Forensic Psychology
Forensic Psychology
Acquisition
Research has also considered the effect of particular types of crime. For example, can
witnesses to a violent crime be as accurate as witnesses to a non-violent crime? Controlled
experimental studies, typically during which witnesses see videotaped crimes of varying
degrees of violence, suggest that violence results in poorer witness accuracy (Clifford &
Hollin, 1981). But strangely, field studies of real-life witnesses suggest that those who are
exposed to highly violent events can give very accurate testimony (Yuille & Cutshall, 1986).
Indeed, adult victims of rape usually give a reasonably accurate account of this extreme
personal experience of violence. (Koss, Tromp & Tharan, 1995).
Retention
During the retention stage, witness memory may be subject to various influences, such as
discussion with other witnesses and exposure to media accounts of the crime, not to mention
the fact that memory becomes less accurate over time. So the time interval between
acquisition and retrieval is an obvious consideration. Several studies have compared the
accuracy of eyewitness face identification over short and long time intervals. Malpass and
Devine (1981), for example, chose short (three-day) and long (five-month) intervals. It was
found that after three days there were no false identifications, but after five months the rate of
false identifications had risen. Conversely, the rate of correct identifications was initially high
but fell significantly after five months.
Retrieval
Finally, during the retrieval stage, factors that potentially influence the accuracy of
eyewitness testimony include an interview style and the use of aids to recall, such as the
photofit and identity parades. Studies of the impact of leading questions show that even
subtle changes in question-wording can influence testimony. For example, Loftus and Palmer
(1974) asked witnesses to a filmed traffic accident to estimate the speed of the cars when
‘they – into each other’: for different groups of witnesses the blank read ‘contacted’, ‘hit’,
‘bumped’, ‘collided’ or ‘smashed’. The witnesses’ estimates of the speed increased according
to the level of force implied by the verb contained in the question. Leading questions contain
information (either intentionally or unintentionally) that can bias the respondent’s reply
Confession in Psychotherapy
La Barre (1964) found that many natives of North and South America believed that physical
and mental health required purity, which in turn required the exposure of misdeeds—often
through elaborate confession ceremonies involving shamans and witch doctors. Similar
notions have permeated Western medicine, as when Breuer and Freud (1895/1955) observed
from psychotherapy sessions that patients often felt better after purging the mind of material
buried beneath consciousness. This discovery spawned Freudian psychoanalysis, the first
systematic ‘‘talking cure,’’ and now forms the basis for most modern psychotherapies and
social support groups. Recent research confirms the healing power of opening up about one’s
problems, traumas, and transgressions. In a series of controlled experiments, Pennebaker
(1997, 2002) and other investigators had research subjects talk into a tape recorder or write
either about past traumas or about trivial daily events. While speaking or writing, subjects in
the trauma group were physiologically aroused and upset. Many tearfully recounted deaths,
accidents, failures, personal wrongdoings, and instances of physical or sexual abuse. Soon,
however, these subjects felt better. Although systolic blood pressure levels rose during the
disclosures, they later dipped below pre-experiment levels. Moreover, these subjects
exhibited a decline in doctor visits over the next 6 months.
Interrogational tactics
The laws relating to the conduct of police interrogation of suspects vary from country to
country. But some psychological principles can be applied whenever one person is seeking
information from another, irrespective of location
Suspects may spend time isolated in police cells before and during interrogation, an
experience that can be frightening and stressful (Irving, 1986). For some, this situation may
create psychological distress or exacerbate existing psychological and emotional conditions.
Police interrogation manuals from both Britain (Walkley, 1987) and America (Inbau, Reid,
& Buckley, 1986) tell us that, from a policy perspective, the interrogator must overcome the
suspect’s natural resistance, to tell the truth, and so must be skilled in the use of strategies to
persuade the suspect to confess. These interrogational tactics, based on the social psychology
of conformity, obedience, and persuasion increase the pressure on suspects so that they will
fall into line with the
OFFENDER PROFILING
Offender profiling means constructing a picture of an offender’s characteristics from their
modus operandi together with the clues left at the crime scene. Offender profiling would be
high on the list of most forensic psychologists as our knowledge base increases, it is likely
that the technique will become increasingly sophisticated (Ainsworth, 2001; Jackson &
Bekerian, 1997). Turvey (2000) distinguishes inductive and deductive methods of profiling.
Inductive methods rely on the expert skills and knowledge of the profiler – a method often
referred to as ‘clinical’ in style. Deductive methods rely on forensic evidence, such as crime
scene characteristics and offense-related empirical data – an approach often referred to as
‘statistical’.
CRIMINAL PSYCHOLOGY
Criminology is the study of the causes of crime and ways to prevent and control it. Criminal
psychology focuses on studying the thoughts, feelings, and behaviors of criminals. A
criminologist might ask questions such as, "Is poverty a driving force behind high crime
rates? while a criminal psychologist might ask "What mental illnesses does Mr. Smith, who
committed a crime, suffer from?" In the 1930s, the focus in mainstream criminology shifted
from the individual to society, and the psychological theories of criminal behavior held little
sway compared to sociological theories. However since the 1990s, there has been an
increasing dialogue between the disciplines as the study of the individual once again becomes
a concern in criminology (Lilly, Cullen & Ball, 2001).
Violent offenders.
A recent World Health Report (Krug et al., 2002) referred to violence as a global public
health problem’. Contemporary psychological theory characterizes violence in this context in
terms of an interaction between the qualities of the individual and the characteristics of their
environment.
Taxonomy of anger
The most influential theory of anger is Novaco’s (1975). According to Novaco, for someone
to become angry,
The environmental event must first trigger distinctive patterns of physiological and cognitive
arousal. This trigger usually lies in the individual’s perception of the words and actions of
another person. When one becomes angry, physiological and cognitive processes are kicked
into action. Increased autonomic nervous system activity includes a rise in body temperature,
perspiration, muscular tension, and increased cardiovascular activity.
The relevant cognitive processes (Novaco & Welsh, 1989) involve various types of
information-processing biases concerned with the encoding of interpretation and triggering
cues.
Attentional cueing is the tendency to see hostility and provocation in the words and actions
of other people,
An attribution error occurs when the individual believes that his or her behavior is
determined by the situation, but that the behavior of other people is explained by their
personality
· The progression from anger to violence is associated with the dis-inhibition of internal
control, which can result from factors such as high levels of physiological arousal, the
perception that there is little chance of being apprehended or punished, and the perpetrator’s
use of drugs or alcohol.
Moral reasoning
Research into the relationship between moral reasoning and offending (Palmer, 2003).
Gibbs has examined the specific association between moral reasoning and violent behavior,
focusing on the bridge between theories of social has examined the specific association
between moral reasoning and violent behavior, focusing on the bridge between theories of
social /of social information processing and moral development. Gibbs and colleagues
suggest that this bridge takes the form of cognitive distortions (Gibbs, 1993; Goldstein, Glick
& Gibbs, 1998) by which we rationalize or mislabel our behavior. Cognitive distortion my
distorted rationalization might be that ‘he was asking for it It’s also seen in my biased
interpretation of the consequences of my behavior.