Cognitive Aging, A Primer
Cognitive Aging, A Primer
RESEARCH
November 2016, Number 16-17
Introduction
Cognitive aging has received growing attention in will generally need more time to learn new skills or
recent years as many researchers have documented a concepts; and 2) many retirees can continue to man-
significant age-related decline in the brain’s process- age their own financial affairs in their 70s and 80s,
ing ability. This decline could potentially undermine though about one quarter will likely develop a cogni-
retirement security in two ways: 1) by limiting the tive impairment that will pose a threat to their finan-
ability to work longer; and 2) by eroding the capacity to cial independence.
manage finances in retirement.
This brief summarizes the explosion of recent
research on cognitive aging by answering basic ques- Defining and Measuring
tions about what researchers are learning and why
their findings matter to retirement experts and the
Cognitive Ability
public. This overview is the first brief in a series of
A basic definition of cognition – “the act or process of
three; the other two will focus on how cognitive aging
knowing” – points out two aspects of cognitive ability:
affects the ability of individuals to work between ages
1) having knowledge; and 2) acquiring knowledge.1
50-70 and to handle personal finances between ages
Psychologists commonly refer to knowledge as “crys-
70-90.
tallized intelligence” and the ability to process new in-
The discussion proceeds as follows. The first sec-
formation as “fluid intelligence.” Figure 1 (on the next
tion introduces definitions and measures of cognitive
page) illustrates how overall cognitive ability depends
ability. The second section discusses how researchers
on these two aspects of cognition and provides some
identify changes in cognitive ability with age, while the
examples.
third summarizes their findings. The fourth section
Most real-world activity relies on both crystal-
discusses how age-related changes in different cogni-
lized and fluid intelligence. Some researchers have
tive capacities can affect real-world performance. The
attempted to measure performance on everyday
final section concludes that: 1) most older workers can
tasks directly. For example, driving performance
maintain their productivity up to age 70, although they
* Anek Belbase is a research fellow at the Center for Retirement Research at Boston College (CRR). Geoffrey T. Sanzen-
bacher is a research economist at the CRR.
2 Center for Retirement Research
educational attainment. In longitudinal studies, one This loss in neural matter follows a pattern – from the
can figure out how much, on average, practice boosts front to the back of the brain and from the outer parts
test scores, and adjust the scores of people who take of the brain to the inner parts of the brain.7 Figure 3
the test often to reflect the boost they received from shows these patterns in a brain scan, with the older
this practice. Figures 2a and 2b show the value of brains having lower concentrations of neurotransmit-
these adjustments. ter receptors in the front of the brain, as indicated by
low amounts of red and green shading.
-10
-20
Cross-sectional data (unadjusted) 24 years 44 years 86 years
Longitudinal data (unadjusted)
-30 Note: These images are PET scans from three subjects,
35 40 45 50 55 60 65 70 75 80 85 showing the concentration of dopamine receptors in the
Age brain. Red indicates highest concentrations, dark spots
indicate lowest.
b. Adjusted Source: Brookhaven National Laboratory (1998).
10
Estimated memory change (T score)
Of course, within any age group, the level of and 3) the capacity of the brain to acquire completely
cognitive ability varies, as does the degree of cognitive new functions, such as learning how to play a musical
change. Figure 4 illustrates these variations using a instrument for the first time, is severely diminished
test of cognitive function that incorporates both fluid due to changes in brain structure.
and crystallized intelligence. The test was adminis- Since different cognitive capacities decline at dif-
tered several times over a 7-year period to a sample of ferent rates, the effect of cognitive aging on real-world
3,075 individuals who were ages 70-79 at the start of performance depends on the situation. In situations
the study. In addition to showing variation in the level where an individual has direct experience and exten-
of cognitive score at age 70, the figure identifies three sive practice – like paying bills or speaking to a cus-
distinct groups who differ in the changes they experi- tomer service representative – age generally improves
ence over time. The “maintainers” have a higher ini- performance through one’s 50s and 60s, with little
tial score and show no decline over the 7-year period, decline thereafter; performance speed, however, could
while the other two groups have lower initial scores decrease, especially in one’s 70s and 80s. In situa-
and show minor or major declines. tions where an individual has related experience, but
reactions are not automatic, like driving on unfamiliar
streets, age generally leads to decreased performance
starting in one’s 60s. Declines in performance are
Figure 4. Cognitive Scores Over 7-Year Period for
especially significant when a task is complex, such
Individuals Initially Ages 70-79
as driving on unfamiliar streets on a rainy night. In
100 situations where an individual has little or no relevant
experience, like learning a new language, age often
results in significant declines in the quality and speed
of performance starting as early as childhood.
Cognitive score
90
80
Conclusion
Maintainers
Minor decliners Reviewing the ways in which cognitive aging could
Major decliners affect real-world performance provides useful insights
70 for anyone concerned with the well-being of retirees,
Y1 Y3 Y5 Y8
from policymakers to financial planners to individuals
Testing year
themselves.
Source: Reproduced from Yaffe et al. (2009). One way that cognitive change could affect retire-
ment security is by limiting the ability to work. In
this respect, the wide range of cognitive skills that
older workers build and maintain as they age implies
How Change in Ability Affects that most workers can remain productive in their
Real-World Performance 50s and 60s despite cognitive aging. This finding is
supported by numerous studies that show insignifi-
cant age-related losses in productivity across a variety
The research clearly shows that fluid intelligence de-
of occupations.11 However, declines in processing
clines with age while crystallized intelligence accumu-
capacity and speed can affect the performance of
lates, and that the brain changes in both structure and
some workers engaged in complex tasks under time
activity. These changes together predict the follow-
pressure. For example, the performance of air-
ing three patterns: 1) the brain acquires the ability to
traffic controllers, who must track the flight paths of
react intelligently and automatically to a wider range
multiple airplanes at once while also quickly react-
of situations with age due to increased crystallized
ing to unusual situations, declines significantly by
intelligence; 2) the maximum capacity and speed for
age 56.12 And diminished ability to adapt, or acquire
processing information at any given moment dimin-
completely new skills, implies that older workers will
ishes with age, largely due to declining fluid abilities;
require more time to adjust to occupational changes,
Issue in Brief 5
10 Ackerman (2000).
13 Salthouse (2012).
References
Ackerman, Phillip L. 2000. “Domain-Specific Knowledge Ivanoff, Jason, Philip Branning, and René Marois.
as the ‘Dark Matter’ of Adult Intelligence: Gf/Gc, 2009. “Mapping the Pathways of Information
Personality and Interest Correlates.” The Journals of Processing from Sensation to Action in Four Dis-
Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social tinct Sensorimotor Tasks.” Human Brain Mapping
Sciences 55(2): 69-84. 30(12): 4167-4186.
Agarwal, Sumit, John C. Driscoll, Xavier Gabaix, and Jeske, Debora and Christian Stamov Rossnagel. 2015.
David Laibson. 2009. “The Age of Reason: Finan- “Learning Capability and Performance in Later
cial Decisions Over the Life-Cycle with Implica- Working Life: Towards a Contextual View.” Educa-
tions for Regulation.” Brookings Papers on Econom- tion and Training 57(4): 378-391.
ic Activity 2009(2) 51-117.
Langenecker, Scott A., Kristy A. Nielson, and Stephen M.
Andrews, Elizabeth C. and Stephen J. Westerman. Rao. 2004. “fMRI of Healthy Older Adults During
2012. “Age Differences in Simulated Driving Stroop Interference.” Neuroimage 21(1): 192-200.
Performance: Compensatory Processes.” Accident
Analysis & Prevention 45: 660-668. Merriam-Webster’s Collegiate Dictionary. 2010. Spring-
field, MA: Merriam-Webster, Inc.
Brookhaven National Laboratory. 1998. “New Study
Connects Brain Chemistry to Old Age’s Most Mund, Iris, Raoul Bell, and Axel Buchner. 2010. “Age
Common Symptoms: Decreases in Motor Skills Differences in Reading with Distraction: Sensory
and Mental Agility Could be Preventable.” Upton, or Inhibitory Deficits?” Psychology and Aging 25(4):
NY. Available at: https://www.bnl.gov/bnlweb/ 886-897.
pubaf/pr/1998/bnlpr030298a.html.
Ng, Thomas W. H., and Daniel C. Feldman. 2013.
Casey, B. J., Nim Tottenham, Conor Liston, and Sarah “How Do Within-Person Changes Due to Aging
Durston. 2005. “Imaging the Developing Brain: What Affect Job Performance?” Journal of Vocational
Have We Learned About Cognitive Development?” Behavior 83(3) 500-513.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9(3): 104-110.
Nyberg, Lars, Martin Lövdén, Katrine Riklund, Ulman
Craik, Fergus and Timothy Salthouse. 2008. The Lindenberger, and Lars Bäckman. 2012. “Memory
Handbook of Aging and Cognition: Third Edition. Aging and Brain Maintenance.” Trends in Cognitive
New York, NY: Psychology Press. Sciences 16(5): 292-305.
DeCarli, Charles, D. M. Murphy, M. Tranh, C. L. Pantoni, Leonardo and Julio H. Garcia. 1997. “Cogni-
Grady, J. V. Haxby, J. A. Gillette, J. A. Salerno et al. tive Impairment and Cellular/Vascular Changes in
1995. “The Effect of White Matter Hyperintensity the Cerebral White Matter.” Annals of the New York
Volume on Brain Structure, Cognitive Perfor- Academy of Sciences 826(1): 92-102.
mance, and Cerebral Metabolism of Glucose in 51
Healthy Adults.” Neurology 45(11): 2077-2084. Salat, David H. 2011. “The Declining Infrastructure of
the Aging Brain.” Brain Connectivity 1(4): 279-293.
Heil, Michael C. 1999. “Air Traffic Control Specialist
Age and Cognitive Test Performance.” Report to Salthouse, Timothy A. 2012. “Consequences of Age-
the U.S. Department of Transportation Federal Avia- Related Cognitive Declines.” Annual Review of
tion Administration. Psychology 63: 201-226.
Issue in Brief 7
© 2016, by Trustees of Boston College, Center for Retirement Research. All rights reserved. Short sections of text, not to
exceed two paragraphs, may be quoted without explicit permission provided that the authors are identified and full credit,
including copyright notice, is given to Trustees of Boston College, Center for Retirement Research.
The research reported herein was performed pursuant to a grant from the U.S. Social Security Administration (SSA) funded
as part of the Retirement Research Consortium. The opinions and conclusions expressed are solely those of the authors and
do not represent the opinions or policy of SSA or any agency of the federal government. Neither the United States Govern-
ment nor any agency thereof, nor any of their employees, makes any warranty, express or implied, or assumes any legal
liability or responsibility for the accuracy, completeness, or usefulness of the contents of this report. Reference herein to any
specific commercial product, process or service by trade name, trademark, manufacturer, or otherwise does not necessarily
constitute or imply endorsement, recommendation or favoring by the United States Government or any agency thereof.