Memory
Memory
Memory is the means by which we retain and draw on our past experiences to use that
information in the present (Tulving, 2000b; Tulving & Craik, 2000). As a process,
memory refers to the dynamic mechanisms associated with storing, retaining, and
retrieving information about past experience (Bjorklund, Schneider, & Hernández Blasi,
2003; Crowder, 1976). Specifically, cognitive psychologists have identified three
common operations of memory: encoding, storage, and retrieval (Baddeley, 2002;
Brebion, 2007; Brown & Craik, 2000). Each operation represents a stage in memory
processing.
1. ENCODING
Encoding (getting information in), storage (maintaining it), and retrieval (getting it
out).
Encoding involves forming a memory code .For example, when you form a memory
code for a word, you might emphasize how it looks, how it sounds, or what it means.
Encoding usually requires attention, which is why you may not be able to recall exactly
what a penny looks like—most people don’t pay much attention to the appearance of
a penny.
2. SRTORAGE
This concerns the nature of memory stores, i.e., where the information is stored, how
long the memory lasts for (duration), how much can be stored at any time (capacity)
and what kind of information is held.
3. RETRIEVAL
STM is stored and retrieved sequentially. For example, if a group of participants are
given a list of words to remember, and then asked to recall the fourth word on the list,
participants go through the list in the order they heard it in order to retrieve the
information.
LTM is stored and retrieved by association. This is why you can remember what you
went upstairs for if you go back to the room where you first thought about it.
• 2) Recognition: involves realizing that a certain stimulus event is the one that
have been seen or heard before. Example: Multiple choice question
Generation-recognition model
This model suggest that, we can usually recognize more than we can recall; because
recall involves the same mental process involved in recognition plus another process
not required for recognition.
We have to generate information rather than simply recognize the information
presented.
STAGES OF MEMORY
Maintenance Rehearsal
Sensory Encodi
Input Attention ng
Sensory Working or Long-term
Memory Short-term memory
Memory
1. SENSORY MEMORY
Sensory memory preserves information in its original sensory form for a brief time,
usually only a fraction of a second. Sensory memory allows the sensation of a visual
pattern, sound, or touch to linger for a brief moment after the sensory stimulation is
over. In the case of vision, people really perceive an afterimage rather than the actual
stimulus.
The brief preservation of sensations in sensory memory gives you additional time to
try to recognize stimuli. However, you’d better take advantage of sensory storage
immediately because it doesn’t last long. In a classic experiment, George Sperling
(1960) demonstrated that the memory trace in the visual sensory store decays in about
one-quarter of a second. There is some debate about whether stimulus persistence
really involves memory storage (Nairne & Neath, 2013). Some theorists view it as an
artefact of the perceptual processing of incoming stimuli that is attributable to
excitatory feedback in specific N neural circuits (Francis, 1999). In other words, stimulus
persistence may be more like an echo than a memory.
✓ To find out how much information could be retained in sensory memory, early
researchers used a method called the Whole report procedure. In this method,
subjects looked for a fraction of a second at a visual display of nine items as the
one shown below.
B X O
R T P
W Q L
✓ They were then asked to recall as many of the items as they could. On average
subjects could remember only about four of the nine items. Researchers
interpreted this as evidence that the capacity of sensory memory was only four
items.
✓ Anyway, Sperling devised a method called the Partial report procedure. Like the
earlier researchers, he used a 3×3 matrix of letters and flashed the array for a
fraction of a second. However, unlike earlier researchers, he asked the subjects
to report only one row of the array. Immediately after the presentation of the
array of letters, a high, medium, or low tone was presented, indicating to the
subjects which row to recall.
✓ Therefore the subjects could not focus on just one row. It turned out that,
regardless of which row Sperling asked for, the subjects recall was nearly
perfect, thus suggesting that the capacity of sensory memory was about nine
items.
✓ Other experiments with larger arrays confirmed the nine item limit. So in the
whole report method, as the subjects were reporting what they saw, their
sensory memory of the array was decaying. By the time the subjects reported
about four of the stimuli, the memory had already decayed. Sperling’s
procedure, however, avoided this problem.
Sensory Attention
Input
Sensory Working or
Memory Short-term
Memory
Short-term memory (STM) is a limited-capacity store that can maintain unrehearsed
information for up to about 20 seconds. In contrast, information stored in long-term
memory may last weeks, months, or years. You can maintain information in your
short-term store for longer than 20 seconds. By engaging in rehearsal—the process
of repetitively verbalizing or thinking about information.
Durability of Storage
Without rehearsal, information in short-term memory is lost in 15 to 30 seconds
(Nairne, 2003). This rapid loss was demonstrated in a study by Peterson and Peterson
(1959). They measured how long undergraduates could remember three consonants if
they couldn’t rehearse them. To prevent rehearsal, the Petersons required the students
to count backward by threes from the time the consonants were presented until they
saw a light that signalled the recall test. Participants’ recall accuracy was pretty dismal
after only 15 seconds. Other approaches to the issue have suggested that the typical
duration of short-term memory storage may even be shorter (Baddeley, 1986).
Theorists originally believed that the loss of information from short-term memory was
attributable purely to time-related decay of memory traces, but follow-up research
showed that interference from competing material also contributes (Oberauer &
Lewandowsky, 2014; Nairne & Neath, 2013).
Capacity of Storage
Short-term memory is also limited in the number of items it can hold. The small
capacity of short-term memory was pointed out by George Miller (1956) in a
famous paper called “The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two: Some Limits
on Our Capacity for Processing Information.” Miller noticed that people could recall
only about seven items in tasks that required the use of short-term memory. When
short-term memory is filled to capacity, the insertion of new information “bumps out”
some of the information currently in short-term memory. The limited capacity of short-
term memory constrains people’s ability to perform tasks in which they need to
mentally juggle various pieces of information (Baddeley & Hitch, 1974).
The capacity of short-term memory may even be less than widely assumed. Nelson
Cowan (2005, 2010) cites evidence indicating that the capacity of short-term memory
is 4 plus or minus 1. The consensus on the capacity of short-term memory seems to
be moving toward this smaller estimate (Lustig et al., 2009).
According to Cowan, the capacity of short-term memory has historically been
overestimated because researchers have often failed to take steps to prevent covert
rehearsal, or chunking, by participants. It has long been known that people can
increase the capacity of their short-term memory by combining
stimuli into larger, possibly higher-order units, called chunks (Simon, 1974). A chunk
is a group of familiar stimuli stored as a single unit. You can demonstrate the effect
of chunkinby asking someone to recall a sequence of twelve letters grouped in the
following way:
FB - INB - CC - IAIB - M
As you read the letters aloud, pause at the hyphens. Your subject will probably attempt
to remember each letter separately because there are no obvious groups or chunks.
But a string of twelve letters is too long for STM, so errors are likely. Now present the
same string of letters to another person, but place the pauses in the following
locations:
The letters now form four familiar chunks that should occupy only four slots in short-
term memory, resulting in successful recall (Bower & Springston, 1970).
To successfully chunk the letters I B M, a subject must first recognize these letters as a
familiar unit. This familiarity has to be stored somewhere in long-term memory. Thus,
in this case, information was transferred from long-term into short-term memory. This
type of transfer is not unusual. People routinely draw information out of their long-
term memory banks to evaluate and understand information that they are working
with in short-term memory.
One point of view is that all information stored in long-term memory is stored there
permanently. According to this view, forgetting occurs only because people sometimes
cannot retrieve needed information. To draw an analogy, imagine that memories are
stored in long-term memory like marbles in a barrel. According to this view, none of
the marbles ever leak out. When you forget, you just aren’t able to dig out the right
marble, but it’s there—somewhere. In a survey of the general public’s views on
memory, 48% of the respondents endorsed the idea that long-term memory storage
is permanent (Simons & Chabris, 2011). An alternative point of view assumes that some
memories stored in long-term memory do vanish forever. According to this view, the
barrel is leaky and some of the marbles roll out, never to return.
The existence of flashbulb memories is one piece of evidence that has been cited to
support the notion that long-term memory storage may be permanent. At first glance,
flashbulb memories, which are thought to be unusually vivid and detailed
recollections of momentous events, provide striking examples of seemingly
permanent storage (Brown & Kulik, 1977). Research eventually showed that flashbulb
memories are neither as accurate nor as special as once believed (Hirst et al., 2009;
Schmolck, Buffalo, & Squire, 2000). Like other memories, they become less detailed
and complete with time and are often inaccurate (Talarico & Rubin, 2009). Research
suggests that it is not extraordinary accuracy or longevity that distinguish flashbulb
memories. Rather, what makes them special is that people subjectively feel that these
memories are exceptionally vivid, that they have exceptional confidence (albeit
misplaced) in their memories’ accuracy, and that more emotional intensity is attached
to these recollections (Talarico & Rubin, 2003, 2007). So, perhaps flashbulb memories
are “special,” but not in the way originally envisioned.
MODELS OF MEMORY
The multistore model of memory (also known as the modal model/stage model) was
proposed by Atkinson and Shiffrin (1968) and is a structural model. They proposed
that memory consisted of three stores: a sensory register, short-term memory (STM)
and long-term memory (LTM). Information passes from store to store in a linear way,
and has been described as an information processing model (like a computer) with an
input, process and output.
Information is detected by the sense organs and enters the sensory memory. If
attended to this information enters the short term memory. Information from the
short-term memory is transferred to the long-term memory only if that information is
rehearsed (i.e. repeated). If maintenance rehearsal (repetition) does not occur, then
information is forgotten, and lost from short term memory through the processes of
displacement or decay. Each store is a unitary structure and has its own characteristics
in terms of encoding, capacity and duration.
Encoding is the way information is changed so that it can be stored in the memory.
There are three main ways in which information can be encoded:
1. Visual (picture),
2. Acoustic (sound),
3. Semantic (meaning).
Capacity concerns how much information can be stored. Duration refers to the period
of time information can last in the memory stores.
Sensory Memory
• Duration: ¼ to ½ second
• Capacity: all sensory experience (v. larger capacity)
• Encoding: sense specific (e.g. different stores for each sense)
• Duration: Unlimited
• Capacity: Unlimited
• Encoding: Mainly Semantic (but can be visual and auditory)
It has now become apparent that both short-term and long-term memory are more
complicated than previously thought. Working Model of Memory proposed by
Baddeley and Hitch (1974) showed that short term memory is more than just one
simple unitary store and comprises different components (e.g. central executive,
Visuospatial etc.). Baddeley and Hitch (1974) argue that the picture of short-term
memory (STM) provided by the Multi-Store Model is far too simple.
According to the Multi-Store Model STM holds limited amounts of information for
short periods of time with relatively little processing. It is a unitary system. This means
it is a single system (or store) without any subsystems. Working Memory is not a
unitary store.
Working memory is short-term memory However, instead of all information going into
one single store, there are different systems for different types of information
CENTRAL EXECUTIVE
Drives the whole system (e.g., the boss of working memory) and allocates data to the
subsystems: the phonological loop and the visuospatial sketchpad. It also deals with
cognitive tasks such as mental arithmetic and problem-solving. The central executive
decides which information is attended to and which parts of the working memory to
send that information to be dealt with.
For example, two activities sometimes come into conflict, such as driving a car and
talking. Rather than hitting a cyclist who is wobbling all over the road, it is preferable
to stop talking and concentrate on driving. The central executive directs attention
and gives priority to particular activities.
Baddeley suggests that the central executive acts more like a system which
controls attentional processes rather than as a memory store.
This is unlike the phonological loop and the visuospatial sketchpad, which are
specialized storage systems. The central executive enables the working memory
system to selectively attend to some stimuli and ignore others.
▪ Phonological Loop
Part of working memory that deals with spoken and written material. It can be used
to remember a phone number. It consists of two parts
Phonological Store (inner ear) – Linked to speech perception. Holds information
in a speech-based form (i.e., spoken words) for 1-2 seconds.
Articulatory control process (inner voice) – Linked to speech production. Used to
rehearse and store verbal information from the phonological store.
Levels of Processing Model of Memory
The levels of processing model (Craik and Lockhart, 1972) focuses on the depth of
processing involved in memory, and predicts the deeper information is processed, the
longer a memory trace will last. Unlike the multi-store model it is a non-structured
approach. The basic idea is that memory is really just what happens as a result of
processing information. Memory is just a by-product of the depth of processing of
information, and there is no clear distinction between short term and long term
memory.
Craik and Lockhart propose that incoming information can be processed at different
levels. For instance, they maintain that in dealing with verbal information, people
engage in three progressively deeper levels of processing: structural, phonemic, and
semantic encoding. Structural encoding is relatively shallow processing that
emphasizes the physical structure of the stimulus. For example, if words are flashed on
a screen, structural encoding registers such things as how the words are printed
(capital, lowercase, and so on) or their length (how many letters). Further analysis may
result in phonemic encoding, which emphasizes what a word sounds like. Phonemic
encoding involves naming or saying (perhaps silently) the words. Finally, semantic
encoding emphasizes the meaning of verbal input; it involves thinking about the
objects and actions the words represent. Levels-of-processing theory proposes that
deeper levels of processing result in longer-lasting memory codes.
In one experimental test of levels-of-processing theory, Craik and Tulving (1975)
compared the durability of structural, phonemic, and semantic encoding. They directed
subjects’ attention to particular aspects of briefly presented stimulus words by asking
them questions about various characteristics of the words.
The questions were designed to engage the participants in different levels of
processing. After responding to sixty words, the participants received an unexpected
test of their memory for the words. As predicted, the subjects’ recall was low after
structural encoding, notably better after phonemic encoding, and highest after
semantic encoding.
The hypothesis that deeper processing leads to enhanced memory has been replicated
in many studies (Craik, 2002; Lockhart & Craik, 1990). Levels-of-processing theory has
been enormously influential; it has shown that memory involves more than just storage
and has inspired a great deal of research on how processing considerations affect
memory (Roediger, Gallo, & Geraci, 2002).
One of the earliest and most influential distinctions of long-term memory was
proposed by Tulving (1972). He proposed a distinction between episodic, semantic
and procedural memory.
➢ Explicit Memory
Explicit memory also called declarative or conscious memory, memory consciously
recalled or declared. Explicit memory can use to directly respond to a question.
Two subtypes of explicit memory
Episodic information—information about events or “episodes”
Semantic information—information about facts, general knowledge, school
work
▪ Episodic Memory
• Episodic memory is a part of the long-term memory responsible
for storing information about events (i.e. episodes) that we have
experienced in our lives.
• It involves conscious thought and is declarative. An example
would be a memory of our 1st day at school.
• The knowledge that we hold in episodic memory focuses on
“knowing that” something is the case (i.e. declarative). For
example, we might have an episodic memory for knowing that we
caught the bus to college today.
▪ Semantic Memory
• Semantic memory is a part of the long-term memory responsible for
storing information about the world. This includes knowledge about
the meaning of words, as well as general knowledge.
• For example, New Delhi is the capital of India. It involves
conscious thought and is declarative.
• The knowledge that we hold in semantic memory focuses on
“knowing that” something is the case (i.e. declarative). For
example, we might have a semantic memory for knowing that
Paris is the capital of France.
➢ Implicit Memory
Also known as non-declarative memory. It influences your thoughts or behavior,
but does not enter consciousness.
▪ Procedural Memory
• Procedural memory is a part of the long-term memory which is
responsible for “knowing how” to do things, i.e. memory of motor skills.
• It does not involve conscious (i.e. it’s unconscious - automatic)
thought and is not declarative. For example, procedural memory
would involve knowledge of how to ride a bicycle.
Not all information fits neatly into conceptual hierarchies or schemas. Much
knowledge seems to be organized into less systematic frameworks, called semantic
networks (Collins & Loftus, 1975). A semantic network consists of nodes
representing concepts, joined together by pathways that link related concepts.
The ovals are the nodes, and the words inside the ovals are the interlinked concepts.
The lines connecting the nodes are the pathways. The length of each pathway
represents the degree of association between two concepts. Shorter pathways imply
stronger associations.
Semantic networks have proven useful in explaining why thinking about one word
(such as butter) can make a closely related word (such as bread) easier to remember.
According to Collins and Loftus (1975), when people think about a word, their thoughts
naturally go to related words. They call this process spreading activation within a
semantic network. They assume that activation spreads out along the pathways of the
semantic network surrounding the word. They also theorize that the strength of this
activation decreases as it travels outward, much as ripples decrease in size as they
radiate outward from a rock tossed into a pond.
Why do we forget ?
Sensory memory
The senses momentarily register
amazing detail
Short-term memory
A few items are both noticed
and encoded
Long-term storage
Some items are altered or lost
Forgetting as retrieval
Retrieval from failure
long-term memory
Depending on interference, retrieval
Retrieval—process of accessing stored information .Sometimes info IS encoded into
cues, moods, and motives, some
LTM, but we can’t retrieve it
Encoding
Short-term Long-term
memory memory
X
Retrie
Retrieval
Measures of Retrieval
failure
▪ Recall: test of LTM that involves retrieving memories without cues, also termed
free recall.
▪ Cued recall: test of LTM that involves remembering an item of information in
response to a retrial cue.
▪ Recognition: test of LTM that involves identifying correct information from a
series of possible.
Encoding Specificity
Improved recall of information when the context present at encoding and retrieval
are the same.
Memory for a specific stimulus is much better if there is a match between the
mood state at the time of experiencing the stimulus and the mood state when
trying to recall the stimulus.
MEMORY DISTORTION
FORGETTING
Why do we forget? There are two simple answers to this question. First, the memory has
disappeared - it is no longer available. Second, the memory is still stored in the memory
system but, for some reason, it cannot be retrieved. Forgetting information from short
term memory (STM) can be explained using the theories of trace decay and displacement.
Forgetting from long term memory (LTM) can be explained using the theories
of interference, retrieval failure and lack of consolidation.
The Forgetting Curve
Hermann Ebbinghaus first began to study forgetting using nonsense syllables. Nonsense
syllables are three-letter combinations that look like words but are meaningless (ROH,
KUF).
Forgetting theories
➢ Interference Theories
Memories interfering with memories”. Forgetting NOT caused by mere passage
of time, caused by one memory competing with or replacing another memory.
Two types of interference
1. Retroactive Interference
When new memory interferes with remembering old information.
Eg: when new phone number interferes with ability to remember old
phone number.
2. Proactive Interference
Opposite of retroactive interference. When an OLD memory interferes with
remembering NEW information
Example: Memories of where you parked your car on campus the past
week interferes with ability find car today
➢ Motivated Forgetting
Undesired memory is held back form awareness
• Suppression—conscious forgetting
• Repression—unconscious forgetting (Freudian)
Motivated forgetting is what Freud referred to as repressing
memories. According to Freud, there are times when an event or an
action is so painful that we can't deal with the memory of it, so we
repress the memory completely.
By pushing the memory into the subconscious and actively
repressing it, we are unable to recall the memory.
So in essence, motivated forgetting is purposeful forgetting.
100
100%
90
Average 80
percentage of 70
information 60
50
retained 40
30
20
10
0
20 1 8 2 2 6 31
mi gradually
Memories fade away or decay
Interval h original
between hif unused,
4 oftime
learning da dasyllables
plays
nonsense da role
critical
in this. Ability to retrieve info declines with time after
and memory test original encoding.
➢ Retrieval Failure
Forgetting in STM
Forgetting in LTM
Interference: More information will be stored and become
confused together.
Retrieval Failure: Information may be available but temporarily
inaccessible
Motivated forgetting: we may actively work to forget memories.
Two basic forms are
suppression- a conscious form of forgetting.
Repression- and unconscious form of forgetting
AMNESIA
Amnesia is a form of memory loss. Some people with amnesia have difficulty forming
new memories. Others can’t recall facts or past experiences. People with amnesia
usually retain knowledge of their own identity, as well as motor skills.
➢ Retrograde amnesia
When you have retrograde amnesia, you lose existing, previously made
memories. This type of amnesia tends to affect recently formed memories first.
Older memories, such as memories from childhood, are usually affected more
slowly. Diseases such as dementia cause gradual retrograde amnesia.
➢ Anterograde amnesia
When you have anterograde amnesia, you can’t form new memories. This
effect can be temporary. For example, you can experience it during a blackout
caused by too much alcohol. It can also be permanent. You can experience it if
the area of your brain known as your hippocampus is damaged. Your
hippocampus plays an important role in forming memories.
➢ Transient global amnesia
Transient global amnesia (TGA) is a poorly understood condition. If you
develop it, you will experience confusion or agitation that comes and goes
repeatedly over the course of several hours. You may experience memory loss
in the hours before the attack, and you will probably have no lasting memory
of the experience. Scientists think that TGA occurs as the result of seizure-like
activity or a brief blockage of the blood vessels supplying your brain. It occurs
more frequently in middle-aged and older adults.
➢ Infantile amnesia
Most people can’t remember the first three to five years of life. This common
phenomenon is called infantile or childhood amnesia.