The Auditory Analysis Test: An Initial Report
The Auditory Analysis Test: An Initial Report
A new test for auditory perception, the Audi- edged the importance of auditory perception
tory Analysis Test, was given to 284 children in (Evans, 1969;Dykstra, 1968; Smith, 1968), but
kindergarten through grade 6. The instrument, offered little in the way of operational defini-
consisting of 40 items, asks the testee to repeat tions except in such general terms as "listening
a spoken word, then to repeat it again without skills." Many pre-reading auditory training pro-
certain specified phonemic elements - such as a grams have been developed that provide the
beginning, ending or medially-positioned con- child with discrimination tasks that are similar,
sonant sound. Seven categories of item diffi- though not identical, to the test items -
culty were proposed. Test results varied, both apparently with the anticipation that whatever
within and between class groups. Performance basic behaviors are required for competent
tended to improve with age and grade place- auditory discrimination will be realized through
ment Pearson Product-Moment Correlations of repeated practice and generalized to other
individual AAT scores with Stanford Achieve- situations such as reading class. The published
ment Test reading scores yielded significant instruments for testing auditory perception that
relationships (p<01) ranging from .53 (grade
we examined were primarily concerned with
I) to .84 (grade 3). Analysis of errors supported
hearing acuity or, at best, discrimination skills
the validity of test item difficulty and provided
(Weprnan, 1958; Murphy and Durrell, 1949;
direction for the design of a treatment ap-
STAP, 1969). Some provide data supporting
proach to auditory perceptual dysfunction.
the tests' validity for predicting reading per-
The ability to differentiate the sounds of the formance. These tests, however, usually 1
require
language, both in listening and speaking, has responses that are limited to "yes-no, ' "same-
long been recognized as an important factor in different," or the like. Such responses provide a
learning to read. The literature concerned with minimum of information concerning the proc-
the teaching of reading has repeatedly acknowl- esses used to produce the assessed behaviors.
The research reported herein was supported and Few instruments have been constructed that
preprinted by the Learning Research and Developmentattempt to analyze the processes that contri-
Center which is supported in part as a research and bute to auditory perception as related to
development center by funds from the United States reading. One exception, a phoneme blending
Office of Education, Department of Health, Educa- test by Jean Chall (1963), does require behav-
tion, and Welfare. The opinions expressed in this
publication do not necessarily reflect the position or iors more complex than "same-different" dis-
policy of the Office of Education, and no official crimination responses. Chall relates her instru-
endorsement should be inferred. ment to certain reading skills and has provided
ITHM ANALYSIS
To assess the relative difficulty children had
with individual words in performing the audi-
tory analysis task, item responses were rank
ordered according to several different methods.
Some of the schemes took account of the fact
that not all children were tested on the entire
list; some did not take this into account.
Analysis by all these methods, however, yielded
only small differences in ordering. Further-
more, the first four task "types" (14 words
see Table III) remained at the top of every list.
Table VIII presents the test words ordered by
the simplest, most straight-forward method a
count of correct responses in each class - with
some confidence that this ranking represents
the average difficulty of individual words for
children in grades 1 through 6.
Table IX, which presents the mean percent-
age of correct responses by word type at each
grade level, shows that the children beyond the
first grade found it easier to omit an initial
sound than to omit a final sound. As predicted,
in general it proved harder to omit the first
sound of an initial consonant blend than of an
initial consonant-vowel combination, and still
more difficult to omit a medial consonant. The
longer Type VII words put an additional strain
on short-term memory, and, except for "cre-
ate" and "reproduce," all of these words appear
at the end of the list.
The interclass data indicate that the ability
to respond appropriately to the AAT improved
for all word types in step with maturation
and/or improved reading skills among children
nine cells contain the total number of subjects in kindergarten through 3rd grade. Word Types
in grades 1 through o. Their z-scores have been I through IV were apparently not of sufficient
sorted into one o( three categories: from +1.00 difficulty to provide discriminating powers
upward, between -0.l)l> to +0.1)1), and from within grades 4, 5, and 6. The more difficult
-1.00 downward. Only one child (grade 2) types (V, VI, VII), however, reflect changes
achieved an AAT z-score o\ +1.00 or better and that, again, may be related to age and grade
a language arts <L.A.) z-score- of -1.00 or less. placement.
No child displayed the opposite results that
is, an AAT z-score of -1.00 or less combined DISCUSSION
with an L.A. z-score or +1.00 or better. Thus, The AAT seems to provide a systematic method
in addition to favorable correlation coefficients, of assessing the degree to which a child has
we observe that, in all but one instance, learned to sort, order, and synthesize the
children who performed well above average in perceptual elements of auditory information. In
one of the measured tasks never performed well respect to the initial question asked by this
below average in the other. study, a relatively wide performance range was
46
TABLE IX. Mean percent correct responses to grade. The prior program was based on teaching
word types by grade level the process of decoding a linguistically regular
sound-symbol system. The new program is
GmdeUvei much more analytical in nature, It initially
Typt K 2 3 4 stresses a precise, structured decoding approach
to reading in which individual letters and
I 80.0 100.0 97.6 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 their sounds are taught. After the child has
II 52.0 77.4 82.9 97.3 96.6 100.0 100.0
Ul 20.0 81.8 80.9 91.6 94.9 94.5 94.0 mastered a predetermined number of pho-
IV 7,0 70.2 86.9 93.7 94.8 97.6 97.0 neme-grapheme combinations, he is taught to
V 6.3 44.3 42.9 60.8 63.8 71.1 74.7 blend the individual phonemes into units that
VI 0.5 22.6 33.5 53.1 56.9 62.1 74.1
VII 0.2 13.7 24.6 29J 33.4 38.3 44.9 form the basis of his beginning reading vocabu-
lary. Does the very narrow gap between the
grades 1 and 2 AAT scores, reported here,
achievement? The answer to this question is suggest a better than average first grade, or a
essential before any firm conclusion regarding difference in the teaching method? We do not
cause and effect is to be drawn. yet have any other samples for comparison. If
Yet another factor must be mentioned. In the new teaching system does assist the child in
our judgment, reading is primarily an auditorily developing more analytical auditory skills
based skill. The analysis of the visual symbols which, in turn, support him in learning to read
presented in reading is less demanding, and more efficiently, the AAT may be useful in
perhaps less important to the typical first grade assessing the effectiveness and uniqueness of
child, than the analysis of verbal sounds. Visual that instructional program. If this is the case,
analysis is, nonetheless, a factor in learning to there are implications for pre-reading instruc-
read. With this in mind, we examined the effect tion in the skills needed for successful AAT
upon the correlation between L.A. and AAT at performance.
the first grade level after a visual-motor score*
was added to the AAT. The correlation coeffi- One final consideration: Are there individual
cient did rise - from .53 to .62, accounting for differences in modality preference? Are there
an additional ten percent of the variance. The certain children who would profit more from a
correlation between the first grade visual-motor program that strongly stressed the visual differ-
scores and language arts, omitting the AAT ences of graphemes before introducing the
scores, on the other hand, was only .32. phonetic relationships? Are there children of
The third question asked what could be the opposite type - those who initially should
learned from the item analysis regarding the be taught to analyze verbal sounds and, once
structuring of a training program. Enough data having achieved some skills in sorting and
are available to support the basic sequence of ordering the acoustic elements of the language,
the word types. A thorough analysis of error be taught the graphic code by relating it to
responses directs us to a more reliable and valid sound? Is it logical to expect children to
AAT, and, what is even more important to a progress equally well with either of the two
refined recognition of the criteria for struc- approached or some combination of them? Not
turing the learning objectives into a hierarchy. necessarily; yet in many schools the same
The fourth question, the implication of this reading program is provided for all. Is it
type of study to reading curriculum designers, coincidental that in many schools, also, a
is an important and interesting one. The school significant percentage of the students display
in which our testing was conducted introduced reading problems not explainable by IQ or
a new early reading program to this year's first other predictors? Clearly, the answers are not
yet available; and indeed these questions merit
*The visual-motor score is derived from a quantitative
investigation. Individual differences - apti-
assessment of the subject's responses to the Gesell tudes - do exist. Consideration should be given
Copy Forms and Rutgers Drawing Test. to the design of instructional programs that
NEWS
THE CHILDREN'S TELEVISION Workshop's incorporating the show into regular classroom
new TV show for teaching reading skills to instruction is being prepared for teachers,
children between the ages of seven and 10 will administrators and curriculum supervisors.
be on the air in September. It was described by The CTW Director of Research Edward L.
several Workshop officials at the annual meet- Palmer indicated that pretesting and posttesting
ing of the International Reading Association. of the series will be conducted by both his staff
The new show will have an identity separated and an independent evaluation group to meas-
from Sesame Street, acknowledged Samuel V. ure the impact of the series. He also revealed
Gibbon, who heads the reading effort. It will be that five full-length prototype shows are being
broadcast in the late afternoon and will also be presented to a selected target audience, and a
available earlier in the day for in-school use. test battery is being developed by the Center for
Each program will be independent, so that the the Study of Evaluation at UCLA to determine
viewer would not be required to have seen any curriculum objectives.
previous program. "Within each program," Gib-
EDWARD C. FRIERSON, director of the Nash-
bon asserted, "the instruction would be se-
ville Learning Center, author and editor, will
quenced from simple to complex, reflecting the
speak at the fall symposium of the Association
entire range of skills represented in the curric-
of Learning Disabilities Teacher-Consultants,
ulum."
October 2, 1971. The symposium is scheduled
Ahwanetta Cutler, the curriculum consultant at the East Brunswick High School, East Bruns-
for the show, said that several curriculum wick, N. J., from 9 a.m. to 1 p.m., and costs $1
principles and strategies would be utilized, and for members, $5 for non-members. Register
printed materials will be distributed to both through Noreen Gallagher, Morris County Dept.
children and teachers. Also, a guide including of Education, Court House, Morristown, N. J.
curriculum descriptions and suggestions for 07960.