02 Colour
02 Colour
1 2
:KDWLV/LJKW" /LJKWDV:DYHV
• When light flies through space and • Maxwell described the electromagnetic
interacts with itself, it behaves as a wave. spectrum and showed that visible light was
• When light interacts with matter, it just part of the spectrum.
behaves as a particle.
• The truth is more complicated than both of 102
these! 100 Radio waves
2
10 700 nm
Red
4
10 Orange
Infrared
6 Yellow
10
8
Visible Green
10
Ultraviolet Blue
10
10 Indigo
X-rays
10 12 Violet
400 nm
Wavelength (meters)
3 4
/LJKWDV3DUWLFOHV :KDW:H'R:LWK/LJKW
• At any given moment, a light source emits The eyes and brain turn an incoming emission
some relative amount of photons at each spectrum into a discrete set of values.
frequency.
• We can plot the emission spectrum of a Mathematically, this is accomplished by
light source as power vs. wavelength. integrating the product of emission spectrum
with each of the three cone response curves.
0HDVXULQJWKH(PLVVLRQ
1HZWRQ·V([SHULPHQWV
6SHFWUXP
Newton showed that a prism spreads apart a light
source’s emission spectrum in space (why?).
– Light emerging from the prism cannot be
further decomposed.
– Newton called the colours of these “atomic”
lights primaries.
– We call one-colour light monochromatic.
A spectroradiometer
Newton’s experimental setup (Wandell 4.1)
7 8
(PLVVLRQ6SHFWUXPLV
&RORXU0DWFKLQJ
QRW&RORXU
• Recall how much averaging the eye does. Conjecture: every colour can be uniquely
Light is infinite dimensional! expressed as a mixing of a small number of
primaries. (Why is this plausible?)
• Different light sources can evoke exactly
the same colours. Such lights are called If true, this gives us a meaningful definition of
“metamers”. colour as a set of primaries and the range of
possible combinations between them.
Given a choice of primaries, how can we
verify the conjecture?
7KH&RORXU0DWFKLQJ &RORXU0DWFKLQJ
([SHULPHQW )XQFWLRQV
Subject attempts to match a combination of A colour matching experiment yields a colour
primaries with a test light. matching function for each primary.
It turns out that three primaries suffice to Colour matching functions for primaries at 460, 530
and 650 nm (Wasserman 3.3)
produce all perceivable colours.
How can we use the matching functions to
The experiment tells us a lot more about
match an arbitrary monochromatic light?
colour perception…
11 What about an arbitrary light? 12
&KRRVLQJ3ULPDULHV &RORXUHG6XUIDFHV
So far, we’ve discussed the colours of lights.
How do surfaces acquire colour?
It turns out that colour matching functions are A surface’s reflectance is its tendency to reflect
always linearly related! Why? incoming light across the spectrum.
,OOXVWUDWLRQRI&RORXU
6XEWUDFWLYH0HWDPHUV
$SSHDUDQFH
7KH&,(&KURPDWLFLW\
7KH&,(&RORXU%ORE
'LDJUDP
21 22
3HUFHSWXDO
QRQ 8QLIRUPLW\
The XYZ colour space is not perceptually
uniform!
25 26
+69 &0<
More natural for user interaction, corresponds to A subtractive colour space used for printing.
the artistic concepts of tint, shade and tone.
Involves three subtractive primaries:
The HSV space looks like a “hexcone”: • Cyan - subtracts red
• Magenta - subtracts green
• Yellow - subtracts blue
Mixing two pigments subtracts their opposites
from white.
27 28
5*%YV&0< <,4
29 30
6XPPDU\ 5HDGLQJ
Many different spectra can have the same colour. Such spectra are Further reading:
called metamers. • Gerald S. Wasserman. Color Vision: An Historical
Introduction. John Wiley & Sons, New York, 1978
The CIE XYZ system is a standardized colour space defined in
terms of three matching functions. The chromaticity diagram, • Brian Wandell. Foundations of Vision. Sinauer
derived from the XYZ space, gives a useful interpretation of colour. Associates, Sunderland, MA, 1995.
31 32