1989 - Filloy - Rojano - Solving Equations The Transition From Arithmetic To Algebra
1989 - Filloy - Rojano - Solving Equations The Transition From Arithmetic To Algebra
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms
is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to For the Learning of
Mathematics
20
fe tin
with negative solutions, is working on the equation 8x +
30 = 5x + 9. Using the geometric model, VT derives the
simplified equation 3x + 30 = 9.
•- A-«C-A VT: "How . . . ?"
I: "Do
Step 3: producing the new equation, (C you
- A)x - think
B you can solve this one?"
VT: "Nine minus . . . nine minus a number larger than
Step 4: solving the new equation nine is going to give a negative number, divided by
Step 5: verifying the solution three . . /
1 QDQ..D ■■ ■ I I on a., a
I: "Can you solve it now?"
VI: "No. . .(pause). . . Well, if you want me to, I'll. . ."
(A) objects with equal (B) objects with equal (C) objects with same
(unknown) weight (known) weight (unknown) weight
She takes out her calculator, inverts operations and
obtains x= -7.
(B) objects (C-A) objects model hides its meaning; even the performance of the
actions, which do not work, is not sufficient to make her
voluntarily
Step 3: writing the new equation, (Cabandon
- A)x the
- solution
B path. Since VT has
handled the model swiftly and fluently in many previous
Step 4: solving the equation items, her adherence to it here looks like inertial behavior.
Step 5: verifying the solution
2. The modification of the arithmetical notion of equa-
tion, (a) The modification arises because examples are
encountered whose structure does not correspond to
Those children who displayed a high level of pre-algebraic
that of the examples used in the instructional phase of
proficiency were provided with only the first step of either
modelling.
model, leaving to them the development of the subse-
Example. MT, also
quent steps with as little help as possible from age 13, isthe
working on a sequence of
inter-
viewer. Once they had mastered the items.use
She hasofalready
the abstracted
models from actions
foron the
the type Ax + B = Cx, they were given increasingly
model and ignores it in the resolution ofcom-
the equations. At
plex types: Ax + B = Cx+D;Ax~B= first Cx
she does+not D;A
perceivex-B= Cx of the equa-
the equivalence
- D, etc., so that we could observetions 2xhow
+ 3 = 5jc; they
3 + 2x = 5x;made
5x = 2x + 3; the
5x = 3 + 2x.
21
■ID -D
3. The use of personal codes to indicate actions to be
performed in the solution process. This is an inter-
mediate stage on the path to the development of a fully
algebraic syntax as the personal code becomes inade-
quate when the examples become more complex. 8 6
22
x *- ~^i.
- 1 x - J *-
for algorithmic or analytic approaches, we minimize the
risk of making false generalizations about the evolution
of certain operations from concrete to syntactical form.
23 9 iu We have analyzed in detail the interview protocols of two
girls after the instructional intervention - two girls with
extreme and antagonistic tendencies: MT, in the high
stratum, age 13, with a marked algorithmic tendency, and
VT, in the same stratum, the same age, with a strong
semantic tendency. For both students, the same model
was used in the instructional phase on operating on the
unknown.
With the interviewer's hel
remaining area
Our mostis not
relevant findings were: zero.
VT: "Nine times * minus 7."
1 . The spontaneous development of the use of a concrete
I: "What is this equal to?"
model in order to operate on the unknown in an equa-
VT: "It equals two." Writes 9* + 7 = 2.
tion is not uniform even in students with similar levels
Item 32: 10*- 3 -Ax of pre-algebraic proficiency. The form of the develop-
Without any help, VT says, "That would be six times *, ment is strongly influenced by the individual student's
minus 3, equals zero", and writes 6x - 3 = 0. tendency to choose a particular approach. Extreme
cases were detected in which developments in the use of
5. A breaking away from the model, transferring the the same model were quite dissimilar. In one case (VT)
operativity on the coefficients to the terms containingwith an operative tendency, the development adhered
the unknown. At the time of transition this can lead toto the use of the model context even when the equation
the familiar error of combining terms of different types required very complicated modelling procedures.
degrees. In the other case (MT) there was a constant search for
Item 11: 15* +13=16* the syntactic elements present in the actions on the
model as they were repeated in equation after equation
MT:"16* minus 15* equals one. So one and 13 equals
14." and in type after type. The subject broke away from the
semantics of the model and started associating actions
Her attention is drawn to the geometric model.
on the model with a more abstract language through
MT proceeds again: 15* + 13 = 16* - 15*
the creation of personal codes, belonging neither to the
MT: "One * ... h'm. There should be one times * model nor to algebra.
»♦
23
I
attachment to the model commit these er
their introduction of personal codes.
codes, incidentally, may be produced by
Mu llQs
actions:
Ax + B = Cx + D
►- A- ♦- C-A-H
24
25