Second Language Learning in Elementary Years - Cognitive and Socia
Second Language Learning in Elementary Years - Cognitive and Socia
ScholarWorks@GVSU
4-2021
ScholarWorks Citation
Elenbaas, Brooke, "Second Language Learning in Elementary Years: Cognitive and Social-Emotional
Benefits" (2021). Culminating Experience Projects. 22.
https://scholarworks.gvsu.edu/gradprojects/22
This Project is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate Research and Creative Practice at
ScholarWorks@GVSU. It has been accepted for inclusion in Culminating Experience Projects by an authorized
administrator of ScholarWorks@GVSU. For more information, please contact [email protected].
SECOND LANGUAGE BENEFITS IN ELEMENTARY YEARS i
Brooke Elenbaas
Abstract
All through the world bilingualism is a normal part of school, work, and culture creating
cognitive and social-emotional benefits (Fan, 2016; Marian & Shook, 2012). The United States
especially in the elementary schools. The absence of second language learning at the elementary
grades is a gap districts should seek to fill as research shows brain development positively
effecting cognitive and social-emotional learning during this early stage of development
(Abutalebi et al., 2011). This project is an in-service designed for elementary school districts
pursuing a better understanding of why second language learning will benefit students both
cognitively and social-emotionally. Important pieces of this in-service will be sharing research-
based information and providing scope and sequence to achieve second language learning
success of the in-service program. Stronger presence of second language learning will benefit
student cognitive development and influence the student body with positive social-emotional
learning.
Key words: bilingual, cognitive learning, second language learning, social and
emotional learning.
SECOND LANGUAGE BENEFITS IN ELEMENTARY YEARS ii
Table of Contents
Abstract……………………………………………………………………………………………i
Table of Contents…………………………………………………………………………………ii
Problem Statement………………………………………………………………………...1
Statement of Purpose……………………………………………………………………...3
Review of Literature……………………………………………………………………………..7
Introduction………………………………………………………………………………7
Cognitive Benefits……………………………………………………………….9
Summary…………………………………………………………………………………13
SECOND LANGUAGE BENEFITS IN ELEMENTARY YEARS iii
Conclusion……………………………………………………………………………….14
Project Description………………………………………………………………………………15
Introduction………………………………………………………………………………15
Project Components……………………………………………………………………...15
Project Evaluation………………………………………………………………………..16
References………………………………………………………………………………………..20
Appendices……………………………………………………………………………………….25
SECOND LANGUAGE BENEFITS IN ELEMENTARY YEARS 1
Problem Statement
Bilingualism from a young age is the norm throughout much of the world, aiding students
with speed and accuracy of cognitive functions and providing social-emotional benefits (Fan,
2016; Marian & Shook, 2012). However, elementary schools in the United States have limited
opportunities for students to practice language and reap the benefits of being bilingual.
Elementary Schools should seek to close this gap in learning to spur elementary students forward
not only in cognitive function, but increasing the elementary students social-emotional abilities
as well. The high volume social-emotional needs within our nation’s youth continues to grow at
staggering numbers as a whole, the lack of language instruction adds to this problem. Language
learners have a stronger ability to self-regulate and empathize with social-emotional needs. The
lack of second language learning within the United States is limiting opportunities, cognitive and
This project aims to create a research-based in-service demonstrating the benefits and
needs for second language education in the elementary grades. Research in recent years has
elementary years. Furthermore, there is now research showing greater social-emotional benefits
brain development constructed in the elementary school years (Abutalebi et al., 2011).
Elementary children raised or taught in a bilingual environment continually show better problem-
SECOND LANGUAGE BENEFITS IN ELEMENTARY YEARS 2
solving skills throughout their schooling, which has been attributed to early bilingual
development. Bilingual learning also shifts the linguistic focus from self to others, by allowing
learners to perceive various combinations of when, where, and what kind of language to use
(Fan, 2016). Building a better education system within our nation to mold better citizens for
tomorrow should include, promote, and celebrate bilingual learning as an avenue for stronger
instruction varies given the student family, means and circumstances, but the importance and
early exposure remain steadfast. The United States has a baffling low percentage of students
receiving second language instruction, while around the world in elementary classrooms children
are instructed in a second language. In the United States, only 20% of K-12 students receive
second language instruction (Devlin, 2018). Elementary students are less likely to receive second
language instruction, with only 15% receiving second-language instruction according to the Pew
Research Center (Devlin, 2018). The language deficit can be partially attributed to a culture 20th
century immigration, where assimilation into the English-speaking culture was valued by
immigrant and citizens (Stein-Smith, 2016). This monolingual mind-set coupled with the
distance between the United States and other countries, has created a bilingual gap in our
student’s education.
Fortunately, researchers have collected significant data and research shows cognitive and
education. These benefits increase if the subjects are elementary or younger age children
learning a second language. By learning a second language, the child is required to think not
SECOND LANGUAGE BENEFITS IN ELEMENTARY YEARS 3
only of itself, but is forced to recognize a larger world in choosing a language to use for
communication (Fan, 2016). Cognitive benefits have been well documented. However, social-
emotional benefits have been slow to emerge, but remain inseparable in benefits to second
Our current language learning module relies on language instruction is placed in the
upper grades, which maybe stifling student achievement. Based on the growing social emotional
needs paired with the cognitive benefits, language instruction in the early grades is place for
student growth. This project seeks to provide in-service materials show casing the cognitive and
Statement of Purpose
The cognitive and social-emotional benefits with second language learning in the
elementary provided here, through research, will be presented to administration and school
districts in the form of an in-service. This project serves as a tangible piece of evidence with in-
service presentation and documents to guide administration and districts in support of second
language learning in the elementary. Tangible scope and sequence for enactment of a second
language program as a co-curricular subject within the elementary will give districts and
implementation will aide in bettering the project and molding the project to fit individual needs
of a district. This project is unique as it brings two benefits together, cognition and social-
are the direct objectives for this project through an in-service. This in-service is to be research-
based and to ignite an interest to spur administration or districts to pursue or strongly consider
implementation of second language learning within the elementary. The importance and positive
impact for cognitive student achievement outside the language instruction will be explained. The
connection and researched based social-emotional benefits will also be linked to show the
efficacy of language instruction within the elementary. Although, language instruction benefits
all ages of learners, this project will be limited to the elementary students. This project is to give
an attainable scope and sequence for enactment of a second language program in an elementary.
knowledge that can be tested or evaluated with both speed and accuracy.
Second language learner: an individual learning a second language not native to their current
Social-emotional learner: the process of an individual acquiring and applying the skills to self-
regulate emotions for positive goals of relationships with self and others.
based materials that highlights the cognitive and social-emotional benefits of second language
learning in the elementary. This project is to give an attainable scope and sequence to implement
language learning within the elementary grades. The scope and sequence is designed for
enactment at the start of a school year as a co-curricular subject, not to replace core-curriculum.
Other traditional co-curricular examples are art, physical education, technology, and music. This
the elementary grades. This addition of language as a co-curricular is intended to be a bridge for
a need without the complete change required of language immersion schools, as this requires a
All elementary and second language teachers could benefit from the knowledge from this
in-service and partnering with administration for enactment of second language learning in the
influence a school’s academic and social-emotional learning. This in-service will address the
This in-service will include a presentation and tangible materials of scope and sequence
for implementation of second language learning in the elementary. The in-service will can be
presented at anytime throughout the year, however, the implementation is designed for the start
of the school year. The language instruction as a co-curricular is designed for enactment at the
start of a school year as a co-curricular subject, not to replace core-curriculum. The start of the
school year when most elementary scheduling takes place and would be the least disruptive to
SECOND LANGUAGE BENEFITS IN ELEMENTARY YEARS 6
staff and student scheduling. The timing would also be ideal for filling staffing needs and
consistency of instructor to student throughout the year. Again, the in-service can be considered
at many times of the year, but the implementation of second language learning the elementary is
This project is to provide ideas and research, but is not a singular curriculum for second
language learning in the elementary. If used as single resource, this project would be a limitation
for staff and students. Coupled with various researched based resources will be profitable for
staff and students as there are many methods and skills to be learned through various
curriculums. Administration and staff will need to pursue curriculums to meet district and
This in-service does not address scope and sequence for second language immersion
programs, but could be adjusted to fit this need. Second language immersion programs are
important for bilingual advancement within the educational system, however, are challenging
due to the need of fluent certified staff in the target language for every classroom. This in-
service is a bridge to give schools the tools through tangible materials to implement second
Introduction
Throughout the world, second language learning or bilingualism is the norm for students
and children; however, the United States education shows a deficit in second language learning.
Only 20% of students learn a foreign language in K-12 schools throughout the United States,
compared to 92% of European K-12 students (Devlin, 2018). This creates a deficit not only in
opportunities lost for students, but an absence in cognitive benefits of more accurate, faster
reaction times to questions. Furthermore, there is now research showing greater social-emotional
benefits attributed to second language learning in elementary school. The reason behind
cognitive and social-emotional development has to do with brain development being constructed
in the elementary school years. Elementary children raised or taught in a bilingual environment
continually show better problem-solving skills throughout their schooling, which has been
attributed to early bilingual development. Bilingual learning also shifts the linguistic focus from
self to others, by allowing learners to perceive various combinations of when, where, and what
kind of language to use (Fan, 2016). Building a better education system to mold better citizens
should include, promote, and celebrate bilingual learning as an avenue for stronger cognitive and
social-emotional students.
Bilingualism is the norm throughout the world with European countries averaging
92% of students learning a second language in primary school, whereas the United States
averages 20% (Devlin, 2018). This language deficit can be partially attributed to a culture 20th
century immigration, where assimilation into the English-speaking culture was valued by
SECOND LANGUAGE BENEFITS IN ELEMENTARY YEARS 8
immigrant and citizens (Stein-Smith, 2016). This monolingual mind-set coupled with the
distance between the United States and other countries, has created a bilingual gap in our
student’s education. Another reason for a lower percentage of bilingualism in the United States
is the misconception that a second or competing language will decrease the level of education.
This misconception grew throughout the United States and led to California passing Proposition
227 in 1998, which had the goal of drastically reducing time English language learners spent in
bilingual settings (State of California, Proposal 227, 1998). In 2016 the law was essentially
Cognitive research has shown a heightened sense of attention control in both native and
second languages (Bialystok, 1999). A bilingual speaker uses the information from both
languages simultaneously and the brain makes executive decisions as to which language is being
used to control speech and comprehension and connects pieces of both languages (Marian &
Shook, 2012). Studies note that there is no cognitive difference shown in the first year or lower
levels of language learning as children were only 6 months into their language learning. (Carlson
& Meltzoff, 2008). However, research shows a growth in cognitive function more strongly as
time and quality of language learning increases (Nicolay & Poncelet, 2012). This cognitive
growth of speed and accuracy response is attributed to a bilingual’s brain constantly assessing
Learning a language at an early age helps with cognitive and social-emotional processes,
reaping life-long benefits. Recent studies are suggesting the strengthened cognitive language
switching may delay the onset of neurological diseases of Alzheimer and dementia (Abeutalebi
& Perani, 2015). The benefits flow into a more adept problem-solving learning style and give
Cognitive Benefits. Several studies and tests show that bilingual people often out-
perform in cognitive conflict tasks. A cognitive conflict task is one where the participant has a
set of information and must ignore some of the information in order to reach the correct
response. A common test used is the Stroop task of color and word association (Stroop, 1992).
The task requires the person to name the color of the word in print, (i.e., the word “red” printed
in red) this is an equal task if color name and color of print match (i.e. red). However, if switched
the task demands that the brain ignore the irrelevant word and focus on the color (i.e., the word
“red” printed in blue). “Bilingual people often perform better than monolingual people at tasks
that tap into inhibitory control ability” (Marian & Shook, 2012). This task switching and ability
to ignore the competing information is attributed to the fact that a bilingual’s brain is constantly
regulating inputted language into the language categories they speak or possess.
The brain of a bilingual uses one language, while the other is active at the same
time. This means the bilingual brain is maintaining a balance of possessed languages in order to
regulate competing languages that are simultaneously active (Marian & Spivey, 2003). For
example in the study by Thierry and Wu (2007) if you heard the words “pick up a marker,” your
brain would hear the sound of “marker”, place it in auditory input and activate other words with
a similar sound regardless of the language spoken. A monolingual English brain may elicit
compound words like marker, marker board, marker cap. A bilingual Russian-English brain may
elicit the English word marker, while simultaneously thinking of the Russian word for stamp,
marka, because the sound is the same. This constant control regulation needed for understanding
The constant competition may have some cognitive effects of slowed reaction time to
responding or recall (Gollan et al., 2005). Bilinguals slowed reaction time is attributed to the fact
that competing languages requires more processing time. More time is needed for a bilingual to
respond, but they have better results in cognitive conflicts (Abutalebi, et al., 2011). If accuracy
What does all of this cognitive benefit mean and how can bilingualism increase learning
in core subjects? These are valid questions as we are challenged on the effectiveness of
bilingualism in comparison to the time and cost. In comparison of time of administration costs, a
study showed proportional time devoted to dual language immersion to students enrolled in the
dual language program when compared to monolingual programs of the same area (Steele et al.,
2015).
Building literacy depends on building on known language and transposing new language
study of language immersion students in the state of Oregon, researchers found bilingual
immersion students on average reading a full grade level ahead of their monolingual peers
A similar study of reading accuracy and comprehension was done of students dominant
monolingual students. The English language learners were assumed to have a weaker English
vocabulary by definition. However, the English language learner students had similar reading
scores to their peers. According to a 2009 study, the language learner compensates for lack of
(Bialystok et al., 2005). The English language learner would look back and be sure what they
are reading makes sense with the story line. This enables a bilingual person to get to the same
The above suggests a bilingual brain would be quicker to utilize new strategies, since it
already juggles input from competing auditory language and the brain’s regulation of this
information. An example would be cognates in language such as “rosado”, the Spanish word for
pink. A bilingual brain would be quicker to associate the rose-colored wine as “pink”, since the
competing language suggests this immediately as the wine color is associated with the color
pink.
The extent of transfer in between languages and reading ability for bilinguals is
somewhat correlated and limited to the relationship between the writing of both
languages. Studies have been done to assess the transfer between English and Spanish
(Durgunoglu et al., 1993; Lindsey et. al, 2003) or English and French (Comeau et al., 1999). All
of these languages (English, French, and Spanish) share a common alphabet and phonetics that
reading and comprehension (Luk, 2003; Geva & Siegel, 2000.) In contrast Luk (2003) found a
maintained phonological awareness in both languages even if there is not a common writing
system. Therefore, reading and writing development can be transferred allowing a stronger
literary understanding and growth in both languages if the writing systems share commonalities.
environment have insights into social structures made visible through language. Being bilingual
demands cognitively navigating between sounds, words, language, and gestures. Therefore, a
SECOND LANGUAGE BENEFITS IN ELEMENTARY YEARS 12
bilingual child has a social experience that differs from monolingual children. The bilingual
environment exposes the speaker to a vast working of social-emotional cues as to whom speaks
what language, with whom, and style of language; forming an “understanding of other people’s
linguistic perspective” (Fan et al., 2015 (p. 1060); Yow & Markman, 2011).
In order to assess this, false-belief tasks are often used to compare monolingual and
bilingual ability to understand another’s perspective. False-belief tasks give the participant the
knowledge of a different perspective or event that is directly perceived by the participant. The
participant’s task is to put aside their knowledge and take on the perspective of another person to
emit the correct response. A classic false-belief assessment by Wimmer and Perner (1983)
involves a child placing a chocolate in box A. Unbeknownst to the child, the chocolate is moved
by his mother to box B. The young participant viewing these series of events is then asked,
where the will child look for the chocolate when the child returns? The correct response is that
the child will look for the chocolate in box A, as that is where the child had placed it last and
would have no knowledge of the mother moving the chocolate to box B per Wimmer and Perner
(1983). The participating children assessing these series of events were only successful when
they could separate the boy’s wrong belief (looking in box A) from their own reality (the
This ability to take on another’s perspective and to understand social groupings from a
young age is thought to be related to the ability to better understand other’s perspectives around
them (Arnberg & Arnberg, 1992; Meisel, 1989). Peggy Goetz (2003) wrote:
Children’s recognition that other people cannot understand one of their languages may
lead them to adjust their “theories” about other people’s knowledge or intentions, so
SECOND LANGUAGE BENEFITS IN ELEMENTARY YEARS 13
bilingual children may have an advantage over monolingual children in sorting out these
This proposal suggests that possible outcomes and emotions are dependent on the language
spoken. The idea that bilingual speakers may possess a greater ability to more easily think from
another’s perspective would possibly enable the child to be more thoughtful to others viewpoints,
Does this mean bilingual people are on a whole kinder and have a greater perspective
than monolingual children? Not exactly, as humans are capable of learning quickly. Proving this
learning ability, Goetz (2003) performed two sets of false-belief tasks on a group of three and
four-year-olds. In the first set of tasks the bilingual children out-performed the monolingual
children, but it was noted when tested a second time that both groups performed equally. In
conclusion, bilingual children may have a greater capacity to understand a different perspective,
but it does not indicate that monolingual children cannot be taught to understand different
perspectives.
Summary
Learning a new language in the elementary years has many advantages including stronger
problem solving abilities and an aptitude to understand perspectives of others, which may
qualities, but it does inform the benefits of this ability, which is considered standard throughout
Schools throughout the United States should use opportunities for students to include
Conclusion
In order to better our schools and our society in the United States we should consider the
social-emotional and cognitive benefits of learning a second language in the elementary years
through secondary school. Cognitive benefits able the student to think more readily and make
connections. Social-emotional enables the student to open their hearts to other languages,
cultures and struggles. Bialystok and Hakuta (1994) said it best, “the knowledge of two
languages is greater than the sum of its parts.” More research will be completed, but the
resounding current results show a benefit for being educated and raised with second language
Introduction
Second language learning is important in the elementary grades with benefits of greater
accuracy and speed in cognition tasks. Furthermore, there are positive effects on social-
emotional learning. This project will serve as an in-service to showcase the research-based
benefits, give attainable scope and sequence for implementation of a second language in
elementary as a co-curricular subject, and have sustained evaluations to support and tailor
et al., 2011; Bialystok, 1999) and social-emotional learning (Fan, 2016; Swain, 2013). The goal
is to promote and enhance second language learning in the elementary within the United States
Project components and plans of implementation for an in-service are detailed to ensure
the efficacy of the in-service to promote second language learning in the elementary. Following
implementations of in-service, evaluations to be used in order to support, connect, and assess the
Project Components
implementation. The scope and sequence will align with Michigan’s Department of Education
SECOND LANGUAGE BENEFITS IN ELEMENTARY YEARS 16
and American Council on Teaching Foreign Languages (ACTFL). Language Scope and
Sequence (Appendix A) will align with three ranges of language performance within various
forms of communication. Ranges are novice, intermediate, and advanced as in line with ACTFL.
This in-service is specifically designed for schools seeking to add a foreign language as part of
the co-curricular learning in elementary schools, grades kindergarten through fifth within
Michigan, but could be adjusted to support a school seeking to add language immersion as part
learning that are part of second language learning within the elementary. This information will
The Language Implementation Timeline (Appendix B) includes details for steps to follow
working towards the addition of a foreign language as part of the learning in elementary schools.
Details include estimated needs of staff, classroom materials, informative materials, promotion
materials, and checklist until start-up. An implementation timeline is provided for clarity and to
Likewise, the scope and sequence of second language learning by grade level will enable
educators and administrators to mindfully set goals. The scope and sequence will uphold
standards from the Michigan Department of Education and ACTFL as part of the focused
instruction expected to achieve second language learning success. Similar to the implementation
timeline, the scope and sequence will provide structured goals and learning targets.
SECOND LANGUAGE BENEFITS IN ELEMENTARY YEARS 17
Project Evaluation
Indicators of success are closely linked with key objectives and goals to grow and support
second language learning in the elementary. Evaluations include: shareholder survey, post-
annual evaluation of the program, and assessments for students to ensure learning objectives are
being met.
The Shareholder’s Survey (Appendix C) empowers members to have a voice with the
addition of second language within the elementary. This is an initial survey is meant to better
understand district specific norms and values to ensure shareholder buy-in. The survey will
include opportunity for feedback in regards to educational values and goals of second language
learning within a specific district. This survey may reveal indicators and opportunities to tailor
(Appendix D) will be administered to administrators, teachers, and staff quarterly for the first
year and bi-annually for the following years to ensure program fidelity, growth, and success.
These evaluations will focus on values, goals reached, and improvements to be made by either
the district or the program. The goal of this is to maintain program growth and fidelity so that
educational and social-emotional needs can be attained through second language learning.
learning, and cognitive abilities, so the district and program can see the benefits of the program.
ACTFL has standardized testing (AAPPL) for grades 3-5 focusing on language growth, social-
emotional through cultural content and cognitive abilities of language switching. Lower grades
SECOND LANGUAGE BENEFITS IN ELEMENTARY YEARS 18
are building to a point of standardized assessment, and lack the vocabulary prior to grade three.
It is suggested for lower grades K-2 is formative assessment be given upon each unit of
completion. Student assessment is not to be utilized as a reflection of student ability, but rather a
Project Conclusion
Second language learning in the elementary years positively effects learning both
cognitively and social-emotionally (Abutalebi, et al., 2011; Bialystok, 1999; Swain, 2013; Yow
& Markman, 2011). Educating districts on the benefits of second language learning in
success. Based on the research cited in the literature review section it is expected districts should
Project Implementation
This project is designed to increase the presence of second language learning in the
elementary as research has shown it benefits students cognitive and social-emotional capabilities.
Available resources will be laid-out within an in-service to the elementary school districts
throughout Michigan. This project gives scope and sequence for implementation of second
language learning as part of the co-curricular courses that are integrated into the school
curriculum and culture. The in-service meeting dates will be in January and March of 2021 in
preparation for the following school year. During the in-service, administrators and staff will be
guided with an interactive presentation and provided with a packet of information to launch
second language learning in their elementary schools. Support will be given following these
SECOND LANGUAGE BENEFITS IN ELEMENTARY YEARS 19
dates with in-service feedback. Feedback for district specific implementation needs will be
References
Abutalebi, J., Pasquale, A. D. R., Green, D. W., Hernandez, M., Scifo, P., Keim, R., Cappa, S.
F., & Costa, A. (2011). Bilingualism tunes the anterior cingulate cortex for conflict
Arnberg, L., & Arnberg, P. W. (1992). The relation between code differentiation and language
http://search.proquest.com.ezproxy.gvsu.edu/docview/63152801?accountid=39473
Bialystok, E. (1999). Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child
Bialystok, E., Luk, G., & Kwan, E. (2005) Bilingualism, biliteracy, and learning to read:
Interactions among languages and writing systems. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9(1),
43-61.
http://search.proquest.com.ezproxy.gvsu.edu/docview/85624981?accountid=39473
Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in
https://doi-org.ezproxy.gvsu.edu/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00675.x
Comeau, L., Cormier, P., Grandmaison, E., & Lacroix, D. (1999). A longitudinal study of
http://dx.doi.org.ezproxy.gvsu.edu/10.1037/0022-0663.91.1.29
SECOND LANGUAGE BENEFITS IN ELEMENTARY YEARS 21
Devlin, K. (2018) Most European students are learning a foreign language in school while
european-students-are-learning-foreign-language-in-school-while-americans-lag/
http://search.proquest.com.ezproxy.gvsu.edu/docview/62796813?accountid=39473
Fan, P. (2016). The social consequences of early multilingual exposure (Order No.
http://search.proquest.com.ezproxy.gvsu.edu/docview/1824558631?accountid=39473
Fan, S., Liberman, Z. Keysar, B. Kinzler, K. (2015). The exposure advantage: Early exposure to
26(7), 1090-1097.
http://dx.doi.org.ezproxy.gvsu.edu/10.1177/0267658317744008
Geva, E., & Siegel, L. (2000). Orthographic and cognitive factors in the concurrent development
of basic reading skills in two languages. Reading and Writing, 12, 1–30.
http://search.proquest.com.ezproxy.gvsu.edu/docview/62443908?accountid=39473
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Fennema-Notestine, C., & Morris, S. K. (2005). Bilingualism
affects picture naming but not picture classification. Memory and Cognition, 33(7),
1220–1234. https://doi-org.ezproxy.gvsu.edu/10.3758/BF03193224
SECOND LANGUAGE BENEFITS IN ELEMENTARY YEARS 22
Kamenetz, A. (2106). 6 potential brain benefits of bilingual education. National Public Radio.
https://www.npr.org/sections/ed/2016/11/29/497943749/6-potential-brain-benefits-of-
bilingual-education
Lindsey, K. A., Manis, F. R., & Bailey, C. E. (2003). Prediction of first-grade reading in
482–494.
http://search.proquest.com.ezproxy.gvsu.edu/docview/62169317?accountid=39473
Luk, G. (2003). Exploring the latent factors behind inter-language correlations in reading and
Ontario, Canada.
Marian, V., & Shook, A. (2012). The cognitive benefits of being bilingual. Cerebrum : the Dana
http://dana.org/Cerebrum/2012/The_Cognitive_Benefits_of_Being_Bilingual/
Marian, V., & Spivey, M. (2003) Bilingual and monolingual processing of competing lexical
L. Obler (Eds.), Bilingualism across the lifespan: Aspects of acquisition, maturity and
http://search.proquest.com.ezproxy.gvsu.edu/docview/63149697?accountid=39473
Nicolay, A., & Poncelet, M. (2013). Cognitive advantage in children enrolled in a second-
SECOND LANGUAGE BENEFITS IN ELEMENTARY YEARS 23
language immersion elementary school program for three years. Bilingualism: Language
http://dx.doi.org.ezproxy.gvsu.edu/10.1017/S1366728912000375
Oral Proficiency Levels in the Workplace. (2012) American Council on the Teaching of Foreign
Perani, Daniela; Abutalebi, Jubin (2015). Bilingualism, dementia, cognitive and neural reserve.
doi: 10.1097/WCO.0000000000000267
Steele, J. L., Slater, R., Li, J., Zamarro, G., & Miller, T. (2015). Costs and effects of dual-
Educational Effectiveness.
http://search.proquest.com.ezproxy.gvsu.edu/docview/1773212921?accountid=39473
Stein-Smith K. (2016) A Brief History of Foreign Language Learning in the United States.
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-34159-0_5
http://dx.doi.org.ezproxy.gvsu.edu/10.1037/0096-3445.121.1.15
http://dx.doi.org.ezproxy.gvsu.edu/10.1017/S0261444811000486
Thierry, G., & Wu, Y. J. (2007). Brain potentials reveal unconscious translation during foreign-
Wimmer, H. & Perner, W. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining
103–128. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5
http://dx.doi.org.ezproxy.gvsu.edu/10.1080/15248372.2011.539524
SECOND LANGUAGE BENEFITS IN ELEMENTARY YEARS 25
Appendices
Appendix A
Scope and Sequence of Language Performance
Advanced
Intermediate
Novice
Appendix B
The Language Implementation Timeline
Appendix C
The Shareholder’s Survey
Please use this community wide survey to capture norms and values
*Required
1. Name * ________________________________________________________________
2. Email *_________________________________________________________________
English
Spanish
French
Arabic
Other _________________________________________________________
5. Closing thoughts of how language learning within the elementary years will be
beneficial for the students within this district. *
Appendix D
Staff language course evaluation
Staff, please submit feedback regarding the implementation of language in your classrooms as a
general overview.
*Required
3. Contribution to learning *
Mark only one oval per row
Very
Poor Fair Satisfactory Good Excellent
Level of skill/knowledge at
start of course
Level of skill/knowledge at end
of course
Contribution of course to your
student’s skill/knowledge to
speak a new language
Contribution of course to your
student’s skill/knowledge of
cultures
5. Course content
Mark only one oval per row
Strongly Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly
Disagree Agree
Learning objectives were clear
Course content was organized
and well planned
Course workload was
appropriate
Course was organized to allow
all students to participate fully
within the target language
Course was organized to allow
all students to gain cultural
perspectives
The signature of the individual below indicates that the individual has read and approved the project of
Brooke J Visser-Elenbaas in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Masters of
Educational Leadership.
______________________________________________________
Accepted and approved on behalf of the Accepted and approved on behalf of the
___________________________________ _________________________________
__________________________________ __________________________________
Date Date
SECOND LANGUAGE BENEFITS IN ELEMENTARY YEARS 32