Concept Maps and Processes of Comprehens
Concept Maps and Processes of Comprehens
The author has granted a non- L'auteur a accordé une licence non
exclusive Licence allowing the exclusive permettant à la
National Library of Canada to Bibliothèque nationale du Canada de
reproduce, loan, distri'bute or sell reproduire, prêter, disûî'buer ou
copies of this thesis in microform, vendre des copies de cette thèse sous
paper or electronic formats. la forme de microfiche/fb, de
reproduction sur papier ou sur format
électronique.
Concept maps are said to represent mental models of problems that form in working
memory. Integration of theories of cognition indicates that the structural and procedural
knowledge of concept map links are comparably complex. Integration funher suggeas that
comparably complex cognitive and metacognitive processes govem concept mapping. This
thesis develops methods to mess the structural and procedural knowledge of concept maps,
structurai knowledge in concept maps. The adaptation implies that five levels of inference
afsessment scale for procedural knowledge. Applying the methods to students' concept maps,
shows that their structural and procedural knowledge is comparably complex. It is argued
that concept map procedural knowledge and cognitive processes are the same phenornenon.
Therefore, the procedural knowledge assessrnent scaie is used to rneasure cognition. A set of
interview questions is developed to assess metacognition. The methods indicate that snidents'
Confonnity between concept map properties and the predictions of theory integration is said
2.0 How concept maps are used and the impetus for this thesis
3.6 Scripts 32
3.1 1 A concept rnap to illustrate intesration of structural knowledge and schema theory 40
4. i A description of metacognition
5.1 Novak and Gowin ( 1984) and the structure of concept maps
6.0 Concept map assessment methods and the goals of the thesis
7.6 Assessing the structural knowledge of entire maps and individual concepts
7.7Weaknesses of Method 1
9.1.1 Subject 1
9.3.2 Subject 2
9.2.3 Subject 6
9.3 A summary of the chapter
10.0 An o v e ~ e w
of the chapter
Chapter I l Conclusions
Bibliography
Appendices
Appendix A
Appendix B
Appendix C
Appendix D
Appendi~E
vii
Tables
Figures
This thesis is primarily concemed with the argument that concept maps represent
mental models that form in working memory in response to problems or situations. Theones
of cognition (structural knowledge (Jonassen. Beissner and Yacci. 1993), schema theory
(Marshall, 1999, semantic network theory (Ashcrafl, 1989), and spreading activation theory
(Collins and Loftus. 1975)are integrated to identify properties of the structural and
procedural knowledge associated with mental models. lntegration of the theories is also used
to develop three rnethods for the assessrnent of structural and procedural knowledge in
concept maps. When audents' maps were analyzed with these methods they were found to
exhibit properties that parallel the predicted properties of mental models. This conformity
between the properties of concept maps and mental models is said to be supportive of the
The thesis begins by considering the integration of four theories of cognition (schema
theory, stmcnirai knowledge theory, semantic networking theory and spreading activation
theoiy) and concept map theory. Integration is based on the argument that components of
different theories that perform the sarne function are actuaiiy the same phenomenon. The
process indicates that the structural and procedural knowledge of schemata develop
simultaneously, and with isomorphic' quality. It also indicates that these knowledge forms are
'By isomorphic 1 mean that structurai and procedural knowledge develop simultaneously
in response to the sarne stimuli. Increases in the quality of structural knowledge are associated
with concomitant increases in the quality of procedural knowledge.
operative during the creation of mental models. The second goal of the thesis is to develop
methods for the assessrnent of structurai and procedural knowledge in concept maps. When
these methods are applied to students' concept maps they indicate t hat their structural and
procedural knowledge are isomorphic in quality. Therefore it is argued that concept maps
exhibit properties that compare to the properties of mental models as predicted by theory
integration. This finding suggests that the rationale for theory inteyration is correct, it is also
said to lend weight to the notion that concept maps represent mental models.
Three methods are developed for concept map analysis. Method I assesses the quality
reconciliation and progressive differentiation associated with individual concepts. This use of
methods 1 and 2 complements the earlier work of Novak and Gowin ( 1984). These workers
enumerated links to provide total scores for concept maps. In counting links and awarding
them a score. Novak and Gowin bundled the effects of progressive differentiation and
The third goal of the thesis seeks to strengthen the argument that concept maps
nnicnual and procedural knowledge. Theones of reading comprehension (Jacobs and Paris,
1987 and Kirby, 1991) contend that the cognitive and metacognitive processes that govem an
3
instance of reading are comparable in their complexity. These theories are integrated with
concept map theory by way of a Functional equivalence argument. Integration indicates that
the same cognitive and metacognitive processes control reading comprehension and concept
map creation. lt also susgens that the procedural knowledge of concept mapping is
measuring the quality of procedural knowledge. Method 3 is said to assess the cognitive
especially for this thesis. When Method 3 and the interview scnpt were applied to students'
concept maps, they indicated that cognition and metacognition are comparably complex
processes. This finding indicates that concept maps exhibit properties that conform to the
predictions of theory integation. It therefore supports the rationale for theory integation and
by extension it lends weight to the notion that concept maps represent mental models.
Two descriptions of the thesis are provided in the following pages. Chapter 2
provides an introductory description of the goals and arguments of the thesis. Section 2.0
provides some background information about concept maps. It also descnbes the impetus for
this research. Section 2.1 introduces the thesis and its ~oals.In section 2.2 the argument that
concept maps represent mental models is discussed. The cognitive and metacognitive
processes of concept mapping are described in section 2.3. Methods 1,2 and 3 are described
in section 1.4 and section 3.5 descnbes the structure of concept map links. Section 2.6 closes
present the psychological validity argument. Chapters 5,6 and 7 descnbe methods of concept
map andysis and the empincal component of the thesis is descnbed in chapter 8. Chapters 9
and 10 descriibe the assessrnent of the concept maps of a gifleci grade 9 biology class. These
students were select& for the snidy as it was thought that their academic ability would
increase the likelihood that high quality links would be produced. The students were also
chosen as they were snidying environmental biology. Al1 the maps created in this study were
based upon a target text that descnbes an aspect of environmental biology. The text was
concems water pollution and oii slicks. Conclusions derived from chapters 9 and 10 are
presented in chapter 1 1.
Chapter 2
2.0 Bon concept maps are used and the impetus for this thesis
Concept maps have been put toward a multitude of educational uses. In a discussion
of their utility, Edmondson and Smith (1996) make note of applications for teaching and
learning. Two of the most important applications are, increasing the potential for recalî and
maximizing the usefulness of knowledge. To explain, concept map creation requires the
reprocessing of extant conceptions. Reprocessing strengthens the links between concepts and
thereby increases the potential for h r e recall. The usefûiness of knowledge can be
increased if students reaect on the content of a map. Such reflection provides an awareness
of the "big picturen as it allows for link creation across multiple themes. Multiple thematic
Jacobson and Coulson, 199 1a). Knowiedge transfer can provide unique solutions to problems
as the thematic connections that make it possible are unique to their creator. Teachers can
actively promote the creation of multiple thematic connections by using several concept maps
to present the same knowledge. Each of these maps should present the topic from a dinerent
perspective or theme. Aithough this process makes thematic connections expiicit, dadents
can still create their own connections if they are provided with oppominities for reflection.
m e r uses that teachers have for concept maps include: planning curricula, evaluating
leaming, identdyurg erroneous thinking and making qualitative judgements about the
Edmondson and Smith, 1996, Fisher, 1996, Liu, 1994, Morhe-Dershimer, 1993).
The usefulness of concept maps is often based on the assumption that they render
mental models available for analysis. That is, concept maps are assurned to represent
schernata in some way.Schemata are components of long term memory that develop in
maps represent schemata has proved to be a difficult undertaking. Part of the problem is that
educators are still debating the very existence of schemata. This leaves advocates of concept
maps in the doubly precarious position of having to extol the virtues of a tool that
theoretically explicates (on some unidentifieci level) a phenomenon that is itself only
theoreticai in nature.
The difficulty of determining if concept maps are reflective of schernata provided the
impenis for this thesis. An investigation was launched into the nature of mental models and
concept maps. The author felt that the use of concept rnaps in education is inhibited by a lack
of evidence to indicate thiit they represent mental models. It was hoped that a greater
primary goal of this thesis. The second goal is to develop methds for concept map analysis
and the third is to assess the cognitive and metacopnitive processes of concept mapping. The
latter goal is designed to provide further information about model representation by concept
maps. The second goal led to the production of three concept map assessrnent methods
(Method 1, Method 2 and Method 3) and an intewiew script. Two factors enabled the
development of these methods. The first was the argument that enabled the integration of
concept map theory and the four theories of cognition. When M y developed it suggested
that the structural and procedural knowledge of mental models and concept maps are
isomorphic in quality. It also revealed a iittle of the nature of these knowledge fonns.
them. The second factor was a description of the cognitive and metacognitive processes of
concept rnapping. Here again, descriptions of the processes revealed their nature and
Figure 1 sumrnarizes the goals of the thesis. in regard to the fint goal it illustrates
that structural and procedural knowledge are said to have isomorphic quality in concept
maps. It also suggests that this isomorphism can be checked by way of Methods 1 and 3.
Finally Figure 1 illustrates that the comprehension processes (cognitive and metacognitive)
that govem concept map creation can be elucidated through an i n t e ~ e wand the third
An argument is laid out in this thesis wherein concept maps are said to represent
mental models of problems. These mental rnodels are said to consist of components of
schemata and the problern that stimulateci formation of the model. Supportive evidence for
this argument is sought by assessing whether the structural and procedural knowledge of
concept maps has comparable properties to that of mental models. Fwther evidence for the
argument is sought fiom an investigation hto the cognitive and metacognitive processes of
concept mapping. Here again, the purpose of the investigation is to determine whether these
processes exhibit comparable properties in concept maps and mental models.
Before a more detailed sumrnary of the thesis' argument and investigations can be
provided, some background knowledge of schema theory and concept rnap theory is
required.
Schemata develop fiom repeat experiences (travelling to work, making a cup of tea etceteni)
with an action or problem (Marshall 1995). They are components of long tena memory that
consist of Uitercomected nodes. Explanations for the co~ectionsbetween nodes are known
as schema knowledge. The quality of schema knowledge inmeases as experience with the
action or problern is acquired. During initial exposure to an action or problem type, the
Concept map links are created by a form of procedural knowledge we will cal1 link-
procedural knowledge. The prefix "link" distinguishes this form of procedural knowledge
fiom the procedwal knowledge that enables the performance of tactile operations (sawing
between concepts. Structural knowledge is the expression of understanding that connects two
concepts in a concept map link. It results from the actions of link-procedural knowledge.
That is, link-procedural knowledge foms a basis of understanding that is used to generate a
Descriptions of the components of schema theory and concept maps suggest that
These descriptions also suggest that the procedural and schema (structural) knowledge of
schemata have isomorphic qualities and are active during concept map creation. An
investigation into the two knowledge forms that finds they exhibit comparable quaiity in
concept maps lends would lend weight to the rationale for theory Unegration. By extension it
would also be supportive of the notion that concept maps represent mental models. This is
Besides the notion that structurai and link-procedurd knowledge are isomorphic,
10
theory integration provides two additional revelations that are wonhy of note. The 6rst is the
process by which schema components are retrieved and subsequently expressed in concept
maps. The second is the manner in which structural knowledge guides the actions of
procedural knowledge.
Semantic network theory and spreading activation theory indicate how schema
components are retrieved from long term memory and subsequently expressed. These
theories maintain that constraints of problems (or any environmental stimuli) are carried to
working memory where they provoke a search of long term memory. During this search,
nodes that are contex~allyrelated to the constraints may become activated. Nodes that attain
a threshold level of activation can be retrieved to working memory. Once in working memory
the nodes' relationship to the problem's constraints may be reflected upon and manipulated.
The results of this refleçtive process are used to form a mental mode1 of the problem. The
Jonassen, Beissner and Yacci (1993) refmed to the process by which structural
knawfedge. The knowledge that concept maps represent mentai models of problems was used
describe the sdolding process. Suppose a machinist has to cut a circular hole in a sheet of
aluminum. Suppose also that the hole has to be sized so that a cyündrical bar will pass
through it and create an annulus between the bar and the edge of the hole that is 1 m h e t r e
thick (i.e. the diarneter of the hole has to be 2mrn p a t e r than the diarneter of the bar).
F i y suppose that the ody information available to the machinist is the circumference of
11
the bar. According to Marshall, when a problem is encountered its constraints are carried
from the environment to working memory by way ofa sensory register. Marshall also noted
that components of schemata are recalled to working memory in response to the problem's
constraints. In the hypothdcal problem, the sensory register would carry the circderence
of the bar (20mm)and other problem constraints (create an annulus with a diameter that is
2mm greater than that of the bar) to working memory. Recailed schema components would
knowledge). The rewlt is a mental model that is used by procedurai knowledge to develop
plans for problem resolution. The mental model of the hypothetical problem might be that
"2xr = 20mrn." Procedural knowledge might devise the following steps for resolution: divide
accommodate the annuius and to determine the radius of the circular hole. Procedural
knowledge would then carry out the steps of this plan and find that the circle requires a
radius of 4.183mm.That i s procedural knowledge uses the mental model to develop a plan
of action for problem remlution. Given that mental models are constniaed from s t m d
procedurai knowledge.
ùi the following sections of this chapter the goals of the thesis are discussed in greater
detail. Section 2.2 considers the integration of theories of cognition as means for
12
investigating the nature of the stmctural and procedural knowledge of concept maps. The
isomorphic nature of cognitive and metacognitive activity is discussed in section 2.3. Section
2.4 presents an aside, it describes the structure of concept map links.The anaiytical methods
and the i n t e ~ e wscript that are used to assess structural knowledge, procedural knowledge
and metacognition are presented in section 2.5. Finally, a description of the empirical
This thesis seeks to investigate the nature of the aructural and procedural knowledge
associated with concept maps. The nature of these knowledge forms is revealed through an
integration of concept map theory and four theories of cognition (xhema theory, suuctural
Descriptions of the theones reveal that cenain components in different t heories perforrn the
same functions. These fiinctionally equivalent cornponents are thought to represent different
views of the same phenornenon. Components that occur in multiple theories represent points
Several assertions about the nature of knowledge are derived fkom the integration
process. For example, structurai and procedural knowledge are said to develop
c o n a m . For example, she reports that researchers (Anderson, 1983 and SteRenson, Joag-
dev and Anderson, 1979) have demonstrateci that performance is influenced by prior
expenence. Marshall aiso cites Bransford and Johnson's (1972) work in which stories are
Evidence for the psychological validity of schemata lends weight to the argument that
concept maps are useful educational tools. In this thesis it is assened that schema knowledge
is functionally equivalent to the structural knowledge of concept maps. Concept map theory
holds that mental models contain elements of a problem that have entered working memory.
It also holds that mental models contain components of schemata that are retneved from long
term memory in response to the problem's components. tn this thesis, it is argued that the
structural and procedural knowledge of schemata and mental models are isomorphic in
quality. It is aiso argued that mental models are expressed in concept maps. When the
structural and procedural knowledge of concept maps are assessed they are found to exhibit
isomorphism. The hdîng is taken to indicate that the knowledge associated with concept
maps has properties that compare to the structural and procedural knowledge of mental
models. By extension the finding suggests that concept maps have properties that compare to
schema theory's description of schemata Evidence for the psychologicai validity of schemata
is therefore supportive of arguments for increashg the use of concept maps in education.
The argument that concept maps emulate theories of cognition would be greatly
assisted if the results of the structural-link-proceduraiknowledge analysis could be
of reading comprehension (Jacobs and Paris, 1987 and Kirby, 1991) with concept map
theory. Again the basis for integration is that tiinctionaily equivalent components (in difiierent
theones) are actually the same phenomenon. This second integration reveals that artifacts of
the cognitive processes of reading comprehension are retrieved fiom long tem rnemory and
expressed in concept map links. More specifically, integration suggests that the cognitive
processes that govem reading comprehension are aiso active during the creation of concept
maps. Reading t heory stipulates t hat cognitive and metacognitive processes are hierarchical
and proportionate. If the rationale for integration is correct then the cognitive and
hding would indicate that concept map components behave in the same manner as their
Sugged that concept maps have a psychological validity that is comparable to that of the
reading theories.
An additional revelation can be drawn from the triangulation argument. Tnat is that
components are t h e same phenomenon, this revelation is said to indicate that iink-procedural
knowledge represents the cognitive activity of concept mapping. Thus the tnangulation
isomorphic to structural knowledge and that its quality is comparable to the metacognitive
processes of concept mapping.
Metacognition and cognition are interactive processes that adapt dynamicaily to the
needs of comprehension while learning occus. To Jacobs and Paris (1987), metacognition is
a general concept that concerns the lemer's knowledge of cognitive tasks such as reading,
memory and leaniing. They also say that it comprises executive arategies that plan and
metacognition operates on a higher level than cognition. He posits that metacognition and
cognitive processes. Thus, the application of a metacognitive strategy results in the adoption
activity of concept mapping. Method 3 was used to assess cognitive processes. Given the
integrated nature of cognitive and metacognitive processes, it was anticipated that students
with high Method 3 scores would also score well on the UrteMew. Such a finding would
support the rationale for theory integration. That is, it wouid imply that concept map
The three analytical methods require that concept map Links be constnicted in a
particular way. This Link structure is now discussed dong with an introduction to the nature
m r e 2, The typicai link structure that is requireâ for this thesis
and link-procedural kwwledge. The top half of Figure 2 is an illustration of the ideal link
knowledge). This linking statement summarizes the mapper's understanding of the tink. Items
of data are subsumeci beneath it. Each d a m describes a component of the relationship that is
expressed in the linking statement. Linking statements are formed by generating an inference
that connects data. The connedon should reveal the nature of the relationship that associates
the concepts of the link.
A hypothetical link comecting the concepts Aneries and Veim provides a concrete
example of the requisite Iùik structure. It is also used to illustrate the nature of link-
procedurai knowledge.
Linking Statement: Arteries and veins are co~ectedby capillaries.
Data:
b. Arteries cany oxygen rich blood f'rom the heart to the tissues.
Suppose that this link was created after reading a text that provided the data presented
above. Suppose dso that this text fded to explain the structural arrangement of arteries,
veins and capillaries. Finally suppose that the mapper's objective when creating the link was
to determine how blood flows from arteries to capillaries to veins. The following paragraph
hypothesizes about how the scorer of a concept map might associate the data and linking
staternent of this link to deduce the fùnctioning of the mapper's link-procedural knowledge.
co~ectedto fom an inference about the structural arrangement of artenes, veins and
knowledge might have first considered the oxygen status of blood in artenes and veins. Next
the second and third data items may have been considered as they reveal that arterial blood
bas an abundance of oxygen and that vascuiar blood has a dearth. Next, the knowledge that
capillaries are the site of oxygen exchange rnight have been added to the equation . When the
knowledge that arteries, veios and capillaries fom a continuous system is added to the
picture, an inference about the structurai arrangement can be formed. This iaference rnight be
i8
"if artenes have lots of oxygen and veins have ody a little, then oxygen must be lost or
gained as blood passes between them. If capillaries are the site of oxygen exchange and blood
passes through a continuous systern, then capillaries mua comect artenes and veins."
Aithough this example is a simple one, it illustrates how link-procedural knowledge seeks
connections between items of data. tt also illustrates that al1 connections across data are
Many of the maps created for this thesis contained over 25 links. Placing 25 linking
aatements and their data on a map can lead to clutter so that the logic of the mapper is
difficult to follow. To reduce this tendency toward clutter, the hidents of this study were
asked to number the links of their maps and place the linking statements and data on the
reverse side of the page. This is the Link structure illustrateci in the bottom half of Figure 2.
The second goal of this thesis is to develop methods for concept rnap analysis. Three
methods are developed in total. Method 1 assesses the qudity of structural knowledge. It
undemanding of schema connections, which occurs as a result of the process of learning. The
nrain that is placed on working memory during link creation This quantification is achieved
question was devised by Biggs and Coltis (1 982) and it is referred to as The Structure of t k
Obserwd Leming Outcorne or SOLO.From lowest to highest the SOLO stages are:
stages involve the juggling of evermore data in working memory. They also require that data
be integrated with increasing creativity. The higher the SOLO stage, the higher the quality of
the link. The higher the quaiity of the link the greater the quality of the integrative
reconciliation.
thinking that concepts are never fully leamed. Evermore links can be added to a node as
links associated with a concept. Method 2's scoring system refen to concepts with just one
link as I-Link concepts. A concept with two links is called 2-Link concept. The more links
the higher the Number-Link score and the greater the progressive dserentiation. Method 2 is
not concerned with the argument for the psychological validity of concept rnaps. It is used
alongside Method 1 to complement the evaluation technique of Novak and Gowin ( 1984).
Novak and Gowin's assesment strategy is often quoted in current research efforts. It requires
the enurneration of links in a rnap. The resulting score combines the effects of integrative
reconciliation and progressive düferentiation. Methods 1 and 2 untangle Novak and Gowin
knowledge is the knowledge that generates the inferences that comect the data items of links.
Linking statements summarize the understanding that is acquired fiom the actions of link-
comparing link descriptions to a d e that recognUes five levels of inference quality. This
scde was developed by integrating the work of Biggs and Collis (1982) and Rumelhart and
N o m (1978). Descriptions of the inference levels and illustrative example links are
provided below. Each example link describes the interaction of factors that govem the
morphology of lakes (lake morphology refers to the shape of the undenvater basin). Although
the example links contain inferences of different quality, they were al1 created fiom the
following statement "Lake morphology is a function of undenvater contour lines, the shape
of the lake, and geologic origin" (Home and Goidman, 1994, p. 14). The concepts Lake and
Mwphougy were used to create examples of level 1, level2, leveI3, and level 5 inferences.
An example level4 inference was created with the concepts Lake and Water Sbucture.
Level 1. At this level inferences involve incorrect reasoning. Two scenarios are
possible. In the k s t scenario, the inference may connect two items of data that are
not tmly associated with the concepts of that iink. Although the inference may be
correct, the nature of the data involved renden it irrelevant to the concepts of the
link. In the second situation data may be appropriate but the inference is incorrect.
Example link:
Linking statement: Lake morphology is determined by the shape of the lake, its
geologic origin and prevailing winds.
a) Morphology is determined by the lake's shape.
b) Morphology is determined by geologic origin.
c) Morphology is determined by prevailing winds.
The third datum, datum "c" is incorrect. Prevailing winds affect the structure of lake water
but they do not impact on morphology. The inference of this link would be rated as incorrect
Example link:
Linking statement: Lake morphology is affecteci by the shape of underwater contour
lines. It is also affecteci by the shape of the lake, and it is also a f f e d by the M e ' s
geologic origin.
a) Morphology is determined by the laketsshape.
b) Morphology is determined by geologic origin.
C) Morphology is determined by contour lines.
The data involved in this link are correct. The level2 rating derives from the structure of the
linking statement. It lists component data but rnakes no attempt to integrate them. The
integration of data constitutes the essence of an iderence. The absence of integration implies
that no inference was fonned. In a sense a level2 rating constitutes fded cornprehension as it
Level3. Inferences based on fact (inferences that replicate descriptions found in the
instruction)
This link replicates the inference that was presented in the instruction. It integrates data in
Level4. Merences based on facts that were not ovenly linked to the concepts in the
instruction (correct reasoning that builds upon information presented in the
instruction).
Example tink:
Linking statement. Once the lake basin is formed, physical, chemical and biological
factors interact to produce discemible structure within the water.
a) Water structure develops d e r the basin is fomed.
b) Water structure is determined by physical factors.
C) Water structure is determined by chemical factors.
d) Water stmcture is determineci by biological factors.
LeveI4 inferences associate concepts in the instruction to related concepts that the mapper
was aware of before the instruction was made available. In this case the concept Water
Stmcl~rewas retrieved fiom the mapper's long term memoty and associated with Water. A
level4 rating is applied whenever extant knowledge is associated with information presented
in the instruction.
Example link:
Linking staternent: Lake morphology is determined by the interaction of underwater
contour lines, lake shape, geologic origin and sedimentation.
a) Morphology is determined by the lake's shape.
b) Morphology is determined by geologic origin.
C) Morphology is determined by contour lines.
d) Morphology is determined by the sedimentation of biologicai matter.
This link incorporates extant knowledge of the deposition of biological matter into the
The interview script was developed fiom the work of Paris and Jacobs ( 1984). These
workers developed a series of questions to assess the metacognitive activity of reading. I"neir
questions were adapted to assess the metacognitive activity of concept mapping. Paris and
Jacobs also provided example answers for the thirteen questions. The answers were also
23
adapted to the interview. Three example answers were developed for each question. Student
answers were rated against the example answers. Students were interviewed individuafly and
class. The study began with the author providing a short lecture to introduce concept
mapping techniques. Students were then asked to read a target text and create a map of it.
The process took place over two consecutive class periods (the Thursday and Friday of a
school week). The target text concemed water pollution and oil spills. When the maps were
complete, the author rated each of them by methods 1 and 3. A positive correlation was
obsened between Method 1 and Method 3 scores. This finding was taken to be supportive
evidence for the notion that the structural and procedural knowledge of concept mapping are
Once the maps were rated, six subjects were selected for metacognitive inte~ew.
The interviews were run over three consecutive lunch houn (the Tuesday, Wednesday and
Thursday) in the week foiiowing the mapping exercise. One interviewee was selected as he
created a map with low Method 3 scores.Two inte~eweeswere chosen as they produced
maps with medium scores. The remaining inte~eweeswere used as they produced maps
with high Method 3 scores. It was a s m e d that high Method 3 scores were achieved by
applyiag superior cognitive processes toward the creation of concept maps. It was aîso
24
assumeci that the higher the interview scores the greater the quality of the metacognitive
processes that were put toward concept map creation. When the Method 3 and i n t e ~ e w
scores of individual students were coneiated they were found to be comparable. The 6nding
was taken to be supportive evidence for the thinking that cognitive and metacognitive
processes have comparable quality. It also indicates that concept maps emdate behavioun
predicted from the integration of concept map theory and theories of cognition. Finaily it
The strategy for integrating concept map theory and the four theories of cognition
(stnictural knowledge theory, schema theory, semantic network theory and spreading
activation theory) can now be descnbed in detail. This description assumes that mental
memory, a sensory register, long term memory, and working memory. Information passes (by
way of the sensory register) from the environment to working memory in response to a
problem. The problem may also stimulate information retneval from long term memory. In
working memory relationships are sought between the information cornponents. Once
identified these relationships permit the construction of new knowledge structures that can be
activation theory. These summaries explain how concepts are retrieved tiom long term
memory and expressed in concept maps. Integration then identifies relationships between
structurai knowledge and schema knowledge. The relationships identifieci are used to develop
is argued that these mental models guide the actions of procedural knowledge. Imegration
also reveais paraiiels between schemata and concept maps. These p d e l s are used to
26
propose a method to investigate the contention that concept maps represent mental models.
related concepts are formed. Each category has a superordinate descriptor concept and a
number of subordinates. The more a subordinate concept is accessed, the shoner its link to
woikers such as Marshall (1995), as it allows for the human capacity for rapid information
retrieval.
the structure of knowledge the processes of retrievai are the same. A description of concept
retrieval is provided by the Collins and Loftus' ( 1975) interpretation of Quillian's (1966)
spreading activation theory. According to Coilins and Loftus, Quillian said that activation of
semantic memory begins with the recognition of input words. Starting nodes become
activated when input words are rewgnized. This activation is foliowed by a search of
semantic memory. At first nodes that are directly attached to the starting nodes become
activated. From there the activation spreads outward to nodes that are attached to the nodes
that are attached to the starting nodes. Activation may continue in this rnanner unal several
When a node is activateci, a "tag" is attached to it that hdicates which node was
activated previously. The tag also rnakes note of the node where activation began (the
starting node). Eventually a node is located that is on the path of the spread of activation
from two or more starting nodes. This intersecting node is evaluated by tracing tags back to
the starting nodes. Once the starting nodes are located the evaluation determines if they are
semantically matched. For exarnple, the input words Full and l e m s may stimulate activation
at the nodes Fall and lemes. This activation may spread to the intersection node Fall leaves.
Tags are traced fkom Fall leaves back to the starting nodes and contexts of al1 three are
compareci. If the concept Fall and the intersection Full lemes both referred to the third
season of the year a point of intersection would be confirmeci. If the input word fdlreferred
to the effect of gravity the intersection Falf ieaves would be rejezted. Intersections are
evaiuated to accommodate multiple meanings of the same terms. Several intersections may be
Collins and Loftus consider the process of intersection evaluation a Little further.
They say that every intersection located in a memory search provides either positive or
negative evidence of semantic matching. For example, locating the concept c u n q is strong
negative evidence for the suitement ail bir& me robinç. Collins and Loftus propose several
processes for evaluating intersections. Each involves the nimmation of evidence until a
decision about a semantic match is made. Only concepts and links that reach a positive
Spreading activation theory reveals the means of concept map creation. Concepts
retneved fiom long term memory are ananged in working memory to form a mental mode1 of
28
a problem. These models are expressed as concept maps. The following sections interpret
structural knowledge theory and schema theory. These interpretations reveal the processes
behind the creation of mental models. They also indicate the role that mental models play in
problem resolution.
theory that is adaptable to our needs. They say that dalarative knowledge is knowing what,
reviewing several studies, Jonassen et al. concluded that anictuml knowledge mediates the
ûther workers add weight to this conclusion. For example, Shavelson (1972) and Thro
(1 975) determined that a student's knowledge structure cornes to resemble that of the
instnictor during leaming. Gordon and Gill(1989) found that problem solving was facilitated
when knowledge structures resembled the insmictor's. Chi and Glaser (1985) deduced that
concluded that havhg the right structure is more important than ability when it cornes to
Mandlef s (1983) study (as cited by Jonassen et al.) claimed that the understanding
then, Mander wouid hold that relationships (stnictural knowledge) contain concept meaning
or more accurately the leamer's understanding of concepts. Jonassen et al. go so fàr as to say
that an understanding of the relationships between concepts are essential for the
comprehension of ideas. If the above is true it implies that structural knowledge is the
platform on which successful problem solving is built. Successful problem solving requires
h e w o r k for the actions of procedural knowledge (Winograd, 1977). Jonassen et ai. say
between concepts. That is, it manifests in concept rnaps as the links between concepts. An
examination of concept map links should therefore reveal the quaiity of structural knowledge
a declarative format. It passes then through a structural phase before it reaches maturity as
procedural knowledge. Kooeman and Jonassen (1 994) add weight to this thinking. They
clah that besides knowing whar, students have to know why before they can know how. This
argument is flawed. Someone can be told to change the oil in a car and they can be told how
to do it. Someone can be told how to wash their hands before preparing a meal. in both
cases, knowing why is not an essentid prerequisite to the performance of the task.
It is possible to infa how not telling sorneone why a task should be performed will
impact on their concept map of a topic. Suppose a person firom a mie with no knowledge of
machines was told how to change a car's oil. Their concept map would likely be a series of
30
boxed concepts (car components involved in the oil change)joined in a linear fashion by
arrowed links. Link explanations in this case would be non-existent. If a person with
experience of machines other than cars were asked to create the same map then their
knowledge of the functions of oil would likely be incorporateci into the links. The greater
their understanding of oil's role in the functioning of machines, the more detailed their link
explanations. The mapper automatically associates the car's oil change procedure to other
machine systems and expresses this association in the links of the oil change map.
Three things are apparent kom the above. First, explanations of concept relationships
are found in concept map links. That is, the knowledge of why (stmctural knowledge) is
found in concept map links. This supports Mander's ( 1983) conception of structural
performance. Third, if the why is missing from an explanation it rnay be inferred from existing
Marshaii ( 1995)traces the meaning of the word scherna back to the ancient Greeks.
Plato and Aristotle thought that a schema was an aspect of memory that coaains a
hnework so that similar phenornena could be recognked in the £Ùture. The modem view of
schemata is that they assist in the resolution as weil as the recognition of problems.
31
Marshall goes on to describe the contribution that Bartlett and Piaget have made to
thought that schema knowledge consists of generalized data denved fiom multiple, similar
interactions with the environment. Marshall provided examples of repetitious interactions that
lead to schema formation: the joumey to work, cooking a favourite meal, and solving
added to the schema. ui the early stages of schema formation the information that is extracted
is specific and concrete. Increasingly abaract information is extracted and added to the
schema over repeat exposure to the action. Eventually the schema contains a generalized or
separate schema fot different repetitious actions. Although people have similar SChemata for
sirnilar actions, dserences arise in response to different prior knowledge as well as different
3.5 Frames
and Rurneihart and Onony, 1977) have been the primary infiuences on schema theory
development. This section concerns Minsky's work with h e thmry. The work of Schank
and Rurnelhart and Ortony is considered in section 3.6. These works are the foundation
blocks of Marshall's schema theory. In tum Marshall's theory serves as a building block for
the descriptions of structural and link-procedural knowledge that are developed in this work.
Like schemata, h e s are theoretical data structures that hold large chunks of reiated
32
information. According to Marshall, Minsky held that vimially any repeat expenence qualifies
as the subject of a frame. Section 3.4 makes note of Marshall's claim that schemata form ffom
repetitious acts such as the joumey to work, cooking a favourite meal etcetera. In Minsky's
theory these acts lead to the formation of frames. Like schema theory, h e theory hoids
that the typical characteristics of an action (or problem type) are extracted from repeat
experiences and nored as the default values of the fiame. Each action or problem type
Schemata and frames are similar in that both can evolve from almost any repeat
type. Before a frame can be applied, the fixed attributes of the problem are matched against
the generalized characteristics of various m e s . Only the fiame that matches the problem's
fixed attributes is applied. During problem resolution the unique aspects of the problem, its
variables, replace the h e ' s default values or dots. If information about problem variables is
incomplet+ the fhne may still be applied by replacing information gaps with default values.
The mechanism that lads to schema application is similar. Various schemata are matched
againa a problem to determine its type. During schema application, default values may be
3.6 Scripts
Another variant on the schema theme is Roger Schank's scripts. A script is "a special
(Marshall, 1995, p. 19). Scripts are concemed with repetitious social events whose
components unfold in a specific order. Acwrding to Marshall, Schank's ( 1975) study
provided a restaurant script as an example. The fixed data of the restaurant script data
unfolds sequentially as the meal progresses. For example, the menu arrives before the food
and the food precedes the tip. Variable data such as the type of food and the particular waiter
are integral to the script just they are in a frame. Marshd viewed scripts as subsets of
schernata that lack the flexibility to resolve new situations and problems.
Marshall embraced M e theory and script theory for their ability to partially explain
her conception of schemata. Rumelhart and Ortony (1 977) provided the following points for
1.Schemata are data structures that represent generic concepts stored in memory.
2. Schemata exist for generalized concepts that underlie objects, situations, events,
3. Schemata are not atoMc. Each contains, as part of its specification, the network of
question.
abstracted during repeat exposûe to an action type. These regularities CapNe the
essence of the abject matter and as such they are thought to be stereotypical of the
action-
3.7 Marshall's schema theory: A sumrnary
takes from the work of Minsky (1 979, Schank (1 973, Rumelhart and ûrtony (1 977) and
*cm access a genenc framework that contains the essential elements of al1 these
* c m draw inferences, make estimates, create goals, and develop plans using the
fiarnework;
*cm utilize skills, procedures, or rules as needed when faced with a problem for
Marshail's schema theory is readily applicable to the present study. Like Collins and
Quiuian (1972), Marshall says that schemata and their contents are interrelated through a
nested structural arrangement. The idea of nesteâ schemata aiiows concepts (nodes) to be
f'unctional components in more than one schema The understanding (of an action) containeci
in schemata is represented by concept knowledge and schema knowledge. Marshall holds that
35
concept knowledge is the knowledge extracted during early exposure to an action. It evolves
a dictionary definition that can be looked up quickly to differentiate one concept from
another. She describes schema knowledge as a higher fonn of knowledge that allows one to
reason about a concept and solve problems. Concept knowledge declares what a concept is
the Jonassen, Beissner and Yacci (1 993) interpretations of declarative and stmctural
what a concept is in dictionary definition format. Structural knowledge was calied the
schema knowledge are fùnctionaliy equivalent to Jonassen et al's declarative knowledge and
Marshail was also influenced by Wînograd (1977). Winograd intimated that schemata
are primarily composed of declarative knowledge. More important for this work, Marshall
guide for procedural knowledge" (Marshall, 1995, p.24). Jonassen et al. suggested a similar
fiinction for stmctural knowledge. They said that it mediates the acquisition of procedurd
knowledge and that it is essential for the comprehension of ideas. These descriptions hint at
We are interested in plan development and ski11 utilization once the generic
framework of a schema has been accessed. The reason for this interest is that the processes of
plan development and ski11 utilkation can be used to speculate about the scafTolding of
procedural knowledge. Marshall proposes that schemata contain several kinds of knowledge:
problem enter working rnemory, they are compared to identification knowledge in a process
derives fiom repeat exposure to situations that stimulate schema development. Once a
used to forrn a mental model of the current exemplar. Elaboration knowledge is aiso used to
compare the mental model to the schema's template of this problem type.
ofa problem. Elaboration lmowledge tests the hypothesis to see ifit is correct.A schema can
be adopted with somejustification once the problem's type has been identifid correctly. The
adopted schema is applied with planning knowledge that breaks down the resolution of a
37
problem into goals and sub-goals. Planning knowledge becomes more efficient with repeated
use of the schema. The goals and sub-goals of planning knowledge are carried out by
execution knowledge.
together. Solving the addition problem, 9 plus 7 plus 5, begins with the application of
knowledge is used to fom a mental mode1 of the problem and venfL that the problem is
indeed one of addition. Once it has been venfied that the problem's type has been correctly
identified the addition schema is adopted. The addition schema's planning knowledge breaks
resolution of the problem up into sub-goals. For example, "If there is only one coiumn of
numbers, add the second digit to the first to generate a subtotal. Add subsequent numbers
one by one generating subtotals with each addition untii al1 the numbers have been added".
Execution knowledge resolves the problem by carrying out the sub-goal steps that planning
knowledge generates.
knowing whu? type of problem has been encountered. Thus identification and elaboration
knowledge are forms of declarative or structural knowledge. Their actions culminate in the
Jonassen, Beissner and Yacci ( 1993) called procedural knowledge the knowledge of
they identie a plan for solving a problem and then they carry out the steps of those plans.
The plans fomed by procedural (planning or execution knowledge) knowledge are based on
the mental model that forms in working memory. The implication taken from this is that
The case of an oil change for a car can be used to illustrate the scaffiolding of
procedural knowledge. The problem of changing a car's oil begins with the identification of
concepts that are involved in the oil change. The concepts are retrieved to working memory
and relationships between them are used to assemble a mental model of the oil change
process. An oil change concept map is an expression of this mental model. Tactile procedural
knowledge uses the mental model to plan and cany out the oil change procedure.
related to that of procedural knowledge. If srnictural knowledge were less sophisticated than
procedural knowledge then the mode1 may lack the complexity that problem resolution
requires. If the structural knowledge were more complex than the correspondhg procedural
knowledge, the mental model could not be acted upon. Thus the contention that stnimrai
and procedural knowledge have comparable qualiàes is a conclusion that arises fiom their
funaional relationship.
The relationship between schema theory and concept maps bas been touched upon
39
previously. Boxed concepts in concept rnaps have been equated to declarative (concept)
map cornparison. Her descriptions of the components and elements of schemata wggest that
they are fùnctionally equivalent to the concepts and links of concept maps. For exarnple,
knowledge. Thus far in our integration of theories, schema knowledge has been equated to
structurai knowledge and structural knowledge has been equated to concept map links. If
elements contribute to schema knowledge they must also contribute to the understanding
The integration of schema theory and concept map theory would be made easier if
schema components could be equated to concepts in concept maps. Suitable evidence for this
to the number of elements associated with it. Kconcept maps containing equd numbers of
concepts are compared, the one with the most links wodd be regardecf as the most comected
to increasing the number of links to a concept in a concept map. This fùnctiond equivalence
is said to suggest that schema components are concept rnap concepts and that schema
3.11 A concept rnap to iiiustrate the integratioa o f stmctuttl knowledge and schema
theory
Severai revelations about the nature of different forms of knowledge were derived in
the above. These revelations are illustrateci by the blue links of Figure 3. The most important
are that: stnictural knowledge is schema knowledge and that the quality of structural and
Links 1 to 6 illustrate the thinking that concept maps represent mental models. Link
number 1 indicates that concept map concepts have been equated to schema theory
components. Link number 2 serves a similar function by illustrating that concept map links
have been equated to schema elements. The supposition that declarative knowledge and
concept knowledge are fùnctionally quivalent is indicated by links 3 and 4. Links 3 and 4
also indicate that declarative knowledge and concept knowledge both serve as the source of
structural knowledge. Link 5 consolidates the rûwning of links 3 and 4. tt proposes that
on the left of Figure 3 to the concept-schema knowledge continuum on the right. Link 6
knowledge. Link 6 seems reasonable as both knowledge types elaborate concept meaning and
knowledge. Wth this thinking in place the scaffolding of procedural knowledge can be
42
considered. Blue links 7 to 1 2 illustrate the scaffolding of procedural knowledge. Links 7 and
8 point to the supposition that identification and elaboration knowledge are two forrns of
declarative knowledge. Links 9 and 10 have it that procedural knowledge comprises planning
declarative knowledge used to fom mental models. Links 1 1 and 12 indicate that structural
and schema knowledge are sophisticated foms of declarative knowledge that are used to
form mental models. Planning (procedural) knowiedge uses mental models to determine the
steps for problem resolution. That is, structural knowledge scafTolds procedural knowledge
Section 3.3 (structural knowledge) begins with the Jonassen, Beissner and Yacci
( 1993) contention that knowledge changes fonn as it evolves. Their view is that knowledge is
declarative and structural knowledge are the same f o n of knowledge. They differ only in the
procedurai knowledge. The intent is not to imply that structural and procedural knowledge
are independent of one another. Rather. knowledge is a singular entity that possesses both
structural and procedurai modalities. Marshall's schema theory (section 3.8) reveais that the
structural and procedural modalities cm be divided further. The structural modality has
43
identification and elaboration functions. The procedural modality has d e s in planning and
a subcomponent of the procedural mode that has both planning and execution capabilities..
needed to elucidate the nature of link-procedural knowledge was also described in that
chapter. Links should consia of a linking statement and one or more items of data. In CIP
tenninolom, items of data represent chunks of information that enter working memory in
when relationships between items of data (chunks) have been identified. Link-procedural
knowledge is the knowledg that seeks relationships between items of data to form the
(linking statements).
model of a problem. This mental model scaffolds the actions of procedurai knowledge. In
elucidating the nature of the scaffolding process, theory integration indicates that link-
procedural, stnictural and procedural knowledge are isomorphic. This indication derives from
the realization that the scafTo1ding process can only be successful if the t h e knowledge
forms are complementary in quality. Knowing that these knowledge forms are isomorphic
suggests two rnethods for investigating the psychological validity of concept maps. The
relationship between stmcturd knowledge and procedural knowledge can be explored as can
the relationship between amctuml and link-procedural knowledge. The assessments are
equivalent as both involve a cornparison of the structural and procedurai modalities. In both
44
investigations, finding that the structural and procedural modes are isomorphic indicates that
Thus such a findiny supports the rationale for theory integration. By extension, it aiso
The argument that concept maps represent mental models would be enhanced if
additional evidence could be found to illustrate that concept maps exhibit propenies predicted
by theories of coynition. This chapter seeks such evidence by integrating two theories of
reading cornprehension (Jacobs and Paris. 1987 and Kirby, 1991). The integration process
hints at properties of the cognitive and metacognitive processes of concept map creation. in
of the cognitive strategies of leaming are also rare. Scarcer still are studies that clearly
delineate the dynarnic interactive roles played out by metacognitive and cognitive processes
dunng leaming. A search of the literature Failed to locate a description of the roles played
out by metacognitive and cognitive processes dunng concept map creation. This problem can
concept mapping is a metacognitive tool for meanin@l learning (Edmonson and Smith
1996).
Concept mapping retrieves and expresses the products of meaningful leaming. If that
learning is acquired from reading then the metaco_oiitive and cognitive processes of reading
comprehension will influence its expression in a map. Jacobs and Paris ( 1987) and Kirby
reading comprehension. Their work is integrated here and posited as a description of the
4. 1 A description of metacognition
To Jacobs and Paris ( 1987), metacognition is an executive process that plans and
monitors thinking. Kirby ( 1991) also maintains that metacognition operates on a higher level
than cognitive processes. During reading (and concept map creation) metacognition guides
the selection and operation of cognitive arategies. According to Kirby. little is known about
cognitive strategies. He holds that interpretations of them depend upon the scale on which
tum guide the activities of co_gitiveprocesses. Cognitive processes hold units of information
in working rnernory and seek relationships between them. They are fùnctionally equivalent to
link-procedural knowledp.
Jacobs and Park (1987) define metacognition as "knowledge about cogitive States
or processes that cm be shared between individuals" (p. 258). This definition limits
metacognition to coh@ive activities that can be reponed to others. Once an activity becomes
metacognitive tool. For example, a novice leamer may consciously reread words or phrases
to better determine their meaning. When asked ifwords are reread the leamer would give a
Once the action bypasses the awareness of the learner Jacobs and Paris say that it ceases to
be metacognitive.
cognition and selfhanagement of thinking. Each component is funher divided into three
elements. Declarative. procedural and conditional knowledge are elements of the self-
appraisal of cognition. Declarative knowledge is knowing thm. Leamers may know thut
underlininy a word increases retention of it. Procedural knowledge refers to knowing huw.
Leamers rnay understand huw underlining increases retention and recall. Conditional
knowledge is the knowledge of why strategies work and whrtl they can be used appropriately.
Paris?Cross and Lipson (1 984) include declarative. procedural and conditional knowledge
within the rubric "knowledge about cognition." Element s of the self-appraisal of cognition
distinyuishes these knowledge foms fi-omthe cognitive equivalents (Jonassen Beissner and
The components of self-manasement that Jacobs and Paris describe are executive
leaniing goal. Electing to read quickly when an oveMew is required is the example of
planning that Jacobs and Paris provide. Evalrrarion is the process of examinin8
comprehension as leamhg progresses. The third component, regzdation. takes feedback fiom
and adjusts the manner in which strategies are applied. Adjustments are made to
rv~Iucftio~i
meet a desired level of comprehension. Paris, Cross and Lipson incorporate these self-
Jacobs and Pans' work can be interpreted to expiain the role played by metacognition
in concept map link creation. At the nart of the linking process, platz~ii~ig
is used to select a
selection as the learner has to know when and why strategies are effective before making a
knowledge of what the strateg does. The strategy is applied by metacognitive procrcl11rd
knowledge. Durinç concept map creation rvalrcatiml ensures that metacognitive procedural
and processes.
follows:
elements can be recent input or can have been recalled fiom long-term memory. When
relationships are recoynized, the elements may then be integrated, to form a new,
higher-level unit in memory, which may then be stored or used in subsequent relating
Kirby argues that reading can occur on ei&t ievels: features. letters, syllables, words,
syntactic chunks (phrases) ideas, main ideas and themes. Beginning readers operate below the
level of words. Their cognitive resources are so absorbed in recognition tasks that they are
incapable of the inregration that comprehension requires. From the word level up recognition
Comprehension begins with the chunking of incoming data from the text. The product of
chunking may be a phrase. an idea or a main idea depending on the proficiency of the reader.
Integration of the chunks cornpletes the comprehension process. For the purposes of this
thesis. the thematic level of reading is thought of as a process that connects chunks to extant
knowledge in other domains. This processing facilitates the use of knowledge in domains
other than that in which it was acquired. Reading to the level of themes is a popular goal in
education as knowledge that is associated with multiple themes has a high degree of utility.
The level at which readers operate depends on interaction between their reading skills
and goals and the complexity of the text. Kirby maintains that adults generally read at the
Ievel of main ideas. At this level little effort is required to chunk incoming data and form main
ideas. Two or three main ideas may reside in working memory simultaneously. Newly input
information is compared to the main ideas and reasons for integration are sought. By
50
operating at the level of main ideas the learner is able to comprehend the plot of a novel or
Readers operating at the level of main ideas may be unaware of themes if the text
does not point them out explicitly. Functioning at the level of themes requires that the set of
main ideas that make up an idea or notion be present in memory. Concept maps can make a
set of main ideas explicit to the leamer. Reflecting on the main ideas in a map serves as a
staning point for the generation of thematic links. Concept maps provide audents with the
"big picture" and thereby encourage the transfer of knowledge across multiple themes.
recognized. Successive processing forms discrete units from incoming information and
t hese discrete units of knowledge. Kirby contends that sirnultaneous processing can be
bottom up or top down. In bottom up processing two units of information are integrated to
fonn a higher level unit. Top d o m processiny dismantles high level units into their
component parts.
Comprehensions are saved to long terni memory while reading occurs. If a concept
map simply regurgtates comprehensions that were acquired from reading then its expression
will be controlled by the very processes that control cornprehension. To explain. link data
items represent discrete units of information that fom in working memory during the reading
process. They represent the products of successive processing. The linking statement
link-procedural knowledge.
cognition indicates that complex metacognitive activity results in the application of complex
cognitive strategies and processes. This means t hat measurements of metacognition should be
will be taken as supportive evidence that metacognition and cognition are integated,
hierarchicd processes. The finding will also be taken as fùnher suppon for the thinking that
concept maps mode1 the properties of theories of cogition. These theones hold that concept
maps represent mental models, finding that concept maps emulate theories will be taken as
Figure 4 was created fiom Brock and Madigan ( 1988). It depicts the three stages of
fermentation in yeast. The ethanol and CO, by-products of this fermentation are useful to the
brewiny industry. Different products are produced when _glucosefermentation is camed out
by other prokaryotes (fermentation agents). For exarnple lactic acid bactena produce lactic
acid. Al1 fementing organisms store fermentation energy by adding phosphate to adenosine
diphosphate (ADP). The resulting adenosine triphosphate (ATP) subsequently releases its
The fermentation of one molecule of gucose is depicted in Fiyure 4. The first stage of
fermentation is a preparatory stage in which glucose is split apan. The energ needed to
perform the reactions of stage I is provided by two ATP molecules. Two molecules of
glyceraldehyde-3-phosphate are formed by the end of stage 1. These products are represented
at the start of stage 2 on the ieft and rîght sides of Figure 4. Each glyceraldehyde-3-
phosphate molecule undergoes the same reactions in the second and third stages of
fermentation.
diphosphoglycerate. This reaction. Wte all the reactions of fermentation, is made possible by
enzymes. Er~ymeschange the rate at which a reaction occurs. Coenzymes are Ioosely
associated with a number of enzymes; they carry smaü molecules between them. in
53
diphosphoglycerate production two hydrogens are added to two coenzyme NAD+ molecules
molecules of ATP. At this stage of fermentation there has been no net loss or gain of energy.
A positive energy balance is achieved in the last reaction of stage 2. The production of
The first two stages of fermentation are collectively known as @ycolysis.The third
aage of fermentation is necessary to convert NADH back to NAD+. The yeast ce11 has a
limited nipply of NAD+, if al1 of it were converted to NADH, glucose fermentation would
stop. The third aage also produces ethanol and carbon dioBde (CO3
&a ,.
Concept map o f fermentation
Figure 5 is a concept rnap of the fermentation process. Two links emanate From the
concept map that are separate from one another. A concept map region contains a number of
intercomected concepts. Regions are discrete fiom one another in that few if any links pass
from one region to another. The first region of Figure 5 surnrnarizes the process of
fermentation. It consists of jun two concepts: Erwrgv and A P.These concepts are referred
to collectively as the Smmary region. The other region, the Example region, illustrates the
fermentation of glucose in
yeast . Within the Ewampk region, Stage I , Slagr 2 and Stage 3 and the concepts they
subsume fom t h e smailer regions The importance of regions is discussed in section 1.4.3
Figure 5 was created by the author of'this thesis with WordPerfecttsdraw feature. in
fermentation. WordPerfect's draw feature does not provide sufficient space for the inclusion
of complete link descriptions. Soflware that is specifically designed for concept map creation
electronically alongside the map. Paper concept maps offer a cheap solution to the problem of
Complete descriptions of Figure 5's links were developed by the author and are
presented in Table 1. The nght hand column of the table requires a little explmation. The top
line of each row in the right hand column is the linking staternent of that link. The numbered
statements beneath the Iinkins statement represent items of data that make up or are
The first step the author engayed in when creating Table 1 was to generate linkins
statements for every link. In the next step. items of data were created for each link by
reflecting on the thousht processes that contributed to link creation. Each link was
considered in tum. Each datum represents a single factor that contributed to a link
relationskp. When the data of these links are reviewed. the logic behind link creation c m be
deduced. The product of this deductive process represents the inference or link-procedurd
knowledye that the author used to coordinate link data and yenerate a linking statement. For
example the data of iink 12 describes the reactions that lead to ATP production. Deductive
reasoning reveds that the author realited that if these reactions occur in stage 2 then this
nage m u a be a source of ATP. When marking student's maps assesson use the sarne
'SemNet Academic, intellimation, Dept. 5SCY 130 Cremora Drive, PO Box 1922 Santa
Barbara. CA- 93 1 16-1922
deductive reasoning to determine how the data of each link were combined to form the
Table 1
Descriptian
Link 9
--
5.1 Novak and Gowin (1984) and the structure of concept maps
Novak and Gowin ( 1984) based their concept map scorins technique on three
number and quality of links in a concept map. Progressive dxerentiation refers to the
thinking that concepts are never fùlly lemed and that propositional links are continuaily
that facilitates the discovery of relationships between concepts. Newly discovered links may
reconciliation aiso has a reflective component that can increase the quality of understanding
reconciliation determine the hierarchical structure of concept maps. Dunng concept map
concepts to appear near the top of the map. Specific concepts are subsumed beneath inclusive
concepts. Figure 5 exemplifies these structurd trends. Inclusive concepts such as Glyco!v.si.s
appear near the top of the map. Specific concepts such as E f h a d appear near the bottom.
Novak and Gowin's sconng syaern accounts for the structural influences of
levels. During concept map creation progressive differentiation and integrative reconciliation
are placed in clusters or regions. In yeneral concept maps regions are largely discrete fiom
one another. Most links are intraregional. they connect concepts that reside within the same
region. Only a few links pass fiom one region to another and as a result concept map regions
are largely discrete fiom one another. Novak and Gowin used the name crosslinks to refer to
these interregional links. They thought that crosslinks form when mappers aep back fi-oma
map and engage in reflective thinking or integrative reconciliation. The reflective infiuence
behind these links means that on occasion they contain examples of creative thinkiny. Novak
and Gowin also were aware that crosslinks may also be lemed Eom instruction. They
suggested that crosslinks be examined carefùlly for evidence of creative thinking. Crosslinks
that contain true original thinking were awarded a higher score than intraregional l i s . By
59
awarding different scores to crosslinks and intraresional links. Novak and Gowin introduced
the idea that different types of concept map link have different qualities. Method I builds on
this thinkiny as it recognizes five types of link and five levels of link quality.
In Fisure 5 the concepts Stage 1, Stage 7 and Stage 3 are used to anchor three
regions. Unlike the Swnrnay and Fkampie regions. the stage regions are not completely
separate form each other. They are connected by links 1O and 16 which attach the Stage I
region to the Stage 2 resion and the Stage 2 region to the Shge 3 region. These links repeat
information provided in Brock and Madiyan ( 1988).They do not contain creative thinking
Novak and Gowin claimed that accurate assessrnent of concept maps requires they
have a hierarchical structure with clearly labelled and categonzed (cross or intraregional)
links. When these conditions are met they said that values can be assi~nedto links and
structures on the basis of reasonable judgement. The followinç pidelines are adapted form
1. Give a score of 1 to al1 valid intraregional links. Subtract a score of 1 for each
invalid intraregional link. Enurnerate al1 vaiid crosslinks that do not contain creative
2. Count al1 vdid levels of hierarchy and allot a score of 3 to each level.
reconciliation. They may be better indicaton of meanîngful learning than the number
of hierarchicd levels.
60
4. If example links were asked for and provided. award a score of 1 to each level.
Table 2 contains a Novak and Gowin score for Figure 5 . Some explanation is
required. There are 16 links in total. and five hierarchical levels. A point is awarded for each
link and three points are awarded for each hierarchical level. The final score for Figure 5 is
Yenst) link connects two concept layers. The link fi-om Ymst to (i!tw/jsis provides another
layer and the C'lyco~vsis-StageI and Stage /-il TP links make up two more. Link 3 was
awarded a score of 1 as it is an example link. Links IO and 16 were also awarded a score of I
Ta bie 2
A Concept M ~ Scorin
D Sytem lafter Novak and Gowin 1198411 for Figure 4
Merentiation and integrative reconciliation to provide scores for entire maps. Only two
levels of link quality are recognized, intraregional links and crosslinks. Methods 1.2 and 3
61
explicate the cognitive and metacognitive processes associated with individual concepts.
differentiation and they recognize five levels of link quality Chapters 6 and 7 discuss these
methods in tùll.
Chapter 6
6.0 Concept map assessment methods and the goals o f the thesis
methods are developed. Method 1. Method 2 and Method 3. Method I assesses the quality of
Method 2 enumerates concept rnap links. The methods are used to meet the first and third
goals of the thesis. In regard to the first goal, ratinys from methods 1 and 3 are correlated to
investigate the thinking that structural and link-procedural knowledse evolve in parallel and
with comparable quality. A positive correlation is said to indicate that concept maps emulate
behaviours indicated by the integration of theories. These theories indicate that concept maps
Method 3 also estimates the cognitive activity of concept mappen. An interview is developed
to assess the metacognitive activities of concept mappers. Evidence that Method 3 scores are
proportional to interview scores will be interpreted as supportive evidence for the argument
qualities. Such a finding indicates that concept maps exhibit properties indicated by the
integration of theones. Once again, findins that concept maps emulate properties derived
from theory integation is said to support the thinhg that concept maps represent mental
models.
63
In an aside to the main goals of the thesis. methods 1 and 2 are used to illustrate the
utility of concept maps. Method 1 is used to assesses the results of integrative reconciliation.
That is, it is used to detennine the quality of aructural knowledge. Method 2 is used to
account for progressive differentiation. That is it is used to enumerate the links associated
with individual concepts. All three methods are based in the work of Biggs and Collis ( 1982).
Biggs and Collis ( 1982) proposed a system for identifjmg the quality of students'
responses to questions. They called this system, the Structure of the Observed Learning
behind the evaluation strategy of Method I . SOLO assesses the understanding contained in
has been integrated. Method 1 adopts the same strateyy, it considers how items of data are
Fiyures 6 to 1O illustrate the five levels of response qudity that are recognized by
SOLO. In each of these figures. responses to cues are generated using one of three types of
data: imelevant data not contained in the instruction relevant data contained in the
instruction, relevant data that is not contained in the instruction. SOLO assumes that
response quality is a reflection of the arnount and type of data in a link. Data that are not
contained in the instruction have to be interpreted by the learner. If the learner interprets
these data correctly. the response is said to be of high quality. Lower quality responses are
associated with relevant data contained in the instruction. In both cases the more data
64
involved the higher the quality. Data may or may not be integrated in the response. A higher
quality response is sipified when data are integrated. Incorrect responses are associated with
irrelevant data. Even if data are integated in an incorrect response the quality is still
considered to be low.
Figure 6 illustrates the lowest SOLO level, the prestmctural response. Inappropriate
or irrelevant data are used to pnerate a response to a cue or stimulus. According to Biçgs
and Collis, this is the type of response senerated by a student with no prior knowledge of a
Cue Response
The foiIowing question wiI1 be used to exempli@aii of the SOLO response levels
65
"why is Iraq in a bad political situation?" It will be assumed that current news progams serve
as the source of instruction. A prestructurai response wouid be "it is bad because of Iraq's
gold mines"
structure. In a prestructural response there is no need to simultaneously retain the cue and
response in working mernory and the requisite capacity is minimal. Biggs and Collis also
considered the processes of consistency and closure. Closure refers to the need to corne to a
to the need to consider al1 the data and ensure that conclusions are correct. In the low level
prestructural response forces of closure predominate and low consistency results. Hiçh-level
Cue
Cue Response
ILb
, Hypothetical, not @en -
-R
SOLO structures place greater demands on capacity. These structures are less impacted by
closure than consistency. In fact at the highest SOLO level (extended abstract) closure has no
Figure 7 illustrates that the unistructural SOLO response is generated by relating the
cue to a single relevant datum that was provided in the instruction. Biggs and Collis referred
to the integration of eue and data as the process of induction. The arain on capacity is still
low in the unistructural response as only the datum and cue need be manipulated in working
memory.
A unistnictural response to the question would be "it'sin a bad situation because of the oil."
68
The question acts as a cue and "oil" acts as a datum. The response linked one datum to the
cue.
as the cue and two or more related items of data are simultaneousiy manipulated in working
memory. A multistructural response to the Iraq question wouId be "it's bad because of the oit
and it is also bad because of Saddam Hussein." The data "oil" and "Saddam Hussein" are
associated with the cue through the conjoining phrase "and it is also." Conjoining phrases
typifi the multistructural response. In hisher level relational links the data and cue are
In Figure 9 induction coordinates most or al1 of the instructional data into a single
response. The strain on capacity is hish in relational responses such as Figure 9. At this
SOLO level the Iraqi question might stimulate the following response "Iraq's troubles stem
from Saddam's pan-Arabian ideoiogy which threatens the World's access to cheap oil." The
response is relational as the data "politics, oil and money" are integrated into a single phrase.
Capacity is strained to the full in the extended abstract response. Figure 10 typifies
extended abstract thinking It coordinates material fiom inside and outside of the instruction
69
and it generates multiple logical deductions. These deductions may involve new hypotheses,
abstract principles and analo$es. The example question might elicit the following response at
the extended abstract level "Iraq's troubles stem corn 1923 when the Middle East was
divided to suit the political aspirations of Colonial powers. Recognizing that extemally
imposed land division attenuates Arabian power. Saddam Hussein sought to unite the Middle
East. A united Middle East could undoubtedly change the balance of global politics by
controlling the movement and price of oil. It is the çlobal threat imposed by Saddarn's
The above answer is more detailed than that provided at lower SOLO levels but this
is not the reason for the extended abaract rating. Rather the rating is derived fi-om the
association of data such as "the current political situation. oil and money" to the land
divisions of 1923. Current news programs have neglected to repon the impact of Colonial
strategies on current events. Thus it cm be said that the response associates the cue with a
daam that was taken from outside the instruction (current news prograrns). The use of non-
The general trend in Figues 6 to 10 is that more data has to be considered alonyside
the cue as the SOLO structure becornes more cornplex. Furthemore, it seems that creatiny
of concept maps. For example. the cues and responses of SOLO structures compare to the
two concepts involved in a concept rnap Iink. Also the connections between cues and
cornparison suggests methods for concept map assessment. These methods are descnbed in
It rates concept map links against the SOLO response scale. Links consist of one or more
items of data and a linkins statement. Each item of data expresses one element of the
relationship between two concepts. Dunng Iink creation, simultaneous processing (link-
procedural knowledge) forms an inference that connects items of data. The understanding
contained in this connection is expressed in the linking statement. Method I determines the
knowledge.
terms of CIP and concept map links. Al1 these figres associate a cue and response through
data. The cue and response can be thought of as the two concepts of a Iink. The lines
co~ectingthe cue. the data and the response represent the cognitive processes of link
creation. Each item of data represents a discrete unit of information that has entered working
memory in response to a problem. The lines between cues and items of data represent the
actions of successive processiny. Lines comecting items of data to the response represent the
simultaneous processing syrnbolizes the inference that connects items of data and thereby
The inference of Figure 7 (unistructurai Iînk) concems one item of data that was
72
provided to the mapper. The two concepts of the Iink are connected by a single item of data.
data) that were forrned by successive processing. The linkins statement of this link conjoins
each datum with phrases such as "as well as" and "and also." No attempt is made to integrate
this data and form a contiguous expression of understanding. Figures 9 and 10 portray higher
level links. Their linkiny statements are expressions of understanding that were forrned by
integrating data. Figure 1O difTers fiom Figure 9 in that its inference connects one or more
Figures 6 to 10 illustrate that linking staternents differ in quality. The factors that
These factors suggest a method for assessing concept maps. This method. Method 1.
is explored below by examining the linking statements of Figure 5. The method is applied in
four aeps:
I . examine each link to ascertain that its linking statement and datum (or data) are
correct
4. determine whether the linking staternent has associated its data with
multistnictural. relational or extended abstract logic.
The factors that affect link qudity are the amount of data the manner of its integration and
whether it was provided in the instruction. SOLO sugsests that these factors generate five
extended abaract. A sconng system that recognizes the five SOLO levels is presented below.
responses were provided to the exarnple question "why is Iraq's political situation bad?" In
tems of concept maps these responses miyht be thought of as linkiny statements that connect
appear in a concept map link as per Table 3. Table 3 represents links in the same manner as
Table 1. The lefi hand column of the table contains the link's number and SOLO rating. The
top line of the right hand column contains the linking statement. Subsequent lines in the nght
hand column represent items of data. There are no preaructural links in Figure 5 .
Table 3
1 Link 1 Data 1
x It is bad because of iraq's gold mines
Prestnictural It is bad because of Iraq's gold mines
7.2 Unistructural links
When concepts are associated through a single relevant datum their linkiny statement
is classified as unistructural. The exarnple unistructural response to the Iraq question would
Table 4
1 Link 1 Data 1
11 xUnistructural 1 It's in a bad situation because of the oil 1
1 ( I )Ittsin a bad situation because of the oil 1
links the data item and linking statement are identical. This is thought to be a reflection of the
fact that these Iinking statements are influenced by just one item of data. Relational linking
statements are not identical to any one datum. Their construction is influenced to varying
degees by each item of data in the link. Some of Table 5's links contain a singe item of data
that differs corn the Iinking statement. These links are thought to be intermediary to the
unistructural and relational stages. Perhaps these linking statements are influenced by
additional items of data that the mapper is incapable of expressing or is only unconsciously
1 Link 1 Data 1
1 Link 8 1 The preparatory reactions of stage 1 consume 2 ATP molecules
-- - - - - - - --
Linking statements are thought to have multistructurai quaiity when they connect
component data with phrases such as "and." "or." and "and also". At this level of
understanding the leamer cannot generate the inferences that would synthesize data into a
single relational phrase. The exarnple multistructural response of Chapter 6 mizht appear in a
Table 6
1 Link 1 Data 1
1 1 It's bad because of the oil and it is also bad because of Saddam Hussein 1
I ( 1 ) It's bad because of the oil
(2) It's bad because of Saddam Hussein
Table 7 contains the relational response of chapter 6. The link's relational status
refers to the fact that its linking statement encompasses two or more items of data. Thus far
this description of Method I has made use of the SOLO strate3 of rating quality against data
integration. The second factor that influences Iink quality is the number of data items. This
numerical factor can be incorporated into Method 1 by adding it to Iink ratinys as a suffix.
Thus relational links containiny two data items become relational-î links. Links with three
data items are relational-3 links and relational-4 Iinks contain four items of data. The number
of data items can also be applied to multistructural and extended abstract links. Link quaiity
is also influenced by whether the relevant data is provided in the instruction. This factor is
Table 7
Link Data
x Iraq's troubles stem from Saddam's pan-Arabian ideolog which threatens
the World's access to cheap oil
( 1 ) It's bad because of the oil
(7) It's bad because Saddam's political desires seek to increase Arabian
control over oil supplies
i
(3) Increased Arabian control would increase Global oii prices
Table 8 contains three Figure 5 links that were judged to have relational quaiity.
Table 8.
1 Link 1 Data 1
Fermentation is a biochemical pathway that produces energy
( 1 ) Pathway oxidizes glucose
(2) Catabolism of oxidized compound releases a small amount of energy
Link 7 Stage 2 is the last phase of glycolysis
Relational-3 ( 1)Stase 2 is an oxidative phase
(2) NAD+ is reduced
(3)ATP is produced
Link 16 NADH is recycled to NAD+
Relational-3 ( 1 )Ce11 has lirnited supply of NAD+
(?)Fermentationrequires NAD+ to proceed
(3)As acetaldehyde is reduced NADH is oxidized
The use of matenai fiom outside the instruction is the third factor that influences
SOLO quality. Only hi* quality inferences can incorporate extemal data. Rumelhart and
Norman ( 1978) indicate why this is so. They recogize three leaming modes: accretion.
stnicturing and tuning. Accretion occurs when new knowledge is added to long t e n
memory. Stnicturhg involves the addition of knowledge to existing schema. Tuning requires
the retooling of schema structure. It represents expen performance. Links that simply repeat
an instruction may represent the accretion or the stnicturing mode. Links that upon
explanations provided in an instruction represent the tuning mode. Tuning links incorporate
the learner's their existing knowledge into an instruction. The result is a finicino aatement that
Extended abstract links represent the tuning mode. They contain one or more items of
78
data that were not presented in the instruction. Inferences at this level of cognition should be
exarnples of creative thinking. There are no extended abstract links in Fiyure 5 . ï h e extended
abstract response presented in chapter 6 would appear in link format as per Table 9.
Table 9
Link Data
Iraq's troubles stem fi-om1923 when the Middle East was divided to suit
the political aspirations of Colonial powers. Recognizing that extemally
imposed land division attenuates Arabian power, Saddam Hussein
sought to unite the Middle East. A united Middle East could
undoubtediy change the balance of global politics by controlling the
movement and pnce of oil. It is the global threat imposed by Saddam's
ideology that makes Iraq's politicai situation so bad.
( 1 ) It's bad because of the oil
(7) It's bad because Saddam's political desires seek to increase Arabian
control over oil supplies
(3) increased Arabian control would increase Global oil prices
(4) The above problems stem from the division of the Middle East in
I 923
Method I takes the different leamhg modes into account. In the empirical component
of this thesis, students read a text and then map it. If a student knows absolutely nothing
about the contents of the text then leaming will occur in the structuring mode. if the text
adds to students' existing knowledge then the accretion mode of leaming will be at work.
When midents map leaming that occurred in the stmcturing mode they will likely regurgitate
the text. Students for whom the text provides accretion type lemirtg may also choose to
simply reproduce the text. If two sudents produce maps that reproduce the text then their
79
unistructural links will be of the same quality. Similarly their relational links will involve
However, students learning in the accretion mode need not simply reproduce the text.
They cm provide structures that indicate how they associated the text with their existing
knowledge. Their maps may contain a representation of t he text as well as links that illustrate
how the text was connected to their existing knowledge. Links that reproduce the text
contain inferences that are of the same quality as the structuring mode. Links that associate
the text with existing knowledge contain inferences of a higher quality. Their superiority
stems from the fact that the text is not used to guide their creation. Inferences created in the
tuniny mode are of higher qudity still. Learners operatin%in this mode reason as to how their
current understandinç of a link fits in with the description provided in the tea. Then they
integate the two underaandinss to generate a superior representation. The tuning mode is
the unistmctural. multistructural and relational SOLO type links descnbed above. A more
cornpiete scde is needed to account for the tuning and accretion modes. Such a xale is
presented beiow .
item of data.
through a correct inference that involves a single item of data. The linking statement
through a correct inference that involves a single item of data. The matenai is relevant
associate the two concepts of the link. That is the Iinking statement is a conjoined list
of the data items. No inferences were formed. the material was presented in the text
data to associate the two concepts of the iink. No inferences were formed. The
correct inference or inferences that involve relevant items of data. The linking
through correct inferences that involve relevant items of data. The matenal is relevant
throuyh correct inferences that involve relevant items of data. The statement improves
upon a relationship presented in the text. It incorporates one or more additional items
of data.
7.6 Assessing the stnictuml knowledge of entire maps and individual concepts
structural knowledge is quantified by counting Method 1 Iink types. Table 10 quantifies the
Table IO.
A. 8 unistnrcturai
B. 3 relationai-2
C. 5 relational-3
A. 3 unistmctural
B. 3 relational -2
A. 3 unistructural
B. 3 relational-3
Problems anse when iMethod I is used to compare individual concepts or entire maps
that do not have the same number of links. Method 1 considers the diference between
unistructurd and rnultistnictural quality to be the same as the difference between relational
and extended abstract quality. Method 1 also considers that links of the same type are of the
same quality. In actuality, links of the same type do differ in quality accordiny to the concepts
they connect and the data they contain. Further. even when two links are identical their
bearing on understanding wiil likely Vary according to the abilities of students who created
them.
Two hypothetical maps (not shown) help to illustrate that specific cornparisons are
not possible when maps contain different numbers of links. Suppose that map A contains 15
[inks
a. 10 unistructurai
b. 4 relationai-2
c. 1 relationai-3
and that map B contains 10 links
a. 4 unistructuraI
b. 4 relational-3
c. 3 relational-3
Method 1 cannot determine which map is better as it is incapable of companny the coynitive
worth of different link types. In the case of maps A and B a scale is needed that is capable of
When maps (or concepts) do not contain the same number of links. Method 1 permits
only a yeneral commentary about link quality. It is possible to say "this map (or concept)
contains Say five links of unistructural quality. and four of relational-2 quality " while another
contains "four of unistmctural and two of extended abstract quality." These commentaries
may facilitate rough rankings but unless the differences between maps (or concepts) are
Method 1 cornparisons may also suffer when assessinç generalized concepts. Links
h m these concepts summarize the lower level concepts they encompass. In makins a
surnmary the student has to decide which data to include and exclude. For example. link 1 of
Figure 5 summarizes the process of fermentation with N O data items. It is conceivable that
another author would use more data to generate a summary. The additional data may be
unnecessary to the link or it may be repeated at lower points in the hierarchy. The links
associated with generalized concepts rnay receive infiated Method 1 scores unless the
Despite the weaknesses identified above. Method I is well suited to Our purposes.
The goal is not to compare the quality of structural knowledg across maps or concepts.
Rather we are interested in cornparhg the quality of the structural knowledge of individual
relationships and identifies new ones. It also identifies erroneous links. By assessing the
Proyressive differentiation is based on the reasoning that concepts are never hlly understood
(Novak and Gowin. 1984). It assumes that links can always be added to concepts as leaming
is never complete. Method 2 accounts for progressive differentiation by quantifjing the links
associated with individual concepts. It assumes that more links signiQ more progressive
differentiation.
structures comprises a particular concept and its links to superordinate and subordinate
concepts. Number-Link structures are readily compared in different maps. In Figure 5 the
concept Ethat~oihas a I-Link structure as it has oniy one link to its superordinate concept
superordinates and subordinates. Glyco!vsis is linked to two subordinates (links 6 and 7) and
one superordinate (Li& 4). It is a 3-Link structure. The concept Stage 2 is a CLink structure
(links 7, 10, 1 1 and 12) .
Unlike Method 1. Method 2 does not require lenghy consideration of the data.
Number-Link structures are quickly identified in maps by visual inspection. A complete list of
When the links of Table 1 I are counted a total of 32 is attained. There are only 16
links in Figure 5. The discrepancy arises because Table 1 1 registers each link twice. once for
the concept at the head of a link and once for the concept at the tail. Method 7 can be used to
compare the
Table 11.
1 Fermentation
1 ATP
Glycolysis 3-Link
Stage 1 3-Link
Stage 2 4-Link
progressive differentiation of entire maps provided one remembers that each link is registered
twice. Met hod 2 is better suited to the cornparison of individual concepts. For example.
instruction. It can be said that the instruction stimulated progressive differentiation to the
extent that three new links were generated. If the concept changed from 1 -Link to 3-Link in
another map the progressive differentiation would be considered inferior as only two new
links were generated. Unlike the situation in Table 1 1. cornparisons of individual concepts
Method 2 is included in this thesis to illustrate some of the uses to which concept
simultaneously duhg the learning process. Methods 1 and 2 can be used to reflect learning
associate a concept with the same sub and superordinates then the understanding they express
can be compared through Method 1 (the need for the wne super and subordinates is
expiained in the next paragraph). The concept with the highea Method I rating is said to
each concept. Table 12 illustrates the information that is needed to compare the
and a unistructural link (link 3). Another author creatinç a rnap of fermentation fiom Brock
relational-3 link. Cornparison in this case is a simple matter. the second author exhibits
Bacterirr by a relational-2 link and a unistructural link the cornparison is more cornplicated.
The question of how much more difficult it was to link Lactic dctd Hïcc~rriuas opposed to
Yrast has to be considered. Method 1 cannot answer this question as it fails to consider the
Table 12
1
f
Glycolysis 3-Link Yeast Stage 1 Relational-3
Stage 2 Relational-3
Method 1 and Method 2 are both limited by the fact that their results can't be
integrated into a sinyle expression that ponrays the learning exhibited in a map. Single
expressions of learning make for easy comparisons as the map with the hi&est score can be
the leaming process. Future studies wishins to compare concept rnap knowledge would do
between rnethods 1 and 3 is a subtle one. Both assess the sarne thinç. knowledge. but they do
Biggs and Collis ( 1982) suggest that students' responses to questions have five levels
of quality. This infers that links are created by five levels of inference quality. The following
descriptions of the five inference levels were developed by integrating Biggs and Collis'
SOLO levels with the learniny modes (accretion, amauring, and mning) of Rumelhart and
Norman.
Level 1. At this level inferences involve incorrect reasoniny. Two scenarios are possible. In
89
the first instance the inference connects two data items that are not truly associated with the
concepts of the link. Althouyh the inference is correct the data involved render it irrelevant to
the link. In the second situation the data is appropriate but the inference is incorrect.
Level7. Simple listings of facts that fail to connect data throu@ inferences.
Level 3. Inferences based on fact (inferences that replicate descriptions found in the
instruction)
Level4. Inferences based on facts that were not ovenly linked to the concepts in the
instruction (correct reasonins that builds upon information presented in the instruction).
LeveI 5 . inferences based on facts that were taken from outside the instruction (correct
reasoning that could not have been inferred From information presented in the instruction).
Levels 1.2 and 3 represent the structuring mode of learning. They involve text replication
and relational. The accretion mode of leaMng is represented by level4. Level4 inferences
build upon information presented in the text. Extended unistructural and extended relational
links contain level4 inferences. Level 5 accommodates creative thinking, it is reserved for
extended abstract links and the tuning mode of leaniing. The inference levels represent five
levels of link-procedural knowledge. Method 1 and the works of Biggs and Collis and
90
Rumelhart and Norman susgea that the five inference Ievels of link-procedural knowledge
presented in the te*. The result is a link that connects concepts through inappropriate items
of data.
Stage 7. Concepts are linked through a listing of one or more items of data. No inferences
Stase 2aa.One item of data is lined to link the concepts. No inferences are made.
Stase Ib. Two items of data are listed to link the concepts. No inferences are made.
Stage 3. One item of data links the concepts through an inference based on facts (the
Stage 4. Two or more items of data are linked through inferences based on facts. The
Stase 4a. Two items of data are linked through an inference based on facts.
Stage 4b. Three items of data are linked through two inferences based on facts.
Stage 6. Two or more items of data are linked through inferences based on facts. The
Stage 6a. Two items of data are linked through an inference that was not presented in
presented.
Stage 6b.Three items of data are linked through an inferences. The inference is not
Stage 7. An item of data connects the concepts through an iinfrence based on fact. The
inference does not build on information presented in the text. It is an example of creative
thinking.
Stage 8. Two or more items of data are comected through inferences based on facts. Rather
than building on information presented in the text the inferencets) represent creative thinking.
Stage 8a. Two items of data are linked through an inference that was not
presented in the text. Rather than building on information presented in the text
the inference is an example of creative thinking
Stages 2. 3 and 4 involve text replication. They represent the stmcturing mode of
learning. The building process that typifies the accretion mode is accomrnodated by stases 5
and 6 Stage 7 and 8 are desiyned to represent link structures of the tuning mode. The
incorrect thinkiny of stage 1 links may occur in al1 three leaming modes. The association
between the inference levels and methods I and 3 indicates the author's expectations
relationai links are thought to be associated with stases 5 and 6. Finally. it is expected that
Table 13 contains Method 1 and Method 3 ratings for the links of Figure 5 . These
ratings conform to the author's expectations. Hi&er level structural knowledge is associated
with higher levei link-procedural knowledge. Unistructural links were created by stage 3
inferences. Stage 4a inferences created the relational-? links and relational-3 links are
associated with inferences of the stage 4b level. Figure 5 replicates the description provided
in Brock and Madigan (1988). Consequently the map contains no extended unistructural.
extended multistructurai or extended relational links. These link types would be found in
maps that contain Iinks that are representative of the accretion mode of leaming. Links that
represent the accretion mode connect a concept from the text to a related concept that the
93
Table 13
3 Unistructural 3
4 Relational-? 4a
- - - - - -- -- -- -- -- -
6 Relationai-3 4b
7 Relational-3 4b
8 Unistructurai 3
10 Unistructural 3
11 Unistructurai 3
12 Relational-3 4b
I
13 Unistructural 3
14 Lrnistructurai 3
7.1 1 The interview
link creation. Therefore in assessing link-procedural knowledge Method 3 also assesses the
activity of concept mapping. It is used alongide Method 3 to investigate the cognitive and
metacognitive processes of concept mapping. The format of the i n t e ~ e wwas modeiled after
Method 3 scores were used to select six students from the gifted grade 9 Biology
class. Three of these students exhibited hi@ Method 3 scores or hi@-level cognitive activity
Two exhibited medium-level cognition and low-levei coynitive activity was displayed by the
remaining nudent. The students were interviewed individually over three consecutive lunch
hours in the week followins the mapping exercise. During the interview the interviewer (the
author) and student sat side-by-side at a table with a tape recorder between them. Students
were told that there are no right or wrong answers and that the object of the exercise was to
find out what they think. The i n t e ~ e wquestions were presented in a conversational manner.
If a audent failed to understand a question it was repeated. If repetition of the question failed
to elicit a response it was rephrased. The entire interview was tape recorded.
The i n t e ~ e wquestions and the example answers are represented below. They are
numericd order. Students were prompted with example answen if they codd not respond to
a question or if it was obvious that they had rnisunderstood it. Questions 7, 12 and 13 were
presented differently. Here, students were told to select the example answer that they felt was
most appropriate for the question. They were also asked to explain why they selected that
particular answer. In each of the questions presented below, the first answer typifies
responses that would receive a score of zero. Answers that score zero represent inadequate
metacognitive activity. The second answer is typical of responses that would receive a score
of one. .4 score of one signifies adequate metacognition. Answen that would receive a score
of two are represented by the third answer. Superior metacognitive activity is represented by
answers that would receive a score of two. There are 13 i n t e ~ e wquestions and the
3.How do the concepts at the top of a concept map differ corn those at the bottom?
There is no difference between them
Concepts at the top represent the beginning of the story
Concepts at the top are general, they encompass specific concepts that reside at the
bottom of maps
7. Which of the following do you think would be easier to rnap and why?
A passage with a lot of pictures
A passage that is easy to understand
h passas you have read before
8. Why do you reread the text before you linli some concepts together?
Because you forgot what you had read
Because it is sood practice to do so
Because you don't understand the concept's relationship
10. Which parts of the text do you omit tiom the concept map?
You include everythiny
The parts you don't understand
The unimponam parts that don't add to the concept map
1 1. While you read the text what do you do to help you remember it?
1 don't do anything to help me remember
Concentrate hard and try to remember
Ask purself questions about the important parts
12. If you were creating a rnap in preparation for a test, whîch of the following would help
you the most and why?
Saying the concepts over and over
Read the rnap as many times as possible
Discuss the map with someone else to make sure you understand it
13. If you are creating a map ffom a technical passage. which would help you the most and
why?
Sounding out the words you don't know
Skipping the parts you dont understand
Wnting the passage out in your own words before starting the map
The students' tape recorded responses were transcribed. Lengthy responses were
paraphrased and summarized. The transcribed responses were rated against the example
answers. Pans and Jacobs ( 1984) claimed t hat this marking system reflects metacogitive
awareness. Therefore the higher the intewiew score the higher metacoyitive activity of' the
mapper.
This section summaizes the sconng systems of the three rnethods. In Metbod 1 link
rankings are based upon three factors: the amount of data in a link. the extent to which that
data are synthesized. and the novelty of the data. Links that simply repeat the text are
concepts with irrelevant data. Lhistructural, multistmcturai and relational links are identified
by counting relevant data items. Unistructural links contain jus one item of data.
Multistructural and relational links contain two items of data or more. Unistructural and
relationai links synthesize data into a single phrase (the linking statement). Multistructural
links fail to exhiiit data imegration. UniaructuraL multistnictural and relational links
replicate the text. They may represent leaming that occurred in the aructuring or accretion
modes.
98
Links that build upon information provided in the text represent a higher b e l of
structural knowledge. They represent the accretion mode of Ieaming. These Iinks are Iabelled
abstract links contain knowledge from outside the instruction. They represent the tuning
mode and they irnprove upon the tefi's description of links. Extended abstract links integrate
data and may provide multiple conclusions, they represent the hi&est level of structural
knowledge.
Met hod 2 enurnerates the links associated with individual concepts. Once enumerated
the Iinks are used to provide each concept with a Number-Link score. Method 2 is well suited
not well suited to the assessrnent of entire maps. Funhermore it cannot distinpish between
concepts that have an equal number of links. Methods 1 and 7 can be used in conjunction to
assess the progressive differentiation and integrative reconciliation associated with individual
concepts. If concepts have identical Nurnber-Link and SOLO ratinys. a finer scde is required
for differentiation. Methods 1 and 1 would benefit greatly if future studies locate a means by
which the cognitive wonh of one link type cm be expressed in terms of another. Such a
system would make it possible to compare maps whose links differ in number and quality.
Method 3 recognizes the same factors as Method 1. Links that replicate the text
contain either structunng or accretion inferences. They are accorded the lowest score. A
higher level of inference occurs in accretion links that build upon the text. Links that
represent the tuning mode contain the highest level of inference. Regardless of the leamhg
mode. the more data involved in a link the higher the quality of the link-procedural
knowledge.
Table 13 indicates that the quality of structural knowledge is related to the quality of
link-procedural knowledge. This finding is encouraging as it suygests that empincal data will
generate a similar finding. Comparable structural and link-procedural knowledge supports the
thinking that the structural and procedural modalities are isomorphic phenomena.
permit a comparison of the cognitive and metacognitive processes of mappers. Findiny that
Method 3 and intewiew scores are comparable is interpreted as beiny supportive of the
argument that the cognitive and metacognitive processes of concept mappins are related
processes.
Chapter 8
The main aryument developed in this thesis has been that the quality of structural
is exarnined here by applyiny methods 1 and 3 to links that exhibit high and low quality
knowledge. Given the need for hi@ quality links, a gifted grade nine Biology class was
chosen for this snidy. It was felt that their high academic abilities would enable the necessary
production of high quality links. Students of lesser ability may be incapable of producing high
level links. A second reason for choosing a bioiogy class is that the researcher has some
experience in this domain. This experience is needed to gauge the accuracy of students' links.
The research was nin over five school days. In the first session the class' 27 students
were introduced to concept mapping through a twenty minute lecture (section 8.7) that was
delivered by the researcher. On completion of the lecture the researcher ran the students en
masse through a concept map that depiaed the making of a cup of tea. It was thouçht that
this farniliar activity would introduce mapping with a minimum of cognitive strain. For the
remainder of the class (and for homework) the students were asked to pnerate a concept
map that depiaed their understanding of the causes and consequences of oil spills. They
were provided with three key concepts Tattkws, Oil Spilis and C'lran Cl'. They were aiso
told to think of 7at1ker.sand CIem [Jp as anchor concepts for two regions subsumed beneath
Asking students to map their understanding of oil spills had three purposes. First it
101
provided students with an opportunity to practice concept mapping. Second it allowed the
researcher to identiq and correct problems with student's mapping techniques. Incorrect iink
The third purpose of the mapping exercise was to prime students' thinking so that they could
easily incorporate the target text "Water Pollution and Oil Slicks" into their existing
knowledge. The target text was taken from the "Environmental Database for use in SchooIs"
affiliated with the University of Southampton in the United Kingdom. The target text is
presented in Appendix A.
In the second class students read the tarset text and began to map it. The class ran on
a Fnday and students were yiven the weekend to complete the maps. M e r markiny the maps
the researcher selected six students for metacognitive interviews. Two of the six subjects had
Iow Method 3 (cognitive process) scores, two had medium scores and two had hi@ hlethod
3 scores. Students with low. medium and high Method 3 scores were selected in response to
the reasoning that Method 3 and interview scores would increase in parallet. In other words a
range of cognitive abilities were selected for i n t e ~ e wso that the cornparability of cognitive
Before the midy began students were asked to sign a letter of consent. The letter is
presented in Appendix B. It explained the nature and purpose of the snidy and it assured
participants that al1 Somation would be kept contidential. Students who wished to opt out
1Oz
of the study were given the option of reading the target text and continuing with the regular
curriculum. Altematively students could simply continue with the regular cumculum. Letters
of consent were also presented to the teacher who nomally teaches the class and the school
Running the research successfilly requires participants who are farniliar with concept
rnapping and the research pals. The followiny section presents the written instructions
Concept maps are created by identifying relationships that link one concept to
another. In most maps the concepts are arranged hierarchicdly. General concepts tend to
appear at the top of the hierarchy and specific concepts tend to appear at the bonom. The
completed map reveais students' understanding of a topic. White and Gunstone ( 1992)
explain the benefits and educational uses of concept mapping. Their explmation is
surnrnarized below.
cognitive resources are so fûlly taken up with knowledge acquisition that they never engage
in the reflective processes that cari Link a topic to other subjects. Concept mapping presents
midents with the opponunity to step back, see "the big picture" and create connections
across topics or themes. Creating links forces students to confiont their cornprehensions.
maps are also a tool of rnernory that requires the retrieval and reprocessing of knowledge.
Reprocessing strengthens the links between concepts. It also increases the potential for future
recall.
behind their comprehension enables them to better expiain the mechanics of comprehension
to their students. Each time a teacher maps a topic a different aspect (or perspective) of it
may be emphasized. Providing students with multiple perspectives leads to more cornpiete
understanding than the presentation of a single perspective (Spiro, Jacobson. Feltovich and
Concepts such as Wuter, Tra Bug and Tra C i p are presented in rectangles. The rectangles
cm be thought of as iconic representations of the concept narned within. Links express the
mapper's understandimg of concepts. They are the hart of concept maps. Each link reveais
somethlng of the understanding surnmarized by the rectanqular icon. The nurnber and quality
1O4
of links reflect the mapper's understanding. Table 14 contains descriptions of the links of
Figure 1 1.
This research asks students to create concept maps. Three methods have been devised
to assess these maps. The methods are desiged to estimate the quality of student learning.
They also reveal something of the cognitive processes that students engage in as they acquire
comprehension. Mer the concept maps have been assessed. six students will be inte~ewed
to
Table 14.
elicit the metacognitive processes they used to guide their thinking while mapping. It is
1 O5
suspected that high level metacognitive activity will be displayed by students who create high
quality links.
If the methods are to work they require that students provide complete descriptions
of links. Complete Iink descriptions reveal something of the cognitive processes that students
statement and a listing of the items of information that were used to yenerate the statement.
Linking statements stipulate the relationship between link concepts. Each piece of
information concems one aspect of the link relationship. For example, link 3 of Figure I 1 has
the linking statement "Allow the tea to steep to desired concentration." Four pieces of
Each item of information explains one aspect of the steeping process. It is these pieces of
information that reflect the mapper's cognitive processes. Some encouragement may be
needed to engender students with the confidence they need to e.xpress their cognitive
processes in links.They should be informed of the fact that if' items of information are
Linking staternents vary in qudity. Any linking statement that comprises a single piece
of information is cailed a unistructural link. Links 1, 2,4. 5, and 6 of Figure 1 1 are
unistructurai (see Table 14). Their single piece of information is identicai to the linking
statement. Such links are pefiectly leyitimate. In other unistructural links the single piece of
information may differ from the linking statement. These links are also legitimate.
statements conjoin pieces of information with phrases such as "and," "or" and "also."
Relational links differ from multistnictural links in that their linking statements inteyrate
pieces of information into a single phrase. Link 3 of Figure 1 1 is a relational link. Its linkiny
statement "allow the tea to neep to desired concentration" represents an integration of four
items of data. If link 3 were a multistnictural link its linking statement might be phrased
"boiling water dissolves cornponents of tea and convection currents distnbute them. The
prevent oversteeping."
relational. Extended abstract links represent an even higher quality. These links contain at
least one piece of information that was not presented in the instruction. They represent an
attempt to add extant knowledge to descriptions provided in the instruction. There are no
extended abstract links in Figure 1 I but several could be created. For example. Iink 3 could
Sometimes links will contain invaiid linking statements andor pieces of information. These
links are cded prestructurai links. They are not included in the assessrnent procedure.
8.3 lntroducing students to concept maps
Research with concept maps requires students who are competent concept mappers.
White and Gunstone ( 1992) provide a method that can produce competent mappers in shon
order. The following points represent an adaptation of t heir method. The researcher used this
adaptation to introduce concept maps and the research. The adaptation was presented to
1. Prior to class select a simple topic that is familiar to the students. Choose a few key
concepts and senerate a concept map. Insert linking statements and appropriate data into the
map.
3. At the beçi~ingof class present the typical hierarchical structure of a concept map on an
overhead projector or blackboard. Point out the significance of hierarchical levels. regions,
and seneralized and specific components. Inform students that creating maps can improve
4. Display the key concepts from point 1 on a blackboard or overhead projector. Arrange the
concepts according to the concept map created before class. DO NOT connect the concepts
at this stage.
6. Ask students to explain how the concepts are related to each other. Place these
explanations on the blackboard and comect the appropriate concepts with an arrowed link.
7. After drawing each link. ask the students for the rationale behind the positioning of the
arrow's head and tail. If no answer is forthcornhg provide one. Invariably the explmation will
be that the direction is expressed explicitly or implicitly in the linking statement. An explicit
directional cue might be "concept A performs action X on concept B." An implicit cue such
as "action X impacts on concept B " indicates t hat the arrow's head should be adjacent to
8. Ask the students to identify how they fonned the linking statements. One way to do this is
to ask what "information" they used to make the linking statement. If this open-ended
question fails to elicit the desired response, provide clues usin3 the concept map created in
point 1. Table 1.1 contains the linking statements and data of Figure 1 1 . It illustrates correct
linking procedure. each item of information is numbered and liaed beneath the linking
statement. Be sure to model correct link structure when creating links from information that
audents provide.
9. Once data items are identified and the map is cornpiete the audents should be made aware
of the following:
a) items of information that match the linking statement are acceptable when there is
10. Reiterate the point of the exercise using the following as a guide.
a) Tell students what they cm expect to set from the exercise. Perhaps the most
tangible benefit that students wil1 latch ont0 is that concept mapping has the potentid to
1O9
improve their comprehension and retention. tnform them that concept mapping is a good
habit to develop.
b) Note that the aim of t he research is to leam about the cognitive processes that
students enyage in as t hey lem. Make students aware t hat the success of the research
depends on their generating a linking statement and data for each link.
I I . Ask nudents to generate a practice concept map. inform them that the subject will be the
causes and consequences of oil spills. Provide the followiny key concepts Od .Sp/Ls, 7a11ker.s
and ('iean tlp and tell them to use T~mkersand <'lean Up to anchor regions beneath the
concept 0 1 1Spdls. Hand out the written instructions for concept mapping. These instructions
13. Provide each student with a sheet of bristol board and a set of post-it notes. Students will
arrange concepts on the bristol board as they see fit. When students are satisfied with their
concept arransements they will stick the post-it notes onto the bristol board and create
14. Check to ensure that linking staternents and data are entered correctly. Provide corrective
instruction as necessary.
15. Hand out the target text and another sheet of bristol board.
16. Ask students to read the target text and map it. They should use the three key concepts
Oïl Spills, Tankers and C'lm Clp. In addition they should incorporate information fiom the
instructions were designed to reinforce the researcher lecture. They describe the vaiious steps
that have to be followed in order that a concept map can be created. The instructions are
provided in appendix C. Appendix C also contains written instructions that explain how to
apply methods 1.2 and 3 to the students concept maps. These marking instructions were
Some assurance is needed that the taqet text is suited to concept rnapping and the
goals of the thesis. To provide this assurance a concept map of the target text is provided in
Figure 12. Table 1 5 describes the links of Figure 12. Table 16 contains method 1, and 3
ratings for Figure 12's links. it also contains Method 2 ratings for the key concepts Tmrkrrs.
Oil Spills and ('lean ilp. Methods 1.2 and 3 are applied to Figure 12 to meet the first and
second p a l s of the thesis. The third goal was not assessed as it requires multiple i n t e ~ e w
scores.
Table 15.
1 Link Number
-- -- --
Description
The Gulf was is an example of an oil spill
1. The Gulf was is an exarnple of an oil spi11
- - -
Table 16.
Tankers 3-Link
Link 4 Unistructural 3
Link 5 Unistructural 3
Link 6 Unistructural 3
Link 10 Relational-2 4a
Link 1 1 Relationai-2 4a
Link 12 Relational-3 4b
.Link 13 Unistructural 3
Clean Up 6-Link
Link 7 Unistructural 3
Link 8 Unistructural 3
1
Link 9 Unistructural 3
Link 14 Relational-4 3c
Link 15 Relational4 4c
Link 16 Relational-3 4b
The links of Figure 12 repeat descriptions of concept relationships that are provided
in the text. ïhey represent leaming in the amturing. mode. Al1 unistructural links were rated
at the stage 3 Ievel of Method 3. Higher quality relational links were rated among the
divisions of stage 4. This finding is encouraging as it supports the thinking that structurai
behaviour predicted by theory integration. It also suppons the reasoning that concept rnaps
differentiation and integrative reconciliation. It provided scores for entire maps. Method 1
and Method 2 cm be used alongside the Novak and Gowin rnethod, they provide assessments
individual concepts. Unlike Novak and Gowin they provide scores for individual concepts.
links. (-%cm I/p was rated as a 6-Link concept. Three of its links were rated at the
unistructural level, one link was a relational-3 Iink and two were reIational-4 links It cm be
associated with more concepts. It can also be said that ( ' I ~ C I ~ilp
I exhibits hiyher level
integrative reconciliation than Tui~kers.On average the ('lem iIp links have a higher Method
1 ratiny than the Iinks associated with Ïàtzker-S.Oil Spilis exhibits intermediary levels of
progressive differentiation and integrative reconciliation. It is a 4-Link concept and al1 of its
links are unistructural links. The links associated with OilSpilLs are fewer in number and on
average are of Iower quality than the links associated with C'ka11 Ilp. The links associated
with Oil Spills have the same quality as links associated with Tcnlkers. but there are more of
them.
The above analysis demonstrates that the target text is suited to this thesis as it is
arnenable to concept rnapping. Furthemore methods 1.2 and 3 can be applied towards an
analysis of a map of the target tes. The next chapter presents the concept maps of three
students. The remainder of the thesis concems the analysis of these maps.
Chapter 9
Each of the grade 9 biology class' 27 students produced a map of the target text.
Data analysis began with the application of methods 1 and 3 to these maps. The Method 3
ratin~swere used to select six subjects for the metacoynitive interview. The intent when
applying Method 3 was to find two interviewees from each of the followinç categories. low.
medium and high cognitive activity. Only one subject exhibited low level cognitive activity
(low Method 3 ratings), so a third subject of hi& level coyitive activity was selected for
This chapter discusses an example of each of the Method I link types contained in the
rnaps of subjects 1, 2 and 6. Assessments of ail six subjects are presented in appendix D.
Subjects 1.2 and 6 were selected for discussion as they represent examples of high, medium
and Iow level cognitive activity. Discussion of their links reveals the logic behind Method 1
and Method 3 ratings. It aiso reveals the nature of link-procedural knowiedge. In discussing
the links reference is made to the accretion, nmcturing and tuning modes ofleaming. The
discussion also considers the inference levels that govem link quality. These inference Ievels
Level 2. Simple listings of facts that fail to connect data throuyh inferences.
Level3. Inferences based on fact, (inferences that replicate descriptions found in the
instruction)
Level4. Inferences based on facts that were not overtly linked to the concepts in the
instninion (correct reasoning that builds upon information presented in the
instruction).
Level 5 . Inferences based on facts that were taken corn outside the instruction
(correct reasoning that could not have been inferred from information presented in the
instruction).
Interrater reliability was established for metbods 1, 2 and 3 by comparing the ratings
of the researcher and two additional raters. The maps involved in this cornparison were the
maps of subjects two and three. Subjects two and three produced a total of 47 links. Six of
these links were general knowledge links. GeneraI knowledge links represent points at which
the learner's enant knowledge has been associated with the target text. They add context to a
map rather than replicatinç or building upon it. For example, link 1 of subject 2 states that the
"shipping industry manufactures tankers." This fact, while accurate?is not mentioned in the
text. nor is it an example of creative thinking. The link represents a point where the mident
associated the text with eisting knowledge. That is, it places the contents of the target text
within the larger context of the knowledge that the student considers to be associated with
oils spills.
General knowledge links were handled in the following way.Raters were provided
with representations of the maps of subjects two and three. The general knowledge links of
these representations were preidentified by the researcher. The critena for identification were
that a link should not replicate or build upon the text, not should it involve creative thinking.
Raters were told to rate the remaininy links by the scales of methods I and 3. They were not
asked to identiG geeoeeral knowledge links as it was felt that they would find it too difficult to
distinguish them From other link types. They were toid to be on the look out for links that
represent generd knowledge. They were also told to brins such links to the researcher's
attention.
For Method 1 the three raters agreed on 14 of the 4 1 none yeneral knowledp links.
Expressing the agreed upon number of links as a percentage of the total generated an
interrater reliability of62%. Interrater reliability was cdculated for Method 3 in the same
way. It was found to be 5 1%. These low-Ievel interrater reliabilities may be due to poor
training of the raters? inadequate descriptions of the marking scales. and lack of rater
communication. The issue of training is considered fira. The raters were teachers who ageed
to mark the concept maps in the last week of tem. They had no time to partake in practice
marking exercises. One or more durnrny nins with exemplary links would no doubt increase
interrater reliability Ideally these practice runs would garner feedback from the raters that
would be used to irnprove the descriptions of the marking scaies of Method 1 and Method 3 .
It would dso be preferable if the raters were to work together during the dummy mns. Their
communications could be focussed toward reaching a consensus on link types that are
118
difficult to identifi (general knowledge links, links with fiapented data etcetera). Intenater
reliability was 100% for Method 2. The three raters reached agreement on number-link
ratings for the three key concepts of the maps of subjects 2 and 3.
formed will reveal the nature of link-procedural knowledge. Methods 1 and 3 use two
that concept mappers express items of data in ways that reveal how inferences are formed.
The second assumption is that linking statements describe relationships between concepts. If
these assumptions are not met it is hoped that linking aatements and data will be worded in
such a Nay that simple reasoning can be used to divulee how inferences were formed. Most
of the links created for this thesis failed to render the reasonins behind inference creation
explicit. It is suggested that a more extensive training program (for the students) would
increase the incidence of links that are stmctured so as to reveal the manner in which their
9.2.1 Subject L
The paper concept rnap of subject 1 is reproduced in Figure 13. The links of this rnap
are described in Table 17. By Method 1 there are: nine extended relational-7, links. eight
extended unistmctural links, three relational-2 links and two prestructural links. By method 3
there are nine aage 6a links, eight aage 5 links, three aage 4a links and two aage 1 Links.A
number of links could not be classified. For example. link I was not suited to analysis by
Method 3 as it was deemed to be a general knowledge Iiink. Method 3 cannot account for
general knowledge links. It recognizes inferences that replicate or build upon the text. It also
recognizes inferences that contain creative thinking. Links 22, 26 and 27 were excluded from
the analysis as they contain imprecise references to inferences contained in other links. It was
Tankm 7 10 Cltgi Up
3 *-
-r-->-13 --T-F14
.--A- _Y - -
44 7 - - Ai?
DoubleHuil Am A-
N a v i g a t W \ Pawerful wulaJ
- - - -. .
- -- -- - -
Table 17
Oil mntaincrs with the sarnc inna prwsurr: as outsidc pnwurc will
spill ILM oil
a) I w oil siills
b ) the dis;i';ttx is ILSS p r i t
Tankers wt $
a) hypothctiçally: morc S. bcttcr ship
Satkh p r d u r c s cost $
a ) hpothetically : morc S. bctttr prwedurt-s
b 1- S. w o w p r o d m s
iank~rsrcquih' w o f k ~ r ~
a ) more workm. l a s r;hanc;c.of probiems
b ) l a s workm- morc chrince of probltms
W o r k m cost S
a) more S. bcita qliiiiity of w o r k m
b) las S. Iowx quafi5 of worktxs
Workcxs app- de' p r o d m
a ) b s t t a quriity w o r k m = bettm quatic p r d m
b) 1 0 % quality
~ ~ work~rs= low-tr qualin. pror;cdm?i
A clmn up requins worktm
a) higher qualie w o r k m = higher qualie dem up
Iowm qualie w o r k m = Iowa quaic çlem up
Warktxs must apply p d m
a) highm qualie worktrs = bighm quality appliçaiion of p d u r e
b) lowm quaiity worktrs = Iowa quality application of p r d u n . ;
Naîural d i s i i o n is a clean up p d m
a) it is 80N cfkwtiuc
b) somctim~?ibas sidc ctlitxts
- . -- - - - - - - -. .-- - -. - -.
T h t x wst mon.;
CMrction and ('km Up PrucrJiires. Its data stipulates that collection gets al1 of the oil that
is spilled. It also concludes that collection is a 100% effective clean up method. The text does
state that oil is completely removed from the environment once it has been collected.
122
However it does not imply that al1 the oil that spills frorn a tanker can be collected. Thus
collection is not 100% effective as some of the oil that is spilled will not be collectible. The
and Chernid Dispersim. The linking statement stipulates that chemical dispersion is an
example clean up method. The data accurately relates the teds description of the
effectiveness of chemical dispersion. One datum States that chernical dispersion "is sometimes
effective" the other says that "it always has side effects." The data of this link does not elicit
how the linking statement was foned. Furthemore reasoning about the data and linking
statement fails to determine the rnethod of inference formation. This link was said to be
disconnected.
A nurnber of the links exhibit the disconnected structure of link 18. They were rated
by f i r ~assuming that the mapper would have described the reasoning behind their formation
if they had been exposed to a more extensive training progam. Next, data were counted and
it was determined whether the link replicated. built upon or restructured the text. Links that
repeat the text represent the strucniring mode of leaniing. The tuning mode is represented by
links that improve upon descriptions of relationships as provided in the text. That is. tuning
links integrate the mapper's extant knowledge into the text's description of a relationship.
Accretion links coruiect a relevant concept that is not mentioned in the text to a concept that
the text does mention. That is mappen add extant knowledge to the text when creating
accretion links. The amount of data and the learning mode constitute the structure of the link.
Once link structure is known it is rnatched againa the scales of Methods 1 and 3. Table 18
123
indicates the association between the leaming modes and the scales of Methods 1 and 3. Link
18 integrates two items of data and it represents the structunng mode of leaming. This
structure corresponds to the relationai-2 rating of Method I and stage 4a of Met hod 3.
understanding of the inference that lead to link creation. Link 18 was not interpreted in terms
of the theones of readiny comprehension as its disconnected data failed to elicit the reasoniny
that lead to the creation of its inference. Ascertaining the method of inference formation also
elicits the functioning of link-procedurai knowledge. In links were the rationale behind
inference formation was identified. link-procedurai knowledge was Found to mimic the
Table 18
Link 6 can be interpreted in tems of the Kirby ( 1991 ) and Jacobs and Paris ( 1987)
theories. Successive processing held the concepts Nmigationul Radar and Safey Procedztres
accidents. This knowledge lead to the conclusion that the goal of Scrftiiy PruceJ11re.s(lower
Link 6 is an example extended unistructural link. Its single datum (less crashes will
occur) is used to generate a linking statement (better navigational radar will allow obstacles
to be avoided). The linking statement irnplies that navigational radar should be adopted as a
knowledge of navigational radar with the text's admonition for the adoption of safety
procedures. Thus link 6 involves a singe item of data and an inference that is relevant to. but
was not presented in the text. This stnicture matches Met hod 1 's description of ex~ended
link 6. link 7 represents the accretion mode of leaming. The concept More Pu~tv~$d
E~zgines
is relevant to the text but was not presented in it. The inference of this link involved the
knowledge that safety procedures seek to lower accident rates. It also involved the
understanding that more powerful engines can increase tuming speed and thereby increase
the tanker's ability to avoid obstacies. Thus it was concluded that more powerfbl engines can
reduce the number of accidents and rneet the goal of d e t y procedures. When simple
reasoning was applied to the data and linking statement of this link it provided an indication
of how the inference was forrned. Sequential processing held both concepts in working
memory (as discrete units) while simultaneous processing sought an inference to comect
125
them. Thus reasoning indicates that the formation of this link emulated the comprehension
rating was derived h m the fact that the link is an accretion link. It took extant knowledge
about engine power and associated it with the text's description of safety procedures. The
relationaI-2 component was derived From the link's structure. Two items of data were
integated to fonn a Iinking statement. The structure of link 7 matches the Method 3
description of stage 6a. Stage 6a refers to link-procedural knowledge that connects two items
It is suspected that Subject 1 ran out of time when creating Figure 13. This
supposition is based on the fact that links 19 to 25 are incomplete. They contain linking
aatements but no data. Methods 1 and 3 were associated with these links by assuming that
they contained one item of data that is identical to their linking statements. The iinking
statement of link 19 "workers are not needed for natural dispersion" builds upon information
provided in the text. Thus link 19 was said to represent the accretion mode of learning.
Accretion links that contain a single datum are extended unistructural or stage 5 links. Links
20 to 25 also received extended unistructuraYstage 5 ratings. they involve the accretion mode
the Method 3 (cognitive activity) scores, 39% were of stage 6a 39% were of nage 5. 13%
were of stage 4a and 9% were of stage 1. These results can a h be interpreted in tems of
inference levels. The inferences of stage 6a and stage 5 involve correct reasoning that builds
126
upon information presented in the text. They constitute 78% of the links in Figure 13 and
they contain level4 inferences. The inferences of the stage 4a links replicate descriptions
found in the text. They contain level3 inferences and represent 13% of Figure 13's links.
Stage 1 links contain incorrect reasoniny. They contain level 1 inferences and make up 9% of
Figure 13's links. The predominance of level4 inferences in Fiwre 13 lead to the hi$ level
When Methods 1 and 3 were applied to the map of subject ? they revealed a
predominance of level3 inferences. Level3 inferences were the only inferences produced by
subject 6. .411 of subject 6's links were unistructural links. Subject 2 produced higher quality
links than subject 6. Subjects 2 and 6 were said to exhibit medium and low level cogitive
following sections.
9.2.2 Su bject 2
The paper concept rnap of subject 2 is reproduced in Figure 14. The links of Figure
14 are described in Table 19. By Method 1 there are: one extended relational-3 link. one
extended relational-2 link, one relational-4 link, six relationai-3 links. six relational-2 links
and seven unistructural links. By Method 3 there are one aage 6b link, one aage 6a link one
stage 4c link six stage 4b links, six stage Ja links and seven stage 3 links. Link 1, was
classified as a general knowledge link as were links 3 and 4. Link 9 was deemed unmarkable
by Method 1. The problem is that the data of link 9 are overly tiagrnented. At least two of the
data items are surplus to requirements as they are part and parcel of ideas presented
elsewhere in the data. For example. the datum "preventing oil spills" encompasses "if
accident occurs. stop them from leaking." The data "stop them from haviny accidents" is
repeated in the datum " avoidiny oil spills." Furthemore this datum encompasses the notion
of "plotting routes to avoid accidents." Let's take a closer look at these data.
The "preventing oil spills" refers to actions that can be taken to prevent oil spilis. The text
sussested that tankers be fitted with hydrostatic controls and double hulls to inhibit oil
leakage &er a collision occurs. These control measures are referred to in the daturn "if
accident occurs. stop them tom leaking." Raters should be wary of mappers who overly
frament their ideas and knowledge. Lhdetected. this situation cm lead to erroneously
ci
eievated Method 1 scores. For example, if al1 the data of link 9 were considered appropnate
it would have gamered a "relational-5" score. Ideally the data items "preventingoil spills" and
"if accident occurs. stop them from leakin&' would have been presented as a single datum.
This datum might have been "measures have been implemented to prevent oil spills once a
collision occurs."
The followin%(link 9) data items also suffer from too vigorous a fkagnentation.
The data "avoiding oiI spills" and "stop them tom having accidents' are essentially redundant.
They both refer to the measures that Maritime Authonties recommend should be taken to
128
avoid tanker accidents. "Plotting routes to avoid accidents" is actually part of the datum
"avoidiq oil spills," it refers to one of the Maritime Authonties' prevention measures. ldeally
dl three data items would have been presented as a single datum. This datum miyht have
been "Maritime Authorities have introduced measures to avoid accidents. One of these
measures is the plotting routes to avoid other vessels and inclement weather." A follow up
interview with subject 9- would be needed to determine how many lqitirnate data items are
present in link 9.
- - -- --
Fiyures 13 and 14 have several linking structures in common. For example. they both
subjects. The sequential processing of link 35 held the concepts, d i r p l m s and Bisp~.r.wm.
the concepts. The second datum of this link "Airplanes spray dispersant on the oil" refers to
the manner in which airplanes are used in clean up. The first datum "Responsemust be fast"
reveais that dispersants have to be applied quickly. The inference of link 25 generates the
realization that airplanes meet the needs of chemicai dispersants. They are the fastest means
two items of data in a manner that replicates the text. fts actions correspond to those of the
Uniike Figure 13, Figure 14 contains links that were rated as unistructural/stage 3.
these link types is provided below. The description omits reference to the theories of reading
132
comprehension. In al1 cases it is assumed that link-procedural knowledye fùnctions as per the
description of simultaneous processing that was provided for the links of Figure 13. That is it
is assumed that concepts are held in working memory as discrete units while simultaneous
Link 22 is the example unistructurai/sta~e3 Iink. It contains one item of data and it
represents the structuriny mode of leaming. Its linking statement notes t hat ('hrmical
Bispzr.si~nis an example < 'ka11I/p procedure. Only one item of data is present in this link. It
states that "chernicals are used to disperse oil."This information reveds that chemical
dispersion is a clean up method for once oil is sufficiently dispersed it is said to be cleaned up.
The single datum of link 22 does not match the Iinking statement. Page 74 of this
thesis discussed unistructural links whose datum differs from the linking statement. They
were said to involve an item of data that the mapper is unaware of or unable to express. The
understanding of these links was said to be intemediary to the unistructural and relational
links in that it contains an additional item of data. The concepts Dzspersu~rtsand Chrmtc~af
Disperrsa~sare comected in link 24. Two items of data are used to indicate how dispersants
work. They "'break up oil slicks" and the "slick dilutes into water." Both of these data items
are integrated in the linking statement which states that "dispersantsbreak up oil slicks."
Using these data and the linking statement it is possible to reason as to how link-procedural
knowledge comected the concepts of the link. If it is known that "dispersants cause oil to
disperse" and that "dispersed oil is diluted in water." then it is possible to conclude that
"dispersants break up oil slicks."
The third datum of link 24 stipulates that dispersants "can only be used on certain
oils." It is difficult to reason how this datum was associated with the data items "break up oil
slick" and "slick dilutes into water" by the inference of this link. The problem is not fatal
however provided we assume that subject 2 did integrate the three items of data but lacked
the traininy to express the relationship in full. This assumption is not unreasonable for a
relationship associatins al1 three data items is described in the text. Subject 2 would have
been aware of this relationship. Links which use three items of data to replicate the text
match the Method I description of relational-3 links. They also correspond to the 4b stase of
Method 3 .
Link 20 is the example reiational-4 link. Like link 24 it represents the structurin~
mode of leaming. Stnicturing is indicated whenever a link repeats descriptions that are found
in the teM. Text replication suggests that the mapper possessed no additional knowledge
regarding the link in question. Link 20 connects the concepts Adhrsioti and Skimmrrs. Its
example of the adhesion clean up method. The data of this link is presented below. It
describes the skimmins operation and in doing so it indicates that skimming is an adhesion
method.
The data of Iink 20 exemplifies the link structure that was envisioned during the creation of
explicating their lin k-procedural know ledge. Further training wouid increase the production
Link 13 is the exarnple extended relational-3 link. The text describes a number of
clean up methods and hints at the need for safety. Link 13 improves on the text as it correctly
assumes that before clean up cm beçin, some time should be spent selecting the clean up
method that will minimize environmental impact. Link l 3 is not an extended abstract link as
the improvement foilows logically from the text. it does not represent an example of creative
thinking. Like link 20. the data of link 13 is worded to reveal the actions of its link-
procedural knowled~e.
Figre 14 represents medium level cognitive activity. Twenty two of the twenty six
links were marked. Of the Method 3 scores, 5% were of stage 6b. 5% were stage 6a 5%
were stage 4c. 27% were stage 4b. 27% were stage 4a and 32% were stage 3. The links of
stages 6b and 6a build upon information presented in the text. They conaitute 9% of the
rnap's links and represent level four inferences. The remainin_glinks repeat information that
was presented in the text. They represent 91% of the map links and contain a predominance
of level3 inferences. Subjects whose maps contained a predominance of level3 links were
The paper concept map of subject 6 is reproduced in Figure 1 5 The links of this map
are described in Table 20. Each link is a unistmcturall stage 3 link. Link 3 was selected as an
example
unistructural link. It connects the concepts Cases and Politicx The linking statement of this
Link is exactly the same as its datum "Politicsare a cause."One item of data and a correct
inference matches the unistmctural iink of Method 1 and stage 3 of Method 3. The links in
Figure 15 contain level 3 inferences, they repeat descriptions of concept relationships that are
provided in the text. The predominance of unistnictural Iuiks lead to the low level CO-@ive
activity rating.
Table 20
Subjects 1,2 and 6 were selected for the above analysis as their maps exhibit high,
medium and low level cognitive activity. A number of problems were encountered when
trying to assess the link-procedural knowledge of the midents maps. The sarne problems
137
arose during the assessment of the maps of subjects 3 . 4 and 5 . These assessment problems
were caused by students' failure to structure links correctly. Methods 1 and 3 require that
linking statements describe relationships, they also require that data reveal how the
described concept relationships. data oflen failed to elicit inference formation. When the
requisite link structure was not produced. raters had to determine the quality of link-
between data and linkiny statements. Links such as link 13 and link 30 of subject 2 did
contain the requisite structure. The presence of these links lead to the suggestion that a more
The interpretive rating method elicited the nature of Iink-procedural knowledge. Its
actions were said to compare to those of sirnultaneous processinç. That is, it sought to
identiS, relationships that connect discrete units (items of data) in workiny memory.
5 and 3 were also exarnined by the above methods. Subjects 4 and 5 exhibited high level link-
procedural knowledge (high level cognitive activity). Most of their inferences were of leveI4.
This chapter examines the concept maps of the six subjects who were chosen for the
metacognitive interview. Methods 1 and 3 are used to investigate the daim that concept maps
represent mental models of problems. Further evidence for this argument is presented by
examining the students' Method 3 and metacognitive interview scores. The results of both
investigations support the suppositions derived from theory integration. Structural and link-
procedural knowledge were found to have comparable quality. Metacognitive activity was
The concept maps of the six subjects are also subjected to an e:wnination by Methods
1 and 2. The methods were used to assess the progressive differentiation and inteyrative
reconciliation associated with individual conceptS. The investigation met the second goal of
the thesis in that it demonarated that methods 1 and 3 cm be used to assess concept maps. It
also indicated that Methods 1 and 2 provide assessments of individuai concepts that
complement total map scores that result fiom the application of Novak and Gowin's ( 1984)
method .
Methûd 1 and Method 3 ratings can be used to connrua a picture of the relationship
between structural and link-procedural knowledge. Table 2 1 illustrates the link types that
were produced by subject 1. Two thinçs are apparent in this table. Fust, links with the same
Method 1 rat@ are associated with the same stage of Method 3. For example. each
prestructurai link had a stage I rating and each relational-:! link had a stage 4a ratiny.
Second, the higher the quality of the met hod 1 rating,, t he higher the Method 3 stase.
Table 21
structurai and link-procedural knowledge. The analyses of Figures 13 and 14 are combined in
Table 72. The findings of Table 22 are not unique to subjeas I and 2, al1 six subjects
exhibited this comparable quality of knowledge across methods I and 3. Every link of a
particular Method 1 rating was associated with the same stage of Method 3.
Table 22 illustrates thar concept maps behave as per the predictions of theory
knowledge. Ascertaining that the two knowledge forms exhibit comparable quality indicates
that concept maps exhibit properties that correspond to the predictions of theory integration.
Concept rnap theory has it that maps represent mental models of problems. Consequently the
finding that concept rnaps behave as per theoretical prediction is taken to be supportive of the
notion that mental models are represented in maps.
Table 22
1 Relational-4 1 4c
1 Extended Relational-2 1 6a
1 Extended Relational-3 1 6b
Novak and Gowin ( 1987) recognized the roles that progressive differentiation and
integrative reconciliation play in the learning process. They also recognized that these
processes influence the nature and form of concept maps. Progressive differentiation affects
the number of links. Inteyative reconciliation determines link quality. Novak and Gowin's
scoring technique provides a total score for each map. Their score bundles the effects of
Novak and Gowin technique. They mess individual concepts rather than entire maps. They
Table 23 provides the information that is needed to compare the key concepts (Oïl
141
SpilLs, Tat1kers and ('Itratz I l p ) of subject 1 to the key concepts of ot her subjects. Table 24
and Table 25 provide similar information for the Oil SpiI1.s concept of subject 2 and subject 6
different maps by Iink enurneration. Oii SpilLs was presented as a 4-Link concept by subjea
1. a 5-link concept by subject 2 and a 2-iink concept by subject 6. Subject 7 created the rnoa
links and can be said to exhibit supenor integrative teconciliation for the Oil Spiils concept
Table 23
1 Suborûinntc Concepts
I SOLO Rating of Luilr
Oil SpilLs
Table 24
1 Ckm IJp
Table 25
complicated flair. The complications derive from the weaknesses of Method 1. Section 7.7
covers these weaknesses in full. It notes that maps can be compared through their integative
reconciliation when a concept is associated with the same superordinates and subordinates
across different maps. For example, suppose a concept has three unistruchird links in one
map and three relational-2 Iinks in another. If the concepts involved are identical it cm it be
said that the integative reconciliation and the leaming of the latter map is supenor to that of
the former.
When super- and subordinates differ across maps so does the progressive
linked to the same number of subordinates in two maps. but the concepts involved are
different. For example. Tunkers might be linked to ('ompatiirs and Oil SpIlls in a hypothetical
('omparrie.~llnk can still be compared through Method 3. The rnap with the highest Method 3
rating can be said to exhibit superior learning for that link. In the Tutrkers-Oil SpilI-sand the
qualitative differences exia between the proyressive differentiation of the two links. Method
identical across the maps and rankings are provided throu& Method 3 ratings. Suppose both
link of Map B is
links of Map A are unistructurai. Suppose also that the Tn,~ker~-C*umpcit~ks
Cornparison may dso be possible when a concept contains more links in one rnap than
another. For exarnple, suppose a concept has three uniamaurd links in one map and two
unistructural and two relational-2 links in another. The combination of more and bener
quality links in the second rnap means that it exhibits supenor progressive differentiation and
superior integrative reconciliation. The second rnap is aiso said to exhibit superior Iearning as
144
leaming manifests in concept maps as the combined effects of progressive differentiation and
integrative reconciliation.
Methods 1 and 2 are of little use when the number and qudity of links are in
opposition across different maps. This limited utility is related to the fact that the methods
cannot compare the cognitive wonh of one link type against another. For example. suppose a
concept is linked to five unistructural links in one rnap and to three relational-3 links in
another. Methods I and 2 cannot detemine which map is superior as they cannot express the
cognitive value of unistructural links in tenns of relational links. That is they cannot
The following paragraphs compare the prog-essive differentiation and the integrative
subject. Subject 6 links Oz/ Spifis to (la.se.s and ('ieat~I l p through two unistnictural links
Suppose the hypothetical subject links Oil Spilfs to C1asc.sand CIea~tI l p through a
unistnictural link and a relational-2 Iink. These maps differ only in their link quality and it can
be said that the hypothetical subject exhibits superior interyative reconciliation and supenor
learning.
Subjects 1. 2 and 6 presented Oil Spiiis as a superordinate concept. They al1 linked it
to a different number of subordinates. Only one subordinate, C11ra7r Up. was cornmon to al1
three maps
Subject 2 produced 3 markable links. The general knowledye link (link 3 ) could not be
marked as Method 1 is insensitive to the inferences of this link type. Link 9 was unmarkable
as it contains redundant items of data (see Chapter nine). Subject 6 produced two markable
links. Two of subject 2's links were of higher quality than the links produced by subject 6 .
The combination of more and better quality links leads to the conclusion that subject 2
Subject 1 linked Oil Spilh to four concepts. Only one of those links was markable. Link 1
could not be marked as Method 1 is insensitive to the inferences of general knowledge links.
Links 22 and 27 were not marked as they refer to inferences in other links. These references
failed to reveal the manner of data integration (see Chapter 9). The links of subject 2 exceed
the links of subject 1 in nurnber and quality. It can be said that subject 2 exhibited supenor
integrative reconciliation and superior progressive diflerentiation. The extent to which the
Differences in the nurnber and quality of links are in opposition across the two maps.
.4lthough subject 6 produced more markable links. they were of lower quality than the
extended relationai-2 link of subject 1. It is not obvious which of the two maps is supenor. A
scale that considers the cognitive wonh of the different link types is needed for this
The second goai of this thesis was to develop methods for concept map anaiysis.
Methods 1 and 2 meet the needs of this goal. Method 2 can readily assess the progressive
differentiation associated with a concept across different maps. If the same superordinates
and subordinates are associated with a concept across different maps then Method 1 can be
Table 26
6a 39% 5% 33%
5 39% II%
4c 5%
I
4b -97% 36%
4a 13Y0 18% 3 1% 11%
L.
3 36% 19%
1 9% 5% 10%
Table 27
Table 26 reveds the percentages of different link types that were found in the maps of
148
the six subjects. It summarizes their cognitive activity. Table 27 collapses the Method 3
rating of Table 26 into the inference levels of link-procedural knowledye. For example. the
stage 5 and stage 6a links of subject 1 have been collapsed into level4. A high percentage of
level four inferences is exhibited by subjects 1. 4 and 5. The predominance of high level
inferences lead to a high level cognitive activity rating for these subjects. Level 3 inferences
predominate in the maps of subjects 2 and 3. They were said to exhibit medium level
cognitive activity. AI1 the links of subject 6 were of level3 quality. Unlike subjects 7 and 3.
subject 6 did not produce level4 inferences. The absence of level4 inferences in the map of
In chapter 4 it was reasoned that the higher the cogitive activity of map creation. the
higher the level of the accompanyiny metacognitive activity. The validity of this reasoning is
examined in Table 28. Table 28 presents cognitive activity and metacognitive interview
scores for the six subjects (see appendix E). The mean interview scores of the low. medium
and high cognitive activity groups were 15.0, 18.5 and 37.0 respectively. The general trend is
that the higher the cognitive activity level, the higher the level of the metacognitive strategy.
Furiher analyses with many more subjects are needed to confinn this trend but it is
encouraging. It corroborates suppositions taken from the integation of the theories of Kirby
( 1991) and Jacobs and Paris ( 1987). That is it indicates that the cogitive and metacopnitive
processes of concept mapping have comparable quality. Agreement between the suppositions
of theory integration and the results of methods 1 and 3 is taken to mean that concept maps
have a psychological vdidity that is comparable to that of the theories of Kirby ( 199 1 ) and
1 High 30 22
4 High 33 22
5 High 23 22
7
II Medium 18 18.5
3 Medium 19 18.5
6 Low 15 15
multistructural (stage 3) and eaended abstract (stage 7 and stage 8) links. Severai lines of
reasoning are submitted to explain this finding. First. it is possible that the sample size of the
study was too small to ensure the production of al1 link types. If more maps were produced
the potential for producing more link Npes would increase as weIi. Second, it might be that
the mapping exercise was not conducive to the production of extended abstract links. More
thou@t may be needed to provide mapping exercises that encourage creative thinking. Third
it is possible that humans are loathe to express the multistructural stage of comprehension.
Multistructurai links would be created by someone who has mastered the successive
processing but not the sirnultaneous processing stage of comprehension. That is. sorneone
creating a multistmcturd link can hold concepts in working memory as discrete units but is
10 of the metacopitive interviews. students suggested that they would omit or gloss over
Conclusions
Conclusions drawn fkom the data analysis are limited by the fact that only six subjects
were involved in the analysis. The low level interrater reliability gamered for Methods 1 and
significant sample of the student population may yield results that differ considerably from
those presented here. At best interpretations derived in this study may point the way for
future study into the cognitive alsorithms that senerate concept maps. At worst the
interpretations may simply be misieadiny and erroneous. it is with these caveats in mind that
Methods 1 and 3 were developed under the assurnption that items of data would
reveal the simultaneous processing that led to the creation of a linking statement. Most links
fàiled to meet this expectation. They listed data but failed to explain its interconnections and
its association to the linkins statement. These connections contain the nature of link-
procedural knowledge. Inadequately aructured links were rated by assurning that the mapper
comected data in the rnanner described in the target text. A few links, such as links 13 and 20
of subjea 2. met the structural expectations of Methods 1 and 3 . Their data were
interco~ectedand they explicated the thinking that lead to link creation. It was suggested
that a more extensive training program would increase the incidence of appropnately
structured links.
and tuning. Structuring links connect concepts d e r the logic presented in the target text.
Accretion links connect concepts rnentioned in the target text to related concepts that the text
fails to mention. That is, they associate extant knowledge with the taryet text. The
production of accretion links does not involve creative thinking. Creative thinking is
associated with the tuning mode. Tuning links improve upon the text by adding extant
knowledye to its descriptions of relationships. On occasion a link will connect two concepts
that the text fails to mention. if it contains creative thinking, it is a tuning link. lf creative
thinking is absent it is an accretion link. The different leaming modes generate links of
different quality. The tuning mode generates links of the highest quality. Medium quality links
are produced by the accretion mode and the lowest quality links are ~eneratedby the
structunng mode.
In reyard to Method 1. the tuning mode is associated with extended abstract links.
Extended relationai. extended multistructurai and extended unistructural links are produced
by leamers operating in the accretion mode. The structunng mode produces relational.
multistructural and unistmctural links. In regard to Method 3, the tuning mode generates
links of stage 7 and stage 8. Stage 5 and stage 6 links are senerated by the accretion mode.
Stages 2,3 and 4 are associated with the stmcturing mode. Al1 three leaming modes may
generate inaccurate links. Links are considered inaccurate if their linking statement is
incorrect or Zone or more items of data are inappropnate. Regardless of the leaming mode,
Predictions derived by integrating theories of cognition and concept map theory were
were found to have comparable quality in individual links. This finding supports the reasoning
that concept maps represent mental models. More specifically it supports the reasoning that
concept maps exhibit properties that compare to those of mental models as predicted by the
integration of theories of cogition. Predictions about the properties of mental models were
further supported by the analysis of data from Method 3 and the interview. Students' scores
in the two techniques were positively correlated. This finding supports the aryurnent that the
cognitive and metacognitive processes that govem reading comprehension are also active
durin$ concept map creation. It also implies that concept maps emulate the properties of
Method 1 and Method 3 exhibit a positive correlation because they are sensitive to
the same factors. They rate link quality against three aspects of link data: extent of synthesis
in the linkiny statement. novelty of the thinking and the number of data items. Aithouçh both
methods assess knowledge. they do so from different perspectives. Thus they are not
There are a number of problems associated with methods 1 and 3 . The most
important of these was introduced at the begming of this chapter. When audents fail to
provide proper descriptions of their link-procedural knowledge, the rater is left to reason as
to how a link was formed. In this situation it is impossible to gauge how closely the rater's
deductions match the thinking of the mapper. The problem is best resolved by providing
training that reduces the incidence of inadequate link descriptions by audents. Training could
also be appiied to reduce the incidence of other linking problems such as overly fiagrnented
data and references to the inferences of ot her links.
General knowledge links were also a problem for methods 1 and 3. In this thesis the
author identified these links and excluded them from the rating process. The identification
process was subjective. The subjectivity of this identification process will likely hinder the
exercises so that students simply reproduce the text. Althouyh this approach will eliminate
general knowledge links, it is also likely that it will stifle creativity. A better tactic rnay be to
accept general knowledge links as a necessary (if inane) component of concept maps. They
In this thesis. links were rated at the prestnictural level if they contained just one
incorrect item of data. When a student's thinking is incorrect. the teacher is required to
provide feedback that informs students why they are incorrect. The identification of
preamcturd links can assist in the feedback process as it pinpoints the source of students'
rnisconceptions.
Method 2 enurnerates links. It was used aiongside Method I to assess the progressive
concept is associated with the sarne superordinates and subordinates in different maps, it
exhibits the same progressive differentiation. In this situation, ody Method 1 ratings need be
considered when cornparhg understanding across maps. Maps exhibitiny higher Method 1
subordinates dBer across maps. In this situation, both progressive differentiation and
155
integrative reconciliation have to be considered. If one map contains more and better quality
links than another it can be said that it exhibits superior progressive differentiation. superior
integrative reconciliation and superior understandiny. lf link number and link quality are in
opposition across maps, simple comparison is no longer vaiid. In cases such as this. the
cognitive worth of one link type has to be compared to the copitive wonh of another. For
example, suppose maps A and 6 (not show) contain five unistructural links and two
relational-2 links respectively. It can be said that the progressive differentiation of map A is
reconciliation requires a system that compares different link types in terms of their cognitive
relational-2 links.
pedagogy and student learning. Implications for pedagogy are considered first. Five of the six
inte~eweesrealized that concepts become more specific fiom the top to the bottom of the
map. Most inte~eweesthought that the concepts at the top of the map were the most
important and they said that they would select these concepts to summarize a topic.
interestingly, two students suggested that concepts in the rnidde of the map are the most
important. Two more subjects said that the rniddle concepts should be used to summarize a
topic. It is felt that these students answered the questions incorrectly because they interpreted
156
thern differently than their peers. They recognized that the Iinks from middle concepts to
recognized the importance of links From middle to low level concepts. These links detennine
the quantity of understanding as they are superior in number to the links to superordinates.
They are also the links that explicate the specifics of a topic. Thus the students ernphasized
the importance of middle concepts as they are the seat of understanding Th- affect the
should be given opportunities to discuss their understanding with others. The importance of
to compare them to the understanding of others. Question six of the interview revealed that
students failed to make plans before aarting their maps. Discussion could be used as an
alternative to planning.
concept maps. The first map would be created in a free-form fashion that contains the
attempt at summarizing. The fim map might serve as a set of plans that a student cm use to
guide creation of the second. Reflecting on the first map provides an opportunity for snidents
double mapping approach might be to get audents to read a text twice and take notes during
157
the second reading. The mapping approach is preferable as it forces students to connect
Two implications can be denved fiom the interviews to benefit student learning. The
first concems the issue of audents askiny for clarification when they corne upon a link that
they do not understand. Students in this study said that they would gloss over or omit the
links that they did not understand Fully. The matter is important as the interviewees are
confident. accomplished students. Less able students, it is felt. would have less confidence
and feel even less inclined to ask the teacher for clarification. It is likely that this matter will
best be resolved in classrooms where the teacher takes time to develop an environment that
encourases enquiry.
The second implication for student leaming is that the production of two maps can
increase cognitive flexibility. Dunng production of the first map students should be
encouraged to look for dl anyles and potential links. They should also be provided with an
opportunity to discuss this initial map. By comecting the concepts of maps across many
angles?students link knowledge across multiple perspectives. Students who can apply
Ruiz-Primo and Shavelson ( 1996) make note of the wide variety of concept rnapping
techniques. They express concem that despite dserences in task expectations and assessment
the most appropriate technique they c d for research that makes a comection between "how
158
Primo and Shavelson 1996, p. 585). They feel that such a connection will allow for
organization.
knowledge organization. Method 3 explores the thinking that links are yenerated by the
assumes that its quality is a refiection of the link-procedural knowledye that generated it. The
latter assurnption was denved from the integration of the theories of cognition. It found
support in the determination that ratinys For Method 1 and Method 3 were comparable across
the concept maps of the six subjects. Thus. Methods 1 and 3 do not simply provide evidence
also support the aructural assumptions of the theones of cognition. The preliminary findings
of this study require funher exploration in hture studies. These studies would do well to
involve a significant number of participants. They would aiso benefit from training programs
designed to generate links whose data elicits the actions of link-procedural knowledge. These
training programs would concentrate on ways in which data can be worded so that the
connections between items of dara and the linking statement are rendered eqlicit.
The problem of inadequately structured links is a serious one. Liu ( 1994) touches
upon the subject noting that link correctness is a variable that depends upon assumptions
made by the rater. The assumptions Liu was refemng to are the assumptions made while
marking maps with techniques that enumerate links. Liu ( i 994) notes the popularity of
enurneration techniques and States that the following workers have used them to score maps:
Cleare ( 1983), Novak and Gowin ( 1984). Schreiber and Abegg ( 1991 ), Vargas and Alvarez
( 1992). and Wallace and Mintzes ( 1990). In enumerating a link, raters assume that the stages
of its production were completed correctly. This is a considerable assumptive leap for correct
linking statements can still be produced when one or more items are incorrect. By addins
data to concept map production. methods 1 and 3 eliminate the assumptive leap of
enurnerative methods. When data are available, the accuracy of data and their connections are
readily observed.
Future research should also seek to improve Methods 1 and 3. Both methods are
limited by the fact that they use ordinal assessrnent scales. Links of the same type are
indistinguishable. Differences in quality between link types are thought to be uniform across
the range of links that the methods recognize. For example. the difference in quality From the
unistructural to the multistnictural tevel is assumed to be the same as the difference between
the relational and extended abaract levels. In actuaiity. links with the sarne Method 1 or
Method 3 ratinys differ according to the concepts involved. Improvements to Methods 1 and
This work has made some prorgess toward clarifjmg the terminology of cognitive
knowledge and declarative knowledge. Integration also lead to the suggestion that
knowledge has two moddities, structural and procedural. These modalities were said to CO-
evolve and be comparable in quality. It is thought that the ciaim for bimodai knowledge and
the constmct of link-procedural knowledge are unique to this thesis. Although the
interpretations of the six students' maps lend weight to the idea of bimodal knowledge that
The impetus for this theory was the intellectual challenge of dernonstratins that
concept maps represent mental models. Althoueh this work provides indirect evidence that
concept maps represent mental models, it is unlikely to impact on the use of concept maps in
schools. The restrictions on greater use are two-fold. First. the three methods and the
interview reguire extensive time commitrnents on the pan of the rater. Second, considerable
skill is required to rate links accurately. An extensive training period would be needed to
impan the requisite skills to teachers. Similarly, extensive training would be needed to
The time commitments associated with methods 1.7 and 3 are unlikely to be
overcome. A workable alternative requires a method that allows for the rapid generation of
links and has a rapid marking synem. Novak and Gowin's ( 1984)method fits the bill. Liu
(1 994) compared scores generated by Novak and Gowin's technique and item response
theory and found them to be comparable-The result is encouraging as Liu ( 1994) proposed
that item response theory provides valid assessments of student ability. Future study that
compares Novak and Gowin scores to a method that combines Method 1 and Method 2
correlation could be seen as fùrther evidence for the contention that concept maps reflect
mental models.
16 I
Bibliognphy
Company.
11),37-52.
Incorporated.
Chi. M. T. H. & Glaser. R. ( 1985). Problem solving ability In Sternberg, R.J. (Ed.).
Revirw.
P~ychc~lo~c*ai a ( 6 ) . 407-128.
Collins. A. M.& Quillian ( 1972). How to make a l a n g a ~ user.
e In E. Tulving, & W.
Edmondson. K. M. (1 993). Concepts for the development of medical cunicula. (Repon No.
TM 020 1 19) Atlanta. GA: Paper Presented at the Annual Meeting of the Arnencan
360 322).
162
York, NY: Paper Presented at the Annual Meeting of the Amencan Educational
Fisher. K. M. ( 1996). Supporting knowledge construction and reflection. Ilwk 9 (2L 1-34.
Base.
. J O I I ~ I Ir.Qf ~Eclt~criiiu~zai
Psychuiogy. 83 ( 1 ). 88-96.
Hill.
178.
trpreseriir~igam./ acpiri~îgsiruc~~irui
k~owirJRe.NJ: Ehrlbaum.
Research
163
kr~owicdgt!ac~piisirior~
(report N0.R 016 737). Nashville. ïN: National Convention
Liu. X.( 1994). 7ne validity ami reliabilip rfcomepi mappi~gas un ulter,wrivr scietrcr
New Orleans, LA: Paper Presented at the Annual Meeting of the Amencan
370 992).
Press.
Myers. M. II & Pans. S. G.( 1 978). Children's metacogitive knowledge about reading.
Journal: of Ecluc*a~iot~ai
Psychoiogy. 70 (5). 680-690.
University Press.
Paris. S. G., Cross D.H.& Lipson, M. Y. (1984). informed strategies for leaniing: A
164
Wi~catio~ial
P.~yc~hoIogy.
76 (61- 123 9-12 5 2.
Paris, S. G. & lacobs. J. E. (in press). The benefits of infomed instruction for children's
N,fumafiot~pr~~cc!.s.~i~ig
Cambridge MA: M . 1. T. Press).
Ruiz-Primo. M. A. 7 Shavelson. R. J. (1996). Problems and issues in the use of concept maps
Rumelhart. D. E. & Norman, D. ( 1978). Accretion, tuning and restnictunng: Three modes of
Press.
Shavekon, R. I. (1972). Some aspects of the correspondence between content strucniml and
Spiro, R. J.. Feltovich. P. J., Jacobson, M. J., Br Coulson. R. L. ( 199 1 a). Random access
3 1 (51, 24-3 3
Tec~ht~oiugy
Spiro, R. J., Feltovich. P. J.. Jacobson. M. J.. & Coulson, R. L. ( 1991b). Knowledye
a,rJphysicv yer_/i)rmc~~~ce.
Paper presented at Amencan Educational Research
Erlbaum.
323 957)
Appendix A
Oil Spills
-
Everyone knows about oil slicks the huge media attention surrounding accidental
spills wch as the Exxon Valdez and Braer, and the huge spills caused by Saddarn Hussein in
the Gulf War has made "oil slick" a househoid word. Although measures have been taken to
prevent spillage from oil tankers, there will still be accidents as long as the world uses oil, and
there is always the possibility of oil being spilled in war or by terronst activity. Oil also sets
into the sea fiom rnany other sources. There are already techniques for clearing up oil slicks,
these al1 have problems. There is no simple solution to the problem of oil slicks.
Exxon Valdez
On the 24th of March 1989' the Exxon Valdez oil tanker ran aground in the Prince
William Sound off Alaska spilling 232,000 barrels of oil. The effects of the incident are not
really known, but it is known that only 25% of the migratory salmon population retumed to
the area the following season, thousands of otters were poisoned, and thousands of birds
died. The scaie of the incident prompted the development of the Valdez Pnnciples, which are
slowly being adopted by industry. It also highlighted jus how expensive a major oil spill can
be - Exxon spent some 3 billion dollars cleaning up the spill, and a fùrther 1 billion to settle
On the 5th of January 1993, the oil tanker Braer ran aground off Sumburgh Head in
Shetland. At 5.30 in the moming, the tankers engines failed, leaving the ship drifiing in
stormy seas. Emergency rescue seMces were called out, and by 9.00 the last of the 34 crew
had been airlifked o f t h e ship. At 10.55, two tugs began trying to pull the tanker away from
the rocky coastline, but twenty minutes later the Braer with it's 6 19,300 barrels of oil (about
The Shetland Islands Council already had plans to deal with such an evenr, but
because of the harsh weather none of the plans could be carried out. Six aircraft were sent
out to spray chernical dispersants on the slick, but were dnven back by the high winds. The
action plan also involved using booms and skimmers, but the waves prevented their use.
However. the very weather which prevented the clean-up operation actually helped it - the oil
the Braer was canying was a Light crude oil which is easily dispersed by large waves, and
One of the greatest ecological disasters in history happened when Saddam Hussein
used oil as a weapon in the Gulf War. The environmental attack came in two parts: the
release of oil into the Gulf, and the burning of Kuwait's oil wells.
The Water
This prong of the attack was the release of a huge arnount ofoil into the Gulf-
168
estimates range from 0.5 million to 1 1 million barrels, and average at about 6 million barrels.
Compare this with the tremendous consequences of the Exxon Valdez oil spill, which was
"only" 230,000 barrels. The oil formed a slick 30 miles long and 8 miles wide. which is about
the same area as the Isle of Wight. The slick is thought to have killed between 1 5,000 and
30,000 birds.
The problem was not just the vast amount of oil in the Gulf, but also the nature of the
Gulf itself This inland sea is at most only 35x11 deep. and only has a narrow comection to the
Indian Ocean through the Straits of Honuz, which means that it takes between three and
for al1 the water in the Gulf to be changed, so there was no hope of the oil simply dispersing,
The Air
Although the buming of Kuwait's oil wells may have been intended as an economic
weapon, the environmental consequences were no less severe than those of the oil slick. It is
estimated that about 67 million tonnes of oil were bmed in total, which produced about 2.1
million tonnes
of soot and 2 million tonnes of sulphur, one of the main causes of acid min. Fortunately, the
soot particles did not go any higher than 5,000 metres, which meam that they were washed
down with the min over the next two weeks. If the soot had got above the rainclouds, it
would have remained up there, with severe consequences to the world's cbate. As it was,
the consequences for Kuwait were still harsh. The soot cloud made the daytime almost as
169
dark as the night, and the World Health Organisation estimated that death rates in Kuwait
rose by 10% over the following year because of associated breathing difficulties and skin
problerns.
There are two main methods used to prevent oil spilling fiom tankers. The first is to
stop them having accidents, and the second is to stop oil leaking if they do have an accident.
Avoiding Accidents
Maritime authorities recommend keeping at leaa 10 miles away from any Coast
(unless they are docking of course!). Tankers also plot their routes according to the weather
forecast. but the weather cm only be predicîed with any accuracy for the next three days,
which is of limited use to a tanker which can take a whole day to stop. Oil tankers and other
sea vessels avoid collisions by filing route plans with maritime authorities who keep the
routes well separated in the same way that air t r a c cornrollers keep aeroplanes well apart in
the sky.
Preventing SpiiIs
The Oil Pollution Act was passed in the USA in 1990 in response to the E n o n
Valdez oil spill, and means that al1 new oil takers must be built with a double hull so that if
they do hit anything the outer hull cm be pierced without causing a leak. Another meanire
used to prevent oil spilling ifthe oil tanks are pierced is to fit the tanker with hydrostatic
1 70
controls to make the pressure inside the tank the same as outside, so no oil will come out of
the tank. However, both these measures are very expensive to implement, and as they are
The oceans are impacted by sources of oil other than oil slicks. These sources are:
Atmosphere 9%
Natural Sources 7%
HI
IUI
-.-
.--
---
.--
---
---
--
Naturd Dispersion
Collection
Chemical dispersion
Naturai Dispersion
Oil slicks cm simply be lefi to disperse and break d o m nahidly, which can be
17 1
helped by certain weather conditions, such as in the case of the Braer. Most of the oil is
dispersed this way even when other clean-up methods are used. It is estimated that in the
Exxon Valdez spill, no more than 8 percent of the oil spilled was recovered, despite the huge
sums spent on the clean-up the rest was dispersed by the fierce Arctic storms of the region.
Collection
The best way to clean up an oil slick is to actudly collect the oil. This means that not
only is the oil completely removed from the environment (unlike chernical and natural
The first stage in collecting the spilled oil is to contain it. This is usually done using
booms, which corne in two types. The first boom is inflatable, with a chain almg the bottom
to keep it the right way up. The second boom uses solid floats, like those you used when you
leamed to swim.Inflatable booms are easier to store, as they take up much less space, but are
not as resistant to damage. Where such specialist equipment is unavailable, booms can be
improvised fiom locaily available materials, such as fences and bafes of hay:
Once the oil has been contained by a boom it cm be collected using skimmers or
sorbants. Skimers can be broken down into two types: suction and adhesion. Suction
skimmers operate Sie giant hoovers, sucking the oi1 off the water. However, these suck up
lots of water as well - oflen more than 90 percent of the liquid collected is water. Adhesion
through the spiIl. The oil sticks to the matenal. and is carrieci away from the spiu. The oil is
Sorbents are materiais which absorb oil. These can be either natural, such as straw, or
synthetic, such as polypropylene. They can be used in a variety of shapes, such as sheets and
mops. They are not really suitable for collecting large volumes of oil, so tend to be used in
the final stages of cleaning when only small pockets of oil are left.
There are ships which integrate these methods to make a complete oil collection
systern, such as the Al Waasit used to clean up afier the Gulf War. This ship lays inflatable
booms around a section of the slick, and then sucks the oil off the water using vacuum
pumps. The mixture of oil and water sucked in is then separated, and the water is then
pumped back out into the sea, while the oii is kept in storage tanks on the ship, from where it
cm be off-loaded to a tanker or a refinery. The Al Waasit can clean up to half a square mile
per day.
Chernical Dispersion
One of the main problems with oil slicks is the way they stick together and float on
the surface of the water, so chemicais have been developed which cm be sprayed on the oil
Once it has been broken up, the slick dilutes into the water without causing any major
problerns. However, because of the way these dispersants work they cannot be used on
viscous oils, which includes some crude oils and dl heavy fuel oils. Also, most crude oils
which can be dispersed change their characteristics within a few hours in cold water, and cm
no longer be dispersed. This means that any dispersant response mua be fast, so
Scientists working in Amenca have developed a new breed of oil dispersants which
are actually living organisms which feed on oil. These have b e n very successtlll in laboratory
tests, but they have not been tested on real spills because of concems about releasing
Although modem dispersants are much less toxic than earlier types, they are still a
pollutant in themselves, and before they are used it must be decided whether they may cause
more environmental damage than the spill would if it was left untreated. Whether dispersants
should be üsed or not depends on the depth of the water, how strong the currents are, and
what lives in the water there. For example, spills in the middle of the Atlantic Ocean could be
sprayed with chernical dispersants without much worry, but a spill in a salmon f m off
Letters of Consent
Principal
School
Hamilton, Ontario
Dear Principal,
Following Our first conversation 1 would like to take this opponunity to funher
infonn you of the research t am conducting involving science education. 1 am most grateful
and appreciative of your cooperation and enthusiasm in this research endeavour. 1 am a
doctoral student in Curriculum, teaching and leaniing at the lnstitute for Studies in
Education. My research concerns concept maps and their ability to elucidate the processes of
comprehension. The objectives of the study are: to provide evidence that concept maps
reflect the structuring of concepts in memory, to test the utility of three methods of concept
map analysis, and to elucidate the cognitive and metacognitive strategies of comprehension.
A mernber of your staff has agreed O participate in this audy and will be invited to
offer opinions and advice that might further improve the three analytical methods. Data for
the audy will be collected in two ways. Over a period of one week concept maps will be
collected both before and after a text is read. At the end of the week a number of tape
recorded interviews will be collected. If further cornmentaries are needed to clahfi maps or
interviews they will be negotiated on a case-by-case basis with the teacher and mdents.
1 look fonvard to visiting your school and working with a member of your faculty. If
you have any questions please do not hesitate to contact me at (905) 383 0735. Thank you
for your support, and for your interest in concept mapping.
Thank you
Sincerely,
Graham Passrnore
Department of Cumculum Teaching and Learning
OISE
Principal Consent Form
Signature
Date
Dear Participating Teacher,
I would like to take this opportunity to formally invite you to participate with me in a
research project with the Curriculum, teaching and Leanting Department at the Ontario
Institute for Studies in Education from March 9 to March 13, 1998. This research project will
be submitted as partial fulfillment of the requirements of the degree of Doctor of Philosophy
at the University of Toronto. Your agreeing to join in this work will allow us to explore the
cornprehension processes that students engage in as they lem.
The Research
The project requires that students create two concept maps. One pre-instructional
map will be created before reading a target text. One post-instnictional map will be created
after reading the text. The pre- and post- maps will be assessed by three interpretation
methods that have been developed for this project. Six of the nidents will be selected for a
short i n t e ~ e won the basis of the cognitive change they demonstrate. Your participation is
invited so that the content of the target text may be better matched to the interests and
abilities of the students. You are also invited to participate in the interpretation of the maps
created by the six students selected for interview. You experience, feelings and ideas are
needed to furiher improve the utility of the interpretation methods.
Et hical Considerations
A number of neps shall be taken to ensure the anonymity and confidentiality of the
research. Al1 proper names and identifjmg details wiil be changed on the maps and on the
i n t e ~ e wtapes. The maps and tapes will remain confidential (with the exception of my thesis
cornmittee) and will be locked away once the project is completed. if further commentaries by
students are needed to elaborate on the maps or interviews they will be discussed on a case-
by-case basis with the teacher and midents involved.
i will share my findings and conclusions with you and provide interpretive accounts
prepared fom the data as the audy progresses. You are invited to read and respond to these
accounts and suggest corrections and differences of opinion. These responses will be use do
Uiform further drafts for the purpose of the thesis. A final surnmary of the study will be made
available to you.
You are f kto delete any data that you feel compromises your position in any way.
Fially 1 acknowledge that you have the right to withdraw fiom the study at any time without
reason.
If you are willing to participate in this research project, please complete the attached
form. Thank you for your interest and cooperation.
Sincerely,
Graham Passmore
Teacher Consent Fonn
1, .
agree to participate in the study "ConceptMaps and the
Processes of Comprehension"as outlined in the attached letter.
Date
Student Consent Form
You are invited to take part in a study conducted with the Curriculum, teaching and Leaniing
department at the Ontario Institute for Studies in Education. The study will run from March 9
to March 13, 1998. By agreeing to join in this work you will allow us to explore how
students lem.
The research
the project requires that students m a t e maps of their knowledge. One map will be
created before a short text is read and one will be created afler the text is rad. Both maps
will be assessed to identiQ thinking and leaminy processes. Some students will be selected
for a shon interview on the basis of the leanllng that they demonstrate.
Ethical Considerations
A number of steps shall be taken to ensure the anonymity and confidentiality of the
research. Ail proper narnes and identifjing details will be removed from the maps and
interviews. The map and interview data will remain confidentid and will be locked away once
the project is completed.
If funher commentaries by students are needed to elaborate on the pas or intewiews
they will be discussed on a case-by-case basis with the tacher, guardians or students
involved. Findinys and conclusions will be made availabie to the student on request.
Finally 1 acknowledge that you have the right to withdraw from the audy at any time
without reason.
If you are willing to participate in this research project, please complete the anached
form. Thank you for your interest and cooperation.
Sincerely,
Graham Passmore
Student Consent Form
1, . agree
to participate in the
Processes of Comprehension"as outlined in the attached
study "Concept Maps and the
letter.
Signature
Date
Appendix C
Rate each link ayainst the scaies of methods 1 and 3. Once a link has been identified record
its score in the appropriate column of Table 1. In regard to Method 2, enumerate the links
associated with the key concepts and place the Number-Link score in the Method 2 column
of Table 1.
Links are created by inferences. Four levels of inference are recoynized by Methods 1 and 3.
a) incorrect inferences
C) inferences that add existing knowledge to information in the text. Example. tea describes
only links from concept A to concepts B and C. An inference that adds to the text would be a
d) inferences that take existing knowledge and incorporate it into the text. Example, text
says that concepts A and B are linked via points X and Y. An inference that incorporates
information into the text would include point Z as well as X and Y in the link from A to B.
Method 1-
1. If the linking aatement or one or more items of data are incorrect, mark the link as
prestructurd
2. If the linking statement was formed from one item of data it is unistructural
3. If the linking statement was formed fiom two or more items of data it is either
multistructural or reIational.
Example: Suppose the rnapper has to respond to the question "why is Iraq's political
situation bad?" by linking the concepts "Iraq" and "Political Situation." A multistructural link
would appear as
Linking statement: It is bad because of the oil and it is also bad because of Saddarn"
Each item of data is listed and conjoined in the linking statement through phrases such as
The number of data items shodd be added to the nomenclature. Thus links with two data
items would be multistructural-2, Links with three data items are multistnictural-3.
Whatever the number of data items, add it to the link rating as a suffix. The example link
descnbed above is multistnictural-2. The number of data items should also be included when
4 Relational links differ from multistructural links in that their linking statements are fonned
by integrating data. Even if the integration process is not made explicit in the link, the rater
should be able to rationalize as to how data integation took place. Relational links compare
to multistnictural links in that al1 their data was denved fiom the text.
An example relational link (responds to the Iraq political question mentioned in point 3a).
Linking Statement: Iraq's troubles stem from Saddam's pan-Arabian ideolog which threatens
NB. Here the data is integrated into a single phrase. The result is a relational-3 link
1 . Extended Abstract links. Like relational links these links htegrate data in the linking
183
statement. Unlike relational links they include at least one item of data that was obtained corn
Linking Staternent: Iraq's troubles stem frorn 1923 when the Middle East was divided to suit
the political aspirations of Colonial powers. Recognizing that extemally imposed land division
attenuates Arabian power, Saddam Hussein sought to unite the Middle East. A united Middle
East could undoubtedly change the balance of global politics by controlling the movement
and price of oil. It is the ylobal threat imposed by Saddam's ideoiogy that makes Iraq's
Suppose the multistructural and relational links were created fiom information provided in
current news programs. Current news programs have neglected to report the impact of
Colonial strategies on current events. Thus it can be said that this link associates the concepts
with a datum that was taken tiom outside the instniction (current news programs). The use
1. Extended Multistmcturai links. Same as multistructural links except that they link a
concept mentioned in the text to one that was not mentioned in the text. OR they concem a
Iink that has taken severai points mentioned in the text and used it to form a linking statement
2. Extended Relational links. Same as relational links except that only one of their concepts
Method 2
Purpose: to identify the number of links associated with the key concepts.
1. Enurnerate the links associated with the three key concepts Oif Spilis, Tankers and C'lem
Ilp. Inciude links to superordinate and subordinate concepts. If there are two links the
concept is a 2-Link concept. If there are three links the concept is a 3-Link concept etcetera.
Method 3-
presented in the text. The result is a link that connects concepts through inappropnate items
of data.
Stage 2. Concepts are linked through a listing of one or more items of data. No inferences
Stage 2a. One item of data is listed to link the concepts. No inferences are made.
Stage 2b. Two items of data are listed to link the concepts. No inferences are made.
Stage 3. One item of data links the concepts through an inference based on facts (the
Stage 4. Two or more items of data are linked through inferences based on facts. The
Stage 4a. Two items of data are linked through an inference based on facts.
Stage 4b. Three items of data are linked through two inférences based on facts.
Stage 5. One item of data links the concepts though an inference based on faas. The
Stage 6. Two or more items of data are linked through inferences based on facts. The
Stage 6a. Two items of data are linked through an inference that was not presented in
presented.
Stage 6b. Three items of data are linked through an inferences. The inference is not
Stage6c. Four . . .
Stage 7. An item of data connects the concepts through an inference based on fact. The
inference does not build on information presented in the text. It is an example of creative
t hinking.
Stage 8. Two or more items of data are co~ectedthrough inferences based on facts. Rather
than building on information presented in the text the inference(s) represent creative thinking.
Stage 8a. Two items of data are Iinked through an inference that was not
presented in the text. Rather than building on information presented in the text
The following instructions were provided to the students before they created the practice
map.
1. Arrange the KEY concepts on the bristol board as you see fit.
2. Write out any additional concepts on post-it notes and add them to the map.
3. Make as many links as possible by drawing arrowed lines between the concepts. Number
4. Remember ALL L M S must have a linking statement--do this as you draw the lines-
NOT LATER
5. When dl the links are described go back and add the pieces of information that you used to
6. Each piece of information should concem oniy ONE aspect of the linking statement.
This is often the most difficult aspect of concept mapping. When you cannot think of the
information you used try breaking the linking statement down into its components parts. It
should be quite easy to expand these components to form the pieces of information.
7. Some links will have just one piece of information and others will have several.
8. When just one piece of information is involved remember that it can be the same as the
linking statement or it can be dEerent. If severai pieces of information are involved in a Iink,
Table 1
oir Spilfs
Tankers
ïlean (/p
.
Appeodix D
Subject 1
L-
Tsikas w-.
m r e D l Subject 1
Table D 1
.
ethod 1 and Methpd 3 Scores for S u e e t I
1 ~ e concept
y 1 Nurnber-Link Score 1
Oil Spills 4-Link
Tankers 4-Link
-
Clean Up 3-Link
Sobject 2
--
m
w
L
se-Tmmi-16
-
, 1 CtlmbI \--j~
- --7
L ----
Dhpcahi -23!l
Colfactioo -
w=-
A
- --
*c - - - 7--
---Ji---
19 - z l ï
W r e D f Subject 2
Table D3
Prestructural
I 3
1
conditions
a) oil dispersed by waves and harsh conditions
b) waves break up the oil
Another technique is collection Unistructural 3
a) oil is collected and removed from water and used again
Oil first contained by booms Unistnictural 3
a) surround oil, stops From spreading
oil is collected using skimers Unistructural 3
a) two method ofskimming to coliect oil
One method of skimming is suction Relational-2 4a
a) suck oil off water
b) also suck up lots of water: 90 %
Other method of skimming is adhesion
a) sweep matenal through spi11
b) oil sticks to material
C) oii carried away from spiii
d) oil squeezed out of material 1 1
Sorbents used to absorb oil collected by booms
a) can be natural or synthetic
variety of shapes- sheets mops etc.
C) used for smalf pockets usually
Table DS
Table D5
Table D6
m e D4 Subject 4
Table D7
Table D8
1~ e Concept
y 1 Number-Link Score 1
Tankers Concept was not used
Clean Up Concept was not used
Subject 5
Fieure D5 Subject 5
Table 159
I
.- - - - - .-
1 ~ e concept
y 1 Nurnber-Link Score 1
1 Tankers 1 3-Link 1
Subject 6
Figure D6 Subject 6
Table D l 1
3
I Tanker accidents are a cause
a) tanker accidents account for 12%of oil in the ocean
Unistructural
3
3
a) politics are a cause A
Vessel operation is a cause Unistructural
a) vesse1 operation accounts for 33%
Revenge is a politicai tactic Unistructural
a) revenge is a political tactic
Safety is a large concem
a) the Oil Pollution Act
A clean up is required afler an oil spill Unistructural
a) a clean up is required after an oil spill
- - - - - - - - - --- ----
Tabte Dl0
1 Number-Link Score I
1 Tankers
Appendix E
This appendix reports the six subjects answers to the interview questions. It also
provides a score for each subject. On occasion the inte~ewerhad to prompt or guide
subjects to elicit an appropnate answer. The interviewer's actions are presented in italics.
Table E l
i
h s w e r . Making s w ihat you'vc w v c d aU the -
1 Question 1. What is ihc h d a t pan about w o q ~mapping 18r o u ' ?
thet would bc. applicable. You linow you
problem from many angles and makr sure that you'vc covc~rxtt h ~ m
to SC* the
Question 5. Supposr: you don7 bave timr to m a t e r wmplztr m n c q t map. Which c;onccp~sdo >ou incIdr fo
convey ihe most iat'ormation'?
--- - - -
'ou n d to includr ttic: OINS in the midrllc. The! ;in- sort of much mon:vitritxi than the on* at the top so the!
w n v q more intonnation. T&e oil sprlls ( p l a d at top af mrrp) it dmn't m w q much information, A lot o h a l l e r
c o n q i s c m bs p l a d bmeath s d d y pmdures ( p l a c d m third lewl of map) 'io it i'i casier to w n v q more
intonnation
Question 6. &fore !ou start to mDatea map what plans do you makd?
I
1
AIWVCX. 1 didn't m d t ' ~ycciîïcplans for this map. 1 Sort oh-bcgan it and Ici thr i d a s tlow C i fom. 1 t ' thrt's
probûbly a vcq good way to do this. 1 might try 10 corne up with a List of d i t r m t wnwpîs h t I mi@t wmt to
imludr: in the map-if 1 was rushed for the.
Question 7. Which of the following do !ou thinli wouid bc sasitx io rnap and why'?
A passage wiîb picm
A pa~iiilgcihat's eliq to undastand
A pawgc 'au have w d khn:
A passage that wc'w rcad kforc. II would br: casier to nnismbcr morc details and the idcas would br: mon: flowing
so w ' d bc able IO rnap thm more quickl? and in greattr detail
- -
Quc-stion 9. What do 'ou do if you Mme to a liak that !ou Jon't und~rstruid'?
AIISWLT.I t h i d I'd stiil indude the 1h.k but gioss OVLT rhc answtr by providing a d-ription of what I did undcrstand
- - -- - -- - - - - - - - -- - - - - -- -
Question 10. Which parts d'the test do 'ou omit fmm ihe i ; o n q t map?
Question 1 1. Wliilc you rcad the test whrt do you do to help !ou rcrn~mbcrit'?
-
h w r . WeU tvficn 1rcad an~thing1 0 to summarim the ideas in rn? hcad. Sort ot'mdic a mental point form list
Question 12. If '.ou w m çreating a map in prqamtion for a test. which of tbc following would help y u the most and
w hy '?
Savine the concepts over and ovcr
Read thc rnap as many timcs ris possiblc
D k u s the map with someonc CL% to mitkc sure you undcrsimd it
h s w r . Discussing the rnap with somconr sLw hcausc with rtpt.iition !ou s o m c i i m ~90~ b l d aHer ü certain ?oint.
If you d k u ~ it,
s ncw idem md wordings will wmc up iio you'll ~ m t m b c itr in man! din.~xcntfoms
Question 13. If you are creating a rnap fmm a techcal passage. whiçh would help !ou the most and tvh!'?
Sounding out the wods !OU doa't kaow
Skipping thc p a s you don't unrlcmtmd
Writing the passage out in Four o\m ~ o r û beet'ore
s startïng the map
~ S W L T .Writing the passage out in my o w wor& wouId be most hclptiil bexause th~mit would help to gain a kiad
of undmtmding for it-
Table E2
-rotal 20
Su bject 2
Table E3
M e t m ~ n i t i v eInterview: Subiect 2
1 Question 1 . What is the hardesi part about concept mopping for p u ' ?
Anwtr. Sm not s m . What do !ou meau'?Ilèfiyoitr twp \tus dwec l e w b how tb tftr Irwfs tI~,@r?Bcfnre and
it will lead to clcm up
aftc~-iheoil spi11 happend bcLorc and ~~cntually
I hwer.
subjat 2
'fie intmiowr was distractcd by the conhion tfiat aro'lc:in question 3. 'mis question was not askd of
AIISWLT. Tdm. oil spills and clcm up of cou= as t h ' s rcitUy wh~rr:!ou start Faur inl'onnatiou tirun. Stut u-ith
oil spiils and aprcad out h m &a(. You start with tank~xsluid spread out h m that. same [or clem up
I
Question 6. Bcfore !ou start to creatc a map what plans do !ou m;rkcb?
- - - - - - - - -
ti U~IIIPI
sure 1 have cnough room to Hnte it al1 down. ~ ~ c ' ohud ~ncmhow worrid llrur
chunge? 1 g u s for thr: onc's I know havc a lot ut*points1 will @vc: h~xn morc m m .L i k clmn up 'ou h o u it has a
lot of points so 1 kind of mtcrwl it so tht~e'dbr: mon: room for it.
Question 7. Wbicb of ibc foUo\ving do !ou tbink would bc: casier to rnap ;mi tvhy'?
A passage with picnires
A passage thst's e a to~undastand
A p ~ s a g >ou
e have =ad bcforc
h l ~ a . A passage that's as? to undmtiind or one I'vr: read kfore. 1 pess with wods I'm not sure of I muld
organize the wonis, comment on idas etc..
Question 8. Wh! do o u ratad the text &tore you Link Mme concepts to~ethrr'?
Ansver. Weil ?ou çm't d y juït reûd it once and then go on to creatc the map. You have to go back and make sure
you have al1 the pointc.
A n m m WcîL I'd look through the k'it and try to h d some intomation in it. if not I might l a v e it oul that w e
could end it thm.
Question 10. Whch parts of the tcxt do 'ou omit h m thc concept map?
I
hswt'r. 1 omittd things that bad to do with s p i î i c açcidcrits Ur:E n o n VaIda. I didn't msntion anythmg spccifc
about that but gavc aii o v e n i w or lilic il s u m r n q of al1 the test
I1
- - - - -
We11.i took it in wtioas 1 startd out with tankm and th- I read al1 ihr: siutrabout tankers and thtm ail spills. Bid
FOU nrup each section individuul&:~First I w d the cmtk test then put ail spills, tankers and deon up dowm thm
~ clcm up.
rcad aU hc. links about oil spiiis thcri a11 tûe links aboui t d c thm
Qucstion 12. If o u 1vtn: creating o rnap in pnplrntioa 10r r test. which of the Collowving would help ?ou the rnost and
w hf?
1
Saping I ~ LconçcpLs
' OVLT and OVLT
R a d the rnap as man): timw as possible
Discuss Lhe rnap with somwnc e1.w IO milJic sure you undtxstand ii
tbwm. I'm not sure I'd do any of t h . 1 guc?is I'J go over it and rniikc: sun: i'd u a d e r s t d what I'd writtcm down
Qucstion 13. Ifyou are mcating a mrp tiom a tcchnical passage. which would hcip you the most and why?
Sounding out the words ?ou don't know
Skipping the parts !ou don't und~mtand
Writùig the p a w g e out in vour own woh .ifore st;trting hr:map
I
h w e r . Writing it out. that wa? !ou can gct a bcttcr p p ofccxtain conwpts irnd samctimcs Xyou cm't gct iato
Four homework ît muld m d c it casier.
Table E l
Table ES
Question 1. Whot is the hardwt part about concept mapping for >ou?
Anwcr. Actually I found it mcult to s e whi~bway the m w s wat. But da 1 got uwAl to it 1 hund it oasim
Question 2. Whst would hdp !ou b w m e a bettrr wawpt mappm'?
Answcr. Isupposr: ifyou wcn: io pvc an esmplc of which way thc arrows writ
Quation 3. F iow do wnwpts at the top of the concept rnap ditkr from thow at the bottom'?
Ansver. At the bottom th+ gct mucb more dctail~dbaausc ai thc top t h q an.kind of basic gczieral concept-smd
th+ get more detailai as you go d o m
-- - -- - - - - -
Question 7. Which of ihc: foliowing do !ou thdi would be casier to niap and wb'?
A pasirigc with pic-
A passage that's e a to~undastand
A pi~~~sageyou have r a d bsfori:
Aaswer- A p-p you'vl: rcd brfon: would br casier ba-ausc. if you rad wmrthing more thm once thro you get
more out of it.
Question 8. Why do you reread the test before you link some concepts togethm'?
- -
Question 10. Whiçh p a s OC the text do FOU omit fiom îhe concept rnap*?
Answtr. Rtxause it wir?in't important LWOU@ or it didn't go with w hat 1 wris tqing to do. Ilow did yorr clrcrtfe whar
wm inrponunt? [ s u p p w the b i g p topics likc gov~rnmcntinvolvtmt~tin clt';ui up. but i woulh't put down thc
iavolv~mcntot'an individual person.
Qut3tion 1 1. mile ?ou r a d the tcxt what do 'ou do to help o u r m e m k it'?
- - - -
Question 12. ICyou wtw crcating a map in prcparation for a tcst. rvhiçh ot'tbc tellowing would hdp 'ou thc most and
w hy0?
Saying the conwpts ovtr and over
Rcad ibr: map as ma! tirnt3 as possible
Discws tht. rnap wiib sornconz ~1st.to rnakc: sun: y u understand it
A n s w r . Probably Ji-wuss the map. I ï o u r d Ihc rnap o u oniy remcmbm what p u r,-rofc but if !ou discuss it !au
wver the whok tcst iastead of ju.t the arts >ou W V L T ~
Qut.stion 13. If 'ou art. creating a map from a technical passage. which would help !au the mast and why'?
.%undilie, out the words !ou dodi know
Skipping the parts !ou don't und~mtmd
Writing the pil.ssiip out in vour own words berore starting the map
Answcr. Probably witing thc passagr: out in my own words. Saunding out the uords wouliil't help mc undcrsiand il.
But if 1 \rote it out in my o w rvords would widtxstand what 1 wrotr md would undmtand it r e d y well
Table E6
Question SCOK
1 1
Subject 4
Table E7
Interview: Subiect 4
etaco~nitive
-
I Question 5. Supposc: !ou don't have timc to crcritc a cornpkte conccpt map. Which c o n q t s do you uicludr: to
con\- the most Latorrnation'?
h u ' c r . Which gcneralizcd ours art. ri@-w ill d k t the outcomw OC more qxciiilizrxi ones. Chm carch& so
dcn't go a i ï topic
Question 7. Which of the tollowing do !ou thid would bt: t.aster to map md why'?
A pwagc. with picturcs
A passage that's cas? to understaud
A passage >ou have read kforr
Question 8. Why do !ou mead the test before !ou Link ,=me wnwpts togethers?
Ansaver. Btxauss: whik: o u rad o u don? h o w what's going to k a p p Iater w-ha !ou =ad. You mi@ forget and it
helps you further understand the f i timds just a triai. You don't pa! much attention !ou just \vant io k n o ~what it's
about.
-- - -- - - - - - - -- -
Quwtion 13. If you are cmating a map tlom a tcchnical paw7gc. tvhich would hslp !ou the most and why'?
Sounding out the wods !ou don? knaw
Skipping the parts you don't undmtnnd
Wnting the pa.~riilgt.out in your OHII w o d s bdow starting thc rnap
Table ES
Qucstian Swn:
1 3
2 1
3 2
4 2
5 2
6 2
7 O
8 2
9 2
1O 2
11 2
12 2
13 2
Total 23
Subject 5
Table E9
- -- - -
Answur. The data itrrns. Structuriag the links. Thc way ibis part c;onncxts to that p u t md how to writc that out.
Quesiion 2. What woufd help !ou b~xomca k t t e r concept mapprr'?
Question 5. Supposc.'ou don't haw tirne to crcatc: a complctc conccpi rnirp. Whiçh wnwpts do !ou uiçludc: to
con\;+ the most Xormation'?
t h i v e r . Thc top b a a w ?ou cm do thc top hzlf without the bonom. Bottom LS just clrborriting more on the top.
Qut?ition 6. Berore you stûri to creatr: a rnap what plans do !ou mdd?
Question 7. Which of the foiiowing do you thdi wouId bc casier to rnap and ~ d g ?
A pac;sage with picturcr;
A passage that's e a q to undtrstmd
A passage you ha= r a d before
Answer. A passage I've nad before (i-; I'd bt: mort: cornfortable w i h the subject matter
Question 9. What do you do if ?ou corne IO a LinCi that !ou don't undmtmd?
h 7 v r r . I'd probably skip it and go back to it later. rd sort of fiusricxl on other parts of the maps and then uk
somebcdy
- -
Question 10. Which parts of the text do you omit h m the çonccpt map?
Answcsr. 1 don't think f r e d y omitted anphhg maybe tfie ~pecificpiirts of what b p p m d to s v q t h h g at the
kpinnin@
Question 12. If 'ou w m mcating a map in prcparatian for a tmt. wbicb of the following would hdp !ou the most and
why'?
Swing the macepis over and o v w
Read thc msp a!s mm! timrs as possiblr:
Discuss ihc map with somwnr: CL* IOmdt. surc !ou und~rstsndit
Table EIO
Table EL 1
AUSTVLT.Pmbably widcxstanding what ha5 Io be donc was h c most difiçult part for me. I rcally didn't u n d ~ n t m d
what had to bt: donc in thc kginning. Lsacify hot, it should bc sct up. whiçh May thc m o ~ go.
s what gocy w r c
1 Question 2. M a t would hrlp p u b ~ w r n ar bcttcr wnwpt rnoppm*? 1
Answw. 1 pues5 thc woy it wali esplaind. I don't know bow it would be a s i m !or rnc to understand that. Did 1 do it
C)K so 1 didn't catch on ma~bc:ifvou wuld h d a dift'~nat\vay ofesplaining ir.
ri@? ~'L'.P
1
Questiou 3. 1IOH. do c'onwpts al the [op of tbc concept mop ditlia tom those at ihc bottom'?
Anwtr. I ~ U L Wthe top one is the one that a f i a t s thc most m a . Acrually uow that 1 look at it 1 g u ~ ~safkty
i s akT~r:tsa
loi of people. 'ïhat LS 1 ptxs the top one is more f w u s d to the subject in i t s d tbun the bottom on&%..Ire yrm
s-.ing rhar rire nne ut rltc hortcrtii uw rtrorr gctrrrcil? Ycah .lrid rlir orle sur rhc [op are n i c m .rpt.ciJk? Ycih
I
1 hm~r. Probob. the ones at the top as th~$re thr: most qxciîiï IO the topiç 1
Question 5 . Suppose ?ou don't have the to creatr: a mmpletc: concept mûp. Which c o n ~ ~ pd~
t syou IllçIUdc: 10
convcy thr: most d'ormation'?
Question 7. W c h of the toilowing do !ou thinCi would bt: saster to map and why'?
A passage with picturc5
A passage that's t'a+- to undt'~?itaud
A passage o u have =ad beforc
AIMWLT.Probobly a passage that i've =ad bet'ore Lt'you Cr:me rn opportunity to rcad it thm w h a I'm mapping it
I'm obviously familiar wth it-then to retir=sh if I hat-e it wit me it hclps 1
- --- - - -- -
Probably if somethnlg wasn't CIW to me or it- 1 had forgottcn what the test had said
1 l
Answtx- Robably stop and ignore that pari CCbuldo u r+r back lu iltr zerl? Wetl it dqxnds. if it ixiw a th! simal1
topic: t h t wasi't n w he top lmel then i probabh wouldn't
1 Question 10. hi ch pvrs of the tzvt do 'ou omit h m the concept mapy I
Answtr. 1 cm't givc you )rpwitics5s 1 don't rcmcmbtr thc test ruijrnorc. 1 g u a s anythuig that d-n't .ama!!large a
w n c m as those 1 did chose.
1 Question 1 1. Whilc o u r a d the test wliat do ?-oudo to help )ou wmwnbcr ilo?
Quatioa 12. It'you werc crwting a map in ptwparatian for a tmt. rvhich of the foIlowiag would hclp !ou thc most mci
w h!?
~ ovtr
Saying the concepts o v t and
I h w c r . Disussing it with somwne elsc. Myxiï I cm't rcmcmbtr v e n wsll Espc-iaily if' it's j uït reading or ing
it ta rnyseif If I'm taking about it and heu d i f f a n t ideos 1'11 probably casily i m i a n b c ~it ai ihr tirne of the test
C)uestion 13. If !ou arc cxatuig a rnap h m a twhnical passage. whiçh would help !ou the most md why?
Sounding out the wanls !ou don7 how
Skippmg the parts !ou don't undtr'itaad
Writing the pwsagc out in Four own wonts kforc starting thc rnap
A n s w x . Weil I woulda't do thc tint onc as 1 have bad mcmory. if 1 skippd parts t dih't undmtmd thtm 1 might
not undastand things Lirrthrr on in the passage. So I gurris writing it out in rny own words. Finding d&nitians and
exact mcaning of certain things.
Table El2