Predictors and Parameters of Resilience To Loss: Toward An Individual Differences Model
Predictors and Parameters of Resilience To Loss: Toward An Individual Differences Model
It’s evident
the art of losing’s not too hard to master
though it may look like (Write it!) like disaster.
—Elizabeth Bishop
The research described in this article was supported by National Institutes of Health
Grants R29-MH57274 (to George A. Bonanno).
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Anthony D. Man-
cini, Department of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, Teachers College, Columbia
University, 525 West 120th Street, Box 102, New York, NY 10027. E-mail: adm22
@columbia.edu.
Indeed, the absence of distress after loss has in itself been considered
pathological and a likely harbinger of future difficulties (Middleton,
Moylan, Raphael, Burnett, & Martinek, 1993). Persons who fail to
display the expected distress reaction were considered to be suppress-
ing their grief (Middleton et al., 1993) or to lack an attachment to
their spouse (Fraley & Shaver, 1999). Alternately, healthy adjustment
to loss was often ascribed to exceptional strength and thus thought to
be relatively rare. In stark contrast to these extreme views, a growing
body of research now demonstrates that most bereaved persons dis-
play stable, healthy levels of psychological and physical functioning
as well as the capacity for generative experiences and positive emo-
tions even relatively soon after a loss (Bonanno, 2004).
A large number of contextual and situational factors potentially con-
tribute to the likelihood of a resilient outcome (Bonanno & Mancini,
2008). These include characteristics of the loss and the person’s envi-
ronment. However, our primary concern in this article is person-cen-
tered factors or individual differences. Because of the dearth of
prospective studies of bereavement, our understanding of these factors
is still evolving. Nevertheless, convergent evidence has now identified a
number of individual difference variables that are associated with a re-
silient trajectory. These include self-enhancing biases, attachment style,
repressive coping, a priori beliefs, identity continuity and complexity,
and positive emotions (Bonanno, Field, Kovacevic, & Kaltman, 2002;
Fraley & Bonanno, 2004; Keltner & Bonanno, 1997). These factors may
interact with one another and with environmental factors in complex
ways that we are only beginning to understand. Surprisingly, some fac-
tors that promote resilience to loss may be maladaptive in other con-
texts, whereas other factors are more broadly adaptive. In this paper, we
first provide an operational definition of resilience and describe how it
differs from other trajectories of response to loss. We describe evidence
for its prevalence and for the heterogeneous individual differences as-
sociated with resilience. We then integrate these individual differences
into a proposed model of resilience, considering shared mechanisms re-
lated to appraisal processes, coping strategies, and the use of exogenous
resources, such as social support.
Chronic Grief
Recovery
Resilience
Figure 1
Prototypical patterns of disruption in normal functioning across time
after bereavement. Reproduced with permission from Bonanno, G. A.
(2004). Loss, trauma, and human resilience: Have we underestimated
the human capacity to thrive after extremely aversive events? Amer-
ican Psychologist, 59, 20–28.
1808 Mancini & Bonanno
Repressive Coping
Attachment Dynamics
Self-Enhancing Biases
Views of the self are another factor that appear to influence coping
and are associated with resilience to loss (Mancini & Bonanno,
2006). The emotional upheaval surrounding the loss of a loved one
may be particularly damaging to a person’s underlying sense of
identity. Familiar routines or rituals are often disrupted, and social
roles may be dramatically changed. Persons with the most serious
forms of grief sometimes report that they feel as if a piece of them is
missing, as if the self is incomplete (Shuchter & Zisook, 1993). Sim-
ilarly, traumatized individuals commonly experience the self as dam-
aged or inferior (Brewin, 2003). By contrast, resilient individuals
appear to experience minimal change in their sense of themselves
(Bonanno, Papa, & O’Neill, 2001). We have argued that resilient
persons seem to experience an underlying continuity in the self and,
armed with that continuity, are better able to respond flexibly to the
demands of a changed world (Mancini & Bonanno, 2006).
This perspective found clear support in a recent preliminary lon-
gitudinal study in which bereaved individuals and nonbereaved con-
trols participated in a collaborative task designed to elicit the
identity traits that they use to define themselves (Galatzer-Levy,
Bonanno, & Mancini, 2009). Participants completed the task with a
researcher, who helped them list and clarify their traits (e.g., ‘‘kind’’
or ‘‘thoughtful’’). Once the list was finalized, the researcher then
asked the participant to indicate the extent that each trait had
changed since the loss. Resilient individuals reported a relatively low
level of identity change (i.e., high identity continuity) at both 4 and
18 months postloss and did not differ from the degree of identity
change reported by nonbereaved participants. By contrast, symp-
tomatic bereaved persons experienced significantly greater identity
change. Interestingly, the number of traits a person reported, which
we defined as identity complexity, was not associated with resilience.
1818 Mancini & Bonanno
Positive Emotions
of means. There are multiple risk and protective factors across in-
dividuals, and it is the totality of these factors, rather than any one
primary factor, that determines the likelihood of a resilient outcome
(e.g., Bonanno, Galea, Bucciarelli, & Vlahov, 2007). Nonetheless,
our understanding of resilient processes can be advanced by inte-
grating and organizing these factors along the lines of common or
shared mechanisms. These shared mechanisms are graphically illus-
trated in a hypothesized model of resilience (Figure 2). As can be
seen from this diagram, we propose that individual differences have
both direct and indirect effects on coping with loss. The indirect
effects are channeled through at least two mechanisms of resilience—
appraisal processes and social support—each of which can play a
salutary role in promoting more effective coping. Next we briefly
review evidence to support the critical role of appraisal processes and
the use of social resources as shared pathways or mechanisms for
resilience.
Appraisal Processes
Figure 2
Hypothesized model of resilience.
ment was not only beneficial for coping with ordinary bereavement
but also exerted an additional buffering effect on the negative con-
sequences of traumatic deaths. Caregiving strain, by contrast, ap-
pears not to be related to resilience per se, but it does predict an
unusual trajectory of improved functioning following loss (Bonanno,
Wortman, et al. 2002). Additional evidence for the crucial role of
contextual factors was found in a recent analysis of a large panel
data set. Using a latent class framework, we not only validated the
resilient trajectory empirically but we also found that one of the
factors associated with resilience was less reduction in income fol-
lowing loss (Mancini & Bonanno, 2008b).
Another question raised by the present review is whether the pro-
cesses that lead to resilience to loss, a stressor most people are ex-
posed to, are qualitatively different from resilience to other more
extreme and violent stressors, such as combat or physical or sexual
assault. Extant research suggests that a number of the factors asso-
ciated with resilience to loss also serve a protective function against
these other more extreme stressors. These include self-enhancing bi-
ases (Bonanno, Field, et al., 2002; Bonanno, Rennicke, et al., 2005),
repressive coping (Bonanno, Noll, Putnam, O’Neill, & Trickett,
2003), and positive emotions (Fredrickson, Tugade, Waugh, & Lar-
kin, 2003). Nevertheless, it would appear likely that resilience to loss
and to more extreme and violent stressors would have both common
and unique predictors. However, research has yet to elucidate the
distinctions between these types of resilience.
Although this paper is concerned with individual differences in
resilience, a final important question is the role of ethnic and cultural
variations in resilience during bereavement. Western, independence-
oriented countries tend to focus more heavily than collectivist coun-
tries on the personal experience of grief. What are the implications of
such cultural beliefs about the self for the study of resilience? Un-
fortunately, research on the extent to which bereavement reactions
might vary across cultures is still nascent. Preliminary evidence sug-
gests that bereaved people in China recover more quickly from loss
than do bereaved Americans (Bonanno, Papa, Lalande, Nanping, &
Noll, 2005). Moreover, for the Chinese, coping is enhanced by a
continuing psychological bond with the deceased, whereas the evi-
dence for the salutary role of a continuing bond among Americans is
less conclusive (Lalande & Bonanno, 2006). These findings suggest
that cultural beliefs may even influence the degree to which resilience
1826 Mancini & Bonanno
REFERENCES
Allport, G. W. (1937). Personality: A psychological interpretation. New York:
Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young
adults: A test of a four-category model. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 61, 226–244.
Bonanno, G. A. (2004). Loss, trauma, and human resilience: Have we underes-
timated the human capacity to thrive after extremely aversive events? American
Psychologist, 59, 20–28.
Bonanno, G. A. (2005). Resilience in the face of potential trauma. Current Di-
rections in Psychological Science, 14, 135–138.
Bonanno, G. A., Colak, D. M., Keltner, D., Shiota, M. N., Papa, A., Noll, J. G.,
et al. (2007). Context matters: The benefits and costs of expressing positive
emotion among survivors of childhood sexual abuse. Emotion, 7, 824–837.
Bonanno, G. A., Davis, P. J., Singer, J. L., & Schwartz, G. E. (1991). The re-
pressor personality and avoidant information processing: A dichotic listening
study. Journal of Research in Personality, 25, 386–401.
Bonanno, G. A., & Field, N. P. (2001). Examining the delayed grief hypothesis
across 5 years of bereavement. American Behavioral Scientist, 44, 798–816.
Bonanno, G. A., Field, N. P., Kovacevic, A., & Kaltman, S. (2002). Self-en-
hancement as a buffer against extreme adversity: Civil war in Bosnia and
traumatic loss in the United States. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
28, 184–196.
Bonanno, G. A., Galea, S., Bucciarelli, A., & Vlahov, D. (2007). What predicts
psychological resilience after disaster? The role of demographics, resources,
and life stress. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 671–682.
Bonanno, G. A., & Keltner, D. (1997). Facial expressions of emotion and the
course of conjugal bereavement. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 126–
137.
Bonanno, G. A., Keltner, D., Holen, A., & Horowitz, M. J. (1995). When avoid-
ing unpleasant emotions might not be such a bad thing: Verbal–autonomic
response dissociation and midlife conjugal bereavement. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 69, 975–989.
Bonanno, G. A., & Mancini, A. D. (2008). The human capacity to thrive in the
face of potential trauma. Pediatrics, 121, 369–375.
Bonanno, G. A., Moskowitz, J. T., Papa, A., & Folkman, S. (2005). Resilience to
loss in bereaved spouses, bereaved parents, and bereaved gay men. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 827–843.
Predictors and Parameters of Loss 1827
Bonanno, G. A., Noll, J. G., Putnam, F. W., O’Neill, M., & Trickett, P. K. (2003).
Predicting the willingness to disclose childhood sexual abuse from measures
of repressive coping and dissociative tendencies. Child Maltreatment, 8,
302–318.
Bonanno, G. A., Papa, A., Lalande, K., Nanping, Z., & Noll, J. G. (2005). Grief
processing and deliberate grief avoidance: A prospective comparison of be-
reaved spouses and parents in the United States and the People’s Republic of
China. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 86–98.
Bonanno, G. A., Papa, A., Lalande, K., Westphal, M., & Coifman, K. (2004). The
importance of being flexible: The ability to both enhance and suppress emo-
tional expression predicts long-term adjustment. Psychological Science, 15,
482–487.
Bonanno, G. A., Papa, A., & O’Neill, K. (2001). Loss and human resilience. Ap-
plied & Preventive Psychology, 10, 193–206.
Bonanno, G. A., Rennicke, C., & Dekel, S. (2005). Self-enhancement among high-
exposure survivors of the September 11th terrorist attack: Resilience or social
maladjustment? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 984–998.
Bonanno, G. A., & Singer, J. L. (1990). Repressor personality style: Theoretical
and methodological implications for health and pathology. In J. L. Singer
(Ed.), Repression and dissociation (pp. 435–470). Chicago: University of Chi-
cago Press.
Bonanno, G. A., Wortman, C. B., Lehman, D. R., Tweed, R. G., Haring, M.,
Sonnega, J., et al. (2002). Resilience to loss and chronic grief: A prospective
study from preloss to 18-months postloss. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 83, 1150–1164.
Bonanno, G. A., Wortman, C. B., & Nesse, R. M. (2004). Prospective patterns of
resilience and maladjustment during widowhood. Psychology and Aging, 19,
260–271.
Bowlby, J. (1980). Loss: Sadness and depression (Attachment and loss, Vol. 3).
New York: Basic Books.
Brewin, C. R. (2003). Posttraumatic stress disorder: Malady or myth? New Haven,
CT: Yale University Press.
Brewin, C. R., Andrews, B., & Valentine, J. D. (2000). Meta-analysis of risk fac-
tors for posttraumatic stress disorder in trauma-exposed adults. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 748–766.
Buchanan, T. W., al’Absi, M., & Lovallo, W. R. (1999). Cortisol fluctuates with
increases and decreases in negative affect. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 24, 227–
241.
Burke, C. T., Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2007). Individual differences in ad-
justment to spousal loss: A nonlinear mixed model analysis. International
Journal of Behavioral Development, 31, 405–415.
Carr, D., House, J. S., Wortman, C., Neese, R., & Kessler, R. C. (2001). Psy-
chological adjustment to sudden and anticipated spousal loss among older
widowed persons. Journals of Gerontology B: Psychological Sciences and Social
Sciences, 56, S237–S248.
Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. (Eds.). (1999). Handbook of attachment: Theory, re-
search, and clinical applications. New York: Guilford.
1828 Mancini & Bonanno
Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the buffering hy-
pothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 310–357.
Coifman, K. G., Bonanno, G. A., Ray, R. D., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Does repres-
sive coping promote resilience? Affective-autonomic response discrepancy dur-
ing bereavement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 745–758.
Epel, E. S., McEwen, B. S., & Ickovics, J. R. (1998). Embodying psychological
thriving: Physical thriving in response to stress. Journal of Social Issues, 54,
301–322.
Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and society (2nd ed.). New York: Norton.
Fraley, R., & Bonanno, G. A. (2004). Attachment and loss: A test of three com-
peting models on the association between attachment-related avoidance
and adaptation to bereavement. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
30, 878–890.
Fraley, R. C., Davis, K. E., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Dismissing-avoidance and the
defensive organization of emotion, cognition, and behavior. In J. A. Simpson
& W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 249–279).
New York: Guilford.
Fraley, R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (2000). Adult romantic attachment: Theoretical
developments, emerging controversies, and unanswered questions. Review of
General Psychology, 4, 132–154.
Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General
Psychology, 2, 300–319.
Fredrickson, B. L., & Losada, M. F. (2005). Positive affect and the complex dy-
namics of human flourishing. American Psychologist, 60, 678–686.
Fredrickson, B. L., Tugade, M. M., Waugh, C. E., & Larkin, G. R. (2003). What
good are positive emotions in crisis? A prospective study of resilience and
emotions following the terrorist attacks on the United States on September
11th, 2001. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 365–376.
Galatzer-Levy, I., Bonanno, G. A., & Mancini, A. D. (2009). Reexamining identity
continuity and complexity in adjustment to loss. Self and Identity, 8, 162–175.
Goorin, L., & Bonanno, G. A. (2008). Would you buy a used car from a self-
enhancer? Social benefits and illusions in trait self-enhancement. Manuscript
submitted for publication.
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation
processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of
Personality & Social Psychology, 85, 348–362.
Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1993). Emotional suppression: Physiology, self-
report, and expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
64, 970–986.
Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1997). Hiding feelings: The acute effects of in-
hibiting negative and positive emotion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106,
95–103.
Helgeson, V. S., & Taylor, S. E. (1993). Social comparisons and adjustment
among cardiac patients. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 23, 1171–1195.
Hock, M., & Krohne, H. W. (2004). Coping with threat and memory for ambig-
uous information: Testing the repressive discontinuity hypothesis. Emotion, 4,
65–86.
Predictors and Parameters of Loss 1829
Horowitz, M. J., Siegel, B., Holen, A., Bonanno, G. A., Milbrath, C., & Stinson,
C. H. (1997). Diagnostic criteria for complicated grief disorder. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 904–910.
Janoff-Bulman, R. (1992). Shattered assumptions: Towards a new psychology of
trauma. New York: Free Press.
Kaltman, S., & Bonanno, G. A. (2003). Trauma and bereavement: Examining the
impact of sudden and violent deaths. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 17, 131–
147.
Keltner, D. (1996). Facial expressions of emotion and personality. In C. Magai &
S. H. McFadden (Eds.), Handbook of emotion, adult development, and aging
(pp. 385–401). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Keltner, D., & Bonanno, G. A. (1997). A study of laughter and dissociation:
Distinct correlates of laughter and smiling during bereavement. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 687–702.
King, A. C., Taylor, C., Albright, C. A., & Haskell, W. L. (1990). The relationship
between repressive and defensive coping styles and blood pressure responses in
healthy, middle-aged men and women. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 34,
461–471.
Langens, T. A., & Moerth, S. (2003). Repressive coping and the use of passive
and active coping strategies. Personality and Individual Differences, 35,
461–473.
Lalande, K. M., & Bonanno, G. A. (2006). Culture and continuing bonds: A
prospective comparison of bereavement in the United States and the People’s
Republic of China. Death Studies, 30, 303–324.
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York:
Springer.
Lepore, S. J., Ragan, J. D., & Jones, S. (2000). Talking facilitates cognitive-emo-
tional processes of adaptation to an acute stressor. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 78, 499–508.
Lepore, S. J., Silver, R. C., Wortman, C. B., & Wayment, H. A. (1996). Social
constraints, intrusive thoughts, and depressive symptoms among bereaved
mothers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 271–282.
Malatesta, C. Z. (1990). The role of emotions in the development and organiza-
tion of personality. In R. A. Thompson (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Mo-
tivation (Vol. 36, pp. 1–56). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Mancini, A. D., & Bonanno, G. A. (2006). Resilience in the face of potential
trauma: Clinical practices and illustrations. Journal of Clinical Psychology: In
Session, 62, 971–985.
Mancini, A. D., & Bonanno, G. A. (2008a). The role of worldviews in grief reac-
tions: Longitudinal and prospective analyses. Manuscript submitted for publi-
cation.
Mancini, A. D., & Bonanno, G. A. (2008b). Stepping off the hedonic treadmill: A
latent class analysis of spousal bereavement. Unpublished manuscript.
Mancini, A. D., Pressman, D. L., & Bonanno, G. A. (2006). Clinical interventions
with the bereaved: hat clinicians and counselors can learn from the CLOC study.
In D. Carr, R. M. Nesse, & C. B. Wortman (Eds.), Late life widowhood: New
directions in theory, research, and practice (pp. 255–278). New York: Springer.
1830 Mancini & Bonanno
Mancini, A. D., Robinaugh, D., Shear, K., & Bonanno, G. A. (2009). Does at-
tachment avoidance help people cope with loss? The moderating effects of rela-
tionship quality. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65, 1–10.
Maslow, A. H. (1950). Self-actualizing people: A study of psychological health.
Personality: Symposium, 1, 11–34.
Middleton, W., Moylan, A., Raphael, B., Burnett, P., & Martinek, N. (1993). An
international perspective on bereavement related concepts. Australian and New
Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 27, 457–463.
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dy-
namics, and change. New York: Guilford.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., McBride, A., & Larson, J. (1997). Rumination and psy-
chological distress among bereaved partners. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 72, 855–862.
Olff, M., Langeland, W., & Gersons, B. P. R. (2005). Effects of appraisal and
coping on the neuroendocrine response to extreme stress. Neuroscience and
Biobehavioral Reviews, 29, 457–467.
Osterweis, M., Solomon, F., & Green, F. (1984). Bereavement: Reactions, conse-
quences, and care. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.
Ozer, E. J., Best, S. R., Lipsey, T. L., & Weiss, D. S. (2003). Predictors of post-
traumatic stress disorder and symptoms in adults: A meta-analysis. Psycho-
logical Bulletin, 129, 52–73.
Park, C. L., & Folkman, S. (1997). Meaning in the context of stress and coping.
Review of General Psychology, 1, 115–144.
Paulhus, D. L. (1998). Interpersonal and intrapsychic adaptiveness of trait self-
enhancement: A mixed blessing? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
74, 1197–1208.
Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Arndt, J., & Schimel, J. (2004). Why
do people need self-esteem? A theoretical and empirical review. Psychological
Bulletin, 130, 435–468.
Robins, R. W., & Beer, J. S. (2001). Positive illusions about the self: Short-term
benefits and long-term costs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80,
340–352.
Rubin, Z., & Peplau, L. A. (1975). Who believes in a just world? Journal of Social
Issues, 31, 65–89.
Schwartzberg, S. S., & Janoff-Bulman, R. (1991). Grief and the search for mean-
ing: Exploring the assumptive worlds of bereaved college students. Journal of
Social and Clinical Psychology, 10, 270–288.
Shuchter, S. R., & Zisook, S. (1993). The course of normal grief. In M. S. Stroebe,
W. Stroebe, & R. O. Hansson (Eds.), Handbook of bereavement: Theory, re-
search, and intervention (pp. 23–43). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Taylor, S. E., & Armor, D. A. (1996). Positive illusions and coping with adversity.
Journal of Personality, 64, 873–898.
Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social
psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103,
193–210.
Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1994). Positive illusions and well-being revisited:
Separating fact from fiction. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 21–27.
Predictors and Parameters of Loss 1831
Taylor, S. E., Lerner, J. S., Sherman, D. K., Sage, R. M., & McDowell, N. K.
(2003). Are self-enhancing cognitions associated with healthy or unhealthy bi-
ological profiles? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 605–615.
Taylor, S. E., Wood, J. V., & Lichtman, R. R. (1983). It could be worse: Selective
evaluation as a response to victimization. Journal of Social Issues, 39, 19–40.
Tomaka, J., & Blascovich, J. (1994). Effects of justice beliefs on cognitive ap-
praisal of and subjective physiological, and behavioral responses to potential
stress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 732–740.
Tomaka, J., Blascovich, J., & Kelsey, R. M. (1992). Effects of self-deception, so-
cial desirability, and repressive coping on psychophysiological reactivity to
stress. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 616–624.
Tomaka, J., Blascovich, J., Kelsey, R. M., & Leitten, C. L. (1993). Subjective,
physiological, and behavioral effects of threat and challenge appraisal. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 248–260.
Tomaka, J., Blascovich, J., Kibler, J., & Ernst, J. M. (1997). Cognitive and phys-
iological antecedents of threat and challenge appraisal. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 73, 63–72.
Tomarken, A. J., & Davidson, R. J. (1994). Frontal brain activation in repressors
and nonrepressors. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 339–349.
Tugade, M. M., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). Resilient individuals use positive
emotions to bounce back from negative emotional experiences. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 320–333.
Vaillant, G. (1977). Adaptation to life. Boston: Little Brown.
Weinberger, D. A., Schwartz, G. E., & Davidson, R. J. (1979). Low-anxious,
high-anxious, and repressive coping styles: Psychometric patterns and behav-
ioral and physiological responses to stress. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
88, 369–380.
Zisook, S., & Shuchter, S. R. (1993). Major depression associated with widow-
hood. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 1, 316–326.
1832