MIT9 00SCF11 Text-1
MIT9 00SCF11 Text-1
Eventually, I found some techniques to help my students understand and appreciate what I found
to be important. First, I realized that psychology actually did matter to my students, but that I
needed to make it clear to them why it did. I therefore created a more consistent focus on the
theme of behavior. One of the most fundamental integrating principles of the discipline of
psychology is its focus on behavior, and yet that is often not made clear to students. Affect,
cognition, and motivation are critical and essential, and yet are frequently best understood and
made relevant through their links with behavior. Once I figured this out, I began tying all the
material to this concept: The sympathetic nervous system matters because it has specific and
predictable influences on our behavior. Piaget’s findings matter because they help us understand
the child’s behavior (not just his or her thinking). And social cognition matters because our
social thinking helps us better relate to the other people in our everyday social lives. This
integrating theme allows me to organize my lectures, my writing assignments, and my testing.
Second was the issue of empiricism: I emphasized that what seems true might not be true, and
we need to try to determine whether it is. The idea of empirical research testing falsifiable
hypotheses and explaining much (but never all) behavior—the idea of psychology as a science—
was critical, and it helped me differentiate psychology from other disciplines. Another reason for
emphasizing empiricism is that the Introduction to Psychology course represents many students’
best opportunity to learn about the fundamentals of scientific research.
I wrote this book to help students organize their thinking about psychology at a conceptual level.
Five or ten years from now, I do not expect my students to remember the details of most of what
I teach them. However, I do hope that they will remember that psychology matters because it
helps us understand behavior and that our knowledge of psychology is based on empirical study.
This book is designed to facilitate these learning outcomes. I have used three techniques to help
focus students on behavior:
1. Chapter openers. I begin my focus on behavior by opening each chapter with a chapter
opener showcasing an interesting real-world example of people who are dealing with
behavioral questions and who can use psychology to help them answer those questions.
The opener is designed to draw the student into the chapter and create an interest in
learning about the topic.
2. Psychology in everyday life. Each chapter contains one or two features designed to link
the principles from the chapter to real-world applications in business, environment,
health, law, learning, and other relevant domains. For instance, the application in Chapter
6 "Growing and Developing"—“What Makes a Good Parent?”—applies the concepts of
parenting styles in a mini handbook about parenting, and the application in Chapter 3
"Brains, Bodies, and Behavior" is about the difficulties that left-handed people face
performing everyday tasks in a right-handed world.
3. Research focus. I have also emphasized empiricism throughout, but without making it a
distraction from the main story line. Each chapter presents two close-ups on research—
well-articulated and specific examples of research within the content area, each including
a summary of the hypotheses, methods, results, and interpretations. This feature provides
a continuous thread that reminds students of the importance of empirical research. The
research foci also emphasize the fact that findings are not always predictable ahead of
time (dispelling the myth of hindsight bias) and help students understand how research
really works.
`In short, I think that this book will provide a useful and productive synthesis between your goals
and the goals of your students. I have tried to focus on the forest rather than the trees and to bring
psychology to life—in ways that really matter—for the students. At the same time, the book
maintains content and conceptual rigor, with a strong focus on the fundamental principles of
empiricism and the scientific method.
Because we are frequently exposed to the work of psychologists in our everyday lives, we all
have an idea about what psychology is and what psychologists do. In many ways I am sure that
your conceptions are correct. Psychologists do work in forensic fields, and they do provide
counseling and therapy for people in distress. But there are hundreds of thousands of
psychologists in the world, and most of them work in other places, doing work that you are
probably not aware of.
Most psychologists work in research laboratories, hospitals, and other field settings where they
study the behavior of humans and animals. For instance, my colleagues in the Psychology
Department at the University of Maryland study such diverse topics as anxiety in children, the
interpretation of dreams, the effects of caffeine on thinking, how birds recognize each other, how
praying mantises hear, how people from different cultures react differently in negotiation, and
the factors that lead people to engage in terrorism. Other psychologists study such topics as
alcohol and drug addiction, memory, emotion, hypnosis, love, what makes people aggressive or
helpful, and the psychologies of politics, prejudice, culture, and religion. Psychologists also work
in schools and businesses, and they use a variety of methods, including observation,
questionnaires, interviews, and laboratory studies, to help them understand behavior.
1. Explain why using our intuition about everyday behavior is insufficient for a complete understanding of
2. Describe the difference between values and facts and explain how the scientific method is used to
Despite the differences in their interests, areas of study, and approaches, all psychologists have
one thing in common: They rely on scientific methods. Research psychologists use scientific
methods to create new knowledge about the causes of behavior, whereas psychologist-
practitioners, such as clinical, counseling, industrial-organizational, and school psychologists,
use existing research to enhance the everyday life of others. The science of psychology is
important for both researchers and practitioners.
In a sense all humans are scientists. We all have an interest in asking and answering questions
about our world. We want to know why things happen, when and if they are likely to happen
again, and how to reproduce or change them. Such knowledge enables us to predict our own
behavior and that of others. We may even collect data (i.e., any information collected through
formal observation or measurement) to aid us in this undertaking. It has been argued that people
are “everyday scientists” who conduct research projects to answer questions about behavior
(Nisbett & Ross, 1980). [1] When we perform poorly on an important test, we try to understand
what caused our failure to remember or understand the material and what might help us do better
the next time. When our good friends Monisha and Charlie break up, despite the fact that they
The results of these “everyday” research projects can teach us many principles of human
behavior. We learn through experience that if we give someone bad news, he or she may blame
us even though the news was not our fault. We learn that people may become depressed after
they fail at an important task. We see that aggressive behavior occurs frequently in our society,
and we develop theories to explain why this is so. These insights are part of everyday social life.
In fact, much research in psychology involves the scientific study of everyday behavior (Heider,
1958; Kelley, 1967). [2]
The problem, however, with the way people collect and interpret data in their everyday lives is
that they are not always particularly thorough. Often, when one explanation for an event seems
“right,” we adopt that explanation as the truth even when other explanations are possible and
potentially more accurate. For example, eyewitnesses to violent crimes are often extremely
confident in their identifications of the perpetrators of these crimes. But research finds that
eyewitnesses are no less confident in their identifications when they are incorrect than when they
are correct (Cutler & Wells, 2009; Wells & Hasel, 2008). [3] People may also become convinced
of the existence of extrasensory perception (ESP), or the predictive value of astrology, when
there is no evidence for either (Gilovich, 1993). [4] Furthermore, psychologists have also found
that there are a variety of cognitive and motivational biases that frequently influence our
perceptions and lead us to draw erroneous conclusions (Fiske & Taylor, 2007; Hsee & Hastie,
2006). [5] In summary, accepting explanations for events without testing them thoroughly may
lead us to think that we know the causes of things when we really do not.
research demonstrated that, at least under certain conditions (and although they do not know it), people
frequently prefer brand names that contain the letters of their own name to brand names that do not
The research participants were recruited in pairs and were told that the research was a taste test of
different types of tea. For each pair of participants, the experimenter created two teas and named them by
adding the word stem “oki” to the first three letters of each participant’s first name. For example, for
Jonathan and Elisabeth, the names of the teas would have been Jonoki and Elioki.
The participants were then shown 20 packets of tea that were supposedly being tested. Eighteen packets
were labeled with made-up Japanese names (e.g., “Mataku” or “Somuta”), and two were labeled with the
brand names constructed from the participants’ names. The experimenter explained that each participant
would taste only two teas and would be allowed to choose one packet of these two to take home.
One of the two participants was asked to draw slips of paper to select the two brands that would be tasted
at this session. However, the drawing was rigged so that the two brands containing the participants’ name
stems were always chosen for tasting. Then, while the teas were being brewed, the participants completed
a task designed to heighten their needs for self-esteem, and that was expected to increase their desire to
choose a brand that had the letters of their own name. Specifically, the participants all wrote about an
After the teas were ready, the participants tasted them and then chose to take a packet of one of the teas
home with them. After they made their choice, the participants were asked why they chose the tea they
had chosen, and then the true purpose of the study was explained to them.
The results of this study found that participants chose the tea that included the first three letters of their
own name significantly more frequently (64% of the time) than they chose the tea that included the first
unconsciously; the participants did not know why they chose the tea they chose. When they were asked,
more than 90% of the participants thought that they had chosen on the basis of taste, whereas only 5% of
them mentioned the real cause—that the brand name contained the letters of their name.
Once we learn about the outcome of a given event (e.g., when we read about the results of a
research project), we frequently believe that we would have been able to predict the outcome
ahead of time. For instance, if half of a class of students is told that research concerning
attraction between people has demonstrated that “opposites attract” and the other half is told that
research has demonstrated that “birds of a feather flock together,” most of the students will
report believing that the outcome that they just read about is true, and that they would have
predicted the outcome before they had read about it. Of course, both of these contradictory
outcomes cannot be true. (In fact, psychological research finds that “birds of a feather flock
together” is generally the case.) The problem is that just reading a description of research
findings leads us to think of the many cases we know that support the findings, and thus makes
them seem believable. The tendency to think that we could have predicted something that has
already occurred that we probably would not have been able to predict is called
the hindsight bias.
All scientists, whether they are physicists, chemists, biologists, sociologists, or psychologists,
use empirical methods to study the topics that interest them. Empirical methods include the
processes of collecting and organizing data and drawing conclusions about those data. The
empirical methods used by scientists have developed over many years and provide a basis for
collecting, analyzing, and interpreting data within a common framework in which information
can be shared. We can label the scientific method as the set of assumptions, rules, and
procedures that scientists use to conduct empirical research.
Although scientific research is an important method of studying human behavior, not all
questions can be answered using scientific approaches. Statements that cannot be objectively
measured or objectively determined to be true or false are not within the domain of scientific
Because values cannot be considered to be either true or false, science cannot prove or disprove
them. Nevertheless, as shown in Table 1.1 "Examples of Values and Facts in Scientific
Research", research can sometimes provide facts that can help people develop their values. For
instance, science may be able to objectively measure the impact of unwanted children on a
society or the psychological trauma suffered by women who have abortions. The effect of capital
punishment on the crime rate in the United States may also be determinable. This factual
information can and should be made available to help people formulate their values about
abortion and capital punishment, as well as to enable governments to articulate appropriate
policies. Values also frequently come into play in determining what research is appropriate or
important to conduct. For instance, the U.S. government has recently supported and provided
funding for research on HIV, AIDS, and terrorism, while denying funding for research using
human stem cells.
Welfare payments should be reduced for The U.S. government paid more than $21 billion in
unmarried parents. unemployment insurance in 2010.
It is important to quit smoking. Smoking increases the incidence of cancer and heart disease.
Source: Stangor, C. (2011). Research methods for the behavioral sciences (4th ed.). Mountain View, CA:
Cengage.
Although scientists use research to help establish facts, the distinction between values and facts
is not always clear-cut. Sometimes statements that scientists consider to be factual later, on the
basis of further research, turn out to be partially or even entirely incorrect. Although scientific
procedures do not necessarily guarantee that the answers to questions will be objective and
unbiased, science is still the best method for drawing objective conclusions about the world
around us. When old facts are discarded, they are replaced with new facts based on newer and
more correct data. Although science is not perfect, the requirements of empiricism and
objectivity result in a much greater chance of producing an accurate understanding of human
behavior than is available through other approaches.
The study of psychology spans many different topics at many different levels of explanation,
which are the perspectives that are used to understand behavior. Lower levels of explanation are
more closely tied to biological influences, such as genes, neurons, neurotransmitters, and
hormones, whereas the middle levels of explanation refer to the abilities and characteristics of
individual people, and the highest levels of explanation relate to social groups, organizations, and
cultures (Cacioppo, Berntson, Sheridan, & McClintock, 2000). [7]
Understanding and attempting to alleviate the costs of psychological disorders such as depression
is not easy, because psychological experiences are extremely complex. The questions
psychologists pose are as difficult as those posed by doctors, biologists, chemists, physicists, and
other scientists, if not more so (Wilson, 1998). [10]
Because of the many individual difference variables that influence behavior, we cannot always
predict who will become aggressive or who will perform best in graduate school or on the job.
The predictions made by psychologists (and most other scientists) are only probabilistic. We can
say, for instance, that people who score higher on an intelligence test will, on average, do better
Another reason that it is difficult to predict behavior is that almost all behavior is multiply
determined, or produced by many factors. And these factors occur at different levels of
explanation. We have seen, for instance, that depression is caused by lower-level genetic factors,
by medium-level personal factors, and by higher-level social and cultural factors. You should
always be skeptical about people who attempt to explain important human behaviors, such as
violence, child abuse, poverty, anxiety, or depression, in terms of a single cause.
Furthermore, these multiple causes are not independent of one another; they are associated such
that when one cause is present other causes tend to be present as well. This overlap makes it
difficult to pinpoint which cause or causes are operating. For instance, some people may be
depressed because of biological imbalances in neurotransmitters in their brain. The resulting
depression may lead them to act more negatively toward other people around them, which then
leads those other people to respond more negatively to them, which then increases their
depression. As a result, the biological determinants of depression become intertwined with the
social responses of other people, making it difficult to disentangle the effects of each cause.
Another difficulty in studying psychology is that much human behavior is caused by factors that
are outside our conscious awareness, making it impossible for us, as individuals, to really
understand them. The role of unconscious processes was emphasized in the theorizing of the
Austrian neurologist Sigmund Freud (1856–1939), who argued that many psychological
disorders were caused by memories that we have repressed and thus remain outside our
consciousness. Unconscious processes will be an important part of our study of psychology, and
we will see that current research has supported many of Freud’s ideas about the importance of
the unconscious in guiding behavior.
KEY TAKEAWAYS
• Though it is easy to think that everyday situations have commonsense answers, scientific studies have
found that people are not always as good at predicting outcomes as they think they are.
predicted.
• Psychologists use the scientific method to collect, analyze, and interpret evidence.
• Employing the scientific method allows the scientist to collect empirical data objectively, which adds to
• Psychological phenomena are complex, and making predictions about them is difficult because of
individual differences and because they are multiply determined at different levels of explanation.
1. Can you think of a time when you used your intuition to analyze an outcome, only to be surprised later to
find that your explanation was completely incorrect? Did this surprise help you understand how intuition
2. Describe the scientific method in a way that someone who knows nothing about science could
understand it.
3. Consider a behavior that you find to be important and think about its potential causes at different levels
[1] Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice Hall.
[2] Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in
social psychology. In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 15, pp. 192–240). Lincoln: University of Nebraska
Press.
[3] Cutler, B. L., & Wells, G. L. (2009). Expert testimony regarding eyewitness identification. In J. L. Skeem, S. O. Lilienfeld, & K. S.
Douglas (Eds.), Psychological science in the courtroom: Consensus and controversy (pp. 100–123). New York, NY: Guilford Press;
Wells, G. L., & Hasel, L. E. (2008). Eyewitness identification: Issues in common knowledge and generalization. In E. Borgida & S.
T. Fiske (Eds.), Beyond common sense: Psychological science in the courtroom (pp. 159–176). Malden, NJ: Blackwell.
[4] Gilovich, T. (1993). How we know what isn’t so: The fallibility of human reason in everyday life. New York, NY: Free Press.
[5] Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (2007). Social cognition: From brains to culture. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.; Hsee, C. K., & Hastie,
R. (2006). Decision and experience: Why don’t we choose what makes us happy? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(1), 31–37.
product specific needs are active. Journal of Consumer Research, 32(3), 405–415.
[7] Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Sheridan, J. F., & McClintock, M. K. (2000). Multilevel integrative analyses of human
behavior: Social neuroscience and the complementing nature of social and biological approaches. Psychological Bulletin, 126(6),
829–843.
[8] Williams, N., Simpson, A. N., Simpson, K., & Nahas, Z. (2009). Relapse rates with long-term antidepressant drug therapy: A
[9] Chen, P.-Y., Wang, S.-C., Poland, R. E., & Lin, K.-M. (2009). Biological variations in depression and anxiety between East and
West. CNS Neuroscience & Therapeutics, 15(3), 283–294; Seedat, S., Scott, K. M., Angermeyer, M. C., Berglund, P., Bromet, E. J.,
Brugha, T. S.,…Kessler, R. C. (2009). Cross-national associations between gender and mental disorders in the World Health
Organization World Mental Health Surveys. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66(7), 785–795.
[10] Wilson, E. O. (1998). Consilience: The unity of knowledge. New York, NY: Vintage Books.
3. Outline the basic schools of psychology and how each school has contributed to psychology.
In this section we will review the history of psychology with a focus on the important questions
that psychologists ask and the major approaches (or schools) of psychological inquiry. The
schools of psychology that we will review are summarized in Table 1.2 "The Most Important
Approaches (Schools) of Psychology", and Figure 1.5 "Timeline Showing Some of the Most
Important Psychologists" presents a timeline of some of the most important psychologists,
beginning with the early Greek philosophers and extending to the present day. Table 1.2 "The
Most Important Approaches (Schools) of Psychology" and Figure 1.5 "Timeline Showing Some
of the Most Important Psychologists" both represent a selection of the most important schools
and people; to mention all the approaches and all the psychologists who have contributed to the
field is not possible in one chapter.
Uses the method of introspection to identify the basic elements or Wilhelm Wundt, Edward B.
Structuralism “structures” of psychological experience Titchener
Focuses on the role of our unconscious thoughts, feelings, and Sigmund Freud, Carl Jung,
Psychodynamic memories and our early childhood experiences in determining behavior Alfred Adler, Erik Erickson
The study of mental processes, including perception, thinking, Hermann Ebbinghaus, Sir
Cognitive memory, and judgments Frederic Bartlett, Jean Piaget
The study of how the social situations and the cultures in which people Fritz Heider, Leon Festinger,
Social-cultural find themselves influence thinking and behavior Stanley Schachter
• Nature versus nurture. Are genes or environment most influential in determining the
behavior of individuals and in accounting for differences among people? Most scientists
now agree that both genes and environment play crucial roles in most human behaviors,
and yet we still have much to learn about how nature (our biological makeup) and nurture
(the experiences that we have during our lives) work together (Harris, 1998; Pinker,
2002). [2] The proportion of the observed differences on characteristics among people
(e.g., in terms of their height, intelligence, or optimism) that is due to genetics is known
as the heritability of the characteristic, and we will make much use of this term in the
chapters to come. We will see, for example, that the heritability of intelligence is very
high (about .85 out of 1.0) and that the heritability of extraversion is about .50. But we
will also see that nature and nurture interact in complex ways, making the question of “Is
it nature or is it nurture?” very difficult to answer.
• Free will versus determinism. This question concerns the extent to which people have
control over their own actions. Are we the products of our environment, guided by forces
out of our control, or are we able to choose the behaviors we engage in? Most of us like
to believe in free will, that we are able to do what we want—for instance, that we could
get up right now and go fishing. And our legal system is premised on the concept of free
will; we punish criminals because we believe that they have choice over their behaviors
and freely choose to disobey the law. But as we will discuss later in the research focus in
this section, recent research has suggested that we may have less control over our own
behavior than we think we do (Wegner, 2002). [3]
• Accuracy versus inaccuracy. To what extent are humans good information processors?
Although it appears that people are “good enough” to make sense of the world around
them and to make decent decisions (Fiske, 2003),[4] they are far from perfect. Human
judgment is sometimes compromised by inaccuracies in our thinking styles and by our
motivations and emotions. For instance, our judgment may be affected by our desires to
Early Psychologists
The earliest psychologists that we know about are the Greek philosophers Plato (428–347 BC)
and Aristotle (384–322 BC). These philosophers asked many of the same questions that today’s
psychologists ask; for instance, they questioned the distinction between nature and nurture and
the existence of free will. In terms of the former, Plato argued on the nature side, believing that
certain kinds of knowledge are innate or inborn, whereas Aristotle was more on the nurture side,
believing that each child is born as an “empty slate” (in Latin atabula rasa) and that knowledge
is primarily acquired through learning and experience.
European philosophers continued to ask these fundamental questions during the Renaissance. For
instance, the French philosopher René Descartes (1596–1650) also considered the issue of free
will, arguing in its favor and believing that the mind controls the body through the pineal gland
in the brain (an idea that made some sense at the time but was later proved incorrect). Descartes
also believed in the existence of innate natural abilities. A scientist as well as a philosopher,
Descartes dissected animals and was among the first to understand that the nerves controlled the
muscles. He also addressed the relationship between mind (the mental aspects of life) and body
The fundamental problem that these philosophers faced was that they had few methods for
settling their claims. Most philosophers didn’t conduct any research on these questions, in part
because they didn’t yet know how to do it, and in part because they weren’t sure it was even
possible to objectively study human experience. But dramatic changes came during the 1800s
with the help of the first two research psychologists: the German psychologist Wilhelm Wundt
(1832–1920), who developed a psychology laboratory in Leipzig, Germany, and the American
psychologist William James (1842–1910), who founded a psychology laboratory at Harvard
University.
Wundt’s research in his laboratory in Liepzig focused on the nature of consciousness itself.
Wundt and his students believed that it was possible to analyze the basic elements of the mind
and to classify our conscious experiences scientifically. Wundt began the field known
as structuralism, a school of psychology whose goal was to identify the basic elements or
“structures” of psychological experience. Its goal was to create a “periodic table” of the
“elements of sensations,” similar to the periodic table of elements that had recently been created
in chemistry.
Structuralists used the method of introspection to attempt to create a map of the elements of
consciousness. Introspection involves asking research participants to describe exactly what they
experience as they work on mental tasks, such as viewing colors, reading a page in a book, or
performing a math problem. A participant who is reading a book might report, for instance, that
he saw some black and colored straight and curved marks on a white background. In other
studies the structuralists used newly invented reaction time instruments to systematically assess
not only what the participants were thinking but how long it took them to do so. Wundt
discovered that it took people longer to report what sound they had just heard than to simply
Perhaps the best known of the structuralists was Edward Bradford Titchener (1867–1927).
Titchener was a student of Wundt who came to the United States in the late 1800s and founded a
laboratory at Cornell University. In his research using introspection, Titchener and his students
claimed to have identified more than 40,000 sensations, including those relating to vision,
hearing, and taste.
An important aspect of the structuralist approach was that it was rigorous and scientific. The
research marked the beginning of psychology as a science, because it demonstrated that mental
events could be quantified. But the structuralists also discovered the limitations of introspection.
Even highly trained research participants were often unable to report on their subjective
experiences. When the participants were asked to do simple math problems, they could easily do
them, but they could not easily answer how they did them. Thus the structuralists were the first to
realize the importance of unconscious processes—that many important aspects of human
psychology occur outside our conscious awareness, and that psychologists cannot expect
research participants to be able to accurately report on all of their experiences.
In contrast to Wundt, who attempted to understand the nature of consciousness, the goal of
William James and the other members of the school of functionalism was to understand why
animals and humans have developed the particular psychological aspects that they currently
possess(Hunt, 1993). [5] For James, one’s thinking was relevant only to one’s behavior. As he put
it in his psychology textbook, “My thinking is first and last and always for the sake of my doing”
(James, 1890). [6]
James and the other members of the functionalist school were influenced by Charles Darwin’s
(1809–1882) theory of natural selection, which proposed that the physical characteristics of
animals and humans evolved because they were useful, or functional. The functionalists believed
Although functionalism no longer exists as a school of psychology, its basic principles have been
absorbed into psychology and continue to influence it in many ways. The work of the
functionalists has developed into the field ofevolutionary psychology, a branch of psychology
that applies the Darwinian theory of natural selection to human and animal behavior(Dennett,
1995; Tooby & Cosmides, 1992). [7]Evolutionary psychology accepts the functionalists’ basic
assumption, namely that many human psychological systems, including memory, emotion, and
personality, serve key adaptive functions. As we will see in the chapters to come, evolutionary
psychologists use evolutionary theory to understand many different behaviors including romantic
attraction, stereotypes and prejudice, and even the causes of many psychological disorders.
A key component of the ideas of evolutionary psychology is fitness. Fitness refers to the extent to
which having a given characteristic helps the individual organism survive and reproduce at a
higher rate than do other members of the species who do not have the characteristic. Fitter
organisms pass on their genes more successfully to later generations, making the characteristics
that produce fitness more likely to become part of the organism’s nature than characteristics that
do not produce fitness. For example, it has been argued that the emotion of jealousy has survived
over time in men because men who experience jealousy are more fit than men who do not.
According to this idea, the experience of jealously leads men to be more likely to protect their
mates and guard against rivals, which increases their reproductive success (Buss, 2000). [8]
Despite its importance in psychological theorizing, evolutionary psychology also has some
limitations. One problem is that many of its predictions are extremely difficult to test. Unlike the
fossils that are used to learn about the physical evolution of species, we cannot know which
psychological characteristics our ancestors possessed or did not possess; we can only make
guesses about this. Because it is difficult to directly test evolutionary theories, it is always
possible that the explanations we apply are made up after the fact to account for observed data
(Gould & Lewontin, 1979). [9] Nevertheless, the evolutionary approach is important to
Psychodynamic Psychology
Perhaps the school of psychology that is most familiar to the general public is the psychodynamic
approach to understanding behavior, which was championed by Sigmund Freud (1856–1939)
and his followers. Psychodynamic psychology is an approach to understanding human behavior
that focuses on the role of unconscious thoughts, feelings, and memories. Freud developed his
theories about behavior through extensive analysis of the patients that he treated in his private
clinical practice. Freud believed that many of the problems that his patients experienced,
including anxiety, depression, and sexual dysfunction, were the result of the effects of painful
childhood experiences that the person could no longer remember.
Freud’s ideas were extended by other psychologists whom he influenced, including Carl Jung
(1875–1961), Alfred Adler (1870–1937), Karen Horney (1855–1952), and Erik Erikson (1902–
1994). These and others who follow the psychodynamic approach believe that it is possible to
help the patient if the unconscious drives can be remembered, particularly through a deep and
thorough exploration of the person’s early sexual experiences and current sexual desires. These
explorations are revealed through talk therapy and dream analysis, in a process
called psychoanalysis.
The founders of the school of psychodynamics were primarily practitioners who worked with
individuals to help them understand and confront their psychological symptoms. Although they
did not conduct much research on their ideas, and although later, more sophisticated tests of their
theories have not always supported their proposals, psychodynamics has nevertheless had
substantial impact on the field of psychology, and indeed on thinking about human behavior
more generally (Moore & Fine, 1995). [10] The importance of the unconscious in human
behavior, the idea that early childhood experiences are critical, and the concept of therapy as a
way of improving human lives are all ideas that are derived from the psychodynamic approach
and that remain central to psychology.
Although they differed in approach, both structuralism and functionalism were essentially studies
of the mind. The psychologists associated with the school of behaviorism, on the other hand,
were reacting in part to the difficulties psychologists encountered when they tried to use
introspection to understand behavior. Behaviorism is a school of psychology that is based on the
premise that it is not possible to objectively study the mind, and therefore that psychologists
should limit their attention to the study of behavior itself. Behaviorists believe that the human
mind is a “black box” into which stimuli are sent and from which responses are received. They
argue that there is no point in trying to determine what happens in the box because we can
successfully predict behavior without knowing what happens inside the mind. Furthermore,
behaviorists believe that it is possible to develop laws of learning that can explain all behaviors.
The first behaviorist was the American psychologist John B. Watson (1878–1958). Watson was
influenced in large part by the work of the Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov (1849–1936), who
had discovered that dogs would salivate at the sound of a tone that had previously been
associated with the presentation of food. Watson and the other behaviorists began to use these
ideas to explain how events that people and other organisms experienced in their environment
(stimuli) could produce specific behaviors (responses). For instance, in Pavlov’s research
the stimulus (either the food or, after learning, the tone) would produce the response of salivation
in the dogs.
In his research Watson found that systematically exposing a child to fearful stimuli in the
presence of objects that did not themselves elicit fear could lead the child to respond with a
fearful behavior to the presence of the stimulus (Watson & Rayner, 1920; Beck, Levinson, &
Irons, 2009). [11] In the best known of his studies, an 8-month-old boy named Little Albert was
used as the subject. Here is a summary of the findings:
The boy was placed in the middle of a room; a white laboratory rat was placed near him and he
was allowed to play with it. The child showed no fear of the rat. In later trials, the researchers
made a loud sound behind Albert’s back by striking a steel bar with a hammer whenever the
baby touched the rat. The child cried when he heard the noise. After several such pairings of the
In line with the behaviorist approach, the boy had learned to associate the white rat with the loud
noise, resulting in crying.
The most famous behaviorist was Burrhus Frederick (B. F.) Skinner (1904–1990), who expanded
the principles of behaviorism and also brought them to the attention of the public at large.
Skinner used the ideas of stimulus and response, along with the application of rewards
or reinforcements, to train pigeons and other animals. And he used the general principles of
behaviorism to develop theories about how best to teach children and how to create societies that
were peaceful and productive. Skinner even developed a method for studying thoughts and
feelings using the behaviorist approach (Skinner, 1957, 1968, 1972). [12]
nurture and about free will. In terms of the nature-nurture debate, the behaviorists agreed with the nurture approach,
believing that we are shaped exclusively by our environments. They also argued that there is no free will, but rather
that our behaviors are determined by the events that we have experienced in our past. In short, this approach argues
that organisms, including humans, are a lot like puppets in a show who don’t realize that other people are controlling
them. Furthermore, although we do not cause our own actions, we nevertheless believe that we do because we don’t
Recent research in psychology has suggested that Skinner and the behaviorists might well have been right, at least in
the sense that we overestimate our own free will in responding to the events around us (Libet, 1985; Matsuhashi &
[13]
Hallett, 2008; Wegner, 2002). In one demonstration of the misperception of our own free will, neuroscientists
[14]
Soon, Brass, Heinze, and Haynes (2008) placed their research participants in a functional magnetic resonance
imaging (fMRI) brain scanner while they presented them with a series of letters on a computer screen. The letter on
the screen changed every one-half second. The participants were asked, whenever they decided to, to press either of
two buttons. Then they were asked to indicate which letter was showing on the screen when they decided to press the
button. The researchers analyzed the brain images to see if they could predict which of the two buttons the participant
was going to press, even before the letter at which he or she had indicated the decision to press a button. Suggesting
found that the prefrontal cortex region of the brain showed activation that could be used to predict the button press as
long as 10 seconds before the participants said that they decided which button to press.
Research has found that we are more likely to think that we control our behavior when the desire to act occurs
immediately prior to the outcome, when the thought is consistent with the outcome, and when there are no other
[15]
apparent causes for the behavior. Aarts, Custers, and Wegner (2005) asked their research participants to control a
rapidly moving square along with a computer that was also controlling the square independently. The participants
pressed a button to stop the movement. When participants were exposed to words related to the location of the square
just before they stopped its movement, they became more likely to think that they controlled the motion, even when it
[16]
was actually the computer that stopped it. And Dijksterhuis, Preston, Wegner, and Aarts (2008) found that
participants who had just been exposed to first-person singular pronouns, such as “I” and “me,” were more likely to
believe that they controlled their actions than were people who had seen the words “computer” or “God.”
The idea that we are more likely to take ownership for our actions in some cases than in others is also seen in our
attributions for success and failure. Because we normally expect that our behaviors will be met with success, when we
are successful we easily believe that the success is the result of our own free will. When an action is met with failure,
on the other hand, we are less likely to perceive this outcome as the result of our free will, and we are more likely to
[17]
blame the outcome on luck or our teacher (Wegner, 2003).
Science is always influenced by the technology that surrounds it, and psychology is no
exception. Thus it is no surprise that beginning in the 1960s, growing numbers of psychologists
began to think about the brain and about human behavior in terms of the computer, which was
being developed and becoming publicly available at that time. The analogy between the brain
Although cognitive psychology began in earnest in the 1960s, earlier psychologists had also
taken a cognitive orientation. Some of the important contributors to cognitive psychology
include the German psychologist Hermann Ebbinghaus (1850–1909), who studied the ability of
people to remember lists of words under different conditions, and the English psychologist Sir
Frederic Bartlett (1886–1969), who studied the cognitive and social processes of remembering.
Bartlett created short stories that were in some ways logical but also contained some very
unusual and unexpected events. Bartlett discovered that people found it very difficult to recall
the stories exactly, even after being allowed to study them repeatedly, and he hypothesized that
the stories were difficult to remember because they did not fit the participants’ expectations
about how stories should go. The idea that our memory is influenced by what we already know
was also a major idea behind the cognitive-developmental stage model of Swiss psychologist
Jean Piaget (1896–1980). Other important cognitive psychologists include Donald E. Broadbent
(1926–1993), Daniel Kahneman (1934–), George Miller (1920–), Eleanor Rosch (1938–), and
Amos Tversky (1937–1996).
Bartlett found that even when his British research participants were allowed to read the story many times they still
could not remember it well, and he believed this was because it did not fit with their prior knowledge.
One night two young men from Egulac went down to the river to hunt seals and while they were there it
became foggy and calm. Then they heard war-cries, and they thought: “Maybe this is a war-party.” They
escaped to the shore, and hid behind a log. Now canoes came up, and they heard the noise of paddles, and
saw one canoe coming up to them. There were five men in the canoe, and they said:
“What do you think? We wish to take you along. We are going up the river to make war on the people.”
So one of the young men went, but the other returned home.
And the warriors went on up the river to a town on the other side of Kalama. The people came down to the
water and they began to fight, and many were killed. But presently the young man heard one of the warriors
say, “Quick, let us go home: that Indian has been hit.” Now he thought: “Oh, they are ghosts.” He did not feel
So the canoes went back to Egulac and the young man went ashore to his house and made a fire. And he told
everybody and said: “Behold I accompanied the ghosts, and we went to fight. Many of our fellows were
killed, and many of those who attacked us were killed. They said I was hit, and I did not feel sick.”
He told it all, and then he became quiet. When the sun rose he fell down. Something black came out of his
mouth. His face became contorted. The people jumped up and cried.
[18]
He was dead. (Bartlett, 1932)
In its argument that our thinking has a powerful influence on behavior, the cognitive approach
provided a distinct alternative to behaviorism. According to cognitive psychologists, ignoring the
mind itself will never be sufficient because people interpret the stimuli that they experience. For
instance, when a boy turns to a girl on a date and says, “You are so beautiful,” a behaviorist
would probably see that as a reinforcing (positive) stimulus. And yet the girl might not be so
easily fooled. She might try to understand why the boy is making this particular statement at this
particular time and wonder if he might be attempting to influence her through the comment.
Cognitive psychologists maintain that when we take into consideration how stimuli are evaluated
and interpreted, we understand behavior more deeply.
Cognitive psychology remains enormously influential today, and it has guided research in such
varied fields as language, problem solving, memory, intelligence, education, human
development, social psychology, and psychotherapy. The cognitive revolution has been given
even more life over the past decade as the result of recent advances in our ability to see the brain
in action using neuroimaging techniques. Neuroimaging is the use of various techniques to
provide pictures of the structure and function of the living brain (Ilardi & Feldman,
2001). [19] These images are used to diagnose brain disease and injury, but they also allow
Social-Cultural Psychology
A final school, which takes a higher level of analysis and which has had substantial impact on
psychology, can be broadly referred to as the social-cultural approach. The field of social-
cultural psychology is the study of how the social situations and the cultures in which people find
themselves influence thinking and behavior. Social-cultural psychologists are particularly
concerned with how people perceive themselves and others, and how people influence each
other’s behavior. For instance, social psychologists have found that we are attracted to others
who are similar to us in terms of attitudes and interests (Byrne, 1969), [20] that we develop our
own beliefs and attitudes by comparing our opinions to those of others (Festinger, 1954), [21] and
that we frequently change our beliefs and behaviors to be similar to those of the people we care
about—a process known as conformity.
Norms in the East Asian culture, on the other hand, are oriented toward interdependence
or collectivism. In these cultures children are taught to focus on developing harmonious social
relationships with others. The predominant norms relate to group togetherness and
connectedness, and duty and responsibility to one’s family and other groups. When asked to
describe themselves, the members of East Asian cultures are more likely than those from
Western cultures to indicate that they are particularly concerned about the interests of others,
including their close friends and their colleagues.
Another important cultural difference is the extent to which people in different cultures are
bound by social norms and customs, rather than being free to express their own individuality
without considering social norms (Chan, Gelfand, Triandis, & Tzeng, 1996). [25] Cultures also
differ in terms of personal space, such as how closely individuals stand to each other when
talking, as well as the communication styles they employ.
It is important to be aware of cultures and cultural differences because people with different
cultural backgrounds increasingly come into contact with each other as a result of increased
travel and immigration and the development of the Internet and other forms of communication.
In the United States, for instance, there are many different ethnic groups, and the proportion of
the population that comes from minority (non-White) groups is increasing from year to year. The
social-cultural approach to understanding behavior reminds us again of the difficulty of making
Psychology is not one discipline but rather a collection of many subdisciplines that all share at
least some common approaches and that work together and exchange knowledge to form a
coherent discipline (Yang & Chiu, 2009). [26]Because the field of psychology is so broad,
students may wonder which areas are most suitable for their interests and which types of careers
might be available to them. Table 1.3 "Some Career Paths in Psychology" will help you consider
the answers to these questions. You can learn more about these different fields of psychology
and the careers associated with them at http://www.apa.org/careers/psyccareers/.
These psychologists study people and Most work in academic settings, but the skills of
the differences among them. The goal is personality psychologists are also in demand in business—
to develop theories that explain the for instance, in advertising and marketing. PhD programs
Personality psychological processes of individuals, in personality psychology are often connected with
psychology and to focus on individual differences. programs in social psychology.
learning and study skills. Psychological research has provided a substantial amount of knowledge about the principles
of learning and memory. This information can help you do better in this and other courses, and can also help you
better learn new concepts and techniques in other areas of your life.
The most important thing you can learn in college is how to better study, learn, and remember. These skills will help
you throughout your life, as you learn new jobs and take on other responsibilities. There are substantial individual
differences in learning and memory, such that some people learn faster than others. But even if it takes you longer to
learn than you think it should, the extra time you put into studying is well worth the effort. And you can learn to
learn—learning to effectively study and to remember information is just like learning any other skill, such as playing a
To learn well, you need to be ready to learn. You cannot learn well when you are tired, when you are under stress, or if
you are abusing alcohol or drugs. Try to keep a consistent routine of sleeping and eating. Eat moderately and
nutritiously, and avoid drugs that can impair memory, particularly alcohol. There is no evidence that stimulants such
as caffeine, amphetamines, or any of the many “memory enhancing drugs” on the market will help you learn (Gold,
[27]
Cahill, & Wenk, 2002; McDaniel, Maier, & Einstein, 2002). Memory supplements are usually no more effective
than drinking a can of sugared soda, which also releases glucose and thus improves memory slightly.
Psychologists have studied the ways that best allow people to acquire new information, to retain it over time, and to
retrieve information that has been stored in our memories. One important finding is that learning is an active process.
To acquire information most effectively, we must actively manipulate it. One active approach is rehearsal—repeating
the information that is to be learned over and over again. Although simple repetition does help us learn, psychological
research has found that we acquire information most effectively when we actively think about or elaborate on its
When you study, try to elaborate by connecting the information to other things that you already know. If you want to
remember the different schools of psychology, for instance, try to think about how each of the approaches is different
one and then relate it to the features of the other approaches. In an important study showing the effectiveness of
[28]
elaborative encoding, Rogers, Kuiper, and Kirker (1977) found that students learned information best when they
related it to aspects of themselves (a phenomenon known as the self-reference effect). This research suggests that
imagining how the material relates to your own interests and goals will help you learn it.
An approach known as the method of loci involves linking each of the pieces of information that you need to
remember to places that you are familiar with. You might think about the house that you grew up in and the rooms in
it. Then you could put the behaviorists in the bedroom, the structuralists in the living room, and the functionalists in
the kitchen. Then when you need to remember the information, you retrieve the mental image of your house and
One of the most fundamental principles of learning is known as the spacing effect. Both humans and animals more
easily remember or learn material when they study the material in several shorter study periods over a longer period
of time, rather than studying it just once for a long period of time. Cramming for an exam is a particularly ineffective
way to learn.
Psychologists have also found that performance is improved when people set difficult yet realistic goals for themselves
[29]
(Locke & Latham, 2006). You can use this knowledge to help you learn. Set realistic goals for the time you are going
to spend studying and what you are going to learn, and try to stick to those goals. Do a small amount every day, and
Our ability to adequately assess our own knowledge is known asmetacognition. Research suggests that our
metacognition may make us overconfident, leading us to believe that we have learned material even when we have
not. To counteract this problem, don’t just go over your notes again and again. Instead, make a list of questions and
then see if you can answer them. Study the information again and then test yourself again after a few minutes. If you
made any mistakes, study again. Then wait for a half hour and test yourself again. Then test again after 1 day and after
2 days. Testing yourself by attempting to retrieve information in an active manner is better than simply studying the
material because it will help you determine if you really know it.
In summary, everyone can learn to learn better. Learning is an important skill, and following the previously
• The first psychologists were philosophers, but the field became more empirical and objective as more sophisticated
• Some basic questions asked by psychologists include those about nature versus nurture, free will versus determinism,
• The functionalists based their ideas on the work of Darwin, and their approaches led to the field of evolutionary
psychology.
• The behaviorists explained behavior in terms of stimulus, response, and reinforcement, while denying the presence of
free will.
• Cognitive psychologists study how people perceive, process, and remember information.
• Psychodynamic psychology focuses on unconscious drives and the potential to improve lives through psychoanalysis
and psychotherapy.
• The social-cultural approach focuses on the social situation, including how cultures and social norms influence our
behavior.
EXERCISES AND CRITICAL THINKING
1. What type of questions can psychologists answer that philosophers might not be able to answer as completely or as
accurately? Explain why you think psychologists can answer these questions better than philosophers can.
2. Choose one of the major questions of psychology and provide some evidence from your own experience that supports
3. Choose two of the fields of psychology discussed in this section and explain how they differ in their approaches to
understanding behavior and the level of explanation at which they are focused.
[1] Benjamin, L. T., Jr., & Baker, D. B. (2004). From seance to science: A history of the profession of psychology in America.
[2] Harris, J. (1998). The nurture assumption: Why children turn out the way they do. New York, NY: Touchstone Books; Pinker,
S. (2002). The blank slate: The modern denial of human nature. New York, NY: Penguin Putnam.
[3] Wegner, D. M. (2002). The illusion of conscious will. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
[4] Fiske, S. T. (2003). Social beings. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
[5] Hunt, M. (1993). The story of psychology. New York, NY: Anchor Books.
[7] Dennett, D. (1995). Darwin’s dangerous idea: Evolution and the meanings of life. New York, NY: Simon and Schuster; Tooby,
J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In J. H. Barkow & L. Cosmides (Eds.), The adapted mind:
Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (p. 666). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
[8] Buss, D. M. (2000). The dangerous passion: Why jealousy is as necessary as love and sex. New York, NY: Free Press.
[9] Gould, S. J., & Lewontin, R. C. (1979). The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: A critique of the
adaptationist programme. In Proceedings of the Royal Society of London (Series B, Vol. 205, pp. 581–598).
[10] Moore, B. E., & Fine, B. D. (1995). Psychoanalysis: The major concepts. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
[11] Watson, J. B., Rayner, R. (1920). Conditioned emotional reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 3(1), 1–14; Beck, H.
P., Levinson, S., & Irons, G. (2009). Finding Little Albert: A journey to John B. Watson’s infant laboratory. American Psychologist,
64(7), 605–614.
[12] Skinner, B. (1957). Verbal behavior. Acton, MA: Copley; Skinner, B. (1968). The technology of teaching. New York, NY:
Appleton-Century-Crofts; Skinner, B. (1972). Beyond freedom and dignity. New York, NY: Vintage Books.
[13] Libet, B. (1985). Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role of conscious will in voluntary action. Behavioral and Brain
Sciences, 8(4), 529–566; Matsuhashi, M., & Hallett, M. (2008). The timing of the conscious intention to move. European Journal
of Neuroscience, 28(11), 2344–2351; Wegner, D. M. (2002). The illusion of conscious will. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
[14] Soon, C. S., Brass, M., Heinze, H.-J., & Haynes, J.-D. (2008). Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human
[15] Aarts, H., Custers, R., & Wegner, D. M. (2005). On the inference of personal authorship: Enhancing experienced agency by
priming effect information. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 14(3), 439–458.
[16] Dijksterhuis, A., Preston, J., Wegner, D. M., & Aarts, H. (2008). Effects of subliminal priming of self and God on self-
attribution of authorship for events. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(1), 2–9.
[17] Wegner, D. M. (2003). The mind’s best trick: How we experience conscious will. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(2), 65–69.
[19] Ilardi, S. S., & Feldman, D. (2001). The cognitive neuroscience paradigm: A unifying metatheoretical framework for the
science and practice of clinical psychology. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 57(9), 1067–1088.
[20] Byrne, D. (1969). Attitudes and attraction. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 35–
[21] Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117–140.
[23] Fiske, A., Kitayama, S., Markus, H., & Nisbett, R. (1998). The cultural matrix of social psychology. In D. Gilbert, S. Fiske, & G.
Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology(4th ed., pp. 915–981). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill; Markus, H. R., Kitayama,
S., & Heiman, R. J. (1996). Culture and “basic” psychological principles. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social
psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 857–913). New York, NY: Guilford Press; Matsumoto, D. (Ed.). (2001). The
handbook of culture and psychology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
[24] Mesoudi, A. (2009). How cultural evolutionary theory can inform social psychology and vice versa. Psychological Review,
116(4), 929–952.
[25] Chan, D. K. S., Gelfand, M. J., Triandis, H. C., & Tzeng, O. (1996). Tightness-looseness revisited: Some preliminary analyses
in Japan and the United States. International Journal of Psychology, 31, 1–12.
[26] Yang, Y.-J., & Chiu, C.-Y. (2009). Mapping the structure and dynamics of psychological knowledge: Forty years of APA
[27] Gold, P. E., Cahill, L., & Wenk, G. L. (2002). Ginkgo biloba: A cognitive enhancer?Psychological Science in the Public Interest,
3(1), 2–11; McDaniel, M. A., Maier, S. F., & Einstein, G. O. (2002). “Brain-specific” nutrients: A memory cure? Psychological
[28] Rogers, T. B., Kuiper, N. A., & Kirker, W. S. (1977). Self-reference and the encoding of personal information. Journal of
[29] Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2006). New directions in goal-setting theory. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
15(5), 265–268
Psychology is the scientific study of mind and behavior. Most psychologists work in research
laboratories, hospitals, and other field settings where they study the behavior of humans and
animals. Some psychologists are researchers and others are practitioners, but all psychologists
use scientific methods to inform their work.
Although it is easy to think that everyday situations have commonsense answers, scientific
studies have found that people are not always as good at predicting outcomes as they often think