Prefrontal Regions Orchestrate Suppression of Emotional Memories Via A Two-Phase Process
Prefrontal Regions Orchestrate Suppression of Emotional Memories Via A Two-Phase Process
If you wish to distribute this article to others, you can order high-quality copies for your
colleagues, clients, or customers by clicking here.
Updated information and services, including high-resolution figures, can be found in the online
version of this article at:
http://www.sciencemag.org/content/317/5835/215.full.html
Supporting Online Material can be found at:
http://www.sciencemag.org/content/suppl/2007/07/10/317.5835.215.DC1.html
A list of selected additional articles on the Science Web sites related to this article can be
found at:
http://www.sciencemag.org/content/317/5835/215.full.html#related
This article cites 29 articles, 12 of which can be accessed free:
http://www.sciencemag.org/content/317/5835/215.full.html#ref-list-1
This article has been cited by 43 article(s) on the ISI Web of Science
This article has been cited by 25 articles hosted by HighWire Press; see:
http://www.sciencemag.org/content/317/5835/215.full.html#related-urls
This article appears in the following subject collections:
Neuroscience
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/collection/neuroscience
Science (print ISSN 0036-8075; online ISSN 1095-9203) is published weekly, except the last week in December, by the
American Association for the Advancement of Science, 1200 New York Avenue NW, Washington, DC 20005. Copyright
2007 by the American Association for the Advancement of Science; all rights reserved. The title Science is a
registered trademark of AAAS.
Geisa Carolinne Ferreira Mendes - [email protected] - CPF: 106.772.336-63
RESEARCH ARTICLE
using faces as cues and pictures as targets, suggests
that the impact of exerting control is larger for
Prefrontal Regions Orchestrate emotional than for nonemotional information
(6). Because of these findings, as well as the
1
Department of Psychology, University of Colorado,
created (1) and whether they can be suppressed. used the Think/No-Think paradigm (T/NT) of Boulder, CO 80309, USA. 2Center for Neuroscience,
Although some researchers have provided ini- Anderson and colleagues, in which individuals University of Colorado, Boulder, CO 80309, USA. 3Institute
of Cognitive Science, University of Colorado, Boulder, CO
tial evidence for memory suppression (2, 3), attempt to elaborate a memory by repetitively 80309, USA. 4Department of Psychiatry, University of
others claim that memory repression or suppres- thinking of it (T condition) or to suppress a mem- Denver Health Sciences, Denver, CO 80208, USA.
sion is a clinical myth in search of scientific ory by repetitively not letting it enter conscious- *To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail:
support (4). ness (NT condition) (2, 3). Our recent T/NT work, [email protected]
A B
100
Training Phase
90
80
+ x 40 faces
Percentage Recalled
70
Base
60
T
Experimental Phase 50
NT
Think 40
Think of 30
the previously m (Base) = 62.5%
associated picture 20
m (T) = 71.1%
10 m (NT) = 53.2%
0
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16
OR x 12 per face
No-Think Number of Participants
Do not let the Fig. 1. (A) Experimental procedure. Individuals were first trained during
previously associated structural scanning to associate 40 cue-target pairs. During the experimental
picture enter
consciousness phase, brain activity was recorded using fMRI while individuals viewed only the
face (16 faces per condition, 12 repetitions per face; 3.5 s per face). On some
trials they were instructed to think of the previously learned picture; on other
trials they were instructed not to let the previously associated picture enter
consciousness. The presentation of only the cue (i.e., the face) ensures that
Testing Phase individuals manipulate the memory of the target picture. The additional faces (8
items) not shown during this phase acted as a behavioral baseline. During the
Give Short test phase, the individuals were shown the 40 faces and asked to describe the
+ Description x 40 pairs
previously associated picture. (B) Behavioral results: percentage recall for each
participant for T trials (green) and NT trials (red), with the dotted line indicating
baseline recall for items not viewed in the experimental phase. Recall differed for
T and NT items [t(15) = –4.29, P = 0.0006] because of a trend for greater recall in the T condition [t(15) = 1.49, P = 0.07] and a significant reduction
in recall in the NT condition [t(15) = –2.28, P = 0.02], calculated relative to baseline.
www.sciencemag.org
Geisa Carolinne Ferreira Mendes - [email protected] SCIENCE VOL 317
- CPF: 106.772.336-63 13 JULY 2007 215
RESEARCH ARTICLE
determine whether activity in the regions of NT trials than for T trials—irrespective of recall also a negative signal change for NT trials and,
interest (ROIs) actually decreased (5). Although accuracy (5). Additionally, because we were conversely, a positive signal change for T trials,
Anderson and colleagues (2) demonstrated the specifically interested in effective control over relative to baseline. Brain areas underlying the
involvement of prefrontal regions in cognitive memory, we examined this same contrast only sensory representation of memory that showed
control over memory, they examined the contrast for NT trials in which the picture was forgotten such an effect were the visual cortex, including
between activation on NT and T trials without (NTf) and on T trials in which it was remem- bilateral BA17, BA18, and BA37 [fusiform gyrus
reporting fixation baseline contrasts, making it bered (Tr). Brain areas are discussed according (FG)], and the pulvinar nucleus of the thalamus
difficult to associate activity with a specific to their putative functional roles: (i) cognitive (Pul; Fig. 2B). Suppression of emotional memo-
condition (e.g., T or NT). control, (ii) sensory representation of memory, ries thus involved decreased activity in sensory
Second, in the experimental phase, each face and (iii) memory processes and emotional cortices that are normally active when memories
was shown 12 times with the presentation of the components of memory. are being retrieved, as well as in regions (i.e., Pul)
faces pseudo-randomly intermixed. Although in Sources and sites of cognitive control. Pre- that play a role in gating and modulating attention
our previous study (6) memory increased lin- frontal regions, right-sided and spanning BA 8, toward or away from visual stimuli (16, 17).
early with the number of times cognitive control 9/46, 47, and BA 10, exhibited NT > T contrast. Brain areas involved in memory processes
was exerted for T trials, this was not the case for A conjunction analysis indicated that these and emotional components of memory repre-
NT trials. Hence, we divided our data into quar- differences resulted from an increase in activity sentation that exhibited significant condition-
tiles (three repetitions per cue during each quar- for NT trials rather than a decrease in activation specific activity were the hippocampal complex
tile) to determine whether the neural mechanisms for T trials relative to baseline (Fig. 2A), which (Hip) and the amygdala (Amy; Fig. 2C), which
involved in memory suppression change with suggests that these regions are specifically are strongly involved in emotional learning and
repeated attempts. involved in controlling the suppression of emo- memory (18, 19), as would be required by the
The behavioral results shown in Fig. 1B tional memories. face-picture association in our paradigm. Hence,
indicated that individuals effectively suppressed Next, we sought to determine which regions the suppression of emotional memory also
memory. To investigate the neural basis of this support components of the memory representa- involved suppression of memory processes and
effect, we first analyzed the functional magnetic tion. These regions showed condition-specific emotional aspects of the memory representation.
resonance imaging (fMRI) data for instances of changes, which we defined as yielding not only a Two phases of memory suppression. We
NT > T—that is, significantly greater activity for significant difference for the NT > T contrast but next examined how brain activity changed with
% Signal Change
0.10 0.10
creased activity only for the first
and second quartiles [quartile 1,
t(15) = 3.65, P = 0.001; quartile 2, 0.00 0.00
t(15) = 2.66, P = 0.01], whereas the
activities of Pul and FG decrease to -0.10 -0.10
significantly below baseline during
quartile 4 [Pul, t(15) = –2.11, P = -0.20 -0.20
0.05; FG, t(15) = –1.96, P = 0.05]. 1 2
Quartile
3 4 1 2 3 4
Quartile
(B) rMFG displays significantly ele-
vated activity during the second, third, and fourth quartiles (Fig. 4B) [quartile 2, t(15) = –4.42,
P = 0.0005; quartile 3, t(15) = 2.10, P = 0.05; quartile 4, t(15) = 2.08, P = 0.05], whereas Hip
C IFG&Pul IFG&FG MFG&Hip MFG&Amy
0.20
and Amy initially exhibit increased activity in quartile 1 but show significantly decreased activity
by quartile 4 [Hip quartile 1, t(15) = 3.11, P = 0.007; Hip quartile 4, t(15) = –1.98, P = 0.05;
Correlation Coefficient
0.00
Amy quartile 1, t(15) = 3.11, P = 0.007; Amy quartile 4, t(15) = –2.15, P = 0.04]. (C) -0.20
Correlation coefficients for rIFG and rMFG grouped with respective associated posterior brain
regions as a function of quartile. *P ≤ 0.05, +P ≤ 0.01, #P ≤ 0.001. -0.40
-0.60
-0.80
-1
1 2 3 4
Quartile
www.sciencemag.org
Geisa Carolinne Ferreira Mendes - [email protected] SCIENCE VOL 317
- CPF: 106.772.336-63 13 JULY 2007 217
RESEARCH ARTICLE
cused attention. However, we found that BA10 suppression is most readily accomplished by pre- cating that Hip activity has been effectively
activity precedes and covaries with the activity of frontal modulation of activity in sensory cortices suppressed.
two separate prefrontal areas that are initiated that is likely to recreate the sensory percept. This At a broader level, our findings extend re-
at different temporal points in the process of process, however, does not predict subsequent search suggesting that prefrontal brain areas asso-
suppressing activation in two different sets of behavioral recall and may be a “late-correction” ciated with inhibitory mechanisms (BA 10 and
posterior brain regions. This time-phased, region- mechanism (26) to suppress prepotent activity in superior, inferior, and middle FG) are lateralized
specific activity pattern of BA10 is more complex sensory cortices generated as a result of viewing predominantly to the right hemisphere (27, 28).
than that expected for simple mind-wandering the cue. In contrast, activity of rMFG (over all We have shown the involvement of these areas in
during NT trials, and is more consistent with quartiles) and Hip (during the last quartile) pre- the suppression of emotional memories, which
BA10’s proposed role of orchestration of mul- dicts behavioral suppression (5). This suppres- replicates current literature suggesting that these
tiple prefrontal processes involved in guiding sion process may reduce access to the memory areas are active in the suppression of emotional
complex tasks (21, 22). itself, by blocking Hip and Amy activity needed reactivity (29, 30). Activity in these brain areas,
Our findings suggest that the suppression of for retrieval of the emotional memory. However, along with inhibition over Hip and Amy, sug-
emotional memory involves at least two path- this mechanism becomes effective only after re- gests that suppression of emotional memories may
ways with staggered phases of their modulatory peated attempts at control, as shown by the in- use mechanisms similar to those used in emotion
influence. The first pathway involves cognitive creased correlation in activity between rMFG-Hip regulation. Thus, various right-lateralized PFC
control by rIFG over sensory components of and rMFG-Amy over quartiles (Fig. 3C). Once areas may be involved in coordinating suppres-
memory representation, as evidenced by reduced successful suppression of hippocampal activity is sion processes across many behavioral domains,
activity in FG and Pul. This finding is consistent achieved, the need to invoke the “late-correction” including memory retrieval, motor processes, feel-
with computational models that posit that activa- process apparently decreases, as reflected in the ings of social rejection, self motives, and state
tion and inhibition of the thalamus is a critical decreased rIFG-Pul and rIFG-FG correlations. emotional reactivity (27–32).
means of gating working memory information Our data also provide an intriguing hint that, Our findings may have implications for ther-
(24). A second pathway involves cognitive con- as suggested in clinical practice, it is necessary apeutic approaches to disorders involving the
trol by rMFG over memory processes and emo- to “revisit” an emotionally distressing memory inability to suppress emotionally distressing mem-
tional components of memory representation via before it can be controlled. We found that early ories and thoughts, including PTSD, phobias,
modulation of Hip and Amy (2, 25). The overall in the course of suppression (during quartiles ruminative depression/anxiety, and OCD. They
timing of these suppression effects appears to be 1 and 2), greater Hip activity was observed for provide the possibility for approaches to control-
orchestrated by a modulatory influence of BA10, NTf trials than NTr trials, as was also found by ling memories by suppressing sensory aspects
first over rIFG, then over rMFG. Further research Anderson et al. in nontemporal analyses (2, 5). of memory and/or by strengthening cognitive
is needed to determine the extent to which the We speculate that memories that activate Hip to control over memory and emotional processes
two pathways act independently or interactively. a greater degree are more amenable to cognitive through repeated practice. Refinement of ther-
Our findings also provide insight into the control because they are more elaborated and apeutic procedures based on these distinct means
potential neurocognitive mechanisms by which may provide increased access. By the fourth of manipulating emotional memory might be an
emotional memories are suppressed. Our data quartile, activation in Hip for NTf trials (but not exciting and fruitful development in future
are consistent with the idea that initial memory NTr trials) is significantly below baseline, indi- clinical research.
Fig. 4. (A) Time course of percent A BA10 IFG MFG B BA10&IFG BA10&MFG
signal change for BA10, rIFG, and
0.20 0.8
rMFG. (B) Correlations of rBA10
with rIFG and rMFG as a function 0.6
Correlation Coefficient
0.4
Correlation Coefficient
0.4
rMFG (r = 0.77, P = 0.001), but 0.2 0.2
not with other brain areas [Pul, r = 0 0
–0.16; FG, r = –0.16; Hip, r = –0.22;
-0.2 -0.2
Amy, r = –0.09; Ps > 0.40 (5)]. *P ≤
0.05, +P ≤ 0.01, #P ≤ 0.001. -0.4 -0.4
-0.6 -0.6
-0.8 -0.8
-1 -1
1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4
Quartile Quartile
REPORTS
The data that we present were obtained from a re-
Scattering and Interference in gion identified as bilayer graphene (11). Scanning
tunneling microscopy (STM) measurements were
Epitaxial Graphene performed in a custom-built ultrahigh-vacuum,
low-temperature instrument. We measured the
scanning tunneling spectroscopy (STS) differential
G. M. Rutter,1 J. N. Crain,2 N. P. Guisinger,2 T. Li,1 P. N. First,1* J. A. Stroscio2*
conductance dI/dV (where I is current and V is
voltage) with lock-in detection by applying a small
A single sheet of carbon, graphene, exhibits unexpected electronic properties that arise from
modulation to the tunnel voltage at ≈500 Hz.
quantum state symmetries, which restrict the scattering of its charge carriers. Understanding the
Differential conductance maps were obtained by
role of defects in the transport properties of graphene is central to realizing future electronics
recording an STS spectrum at each spatial pixel
based on carbon. Scanning tunneling spectroscopy was used to measure quasiparticle interference
in the topographic measurement. All measure-
patterns in epitaxial graphene grown on SiC(0001). Energy-resolved maps of the local density of
ments reported here were taken at 4.3 K.
states reveal modulations on two different length scales, reflecting both intravalley and intervalley
STM topographic images (Fig. 1) show the
scattering. Although such scattering in graphene can be suppressed because of the symmetries of
atomic structure and different types of disorder
the Dirac quasiparticles, we show that, when its source is atomic-scale lattice defects, wave
for epitaxial graphene on SiC(0001). At the
functions of different symmetries can mix.
atomic scale, the graphene is imaged as a tri-
angular lattice (Fig. 1B), characteristic of imag-
uilt of a honeycomb of sp2-bonded car- effect (1, 2) and weak antilocalization (3, 4), that ing only one of the two graphene sublattices.
www.sciencemag.org
Geisa Carolinne Ferreira Mendes - [email protected] SCIENCE VOL 317
- CPF: 106.772.336-63 13 JULY 2007 219