Example 04
Example 04
INTRODUCTION
In everyday life, we are continuously overloaded with a mass of sensory stimuli. Attention, is
the cognitive process that enables us to filter and select relevant sensory information, so we
can achieve optimum situational awareness. Thus, our ability to appropriately detect and
perceive salient changes in our visual field, especially when we do not expect them, a
One cognitive explanation for IAB is the ‘perceptual load theory’ (Lavie, 2010) which
suggests that when faced with tasks requiring high perceptual load, IAB is more likely to
occur as little capacity is left to process irrelevant distractors. Hence, IAB is less likely when
carrying out low perceptual load tasks, which allow for extra capacity to remain, thereby
increasing our ability to process distractors. Lavie’s perceptual load theory could therefore
explain the problem of task difficulty as an explanation for IAB. Various literature shows that
more difficult tasks (of higher load) call for more attentional resources which makes it more
A study by Chabris, Weinberger, Fontaine and Simons (2011), examined IAB using a
simulated real-world event. In their study, participants had to chase a confederate along a
road where three other confederates staged an assault nearby (the fight acting as the
unexpected stimulus). During this task, participants had to keep count of how many times
the confederate they were chasing touched his head. The amount of attentional load was
they were instructed to keep a separate mental count of the number of times the
confederate touched his head with his right and left hand whilst participants in the ‘single
counting condition’ (low load) were simply told to keep count of the total number of times
the confederate touched his head. The results from Chabris et al’s study demonstrated that
participants in the hard task (dual counting) condition were more affected by IAB as only
42% noticed the fight taking place compared to 72% in the easy task (single counting)
(Hyman, Boss, Wise, McKenzie, & Caggiano, 2010) using the naturalistic example of walking
whilst talking on the phone has obtained similar results. Hyman et al discovered that only
25% of subjects who were walking and engaging in a phone conversation simultaneously
noticed a flamboyant unicycling clown near their walking path compared to 51% of subjects
walking whilst not engaging in cell phone conversation who noticed the clown. Both Hyman
et al and Chabris et al’s experiments highlight that more difficult tasks lead to a higher level
affected the likelihood of IAB. We hypothesised that more participants in the hard task
condition would fail to notice a moving monkey on a screen compared to participants in the
METHODS
IAB experiment. We conducted our study using a between-subjects design and proceeded to
randomly assign the participants to two conditions; the easy task (A) or the hard task (B)
THE LINK BETWEEN TASK DIFFICULTY AND ATTENTIONAL BLINDNESS
Both groups were instructed to watch two ten-second videos displaying one white
shape and five black shapes which bounced off the sides of the box. The first video did not
participants with the task, whilst in the second video, a monkey appeared on the right-side
of the box after the first three seconds of the video and slowly moved to the left. In the
second video, the monkey was visible for approximately 4 seconds. Participants in the easy
task condition were told to count the number of times the white object bounced off the
boarders of the box whilst participants in the hard task condition were told to keep a
separate count of the number of times the white object bounced off the horizontal and
of the unexpected stimulus), after watching the video and recording their mental count of
bounces, all participants were asked three standardised questions; (1) Did you notice
anything unusual in the second video which differed from the first video? (2) Did you notice
anything unusual in the background of the second video aside from the other black objects?
(3) Did you see a black monkey moving across the screen in the second video? If a
participant responded with ‘Yes’ to the third question, they were counted as having noticed
the monkey.
THE LINK BETWEEN TASK DIFFICULTY AND ATTENTIONAL BLINDNESS
Group A: 58.7%
Group B: 40.0%
RESULTS
70.00
Monkey seen
60.00
50.00
40.00
30.00
20.00
10.00
0.00
Group A (Easy Task) Group B (Hard Task)
The results from Figure 1 above show that more participants in the Easy task
condition (58.7%) saw the monkey in the second video compared to participants in
the hard task condition where only 40.0% noticed the monkey moving across the
screen.
DISCUSSION
THE LINK BETWEEN TASK DIFFICULTY AND ATTENTIONAL BLINDNESS
It is evident that task difficulty influenced IAB as participants in the hard task
condition failed to see the monkey despite its salience as their attention was more focused
on counting and separating the number of times the white object bounced vertically and
horizontally which left few attentional resources to process the unexpected stimulus (the
monkey). On the other hand, participants in the easy task condition were more likely to
notice the monkey as their task (counting the total number of white object bounces)
required less attentional resources. Thus, our experimental hypothesis was confirmed. Our
results also confirm Lavie’s perceptual load theory (2010), as reduced ability to process
processing (processing with the aid of prior knowledge). When completing selective-looking
tasks, objects other than the target tend to be ignored and we use our schemas (mental
representation of past experiences and expectations) to help us make sense of any gaps.
Therefore, it could be argued that subjects merely assumed the moving monkey was
another black object (as the first video only displayed black and white objects), which made
Our results are parallel to the findings from Chabris et al’s (2011) and Hyman et al’s
research (2010) in which the percentage of subjects that observed the unexpected stimulus
was lower in the hard task conditions compared to the easy task conditions. We also
compared our results to those obtained from Simons and Chabris’ famous ‘gorilla study’
(1999) which showed that a lower percentage of participants (45%) completing a difficult
task (keeping a separate count of aerial and bounce passes in a video of a basketball game),
noticed either a person in a gorilla suit or a woman with an umbrella walk through the
action compared to 64% of subjects only keeping count of the total number of passes (easy
THE LINK BETWEEN TASK DIFFICULTY AND ATTENTIONAL BLINDNESS
Despite the relevance our findings provide to IAB research, our study is not without
flaws. It was discovered that as part of Simons and Chabris’ data collection process (1999),
the data of subjects who had previously heard of or participated in similar experiments
investigating IAB was removed, a procedure we did not follow in our study. This potentially
raises concerns about the reliability of our results as participants may have responded to the
follow-up questions based on prior knowledge acquired during their A-level studies or
through wider reading. Thus, as an implication for future research, experimenters could
ensure that the data from such participants is removed so that it does not reduce the
This brings attention to the problem of reactivity which may have acted as a
confounding variable due to the use of leading questions e.g., ‘did you see a black monkey
moving across the screen in the second video?’. It could be argued that such questions may
have prompted participants to falsely claim that they had perceived the monkey. This issue
could be rectified in future research by simply asking participants if they noticed any
unusual occurrences and using more open-ended questions (e.g. ‘if yes, please give details
of what you saw’) which enable participants to properly describe what they claimed to have
Furthermore, Simons (2000) avers that paradigms of explicit attentional capture may
not be a clear, definitive measure of attentional capture (as simply asking participants if they
noticed the unexpected stimulus does not provide conclusive evidence as to whether they
saw it). Therefore, future research which combines measures of implicit attentional capture
(i.e. examining if the presence of an unexpected stimulus affects task performance) and
References
You do not talk about fight club if you do not notice fight club:
Hyman, I. E., Boss, S. M., Wise, B. M., McKenzie, K. E., & Caggiano,
143.