Module 3
Module 3
Module 3
References: Tomlinson, Brian (2011). Material development in Language Teaching (2nd Ed.).
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Objectives: At the end of the module the students are expected to:
TOPIC:
Materials should take into account that the positive effects of instruction are usually delayed
Research into the acquisition of language shows that it is a gradual rather than an instantaneous
process and that this is equally true for instructed as well as informal acquisition. Acquisition results
from the gradual and dynamic process of internal generalization rather than from instant adjustments to
the learner’s internal grammar. It follows that learners cannot be expected to learn a new feature and
be able to use it effectively in the same lesson. They might be able to rehearse the feature, to retrieve it
from short-term memory or to produce it when prompted by the teacher or the materials. But this does
not mean that learning has already taken place. I am sure most of you are familiar with the situation in
which learners get a new feature correct in the lesson in which it is taught but then get it wrong the
following week. This is partly because they have not yet had enough time, instruction and exposure for
The inevitable delayed effect of instruction suggests that no textbook can really succeed if it
teaches features of the language one at a time and expects the learners to be able to use them
straightaway. But this incremental approach is popular with many publishers, writers, teachers and
learners as it can provide a reassuring illusion of system, simplicity and progress. Therefore, adaptation
of existing approaches rather than replacement with radical new ones is the strategy most likely to
succeed. So, for example, the conventional textbook approach of PPP (Presentation-Practice-Production)
could be used to promote durable learning if the objective of the Production phase was seen as
reinforcement rather than correct production and if this was followed in subsequent units by more
exposure and more presentation relating to the same feature. Or the Production phase could be
postponed to another unit which is placed after further exposure, instruction and practice have been
provided. Or the initial Production phase could be used to provide output which would enable the
learners to notice the mismatch between what they are doing and what proficient speakers typically do.
In my view, in order to facilitate the gradual process of acquisition, it is important for materials to
recycle instruction and to provide frequent and ample exposure to the instructed language features in
communicative use. This is particularly true of vocabulary acquisition, which requires frequent, spaced
and varied recycling in order to be successful (Nation 2003, 2005; Nation and Wang 1999).
It is equally important that the learners are not forced into premature production of the
instructed features (they will get them wrong) and that tests of proficiency are not conducted
to tertiary level.) Usahay makabungol sigi paminaw ug what is grammar? What is noun? Apan some
students really need to be reminded. As language teacher you should have a patience of a mother. Sa
una grade six ko nabungol ko ug paminaw anang prefix ug suffix.. makalagot gd si maam sigi balik balik,,
hhehhe nigawas si Ladylou ug nanikop ug isda sa suba.. nakasab an lagi kay gabasa basa. When I became
a language teacher hahahhahaha I realized nga akong ubang classmates kingahanglan diay sila ato.. ug
kay kontrabida man au ko kay nakasabot na lagi,, lagot ko sa among maestra that time,,jjajajajja I enjoy
recalling that moment..Hangtod karon maglagot gd ko mutan aw sa among maestra jajjajajajjaja bitaw
gipasaylo na nako siya ,,, kapoya gd,, hahhahahahahah nabunalan lagi ko. That’s the beginning of
wisdom.)
Materials should take into account that learners differ in learning styles Different learners have
different preferred learning styles. (Some students preferred to sing, some like to act it out. We have
multiple ways to learn, that’s why we have to vary our activities and approaches.) So, for example,
those learners with a preference for studied learning are much more likely to gain from explicit grammar
teaching than those who prefer experiential learning. And those who prefer experiential learning are
more likely to gain from reading a story with a predominant grammatical feature (e.g. reported speech)
This means that activities should be variable and should ideally cater for all learning styles. An
analysis of most current coursebooks will reveal a tendency to favor learners with a preference for
stadial learning and an apparent assumption that all learners are equally capable of benefiting from this
style of learning Likewise an analysis of the teaching and testing of foreign languages in formal
education systems throughout the world will reveal that stadial learners (who are actually in the
Styles of learning which need to be catered for in language-learning materials (Multiple intelligences by
include:
features of the language and want to be correct); • experiential (e.g. learners like to use the language
• analytic (e.g. learners prefer to focus on discrete bits of the language and
• dependent (e.g. learners prefer to learn from a teacher and from a book);
• independent (e.g. learners are happy to learn from their own experience
of the language and to use autonomous learning strategies).
A learner’s preference for a particular learning style is variable and depends, for example, on
what is being learned, where it is being learned, whom it is being learned with and what it is being
learned for, For example, I am happy to be experiential, global and kinesthetic when learning Japanese
out of interest with a group of relaxed adult learners and with a teacher who does not keep correcting
me. But I am more likely to be analytic and visual when learning French for examination purposes in a
class of competitive students and with a teacher who keeps on correcting me. And, of course, learners
can be helped to gain from learning styles other than their preferred style. The important point for
materials developers is that they are aware of and cater for differences of preferred learning styles in
their materials and that they do not assume that all learners can benefit from the same approaches as
Materials should take into account that learners differ in affective attitudes
The learner’s motives, emotions, and attitudes screen what is presented in the language
classroom... This affective screening is highly individual and results in different rates and results. (Dulay,
Ideally language learners should have strong and consistent motivation and they should also
have positive feelings towards the target language, their teachers, their fellow learners and the
materials they are using. But, of course, ideal learners do not exist and even if they did exist one day,
they would no longer be ideal learners the next day. Each class of learners using the same materials will
differ from each other in terms of ling- and short-term motivation and of feelings and attitudes about
the language, their teachers, their fellow learners and their learning materials, and of attitudes towards
the language, the teacher and the materials. Obviously, no materials developer can cater for all these
affective variables, but it is important for anybody who is writing learning materials to be aware of the
One obvious implication for the materials developer is ‘to diversify language instruction as much
as possible based upon the variety of cognitive styles’ (Larsen-Freeman and Long 1991) and the variety
of affective attitudes likely to be found amongst typical class of learners. Ways of doing this include:
• providing optional extras for the more positive and motivated learners:
• providing variety;
discussion;
discussion;
• researching and catering for the diverse interests of the identified target
learners;
• giving general and specific advice in the teacher’s book on how to respond to negative learners (e.g.
It has been shown that it can be extremely valuable to delay L2 speaking for beginners of a
language until they have gained sufficient confidence in understanding it. This silent period can facilitate
the development of an effective internalized grammar which can help learners to achieve proficiency
when they eventually start to speak in the L2. There is some controversy about the actual value of the
silent period and some learners seem to use the silence to avoid learning the language.
However, I think most researchers would agree that forcing immediate production in the new
language can damage the reluctant speaker affectively and linguistically and many would agree with
Communication situations in which students are permitted to remain silent or respond in their
first language may be the most effective approach for the early phases of language instruction.
This approach approximates what language learners of all ages have been observed to do
naturally, and it appears to be more effective than forcing full two-way communication from the very
language and they should not force silence either. Ways of giving learners the possibility of not speaking
• starting the course with a Total Physical Response (TPR) approach in which the learners respond
through the use of sound effects, visual aids and dramatic movement by
the teacher;
A possible extension of the principle of permitting silence is to introduce most new language
points (regardless of the learners’ level) through activities which initially require comprehension but not
production. This is an approach which I call TPR Plus and which we used on the PKG Project in
Indonesian secondary schools. It usually involved introducing new vocabulary or structures through
stories which the learners responded to by drawing and/or using their first language, and through
activities in which the whole class mimed stories by following oral instructions from the teacher (see
aesthetic and emotional involvement which stimulates both right- and left-brain activities.
A narrowly focused series of activities which require very little cognitive processing (e.g.
mechanical drills; rule learning; simple transformation activities) usually leads to shallow and ephemeral
learning unless linked to other activities which stimulate mental and affective processing, However, a
varied series of activities making, for example, analytic, creative, evaluative and rehearsal demands on
In order for this deeper learning to be facilitated, it is very important that the content of the
materials is not trivial or banal and that it stimulates thoughts and feelings in the learners. It is also
important that the activities are not too simple and that they cannot be too easily achieved without the
Suggestopedia, in which he ‘enables the learner to receive the information through different cerebral
processes and in different states of consciousness so that it is stored in many different parts of the brain,
maximizing recall’ (Hooper Hansen 1992). Suggestopedia does this through engaging the learners in a
variety of left- and right-brain activities in the same lesson (e.g. reciting a dialogue, dancing to
instructions, singing a song, doing a substitution drill, writing a story). Whilst not everybody would
accept the procedures Suggestopedia, most researchers seem to agree on the value of maximizing the
brain’s capacity during language learning and the best textbooks already do contain within each unit a
variety of different left- and rightbrain activities. Materials should not rely too much on controlled
practice
It is interesting that there seems to be very little research which indicates that controlled
practice activities are valuable. Sharwood-Smith (1981) does say that ‘it is clear and uncontroversial to
say that most spontaneous performance is attained by dint of practice’, but he provides no evidence to
support this very strong claim. Also, Bialystok (1988) says that automaticity is achieved through practice
but provides no evidence to support her claim. In the absence of any compelling evidence most
researchers seem to agree with Ellis, who says that ‘controlled practice appears to have little long term
effect on the accuracy with which new structures are performed’ (Ellis 1990:192) and ‘has little effect on
Yet controlled grammar practice activities still feature significantly in popular coursebooks and
are considered to be useful by many teachers and by many learners. This is especially true of dialogue
practice, which has been popular in many methodologies for the last 30 years without there being any
substantial research evidence to support it (see Tomlinson 1995). In a recent analysis of new low-level
coursebooks I found that nine out of ten of them contained many more opportunities for controlled
practice than they did for language use. It is possible that right now all over the world learners are
wasting their time doing drills and listening to and repeating dialogues.
accuracy of the output can lead to output becoming a profitable source of input. Or in other words, if
the language that the learner produces is evaluated in relation to the purpose for which it is used, that
language can become a powerful and informative source of information about language use.
Thus, a learner who fails to achieve a particular communicative purpose (e.g. borrowing
something, instructing someone how to play a game, persuading someone to do something) is more
likely to gain from feedback on the effectiveness of their use of language than a learner whose language
is corrected without reference to any non-linguistic outcome. It is very important, therefore, for
materials developers to make sure that language production activities have intended outcomes other
than just practicing language. The value of outcome feedback is focused on by such writers on task
ACTIVITIES:
Act.2 (for English majors) Cite one language feature that you think
should be taught thoroughly ( INCLUDING FREQUENT AND AMPLE EXPOSURE)
and students be exposed through communicative use. Discuss. (you may refer
to your own experience.
Act. 2 ( for BPED) Actions, gestures and the like are body language
(includes dancing ,acting etc. ).
1. What do you think should be the first to learn in exercising so it will run
smoothly?
4. Is instruction (esp. the language used) a must in any given physical activity?
Why or why not?