0% found this document useful (0 votes)
5 views7 pages

Buelow 2013

The document discusses a study that examined how math anxiety affects performance on working memory tasks from the WAIS-IV, specifically the Digit Span, Arithmetic, and Letter-Number Sequencing subtests. 172 undergraduate students completed measures of math anxiety, general test anxiety, and the three working memory subtests. Results showed that math anxiety predicted lower scores on the Arithmetic subtest, but not the Digit Span or Letter-Number Sequencing subtests, after accounting for gender, general anxiety, and math achievement.

Uploaded by

Samanta Farias
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
5 views7 pages

Buelow 2013

The document discusses a study that examined how math anxiety affects performance on working memory tasks from the WAIS-IV, specifically the Digit Span, Arithmetic, and Letter-Number Sequencing subtests. 172 undergraduate students completed measures of math anxiety, general test anxiety, and the three working memory subtests. Results showed that math anxiety predicted lower scores on the Arithmetic subtest, but not the Digit Span or Letter-Number Sequencing subtests, after accounting for gender, general anxiety, and math achievement.

Uploaded by

Samanta Farias
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 7

Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 28 (2013) 356–362

Math Anxiety Differentially Affects WAIS-IV Arithmetic Performance


in Undergraduates
Melissa T. Buelow 1,*, Laura L. Frakey2
1
Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University Newark, Newark, OH, USA
2
Department of Medical Rehabilitation, Memorial Hospital of Rhode Island, Warren Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Pawtucket, RI, USA
*Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University Newark, 1179 University Drive, Newark, OH 43055, USA.

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at Aston University on January 26, 2014


Tel.: +1-740-755-7808; fax: +1-740-366-5047.
E-mail address: [email protected] (M.T. Buelow).
Accepted 13 January 2013

Abstract
Previous research has shown that math anxiety can influence the math performance level; however, to date, it is unknown whether math
anxiety influences performance on working memory tasks during neuropsychological evaluation. In the present study, 172 undergraduate
students completed measures of math achievement (the Math Computation subtest from the Wide Range Achievement Test-IV), math
anxiety (the Math Anxiety Rating Scale-Revised), general test anxiety (from the Adult Manifest Anxiety Scale-College version), and the
three Working Memory Index tasks from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV Edition (WAIS-IV; Digit Span [DS], Arithmetic,
Letter-Number Sequencing [LNS]). Results indicated that math anxiety predicted performance on Arithmetic, but not DS or LNS, above
and beyond the effects of gender, general test anxiety, and math performance level. Our findings suggest that math anxiety can negatively
influence WAIS-IV working memory subtest scores. Implications for clinical practice include the utilization of LNS in individuals expressing
high math anxiety.

Keywords: Math anxiety; WAIS-IV; Working memory; Mental arithmetic

Introduction

Math anxiety can have a significant effect on math performance and achievement in school. It can be thought of as a type of
performance-based anxiety (Hopko, McNeil, Zvolensky, & Eifert, 2001) that includes both increased physiological arousal and
negative thoughts that can interfere with one’s attempts to solve basic and complex math problems (Ashcraft & Kirk, 2001;
Dew, Galassi, & Galassi, 1984; Levine, 1995; Richardson & Suinn, 1972).
High levels of math anxiety can have a significant and negative impact on the math performance level (Ashcraft & Faust,
1994; Ashcraft & Kirk, 2001; Ashcraft & Krause, 2007; Miller & Bichsel, 2004). Gender may affect this relationship, as
women report higher levels of math anxiety than men (Miller & Bichsel, 2004; Zettle & Raines, 2000). In a study of
gender and math anxiety, however, Hembree (1990) found that although women reported higher levels of math anxiety
than men, this did not negatively affect their math performance level. At the very least, the presence of math anxiety, independ-
ent of gender, can result in higher numbers of errors and a greater time to completion on math-related tasks (Ashcraft & Faust,
1994; Faust, Ashcraft, & Fleck, 1996; Hopko et al., 2003). The math anxiety level also has a more significant negative effect on
complex (“9 × 16”) versus simple (“2 + 4”) math problems (Ashcraft & Faust, 1994; Faust et al., 1996), but can also exert an
influence on simple object counting (Maloney, Risko, Ansari, & Fugelsang, 2010) and judging numerical distances (Maloney,
Ansari, & Fugelsang, 2011). Collectively these results indicate that the relationship between math anxiety and performance on
math-related tasks is complex and multifaceted, but it is likely that the level of working memory resources needed to complete
a complex versus a simple task may also impact the level of performance.

# The Author 2013. Published by Oxford University Press. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: [email protected].
doi:10.1093/arclin/act006 Advance Access publication on 3 February 2013
M. T. Buelow / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 28 (2013) 356–362 357

Math anxiety can also negatively affect other areas of cognitive and psychosocial functioning. For example, it exerts a nega-
tive effect on academic achievement—both in math (Ma, 1999; Zakaria & Nordin, 2008) and general scholastic aptitude (i.e.,
Scholastic Aptitude Test scores; Cassaday & Johnson, 2002), as well as on performance IQ tasks (Block Design and Picture
Arrangement from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III Edition [WAIS-III]; Hopko, Crittendon, Grant, & Wilson, 2005).
In addition, math anxiety negatively affects some measures of working memory, including visual paper-folding (Miller &
Bichsel, 2004), learning a visual sequence of letters (Ikeda, Iwanaga, & Seiwa, 1996), letter transformations (Eysenck,
1985), and verbal reasoning (Markham & Darke, 1991). Individuals with a high level of math anxiety tend to avoid upper-level,
more complex math classes, which can in turn affect the levels of math achievement (Maloney & Beilock, 2012).
It is possible that math anxiety, including its associated negative cognitions and ruminations, taxes the working memory
system (Ashcraft, 2002; Ashcraft & Moore, 2009; Eysenck & Calvo, 1992). Recent neuroimaging evidence indicates that
math anxiety increases activation in the right amygdala but decreases activity in the posterior parietal and dorsolateral prefront-
al cortices (Young, Wu, & Menon, 2012), areas also implicated in working memory (Hillary, 2008) and mathematical reason-
ing (Menon, Rivera, White, Glover, & Reiss, 2000). Math anxiety can impair working memory resources (Ashcraft & Kirk,
2001; Beilock, 2010) and thus may impair performance on measures of working memory ability.
The WAIS-IV (Wechsler, 2008) is a commonly used measure of adult intelligence. It assesses intelligence across four

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at Aston University on January 26, 2014


primary domains: verbal comprehension, perceptual reasoning, working memory, and processing speed. Factor analyses of
the WAIS-IV normative and secondary samples have supported this factor structure, with working memory having been
assessed by Digit Span (DS) and Arithmetic subtests (Canivez & Watkins, 2010; Holdnack, Zhou, Larrabee, Millis, &
Salthouse, 2011). In our clinical practice, we have seen a series of patients who perform in the Average range on the DS
subtest from the WAIS-IV Working Memory Index (WMI), but who then perform significantly below expectations on the
Arithmetic subtest. A significant portion of these patients also self-reported high levels of math anxiety, both during their
school-aged years and at present. These patients also completed the Letter-Number Sequencing (LNS) supplemental
subtest, in order to more fully assess working memory function, and performed in the Average range, suggesting that math
anxiety may have been negatively affecting their Arithmetic performance. The present study sought to examine relationships
between the math anxiety level and performance on the individual WAIS-IV WMI subtests. It was hypothesized that math
anxiety would negatively affect performance on the Arithmetic but not the DS or LNS subtests.

Materials and Methods

Participants and Procedure

The study was approved by the university’s Institutional Review Board. Participants were 172 undergraduate students (70
men), ages 18– 32 (mean age 19.10 [SD ¼ 1.81]) enrolled in psychology courses at a regional campus of a large university who
received course credit for participation in the study. Racial/ethnic status was as follows: 82.0% Caucasian, 5.2% African
American, 4.1% Asian/Pacific Islander, 1.7% Hispanic, and 7.0% Biracial/Other.
All participants provided written informed consent. Participants first completed a series of questionnaires, including the
Adult Manifest Anxiety Scale (AMAS)-College version (Reynolds, Richmond, & Lowe, 2003), Math Anxiety Rating
Scale-Revised (MARS-R; Plake & Parker, 1982), and State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) state subscale (Spielberger,
1983), as part of a larger study. Next, they completed the DS, Arithmetic, and LNS subtests from the WAIS-IV (Wechsler,
2008). At the end of the study, participants completed the Math Computation subtest from the Wide Range Achievement
Test-IV (WRAT-IV; Wilkinson & Robertson, 2006). All participants were then debriefed and course credit was assigned.
Partway through data collection, the order of the WAIS-IV tasks was changed from the typical administration order to a sec-
ondary order (DS, LNS, Arithmetic) to determine if math anxiety, which may have increased on the Arithmetic section, influ-
enced performance on LNS.

Measures

The MARS-R (Plake & Parker, 1982) is a 24-item revision of the original 98-item MARS (Richardson & Suinn, 1972).
Participants respond to items on a 5-point Likert-type scale, with responses ranging from 0 (not at all) to 4 (very much),
with higher summed scores indicating higher levels of math anxiety. The MARS-R correlates highly with the MARS (r ¼ .97;
Plake & Parker, 1982). Evidence is inconsistent regarding the presence of subscales on the MARS-R, but total scores are moder-
ately correlated with state anxiety from the STAI and with test anxiety from the Test Anxiety Inventory (Hopko, 2003). Internal
consistency was high in our sample (a ¼ 0.96).
358 M. T. Buelow / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 28 (2013) 356–362

The college student version of the AMAS (Reynolds et al., 2003) was administered. The college student version of the
AMAS contains 49 items encompassing four scales: worry/oversensitivity, social concerns/stress, physiological anxiety,
and test anxiety. Individuals respond to a series of statements regarding anxiety with either a “yes” or “no” answer, and
scores are then summed on each subscale. Construct validity of the AMAS was shown through moderate correlations with
other measures of anxiety, including the STAI and the Endler Multidimensional Anxiety Scales, and small correlations
with the Tennessee Self-Concept Scale (Lowe, Peyton, & Reynolds, 2007). One-week (r ¼ .82; Reynolds et al., 2003) and
8-week (r ¼ .84; Lowe et al., 2007) test– retest reliability were both high for the test anxiety subscale, and internal consistency
was high in our sample (a ¼ 0.86).
The STAI (Spielberger, 1983) is a 40-item measure created to assess two aspects of anxiety: current (state) and generalized
(trait). State anxiety may depend more on the situational cues in a given context, whereas trait anxiety is a more stable
and enduring form of anxiety. Factor analyses support this 2-factor structure (Vagg, Spielberger, & O’Hearn, 1980). Only
the state subscale score was used in the present study, with higher scores indicating a higher level of anxiety. The STAI
was designed for use with college students and adults, and test– retest reliability was low for state (as designed for a state-
dependent measure; 1-h r ¼ .16– .33; Spielberger, 1983). In the present study, internal consistency was moderate for this
subscale (a ¼ 0.52).

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at Aston University on January 26, 2014


The Math Computation subtest of the WRAT-IV (Wilkinson & Robertson, 2006) assesses basic computational written math
skills. It was utilized in the present study as an estimate of participants’ math proficiency level. Participants are asked to com-
plete a series of 40 math problems, ranging from simple arithmetic to division by fractions. Performance on this task is calcu-
lated by counting the total number of correct answers. Internal consistency was high across all age groups assessed in the
normative sample (a ¼ 0.83 – 0.95; Wilkinson & Robertson, 2006).
The WMI from the WAIS-IV (Wechsler, 2008) assesses different components of working memory. The DS subtest has three
sections of increasingly more difficult tasks: forwards, backwards, and sequencing (i.e., lowest to highest). The Arithmetic
subtest assesses working memory by having participants complete mental arithmetic problems. The LNS subtest is an alter-
native working memory measure and is not typically included in the calculation of the WMI. The LNS subset is similar to
the DS subtest, only using numbers and letters (i.e., individuals sequence numbers in order lowest to highest, followed by
the letters in alphabetical order). Summed raw scores were calculated for each subtest, with higher scores indicating a
greater level of performance.

Data Analysis

First, scores on the STAI-state were examined to determine if any participants were experiencing a high level of state-
dependent anxiety that may have affected test performance. Six participants reported high levels of state anxiety (i.e.,
scores over 2 SD above the mean anxiety level) and were removed from further analyses. Next, correlations were calculated
between the study variables. A series of linear regression analyses were conducted on the following criterion variables (all
utilized raw scores): DS-Forward, DS-Backward, DS-Sequencing, Arithmetic, and LNS. As gender relates to performance
on the Arithmetic subtest (see Lynn & Irwing, 2008), and there is evidence that situational anxiety, such as can occur in
testing situations, can also affect working memory resources (Sorg & Whitney, 1992), these factors were entered as covariates
into the analyses. Specifically, gender, test anxiety, and math achievement level were entered in Step 1 and the MARS score in
Step 2.

Results

Means and standard deviations for the study variables are provided in Table 1. Significant correlations were noted between
study variables (Table 2). Specifically, gender (coded as women 1 and men 2) was positively correlated with WRAT-math and
Arithmetic (thus, male gender was associated with better performance on these tasks) but negatively correlated with test
anxiety (indicating female gender was associated with higher levels of test anxiety). Math anxiety was positively correlated
with test anxiety but negatively correlated with WRAT-math and Arithmetic scores. WRAT-math scores were positively cor-
related with DS-backwards, DS-sequencing, and Arithmetic scores and negatively correlated with test anxiety. Test anxiety
was negatively correlated with DS-backwards and Arithmetic. Finally, intercorrelations between the WAIS-IV WMI subscales
were as expected (i.e., high positive correlations between tasks).
Results of the linear regression analyses indicated differing relationships between math anxiety and working memory task
performance. There were no significant predictors of DS-Forward, DS-Sequencing, or LNS (all ps . .146; Table 3). For
DS-Backwards, the overall analysis of variance (ANOVA) was significant, F(4,154) ¼ 2.42, p ¼ .051, R 2 ¼ .059.
Participants who scored higher on the WRAT-math (B ¼ 0.069, p ¼ .021) performed better on DS-Backwards than those
M. T. Buelow / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 28 (2013) 356–362 359

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for study variables

Variable M SD Range

AMAS-Test Anxiety 8.23 4.14 0 –15


MARS-R 36.19 19.39 1 –96
STAI-State 38.13 8.94 20–56
WRAT-IV Math Computation 39.83 5.53 25–53
Digit Span-Forwards 10.17 2.05 5 –16
Digit Span-Backwards 7.83 1.98 4 –15
Digit Span-Sequencing 8.53 1.76 4 –14
Arithmetic 12.16 2.94 7 –20
Letter-Number Sequencing 18.92 2.71 11–26
Notes: AMAS ¼ Adult Manifest Anxiety Scale; MARS-R ¼ Math Anxiety Rating Scale Revised; STAI ¼ State-Trait Anxiety Inventory; WRAT-IV ¼ Wide
Range Achievement Test-fourth edition.

Table 2. Correlations between study variables


Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at Aston University on January 26, 2014


Gender — 2.120 .174* 2.269** .019 .040 .120 .324** .020
MARS — 2.282** .417** 2.081 2.083 2.117 2.318** 2.085
WRAT-IV — 2.211** .019 .217** .192** .572** .080
AMAS-C — 2.123 2.161* 2.107 2.227** .019
DS-F — .396** .200** .152* .439**
DS-B — .309** .332** .521**
DS-S — .276** .343**
Arith — .337**
LNS —
Notes: MARS ¼ Math Anxiety Rating Scale; WRAT ¼ Wide Range Achievement Test-IV, Math subtest; AMAS-C ¼ Adult Manifest Anxiety Scale, College
Version, Test Anxiety subscale; DS-F ¼ WAIS-IV Digit Span subtest, Forwards; DS-B ¼ WAIS-IV Digit Span subtest, Backwards; DS-S ¼ WAIS-IV Digit
Span subtest, Sequencing; Arith ¼ WAIS-IV Arithmetic subtest; LNS ¼ WAIS-IV Letter-Number Sequencing subtest.
*p ≤ .05.
**p ≤ .01.

who scored lower. For Arithmetic, the overall ANOVA was significant, F(4,154) ¼ 24.67, p , .001, R2 ¼ .391. Those high in
math anxiety performed worse than those low in math anxiety (B ¼ 20.023, p ¼ .030). In addition, scores on Arithmetic were
higher for men than women (B ¼ 1.210, p ¼ .002) and for those scoring high on the WRAT-math than those scoring low (B ¼
0.259, p , .001).
We further examined LNS performance, broken down by the order of WMI subtest administration. If math anxiety “carried
over” following the completion of the Arithmetic subtest, then scores on LNS should be lower in those who completed this task
after Arithmetic. An independent-samples t-test indicated that participants who completed the typical WAIS-IV order (DS,
Arithmetic, LNS) did not perform differently from those who completed the alternate order (DS, LNS, Arithmetic),
t(163) ¼ 1.314, p ¼ .191. Thus, the negative effects of math anxiety appear limited to the Arithmetic subtest.

Discussion

The present study sought to examine the relationships between math anxiety and performance on the WAIS-IV WMI sub-
scales. As hypothesized, math anxiety was a significant predictor of Arithmetic but not DS or LNS performance, even after
accounting for gender, math achievement, and test anxiety. Although previous research has shown math anxiety to negatively
affect performance on various working memory tasks (Eysenck, 1985; Ikeda et al., 1996; Markham & Darke, 1991; Miller &
Bichsel, 2004), our results suggest that the type of working memory task utilized matters. Tasks that utilize mathematical
operations to assess working memory skills, such as Arithmetic, may activate underlying math anxiety, in turn negatively
affecting scores. Neuroimaging studies have indicated that math anxiety increases activation in structures associated with
working memory and mathematical reasoning (Young et al., 2012), which may help explain the present results.
We also found that the math achievement level, as assessed with the WRAT-IV math computation subtest, was the predict-
ive of both Arithmetic and DS-backwards performance. The link between math achievement and DS-backwards, a task that
does not involve mathematical operations per se, may be due in part to the multifaceted nature of the DS task.
360 M. T. Buelow / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 28 (2013) 356–362

Table 3. Summary of regressions

Criterion Predictors F p R2 B p

DS-F (raw) 0.60 .660 .015


Gender 20.071 .840
AMAS-C 20.057 .205
WRAT Math 20.007 .830
MARS 20.003 .737
DS-B (raw) 2.42 .051 .059
Gender 0.000 .999
AMAS-C 20.066 .121
WRAT Math 0.069 .021
MARS 0.005 .594
DS-S (raw) 1.73 .146 .043
Gender 0.262 .384
AMAS-C 20.012 .754
WRAT Math 0.051 .061
MARS 20.003 .697

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at Aston University on January 26, 2014


Arith (raw) 24.67 .000 .391
Gender 1.210 .002
AMAS-C 0.003 .948
WRAT Math 0.259 .000
MARS 20.023 .030
LNS (raw) 0.46 .763 .012
Gender 0.323 .479
AMAS-C 0.041 .480
WRAT Math 0.015 .706
MARS 20.011 .353
Notes: DS-F ¼ WAIS-IV Digit Span subtest, Forwards; DS-B ¼ WAIS-IV Digit Span subtest, Backwards; DS-S ¼ WAIS-IV Digit Span subtest, Sequencing;
Arith ¼ WAIS-IV Arithmetic subtest; LNS ¼ Letter-Number Sequencing; AMAS-C ¼ Adult Manifest Anxiety Scale, College Version, Test Anxiety sub-
scale; WRAT Math ¼ Wide Range Achievement Test-IV, Math subtest; MARS ¼ Math Anxiety Rating Scale.

DS-backwards may assess cognitive reasoning skills, such as may also be operating during Arithmetic, rather than frank cog-
nitive efficiency, which is likely operating during DS-forwards.
The results of this study have implications for clinical practice. The assessment of working memory skills with the WAIS-IV
WMI is just one set of measures used in clinical practice, but other measures of working memory used by clinicians may also
fall victim to the negative effects of math anxiety. Tasks such as the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task (PASAT; Gronwall,
1977) or Auditory Consonant Trigrams (ACT; Stuss, Stethem, & Poirer, 1987), when administered to those high in math
anxiety, may result in underestimates of working memory abilities. In addition, some tasks assessing working memory may
in fact assess multiple cognitive systems, including attention, working memory, cognitive reasoning, visuospatial perception,
and visual reasoning, depending on the specific task. Although previous confirmatory factor analyses of the WAIS-IV have
supported a WMI comprised of DS and Arithmetic, loadings on the WMI factor were higher for DS and LNS than
Arithmetic across studies (Benson, Hulac, & Kranzler, 2010; Canivez & Watkins, 2010). At least in those with high math
anxiety, we recommend utilizing DS and LNS to estimate working memory abilities on the WAIS-IV. Working memory
tasks are often used in the evaluation of such clinical syndromes as Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and
head injury, where working memory skills may be impaired. Thus, accurate assessment of these abilities—and not of
math-related anxiety—is necessary.
We also found a significant correlation between gender and math achievement level, in that men tended to have higher
scores on the WRAT-IV math than women. This finding correlates with previous studies showing gender differences in
math abilities in older children and adolescents (Heil & Jansen-Osmann, 2008; Liu, Wilson, & Paek, 2008; Rosselli,
Ardila, Matute, & Inozemtseva, 2009). The only previous research found examining gender effects on the WRAT found an
absence of gender differences on the WRAT-R (Haddad & Bardos, 1990). Our results indicate that gender may influence per-
formance on math achievement tests and that this should be taken into account in the interpretation of WRAT-IV test scores.
There were several limitations to the present study. We utilized a sample of undergraduate students who did not present for
clinical evaluation. As noted earlier, weaknesses in working memory are often used as indicators of dysfunction in clinical
conditions such as ADHD and Mild Traumatic Brain Injury (TBI). To determine the potential diagnostic implications of
math anxiety on WMI scores, the clinical correlation of the present findings in a clinical sample (e.g., ADHD, TBI,
M. T. Buelow / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 28 (2013) 356–362 361

Multiple Sclerosis) is warranted. We limited our assessment of working memory to WAIS-IV subtests. It is unclear to what
extent the present results may generalize to other working memory tasks with a math component that are typically used in
neuropsychological batteries, including the PASAT and ACT. Based on our results, it is not unreasonable to suspect that
math anxiety may impact performance on these measures as well; however, this will require additional investigation.
In conclusion, we found preliminary evidence that math anxiety has a significant negative effect on Arithmetic but not DS or
LNS from the WAIS-IV, above and beyond that accounted for by gender, math achievement, and test anxiety. Future studies
should further examine this relationship in an evaluation-seeking sample for clinical correlation. In clinical practice, if a sig-
nificant split is noted in scores between DS and Arithmetic, the level of math anxiety should be evaluated and the LNS subtest
administered. An examination of the extent of the negative effects of math anxiety on other numerically-based working
memory tasks is warranted in order to determine which clinical tasks may be prone to interference from this form of anxiety.

Conflict of Interest

None declared.

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at Aston University on January 26, 2014


References

Ashcraft, M. H. (2002). Math anxiety: Personal, educational, and cognitive consequences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 181–185.
Ashcraft, M. H., & Faust, M. W. (1994). Mathematics anxiety and mental arithmetic performance: An exploratory investigation. Cognition and Emotion, 8,
97– 125.
Ashcraft, M. H., & Kirk, E. P. (2001). The relationships among working memory, math anxiety, and performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General, 130, 224–237.
Ashcraft, M. H., & Krause, J. A. (2007). Working memory, math performance, and math anxiety. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14 (2), 243– 248.
Ashcraft, M. H., & Moore, A. M. (2009). Mathematics anxiety and the affective drop in performance. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 27 (3),
197–205.
Beilock, S. L. (2010). Choke: What the secrets of the brain reveal about getting it right when you have to. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Benson, N., Hulac, D. M., & Kranzler, J. H. (2010). Independent examination of the Wechsler adult intelligence scale—fourth edition (WAIS-IV): What does
the WAIS-IV measure? Psychological Assessment, 22, 121–130.
Canivez, G. L., & Watkins, M. W. (2010). Investigation of the factor structure of the Wechsler adult intelligence scale—fourth edition (WAIS-IV): Exploratory
and higher order factor analyses. Psychological Assessment, 22 (4), 827–836.
Cassaday, J. C., & Johnson, R. E. (2002). Cognitive test anxiety and academic performance. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 27, 270– 295.
Dew, K. M. H., Galassi, J. P., & Galassi, M. D. (1984). Math anxiety: Relation with situational test anxiety, performance, physiological arousal, and math
avoidance behavior. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 31, 580– 583.
Eysenck, M. W. (1985). Anxiety and cognitive-task performance. Personality and Individual Differences, 6, 579–586.
Eysenck, M. W., & Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and performance: The processing efficiency theory. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 409 –434.
Faust, M. W., Ashcraft, M. H., & Fleck, D. E. (1996). Mathematics anxiety effects in simple and complex addition. Mathematical Cognition, 2 (1), 25–62.
Gronwall, D. M. A. (1977). Paced auditory serial addition task: A measure of recovery from concussion. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 44, 367–373.
Haddad, F. A., & Bardos, A. N. (1990). Differential effects of socioeconomic status and gender on the matrix analogies test-expanded form and the wide range
achievement test-revised with above-average elementary school students. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 8, 133– 138.
Heil, M., & Jansen-Osmann, P. (2008). Gender differences in math and mental rotation accuracy but not in mental rotation speed in 8-year-old children.
European Journal of Developmental Science, 2 (1–2), 190 –196.
Hembree, R. (1990). The nature, effects, and relief of mathematics anxiety. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 21, 33– 46.
Hillary, F. G. (2008). Neuroimaging of working memory dysfunction and the dilemma with brain reorganization hypotheses. Journal of the International
Neuropsychological Society, 14, 526–534.
Holdnack, J. A., Zhou, X., Larrabee, G. J., Millis, S. R., & Salthouse, T. A. (2011). Confirmatory factor analysis of the WAIS-IV/WMS-IV. Assessment, 18 (2),
178–191.
Hopko, D. R. (2003). Confirmatory factor analysis of the math anxiety rating scale-revised. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 63, 336–351.
Hopko, D. R., Crittendon, J. A., Grant, E., & Wilson, S. A. (2005). The impact of anxiety on performance IQ. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 18 (1), 17– 35.
Hopko, D. R., McNeil, D. W., Lejuez, C. W., Ashcraft, M. H., Eifert, G. H., & Riel, J. (2003). The effects of anxious responding on mental arithmetic and
lexical decision task performance. Anxiety Disorders, 17, 647–665.
Hopko, D. R., McNeil, D. W., Zvolensky, M. J., & Eifert, G. H. (2001). The relation between anxiety and skill in performance-based anxiety disorders: A
behavioral formulation of social phobia. Behavior Therapy, 32, 185– 207.
Ikeda, M., Iwanaga, M., & Seiwa, H. (1996). Test anxiety and working memory system. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 82, 1223– 1231.
Levine, G. (1995). Closing the gender gap: Focus on mathematics anxiety. Contemporary Education, 67 (1), 42–45.
Liu, O. L., Wilson, M., & Paek, I. (2008). A multidimensional Rasch analysis of gender differences in PISA mathematics. Journal of Applied Measurement, 9
(1), 18– 35.
Lowe, P. A., Peyton, V., & Reynolds, C. R. (2007). Test score stability and the relationship of adult manifest anxiety scale-college version scores to external
variables among graduate students. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 25, 69– 81.
Lynn, R., & Irwing, P. (2008). Sex differences in mental arithmetic, digit span, and g defined as working memory capacity. Intelligence, 36, 226–235.
362 M. T. Buelow / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 28 (2013) 356–362

Ma, X. (1999). A meta-analysis of the relationship between anxiety toward mathematics and achievement in mathematics. Journal for Research in Mathematics
Education, 30, 520–541.
Maloney, E. A., Ansari, D., & Fugelsang, J. A. (2011). The effects of mathematics anxiety on the processing of numerical magnitude. Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 64, 10–16.
Maloney, E. A., & Beilock, S. L. (2012). Math anxiety: Who has it, why it develops, and how to guard against it. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 404– 406.
Maloney, E. A., Risko, E. F., Ansari, D., & Fugelsang, J. (2010). Mathematics anxiety affects counting but not subitizing during visual enumeration. Cognition,
114, 293–297.
Markham, R., & Darke, S. (1991). The effects of anxiety on verbal and spatial task performance. Australian Journal of Psychology, 43, 107– 111.
Menon, V., Rivera, S. M., White, C. D., Glover, G. H., & Reiss, A. L. (2000). Dissociating prefrontal and parietal cortex activation during arithmetic process-
ing. NeuroImage, 64, 97–106.
Miller, H., & Bichsel, J. (2004). Anxiety, working memory, gender, and math performance. Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 591–606.
Plake, B. S., & Parker, C. S. (1982). The development and validation of a revised version of the mathematics anxiety rating scale. Educational and
Psychological Measurement, 42, 551– 557.
Reynolds, C. R., Richmond, B. O., & Lowe, P. A. (2003). The adult manifest anxiety scale professional manual. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological
Services.
Richardson, F. C., & Suinn, R. M. (1972). The mathematics anxiety rating scale: Psychometric data. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 19 (6), 551 –554.
Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Matute, E., & Inozemtseva, O. (2009). Gender differences and cognitive correlates of mathematical skills in school-aged children.
Child Neuropsychology, 15 (3), 216– 231.

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at Aston University on January 26, 2014


Sorg, B. A., & Whitney, P. (1992). The effect of trait anxiety and situational stress on working memory capacity. Journal of Research in Personality, 26,
235–241.
Spielberger, C. D. (1983). State-trait anxiety inventory for adults. Tampa, FL: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Stuss, D. T., Stethem, L. L., & Poirier, C. A. (1987). Comparison of three tests of attention and rapid information processing across six age groups. The Clinical
Neuropsychologist, 1, 139–152.
Vagg, P. R., Spielberger, C. D., & O’Hearn, T. P. (1980). Is the state-trait anxiety inventory multidimensional? Personality and Individual Differences, 1,
207–214.
Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler adult intelligence scale-Fourth edition: Professional manual. San Antonio, TX: Pearson PsychCorp.
Wilkinson, G. S., & Robertson, G. J. (2006). Wide range achievement test-4: Professional manual. Lutz, FL: PAR.
Young, C. B., Wu, S. S., & Menon, V. (2012). The neurodevelopmental basis of math anxiety. Psychological Science, 23, 492– 501.
Zakaria, E., & Nordin, N. M. (2008). The effects of mathematics anxiety on matriculation students as related to motivation and achievement. Eurasia Journal
of Mathematics, Science, and Technology Education, 4 (1), 27–30.
Zettle, R. D., & Raines, S. J. (2000). The relationship of trait and test anxiety with math anxiety. College Student Journal, 34, 246– 258.

You might also like