0% found this document useful (0 votes)
10 views

Nihms 1598354

The study aims to better understand the pathophysiology and causes of tic disorders by prospectively studying children with provisional tic disorder over time. Preliminary results found that 90% of children still had tics after 12 months, meeting criteria for a chronic tic disorder. Baseline factors like tic severity, autism symptoms, anxiety, and multiple vocal tics predicted worse tic outcomes. Poorer ability to suppress tics also predicted greater tic severity later. Larger hippocampal volume at baseline was linked to worse tic severity, while striatal volumes did not predict outcomes as hypothesized.

Uploaded by

Alexandra Ale
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
10 views

Nihms 1598354

The study aims to better understand the pathophysiology and causes of tic disorders by prospectively studying children with provisional tic disorder over time. Preliminary results found that 90% of children still had tics after 12 months, meeting criteria for a chronic tic disorder. Baseline factors like tic severity, autism symptoms, anxiety, and multiple vocal tics predicted worse tic outcomes. Poorer ability to suppress tics also predicted greater tic severity later. Larger hippocampal volume at baseline was linked to worse tic severity, while striatal volumes did not predict outcomes as hypothesized.

Uploaded by

Alexandra Ale
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 31

HHS Public Access

Author manuscript
J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Author Manuscript

Published in final edited form as:


J Psychiatr Brain Sci. 2020 ; 5: . doi:10.20900/jpbs.20200012.

The New Tics study: A Novel Approach to Pathophysiology and


Cause of Tic Disorders
Kevin J. Black1,*, Soyoung Kim2, Bradley L. Schlaggar3, Deanna J. Greene2
1Departments of Psychiatry, Neurology, Radiology and Neuroscience, Washington University in
St. Louis School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO 63110, USA
2Departments of Psychiatry and Radiology, Washington University in St. Louis School of
Author Manuscript

Medicine, St. Louis, MO 63110, USA


3Kennedy Krieger Institute, Baltimore, MD 21205; and Departments of Neurology and Pediatrics,
Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21287, USA

Abstract
We report on the ongoing project “The New Tics Study: A Novel Approach to Pathophysiology
and Cause of Tic Disorders,” describing the work completed to date, ongoing studies and long-
term goals. The overall goals of this research are to study the pathophysiology of Provisional Tic
Disorder, and to study tic remission (or improvement) in a prospective fashion. Preliminary data
collection for the project began almost 10 years ago. The current study is nearing completion of its
third year, and has already reported several novel and important results. First, surprisingly, at least
Author Manuscript

90% of children who had experienced tics for only a mean of 3 months still had tics at the 12-
month anniversary of their first tic, though in some cases tics were seen only with remote video
observation of the child sitting alone. Thus almost all of them now had a DSM-5 diagnosis of
Tourette’s Disorder or Persistent (Chronic) Tic Disorder. Baseline clinical features that predicted
12-month outcome included tic severity, subsyndromal autism spectrum symptoms, an anxiety
disorder, and a history of 3 or more phonic tics. Second, we found that poorer tic suppression
ability when immediately rewarded for suppression predicted greater tic severity at follow-up.
Third, striatal volumes did not predict outcome as hypothesized, but a larger hippocampus at
baseline predicted worse severity at follow-up. Enrollment and data collection continue, including
functional connectivity MRI (fcMRI) imaging, and additional analyses are planned once the full
sample is enrolled.
Author Manuscript

Licensee Hapres, London, United Kingdom. This is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of Creative
Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
*
Correspondence: Kevin J. Black, [email protected]; Tel.: +1-314-362-5041.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
KJB and BLS designed the study. KJB, SK, and DJG performed experiments and analyzed data. KJB wrote the paper with input from
all authors.
DATA AVAILABILITY
The data from before the start of the current funding are deposited at nih.figshare.com (project 68282, currently embargoed), and the
data from the R01 are deposited in the NIMH Data Archive (ID C2692).
Black et al. Page 2

Keywords
Author Manuscript

Provisional Tic Disorder; tic disorders; Tourette syndrome; prognosis; anxiety; tic suppression;
inhibition (psychology); hippocampus; MRI

INTRODUCTION
Background
Tics are brief movements or noises, repeated many times a day that may look intentional but
serve no useful purpose [1]. Common tics include forceful blinking, raising the eyebrows,
turning the head, shrugging, sniffing, or grunting. Tics in a few patients are attributable to
another disease, such as stroke or Huntington disease, but usually no such secondary cause is
found [2]. Tics almost always begin in childhood, most often ages 3 to 10 years, and on
Author Manuscript

average, tics are most severe around ages 9 to 11 [3]. Chronic tic disorders are defined by
the persistence of tics for at least 12 months (meaning tics collectively, because individual
tics come and go over time). If both movements and vocalizations have been present,
childhood-onset idiopathic chronic tics are diagnosed as Tourette’s Disorder (Tourette
syndrome, TS), whereas if all tics are silent the diagnosis is Persistent (Chronic) Motor Tic
Disorder (CTD) [4,5]. The terms and definitions have changed somewhat over time, and for
children who have had tics for less than a year, the nosology has been confusing. For
instance, DSM-IV, DSM-IV-TR, and DSM-5 define quite different, though overlapping,
groups of children with tics for less than a year (see Appendix 1 in ref. [6]). The current
nosology defines Provisional Tic Disorder (PTD) by the presence of tics not better explained
by another brain or systemic illness, with no tic prior to 1 year ago.
Author Manuscript

Despite increasingly intensive efforts, our understanding of the etiology and


pathophysiology of TS/CTD remains fragmentary [7–9]. Furthermore, studies that find an
association of TS with a particular biological marker usually are unable to determine
whether the association is causal, and if so whether the marker is a consequence or cause of
the disease. Treatment is entirely symptomatic, and despite ongoing discovery efforts, many
patients remain frustrated with the limited efficacy of available treatments [10]. In short, in
reviewing this state of affairs, we concluded that a new approach to the study of tic disorders
might be needed. The new approach was suggested by epidemiological data.

A number of careful, independent epidemiologic studies have shown that chronic tic
disorders affect at least 2–3% of children, including TS in about 0.5–1% of all children 5–14
years old [11]. These rates may be underestimates: in one study, after parent, teacher and
child had reported no tics in 8 children, and an expert visiting the classroom had observed no
Author Manuscript

tics, tics were nevertheless observed during an office visit in 3 of the 8 [12]. In any case, it
clearly appears much more common for children to have tics for less than the 12 months
required for a diagnosis of TS/CTD. Several studies that used direct clinical ascertainment
reported tic prevalence as ~20% [13,14], a rate also found in groups of younger children
only [15]. One of the most careful studies to date used a defined population, stratified
sampling, assessment procedures with tested reliability, and personal examination by a
neurologist of all probable cases and of a number of screen-negatives [16]. This study found

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 3

a DSM-IV-TR tic disorder in 17% of mainstream school children ages 6–17. In another
Author Manuscript

study, during 8 visits over a single school year, trained raters directly observed a motor tic in
24% of all children in an elementary school, including 49% of all first-graders [17]. Our
recent review concluded that most children probably have a tic at some point in time [6]. So
what happens to most of these children with tics, if tics last for a year in only ~2–5% of
children? Clinical lore suggests that most children who just started ticcing will remit within
a few months [5,18–25]. However, lore is not data.

Remarkably, almost no research has been done on this “pre-Tourette” population, setting up
the scientific premise of the current research study. Prospective outcome data are sparse and
contradictory [6]. Shapiro et al reported outcomes based on selected clinical features [24] (p.
371–374 and Table 10 in Chapter 12). Bruun and Budman reported telephone follow-up on
58 children who had presented with tics lasting less than a year [26]. Carter and colleagues
[27] performed a 4-year high-risk follow-up study in first-degree relatives of TS probands.
Author Manuscript

Spencer et al prospectively monitored the appearance and disappearance of tic disorders over
the course of 4 years in boys with or without ADHD who did not have Tourette syndrome at
study entry [28]. Peterson et al. [15] followed for up to 15 years a large sample of children
with chronic or recent-onset tics, diagnosed initially by parental report only; the follow-ups
used direct examination for diagnosis. A few other studies report limited prospective data
[29–31]. However, all these studies report only clinical information at baseline. Several
superb studies did follow patients after tics were chronic [32–40]; although these follow-up
studies of chronic tics do not address why some tics fade before TS can be diagnosed, they
help identify reasonable hypotheses [6].

Research has understandably focused on those with chronic tics. But ironically, there are
important questions about TS that cannot be addressed in people already diagnosed with TS.
Author Manuscript

For instance, are there abnormalities of brain function when tics first appear? Why do tics
usually disappear? What is different about children whose tics persist? And perhaps of
greatest interest, could early intervention in those at highest risk prevent tics from becoming
chronic? The present study targets just these questions.

Another way of framing the problem is to realize that two steps occur in every child who
develops TS/CTD: first tics appear; then they fail to remit. Given the 10-fold difference in
accepted prevalence between PTD and TS/CTD, the second step may be more important.
Importantly, this second step can be observed prospectively, minimizing the biases inherent
in retrospective study designs. Equally important, studying persistence vs. remission may
identify causative factors missed by studies of established TS, in which onset and
persistence are confounded and often remote. Studying this earliest phase of tic disorders
Author Manuscript

may revolutionize etiology, prevention or treatment.

Studying pathophysiology of recent-onset tics has been considered laudable but


impracticable, as recent-onset tics may not be identified as such nor bring the child to
medical attention. For instance, in one study the estimated delay between tic onset and first
clinical contact was 10 years [41]. However, by intensive recruitment efforts, we have been
able to recruit at an adequate pace children who had tics for less than 6 months (more
recently allowing up to 9 months; hereinafter “New Tics”). We now have 12-month outcome

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 4

data on 63 children with New Tics. We have discovered fascinating clinical results, and are
Author Manuscript

beginning to identify biological predictors of outcome.

Impact
Chronic tic disorders have substantial public health significance [23], and the proposed
research may have substantial benefits for TS/CTD. The clearest benefits are that it may
identify entirely new directions for further etiologic and pathophysiological research, and it
can clarify the causality of associations we find.

Take new directions first. In addition to the specific hypotheses we are testing, we will
perform data-driven analyses to identify potential new findings. Newly identified brain
regions can later be studied with electrophysiological methods or in the limited available
number of TS autopsy specimens. The information this study can provide on causality is
also very important. Take the example of brain regions whose volume is smaller in adults
Author Manuscript

with TS compared to control adults. This finding may indicate areas whose development
was atypical, causing tics to develop. Alternatively, they could be compensatory changes to
help suppress tics [42,43]. Or, both volumetric changes and clinical symptoms could reflect
some other root cause. Cross-sectional studies generally cannot distinguish these three
possibilities. By contrast, the proposed prospective study, starting before tics become
chronic, can isolate the disappearance of tics. For example, brain regions in which abnormal
volume at baseline predicts improvement of tics have a much clearer causative link with tic
remission [36]. As another example, abnormalities in TS/CTD that are also present in the
New Tics group are unlikely to reflect chronicity or adaptation.

The New Tics population is also important in its own right. Children with recent-onset tics
are seen commonly in pediatrics or child neurology practices. “Most parents want
Author Manuscript

predictions of the future for their child,” wrote one clinician, adding, “This, however, is one
of the more challenging aspects of caring for the child with [tics]” [18]. Clinical lore is that
new-onset tic disorders usually remit, but published follow-up studies tell a different story:
the mean remission rate is only 32% (Table 1 in ref. [6]). In the largest such study, children
whose tics disappeared within the first year were followed up after 2–14 years, and only
17% had remained tic-free throughout [26]. Regardless of the true remission rate, some New
Tics children do go on to have years of clinically problematic tics. Since at present there is
almost no information to determine which of them will need long-term treatment, the
proposed study will be important for the clinical outcome data alone.

More importantly, this population offers the tantalizing possibility of prevention of TS (or, if
one prefers, secondary prevention of tic disorders). Now that large randomized, controlled
Author Manuscript

trials have demonstrated the efficacy of CBIT, a specific behavior therapy for tics with no
known side effects [44], it becomes entirely plausible to envision providing CBIT to those
children with New Tics who are at highest risk of TS/CTD. Conceivably such early
treatment could prevent progression to TS. The proposed work takes preliminary but
necessary steps toward that possibility.

We include the neurobiological measures to help us begin to understand the mechanisms of


tic disappearance. If we understood those mechanisms better—if we knew why tics improve

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 5

in many of these children—we could hope to design rational, mechanism-based treatments


Author Manuscript

for patients who do have persistent tics. Biomarkers that predict remission of New Tics may
conceivably speed medication discovery. They may even identify targets of treatment. For
instance, the association of ADHD at baseline with later tic persistence [15] may indicate
aggressive treatment of ADHD in new tic patients (or may not; as with other links we may
uncover to tic outcomes, this association may not indicate causation and will need to be
tested).

Hypotheses and Aims


Here we discuss the hypotheses and specific aims for the project currently funded by the
National Institutes of Health (NIH) as “The New Tics Study: A Novel Approach to
Pathophysiology and Cause of Tic Disorders” (grant R01MH104030). Briefly, we
hypothesized that specific features would differentiate the New Tics participants from tic-
Author Manuscript

free controls, and predict worse outcome at the one-year anniversary of tic onset (the
accepted minimum duration for diagnosis of TS/CTD).

Given the paucity of knowledge in this area and the substantial effort required to recruit
these children, we are collecting a rich set of data. However, to ensure rigor and
reproducibility we focused our analyses on a limited number of hypotheses that the available
evidence best supports.

The available literature on New Tics suggests a few hypotheses for our proposed large,
prospective study [6]. In a previous prospective study, tic disorders were much more
common in boys with ADHD (37% by study end) than in boys without ADHD (7%) [28].
Similarly, ADHD at baseline was associated with later tic persistence [15]. In a cross-
sectional prevalence study, children with tics for less than a year had lower severity, later age
Author Manuscript

of onset, and were less likely to have vocal tics than did children with TS or CTD [45]. Our
preliminary data suggest that children with New Tics who can suppress tics better to verbal
request may have better outcome at the 12-month anniversary of tic onset [46].

Although only indirectly relevant, many more studies have examined children after tics have
become chronic, and these studies suggest some relevant hypotheses about New Tics.

Children who were less able to suppress tics, with or without immediate reward, tended to
have more severe symptoms [47]. Omission errors on the Connors’ Continuous Performance
Test (CPTII) were negatively correlated (r = −0.63) with suppression ability [48]. About half
of TS adolescents exceeded the clinical cut-off point for hyperactivity and anxiety on the
Child Behavioral Checklist (CBCL) subscales even though only 22.5% met DSM-IV criteria
for ADHD and 7.5% met criteria for anxiety disorders [49]. Relatively impaired manual
Author Manuscript

dexterity (Purdue Pegboard test) was associated with outcome an average of 8 years after
diagnosis of TS [50]. Subjects with TS performed significantly worse on a “weather
prediction” probabilistic classification test, a form of habit learning [51].

Kurlan et al. noted that “there seems to be a strong relationship between the experience of
premonitory sensations and the ability to suppress tics” [52]. That relationship was recently
confirmed empirically [53]. In addition, premonitory urges are generally reported later in the

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 6

course of TS than are simple motor tics, and they are associated with thinner insula and
Author Manuscript

sensorimotor cortex [54]. More relevant, in TS, tics preceded by a premonitory urge tended
to remain worse after psychotherapy [55]. One can reasonably argue about the direction of
the association, but premonitory urges are likely to be important in predicting tic outcome.

When our grant proposal was submitted, reduced caudate volume was arguably the most
consistent structural neuroimaging finding in TS [9]. The likely etiological relevance of this
finding was shown by a study of 43 children with established TS, in which a smaller caudate
nucleus in childhood predicted more severe tics and other symptoms an average of 7.5 years
later [36]. A number of other structural changes have been implicated in TS, including
smaller ventral prefrontal cortex (vPFC), thinning of sensorimotor cortex, and increased gray
matter volume in thalamus and midbrain [56,57].

A handful of studies used [99mTc]HMPAO SPECT to measure blood flow in TS, as reviewed
Author Manuscript

elsewhere [9]. Two of the largest included 38 children with TS and 18 controls [58] and 50
children with TS and 20 controls [59]. Decreased left caudate blood flow was found in most
studies, including both of these. Decreased flow in prefrontal regions was also common.
Blood flow can also be measured with MRI using arterial spin labeling (ASL). The most
important potential advantage of ASL over BOLD (blood oxygen level-dependent fMRI) is
its signal stability over time: comparisons of brain activity between two conditions (such as
at screening versus at follow-up) can be performed most directly using perfusion imaging.
We are not aware of published studies applying ASL imaging to TS, but we have substantial
prior experience with ASL in other patient populations [60–63].

We previously published data from a resting state functional connectivity fMRI (rs-fcMRI)
experiment in 33 children with TS (DSM-IV-TR), age 10–16, and 42 controls [64,65], and
Author Manuscript

compared results to over 200 healthy subjects from age 7 through adulthood [66]. However,
more recently we and others have documented substantial artifactual effects of even small,
repeated head movements on fcMRI [67], and our site has been a leader in developing
analysis methods that avoid such artifacts to the limits of detection [68–70]. We recently
reported results of an rs-fcMRI study of TS in which we applied the best available methods
to minimize movement-related artifact [71]. We applied a multivariate machine learning
technique—support vector machine (SVM) classification—to test whether patterns in brain
network activity, measured with rs-fcMRI, differed between 42 children with TS (8–15
years) and 42 unaffected controls matched for age, IQ, and in-scanner head movement.
Univariate tests identified no significant group differences, but SVM could distinguish TS
from control brains with ~70% accuracy (p < 0.001). These results suggest that multivariate
methods can better capture the complexity of some brain disorders. The pattern of brain
Author Manuscript

activity that distinguished the TS and control groups was complex, but rs-fcMRI within and
between somatomotor and higher-level control networks (fronto-parietal, cingulo-opercular,
salience, dorsal attention, and ventral attention networks) produced classification reliability
closest to that of the complete data set. Other investigators have demonstrated improved
SVM classification accuracy by combining different data types [72,73], so we expect
improved accuracy by combining rs-fcMRI, structural MRI, and behavioral/clinical data in
the current study.

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 7

Bohlhalter et al. identified brain regions in which BOLD signal began to increase during the
Author Manuscript

2 seconds prior to a tic [74]. These regions included supplementary motor cortex (SMA),
known to be involved in movement generation, as well as anterior cingulate (ACC) and
insular regions that have been implicated in processing unpleasant sensations. Hampson et al
found that the SMA activation begins earlier and persists longer than SMA activation in
controls voluntarily performing movements similar to patients’ tics [75].

Building on previous work in adults [76], we described developmental changes in functional


connectivity between basal ganglia voxels and functionally defined rs-fcMRI cortical
systems [77]. Cortical-basal ganglia connections differed most significantly between
children and adults in the functional connectivity of the posterior putamen and pallidum with
the somatomotor “face” network. This correlation decreased significantly from ages 7 to 13
years, and remained low throughout adulthood. We hypothesize that the higher correlation
seen in younger children may also be a marker for tic diagnosis or severity.
Author Manuscript

The deficient emotional self-regulation profile from the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL-
DESR) consists of scores on the Anxiety/Depression, Aggression and Attention subscales of
the CBCL [78,79]. This scale was intended to be sensitive to dysregulation in multiple
domains, including affect (anxiety/mood), behavior (disruptive), and cognition (attention)
and has been thought to represent a global deficit in self-regulation related to processes of
effortful control as defined by Eisenberg and Spinrad [80]. Although this subscale has not
been reported in TS, it is postulated to associate with deficient central inhibitory processes,
which may predict greater difficulty suppressing tics.

Summary of hypotheses—Of the numerous reasonable hypotheses one could make


about a New Tics sample [6], we focus on five primary hypotheses to limit Type I error. We
Author Manuscript

propose that the following features are more likely to be present in the New Tics group than
in the tic-free control group (Aim 1a, below), will be associated with worsening of tics (or
less improvement in tics) at 12 months after tic onset (Aim 2a), and if present at baseline
will predict worse 12-month outcome (Aim 2b).

1. Lesser ability to suppress tics to verbal request (Tic Suppression Paradigm)

2. More omission errors during sustained attention (Continuous Performance Test)

3. Smaller caudate nucleus after correction for total brain volume

4. Greater severity of tics at screening (YGTSS total tic score, rated after
observation during the Tic Suppression Paradigm)

5. More premonitory urges (higher score on the PUTS scale)


Author Manuscript

In addition to this disciplined hypothesis-testing approach, given that so little work has been
done in this area, we will also examine the full set of data using machine learning methods.
Specifically, we will perform SVMs (for group comparisons) and SVRs (for correlations in
Aim 2). Using the full data set (clinical, neuropsychological, and structural and functional
MRI), we will perform SVMs for group comparisons and SVRs for correlations with
improvement in YGTSS total tic score (TTS), as an indication of clinically relevant
symptom change from the screening visit to the 12-month visit. We will then interrogate the

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 8

SVM and SVR results to identify features that drive the group differences or correlations, to
Author Manuscript

identify novel or multivariate findings relevant to pathophysiology or prognosis. This data-


driven approach to hypothesis generation motivates our collecting the other planned clinical
and imaging data.

Aim 1. Study pathophysiology of recent-onset tics.

Aim 1a. Identify clinical, neuropsychological, and brain imaging features that differentiate
children with recent tic onset (“New Tics” group) from tic-free controls. We will test a priori
hypotheses including tic suppression, inattentiveness, caudate nucleus volume, tic severity,
and premonitory urges (see “Summary of hypotheses”). Secondary analyses will apply
support vector machine (SVM) learning to a rich set of data to discover novel, multivariate
differences in the New Tics group [71,81]. These data will also include tic phenomenology,
psychiatric diagnosis, habit learning, motor dexterity, structural MRI, perfusion MRI, and rs-
Author Manuscript

fcMRI.

Aim 1b. Compare New Tics subjects to a group of children who are matched for age but
have already had tics for ≥1 year (“Existing TS/CTD”). Since both groups have tics, this
comparison will highlight abnormalities that cannot be explained by the mere current
presence of tics, including markers of chronicity or adaptation.

Aim 2. Prospective study of tic remission.

We will reevaluate New Tics subjects at the 1-year anniversary of tic onset. The primary
dependent variable was chosen to be the change in tic symptom severity (ΔTTS = change in
YGTSS Total Tic Score from baseline to follow-up). We focus on outcome as a continuous
variable because no reliable estimate exists for how many New Tics subjects will remit
Author Manuscript

versus go on to diagnosis with TS/CTD. Remission rate also depends on definition and on
the thoroughness of the follow-up evaluation [6].

Aim 2a. Study the physiology of tic remission by identifying changes in clinical,
neuropsychological, and brain imaging variables that correlate with changes in clinical tic
severity (ΔTTS). This Aim benefits from prospective observation and within-subject
comparisons. The primary analysis will focus on any markers identified in Aim 1. A
secondary analysis will apply machine learning methods for a data-driven approach (support
vector regression, SVR).

Aim 2b. Identify predictors of improvement or worsening, i.e., clinical, neuropsychological,


and brain imaging features at study entry that correlate significantly with ΔTTS. The 2
primary analyses will relate clinical outcome (ΔTTS) to tic suppression ability and caudate
Author Manuscript

volume at study entry. Secondary analyses will examine other predictors using an SVR
machine learning approach.

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 9

MATERIALS AND METHODS


Author Manuscript

Overview
The original grant proposals, and the current study protocol with additional methodological
details, are archived on the Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/y5vxj/.

The work described herein was approved by the Washington University Human Research
Protection Office (IRB), protocol numbers 201109157 and 201707059. Each child assented
and a parent (guardian) gave informed consent. Data shared from other projects were shared
after appropriate human subjects review and consent.

New Tics subjects have clinical, neuropsychological and MRI assessments within 6 months
of tic onset and again at the 12-month anniversary of their first tic. Tic-free control children,
matched to the New Tics subjects on age, sex, handedness, and if possible ADHD, will have
Author Manuscript

clinical and MRI assessments at screening and a clinical follow-up visit without MRI.
Children with Existing TS/CTD will likewise be matched to the New Tics subjects and have
MRI at baseline and clinical follow-up without MRI.

Figure 1 shows the general outline of initial study participation for children in the New Tics
group. Briefly, children are brought in as soon as possible after their first tic. Effort is
expended to identify as closely as possible the date of that first tic, as described in one of our
early publications [82]. Thorough clinical assessment is accompanied by selected
psychological tests and multimodal MRI (details are provided below). These results can be
compared to those of tic-free control or existing TS/CTD participants to identify differences
and begin to sort out their timing. The children then return at the 12-month anniversary of
their first tic, when TS/CTD can first be diagnosed. Their clinical status at that visit,
Author Manuscript

especially the total tic score from the YGTSS, allows us to determine which features present
at the baseline visit best predict outcome at this follow-up visit a median of 3.5 months later.

Several important project features are not shown in Figure 1. First is that New Tics
participants are followed up again, in person, at 1, 2 and 3 years after the 12-month follow-
up visit. Second, the New Tics group has a second MRI session at 12 months, identical to
their first session. Third, tic-free controls are not shown. Their participation is identical to
that of the New Tics group, except that in-person visits end after the initial follow-up visit.
Their “12-month” visit is timed to be the same duration after the baseline visit as that of the
New Tics participant to whom they are best matched. Its primary purpose is to find out how
many of them, if any, have developed tics during the follow-up period. Fourth, Existing
TS/CTD participants are not shown in Figure 1; their participation is similar to that of the
control group.
Author Manuscript

Subjects
The primary study sample consists of 110 children with New Tics, including those
previously studied and 70 newly enrolled subjects. We examine each of them within 9
months of tic onset using carefully selected clinical, neuropsychological, and MRI methods.
They return at the 1-year anniversary of tic onset for clinical and MRI evaluation, and yearly

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 10

thereafter for clinical follow-up only. For data analysis, the subjects studied in the pilot study
Author Manuscript

will be added to those studied under this project.

To reduce unwanted variance in the sample, we limit our subjects to ages 5–10 at
enrollment. This corresponds to the age of highest risk for onset of TS [3,71,83,84]. Chronic
tic disorder and transient tic disorder also tend to start within this window (mean age of
onset 7.4–8.5 years) [45]. The neuropsychological tests we are using are not normed for
children under age 5, and very few 3- or 4-year olds could complete the awake MRI session.
The upper age limit is arbitrary, but on a practical level, although our pilot study accepted
children age 5–17, almost all subjects were age 6–9 at screening.

There are two comparison groups. The first consists of 70 tic-free control children who
additionally have no first-degree relative with tics by parental history. We match these
subjects 1:1 to New Tics subjects with good 12-month MRI data, based on age, sex and
Author Manuscript

handedness. We also enrich this group for ADHD such that the proportion of tic-free
children with ADHD will match that of the New Tics group. Most matches will be drawn
from existing data from our labs; we have structural and rs-fcMRI studies from over 100
healthy controls [56,64–66,71]. Assuming a 74% rate of good MR scans in this group
(estimated from our preliminary data) means scanning 54 new tic-free control children (41
with ADHD, 13 without) to yield 40 with good MRI data. Also, the added 54 control
subjects will have identical diagnostic information as we will have on the New Tics group.
These control subjects will also return for a “12-month” follow-up visit. Since they have no
tic onset date to calculate the 12 months from, their follow-up visit will occur at the same
time after screening as did the follow-up visit in their matched New Tics subject. The
primary purpose of this clinical follow-up visit is to check whether or not they develop tics
in the intervening months since screening. If needed, we can also request data from the TAA
Author Manuscript

Neuroimaging Consortium, which has rs-fcMRI data from 88 TS subjects and 70 controls in
addition to the data from our site, or from the Human Connectome Project, for which Dr.
Schlaggar is a co-investigator.

The last group consists of 70 children who at the time of screening already have TS/CTD
(“Existing TS/CTD group”). We will follow the same strategy as for the tic-free controls,
leveraging existing data from our labs, including >100 children with structural and rs-fcMRI
data. All of these subjects were personally examined by authors KJB or BLS, and
additionally completed standard symptom rating scales. We add 25 new Existing TS/CTD
subjects with good MRI data, requiring us to scan about 34. They also return for a clinical
follow-up visit at the same interval after screening as their matched New Tics subject.

Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria


Author Manuscript

Inclusion criteria for all subjects are: Age 5–10 at screening. New Tics group: tics now, but
developed them only in the past 9 months. Exclusion criteria: secondary tics, another
neurological disorder (not counting migraine), structural brain disease, severe systemic
illness, lack of proficiency in the English language, and psychiatric illness including mental
retardation, autism, substance dependence, current substance abuse, primary psychotic
illness, bipolar disorder and current major depression. Psychoactive medications are allowed
if their dose has not changed in the past month.

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 11

Inclusion criteria for Existing TS/CTD control group are: children who meet DSM-5 criteria
Author Manuscript

for TS or CTD at enrollment, matched to children from the New Tics group on age (within 1
year), sex, handedness, and ADHD status. (Matching is not required if the children were
thought to be in the New Tics group before the face-to-face screening visit.) Exclusion
criteria are the same as for the New Tics group.

Inclusion for tic-free controls are: matched to children from the New Tics group on age
(within 1 year), sex, handedness, and ADHD status. Exclusion criteria: current or past tic
disorder in the subject or a first-degree relative, plus the exclusions listed for the New Tics
group. We do not exclude OCD or ADHD; such exclusion would introduce another
unwanted variable into the tics vs no-tics comparisons. However, both disorders are assessed
thoroughly at screening.

Clinical and Neuropsychological Measures


Author Manuscript

Data recorded includes demographics, brief IQ estimate (K-BIT II), the Child Behavior
Checklist (CBCL), manual dexterity (Purdue Pegboard test) [50], attention (CPT II) [48], a
“weather prediction” test of probabilistic classification kindly shared with us by Dr. Marsh
[51,85], psychiatric comorbidity (DSM-5 diagnoses by K-SADS, confirmed by Dr. Black
using all data), quantitative autistic traits (Social Responsiveness Scale [86]), quality of life
(PedsQL; [87]), history of birth complications and maternal smoking during pregnancy, and
family history. Dr. Black performs a neurological and psychiatric examination, reviews the
parental self-report measures, rates current symptom severity (YGTSS [88], Children’s Yale-
Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale (CY-BOCS) [89], ADHD Rating Scale [90]), and
makes a final diagnosis of tic disorders, OCD, and ADHD using all data.

The following measures are also completed for subjects with tics: best estimate date of
Author Manuscript

onset, typical features of TS according to the Diagnostic Confidence Index (DCI) [91], the
Premonitory Urge for Tics Scale (PUTS) [92], and a standardized tic suppression paradigm
(TSP) [47]. The TSP includes two 5-minute blocks of each of 3 conditions, seated alone in
an exam room with a microphone and video camera: baseline (no tic suppression), verbal
request to suppress tics, and differential reinforcement of zero-rate ticcing (DRO) using a
remote-controlled token dispenser [82]. Video recordings are made of each 5-minute block
to check scoring offline. The first 3 sessions are done completed in that order, then repeated
in random order (that same order is maintained for future visits with that participant). More
recently, we have prepended one 5-minute observation session with a staff member sitting
quietly in the room with the participant, to measure social influence on tic suppression [93–
95]. Children without tics will be similarly observed but will only complete the baseline
condition (no tic suppression).
Author Manuscript

MRI Methods
At the screening visit, we introduce each child to a mock scanner, which is equipped to give
immediate feedback to the child on head motion. To enhance subject comfort and quality of
scans, we use MRI acclimation materials adapted from the DirecNet MRI study of Type I
diabetes, including an introductory video and a holding-still game to practice at home with a
parent [96].

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 12

No caffeine intake is allowed before scans on study days. Subjects are monitored by video
Author Manuscript

recording for offline identification of any visible tic during the 45 min of functional data
collection. These data can be used to model or ignore fcMRI or ASL frames temporally
related to the tics. Subjects who only partially complete adequate scans in the time allotted
are invited to return, as soon as feasible, to complete the scans. We have adopted the
FIRMM system to monitor head movement during scan sessions, allowing rapid assessment
of whether more resting BOLD frames are needed [97].

We acquire 1 mm3 Tl-weighted MP-RAGE images with volumetric navigators (vNavs) for
prospective motion correction [98], and T2-weighted images. BOLD data are from a whole-
brain BOLD-sensitive asymmetric spin echo EPI sequence, during which participants are
asked to rest quietly and watch a fixation crosshair. For perfusion imaging, we use a pulsed
ASL sequence originally validated in children [99].
Author Manuscript

Image Analysis
Using MP-RAGE structural MRI, we measure the volume and shape of the putamen,
caudate, nucleus accumbens, globus pallidus, and thalamus using our published automated
method involving high dimensional brain mapping [100]. We measure cortical thickness and
volumes using Freesurfer software. In addition to these tests of a priori hypotheses, we plan
a 3D analysis using standard voxel-based morphometry (VBM) methods that we have used
previously [56,101–103].

BOLD pre-processing and rs-fcMRI analyses are carried out as previously described
[64,71,104–106]. We use very stringent criteria for BOLD fMRI data quality [69], since
even small-amplitude head motion can cause spurious changes in rs-fcMRI analysis
[67,107]. Quantitative measures of frame-to-frame head displacement (FD) are used to
Author Manuscript

reduce motion artifact using the following criteria: we will include BOLD-sensitive data
volumes only in temporally contiguous sets of at least 5 volumes with framewise
displacement <0.2 mm each, including BOLD runs only if they retained a minimum of 30
such volumes. Justification of these parameters, which may change with improved
methodological research, appears with additional technical details in ref. [71]. We will
similarly remove tic-related activity (using the audiovisual recordings acquired during
scans). Next, we extract resting state time series from seed ROIs, correlate the BOLD time
series region by region [108], combine correlation coefficients (r) across participants
[109,110], and test whether correlation coefficients are significantly different from zero (2-
tailed t tests, corrected). Interregional correlations are measured over different thresholds
and using network measures (e.g., participation coefficient, local clustering). Group
differences will be analyzed using graph analysis tools [111,112], including quantitative
Author Manuscript

(network modularity [113]) and qualitative methods (Social Network Image Animator [114]
with spring embedding algorithms as in ref. [115]).

Statistical Analysis
The primary approach to analyzing each main hypothesis is a general linear model; we
include nuisance variables if they explain a significant portion of the total variance (e.g.,
total intracranial volume, age, sex, and handedness). The independent variable was

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 13

originally planned to be change in YGTSS total tic score from screening to follow-up
Author Manuscript

(“ΔTTS”); we have since focused on follow-up TTS score as being more clinically
meaningful, and control for baseline TTS score.

We will also perform multivariate analyses to account for the large quantities of high-
dimensional data using support vector machine (SVM) classification. SVMs are algorithms
that take a training set of labeled samples, each with a series of measurements called
features, and learn properties and weights of the features that characterize the labels. Once a
decision function is learned based on the training data, it can be used to predict the class
label of a new, previously unseen, test sample. Support Vector Regression (SVR) extends
SVM to dimensional regression in order to predict a continuous measure for an individual,
rather than a class label. Here we apply methods previously used in our lab [77,81,116–118].
See [119–122] for mathematical descriptions of SVMs. We use the Spider Machine
Learning Toolbox for computations [123]. We will use SVM in several ways. First, we will
Author Manuscript

apply it in Aim 1 as a method primarily for exploratory analysis. In this case the training
data will include all subjects in each of the two groups being compared. An estimate of
reliability of accuracies can be computed testing on the same data set using a leave-one-out
cross validation approach, as previously described [71,81]. We hypothesize that
classification accuracy will be higher for an SVM based on the full data set (clinical data,
structural MRI, perfusion MRI, tic-triggered fMRI, and rs-fcMRI data), in which the
features will include assessment scores, regional volumes, voxelwise rCBF, tic-related
BOLD activity, and interregional temporal correlations, than for an SVM based on one data
type alone. Similarly, we will use SVR in Aim 2 as an exploratory approach to identify
patterns in the rich clinical and MRI data set that best predict follow-up symptom severity
(TTS), rather than categorical outcome.
Author Manuscript

We will also use SVM as a primary approach to identifying features that predict which New
Tics subjects will progress to TS/CTD (or SVR, to predict who will progress to more vs. less
symptomatic). This approach will train the SVM on the Existing TS/CTD and Tic-free
Control data sets, and then test baseline data from individual New Tics subjects to
“diagnose” them as more like Existing TS/CTD or more like Tic-free Control subjects. The
hypothesis is that this SVM approach will identify significantly better than chance which
New Tics subjects will remit (or become asymptomatic) and which will be diagnosed with
TS/CTD (or remain symptomatic).

RESULTS
Study Progress
Author Manuscript

Recruitment sources and follow-up success—In the New Tics group, 33% of
participants came from clinical referrals, while 54% were recruited by advertising (via local
school districts, online, or in traditional media).

Of the 82 New Tics participants who have completed a first visit, 65 have returned for the
12-month follow-up visit and an additional 9 are still awaiting return visits, leaving only 8
(9.6%) who dropped out or were lost to follow-up. After the first 44 participants, we began

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 14

to invite New Tics subjects to return at 24, 36 or 48 months after their first tic. As of April,
Author Manuscript

2020, we have completed 31 24-month, 10 36-month and 6 48-month visits.

A number of other children were brought in by parents who originally reported a tic duration
of only a few months, but definite tics more than a year ago were discovered on review of
history or on examination, giving them a DSM-5 diagnosis of Tourette syndrome. These
children were invited to participate in the Established TS/CTD group. For 9 children, the
baseline tic duration fell in the range of 9–11.5 months; these few are being followed under
separate funding, with a first follow-up visit scheduled 3 months after the baseline visit.

Sample characteristics—Vigorous recruitment efforts have led to a sample that on


average has relatively mild symptoms and was seen within a few months of their first tic (see
Table 1, which includes only the children who have already completed a follow-up visit).
However, the range of severity is wide, and the distribution of time since onset of tics is
Author Manuscript

fairly smooth (Figure 2). Most of the children have experienced several motor tics and at
least one phonic tic by the baseline visit (though some of these were not previously
identified by the children or their parents).

At the follow-up visit, on average tic severity has improved, though often other tics have
appeared in the interim (Table 2). Other classic TS features have also often appeared, such as
premonitory urges or intentional suppression, so DCI score generally has increased.

MRI data quality—Neuroimaging studies in clinical samples of children often have


relatively high rates of data loss [126], and we knew that collecting quality rs-fcMRI data
would be difficult in children with tics as young as age 5, many with ADHD. Our initial
efforts reflected that concern, with adequate quality data for rs-fcMRI analysis in only 3 of
Author Manuscript

the first 14 subjects enrolled. Therefore, we consulted with colleagues and implemented
several steps to improve quality. These included more extensive subject preparation [96],
training in a mock MRI scanner with immediate head movement feedback, and allowance
for a possible second scan day. Since implementing these changes, we have collected
adequate rs-fcMRI data in 34 of the remaining 47 subjects scanned (72%). For the structural
MRI scans, we have high quality images (based on standardized manual ratings) from 41 of
54 children scanned (76%).

Reward Enhances Tic Suppression in Children with PTD


Our first publication from this project came from the first 21 children enrolled [82]. That
report discusses in detail our decisions about dating the first tic. Tic suppression was well
known in TS/CTD, and was known to improve in the presence of an immediate, contingent
Author Manuscript

reward for periods with no tics (differential reinforcement of other, DRO). However, given
how TS/CTD were defined, children in those previous studies had experienced tics for at
least a year, so whether tic inhibition was present initially or learned over time was not
known. Results showed that these children, studied on average 3.5 months after onset of tics,
generally could suppress tics, especially in the DRO condition. More than half suppressed
tics (i.e., had more 10-s tic-free periods) with a verbal request to suppress tics, or with a
control condition in which rewards were given regardless of whether the child was ticcing
(noncontingent reward, NCR). All but 3 of them suppressed tics during the DRO condition.

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 15

Time since tic onset did not correlate significantly with suppression ability. In other words,
Author Manuscript

these children, like children with TS/CTD, do have some capacity to suppress tics, and
immediate reward enhances that capacity.

Data from that report also contributed substantially to a multi-site summary of individual
participant data on voluntary tic suppression in children with PTD or TS/CTD [127]. Since
then, we have confirmed the key findings from that initial report in a later superset of
patients [128].

Transient Tics Aren’t


DSM-IV and DSM-IV-TR diagnosed children who had been ticcing for less than a year with
Transient Tic Disorder. Despite the name, tics in these children did not always prove to be
transient, as every child with TS/CTD originally had tics for less than a year. (This
observation was one motivation for creating the PTD diagnosis in DSM-5; see discussion in
Author Manuscript

ref. [6]). As reviewed above, professional consensus had been that these tics would almost
always disappear, though the limited available data did not confirm that consensus.

We reported clinical outcomes for the New Tics children enrolled in our preliminary data
[124]. These first 43 children comprise the largest ever prospective study of PTD. At the
baseline visit, although tics were relatively mild for most children (only 9 showed a TTS >
20, indicating moderate or severe tic severity), the symptoms generally concerned parents;
12 of 15 families who were asked reported they had taken or would be taking their child to a
doctor because of the tics (this question was added midway through the study).

Thirty-nine of the 43 (90%) returned for the 12-month follow-up visit. To our surprise, every
child still had tics. Tics were observed at the visit in all but one child, sometimes only when
Author Manuscript

observed alone (by audio-video feed) during the tic suppression paradigm. The remaining
child had tics reported by parents in the past week, and was observed to tic on a later
encounter. Thus at follow-up, the DSM-5 diagnosis was Persistent (Chronic) Motor Tic
Disorder in 3 children and Tourette’s Disorder in all the rest. DCI scores had increased by a
mean of 9 points, showing gradual accumulation of more classic TS features. These results
call into doubt the general assumption of complete remission for PTD. Even if all 4 of the
participants lost to follow-up remitted completely, at least 90% (39/43) of the total sample
still showed tics at the one-year anniversary of tic onset. That fraction implies 95%
confidence that the population remission rate at 12 months is <22%.

On the other hand, on average tics had improved. Only six participants met DSM-IV criteria
for Tourette’s Disorder, which required impairment in a life role or marked distress, and only
two had mild or greater impairment in a life role. We asked 29 parents if they were planning
Author Manuscript

to see a doctor for further management of their child’s tics, and only 3 said yes. In fact,
several children and parents felt the tics were gone. In some cases (at least 9 of 28), no tics
were seen at the 12-month visit during a typical neurological history and physical exam
supplemented by information needed for YGTSS rating. In routine clinical practice, these
children would have been considered remitted.

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 16

In a practical sense, the news is still good for these families, as the tics were only clinically
Author Manuscript

problematic at follow-up in 5% (mild impairment or more) to 10% (planning to seek further


clinical care). The difference from previous clinical consensus appears most likely to be in
the fact that many of these patients would not be seeing a doctor for clinical follow-up, and
in the thoroughness of the history and exam at follow-up.

An alternative explanation could be that tics usually do completely remit, but after the 1-year
mark. To begin to explore that possibility, we are continuing the planned 24-, 36- and 48-
month follow-up visits. Thus far, almost every such visit has continued to show evidence of
tics, though they have apparently completely remitted in a small number of cases. We look
forward to completing these visits in a few years.

This report also identified several clinical features at the baseline visit that predicted better
tic outcome at follow-up. Not surprisingly, baseline TTS was correlated with TTS at follow-
Author Manuscript

up, so additional analyses controlled for baseline severity. Worse outcomes were predicted
by the following features at baseline: higher Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) score,
higher DCI score, higher CBCL-DESR score, the presence of an anxiety disorder, and a
history of 3 or more phonic tics. In a stepwise multiple regression analysis, a model with
TTS, SRS score, and anxiety disorder, at baseline, explained nearly half the variance in the
12-month TTS score. An elevated SRS score indicates a greater number of features
associated with autism spectrum disorder, though in this sample all the SRS scores were in
the normal range.

Tic Suppression Ability Predicts Outcome


We had hypothesized that better tic suppression would predict better clinical outcomes [82].
Last year, we reported a test of this hypothesis on the first 45 children to complete their 12-
Author Manuscript

month visits [128]. Those children with better tic suppression in the presence of a reward
had lower tic burden (TTS scores) at the 12-month follow-up visit. Additionally, children
with more premonitory urges tended to suppress tics more successfully. This report identifies
another potential predictor of clinical outcome in PTD and suggests one mechanism that
may help reconcile the persistence of tics, with careful observation when the child is alone,
with the improvement in clinical status. Specifically, clinical improvement is driven in large
part by less frequent or less severe tics when around other people, and perhaps the
experience of social rewards for tic suppression lead over time to (perhaps unintentional) tic
suppression around others. To begin to measure social influence on tic suppression, recently
we have added a session to the tic suppression protocol. A staff member sits with the child
without engaging in conversation and without any explicit direction to suppress tics; this
setting was shown to decrease tic expression compared to the child sitting alone [93–95].
Author Manuscript

Other Behavioral Measures


In the first 58 children to complete the 1-year follow-up visit, we tested the predictive value
of several other cognitive or motor tasks we examined based on past studies in TS/CTD,
namely the Purdue Pegboard Test (measuring dexterity and bimanual coordination), the
Continuous Performance Test (measuring attention and response inhibition), and a weather
prediction task (intended to quantify a form of habit learning). None significantly predicted

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 17

1-year tic outcome, and none correlated significantly with tic suppression ability after
Author Manuscript

controlling for age [129].

Subcortical Volumetry
One of our primary hypotheses was that caudate volume would correlate inversely with tic
severity at follow-up. We recently submitted for publication results from testing this
hypothesis in the 41 children with good-quality structural MRI data at baseline and had
completed the follow-up visit by December, 2019 [130]. We used a method that
simultaneously and reliably quantified the volume of several subcortical structures. Caudate
volume was not a significant prognostic factor. However, a larger hippocampus at baseline
predicted higher tic severity at 12-month follow-up (p = 0.002). Notably, this result was
confirmed in a subgroup of 25 children whose MRI was performed using prospective motion
correction to minimize artifactual changes to regional brain volumes induced by head
Author Manuscript

movement during the scan. In this group, baseline hippocampal volume and baseline TTS
explained over half the variance in 12-month TTS score. Although we did not include
hippocampal volume in our hypotheses for this project, previous studies have identified
larger hippocampal volume in children with TS [131], and both the PTD and TS groups
tended to have greater volume in our study as well. The hippocampus is involved in memory
consolidation for motor habit learning as well as declarative learning [132], and in youth
with TS, there is evidence that those with more severe tics have more persistent motor
memory, in that they take longer to unlearn a previously learned pattern of behavior [133].
These observations allow the hypothesis that greater hippocampal volume may mediate
relatively inflexible habit learning, leading to greater persistence of tics between the initial
assessment and the 1-year mark.

Other Contributions
Author Manuscript

As part of our efforts to recruit children early in the course of PTD, we undertook an
observational study at a local elementary school, similar to a previous study by Snider and
colleagues [17]. Experienced raters including authors KJB and DJG observed various
classrooms for at least an hour, noting children with probable tics observed at least 3 times
in the hour. These children were approximately 5–11 years old. Ratings were done on visits
to the school in each of 3 months (March, April and May). Initial results suggest that tics
were noted in 10–25% of children, depending on the month, roughly consistent with the
mean cross-sectional tic frequency of 24% noted by Snider et al.

We developed software to facilitate recording of the timing of tics during the TSP, and to
automate delivery of rewards under the DRO and NCR conditions, and have made it freely
available [134]. Some of the participants in this study signed a separate consent to let their
Author Manuscript

video recordings be used for the “VISIT-TS,” which is a video-enhanced screening


instrument to ask people if they (or their children) have tics after showing them a 5-minute
video with nearly 100 video clips of tics [135].

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 18

DISCUSSION
Author Manuscript

Many children have tics at least briefly, whereas few are diagnosed with a chronic tic
disorder. This observation inspired the present project, in which the disappearance (or
improvement) of tics can be observed prospectively. Most previous studies on PTD were
retrospective, and most enrolled children in a clinical setting, introducing bias for severity
and treatment availability. However, understanding why tics usually improve over the first
year after their appearance may provide clues to alleviating chronic tics.

Thus far, with vigorous outreach and advertising, we have maintained adequate recruitment
of children with recent onset of tics (mean <4 months), a population that has been difficult to
enroll. We confirmed that, like children with a chronic tic disorder, tic suppression is
possible within the first few months after tic onset and is improved by providing immediate,
contingent rewards for successful tic suppression. More important and more surprising, we
Author Manuscript

have demonstrated that the course of PTD is almost always one of improvement rather than
disappearance of symptoms at the 12-month mark that defines chronicity in the current
nosology. We have further identified several indicators at initial enrollment that predict
worse or better clinical tic status at 12 months. Some of these are clinical measures that are
relatively easily assessed, including baseline tic severity and variety, subsyndromal autistic
features, and anxiety. Better intentional tic suppression under conditions of immediate
reward proved to be another relatively straightforward indicator of improvement at follow-
up. Finally, we identified hippocampal volume as a novel prognostic biomarker over the first
year of tic symptoms. All of these are novel results with potential clinical relevance. The
project continues enrollment, but we have reported important, clinically relevant results from
what is already the largest prospective study of PTD.
Author Manuscript

The primary limitation of our work is that our sample was not a purely representative
epidemiological sample. Such a sample would be extremely difficult to study, since a
relatively large number of children would need to be thoroughly screened, and since most
would be asymptomatic, limiting child and parent enthusiasm for participation. The most
obvious concern related to representativeness is that we may be likely to oversample
children with severe tics or from families better positioned to access medical care. However,
we feel that our sample is fairly representative. Tic severity was fairly low at study entry,
with a mean TTS < 20; about a third of the children came from families experienced with
tics (positive family history or a physician parent); and disadvantaged minorities are
represented at or above the frequency predicted from regional demographics.

Remaining work includes first completing enrollment of the full sample, which was
predicted to provide adequate power for the remaining analyses, especially rs-fcMRI, voxel-
Author Manuscript

based morphometry, perfusion fMRI, and the machine learning analyses. Other planned
analyses include a shape deformation analysis of basal ganglia, thalamus and hippocampus
by our colleague Dr. Lei Wang from Northwestern University [100,136,137]. We are also
eager to find out how often tics completely remit by 2, 3 and 4 years after tic disorder onset.

One possible future direction may be to test whether it is possible to effectively reduce
future tic severity by a randomized controlled trial of behavior therapy in the New Tics

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 19

children most likely to need continuing clinical attention. Potentially this approach would
Author Manuscript

produce secondary prevention of TS/CTD. The prognostic indicators we have identified will
allow directing treatment to those children most likely to benefit from intervention, a
resource allocation benefit in this population with relatively good outcomes overall. The best
proven behavioral treatment for chronic tic disorders is CBIT, but alternatives that may lend
themselves well to children of this age, early in the course of illness, include CBIT-Jr, which
focuses more on family accommodation and attention to tics [138], or exposure and response
prevention (ERP) [139]. For ERP we have proposed a modification, based on the tic
suppression results described above, that may facilitate participation by young children
[140]. Alternatively, perhaps a cognitive remediation strategy could focus on improving the
ability to unlearn habitual maladaptive motor sequences. Of course, novel results after
completion of enrollment may point us in different directions.
Author Manuscript

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors acknowledge the substantial contributions to the New Tics project by the children who participated and
their parents, and by research staff. Vicki Martin summarized recruitment data. Grant proposals and study protocols
were published on the Open Science Framework (osf.io/y5vxj/).

This research was funded by the National Institutes of Health (K24MH087913, R21NS091635, K01MH104592,
R01MH104030, UL1RR024992, UL1TR000448, U54HD087011).

FUNDING

Research reported in this publication was supported by National Institutes of Health, awards K24MH087913 to
KJB; R21NS091635 to BLS and KJB; K01MH104592 to DJG; R01MH104030 to KJB and BLS; the Washington
University Institute of Clinical and Translational Sciences grants UL1RR024992 and UL1TR000448; and the
Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development of the National Institutes of
Health under Award Number U54HD087011 to the Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities Research Center at
Washington University; and by the Mallinckrodt Institute of Radiology MIR-IDDRC Pilot Study Fund. The studies
presented in this work were carried out in part in the East Building MR Facility of the Washington University
Author Manuscript

Medical Center. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official
views of the National Institutes of Health or of the MIR.

CONFLICTS OF INTEREST

KJB reports research funding from Emalex Biosciences, past consulting for Acadia Pharmaceuticals, and past
speakers bureau participation for Acadia and Teva Pharmaceutical Industries Ltd. The other authors declare that
they have no conflicts of interest.

ABBREVIATIONS
TS Tourette syndrome

CTD Persistent (Chronic) Motor or Vocal Tic Disorder

PTD Provisional Tic Disorder


Author Manuscript

CBIT Comprehensive Behavioral Intervention for Tics

NIH National Institutes of Health

YGTSS Yale Global Tic Severity Scale

TTS total tic score from the YGTSS

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 20

MRI magnetic resonance imaging


Author Manuscript

fMRI functional MRI

fcMRI functional connectivity fMRI

rs-fcMRI resting state functional connectivity fMRI

CPT Continuous Performance Test

DCI Diagnostic Confidence Index

ASL arterial spin labeling perfusion fMRI

BOLD blood oxygen level–dependent fMRI signal

VBM voxel-based morphometry


Author Manuscript

SVM support vector machine

SVR support vector regression

DRO differential reinforcement of other behavior

NCR noncontingent reward

SRS Social Responsiveness Scale

CBCL Child Behavior Checklist

DESR deficient emotional self-regulation


Author Manuscript

ERP exposure and response prevention

REFERENCES
1. Black KJ. Tics In: Kompoliti K, Verhagen Metman L, Cornelia C, Goetz C, Goldman J, Kordower J,
et al., editors. Encyclopedia of Movement Disorders. Oxford (UK): Elsevier (Academic Press);
2010 p. 231–6.
2. Schlaggar BL, Mink JW. Movement disorders in children. Pediatr Rev. 2003;24(2):39–51. [PubMed:
12563038]
3. Leckman JF, Zhang H, Vitale A, Lahnin F, Lynch K, Bondi C, et al. Course of tic severity in
Tourette syndrome: the first two decades. Pediatrics. 1998;102(1 Pt 1):14–9. [PubMed: 9651407]
4. The Tourette Syndrome Classification Study Group. Definitions and classification of tic disorders.
Arch Neurol. 1993;50(10):1013–6. [PubMed: 8215958]
5. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth
Edition Arlington (VA, USA): American Psychiatric Association; 2013.
Author Manuscript

6. Black KJ, Black ER, Greene DJ, Schlaggar BL. Provisional Tic Disorder: What to tell parents when
their child first starts ticcing [version 1]. F1000Res. 2016;5:696. [PubMed: 27158458]
7. Leckman JF, Bloch MH, Smith ME, Larabi D, Hampson M. Neurobiological substrates of
Tourette’s disorder. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2010;20(4):237–47. [PubMed: 20807062]
8. Singer HS. Tourette syndrome and other tic disorders. Handb Clin Neurol. 2011;100:641–57.
[PubMed: 21496613]
9. Greene DJ, Black KJ, Schlaggar BL. Neurobiology and functional anatomy of tic disorders In:
Martino D, Leckman JF, editors. Tourette Syndrome. Oxford (UK): Oxford University Press; 2013.

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 21

10. Cuenca J, Glazebrook C, Kendal T, Hedderly T, Heyman I, Jackson G, et al. Perceptions of


treatment for tics among young people with Tourette syndrome and their parents: a mixed methods
Author Manuscript

study. BMC Psychiatry. 2015;15:46. [PubMed: 25879205]


11. Robertson MM. The prevalence and epidemiology of Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Part 1: the
epidemiological and prevalence studies. J Psychosom Res. 2008;65(5):461–72. [PubMed:
18940377]
12. Mason A, Banerjee S, Eapen V, Zeitlin H, Robertson MM. The prevalence of Tourette syndrome in
a mainstream school population. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1998;40(5):292–6. [PubMed: 9630255]
13. Kurlan R, Como PG, Miller B, Palumbo D, Deeley C, Andresen EM, et al. The behavioral
spectrum of tic disorders: a community-based study. Neurology. 2002;59(3):414–20. [PubMed:
12177376]
14. Hornsey H, Banerjee S, Zeitlin H, Robertson M. The prevalence of Tourette syndrome in 13–14-
year-olds in mainstream schools. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001;42(8):1035–9. [PubMed:
11806685]
15. Peterson BS, Pine DS, Cohen P, Brook JS. Prospective, longitudinal study of tic, obsessive-
compulsive, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorders in an epidemiological sample. J Am Acad
Author Manuscript

Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40(6):685–95. [PubMed: 11392347]


16. Cubo E, Gabriel y Galan JMT, Villaverde VA, Velasco SS, Benito VD, Macarron JV, et al.
Prevalence of tics in schoolchildren in central Spain: a population-based study. Pediatr Neurol.
2011;45(2):100–8. [PubMed: 21763950]
17. Snider LA, Seligman LD, Ketchen BR, Levitt SJ, Bates LR, Garvey MA, et al. Tics and problem
behaviors in schoolchildren: prevalence, characterization, and associations. Pediatrics. 2002;110(2
Pt 1):331–6. [PubMed: 12165586]
18. Dooley JM. Tic disorders in childhood. Semin Pediatr Neurol. 2006;13(4):231–42. [PubMed:
17178353]
19. Tics Kuperman S. and Tourette’s syndrome in childhood. Semin Pediatr Neurol. 2003;10(1):35–40.
[PubMed: 12785746]
20. Bloch MH, Leckman JF. Tic disorders In: Martin A, Volkmar FR, Lewis M, editors. Lewis’s Child
and Adolescent Psychiatry: A Comprehensive Textbook. 4th ed. Philadelphia (PA,US): Lippincott
Williams & Wilkins; 2007 p. 569–82.
Author Manuscript

21. Zinner SH, Mink JW. Movement disorders I: tics and stereotypies. Pediatr Rev. 2010;31(6):223–
33. [PubMed: 20516234]
22. Jung HY, Chung SJ, Hwang JM. Tic disorders in children with frequent eye blinking. J AAPOS.
2004;8(2):171–4. [PubMed: 15088052]
23. Scahill L, Sukhodolsky DG, Williams SK, Leckman JF. Public health significance of tic disorders
in children and adolescents. Adv Neurol. 2005;96:240–8. [PubMed: 16383223]
24. Shapiro AK, Shapiro E, Young JG, Feinberg TE. Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome. 2nd ed. New
York (US): Raven Press; 1988 p. 171.
25. Fourneret P, Desombre H, Broussolle E. [From tic disorders to Tourette syndrome: current data,
comorbidities, and therapeutic approach in children]. Arch Pediatr. 2014;21(6):646–51. [PubMed:
24815597]
26. Bruun RD, Budman CL. The course and prognosis of Tourette syndrome. Neurol Clin.
1997;15(2):291–8. [PubMed: 9115462]
27. Carter AS, Pauls DL, Leckman JF, Cohen DJ. A prospective longitudinal study of Gilles de la
Tourette’s syndrome. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1994;33(3):377–85. [PubMed:
Author Manuscript

8169183]
28. Spencer T, Biederman M, Coffey B, Geller D, Wilens T, Faraone S. The 4-year course of tic
disorders in boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry.
1999;56(9):842–7. [PubMed: 12884890]
29. Hong KE. A clinical study of tic disorder in Korea. J Korean Pediatr Assoc. 1981;24(3):198–208.
30. Shin ZH, Jung C-H, Kim HC. Follow-up study of the tic disorders. J Korean Acad Child Adolesc
Psychiatry. 1996;7(1):68–76.
31. Shapiro E, Shapiro AK. Semiology, nosology and criteria for tic disorders. Rev Neurol (Paris).
1986;142(11):824–32. [PubMed: 3469716]

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 22

32. Goetz CG, Tanner CM, Stebbins GT, Leipzig G, Carr WC. Adult tics in Gilles de la Tourette’s
syndrome: description and risk factors. Neurology. 1992;42(4):784–8. [PubMed: 1565232]
Author Manuscript

33. de Groot CM, Bornstein RA, Spetie L, Burriss B. The course of tics in Tourette syndrome: a 5-year
follow-up study. Ann Clin Psychiatry. 1994;6(4):227–33. [PubMed: 7647832]
34. Pappert EJ, Goetz CG, Louis ED, Blasucci L, Leurgans S. Objective assessments of longitudinal
outcome in Gilles de la Tourette’s syndrome. Neurology. 2003;61(7):936–40. [PubMed:
14557563]
35. Coffey BJ, Biederman J, Geller D, Frazier J, Spencer T, Doyle R, et al. Reexamining tic persistence
and tic-associated impairment in Tourette’s Disorder: Findings from a naturalistic follow-up study.
J Nerv Ment Dis. 2004;192(11):776–80. [PubMed: 15505522]
36. Bloch MH, Leckman JF, Zhu H, Peterson BS. Caudate volumes in childhood predict symptom
severity in adults with Tourette syndrome. Neurology. 2005;65(8):1253–8. [PubMed: 16247053]
37. Bloch ΜΗ, Peterson BS, Scahill L, Otka J, Katsovich L, Zhang H, et al. Adulthood outcome of tic
and obsessive-compulsive symptom severity in children with Tourette syndrome. Arch Pediatr
Adolesc Med. 2006;160(1):65–9. [PubMed: 16389213]
38. Leckman JF, Bloch MH, King RA, Scahill L. Phenomenology of tics and natural history of tic
Author Manuscript

disorders. Adv Neurol. 2006;99:1–16. [PubMed: 16536348]


39. Cavanna AE, David K, Orth M, Robertson MM. Predictors during childhood of future health-
related quality of life in adults with Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Eur J Paediatr Neurol.
2012;16(6):605–12. [PubMed: 22381812]
40. Groth C Tourette syndrome in a longitudinal perspective. Clinical course of tics and comorbidities,
coexisting psychopathologies, phenotypes and predictors. Dan Med J. 2018;65(4):B5465.
[PubMed: 29619935]
41. Bäumer T, Sajin V, Munchau A. Childhood-onset movement disorders: A clinical series of 606
cases. Mov Disord Clin Pract. 2017;4(3):437–40. [PubMed: 30363430]
42. Peterson BS, Riddle MA, Cohen DJ, Katz LD, Smith JC, Hardin MT, et al. Reduced basal ganglia
volumes in Tourette’s syndrome using three-dimensional reconstruction techniques from magnetic
resonance images. Neurology. 1993(43):941–9. [PubMed: 8492950]
43. Plessen KJ, Bansal R, Peterson BS. Imaging evidence for anatomical disturbances and neuroplastic
compensation in persons with Tourette syndrome. J Psychosom Res. 2009;67(6):559–73.
Author Manuscript

[PubMed: 19913660]
44. Black KJ. Behavior therapy for Tourette syndrome and other tic disorders. 2017 Available from:
https://tics.wustl.edu/treatment/behavior-therapy-for-tics/. Accessed 2020 May 27.
45. Khalifa N, von Knorring AL. Tourette syndrome and other tic disorders in a total population of
children: clinical assessment and background. Acta Paediatr. 2005;94(11):1608–14. [PubMed:
16352498]
46. Greene DJ, Koller JM, Schlaggar BL, Black KJ. “Can you stop that?” Ability to suppress tics is
present within months of tic onset, and can predict future clinical outcome. Present at Annual
meeting, Society for Neuroscience; 2012 Oct 13-17; New Orleans, LA, USA Poster #764.01.
47. Woods DW, Himle MB. Creating tic suppression: comparing the effects of verbal instruction to
differential reinforcement. J Appl Behav Anal. 2004;37(3):417–20. [PubMed: 15529900]
48. Woods DW, Himle MB, Miltenberger RG, Carr JE, Osmon DC, Karsten AM, et al. Durability,
negative impact, and neuropsychological predictors of tic suppression in children with chronic tic
disorder. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2008;36(2):237–45. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9173-9
[PubMed: 17717739]
Author Manuscript

49. Chang HL, Liang HY, Wang HS, Li CS, Ko NC, Hsu YP. Behavioral and emotional problems in
adolescents with Tourette syndrome. Chang Gung Med J. 2008;31(2):145–52. [PubMed:
18567414]
50. Bloch MH, Sukhodolsky DG, Leckman JF, Schultz RT. Fine-motor skill deficits in childhood
predict adulthood tic severity and global psychosocial functioning in Tourette’s syndrome. J Child
Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47(6):551–9. [PubMed: 16712631]
51. Marsh R, Alexander GM, Packard MG, Zhu H, Wingard JC, Quackenbush G, et al. Habit learning
in Tourette syndrome: a translational neuroscience approach to a developmental psychopathology.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61(12):1259–68. [PubMed: 15583117]

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 23

52. Kurlan R, Lichter D, Hewitt D. Sensory tics in Tourette’s syndrome. Neurology. 1989;39(5):731–4.
[PubMed: 2710364]
Author Manuscript

53. Rozenman M, Johnson OE, Chang SW, Woods DW, Walkup JT, Wilhelm S, et al. Relationships
between premonitory urge and anxiety in youth with chronic tic disorders. Child Health Care.
2015;44(3):235–48. [PubMed: 27110050]
54. Draper A, Jackson GM, Morgan PS, Jackson SR. Premonitory urges are associated with decreased
grey matter thickness within the insula and sensorimotor cortex in young people with Tourette
syndrome. J Neuropsychol. 2016;10(l):143–53. [PubMed: 26538289]
55. McGuire JF, Piacentini J, Scahill L, Woods DW, Villarreal R, Wilhelm S, et al. Bothersome tics in
patients with chronic tic disorders: Characteristics and individualized treatment response to
behavior therapy. Behav Res Ther. 2015;70:56–63. [PubMed: 25988365]
56. Greene DJ, Williams AC III, Roller JM, Schlaggar BL, Black KJ, The Tourette Association of
America Neuroimaging Consortium. Brain structure in pediatric Tourette syndrome. Mol
Psychiatry. 2017;22(7):972–80. [PubMed: 27777415]
57. Greene DJ, Kim S, Black KJ, Schlaggar BL. Neurobiology and functional anatomy of tic disorders
In: Martino D, Leckman JF, editors. Tourette Syndrome. 2nd ed. Oxford (UK): Oxford University
Author Manuscript

Press; 2020 in press.


58. Diler RS, Reyhanli M, Toros F, Kibar M, Avci A. Tc-99m-ECD SPECT brain imaging in children
with Tourette’s syndrome. Yonsei Med J. 2002;43(4):403–10. [PubMed: 12243130]
59. Moriarty J, Costa DC, Schmitz B, Trimble MR, Ell PJ, Robertson MM. Brain perfusion
abnormalities in Gilles de la Tourette’s syndrome. Br J Psychiatry. 1995;167:249–54. [PubMed:
7582678]
60. Black KJ, Roller JM, Campbell MC, Gusnard DA, Bandak SI. Quantification of indirect pathway
inhibition by the adenosine A2a antagonist SYN115 in Parkinson disease. J Neurosci.
2010;30(48):16284–92. [PubMed: 21123574]
61. Black KJ, Duvall LB, Campbell MC, Roller JM. Signal and noise in continuous arterial spin
labeling (cASL) as a function of time. 2008 [updated 2009/12/17/]. Available from: http://
www.abstractsonline.com/Plan/ViewAbstract.aspx?sKey=a9936bf9-50db-482c-8a46-
d6059322deda&cKey=0a4a298e-5775-4248-bc5c-le7cl25489ef. Accessed 2020 May 27.
62. Stewart SB, Roller JM, Campbell MC, Black KJ. Arterial spin labeling versus BOLD in direct
challenge and drug-task interaction pharmacological fMRI. PeerJ. 2014;2:e687. [PubMed:
Author Manuscript

25538867]
63. Stewart SB, Roller JM, Campbell MC, Perlmutter JS, Black KJ. Additive global cerebral blood
flow normalization in arterial spin labeling perfusion imaging. PeerJ. 2015;3:e834. [PubMed:
25802806]
64. Church JA, Fair DA, Dosenbach NU, Cohen AL, Miezin FM, Petersen SE, et al. Control networks
in paediatric Tourette syndrome show immature and anomalous patterns of functional connectivity.
Brain. 2009;132(Pt 1):225–38. [PubMed: 18952678]
65. Church JA, Wenger KK, Dosenbach NU, Miezin FM, Petersen SE, Schlaggar BL. Task control
signals in pediatric Tourette syndrome show evidence of immature and anomalous functional
activity. Front Hum Neurosci. 2009;3:38. [PubMed: 19949483]
66. Fair DA, Cohen AL, Dosenbach NU, Church JA, Miezin FM, Barch DM, et al. The maturing
architecture of the brain’s default network. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105(10):4028–32.
[PubMed: 18322013]
67. Power JD, Barnes KA, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Spurious but systematic
Author Manuscript

correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage.
2012;59(3):2142–54. [PubMed: 22019881]
68. Siegel JS, Power JD, Dubis JW, Vogel AC, Church JA, Schlaggar BL, et al. Statistical
improvements in functional magnetic resonance imaging analyses produced by censoring high-
motion data points. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35(5):1981–96. [PubMed: 23861343]
69. Power JD, Mitra A, Laumann TO, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Methods to detect,
characterize, and remove motion artifact in resting state fMRI. Neuroimage. 2014;84:320–41.
[PubMed: 23994314]

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 24

70. Power JD, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Recent progress and outstanding issues in motion correction
in resting state fMRI. Neuroimage. 2015;105:536–51. [PubMed: 25462692]
Author Manuscript

71. Greene DJ, Church JA, Dosenbach NU, Nielsen AN, Adeyemo B, Nardos B, et al. Multivariate
pattern classification of pediatric Tourette syndrome using functional connectivity MRI. Dev Sci.
2016;19(4):581–98. [PubMed: 26834084]
72. Hoeft F, McCandliss BD, Black JM, Gantman A, Zakerani N, Hulme C, et al. Neural systems
predicting long-term outcome in dyslexia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108(1):361–6.
[PubMed: 21173250]
73. Yang H, Liu J, Sui J, Pearlson G, Calhoun VD. A Hybrid Machine Learning Method for Fusing
fMRI and Genetic Data: Combining both Improves Classification of Schizophrenia. Front Hum
Neurosci. 2010;4:192. [PubMed: 21119772]
74. Bohlhalter S, Goldfine A, Matteson S, Garraux G, Hanakawa T, Kansaku K, et al. Neural correlates
of tic generation in Tourette syndrome: an event-related functional MRI study. Brain. 2006;129(Pt
8):2029–37. [PubMed: 16520330]
75. Hampson M, Tokoglu F, King RA, Constable RT, Leckman JF. Brain areas coactivating with motor
cortex during chronic motor tics and intentional movements. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;65(7):594–9.
Author Manuscript

[PubMed: 19111281]
76. Barnes KA, Cohen AL, Power JD, Nelson SM, Dosenbach YB, Miezin FM, et al. Identifying basal
ganglia divisions in individuals using resting-state functional connectivity MRI. Front Syst
Neurosci. 2010;4:18. [PubMed: 20589235]
77. Greene DJ, Laumann TO, Dubis JW, Ihnen SK, Neta M, Power JD, et al. Developmental changes
in the organization of functional connections between the basal ganglia and cerebral cortex. J
Neurosci. 2014;34(17):5842–54. [PubMed: 24760844]
78. Biederman J, Petty CR, Day H, Goldin RL, Spencer T, Faraone SV, et al. Severity of the
aggression/anxiety-depression/attention child behavior checklist profile discriminates between
different levels of deficits in emotional regulation in youth with attention-deficit hyperactivity
disorder. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2012;33(3):236–43. [PubMed: 22278125]
79. Althoff RR, Verhulst FC, Rettew DC, Hudziak JJ, van der Ende J. Adult outcomes of childhood
dysregulation: a 14-year follow-up study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry.
2010;49(11):1105–16. [PubMed: 20970698]
80. Eisenberg N, Spinrad TL. Emotion-related regulation: sharpening the definition. Child Dev.
Author Manuscript

2004;75(2):334–9. [PubMed: 15056187]


81. Dosenbach NU, Nardos B, Cohen AL, Fair DA, Power JD, Church JA, et al. Prediction of
individual brain maturity using fMRI. Science. 2010;329(5997):1358–61. [PubMed: 20829489]
82. Greene DJ, Roller JM, Robichaux-Viehoever A, Bihun EC, Schlaggar BL, Black KJ. Reward
enhances tic suppression in children within months of tic disorder onset. Dev Cogn Neurosci.
2015;11:65–74. [PubMed: 25220075]
83. Martino D, Leckman JF. Tourette Syndrome. New York: Oxford University Press; 2013.
84. Leckman JF, Cohen DJ. Tourette’s syndrome -- tics, obsessions, compulsions: Developmental
psychopathology and clinical care. New York (US): John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; 1999.
85. Freyer T, Valerius G, Kuelz AK, Speck O, Glauche V, Hull M, et al. Test-retest reliability of event-
related functional MRI in a probabilistic reversal learning task. Psychiatry Res. 2009;174(l):40–6.
[PubMed: 19783412]
86. Constantino JN, Davis SA, Todd RD, Schindler MR, Gross MM, Brophy SL, et al. Validation of a
brief quantitative measure of autistic traits: Comparison of the Social Responsiveness Scale with
Author Manuscript

the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised. J Autism Dev Disord. 2003;33(4):427–33. [PubMed:


12959421]
87. Varni JW, Seid M, Rode CA. The PedsQL: measurement model for the pediatric quality of life
inventory. Med Care. 1999;37(2):126–39. [PubMed: 10024117]
88. Leckman JF, Riddle MA, Hardin MT, Ort SI, Swartz RL, Stevenson J, et al. The Yale Global Tic
Severity Scale: initial testing of a clinician-rated scale of tic severity. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
Psychiatry. 1989;28(4):566–73. [PubMed: 2768151]

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 25

89. Scahill L, Riddle MA, McSwiggin-Hardin M, Ort SI, Ring RA, Goodman WR, et al. Children’s
Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale: reliability and validity. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
Author Manuscript

Psychiatry. 1997;36(6):844–52. [PubMed: 9183141]


90. Conners CR, Sitarenios G, Parker JD, Epstein JN. The revised Conners’ Parent Rating Scale
(CPRS-R): factor structure, reliability, and criterion validity. J Abnorm Child Psychol.
1998;26(4):257–68. [PubMed: 9700518]
91. Robertson MM, Banerjee S, Rurlan R, Cohen DJ, Leckman JF, McMahon W, et al. The Tourette
syndrome diagnostic confidence index: development and clinical associations. Neurology.
1999;53(9):2108–12. [PubMed: 10599790]
92. Woods DW, Piacentini J, Himle MB, Chang S. Premonitory Urge for Tics Scale (PUTS): initial
psychometric results and examination of the premonitory urge phenomenon in youths with tic
disorders. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2005;26(6):397–403. [PubMed: 16344654]
93. Goetz CG, Tanner CM, Wilson RS, Shannon RM. A rating scale for Gilles de la Tourette’s
syndrome: description, reliability, and validity data. Neurology. 1987;37(9):1542–4. [PubMed:
3476860]
94. Goetz CG, Pappert EJ, Louis ED, Raman R, Leurgans S. Advantages of a modified scoring method
Author Manuscript

for the Rush Video-Based Tic Rating Scale. Mov Disord. 1999;14(3):502–6. [PubMed: 10348478]
95. Goetz CG, Leurgans S, Chmura TA. Home alone: Methods to maximize tic expression for
objective videotape assessments in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Mov Disord. 2001;16(4):693–
7. [PubMed: 11481693]
96. Barnea-Goraly N, Weinzimer SA, Ruedy KJ, Mauras N, Beck RW, Marzelli MJ, et al. High
success rates of sedation-free brain MRI scanning in young children using simple subject
preparation protocols with and without a commercial mock scanner--the Diabetes Research in
Children Network (DirecNet) experience. Pediatr Radiol. 2014;44(2):181–6. [PubMed: 24096802]
97. Dosenbach NUF, Kolier JM, Earl EA, Miranda-Dominguez O, Klein RL, Van AN, et al. Real-time
motion analytics during brain MRI improve data quality and reduce costs. Neuroimage.
2017;161:80–93. [PubMed: 28803940]
98. Tisdall MD, Reuter M, Qureshi A, Buckner RL, Fischl B, van der Kouwe AJW. Prospective motion
correction with volumetric navigators (vNavs) reduces the bias and variance in brain morphometry
induced by subject motion. Neuroimage. 2016;127:11–22. [PubMed: 26654788]
99. Wang J, Licht DJ, Jahng GH, Liu CS, Rubin JT, Haselgrove J, et al. Pediatric perfusion imaging
Author Manuscript

using pulsed arterial spin labeling. J Magn Reson Imaging. 2003;18(4):404–13. [PubMed:
14508776]
100. Wang L, Lee DY, Bailey E, Hartlein JM, Gado MH, Miller MI, et al. Validity of large-
deformation high dimensional brain mapping of the basal ganglia in adults with Tourette
syndrome. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2007;154(2):181–90.
101. Perantie DC, Wu J, Koller JM, Lim A, Warren SL, Black KJ, et al. Regional brain volume
differences associated with hyperglycemia and severe hypoglycemia in youth with type 1
diabetes. Diabetes Care. 2007;30(9):2331–7. [PubMed: 17575089]
102. Hershey T, Perantie DC, Wu J, Weaver PM, Black KJ, White NH. Hippocampal volumes in youth
with type 1 diabetes. Diabetes. 2010;59(1):236–41. [PubMed: 19833895]
103. Perantie DC, Koller JM, Weaver PM, Lugar HM, Black KJ, White NH, et al. Prospectively
determined impact of type 1 diabetes on brain volume during development. Diabetes.
2011;60(11):3006–14. [PubMed: 21953611]
104. Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Raichle ME. Intrinsic fluctuations within cortical systems
Author Manuscript

account for intertrial variability in human behavior. Neuron. 2007;56:171–84. [PubMed:


17920023]
105. Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME. The human brain is
intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S
A. 2005;102(27):9673–8. [PubMed: 15976020]
106. Fair DA, Dosenbach NU, Church JA, Cohen AL, Brahmbhatt S, Miezin FM, et al. Development
of distinct control networks through segregation and integration. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A.
2007;104(33):13507–12. [PubMed: 17679691]

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 26

107. Van Dijk KR, Sabuncu MR, Buckner RL. The influence of head motion on intrinsic functional
connectivity MRI. Neuroimage. 2012;59(1):431–8. [PubMed: 21810475]
Author Manuscript

108. Dosenbach NU, Fair DA, Miezin FM, Cohen AL, Wenger KK, Dosenbach RA, et al. Distinct
brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A.
2007;104(26):11073–8. [PubMed: 17576922]
109. Field AP. Meta-analysis of correlation coefficients: a Monte Carlo comparison of fixed- and
random-effects methods. Psychol Methods. 2001;6(2):161–80. [PubMed: 11411440]
110. Salvador R, Suckling J, Schwarzbauer C, Bullmore E. Undirected graphs of frequency-dependent
functional connectivity in whole brain networks. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci.
2005;360(1457):937–46. [PubMed: 16087438]
111. Karrer B, Levina E, Newman ME. Robustness of community structure in networks. Phys Rev E
Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys. 2008;77(4 Pt 2):046119. [PubMed: 18517702]
112. Honey C, Kotter R, Breakspear M, Sporns O. Network structure of cerebral cortex shapes
functional connectivity on multiple time scales. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104(24):10240–
5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0701519104 [PubMed: 17548818]
113. Fair DA, Cohen AL, Power JD, Dosenbach NU, Church JA, Miezin FM, et al. Functional brain
Author Manuscript

networks develop from a “local to distributed” organization. PLoS Comput Biol.


2009;5(5):e1000381. [PubMed: 19412534]
114. Bender-deMoll S, McFarland DA. The art and science of dynamic network visualization. J Soc
Struct. 2006;7(2).
115. Dosenbach NUF, Fair DA, Cohen AL, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. A dualnetworks architecture of
top-down control. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008;12(3):99–105. [PubMed: 18262825]
116. Nielsen AN, Barch DM, Petersen SE, Schlaggar BL, Greene DJ. Machine learning with
neuroimaging: Evaluating its applications in psychiatry. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci
Neuroimaging. 2019;S2451-9022(19)30304-0. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2019.11.007
117. Nielsen AJ, Gratton C, Church JA, Dosenbach NUF, Black KJ, Petersen SE, et al. Atypical
functional connectivity In Tourette Syndrome differs between children and adults. Biol
Psychiatry. 2020;87(2):164–73. [PubMed: 31472979]
118. Nielsen AN, Kim S, Gratton C, Church JA, Black KJ, Petersen SE, et al. Age-dependent
differences in functional brain networks are atypical in Tourette syndrome.
Author Manuscript

MedRxiv:2020:2020.04.06.20049817 [Preprint] 2020 4 07 Available from: https://


www.medrxiv.org/content/10.1101/2020.04.06.20049817v1.full.pdf+html. Accessed 2020 May
21.
119. Schölkopf B,J Smola A. Learning with kernels: support vector machines, regularization,
optimization and beyond. Cambridge, Massachusetts (US): MIT Press; 2002 1 1 p. 626.
120. Jäkel F, Scholkopf B, Wichmann F. Does Cognitive Science Need Kernels? Trends Cogn Sci.
2009;13(9):381–8. [PubMed: 19729333]
121. Ben-Hur A, Ong CS, Sonnenburg S, Scholkopf B, Ratsch G. Support vector machines and kernels
for computational biology. PLoS Comput Biol. 2008;4(10):e1000173. [PubMed: 18974822]
122. Naumovich Vapnik V Statistical learning theory. New York (US): John Wiley & Sons; 1998 1 1 p.
736.
123. Weston J, Elisseefi A, Bakir GH, Sinz FH. The Spider machine learning toolbox 2005 Available
from: https://web.archive.org/web/20200413222733/https://people.kyb.tuebingen.mpg.de/spider/
mainframe.html. Accessed 2020 May 27.
124. Kim S, Greene DJ, Bihun EC, Roller JM, Hampton JM, Acevedo H, et al. Provisional Tic
Author Manuscript

Disorder is not so transient. Sci Rep. 2019;9(1):3951. [PubMed: 30850688]


125. Black KJ. DSM-5 misses an edge case in tic disorders nosology: Case report. F1000Res. 2020
Unpubbshed work.
126. Yerys BE, Jankowski KF, Shook D, Rosenberger LR, Barnes KA, Berl MM, et al. The fMRI
success rate of children and adolescents: typical development, epilepsy, attention deficit/
hyperactivity disorder, and autism spectrum disorders. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30(10):3426–35.
[PubMed: 19384887]

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 27

127. Conelea CA, Wellen B, Woods DW, Greene DJ, Black KJ, Specht M, et al. Patterns and
Predictors of Tic Suppressibility in Youth With Tic Disorders. Front Psychiatry. 2018;9:188.
Author Manuscript

[PubMed: 29875706]
128. Kim S, Greene DJ, Robichaux-Viehoever A, Bihun EC, Roller JM, Acevedo H, et al. Tic
suppression in children with recent-onset tics predicts 1-year tic outcome. J Child Neurol.
2019;34(12):757–64. [PubMed: 31241402]
129. Kim S, Greene DJ, Reiersen AM, Robichaux-Viehoever A, Bihun EC, Roller JM, et al. Exploring
Behavioral Markers Predicting One-Year Tic Outcome in Children with Recent-Onset Tics. J Am
Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2019;58(10):S188–S.
130. Kim S, Greene DJ, Badke D’Andrea C, Bihun EC, Roller JM, O’Reilly B, et al. Hippocampal
volume in Provisional Tic Disorder predicts tic severity at 12-month follow-up.
bioRxiv2020:2020.02.05.935908 [Preprint], 2020 2 7 Available from: https://www.biorxiv.org/
content/10.1101/2020.02.05.935908v1.full Accessed 2020 May 21.
131. Peterson BS, Choi HA, Hao X, Amat JA, Zhu H, Whiteman R, et al. Morphologic features of the
amygdala and hippocampus in children and adults with Tourette syndrome. Arch Gen Psychiatry.
2007;64(11):1281–91. [PubMed: 17984397]
Author Manuscript

132. Schapiro AC, Reid AG, Morgan A, Manoach DS, Verfaellie M, Stickgold R. The hippocampus is
necessary for the consolidation of a task that does not require the hippocampus for initial
learning. Hippocampus. 2019;29(11):1091–100. [PubMed: 31157946]
133. Kim S, Jackson SR, Groom M, Jackson GM. Visuomotor learning and unlearning in children and
adolescents with tourette syndrome. Cortex. 2018;109:50–9. [PubMed: 30292925]
134. Black JK, Roller JM, Black KJ. TicTimer software for measuring tic suppression. F1000Res.
2017;6:1560. [PubMed: 29375812]
135. Vachon MJ, Striley CW, Gordon MR, Schroeder ML, Bihun EC, Roller JM, et al. V1SIT-TS: A
multimedia tool for population studies on tic disorders. F1000Res. 2016;5:1518. [PubMed:
27853509]
136. Csernansky JG, Wang L, Jones D, Rastogi-Cruz D, Posener JA, Heydebrand G, et al.
Hippocampal deformities in schizophrenia characterized by high dimensional brain mapping. Am
J Psychiatry. 2002;159(12):2000–6. [PubMed: 12450948]
137. Khan AR, Wang L, Beg MF. FreeSurfer-initiated fully-automated subcortical brain segmentation
in MRI using Large Deformation Diffeomorphic Metric Mapping. Neuroimage. 2008;41(3):735–
Author Manuscript

46. [PubMed: 18455931]


138. Bennett SM, Capriotti M, Bauer C, Chang S, Keller AE, Walkup J, et al. Development and open
trial of a psychosocial intervention for young children with chronic tics: The CBIT-JR Study.
Behav Ther. 10.1016/j.beth.2019.10.004
139. Verdellen CW, Keijsers GP, Cath DC, Hoogduin CA. Exposure with response prevention versus
habit reversal in Tourettes’s syndrome: a controlled study. Behav Res Ther. 2004;42(5):501–11.
[PubMed: 15033497]
140. Black JK, Black KJ. Software for web-based tic suppression training. F1000Res. 2017;6:2150.
[PubMed: 30228859]
Author Manuscript

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 28
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript

Figure 1. Overall study design.


Children are examined thoroughly at a baseline visit no more than 9 months after their first
tic (median 3.5 months). The follow-up visit occurs as close as possible to the one-year
anniversary of their first tic, when DSM-5 Tourette’s Disorder can first be diagnosed.
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 29
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript

Figure 2. Tic duration at study entry and follow-up visits.


Author Manuscript

The horizontal axis represents days since first tic. Individual participants are shown as
horizontal lines, with their first study visit marked by an open circle and their 12-month
follow-up visit marked by a filled circle. The solid red line marks the current mean duration
at the baseline study visit, the dashed red line marks the original 6-month cutoff for
enrollment, the thick black line marks the current 9-month cutoff, and the thin black vertical
line marks 1 year after the first tic.

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 30

Table 1.

Participant characteristics at baseline among those who completed a 12-month visit. Values indicate number or
Author Manuscript

mean ± SD unless indicated otherwise.

Sample characteristic Baseline


N 65
Age 7.81 ± 2.03
Sex (M:F) 44:21
Handedness (R:not R) 57:8
Non-white (N = 63) 5

Barratt SES 1
52.03 ± 10.52
IQ estimate (K-BIT) 108.94 ± 12.31
Non-tic K-SADS diagnosis 53
ADHD 23
Author Manuscript

OCD 6
Anxiety disorder 33

Medication 2
12
SRS total T scores 49.72 ± 9.48

1
Highest possible score 66.
2
Includes only daily or more frequent brain-active medications.
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.
Black et al. Page 31

Table 2.

Change in tic characteristics among those who completed a 12-month visit. Values in parentheses indicate
Author Manuscript

mean ± SD unless indicated otherwise.

Sample characteristic Baseline 12-month


Days since tic onset 112.28 ± 56.44 374.17 ± 16.51
YGTSS total 24.72 ± 12.33 17.92 ± 12.83
Total tic score 17.06 ± 5.89 13.62 ± 7.39
Motor 10.55 ± 3.89 8.91 ± 4.57
Phonic 6.49 ± 4.50 4.71 ± 4.19
Impairment 7.66 ± 8.00 4.31 ± 6.84
DCI score 32.63 ± 13.01 43.14 ± 15.36
Ever had a phonic tic 53 56
Ever had a complex tic 30 41
Author Manuscript

DSM-IV TS or CTD 0 21

DSM-IV-TR TS or CTD 0 1
58

DSM-5 TS or CTD 0 2
64

1
The other 7 children had a reported interim remission of 3 months or more, for a diagnosis of a repeated episode of Transient Tic Disorder. See
discussion in ref. [124].
2
The remaining child had experienced several phonic tics but only one motor tic [125].
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript

J Psychiatr Brain Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 June 25.

You might also like