expected resources (Hobfoll, 1988; 1989).
Taking time into account as a valued and limited resource, it follows from this theory that
one will experience stress if they lose time or fear they might lose time. Because
work/life balance involves time (e.g., the amount of time one spends at work compared to
pursuing non-work activities), it follows that employees with more of a balance between
work and personal life will experience less pressure for time. In other words, the results
These results regarding time pressure are consistent with the findings for role
overload, except that time pressure was related to all three dimensions of work/life
balance whereas role overload was only related to two. It was not surprising to find
similar results for time pressure and role overload because these constructs are
conceptually similar (e.g., time pressure is related to having too much to do), and
measures of these two constructs were highly correlated (r = .74 and .79 in the two
samples, respectively). However, given the similarity between the two constructs, then
why was PLIW significantly related to time pressure but not role overload? There are
three possible explanations for these results. First, the time pressure scale was more
internally consistent than role overload (a = .84 for time pressure, compared to a = .70 or
.67 for role overload). According to classical test theory, less internal consistency
between variables (Guion, 1998). It follows that the higher the internal consistency of a
scale, the more the correlation between two measures estimates the true relationship
between those variables. In terms of the present studv, the low reliability for role
154
overload may have attenuated the correlation between role overload and work/life
balance. Had the role overload measure been more reliable, it is possible that PLIW and
role overload would have been more highly correlated, as found for time pressure.
A second explanation pertains to the content of the survey items. Two of the five
time pressure items were not specific to work, whereas all of the role overload items
asked respondents about their work role(s). It could be that respondents took time
pressure in their personal lives into account when responding to these survey items,
resulting in more similar responses between time pressure and PLIW compared to role
overload and PLIW. Additional research using a verbal protocol analysis approach, in
which respondents explain their thought process while responding to survey items, might
could exist because time pressure is more conceptually similar to work/life balance than
role overload. To the extent that time may be a more salient component of the work/life
balance construct than goal accomplishment (i.e., accomplishing the tasks set forth as
part of a role), then time pressure may be more closely related to work/life balance.
Taken together, the pattern of the correlations obtained in both the initial and
holdout samples indicates strong evidence of convergent validity for the new work/life
balance scale. Work/life balance was strongly related to measure of other occupational
Discriminant Validity
between dimensions of work/life balance and other study variables (e.g., overall work
155
stress, job satisfaction, etc.) than were found between specific work/life balance
dimensions and very similar constructs (e.g., overall work/life balance, work-to-family
conflict, and family-to-work conflict). For example, the general pattern of correlations
obtained across both samples in the present study indicated that the dimensions of
work/life balance were more strongly related to perceptions of overall work/life balance,
and work/family conflict (as in the very high correlations between WIPL and WFC, and
strong relationship between PLIW and FWC) than to most other variables which were not
as conceptually similar. These results indicate that, although work/personal life balance
was related to other variables in this study, it was empirically distinguished from other,
Criterion-related Validity
this study indicated that work/life balance was negatively related to perceptions of overall
job stress. Support for this hypothesis was based on the strong positive correlation found
between work interference with personal life (WIPL) and stress, as well as the strong
However, perceptions of personal life interference with work (PLIW) were not as
One explanation for why PLIW was not related to perceptions of stress is
consistent with what was previously described for role overload. Specifically, overall job
stress refers to the amount of strain (e.g., feeling tense, overwhelmed, etc.) one feels on
156
the job. It seems that work-related stressors rather than personal-life related stressors are
stress indicated a slightly stronger relationship between work/life balance and pressure
compared to work/life balance and stress. These results may be explained by what is
actually being measured by each dimension of overall work stress. For example,
researchers who developed the overall stress scale employed in the present study (e.g.,
Stanton et al., in press; Fisher et al., 2000; Fisher, Stanton, Grauer, & Smith, 2001) have
suggested that although both dimensions of stress (pressure and threat) assess overall
levels of work stress, pressure assesses more moderate and perhaps benign levels of
managers in the present study reported rather high mean levels of pressure, and more
moderate levels of threat. The distribution of overall stress responses was negatively
skewed for pressure, and more normally distributed for threat. As a result, the highly
skewed distribution of pressure may have attenuated the observed correlation between
work/life balance was generally supported. In other words, the more these employees
performed a variety of time management behaviors, the more they perceived that they
achieved a balance between their work and personal lives. These results were consistent
with Adams & Jex’s (1999) finding that time management behavior predicted work-to-
family conflict and family-to-work conflict. This relationship was stronger for two
157
dimensions, WIPL and WPLE, than for WPLE. In particular, preference for organization
was the only dimension of time management behavior that was significantly related to all
The stronger relationship with regard to organization may have been detected
because it is the organizational aspect of time management that helps employees with
being able to manage the demands in both their work and personal lives. In terms of the
conservation of resources theory, efforts toward being we 11-organized may be one way
suggests that limiting the expenditure of resources (e.g., energy or time) would increase
Evidence to support this explanation was suggested by the finding across both samples
that preference for organization was negatively related to overall job stress (both pressure
and threat).
WIPL was not at all related to either setting goals and priorities, or planning and
scheduling. This unexpected finding could be due to the fact that the time management
measure asked respondents about their time management behaviors in the context of
work. As a result, this measure may have been more likely to explain variance in how
one’s personal life interferes with work (PLIW) rather than in how one’s work interferes
with personal life, or when personal life has positive effects on work or vice versa
(WPLE). Aspects of one's personal life are more likely to enhance one’s work life if
they focus on setting goals and priorities and do a lot to plan and schedule their time and
tasks at work. For example, having to attend to one's personal needs (e.g., going to a
158
doctor or dentist appointment) would be more likely to interfere with work if one has a
Job satisfaction. As predicted, results indicated that work/life balance was related
to job satisfaction for all three dimensions of work/life balance. According to the
conceptual definition of work/life balance, part of achieving this balance involves being
able to accomplish one's goals at work. To the extent that an employee is able to
accomplish her work goals without it interfering with her personal life, she may be more
satisfied with her job. Similarly, one may enjoy her job more if her personal life is not
enhancement was more strongly related to job satisfaction than WIPL and PLIW. This
finding may be explained by the notion that the positive effects of one's job on his
personal life, or vice versa lead one to develop more favorable impressions of his job.
Employees with jobs that do not have a negative impact on their personal life, but make
positive contributions to their personal life (e.g., by putting them in a better mood or
giving them more energy at home) are likely to be more satisfied with their job in general
compared to employees who do not have the same experience with their work. The
conservation of resources theory indicates that one may experience strain in a situation
where he or she expects to gain resources but does not, or loses resources. Actually
gaining resources may serve to not only inhibit the development of strains, but lead to
Another explanation for the positive relationship is consistent with the congruence
model (Frone et al., 1992; Morf, 1989; Staines, 1980). That is, a third variable (e.g., an
159
underlying personality trait) may account for similar perceptions regarding work/life
balance and job satisfaction. For example, negative affectivity is one trait that has been
balance) and strains (e.g., job dissatisfaction; Chen & Spector, 1991)
balance were highly related to their overall satisfaction with life. As with job
feelings toward their work organization. As predicted, work/life balance was positively
conflict research that found that employees who experienced more work-to-family
conflict and family-to-work conflict were less committed to their work organization
commitment obtained in the present study may have been slightly attenuated due to low
of managers who participated in this study had been working for the same organization
for at least five years, and the distribution of mean organizational commitment ratings
was negatively skewed such that most respondents were highly committed to their work
organization. Perhaps results would differ in a sample in which there is more variability
job satisfaction. In other words, results of a hierarchical regression analysis showed that
the majority of the variance in organizational commitment was related to job satisfaction
rather than work/life balance itself; work/life balance variables (WIPL, PLIW, and
WPLE combined) only accounted for a small portion of the variance in organizational
commitment after controlling for job satisfaction. That is, job satisfaction accounted for
31% of the variance in organizational commitment (R2 = .31), and work/life balance
explained an additional 3% (AR2 = .03, F (3, 523) = 8.88, p < .01.). This finding is not
surprising, as the organizational psychology literature is replete with studies that have
documented the strong empirical relationship between job satisfaction and organizational
commitment (e.g., Mathieu, 1991; Mathieu & Farr, 1991; Ostroff, 1992). It is worth
reminding the reader, however, that work/life balance was highly related to job
satisfaction. As a result, it seems that work/life balance is both directly and indirectly
(via its relationship with job satisfaction) related to organizational commitment. Future
research should employ longitudinal research designs and analytic tools available, such as
balance outcomes.
balance was also related to managers' intentions to quit their present job. Specifically,
managers with more positive perceptions of work/life balance were had lower turnover
intentions. Although these results supported the research hypothesis, the same partial
mediating effect of job satisfaction was found with regard to turnover. Specifically, in
161
the total study sample, work/life balance did account for unique variability in turnover
beyond job satisfaction (R2 = .37 for job satisfaction and turnover, AR2 = .014,
F (3, 523) = p < .01), as well as beyond both job satisfaction and organizational
commitment (R2 = .53 for job satisfaction and organizational commitment predicting
turnover, AR2= .01, F (3, 523) = p < .01). However, the effect sizes (the AR2 values
obtained in these hierarchical regression analyses) were small, suggesting that work/life
balance predicted little variance in turnover after taking these organizational variables
into account.
ways across both studies in this research investigation. First, the item content ratings
made by the subject matter experts during the early stages of scale development (Study
One) provided initial evidence for content validity of the overall scale. Subsequent
examination of the item content once the factor structure had been identified indicated
that two of the three scale dimensions (PLIW and WPLE) did not seem to measure the
work/life balance content domain as well as they could. Nonetheless, empirical results
obtained in two samples in Study Two demonstrated strong initial evidence for
measure.
Although not every dimension of work/life balance was related to all dimensions
of each criterion, the findings discussed here indicated strong support for the criterion-
related validity of the work/life balance construct. Given the exploratory nature of this
research and early development of this construct, the results presented here suggest that
162
work/life balance holds promise for increasing our understanding of the intersection
between employees' work and personal lives, and hence for more clearly understanding
Altogether, the relationships between work/life balance and other individual and
relationships between work/non-work stressors and strains (Allen et al., 2000; Kossek &
Ozeki, 1998). Findings that work/life balance was significantly related to strains (e.g.,
job satisfaction, overall job stress, etc.) provided empirical evidence that having a lack of
Theoretical Implications
The purpose of the present study was not to test Hobfoll’s (1988; 1989)
conservation of resources theory, but rather to use the theory to guide development of the
work/personal life balance construct. This theory, combined with a review of previous
identified four potential sources of imbalance between employees’ work and personal
lives: time, goal accomplishment, energy, and strain. The significant correlations
between work/life balance and other stressor and strain measures were consistent with
work/life balance and other variables in the expected direction indicated that not being
able to conserve one’s resources was associated with higher perceptions of other stressors
and strains. To the extent that time, energy, goal accomplishment, and lack of strain are
163
perceived as equally salient resources, the number and content of the items on each of the
work/life balance sub-scales should reflect that. As a result, future scale development
work is warranted.
work environment may lead to a number of potential outcomes for both the individual
and the organization for which she works. This study demonstrated that a lack of
work/life balance is one possible stressor that is related to other stressors (e.g., role
overload and time pressure) as well as strains (e.g., life dissatisfaction, job dissatisfaction,
low organizational commitment, and high turnover intentions). Future research is needed
to develop more specific models regarding the role of work/life balance in the stress
The purpose of the present study was to develop a new construct to extend
previous work/family conflict research both conceptually and empirically. However, one
remaining question is whether the work/life balance scale developed in the present study
regard to content validity, the work/life balance scale items were more inclusive of the
Specifically, the items in the work/life balance scale included the term “personal life” so
that respondents could interpret the item in the context of what was salient to their own
respective personal lives. This more general approach to item wording is desirable for
164
being able to assess more of the non-work content domain than just family, as family may
(e.g., work-to-family conflict and family-to-work conflict; e.g., Netemeyer et al., 1996),
the scale developed as part of the present study also measured positive spillover (i.e.,
enhancement) between work and personal life. Findings in the present study indicated
that the enhancement dimension of work/life balance accounted for additional variance in
some of the criterion variables after accounting for interference, and therefore it is
important to consider the positive effects of one domain on the other. Findings from this
study indicated that both positive and negative work and personal life issues are relevant
to conceptualizing “balance.” In other words, balance does not simply refer to having a
lack of interference, but also includes the extent to which one’s personal life benefits
from work and vice-versa. Reliance on measures that only assess conflict or interference
work/life balance was highly correlated with work-to-family conflict (i.e., r’s above .80
in both samples). Not surprisingly, then, was the pattern of relationships with other
variables similar for WIPL and WFC. However, the PLIW scale (personal life
interference with work) was superior to the family-to-work conflict measure in terms of
its ability to predict other variables. For example, PLIW was at least moderately related
satisfaction, organizational commitment, and turnover intentions, whereas FWC was not.
165
Furthermore, PLIW was more strongly related to overall stress and life satisfaction than
FWC. These results suggest that there may be more to one's personal life than just
family that can interfere or conflict with work, and the FWC measure was criterion
deficient in comparison to the 4-item PLIW scale. Taken together, the more broad item
content, inclusion of positive as well as negative aspects of work and personal life, and
superior predictive validity of the work/life balance scale suggest that the new scale is
Practical Implications
programs without regard to explaining what is meant by the term, “work/life balance.”
(Hill et al., 1998) The construct definition developed as part of this study can assist
organizations in articulating what they mean when marketing their ability to help
Secondly, the scale developed as part of the present study holds promise as a
useful tool for organizations interested in assessing work/life balance perceptions among
employees. To date, no other measure of work/life balance has been developed and
validated. The more inclusive wording of personal life compared to family offers
organizations the opportunity to measure the interface between work and personal life
among employees without regard to employees' marital status and family size. This scale
could also be used to evaluate the effectiveness of work/life balance programs. The
interference could be effective for reducing strains, this study provides initial evidence to
suggest that programs designed to enhance employees’ personal lives via work will be
particularly useful for improving employee attitudes and retaining valued employees.
To increase the practical utility of the work/life balance scale, the work
interference with personal life (WIPL) scale could be reduced in length while retaining its
validity. Reducing the scale could ease the survey administration process and may have a
Limitations
One limitation of the present study was the use of a single sample design using a
holdout sample for cross-validation of the scale. This single sample methodology has
received some criticism (e.g., Murphy, 1983). Although the use of a holdout sample
assists a researcher with adjusting for random sampling error, this approach does not
results specific to a single sample would likely be evident in both the initial and holdout
sample. Therefore, future research should be conducted to continue the scale validation
Although the sample used in the present study was heterogeneous, (i.e.,
geographical location), all participants were managers, few were under the age of 40,
most were married with children, and most had been with the same organization for at
or those with less experience with a particular organization. Samples obtained for future
All data collected from managers in this study relied on their self-reports of the
variables assessed in this study. The use of self-report questionnaires has been a source
Howard, 1994), and has been criticized for resulting in artificially inflated correlations
among measures of psychological constructs. Although method bias due to the use of a
cross-sectional, self-report research design may have artificially inflated the magnitude of
the observed correlations, this effect would not alter the direction of the observed
The constructs included in the present study were most appropriately measured by
asking employees to report their own attitudes and perceptions (Schmitt, 1994). For
to the cognitive appraisal definition of stress (Lazarus, 1966; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984),
perceptions of stress are in the eye of the beholder. Therefore, the use of self-report
questionnaires was the most appropriate methodology. The same rationale was used for
using self-report methodology to gather information about other stressors (e.g., time
Because this study was exploratory and early in the development of work/life
balance research, this methodology has been suggested as appropriate for gathering
information to assess initial construct validity (e.g., Schmitt, 1994). Future research to
gather further validity evidence should incorporate other methods of measurement (e.g.,
168
rule out the explanation that cross-sectional, self-report research methodology accounted
balance. Item and scale analyses based on both classical test theory and item response
theory across two samples of data demonstrated initial evidence for appropriate scale
psychometric properties. In addition, data provided initial evidence for the convergent,
resources of time and energy, lack of goal accomplishment, and strain between work and
personal life role demands. This study identified three specific dimensions of work/life
balance: work interference with personal life, personal life interference with work, and
dissatisfaction, lack of organizational commitment, and intent to quit one’s job. Results
indicated support for measuring positive as well as negative aspects of the work/personal
Clark and Watson (1995) commented that the work of validating a scale is never
complete. Given the early stage of development for the work/life balance construct,
much additional research is warranted to improve the work/life balance scale and
169
continue understanding its role in the stress process. It is the author’s hope that continued
research on work/personal life balance will lead to efforts for improving the quality of
REFERENCES
72-77.
Adams, G. A., King, L. A., & King, D. W. (1996). Relationships of job and
family involvement, family social support, and work-family conflict with job and life
Allen, T. D., Herst, D. E. L., Bruck, C. S., & Sutton, M. (2000). Consequences
associated with work-to-family conflict: A review and agenda for future research.
Balzer, W. K., Kihm, J. A., Smith, P. C., Irwin, J. L., Bachiochi, P. D., Robie, C.,
Sinar, E. F., & Parra, L. F. (1997). Users' manual for the Job Descriptive Index (JDI;
1997 Revision) and the Job In General scales. Bowling Green, OH: Bowling Green State
University.
Barnett, R. & Rivers (1996). She works, he works: How two-income families are
conflict among married male and female professionals. Journal of Management, 14,
475-491.
171
Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New York: John
Wiley.
Carlson, D. S., Kacmar. K. M.. & Williams, L. J. (2000). Construction and initial
Casner-Lotto, J. & Hickey (1999). Holding a job, having a life - Making them
Cattell, R. B. (1978). The scientific use of factor analysis in behavioral and life
Chen, P. Y., & Spector, P. E. (1991). Negative affectivity as the underlying cause
of correlations between stressors and strains. Journal of Applied Psychology. 76, 398-
407.
Cranny, C. J., Smith, P. C., & Stone, E. F. (1992). Job satisfaction: How people
feel about their jobs and how it affects their performance. New York: Lexington Books.
Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction
Eagle, B. W., Miles, E. W., & Icenogle, M. L. (1997). Interrole conflict and the
permeability of work and family domains: Are there gender differences? Journal of
working time around the world. In L. Golden & D. M. Figart (Eds), Working time:
Fisher, G. G. & Hemingway, M. A. (2000). Is there more to life than work and
family? Symposium presented at the 15th Annual Conference for the Society of Industrial
Fisher, G. G., Stanton, J. M., & Grauer, E., & Smith, C. S. (2001). Further
Manuscript in progress.
Fisher, G. G., Stanton, J. M., Thoresen, P., Julian, A. L., Sinar, E. F., Aziz, S„
Balzer, W. K.., & Smith, P. C. (2000). The stress-in-general scale: Exploration and
174
validation. Poster presented at the 15th Annual Conference for the Society of Industrial
Ford, J. K., MacCallum, R. C., & Tait, M. (1986). The application of exploratory
industrial environment and mental health. The Journal of Social Issues, 18, 1-47.
Frone, M. R., Russell, M., & Cooper, M. L. (1992). Antecedents and outcomes of
Frone, M. R., Yardley, J. K.. & Market K. S. (1997). Developing and testing an
integrative model of the work-family interface. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 50, 145-
167.
Ganster, D. C. & Schaubroeck. J. (1991). Work and stress and employee health.
applied to work-family conflict and strain. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 54. 350-370.
Management, 8, 37-60.
Gupta, N., & Beehr, T. A. (1979). Job stress and employee behavior.
Gutek, B. A., Searle. S., & Klepa, L. (1991). Rational versus gender role
Hammer, L. B., Allen, E., & Grigsby, T. D. (1997). Work-family conflict in dual
earner couples: Within-individual and crossover effects of work and family. Journal of
personality, work and nonwork experiences, and domain satisfactions. Journal of Applied
Hill, E. J., Miller, B.C., Weiner, S.P., & Colihan, J. (1998). Influences of the
virtual office on aspects of work and work/life balance. Personnel Psychology, 51,
667-683.
Howard, G. S. (1994). Why do people say nasty things about self reports?
Stone (Eds.), Job satisfaction: How people feel about their jobs and how it affects their
Ironson, G. H., Smith, P. C., Brannick, M. T., Gibson, W. M., & Paul, K. B.
research on role ambiguity and role conflict in work settings. Organizational Behavior
personnel and human resources management. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press, Inc.
177
Kahn, R. L., Wolfe, D. M., Quinn, R., Snoek, J. D., & Rosenthal, R.A. (1964).
Katz & Kahn, R. L. (1978). The social psychology of organizations (2nd ed.).
King, L. A., & King, D. W. (1990). Role conflict and role ambiguity: A critical
family, and interrole conflict: A construct validation study. Organizational Behavior and
Kossek, E. & Ozeki, C. (1998). Work-family conflict, policies, and the job-life
Lazarus, R. S. (1966). Psychological stress and the coping process. New York:
McGraw Hill.
Lazarus R., S. & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York:
Springer.
Lerner, J. V. (1994). Working women and their families. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Macan, T., Shahani, C., Dipboye, R. L., & Phillips, A. P. (1990). College
students’ time management: Correlations with academic performance and stress. Journal
MacDermid, S. M., Barnett, R., Crosby, F., Greenhaus, J., Koblenz, M.. Marks,
S., Perry-Jenkins, M.. Voydanoff, P., Wethington, E., & Sabbatini-Bunch, L. The
Marsh, H. W., Balia, J. R., & McDonald, R. P. (1988). Goodness of fit indices in
confirmatory factor analysis: The effect of sample size. Psychological Bulletin. 103,
391-411.
607-618.
Mathieu, J. E., & Farr, J. L. (1991). Further evidence foe the discriminant
practices for evaluating causal models in organizational behavior and human resources
Merton, R. K. (1957). Social theory' and social structure. Glencoe, IL: Free Press
of Glencoe.
Messmer, M. (1999). Attracting quality job candidates. Business Credit, 101, 55-
57.
the precursors of hospital employee turnover. Journal of Applied Psychology, 63, 408-
414.
Murphy, L. R., Hurrell, J. J. Jr., Sauter, S. L., & Keita, G. P. (Eds.) (1995). Job
Psychology, 81,400-410.
974.
Parcel, T. L. (1999). Work and family in the 21st century. Work and
Unpublished manuscript.
Parsons, T. (1951). The social system. Glencoe, IL: Free Press of Glencoe.
Pavot, W., & Diener, E. (1993). Review of the satisfaction with life scale.
Pelletier & Lutz (1989). Mindbody goes to work: A critical review of stress
E. (Ed.), Health at work, (pp. 189-204). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Quinn, R.P. & Staines, G. L. (1979) The 1977 Quality of Employment Survey.
Rothausen, T., Gonzalez, G. A., Clarke, N. E., & O'Dell, L. A. (1998). Family-
friendly backlash - Fact or fiction? The case of organizations' on-site childcare centers.
the public sector: “Hardiness” and pressure for change. Journal of Vocational Behavior.
46, 17-39.
Stress Index. In C. J. Cranny, P. C. Smith, & E. F. Stone (Eds.), Job satisfaction: How
people feel about their jobs and how it affects their performance (pp. 241-254). New
Sandman, B.A., & Smith, P. C. (1988). The job stress index. Unpublished
manuscript.
Sauter, S. L., & Murphy, L. R. (1995). Organizational risk factors for job stress.
9-48.
Seashore, S. E., Lawler, E. E., Mirvis, P., & Cammann, C. (1982). Observing
and measuring organizational change: A guide to field practice. New York: Wiley.
Shellenbarger, S. (January 29, 1997). New job hunters ask recruiters, “Is there a
spillover into family life. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 52, 51-61.
Smith, C. S., & Sulsky, L. M. (In press). Work and stress. Pacific Grove, CA:
Wadsworth.
385-392.
on the relationship between work and non-work. Human Relations, 33, 111-129.
183
Stanton, J. M., Balzer, W. K., Smith, P. C., Parra, L. F„ & Ironson, G. (In press).
A global measure of work stress: The stress in general (SIG) scale. Educational and
Psychological Measurement.
Stanton, J. M., Sinar, E. F., Balzer, W. K., Julian, A. L., Thoresen, P., Aziz, S.,
Fisher, G. G., & Smith, P. C. (In press). Development of a compact measure of job
Measurement.
Sweet, J. A. (1989). Changes in the life cycle composition of the United States
population and the demand for housing. CDE Working Paper, No. 89-14. Madison,
scoring using item response theory (Version 6). Chicago: Scientific Software.
Ventura, S. J., Martin, J. A., Curtin, S. C., & Mathews, T. J. (1998). Monthly
Vital Statistics Report, Vol. 46, No. 11, supp. Hyattsville, MD: National Center for
Health Statistics.
Viswesvaran, C., Sanchez, J. I., & Fisher, J. (1999). The role of social support in
the process of work stress: A meta-analysis. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 54, 314-
334.
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of
brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS Scales. Journal of
concerns. In S. Zedeck (Ed.), Work, families, and organizations. San Francisco: Jossey-
Bass.
Zickar, M.J. (In press). Modeling data with polytomous item response theory. In
APPENDIX A
5. What constitutes a “balance” for you between work and aspects of your life aside
from your primary job?
6. Do you feel that you currently have a balance? If so, how do you know? If not, what
indicates to you that you don’t have a balance?
7. Do you perceive that striking a balance between work and life outside of work is an
important goal to obtain? Why/ why not?
8. What (in particular) do you do to balance your work and life outside of work?
9. Is it easier to balance some roles than others (including work)? How is it easier/more
difficult? Why do you think this is?
10. What effect(s) does NOT having this balance have on your work? On your life
outside of work? Please explain.
11. What aspects of your job help/hinder you in achieving a balance between work and
life outside of work? Please explain.
12. Is the company you work for mindful of your having a life outside of work? If yes, in
what way?
13. Is your supervisor supportive of your life outside of work? Please explain.
14. Do you have any other comments that come to mind now that you have given some
thought to this issue of work/life balance and the roles that you have in your work and
life outside of work? What other issues seem relevant that weren’t asked about?
186
APPENDIX B
1. There is not enough time in the day to do everything I need to do at work and
at home.
2. I struggle with trying to juggle both my work and non-work responsibilities.
3. I spend more time at work than I would like.
4. My personal life drains me of the energy I need to do my job.
5. I fee, overwhelmed when I try to balance my work and personal life.
6. My work suffers because of everything going on in my personal life.
7. I am too tired to be effective at work because of things I have going on in my
personal life.
8. My job gives me energy to pursue activities outside of work that are important
to me.
9. I have difficulty scheduling vacation time because of my workload.
10. I am unable to relax at home because I am preoccupied with my work.
11.1 am happy with the amount of time I spend doing activities not related to
work.
12. Because of my job, I am in a better mood at home.
13.1 often have to make difficult choices between my work and my personal life.
14.1 would spend more time at work if it were not for my non-work
responsibilities.
15. My career interferes with my ability to have a satisfying personal life.
16. I have to put aspects of my personal life “on hold” because of my work.
17. Due to all the pressures at work, I am too drained to do other activities I enjoy.
18.1 am able to accomplish what I would like in both my personal and work lives.
19.1 am in a better mood at work because of everything I have going for me in
my personal life.
20.1 often neglect my personal needs because of the demands of my work.
21. When I am at work, I worry about things I need to do outside of work.
22. 1 have difficulty getting my work done because I am preoccupied with
personal matters at work.
23. My personal life suffers because of my work.
24.1 have to miss out on important personal activities due to the amount of time I
spend doing work.
25.1 am satisfied with my ability to get things done both at home and at work.
26. My personal life gives me the energy to do my job.
27.1 feel that I allocate appropriate amounts of time to both work and non-work
activities.
28.1 make personal sacrifices to get work done.
29.1 come home from work too tired to do things I would like to do.
30. My job makes it difficult to maintain the kind of personal life I would like.
31. My work allows me enough time to do the things that are important to me.
187
APPENDIX C
American Management
Association
February 2001
Dear Friend:
By completing and returning the enclosed questionnaire, you’ll realize these benefits:
Your confidentiality is assured; neither you nor your organization will be identified to any outside
party without your advance, written consent.
Won't you take the few minutes necessary to complete this questionnaire and return it to us in the
enclosed postage-paid envelope? We thank you very much for your consideration, and we look
forward to your participation. Please feel free to contact me at [email protected] if you
have any questions or concerns.
Sincerely,
APPENDIX D
For each of the following statements, please indicate HOW FREQUENTLY YOU HAVE
FELT THIS WAY DURING THE PAST 3 MONTHS using the scale below. If the
statement is not applicable to you. please fill in “N/A” (Not Applicable) as your response
to that question. Please note that for the following group of statements:
APPENDIX E
Please indicate the extent to which you agree or disagree with the following statements:
Work-to-Family Conflict
StrongUy Strongly
Disagi ee Disagree Neutral Agree Agree
Family-to-Work Conflict
My home life interferes with my 1 2 3 4 5 N/A
responsibilities at work, such as getting to
work on time, accomplishing daily tasks,
and working overtime.
APPENDIX F
Think about your present iob. How well does each of the following, words or phrases
describe it? Please check only ONE answer for each:
Y (for “Yes”)
N (for “No”)
? (for “Cannot decide)
Yes No ?
Demanding Y N ?
Pressured Y N ?
Hectic Y N ?
Calm Y N ?
Relaxed Y N ?
Many things stressful Y N ?
Pushed Y N ?
Irritating Y N ?
Under control Y N ?
Nerve-wracking Y N ?
Hassled Y N ?
Comfortable Y N ?
More stressful than I'd li Y N ?
Smooth running Y N ?
Overwhelming Y N ?
192
APPENDIX G
Think about your present job. How well does each of the following words or phrases
describe it? Please check only ONE answer for each:
Y (for “Yes”)
N (for “No”)
? (for “Cannot decide)
Yes No ?
Pleasant Y N ?
Bad Y N ?
Ideal Y N ?
Waste of time Y N ?
Good Y N ?
Undesirable Y N ?
Worthwhile Y N ?
Worse than most Y N ?
Acceptable Y N ?
Superior Y N ?
Better than most Y N ?
Disagreeable Y N ?
Makes me content Y N ?
Inadequate Y N ?
Excellent Y N ?
Rotten Y N ?
Enjoyable Y N ?
Poor Y N ?
193
APPENDIX H
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree
APPENDIX I
Please indicate the extent to which you agree or disagree with the following statements
about the ORGAN1ZATION/COMPANYfor which you work:
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree
1 2 3 4 5 N/A
I really care about the fate of my
organization.
1 2 3 4 5 N/A
I am proud to tell others what
organization I work for.
1 2 3 4 5 N/A
For me, my organization is the best of all
possible organizations for which to work.
1 2 3 4 5 N/A
If I were offered a job with similar pay
and benefits with another organization, I
would take it.
1 2 3 4 5 N/A
I am willing to put in a great deal of effort
beyond that normally expected in order to
help my organization be successful.
1 2 3 4 5 N/A
me in the way of job performance.
195
APPENDIX J
Demographic Questions
1. On average, how many hours per week do you work on your job IN TOTAL
(including at work and at home)? ______hours per week
2. On average, how many hours per week do you work on your job AT HOME?
_____ hours per week
4. Please indicate the number of children who live with you in your household at least
50% of the time: _____
5. Do you currently have primary responsibility for caring for one or more of your
parents/in-laws/other relatives? _____Yes _____ No
7. Your age:
_____ 18 to 24 40 to 44 60 to 64
_____ 25 to 29 45 to 49 65 or older
_____ 30 to 34 50 to54
_____ 35 to 39 55 to59
14. Please indicate the management level of our current position, in terms of its reporting
relationship to the Chief Executive Officer (CEO) of your organization. If you serve
as CEO, mark Level 1; if you report directly to the CEO, mark Level 2; if you are
further removed from the CEO, please mark the number indicating your management
level, where the CEO equals Level 1.