Salthouse, Age-Related Differences in Basic Cognitive Processes - Implications For Work
Salthouse, Age-Related Differences in Basic Cognitive Processes - Implications For Work
Timothy A. Salthouse
Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, Georgia,
USA
A version of this paper was originally presented at the 2nd International Stein Conference.
Accepted for publication March 1993.
Address correspondence to Timothy A. Salthouse, School of Psychology, Georgia Institute of
Technology, Atlanta, GA 30332. USA.
120
................................................... Professional & Technica
110
Managerial, Clerical, Sale!
100
...................................................
90
................................................... Unskilled
80
70
a I I I I I I
20 30 40 50 60 70
120
90
80
70
I I I I I I
20 30 40 50 60 70
Chronological Age
plausible, the mechanisms of this influence are not yet obvious. For
example, one possibility is that extensive experience leads to maintained
proficiency of basic cognitive processes that would decline in the absence
of that experience. An alternative possibility is that experience contributes
to stable or improved levels of job performance without affecting the
proficiency of basic cognitive processes. Although both of these interpre-
tations could account for the finding of little or no relationship between
age and work performance, they should be distinguished because they
have quite different implications. For example, if it were discovered that
age-related declines in relevant basic cognitive processes were evident in
persons with extensive experience, then low levels of work performance
might be predicted if older individuals were to switch to a new job where
their previous experience was not applicable.
My colleagues and I have found evidence relevant to these two alter-
native mechanisms by which experience might moderate relationships
between age and work performance in three studies (Salthouse, 1991b;
Salthouse et al., 1990; Salthouse & Mitchell, 1990). All of the studies
were designed to investigate the relationships among age, experience,
and performance on cognitive tests assessing various spatial abilities. For
example, one of the tests used in these projects was a paper-folding test
in which the examinee sees rectangles with illustrations of a sequence of
folds followed by a hole punch, and then is asked to select which pattern
of holes would result if the paper were unfolded. Another test was a
surface development test in which a drawing of an object assembled and
unassembled are presented, and the task is to identify corresponding
regions in the two drawings.
The three separate studies were conducted to determine the effects of
presumably relevant experience on the relationships between age and
performance on these types of tests. The research participants in one
study consisted of 47 practicing architects ages 21-71 (Salthouse et al.,
1990). We chose architects as the research subjects because we assumed
that spatial abilities are needed on an almost daily basis in this occupation.
Despite what can be presumed to be extensive and nearly continuous
experience with work requiring spatial ability, the age trends in this
sample’s performance on measures of spatial ability were nearly identical
to those observed in unselected samples of adults.
The participants in the second study (Salthouse, 1991b) were mainly
engineers and other users of computer-assisted design and computer-
assisted manufacturing systems. The tests in this project were designed
to involve materials relevant to these job categories (e.g., recognizing
objects from orthographic drawings). As in the previous study, however,
there was almost no effect of experience on the magnitude of the age
relationships in these tests.
254 T . A. Salthouse
REFERENCES
Davies, D. R . , & Sparrow, P. R . (1985). Age and work behavior. In N . Charness (Ed.),
Aging and human peflortnance (pp. 293-332). New York: Wiley.
Age, Cognition, and Work 255