Previewpdf
Previewpdf
2nd Edition
Concepts in Composition: Theory and Practice in the Teaching of Writing is designed to foster
reflection on how theory impacts practice, enabling prospective teachers to develop their
own comprehensive and coherent conception of what writing is or should be and to consider
how people learn to write. This approach allows readers to assume the dual role of both
teacher and student as they enter the conversation of the discipline and become familiar
with some of the critical issues.
These new directions will inform the content of this revision, reflecting significant advance-
ments in the field. Each chapter addresses a particular theoretical concept relevant to class-
room teaching and includes activities to help readers establish the connection between
theoretical concepts and classroom lessons. The book’s companion website provides
resources for instructors, including PowerPoint presentations and lecture notes. Bringing
together scholars with expertise in particular areas of composition, this text will serve as an
effective primer for students and educators in the field of composition theory.
Irene L. Clark is Professor of English, Director of Composition, and Director of the Master’s
Option in Rhetoric and Composition at California State University, Northridge. She previ-
ously taught at the University of Southern California (USC), where she also co-directed the
university’s Writing Program and directed its Writing Center. She has won multiple awards
from the National Writing Centers Association, and has authored several textbooks for both
undergraduate and graduate students. She holds a B.A. in Music from Hunter College, an
M.A. in English from Columbia University, and a Ph.D. in English Literature from USC.
Concepts in Composition
Theory and Practice in the Teaching of Writing
2nd Edition
Irene L. Clark
with Contributors
Betty Bamberg
Darsie Bowden
John R. Edlund
Lisa Gerrard
Olga Griswold
Sharon Klein
Julie Neff-Lippman
James D. Williams
Instructors: please visit the companion website at
www.routledge.com/9780415885164
1 Processes 1
2 Invention 47
3 Revision 79
4 Audience 109
6 Genre 181
Preface xvii
1 Processes 1
Irene L. Clark
The “Process” Movement 1
The Writing Process Movement: A Brief History 2
The First-Year Course and the Use of Handbooks 3
The Birth of the Process “Movement” 5
Influences on the Concept of Process Teaching 5
The Stages of Writing 7
Renewed Interest in Rhetoric 8
Rhetoric, Exigence, and Process 9
Rhetoric, Authority, and Values 9
Rhetorical Reading 10
Genre and Process 10
Early Research on Composing 11
The Role of Cognitive Psychology 12
Expressivism and the Concept of Personal Voice 15
Social Constructionism 16
Collaborative Learning 17
Collaborative Learning in the Classroom 17
Collaborating Online 19
Criticism of the Process Movement 21
Post-Process Theory 21
Genre Theory and the Concept of Process 22
The Importance of Reflection 27
Note 28
References 28
Readings 30
Composing Behaviors of One- and Multi-Draft Writers 30
Muriel Harris
x CONTENTS
2 Invention 47
Irene L. Clark
The Heritage of Invention 48
Invention in Classical Rhetoric 49
Aristotle 51
Emphasis on Community in Classical Rhetoric 53
The Heritage of Personal Writing 53
Personal Writing in the Early Phase of the Process Movement 54
The Possibility of Teaching Invention 54
Writer’s Block 55
Invention and the Composition Classroom 57
Helping Students Understand the Assignment 57
Creating an Invention-Oriented Classroom Atmosphere 58
Invention Strategies 58
Class and Group Discussion 58
Journals 59
Freewriting and Brainstorming 59
Clustering 59
The Points to Make List 61
Exploring a Topic Through Questioning 61
Discovering Ideas by Engaging with a Text 64
They Say/I Say 65
Invention and Imitation 66
Notes 68
References 68
Reading 69
Rigid Rules, Inflexible Plans, and the Stifling of Language:
A Cognitivist Analysis of Writer’s Block 69
Mike Rose
3 Revision 79
Betty Bamberg
Older Concepts of Revision 80
Contemporary Concepts of Revision in the Composing Process 81
Revision Strategies of Student Writers 82
Obstacles to Revision 84
Helping Students Revise Effectively: Intervening in the Composing Process 85
Using Whole-Class Workshops and One-to-One
Conferences to Encourage Revision 85
Using Peer Response Groups to Encourage Revision 86
Using Computers to Encourage Revision 87
Using Direct Instruction to Encourage Revision 89
CONTENTS xi
4 Audience 109
Irene L. Clark
The Complicated Issue of Audience 109
Perspectives on Audience 110
Rediscovering Audience 110
Cognitive Perspectives 111
The Fictionalized Audience 112
Audience Addressed/Audience Invoked: Fictionalized and Real Audiences 113
Recent Articles by Ede and Lunsford on the Topic of Audience 114
Student Perspectives on Audience 115
Connections Between the Writer and the Audience 116
The Work of James Moffett 117
Using Fictional Characters and Dialogue to Focus Student
Attention on the Concept of Audience 118
Creating Characters 119
Writing a Dialogue 120
Multiple Concepts of Audience in the Context of New Media 121
Audience and the Expanded Discourse Community 122
Distinguishing Between New and Common Knowledge: Complications 122
Fostering Audience Awareness in the Writing Class 123
Invoking Audience Cues in a Text 124
Peer Feedback 125
Audience and the Potential of New Media 125
References 127
Reading 129
Closing My Eyes as I Speak: An Argument for Ignoring Audience 129
Peter Elbow
6 Genre 181
Irene L. Clark
Traditional Notions of Genre 181
Reconceiving Genre in Terms of Function 182
Genre Theory and Other Rhetorical Perspectives 184
Controversies Associated with Genre in the Writing Class 185
The Debate about Explicit Teaching 185
The Sydney School Genre-Based Curriculum 186
Genre Awareness and Transferability 187
The Cognitive Perspective 188
The Ethics of Privileging Academic Writing 190
Autobiographical versus Discoursal Identity 191
Writing Genres in the Composition Class 192
Personal Narrative 192
Academic Argument 193
Genre and Creativity 195
Genre and New Media 197
The Influence of Antecedent Genres 197
Alternative Genres 198
Creative Nonfiction 198
Literary Genres 198
Other Terms Associated with Genre 199
Addressing Genre in the Writing Class 200
CONTENTS xiii
References 203
Essay 205
How Do You Raise That Money? 205
Reading 208
Sites of Invention: Genre and the Enactment of First-Year Writing 208
Annis Bawarshi
Tense/Aspect 370
Negation 372
Existential Sentences 373
Some Other Structures 374
Sounds and Sound Patterns in AAE 375
Discourse, Narrative Structure, and Beyond 380
Notes 388
References 389
Readings 391
Dealing with Bad Ideas: Twice is Less 391
Wayne O’Neil
The Myth of Linguistic Homogeneity in U.S. College Composition 399
Paul Kei Matsuda
Concepts in Composition: Theory and Practice in the Teaching of Writing focuses on schol-
arship in rhetoric and composition that has influenced classroom teaching. Its goal is to
foster reflection on how theory impacts practice, enabling prospective teachers to become
conscious of how they think about writing in the context of a first-year writing class and
develop strategies that can help students improve as writers. Each chapter addresses a par-
ticular theoretical concept that impacts classroom teaching and includes suggestions for
writing, discussion, and further exploration. Over the past several years, I have used this
book in a number of graduate seminars, and I have found that its approach enables prospec-
tive instructors to assume the dual role of both teacher and student as they enter the conver-
sations of the discipline and become familiar with some of its critical issues.
The initial impetus for the first edition of this book was provided by a course in composi-
tion theory and pedagogy that I had taught for prospective high school teachers and gradu-
ate teaching assistants. The course had been organized around concepts that I felt were the
most important for my students to understand in terms of their influence on classroom
practice and which, in many instances, had generated lively and fruitful discussion. Then,
one day, when I was flying home from a conference on college composition, I thought about
how useful it would be for me and perhaps for others, if I transformed my classroom notes
into a book. Thus was Concepts in Composition conceived, and, in accordance with the orga-
nization of my course, each chapter consists of the following:
High in the air, jotting down ideas, I initially thought that I would write every chapter
myself. However, once on the ground, breathing fresh air, I decided that the breadth of
scholarship and diversity of perspective would be significantly improved if I enlisted the
aid of scholars in the field who had expertise in particular concepts in composition. The
result is a more substantive book that is considerably richer, due to the contributions of my
xviii PREFACE
knowledgeable and gracious colleagues, who embarked on revising their chapters for this
second edition with impressive expertise and remarkable good humor.
books, conferences and organizations that are associated with this burgeoning discipline
have brought the recognition that helping students improve as writers requires a thorough
grounding in both theory and practice.
For all contributors to this book, the most daunting challenge has been figuring out how
to address each multifaceted concept in composition within the confines of a single chap-
ter. Composition scholarship is extensive, and the question of what to include and what
to omit was the subject of many conversations among my contributors, all of whom were
concerned about simplifying, but who also recognized the paradox of teaching composi-
tion: that teachers need to know a little about each concept to plan their courses effectively,
but that it is only through classroom experience that they will really be able to understand
the interaction of theory and practice. It is a credit to my contributors that they all man-
aged to find a middle ground between too much and too little information, presenting a
balanced perspective for beginning teachers while pointing the way to further exploration
and research.
It is often the case that new teachers, worried about how to fill class time, become impa-
tient with courses that address theory at all. “Just tell me what I need to teach,” they implore.
“Give me a syllabus and I’ll follow it.” Concepts in Composition: Theory and Practice in the
Teaching of Writing aims to help teachers find answers for themselves by helping them
understand connections between a theoretical concept and a classroom lesson. It is only
when teachers understand that relationship that they will be able to teach effectively.
A companion website is available for use with this volume, with additional materials for
instructors using the book in the classroom: www.routledge.com/9780415885164.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
I am deeply grateful to the contributors to this second edition. Their willingness to update
their chapters, their breadth of scholarship, and their generosity of spirit have been crucial
to the development of this second edition. I would also like to thank Linda Bathgate and the
editorial staff at Taylor & Francis publishers, who provided continued support and encour-
agement, as did my wonderful graduate students at California State University, Northridge,
whose enthusiasm for the first edition sparked my own efforts. In particular, I would like
to acknowledge the contributions of Margeaux Gamboa and Michelle Mutti whose insights
into new media have been so valuable. And then, finally, and always, there is my husband
and family, who have always supported my publishing efforts and encouraged me in all my
endeavors.
1
Processes
Irene L. Clark
• Why and how did writing classes become important in colleges and universities in the
United States?
• What is meant by the term “process”?
• What does rhetoric have to do with the teaching of writing?
• What is the role of reading in the writing class?
• What is the role of reflection in helping students improve as writers?
In the first edition of Concepts in Composition, Chapter 1 was titled “Process,” not “Pro-
cesses,” and in composition scholarship, the term “process” is still widely used. A popular
statement in early journal articles and textbooks was “writing is a process, not a product,”
and today there is general agreement that an important goal for a writing course is to help
students develop an effective writing process. However, it is now also recognized that there is
no one writing “process” that works for everyone, that writers use various processes at differ-
ent times, depending on what sort of text they wish to write, and that reading and research
are also processes. So—the term “process” really refers to many processes, an important idea
for prospective writing teachers to understand.
This chapter traces the history of the “process movement” within the emerging discipline
of rhetoric and composition, discusses several competing views or theories of composing,
explains connections between rhetoric and composition, and explores possibilities for inte-
grating reading into writing courses. It will also discuss the role of reflection and the impact
of new media on the idea of “process” in the teaching of writing.
I often recall that picture when I think about the term “process” in the context of com-
position pedagogy, because the outmoded concept of teaching it portrays constitutes the
antithesis of current ideas about the teaching of writing. Over the past 40 years, the dis-
cipline of rhetoric and composition has emphasized the importance of helping students
become active participants in learning to write, because, as the learning theorist Jerome
Bruner (1966) has maintained:
to instruct someone in [a] discipline is not a matter of getting him to commit results to mind.
Rather, it is to teach him to participate in the process that makes possible the establishment of
knowledge. … Knowledge is a process, not a product. (p. 72)
Because students do not learn to write by having knowledge poured into their heads, one
of the most important goals of a writing class is to enable students to understand that writ-
ing is a “process,” to develop a “process” that works well for them, and to be able to vary that
process as the need arises. The term “process” is of key importance for anyone entering the
field of composition, both as a teacher and as a researcher.
golden age in which students were able to write “better” than they can at present, whatever
“better” might mean, a time when students were more serious, more committed to learn-
ing, etc. And yet, even at Harvard, a recognized elite university, students’ writing had been
deemed inadequate. Even at Harvard, as Chester Noyes Greenough wrote in an early issue
of the English Journal (February 1913), the work of undergraduate “men” was characterized
by “incoherence, lack of unity in sentences and paragraphs, ignorance of certain rules of
punctuation, repeated misspelling of certain words, and so on” (p. 113).
Every year teachers resign, breakdown, perhaps become permanently invalided, having sacri-
ficed ambition, health, and in not a few instances even life, in the struggle to do all the work
expected of them. (Hopkins, p. 1)
Once upon a time, in an age of disciplinary darkness and desolation … writing students were
subjected to cruel and inhuman punishments. They were assigned topics like “Compare Henry
Fleming from The Red Badge of Courage to one of the characters in the Iliad; make sure to
consider the definition of an anti-hero” or “Write about your most humiliating moment.”
They were told, with a straight face, that no decent person ever wrote without outlining first,
that there is a clear distinction between description, narration, exposition, and argument;
4 IRENE L. CLARK
that grammatical errors were moral and mortal sins, and that teachers’ evaluations of student
essays were always objective accurate and fair …
In that dark period of our disciplinary history, teachers rarely explained anything about
the process of writing (unless you count “outline, write, proofread, hand in” as the student’s
process) … Or they would explain some of the rules governing good writing. But they would
say nothing about invention, how to get started, what to do in the middle, or what to do when
the middle turned back into the start, and so on. (pp. 2–3)
Of course, this picture constitutes a generalization, and it is likely that a number of teach-
ers, intuitively understanding what was helpful to beginning writers, did not adhere to this
model of “teaching” writing. Yet, that this model did indeed exist, I can attest to from my
own college experience, where essay topics were assigned regardless of whether students
knew or cared much about them, and where few, if any, process-oriented activities—pre-
writing strategies, multiple drafts, collaborative groups, student–teacher conferences—were
encouraged or even mentioned. When I submitted a paper as a college student, I would wait
with trepidation for the teacher to return it, which he or she would eventually do, usually
without having written anything other than a grade or perhaps a brief evaluative comment
on the front page.
In a recent review essay titled, “Of Pre- and Post-Process: Reviews and Ruminations,”
Richard Fulkerson (2001) characterized the situation of how first-year writing used to be as
similar to “riding a bicycle. If you knew how to do it, then you could demonstrate your abil-
ity on demand; hence the idea of in-class and time-limited writing” (p. 96). Fulkerson (2001)
described his own experience in a “pre-process program” as follows:
In the fall quarter, we had an anthology of readings, a handbook of grammar, and the 2nd edi-
tion of McCrimmon’s Writing with a Purpose. We wrote at least five papers. One assigned topic
was “My First Day at School.” Another was “any philosophical issue.” A third was a limited
research paper about some historic person, who we were to argue was or was not “great” based
on several readings in the anthology. Dr. Staton would assign the topic orally, and we would
have about a week to write. Then he marked the paper, put a grade on it, and a brief comment.
(p. 95)
Learning to write in those days meant being able to figure out what the teacher wanted in
order to create an acceptable “product,” and apparently, few teachers thought that helping
students acquire a workable writing “process” was part of their job. Whatever process stu-
dents used, they had to manage on their own.
This lack of attention to the process writers engage in when they write reflected a con-
cept of creativity that to some extent persists in our culture—that is, that a “good” writer
is someone who can produce an excellent text as quickly, independently, and effortlessly as
a bird learns to fly. This idea suggests that those of us who struggle, for whom writing is a
laborious, time-consuming, and often painful process (i.e., most, if not all, of us), are not,
by definition, “good” writers. One could either write, or one couldn’t. Such was the fantasy
of that time, and even now, our culture continues to value speed and ease of production,
particularly in reference to the speaking ability of our politicians, who are deemed “good
speakers” if they can think on their feet.
Unlike the product orientation of pre-process days, in ancient times, classical rheto-
ricians were aware of how much thought and preparation went into the production of a
PROCESSES 5
seemingly effortless speech. In ancient Greece and Rome, rhetoricians envisioned the com-
posing process as consisting of five stages—invention (the discovery of ideas), arrangement
(putting ideas in a persuasively effective order), style (finding the right language in which to
present the ideas), memory (memorizing the speech), and delivery (using voice and gesture
to present the speech effectively). Apparently, no one at that time was under the impression
that a “gifted” speaker did not have to engage in an elaborate process before he could deliver
an effective speech.
| |
For Writing and Discussion
Write a brief essay describing how you learned to write. To what extent was your experi-
ence similar to Fulkerson’s description? Did your teachers focus on the idea of process?
| |
participation and individual discovery in the learning process. In the context of writing
pedagogy, Bruner’s ideas translated into an emphasis on students engaging in composing
activities so as to discover their own composing process—rather than in analyzing someone
else’s text—and on teachers creating a facilitative learning environment to enable students
to do so—rather than focusing on assigning grades or correcting grammar.
Another important influence on the emerging writing process movement was the Dart-
mouth conference of 1966, a meeting of approximately 50 English teachers from the United
States and Great Britain to consider common writing problems. What emerged from the
conference was the awareness that considerable differences existed between the two coun-
tries in how instruction in English was viewed. In the United States, English was conceived
of as an academic discipline with specific content to be mastered, whereas the British
focused on the personal and linguistic growth of the child (Appleby, 1974, p. 229). Instead of
focusing on content, “process or activity … defined the English curriculum for the British
teacher” (Appleby, 1974, p. 230), its purpose being to foster the personal development of the
student. As Berlin (1990) noted,
The result of the Dartmouth Conference was to reassert for U.S. teachers the value of the
expressive model of writing. Writing is to be pursued in a free and supportive environment in
which the student is encouraged to engage in an act of self discovery. (p. 210)
This emphasis on the personal and private nature of composing was also manifested in
the recommendations of early composition scholars such as Ken Macrorie, Donald Murray,
Walker Gibson, and Peter Elbow.
What does it actually mean to view writing as a process? Broadly speaking, a process
approach to writing and the teaching of writing means devoting increased attention to
writers and the activities in which writers engage when they create and produce a text, as
opposed to analyzing and attempting to reproduce “model” texts. Reacting against a peda-
gogy oriented toward error correction and formulaic patterns of organization, the process
approach, as it evolved during the 1960s and 1970s, was concerned with discovering how
writers produce texts, developing a model of the writing process, and helping writers find a
process that would enable them to write more effectively and continue to improve as writers.
Although to many writers and teachers the concept of a writing “process” was not news, the
increased emphasis in the classroom on helping students acquire an effective process and
on finding out what successful writers did when they wrote, constituted a new pedagogical
approach and a potentially exciting research direction.
| |
For Writing and Discussion
(Adapted from “Chronotopic Lamination: Tracing the Contours of Literate Activity.” Paul
Prior and Jody Shipka, Writing Selves/Writing Societies, Bazerman & Russell, published
February 1, 2003. http://wac.colostate.edu/books/selves_societies/.)
This is an assignment that will enable you to gain conscious awareness of the process
you use to write papers that are assigned in classes (and the extent to which that process
might be different when you write for other purposes). It is an assignment that you can
also use with your students.
PROCESSES 7
Think about a paper you have written recently for a college class. Then draw two
pictures:
1. The first picture should represent how you actually engaged in writing this particu-
lar piece. It might show a place or places where you did that writing.
2. The second picture should represent the whole writing process for this particular
piece. The picture might show how this writing activity got started, interactions
with other people and other texts, experiences that have influenced the direction
or approach you used, the number and type of drafts, your evaluations of and emo-
tions about this activity at different times.
1. Describe the writing assignment and class for which it was intended. How did you
feel about the assignment? Were you interested in it immediately? Did you find it
difficult or confusing?
2. List as many activities associated with that writing as you possibly can recall. Some
questions to consider:
• What was the first action you performed to complete the writing task? What actions
followed?
• Did you think about the writing when you were involved in other activities (driving
a car, for example)?
• Did you talk with anyone about the topic?
• What sort of revision did you do?
• Were these behaviors typical of what you usually do when you are given a writing
assignment?
• Do you feel your writing process is effective?
• Do you use a different process when you are working on something that you, your-
self, have decided to write? If so, how is it different?
| |
to say. It also suggested that once a writer knew what he or she wanted to write, that part of
the process was complete.
We now know that this linear model of writing as a series of discrete stages does not reflect
what most writers actually do, because writers frequently discover and reconsider ideas dur-
ing, as well as before, they write, moving back and forth between the prewriting, writing,
and revision stages as the text emerges. For example, when I wrote this chapter, I began with
a set of points I planned to discuss, but I modified them many times as I wrote, often revis-
ing sentences and generating additional material as new ideas occurred to me. In addition,
with ease of access to new information, writers now can move from writing to research and
back to writing without ever leaving their desks. Moreover, because every person’s writing
habits are different, an insistence on a lock-step sequence of stages can prove inhibiting,
sometimes paralyzing, to beginning writers. Those who believe that writing cannot occur
until every thought is clarified often delay actually writing until the paper is fully outlined
and developed—or until time has run out and the due date forces the writer to begin. For
some students, the idea that a writer must know exactly what he or she is going to say before
beginning to write can create a writer’s block that actually prevents effective writing from
taking place. Although the idea that writing occurs in stages was a more helpful one than
the previous emphasis on grammatical correctness, when it was interpreted rigidly, this idea
did not provide sufficient insight into the composing processes of actual writers; nor was it
always useful in the classroom or in other venues. As Muriel Harris (1989) argued in the
article at the end of this chapter, “when students write essay exams or placement essays and
when they go on to on-the-job writing where time doesn’t permit multiple drafts, they need
to produce first drafts which are also coherent, finished final drafts.”
the service of justice. Although not all writing is persuasive, awareness of how writer, audi-
ence, and situation interact in the creation of a text enables us to know the world and make
informed choices in the construction of a text. John Gage in “The Reasoned Thesis” pointed
out that Aristotelian rhetoric does not have to be understood strictly in terms of persuasion
or coercion, but rather in terms of inquiry, of helping people discover “mutual grounds for
assent” (Gage, 1996, p. 10). M. J. Killingsworth (2005) showed how rhetoric can enable writ-
ers and readers achieve mutual understanding of the values they share.
Modern rhetoric does not so much replace one authority with another; rather it attacks the
very idea of absolute authority. People who use rhetoric don’t have to be atheists or moral rela-
tivists, but they must realize that the constraints on how we discuss issues are different as we
move from community to community, audience to audience. (p. 13)
In writing about an issue such as whether women should submit to their husband in mar-
riage, Killingsworth pointed out, a student would have to base his or her arguments “on
something more than the Bible” (p. 12). Incorporating rhetoric into a writing class will
enable students to understand how the values of an intended audience impact the direction
and credibility of a text.
10 IRENE L. CLARK
RHETORICAL READING
The process movement focused on the idea that “writing is a process.” However, it is now
recognized that reading is a process as well, and that many students, while able to read
or “decode” words on a page, are unable to understand the texts assigned in their college
classes. The term “rhetorical reading,” associated with the work of Bean, Chappell, and
Gillam (2005), can be defined as “attending to an author’s purposes for writing and the
methods the author uses to accomplish those purposes” (p. xxiii). The concept of rhetorical
reading emphasizes the importance of helping students analyze the rhetorical strategies
used in academic texts and helping students understand “academic writing as a process in
which writers engage with other texts” (p. xxiii). It also includes the concept of “critical”
reading as a means of enabling students to recognize biased perspectives in various texts,
including visual texts such as websites and advertisements.
The idea that reading is a process enables students to approach a text purposefully, not
simply to begin reading passively, sentence by sentence without considering why it was
written and the strategies the writer has used to accomplish a particular purpose. In work-
ing with students in a writing class, I urge them to develop a process of reading that I refer
to as “the three-pass approach,” which consists of the following process-oriented activities:
list would be unlikely to involve serious effort—perhaps the addition of other items, a check
placed next to an item already purchased, or a line through an item that was not needed
after all. More complicated genres or high-stakes genres, however, involve more serious
effort, and the “process” of writing might begin with a reflection about what such a genre
requires. In the reading included at the end of this chapter, Mary Jo Reiff (2006) noted that a
writing assignment, particularly one that requires an unfamiliar genre, generates a number
of genre-based questions. Reiff observed that when she was asked to write about the “genre
of the Pedagogical Insight essay,” she called on her “genre knowledge, her past experience
with similar texts in similar situations” (p. 157) in order to orient herself to the expectations
of the genre. Because she knew that she was not familiar with this genre, she asked herself
several questions, such as “What are the actions that this genre performs? How will I posi-
tion myself within this genre—what identity and relations will I assume?” (p. 157). These
questions then prompted her to consider a potential audience and the social and cultural
situation toward which her response would be directed. A consideration of genre, then,
enabled Reiff to begin the process of writing her essay.
A number of scholars (in particular, see Devitt, 2004) have suggested that an important
goal of a writing class is to help students become aware of the genres they are expected to
produce, an approach that is particularly useful for students whose first language is not
English. Such a genre-based class may focus on studying multiple examples of a genre in
order to discern its features, as well as more usual process-oriented activities, such as writ-
ing multiple drafts and obtaining peer feedback.
| |
For Further Exploration
Early Process-oriented Scholarship
Faigley, L., & Witte, S. (1980). Analyzing revision. CCC, 32, 400–414.
Flower, L., & Hayes, J. (1981). A cognitive process theory of writing. CCC, 32, 354–387.
Kroll, B., & Schafer, J. (1978). Error analysis and the teaching of composition. CCC, 29,
242–248.
Matsuhashi, A. (1981). Pausing and planning: The tempo of written discourse production.
Research in the Teaching of English, 15, 113–134.
Perl, S. (1979). The composing process of unskilled college writers. Research in the Teaching
of English, 13, 317–336.
Perl, S. (1981). Understanding composing. CCC, 31, 363–369.
Pianko, S. (1979). A description of the composing processes of college freshmen writers.
Research in the Teaching of English, 13, 5–22.
Sommers, N. (1980). Revision strategies of student writers and experienced adult writers.
CCC, 31, 378–388.
Rhetoric and the Composition Class
Gage, J. T. (1996). The reasoned thesis: The E-word and argumentative writing as a process
of inquiry. In Barbara Emmel, Paula Resch, & Deborah Tenney (Eds.), Argument revis-
ited; Argument redefined (pp. 3–18). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Killingsworth, M. J. (2005). Appeals in modern rhetoric: An ordinary-language approach.
Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.
Lunsford, A. A. (2007). Writing matters. Athens and London: University of Georgia Press.
Lunsford, A. A., & Glenn, C. (1990). Rhetorical theory and the teaching of writing. In Gail
Hawisher, & Anne Soter (Eds.), On literacy and its teaching (pp. 174–189). Albany: State
University of New York.
| |
| |
For Writing and Discussion
Compare Nancy Sommers’ “The Revision Strategies of Student Writers and Experi-
enced Adult Writers” (in Chapter 3) with Muriel Harris’ essay “Composing Behaviors of
One-and Multi-Draft Writers” at the end of this chapter. Note the similarities and dif-
ferences between their perspectives on the writing process. What are the implications
of these perspectives for working with students in a writing class?
| |
understand how students learn to write, one must understand how these structures develop
as an individual matures and acquires knowledge of the world.
Cognitive psychology perceives linguistic and intellectual ability as developing in a natu-
ral sequence, and it is this concept that has had the most significant impact on the study of
writing acquisition and on how a writing teacher can utilize that sequence in the classroom.
Emig (1971), for example, maintained that the ability to write personal, expressive writing
precedes the ability to write on literary or academic topics. She, therefore, urged teachers
to use more of what she referred to as “reflexive”—that is, personal—writing in the class-
room, based on students’ own experiences and feelings. Beginning with personal topics
before addressing more abstract topics, Emig claimed, fosters students’ cognitive develop-
ment. Whether or not this is absolutely true has not been established. However, in order for
students to engage with a topic, it is useful for them to consider what personal involvement
they might have with it. In my own writing classes, students often write about a personal
experience that is related to the topic before they focus on issues or controversies that may
be developed in an argument essay.
Developmental Models of the Composing Process
This notion that the development of writing ability correlates with human linguistic and
intellectual development resulted in a number of publications that suggested that the Eng-
lish curriculum should parallel the sequence in which that development was presumed to
occur. Deriving from Piaget’s notion of cognitive development, James Moffett’s Teaching the
Universe of Discourse (1968) outlined a theory by which a sequential curriculum in language
arts could be based. Moffett’s system consisted of a progression that moved from the per-
sonal to the impersonal and from low to high levels of abstraction based on two horizontal
scales. The first, the audience scale, organized discourse according to the distance between
the writer and his or her audience, according to four categories (Moffett, 1968b):
Moffett’s second scale, the subject scale, categorized discourse by how far away the writer
or speaker is from the subject being considered. For example, a person may be sitting at a
table in a cafeteria eating lunch, noting what is happening at the given moment. Later on,
he or she might report on what happened in the cafeteria during lunch, generalize about
what usually happens in the cafeteria at lunch, or argue that something might or should
happen in the cafeteria at lunch. These two scales, the audience scale and the subject scale,
Moffett suggested, can be used to help students recreate their experience through language,
enabling them to develop facility in writing. Moffett’s concept of a language arts curricu-
lum based on this sequence was explained in considerable detail in his textbook A Student-
Centered Language Arts Curriculum (1968a).
A British study, The Development of Writing Abilities 11–18, published in 1975 by James
Britton, Tony Burgess, Nancy Martin, Alex McLeod, and Harold Rosen, also included the
14 IRENE L. CLARK
notion of sequential development. Aimed at creating a model that would “characterize all
mature written utterances and then go on to trace the developmental steps that led to them”
(p. 6), this system of Britton and his colleagues categorized all student writing by function—
the transactional, which communicates information to an unknown audience, the expres-
sive, which communicates information to a known audience, and the poetic, which deals
only tangentially with any form of audience. Britton et al. characterized most school writing
as transactional, but argued that because students are most engaged in expressive writing,
this is the type of writing that is most likely to foster the development of writing ability.
Both Moffett’s work and the work of Britton et al., evolving from the theories of Jean
Piaget, addressed the question of how children learn to move beyond their early egocentri-
cism to reach out to an audience, a topic addressed by Linda Flower in her seminal arti-
cle “Writer-Based Prose: A Cognitive Basis for Problems in Writing” (1979). According to
Flower, novice writers often have difficulty transcending their own egocentric perspective to
consider the needs of their intended audience. As a result, their texts are often characterized
by what she refers to as “writer-based prose”—that is, texts in which information is omitted
or inadequately explained, definitions unclarified—in other words, texts that reflect what
might be in their writers’ minds at the time of writing but that have not been sufficiently
contextualized or modified for a reader. Often reflecting the order in which the writer first
generated ideas, writer-based prose may be clear to the writer, but a reader may have dif-
ficulty understanding it. Awareness of writer-based prose in the writing class can be used
to help novice writers transform “writer-based” to “reader-based” prose, enabling them to
develop a better understanding of the concept of audience.
Pursuing a similar direction, Andrea Lunsford, in an article titled “Cognitive Develop-
ment and the Basic Writer” (1979), claimed that the difficulties novice or “basic” writers
have with writing are because of their not having reached a level of cognitive development
that would enable them to form abstractions. To remedy this problem, Lunsford suggested
a variety of workshop activities focusing on analysis and active thinking. During that same
year (1979), Sharon Pianko published “A Description of the Composing Processes of College
Freshman Writers,” in which she claimed that the composing process of basic writers is less
developed than that of more skillful writers.
The Work of Flower and Hayes
Several articles by Flower and Hayes continued to explore the writing process based on the-
ories of cognitive psychology. Concerned with avoiding models that suggest that the writing
process is linear, Flower and Hayes (1981) set up a cognitive theory based on four points:
1. The process of writing is best understood as a set of distinctive think processes that writers
orchestrate or organize during the act of composing.
2. These processes have a hierarchical, highly embedded organization in which any process
can be embedded within any other.
3. The act of composing itself is a goal-directed thinking process, guided by the writer’s own
growing network of goals.
4. Writers create their own goals in two key ways: by generating both high-level goals and sup-
porting subgoals that embody the writer’s developing sense of purpose, and then, at times,
by changing major goals or even establishing new ones based on what has been learned in
the act of writing. (p. 366)
PROCESSES 15
Flower and Hayes also focused on the role of problem definition in the writing process,
noting in their article “The Cognition of Discovery: Defining a Rhetorical Problem” (1980)
that although a teacher may give “20 students the same assignment, the writers themselves
create the problem they solve” (p. 23). Using the technique of “protocols” (having students
speak aloud as they compose), Flower and Hayes constructed a model of the rhetorical prob-
lem itself, which consisted of two major units: the rhetorical situation, which consists of the
“writer’s given,” including the audience and the assignment, and the set of goals that the
writer creates for himself. The four goals they noted involved the reader, creating a persona
or voice, building a meaning, and producing a formal text, which, as they point out, “closely
parallel the four terms of the communication triangle: reader, writer, world, word” (p. 25).
Enabling students to use their own powers, to make discoveries, to take alternative paths. It
does not suggest that the world can best be examined by a set of rules … The program gives
the student first, freedom to find his voice and let his subjects find him … for both teacher and
student, a constant reading for truth, in writing and commenting on that writing. This is a
hard requirement, for no one speaks truth consistently. (pp. vii–viii)
For Macrorie, good writers speak in honest voices and tell some kind of truth, a perspective
similar to that of Donald Stewart (1972), for whom the most important goal of a writing
course was for students to engage in a process of self-discovery, manifested in the student’s
text by the appearance in the text of an authentic voice; Donald Graves (1983) also referred
to voice as infusing a text with the writer’s presence. Perhaps the best-known proponent of
the concept of voice, Peter Elbow (1986), although acknowledging the difficulty of defin-
ing voice, nevertheless viewed the discovery of voice as a necessary prerequisite of growth.
“I can grow or change,” Elbow maintained, but “not unless I start out inhabiting my own
voice or style. … In short, I need to accept myself as I am before I can tap my power or
start to grow” (p. 204). Voice, for Elbow, constitutes both a motivating force and a source of
power. In its emphasis on empowering the inner self, the expressivist approach to writing is
sometimes referred to as the “romantic” school of writing, in contrast to the “classical” or
“cognitive” school, which view writing in terms of intellectual development as manifested
in problem solving.
Of course, there is no reason that the “process” approach should be so closely connected to
expressivist writing. As Ladd Tobin (1994) points out, “a teacher could assign a personal essay
16 IRENE L. CLARK
but ignore the writing process or assign a critical analysis yet nurture the process” (p. 6). Nev-
ertheless, process and personal or expressivist writing were often associated with one another
in the early days of the process movement, and today the expressionists remain “a force in
rhetoric and composition studies. Such figures as Peter Elbow, Donald Murray, Ken Macrorie
… continue to explore writing as a private and personal act. It is this group that continues to
insist on the importance of the individual against the demands of institutional conformity,
holding out for the personal as the source of all value” (Berlin, 1990, pp. 218–219).
SOCIAL CONSTRUCTIONISM
Although cognitive and expressivist approaches to composition dominated scholarship
during the 1970s and 1980s, during the mid-1980s, publications began to appear that ques-
tioned both the validity and the utility of focusing on the individual. Patricia Bizzell’s “Cog-
nition, Convention, and Certainty: What We Need to Know About Writing” (1983) argued
that writers are not autonomous individuals, distinct and removed from culture, but, rather,
that individual consciousness is shaped by culture through language. This perspective
implied that all writers, even when they are presumably composing only for themselves
or writing notes for a subsequent piece of writing, are mentally influenced by the “inner
speech,” as Vygotsky (1978) referred to it, that develops in response to a particular culture’s
concept of language and thought. From this perspective, then, writing is socially constructed
because it both reflects and shapes thinking, a position that in composition studies is known
as social constructionism. Social constructionist approaches to composition emphasize the
role of community in shaping discourse and the importance of understanding community
expectations when working with students. Bizzell’s article (1983), for example, pointed out
that although Flower and Hayes’ cognitive-based model described how writing occurs, it
focused too strongly on the individual writer and did not help composition teachers “advise
students on difficult questions of practice” (p. 222).
Social constructionist approaches to composition derive from perspectives in philoso-
phy, as well as other fields, that emphasize the importance of community consensus in
determining knowledge. This view is based on the idea that individuals perceive the world
according to the shared beliefs and perceptions of the community or communities to which
they belong; it is one that writers in fields such as history or ethnography have supported
for some time. The philosopher, Richard Rorty, for example, in Philosophy and the Mirror of
Nature (1979), maintained that knowledge is a “socially constructed belief,” a viewpoint that
straddles a middle position between absolute relativism, in which an individual may choose
to believe anything, and the positivist notion of objective truth derived from an absolute
reality. The anthropologist, Clifford Geertz (1983), similarly argued that modern conscious-
ness is an “enormous multiplicity” of cultural values, and Charles Bazerman (1981) has
emphasized the role of the scientific community in shaping the writing of scientists.
In composition, social constructionist approaches have been associated with the work of
David Bartholomae, Kenneth Bruffee, Patricia Bizzell, and James Berlin, who all focused on
the social context of writing and the role of community in determining the appropriateness
and effectiveness of a text. Kenneth Bruffee’s “Collaborative Learning and the ‘Conversation
of Mankind’ ” (1984) maintained that every person is born into the “conversation of man-
kind” and that it is this conversation that gives meaning and value to what we do, influenc-
ing both thinking and writing. “We can think because we can talk, and we think in ways
PROCESSES 17
we have learned to talk” (p. 87), Bruffee explained. For writing teachers, it is important to
realize (Bruffee, 1984):
that our task must involve engaging students in conversation among themselves at as many
points in both the writing and the reading process as possible and that we should contrive to
ensure that students’ conversation about what they read and write is similar in as many ways
as possible to the way we would like them eventually to read and write. The way they talk with
each other determines the way they will think and the way they will write. (p. 89)
Collaborative Learning
Social constructionism has often associated with collaborative learning, although this ped-
agogical approach has been around for some time. Bruffee’s work acknowledged the use of
collaborative learning in teaching medical students through group diagnosis, and Albert
DeCiccio (1988) has noted its association with progressive education and the educational
approach of John Dewey. Moreover, as Bruffee (1984) has noted, the uses of collaborative
learning in the composition class in the 1970s derived more from practical need than from
a particular philosophical or theoretical perspective on learning. Because open admission
policies brought a large number of students to the university who were educationally dis-
advantaged, collaborative learning developed as a means of enabling students to assist one
another. As Bruffee (1984) pointed out, “For American college teachers, the roots of collab-
orative learning lie neither in radical politics nor in research.” Rather it was based primarily
on “a pressing educational need” (p. 637).
Whatever its roots, the concept of collaborative learning has significant implications for
what occurs in the writing class. Most important, it implies a decentering of the writing
class, a balancing of authority between students and teacher, so that students can participate
in their own learning through peer editing and writing groups. This idea, as Bruffee pointed
out, is actually quite similar to the work of the early process theorists such as Peter Elbow,
who has long advocated the effectiveness of the “teacherless” classroom and the necessity of
delegating authority to writing groups. According to this perspective, students learn only
when they have assumed responsibility for their own learning. They do not develop writing
skills by listening to a teacher lecture about the writing process. Rather they must engage
fully in that process themselves, working together with peers.
Wendy Bishop (1997), in “Helping Peer Writing Groups Succeed,” emphasized the
importance of carefully scripting and modeling what is expected from peer writing groups.
Below are some additional suggestions for maximizing the value of collaborative activities
in the writing class:
• Model the activity by first engaging in it yourself in front of the class. Before putting
students into groups for peer editing, ask students to volunteer a paper to be edited, or
use one from another class. Make copies for the class and demonstrate how you expect
students to proceed.
• Determine the procedures for group work, such as whether students should read papers
silently or aloud, how many copies of the paper students should bring in, how much time
should be allotted for each paper, and the sort of comments that should be encouraged.
• Assign the groups yourself through random selection. If students choose their own
groups, they may spend the time socializing instead of working. To enable the groups
to develop a productive working relationship, keep the groups constant throughout the
semester, unless there is a good reason for changing them.
• For peer editing, develop assignment-specific questions (see the following “For Writing
and Discussion” for an example).
• When possible, require students to report their discussion results to the class. This
works well when students engage in group research because it requires them to take
responsibility for their work. They should be aware that they will be standing in front of
the class and that inadequate preparation will be apparent to everyone!
| |
For Writing and Discussion
Writing Assignment
One important insight arising from research into the composing process is that no
approach or strategy is appropriate for all writers and that, as Muriel Harris points out,
“there is a very real danger in imposing a single ‘ideal’ composing style on students.” In
fact, the more we engage in research about the composing process, the more we realize
how much more we need to learn.
As a writer and a student of writing, you have learned a great deal about the writing
process from your own experiences, some of them useful, some less so, and it is your
writing background that will serve as the primary resource for this assignment. Think
about your own history as a writer—your classroom successes and failures, teachers
and writers who have influenced you, assignments you have completed, and strategies
you have developed as you grappled with various types of writing assignments through-
out your academic career. Then in an essay of three to five pages, respond to the fol-
lowing question:
PROCESSES 19
Based on your experience as a writer and a student, what insights into the com-
posing process do you consider to be the most important?
In discussing insights you have gained, please enliven your prose with specific exam-
ples and anecdotes.
First draft: 3 copies due in class on __. First drafts must be typed.
Final draft due on __.
Peer Editing Question Sheet
Name of Writer _____ Feedback Provided by _____
1. Does the essay respond to the prompt—that is, what insights into the composing
process does the writer consider most important?
2. Examine the introduction. What function does it serve? To attract attention? To
indicate a direction for the essay? How does the introduction prepare the reader for
other sections in the essay?
3. Examine the essay for support. What are the main ideas or themes? What specific
examples, anecdotes, or explanations are used to support these ideas or themes?
4. Examine the essay for coherence. What strategies are used to connect each para-
graph with the next?
5. Examine the essay for style and sentence structure. What specific words does the
writer use to illustrate main points? What kind of person do you sense behind the
prose? How does the writer make this person seem real? How does the writer use
sentence structure to develop the essay? Is the essay’s style sufficiently varied? Are
the sentences choppy?
6. What has the writer done to make the essay interesting? What did you learn from
this essay? What did the writer do to enhance the audience appeal of this essay?
| |
Collaborating Online
Recently, increased access to new media has enabled students to collaborate virtually—
sharing ideas and drafts online, working in groups on various projects without meeting
face to face. Beach, Anson, Breuch, and Swiss (2009) discuss a number of options for using
“digital discussion environments to enhance writing instruction and student engagement”
(p. 48). Defining “digital discussion environments” as those that promote conversation and
collaboration, these authors provide an overview of both synchronous (real-time) discus-
sions, used when students are on the computer at the same time, and asynchronous discus-
sions, when students write separate messages at different times. Synchronous discussions
tend to be more conversational and are sometimes used even when all students are in a
classroom; asynchronous discussions occur when students are not in the same location and
can serve as a written transcript of an ongoing discussion. Both can be useful in enabling
students to learn from one another and build upon one another’s ideas.
Online writing and collaboration can take many forms: having students respond to
a reading, discovering a purpose for writing, finding additional research, responding to
drafts, discussing ideas for a project, or enabling students to become aware of audience.
Whatever purpose it might serve, however, planning is crucial, just as it is in a face-to-face
classroom. When they engage in a digital collaboration, students should have a clear idea
of what they are expected to do or learn in the context of a particular assignment or task.
20 IRENE L. CLARK
| |
For Further Exploration
Social Constructionism
Bartholomae, D., & Petrosky, A. (1986). Facts, artifacts, and counterfacts. Portsmouth, NH:
Heinemann.
Bazerman, C. (1981). What written knowledge does: Three examples of academic discourse.
Philosophy of the Social Sciences, 11, 361–387.
Bruffee, K. Social construction, language and the authority of knowledge. College English,
48, 773–790.
Geertz, C. (1983). Local knowledge. New York: Basic Books.
Harris, J. (1989). The idea of community in the study of writing. CCC, 40, 11–22.
Rorty, R. (1979). Philosophy and the mirror of nature. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University
Press.
Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society (M. Cole, V. John Steiner, S. Scribner, & E. Souberman
Eds.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Collaborative Learning
Bizzell, P. (1983). Cognition, convention, and certainty: What we need to know about writ-
ing. PRETEXT, 3, 213–243.
Brooke, R. (1991). Writing and sense of self: Identity negotiation in writing workshops.
Urbana, IL: NCTE.
Brooke, R., Mirtz, R., & Evans, R. (1994). Small groups in writing workshops. Urbana, IL:
NCTE.
Bruffee, K. A. (1984, November). Collaborative learning and the “conversation of mankind.”
College English, 46, 635–652.
Elbow, P. (1973). Writing without teachers. New York: Oxford University Press.
Forman, J. (Ed.). (1992). New visions of collaborative writing. Portsmouth, NH: Boynton/
Cook.
George, D. (1984) Working with peer groups in the composition classroom. CCC, 35,
320–326.
Gere, A. R. (1987). Writing groups: History, theory, and implications. Carbondale, IL: South-
ern Illinois University Press.
Lunsford, A., & Ede, L. (1990). Singular texts/plural authors: Perspectives on collaborative
writing. Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois University Press.
Mason, E. (1970). Collaborative learning. London: Ward Lock.
Smith, D. (1989). Some difficulties with collaborative learning. Journal of Advanced Com-
position, 9, 45–57.
Spellmeyer, K. (1994). On conventions and collaboration: The open road and the iron cage.
In J. Clifford, & J. Schilb (Eds.), Writing theory and critical theory. New York: MLA.
Trimbur, J. (1989, October). Consensus and difference in collaborative learning. College
English, 51, 602–616.
Collaborating Online
Beach, R., Anson, C. Breuch L. B., & Swiss, T. (2009). Teaching writing using blogs, wikis,
and other digital tools. Norwood, MA: Christopher-Gordon.
Gane, N., & Beer, D. (2008). New media. Oxford: Berg.
Wysocki, A. F., Johnson-Eilola, J., & Selfe, C. L. (2004). Writing new media: Theory and
applications for expanding the teaching of composition. Logan, UT: Utah State University
Press.
| |
PROCESSES 21
Post-Process Theory
Concern about the effectiveness and validity of the process movement has been manifested
in literature concerned with “post-process theory,” a term that refers to the idea that the
process movement is no longer pertinent, either theoretically or pedagogically. In Post-
Process Theory: Beyond the Writing-Process Paradigm (1999), Thomas Kent asserted that
because every communication act requires the writer to “guess” how a text will affect a
reader, it is impossible to model communication, and, therefore, there is no writing pro-
cess that can be presented in the classroom. “No single course can teach a student how to
produce or analyze discourse,” Kent maintains, “for the hermeneutic guessing required in
22 IRENE L. CLARK
all discourse production and analysis can be only refined; it cannot be codified and then
taught” (p. 39). In his essay in that collection, Gary Olson (1999) took this position further,
asserting not only that writing cannot be taught but also that it cannot even be adequately
described. To construct a model of the writing process, Olson states, is to be in conflict
with the postmodern rejection of certainty and the corresponding emphasis on assertion
as a valued academic skill. For Olson, “the vocabulary of process is no longer useful” (p. 9),
and, therefore, compositionists must move away from “a discourse of mastery and asser-
tion toward a more dialogic, dynamic, open-ended, receptive, nonassertive stance” (p. 14).
Olson, however, did not suggest how this perspective can benefit students or be applied in a
pedagogical context.
Another criticism of the process movement focused on the emphasis on formula with
which the writing process has been presented in the classroom. Joseph Petraglia and David
Russell both rejected the rigidity of a “prewrite, write, revise, edit” model, Petraglia noting
in “Is There Life after Process?” (1999) that composition scholarship seems to be increas-
ingly irrelevant to working effectively with students. Russell, embracing an activity theory
orientation, emphasized that there are many writing processes. Instead of teaching one pro-
cess, Russell (1999) maintains, compositionists study those various processes to “(re)classify
them, commodify them, and involve students with (teach) them in a curriculum” (p. 88)
that acknowledges that some writing activities can be performed mechanistically, whereas
others cannot. As yet, post-process theory has not been applied to effective classroom teach-
ing. However, for an excellent and enjoyable review of post-process scholarship, read Rich-
ard Fulkerson’s “Of Pre-and Post-Process: Reviews and Ruminations” (2001).
Many working-class, migrant and Aboriginal children have been systematically barred from
competence with those texts, knowledges, and ‘genres’ that enable access to social and mate-
rial resources. The culprits, they argue, are not limited to traditional pedagogies that disregard
children’s cultural and linguistic resources and set out to assimilate them into the fictions
of mainstream culture. But the problem is also located in progressive ‘process’ and ‘child-
centered’ approaches that appear to ‘value differences but in so doing leave social relations of
inequity fundamentally unquestioned. (p. vii)
Contributors to Cope and Kalantzis’ book The Powers of Literacy: A Genre Approach to
Teaching Writing (1993), which is intended to explain the Australian genre approach to
PROCESSES 23
non-Australians, have maintained that the emphasis on personal voice and the correspond-
ing reluctance of teachers to intervene directly in changing students’ texts has, ironically,
promoted a situation in which only the brightest middle-class children, those whose voices
are already in tune with those in power and whose backgrounds include acquaintanceship
with the genres of privilege, will be able to learn what is needed for social, and, ultimately,
economic success. Noting that “by the 1980s it was clear that the new progressivist curricu-
lum was not producing any noticeable improvement in patterns of educational attainment”
(Cope & Kalantzis, 1993, p. 1), Cope and Kalantzis also pointed out that such a curriculum
“encourages students to produce texts in a limited range of written genres, mostly person-
alised recounts” (p. 6).
It is clear that the process movement has not solved every problem associated with help-
ing students to learn to write. Nevertheless, it was characterized by an intellectual and
moral energy that generated an exciting new discipline and an important ideological focus
that continues to influence composition pedagogy at various levels. Although in many insti-
tutions the concepts of expressive writing and personal voice have been superseded by other
academic emphases, the idea that writing is a recursive process and that teaching writing
means helping students develop a process that works effectively for them now constitutes a
basis for writing curricula across the country.
| |
For Further Exploration
Process
Bartholomae, D. (1980). The study of error. College Composition and Communication, 31,
253–269.
Daniell, B. (1994). Theory, theory talk, and composition. In J. Clifford, & J. Schilb (Eds.),
Writing theory and critical theory (pp. 127–140). New York: MLA.
Dyer, P. M. (1990). What composition theory offers the writing teacher. In L. A. Arena (Ed.),
Language proficiency: Defining, teaching, and testing (pp. 99–106). New York: Plenum.
Elbow, P. (1973). Writing without teachers. New York: Oxford University Press.
Elbow, P. (1981). Writing with power: Techniques for mastering the writing process. New
York: Oxford UP.
Elbow, P. (1981). Embracing contraries: Explorations in learning and teaching. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Elbow, P. (1991). Reflections on academic discourse. College English, 53, 135–155.
Faigley, L. (1986). Competing theories of process: A critique and a proposal. College English,
48, 527–542.
Hairston, M. (1982). The winds of change: Thomas Kuhn and the revolution in the teaching
of writing. College Composition and Communication, 33, 76–86.
Knoblauch, C. H., & Brannon, L. (1984). Rhetorical traditions and the teaching of writing.
Upper Montclair, NJ: Boynton Cook.
Scardamalia, M., Bereiter, C., & Goelman, H. The role of production factors in writing abil-
ity. In M. Nystrand (Ed.), What writers know: The language, process, and structures of
academic discourse (pp. 173–210). New York: Academic.
Selzer, J. (1984). Exploring options in composing. College Composition and Communication,
35, 276–284.
| |
24 IRENE L. CLARK
| |
Writing Assignment
Understanding Yourself as a Writer1
Although all writers engage in planning, drafting, and revising, they do so in a variety
of ways and no writer approaches every writing task the same way. One way to move
away from writing habits that have not worked well for you in the past is to understand
what type of writer you are. Think about the models of writers described here. In the
space provided, indicate the extent to which this description pertains to your own writ-
ing habits.
Writers Who Plan in Advance. These people tend to think about their ideas and plan
their writing in their minds or on paper before they begin to write. As a result, their first
drafts are usually better than most first drafts often are, even though these drafts will
probably still need additional revision before they can be submitted. People who plan
in advance use spare moments of their time to think—while they eat lunch, drive, and
wait in line. When they start to write, they usually have at least some idea about what
they want to say.
Writers Who Discover Ideas Through Writing. Although every writer discovers ideas
through writing, at least sometimes, these writers use writing to find out what they
want to say, planning and revising while they write. They begin by writing whatever
comes into their minds and then reconsider and rewrite again and again. Writers who
discover ideas through writing may even throw out a whole first draft and begin again.
Writers Who Spend Equal Time on Planning, Writing, and Revising. Some writers divide
the writing task into stages, a method that is probably the most effective in writing a
college essay as long as adequate time is allowed for each stage of the process. However,
it is important to keep in mind that an effective plan involves more than a few notes
scratched on a sheet of paper, that sufficient time must be allowed for drafting, and that
a revision means a great deal more than correcting a few sentences.
Writers Who Delay. This is a familiar type of writer—those who delay writing until
right before the paper is due. These writers sometimes stay up all night, trying to write
a complete draft of a paper, and, thus, when they submit their work, they are really
submitting a first draft, not usually their best work. Of course, for various reasons, all
writers probably procrastinate sometimes. But those who always write first drafts under
conditions of pressure or panic are unlikely to do as well as they could.
| |
| |
Writing Assignment
Assessing Your Own Writing Process
This self-assessment will enable you to look at your own writing process, to evaluate
the strategies you use when you write. Try to answer each question as fully as possible.
PROCESSES 25
1. Mark the scale below to indicate how easy or difficult you find the process of
writing.
Easy _________________________________________________________ Difficult
Please explain your selection, indicating the reasons for your response.
2. Describe how you usually write your papers.
3. What practices or “rules of writing” do you find to be useful? Which do you find to
be phony or not useful?
4. Which of the following problems have you experienced?
• Beginning the paper
• Knowing what you want to say
• Writing an introduction
• Getting a first sentence
• Finding a thesis
• Organizing the paper
• Deciding how to structure your ideas
• Writing an outline
• Addressing the needs of your audience
• Knowing what your reader wants
• Deciding what information to include
• Working on coherence and style
• Connecting each paragraph to your overall point
• Writing sentences that connect with one another
• Making your writing lively and interesting to read
• Finding the right words.
5. Based on your responses to the previous questions, what changes would you like to
make in your writing behavior?
| |
| |
Writing Assignment
Part I. Describe the writing class you observed, including many specific details such as
the name of the teacher, the level of the class, how often the class meets, the number of
students, the textbook used, the assignment the students are working on, the handouts
that were used, the seating arrangement—any details that will enable a reader to under-
stand what the teacher and students were doing during various segments of the class.
Part II. Discuss how observing these classes has provided you with information that
you will use to plan your own writing class. Specific points to address in this section
include the teacher, students, classroom dynamics, methodology, and materials. What
26 IRENE L. CLARK
did you learn in this class from both the teacher and the students that will be helpful
to you?
Note that your purpose is NOT to evaluate the teacher or the methods in any way,
but rather to reflect on what you have learned and can apply to your own teaching.
Classroom Observation Guidelines
1. Select the “teacher volunteer” whose class you plan to observe.
2. Plan to observe at least three successive class meetings, or a total of four class meet-
ings over a three-week period.
3. Contact the teacher (via telephone, e-mail, or mail) and make certain that you will
be the only observer in class on the days of your visits. Request that he or she pro-
vides you with a copy of the course syllabus and any other important handouts
when you observe your first class.
4. Find out if the teacher has any preference regarding your “role” as observer in his/
her class. For example, should you get up and walk around if group work is in prog-
ress or remain in your seat and listen in on the closest group?
5. During your first observation, take as many notes as you feel are necessary to pro-
vide you with a full record of your experience.
6. During your second and subsequent observations, focus on what you consider to be
the most informative and important insights you have gained.
7. If possible, have a short “debriefing” session with the instructor when you have
finished the sequence of observations.
8. Be sure to arrive on time for the class. Do not leave until the class has ended.
Points to Note When Observing Composition Classes
Physical Arrangement of Class. How seats are arranged and placement of teacher.
Atmosphere in Classroom/Classroom Dynamics. Formal? Informal? Friendly?
The Lesson. The day’s agenda, the day’s topic, specific skills to be taught, activities
planned (group work, writing/thinking activities, revision, invention, and exercises),
sequencing of activities, materials used, quality of discussion, and applicability of les-
son to student writing/language skills.
Student Behaviors
• What kind of writing are the students doing?
• How many students are participating?
• What are students doing who are not participating?
• Are students reading their work aloud?
• Are students speaking to one another?
• Are students working in groups?
• How are students reacting to the day’s agenda and topic?
Teacher Behaviors
• What sort of voice does the teacher use?
• Has the teacher engaged the students?
• What kind of working relationship does the teacher have with the students?
PROCESSES 27
Overall, you should be aware of how writing and reading are addressed in the class,
how students are engaged, with the instructor and with each other, how the lesson has
been planned, and how much student participation drives and supports the action of
the class.
| |
| |
Writing Assignment
Keeping a Reflective Writing Journal
A useful strategy for gaining insight into your own writing process is to keep a reflective
writing journal. A reflective writing journal will enable you to recall writing experi-
ences in your past, from which you can develop your own “theory” of writing. Insights
fostered by this means will be useful for helping your students to develop an effective
writing process. The following are some suggestions for entries into your reflective writ-
ing journal:
1. Discuss your history as a writer, focusing on particular highlights.
2. Describe your own writing process.
3. Write about one of your best teachers.
4. Find some examples of what you feel is “good writing.” What makes this writing
good?
5. Why have you chosen to study rhetoric and composition?
28 IRENE L. CLARK
NOTE
1 Th is form is based on a discussion of various types of writers in Lisa Ede. (1995). Work in Progress (3rd ed.). Bos-
ton/New York: St. Martin’s Press.
REFERENCES
Appleby, A. N. (1974). Tradition and reform in the teaching of English: A history. Urbana IL: NCTE.
Atwell, N. (1988). A special writer at work. In T. Newkirk and N. Atwell (Eds.), Understanding writing: Ways of learn-
ing, observing, and teaching K–8 (2nd ed.). Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Bazerman, C. (1981). What written knowledge does. Th ree examples of academic discourse. Philosophy of the Social
Sciences, 11, 361–388.
Beach, R. Anson, C. Breuch L. B., & Swiss, T. (2009). Teaching writing using blogs, wikis, and other digital tools. Nor-
wood, MA: Christopher-Gordon.
Bean, J. C., Chappell, V. A., & Gillam, Alice (2005). Reading rhetorically: A reader for writers. New York: Pearson
Longman.
Berlin, J. (1990). Writing instruction in school and college English. In J. Murphy (Ed.), A short history of writing
instruction. Davis, CA: Hermagoras Press.
Berlin, J. (1996). Rhetorics, poetics, and cultures: Refiguring English studies. Urbana, IL: NCTE.
Bishop, W. (1997). Helping peer writing groups succeed. In Teaching lives: Essays and stories (pp. 14–24). Logan, UT:
Utah State University Press.
Bizzell, P. (1983). Cognition, convention, and certainty: What we need to know about writing. PRETEXT, 3, 213–243.
Britton, J., Burgess, T., Martin, N., McLeod, A., & Rosen, H. (1975). The development of writing abilities 11–18. London:
Macmillan Education Ltd.
Bruffee, K. A. (1984, November). Collaborative learning and the “conversation of mankind.” College English, 46,
635–652.
Bruner, J. (1966). Toward a theory of instruction. Cambridge, MA: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.
Clark, I. L. (2010). College argument: Understanding the genre. Dubuque, IA: Kendall Hunt.
Cope, B., & Kalantzis, M. (Eds.). (1993). The powers of literacy: A genre approach to teaching writing. Pittsburgh, PA:
University of Pittsburgh Press.
De Ciccio, A. (1988). Social constructionism and collaborative learning: Recommendations for teaching writing. CCCC
paper: ERIC ED 282212.
Devitt, A. J. (2004). Writing genres. Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois Press.
Dorsey-Gaines, C., & Taylor, D. (1988). Growing up literate: Learning from inner-city families. Portsmouth, NH:
Heinemann.
Elbow, P. (1986). Embracing contraries: Explorations in learning and teaching. New York: Oxford University Press.
Emig, J. (1971). The composing process of twelft h graders. Research report No. 13. Urbana: NCTE.
PROCESSES 29
Flower, L. (1979, September). Writer-based prose: A cognitive basis for problems in writing. College English, 41, 19–38.
Flower, L., & Hayes, J. (1981). A cognitive process theory of writing. College Composition and Communication, 32,
365–388.
Flower, L. S., & Hayes, J. (1980, February). The cognition of discovery: Defi ning a rhetorical problem. CCC, 31, 21–32.
Fulkerson, R. (2001). Of pre- and post-process: Reviews and ruminations. Composition Studies, 29 (2), 93–119.
Gage, J. T. (1996). The reasoned thesis: The E-word and argumentative writing as a process of inquiry. In Barbara
Emmel, Paula Resch, & Deborah Tenney (Eds.), Argument revisited: Argument redefined (pp. 3–18). Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Geertz, C. (1983). Local knowledge. New York: Basic Books.
Graves, D. (1983). Writing, teachers and children at work. Portsmouth: Heinemann.
Greenough, C. N. (1913, February). An experiment in the training of teachers of composition for work with college
freshmen. The English Journal, 2 (2), 109–121.
Harris, J. (1997). A teaching subject: Composition since 1966. New Jersey: Prentice Hall.
Harris, M. (1989, February). Composing behavior, of one- and multi-draft writers. College English, 51 (2), 174–190.
Heath, S. B. (1983). Ways with words: Language, life, and work communities and classrooms. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Hopkins, L. T. (1912, January). Can good composition teaching be done under present conditions? English Journal, 1,
1–12.
Kent, T. (1999). Post-process theory: Beyond the writing-process paradigm. Carbondale: Southern Illinois University
Press.
Killingsworth, M. J. (2005). Appeals in modern rhetoric: An ordinary-language approach. Carbondale: Southern Illi-
nois University Press.
Kinneavy, J. (1971). A theory of discourse. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. (Reprinted 1980. New York: Norton.)
Lunsford, A. (1979). Cognitive development and the basic writer. College English, 41, 38–46.
Lunsford, A. A. (2007). Writing matters. Athens and London: University of Georgia Press.
Lunsford, A. A., & Glenn, C. (1990). Rhetorical theory and the teaching of writing. In Gail Hawisher, & Anne Soter
(Eds.), On literacy and its teaching (pp. 174–189). Albany: State University of New York.
Macrorie, K. (1970). Telling writing. Rochelle Park, NJ: Hayden.
Moffett, J. (1968a). Student-centered language arts curriculum, grades K-13. Boston: Houghton-Miffl in.
Moffett, J. (1968b). Teaching the universe of discourse. Portsmouth, NH: Boynton/Cook.
Neff, J. (1994). Learning disabilities and the writing center. In J. A. Mullin, & R. Wallace (Eds.), Intersections: Theory-
practice in the writing center (pp. 81–95). Urbana, IL: NCTE.
Olson, G. (1999). Toward a post-process composition: Abandoning the rhetoric of assertion. In T. Kent (Ed.), Post-
process theory: Beyond the writing-process paradigm (pp. 7–15). Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.
Perl, S. (1979). The composing process of unskilled college writers. Research in the Teaching of English, 13, 317–336.
Perl, S. (1994). Writing process: A shining moment. In S. Perl (Ed.), Landmark essays on writing process (pp. xi–xx).
Davis, CA: Hermagoras Press.
Petraglia. J. (1999). Is there life after process? The role of social scientism in a changing discipline. In T. Kent (Ed.), Post-
process theory: Beyond the writing-process paradigm (pp. 49–64). Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.
Pianko, S. (1979). A description of the composing processes of college freshman writers. Research in the Teaching of
English, 13, 5–22.
Richardson, P. W. (1994). Language as personal resource and as social construct: Competing views of literacy peda-
gogy in Australia. In A. Freedman, & P. Medway (Eds.), Learning and teaching genre (pp. 117–142). Portsmouth,
NH: Boynton/Cook.
Rohman, D. G. (1965). Pre-writing: The stage of discovery in the writing process. College Composition and Communi-
cation, 16, 106–112.
Rorty, R. (1979). Philosophy and the mirror of nature. New Jersey: Princeton University Press.
Rose, M. (1989). Lives on the boundary: The struggles and achievements of America’s underprepared. New York: Free
Press.
Russell, D. (1999). Activity theory and process approaches: Writing (power) in school and society. In T. Kent (Ed.),
Post-process theory: Beyond the writing-process paradigm (pp. 80–95). Carbondale: Southern Illinois University
Press.
Sommers, N. (1980). Revision strategies of student writers and experienced adult writers. College Composition and
Communication, 31 (4), 378–388.
Stewart, D. C. (1972). The authentic voice: A prewriting approach to student writing. Dubuque, IA: William C. Brown.
Tobin, L. (1994). How the writing process was born—and other conversion narratives. In T. Newkirk, & L. Tobin
(Eds.), Taking stock: The writing process movement in the ‘90s. Portsmouth, NH: Boynton/Cook.
Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society (M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, S. Scrivner, & E. Souberman, Eds). Cambridge, MA.
Harvard University Press.
Williams, J. D. (1998). Preparing to teach writing: Research, theory, and practice (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Readings
Muriel Harris
A belief shared by teachers of writing, one that we fervently try to inculcate in our students, is that
revision can improve writing. This notion, that revision generally results in better text, often pairs
up with another assumption, that revision occurs as we work through separate drafts. Thus, “hand
in your working drafts tomorrow and the final ones next Friday” is a common assignment, as is the
following bit of textbook advice: “When the draft is completed, a good critical reading should help
the writer re-envision the essay and could very well lead to substantial rewriting” (Axelrod and
Cooper 10). This textbook advice, hardly atypical, is based on the rationale that gaining distance
from a piece of discourse helps the writer to judge it more critically. As evidence for this assump-
tion. Richard Beach’s 1976 study of the self-evaluation strategies of revisers and non-revisers dem-
onstrated that extensive revisers were more capable of detaching themselves and gaining aesthetic
distance from their writing than were non-revisers. Nancy Sommers’ later theoretical work on
revision also sensitized us to students’ need to re-see their texts rather than to view revision as an
editing process at the limited level of word changes.
A logical conclusion, then, is to train student writers to re-see and then re-draft a piece of
discourse. There are other compelling reasons for helping students view first or working drafts
as fluid and not yet molded into final form. The opportunities for outside intervention, through
teacher critiques and suggestions or peer evaluation sessions, can be valuable. And it is equally
important to help students move beyond their limited approaches and limiting tendency to settle
for whatever rolls out on paper the first time around. The novice view of a first draft as written-
in-stone (or fast-drying cement) can preclude engaging more fully with the ideas being expressed.
On the other hand, we have to acknowledge that there are advantages in being able, where it is
appropriate, to master the art of one-draft writing. When students write essay exams or place-
ment essays and when they go on to on-the-job writing where time doesn’t permit multiple drafts,
they need to produce first drafts which are also coherent, finished final drafts. Yet, even acknowl-
edging that need, we still seem justified in advocating that our students master the art of redrafting
to shape a text into a more effective form.
The notion that reworking a text through multiple drafts and/or visible changes is generally
a beneficial process is also an underlying assumption in some lines of research. This had been
particularly evident in studies of computer-aided revision, where counts were taken of changes in
macrostructure and microstructure with and without word processing. If more changes were made
on a word processor than were written by hand, the conclusion was that word processors are an
aid to revision. Such research is based on the premise that revision equals visible changes in a text
and that these changes will improve the text.
Given this widely entrenched notion of redrafting as being advantageous, it would be comforting
to turn to research results for clearcut evidence that reworking of text produces better writing.
But studies of revision do not provide the conclusive picture that we need in order to assert that
we should continue coaxing our students into writing multiple drafts. Lillian Bridwell’s 1980 survey
of revision studies led her to conclude that “questions about the relationship between revision and
qualitative improvement remain largely unanswered” (199), and her own study demonstrated that
the most extensively revised papers “received a range of quality ratings from the top to the bottom
of the scale” (216). In another review of research on revision, Stephen Witte cities studies which
COMPOSING BEHAVIORS OF ONE- AND MULTI-DRAFT WRITERS 31
similarly suggest that the amount of re-drafting (which Witte calls “retranscription”) often bears
little relation to the overall quality of completed texts (“Revising” 256). Similarly, Linda Flower and
John Hayes, et al., citing studies which also dispute the notion that more re-drafting should mean
better papers, conclude that the amount of change is not a key variable in revision and that revi-
sion as an obligatory stage required by teachers doesn’t necessarily produce better writing. (For a
teacher’s affirmation of the same phenomenon, see Henley.)
Constricting revision to retranscription (i.e., to altering what has been written) also denies the
reality of pre-text, a composing phenomenon studied by Stephen Witte in “Pre-Text and Compos-
ing.” Witte defines a writer’s pre-text as “the mental construction of ‘text’ prior to transcription”
(397). Pre-text thus “refers to a writer’s linguistic representation of intended meaning, a ‘trial locu-
tion’ that is produced in the mind, stored in the writer’s memory, and sometimes manipulated men-
tally prior to being transcribed as written text” (397). Pre-texts are distinguished from abstract
plans in that pre-texts approximate written prose. As the outcome of planning, pre-text can also
be the basis for further planning. In his study Witte found great diversity in how writers construct
and use pre-text. Some writers construct little or no pre-text; others rely heavily on extensive
pre-texts; others create short pre-texts; and still others move back and forth between extensive
and short pre-texts. The point here is that Witte has shown us that revision can and does occur
in pre-texts, before visible marks are made on paper. In an earlier paper, “Revising, Composing
Theory, and Research Design,” Witte suggests that the pre-text writers construct before making
marks on paper is probably a function of the quality, kind, and extent of planning that occurs before
transcribing on paper. The danger here is that we might conclude that the development from nov-
ice to expert writer entails learning to make greater use of pre-text prior to transcribing. After
all, in Linda Flower’s memorable phrase, pre-text is “the last cheap gas before transcribing text”
(see Witte, “Pre-Text” 422). But Witte notes that his data do not support a “vote for pre-text”
(“Pre-Text” 401). For the students in Witte’s study, more extensive use of pre-text doesn’t auto-
matically lead to better written text. Thus it appears so far that the quality of revision can neither
be measured by the pound nor tracked through discreet stages.
But a discussion of whether more or fewer drafts is an indication of more mature writing is itself
not adequate. As Maxine Hairston reminds us in “Different Products, Different Processes,” we
must also consider the writing task that is involved in any particular case of generating discourse.
In her taxonomy of writing categories, categories that depict a variety of revision behaviors that
are true to the experience of many of us, Hairston divides writing into three classes; first, rou-
tine maintenance writing which is simple communication about uncomplicated matters; second,
extended, relatively complex writing that requires the writer’s attention but is self-limiting in that
the writer already knows most of what she is going to write and may be writing under time con-
straints; and third, extended reflective writing in which the form and content emerge as the writing
proceeds. Even with this oversimplified, brief summary of Hairston’s classes of writing, we recog-
nize that the matter of when and if re-drafting takes place can differ according to the demands of
different tasks and situations as well as the different skills levels of writers.
Many—or perhaps even most—of us may nod in agreement as we recognize in Hairston’s
classes of writing a description of the different types of writing we do. But given the range of indi-
vidual differences that exist among writers, we still cannot conclude that the nature of effective
revision is always tied to the writing task, because such a conclusion would not account for what
we know also exists—some expert writers who, despite the writing task, work at either end of
the spectrum as confirmed, consistent one-drafters or as perpetual multi-drafters. That writers
exhibit a diversity of revising habits has been noted by Lester Faigley and Stephen Witte in “Analyz-
ing Revision.” When testing the taxonomy of revision changes they had created, Faigley and Witte
found that expert writers exhibited “extreme diversity” in the ways they revised:
One expert writer in the present study made almost no revisions; another started with an
almost stream-of-consciousness text that she then converted to an organized essay in the
32 MURIEL HARRIS
second draft; another limited his major revisions to a single long insert; and another revised
mostly by pruning. (410)
Similarly, when summarizing interviews with well-known authors such as those in the Writers at
Work: The Paris Review Interviews series, Lillian Bridwell notes that these discussions reveal a wide
range of revision strategies among these writers, from rapid producers of text who do little revis-
ing as they proceed to writers who move along by revising every sentence (198).
More extensive insights into a variety of composing styles are offered in Tom Waldrep’s collec-
tion of essays by successful scholars working in composition, Writers on Writing. Here too as writers
describe their composing processes, we see a variety of approaches, including some writers who
plan extensively before their pens hit paper (or before the cursor blips on their screens). Their
planning is so complete that their texts generally emerge in a single draft with minor, if any, editing
as they write. Self-descriptions of some experienced writers in the field of composition give us
vivid accounts of how these one-drafters work. For example, Patricia Y. Murray notes that prior
to typing, she sees words, phrases, sentences, and paragraphs taking shape in her head. Her com-
posing, she concludes, has been done before her fingers touch the typewriter, though as she also
notes, she revises and edits as she types (234). William Lutz offers a similar account:
Before I write, I write in my mind. The more difficult and complex the writing, the more
time I need to think before I write. Ideas incubate in my mind. While I talk, drive, swim, and
exercise I am thinking, planning, writing. I think about the introduction, what examples to
use, how to develop the main idea, what kind of conclusion to use. I write, revise, rewrite,
agonize, despair, give up, only to start all over again, and all of this before I ever begin to
put words on paper. … Writing is not a process of discovery for me. … The writing pro-
cess takes place in my mind. Once that process is complete the product emerges. Often I
can write pages without pause and with very little, if any, revision or even minor changes.
(186–87)
Even with such descriptions from experienced writers, we are hesitant either to discard the
notion that writing is a process of discovery for many of us or to typecast writers who make many
visible changes on the page and/or work through multiple drafts as inadequate writers. After all,
many of us, probably the majority, fall somewhere along the continuum from one- to multi-drafters.
We may find ourselves as both one- and multi-drafters with the classes of writing that Hairston
describes, or we may generally identify ourselves as doing both but also functioning more often
as a one- or multi-drafter. Just as we have seen that at one end of the spectrum there are some
confirmed one-drafters, so too must we recognize that at the other end of that spectrum there are
some confirmed multi-drafters, expert writers for whom extensive revising occurs when writing
(so that a piece of discourse may go through several or more drafts or be reworked heavily as the
original draft evolves.) David Bartholomae, a self-described multi-drafter, states that he never out-
lines but works instead with two pads of paper, one to write on and one for making plans, storing
sentences, and taking notes. He views his first drafts as disorganized and has to revise extensively,
with the result that the revisions bear little resemblance to the first drafts (22–26). Similarly, Lynn
Z. Bloom notes that she cannot predict at the outset a great deal of what she is going to say. Only
by writing does she learn how her content will develop or how she will handle the structure, orga-
nization, and style of her paragraphs, sentences, and whole essay (33).
Thus, if we wish to draw a more inclusive picture of composing behaviors for revision, we have
to put together a description that accounts for differences in levels of ability and experience (from
novice to expert), for differences in writing tasks, and also for differences in the as yet largely unex-
plored area of composing process differences among writers. My interest here is in the composing
processes of different writers, more particularly, the reality of those writers at either end of that
COMPOSING BEHAVIORS OF ONE- AND MULTI-DRAFT WRITERS 33
long spectrum, the one-drafters at one end and the multi-drafters at the other. By one-draft writ-
ers I mean those writers who construct their plans and the pre-texts that carry out those plans
as well as do all or most of the revising of those plans and pre-texts mentally, before transcribing.
They do little or no retranscribing. True one-drafters have not arrived at this developmentally or
as a result of training in writing, and they should not be confused with other writers who—driven
by deadlines, lack of motivation, insufficient experience with writing, or anxieties about “getting it
right the first time”—do little or no scratching out of what they have written. Multi-drafters, on
the other hand, need to interact with their transcriptions in order to revise. Independent of how
much planning they do or pre-text they compose, they continue to revise after they have tran-
scribed words onto paper. Again, true multi-drafters have not reached this stage developmentally
or as a result of any intervention by teachers. This is not to say that we can classify writers into two
categories, one- and multi-drafters, because all the evidence we have and, more importantly, our
own experience tells us that most writers are not one or the other but exist somewhere between
these two ends of the continuum.
However, one- and multi-drafters do exist, and we do need to learn more about them to gain
a clearer picture not only of what is involved in different revising processes but also to provide
a basis for considering the pedagogical implications of dealing with individual differences. There
is a strong argument for looking instead at the middle range of writers who do some writing in
single drafts and others in multiple drafts or with a lot of retranscribing as they proceed, for it is
very probable that the largest number of writers cluster there. But those of us who teach in the
individualized setting of conferences or writing lab tutorials know that we can never overlook or
put aside the concerns of every unique individual with whom we work. Perhaps we are overly
intrigued with individual differences, partly because we see that some students can be ill-served
in the group setting of the classrooms and partly because looking at individual differences gives
us such enlightening glimpses into the complex reality of composing processes. Clinicians in other
fields would argue that looking at the extremes offers a clearer view of what may be involved in
the behaviors of the majority. But those who do research in writing also acknowledge that we need
to understand dimensions of variation among writers, particularly those patterned differences or
“alternate paths to expert performance” that have clear implications for instruction (Freedman et
al. 19). In this case, whatever we learn about patterns of behavior among one- and multi-drafters
has direct implications for instruction as we need to know the various trade-offs involved in any
classroom instruction which would encourage more single or multiple drafting. And, as we will see
when looking at what is involved in being able to revise before drafting or in being able to return
and re-draft what has been transcribed, there are trade-offs indeed. Whatever arguments are
offered, we must also acknowledge that no picture of revision is complete until it includes all that
is known and observed about a variety of revision behaviors among writers.
But what do we know about one- and multi-drafters other than anecdotal accounts that con-
firm their existence? Much evidence is waiting to be gathered from the storehouse of various
published interviews in which well-known writers have been asked to describe their writing. And
Ann Ruggles Gere’s study of the revising behaviors of a blind student gives us a description of a
student writer who does not redraft but writes “first draft/final draft” papers, finished products
produced in one sitting for her courses as a master’s degree candidate. The student describes
periods of thinking about a topic before writing. While she doesn’t know exactly what she will say
until actually writing it, she typically knows what will be contained in the first paragraph as she
types the title. Her attention is not focused on words as she concentrates instead on images and
larger contexts. A similar description of a one-drafter is found in Joy Reid’s “The Radical Outliner
and the Radical Brainstormer.” Comparing her husband and herself, both composition teachers,
Reid notes the differences between herself, an outliner (and a one-drafter), and her husband, a
brainstormer (and a multi-drafter), differences which parallel those of the writers in Writers on
Writing that I have described.
34 MURIEL HARRIS
The descriptions of all of the one- and multi-draft writers mentioned so far offer a fairly con-
sistent picture, but these descriptions do little more than reaffirm their existence. In an effort to
learn more, I sought out some one- and multi-drafters in order to observe them composing and to
explore what might be involved. Since my intent was not to determine the percentage of one- and
multi-drafters among any population of writers (though that would be an interesting topic indeed,
as I suspect there are more than we may initially guess—or at least more who hover close to either
end of the continuum). I sought out experienced writers who identify themselves as very definitely
one- or multi-drafters. The subjects I selected for observation were graduate students who teach
composition or communications courses, my rationale being that these people can more easily cat-
egorize and articulate their own writing habits. From among the group of subjects who described
themselves as very definitely either one- or multi-drafters, I selected those who showed evidence
of being experienced, competent writers. Of the eight selected subjects (four one-drafters and
four multi-drafters), all were at least several years into their graduate studies in English or com-
munications and were either near completion or had recently completed advanced degrees. All had
received high scores in standardized tests for verbal skills such as the SAT or GRE exams; all had
grade point averages ranging from B+ to A in their graduate courses; and all wrote frequently in a
variety of tasks, including academic papers for courses and journal publications, conference papers,
the usual business writing of practicing academics (e.g., letters of recommendation for students,
memos, instructional materials for classes, etc.), and personal writing such as letters to family and
friends. They clearly earned their description as experienced writers. Experienced writers were
used because I also wished to exclude those novices who may, through development of their writ-
ing skills, change their composing behaviors, and also those novices whose composing habits are
the result of other factors such as disinterest (e.g., the one-drafter who habitually begins the paper
at 3 a.m. the night before it’s due) or anxiety (e.g., the multi-drafter who fears she is never “right”
and keeps working and re-working her text).
The experienced writers whom I observed all confirmed that their composing behaviors have
not changed over time. That is, they all stated that their writing habits have not altered as they
became experienced writers and/or as they moved through writing courses. However, their
descriptions of themselves as one- or multi-drafters were not as completely accurate as might
be expected. Self-reporting, even among teachers of writing, is not a totally reliable measure. As
I observed and talked with the eight writers, I found three very definite one-drafters, Ted, Nina,
and Amy; one writer, Jackie, who tends to be a one-drafter but does some revising after writing;
two very definite multi-drafters, Bill and Pam; and two writers, Karen and Cindy, who described
themselves as multi-drafters and who tend to revise extensively but who can also produce first
draft/final draft writing under some conditions. To gather data on their composing behaviors, I
interviewed each person for an hour, asking questions about the types of writing they do, the
activities they engage in before writing, the details of what happens as they write, their revision
behaviors, the manner in which sentences are composed, and their attitudes and past history of
writing. Each person was also asked to spend an hour writing in response to an assignment. The
specific assignment was a request from an academic advisor asking for the writers’ descriptions of
the skills needed to succeed in their field of study. As they wrote, all eight writers were asked to
give thinking-aloud protocols and were videotaped for future study. Brief interviews after writing
focused on eliciting information about how accurately the writing session reflected their general
writing habits and behaviors. Each type of information collected is, at best, incomplete because
accounts of one’s own composing processes may not be entirely accurate, because thinking-aloud
protocols while writing are only partial accounts of what is being thought about, and because one-
hour writing tasks preclude observing some of the kinds of activities that writers report. But even
with these limitations I observed patterns of composing behaviors that should differentiate one-
draft writers from multi-draft writers.
COMPOSING BEHAVIORS OF ONE- AND MULTI-DRAFT WRITERS 35
Among the consistent behaviors that one-drafters report is the point at which they can and will
start writing. All of the four one-drafters expressed a strong need to clarify their thinking prior to
beginning to transcribe. They are either not ready to write or cannot write until they have a focus
and organization in mind. They may, as I observed Jacky and Ted doing, make some brief planning
notes on paper or, as Amy and Nina did, sit for awhile and mentally plan, but all expressed a clearly
articulated need to know beforehand the direction the piece of writing would take. For Nina’s lon-
ger papers, she described a planning schedule in which the focus comes first, even before collecting
notes. Ted too described the first stage of a piece of writing as being a time of mentally narrowing a
topic. During incubation times before writing, two of these writers described some global recasting
of a paper in their minds while the other two expressed a need to talk it out, either to themselves
or friends. There is little resorting of written notes and little use of written outlines, except for
some short lists, described by Ted as “memory jogs” to use while he writes. Amy explained that
she sometimes felt that in high school or as an undergraduate she should have written outlines to
please her teachers, but she never did get around to it because outlines served no useful purpose
for her. Consistent throughout these accounts and in my observation of their writing was these
writers’ need to know where they are headed beforehand and a feeling that they are not ready to
write—or cannot write—until they are at that stage. When asked if they ever engaged in freewrit-
ing, two one-drafters said they could not, unless forced to, plunge in and write without a focus and
a mental plan. Ted, in particular, noted that the notion of exploration during writing would make
him so uncomfortable that he would probably block and be unable to write.
In contrast to the one-drafters’ preference for knowing their direction before writing, the two
consistent multi-drafters, Pam and Bill, explained that they resist knowing, resist any attempt at
clarification prior to writing. Their preference is for open-ended exploration as they write. They
may have been reading and thinking extensively beforehand, but the topic has not taken shape
when they decide that it is time to begin writing. Bill reported that he purposely starts with a broad
topic while Pam said that she looks for something “broad or ambiguous” or “something small that
can grow and grow.” As Bill explained, he doesn’t like writing about what he already knows as that
would be boring. Pam too expressed her resistance to knowing her topic and direction beforehand
in terms of how boring it would be. Generally, Bill will do about four or five drafts as he works
through the early parts of a paper, perhaps two to four pages, before he knows what he will write
about. He and Pam allow for—and indeed expect—that their topic will change as they write. Pam
explained: “I work by allowing the direction of the work to change if it needs to. … I have to allow
things to go where they need to go.” When I observed them writing, Pam spent considerable time
planning and creating pre-texts before short bursts of transcribing while Bill wrote several different
versions of an introduction and, with some cutting and pasting, was about ready to define his focus
at the end of the hour. He reported that he depends heavily on seeing what he has written in order
to find his focus, choose his content, and organize. Pam also noted that she needs to see chunks of
what she has transcribed to see where the piece of discourse is taking her.
The other two writers who characterized themselves as multi-drafters, Karen and Cindy, both
described a general tendency to plunge in before the topic is clear. Karen said that she can’t visual-
ize her arguments until she writes them out and generally writes and rewrites as she proceeds, but
for writing tasks that she described as “formulaic” in that they are familiar because she has written
similar pieces of discourse, she can write quickly and finish quickly—as she did with the writing
task for this study. Since she had previously written the same kind of letter assigned in this study,
she did not engage in the multi-drafting that would be more characteristic, she says, of her general
composing behaviors. Cindy, the other self-described multi-drafter, almost completed the task in
a single draft, though as she explained with short pieces, she can revert to her “journalistic mode”
36 MURIEL HARRIS
of writing, having been a working journalist for a number of years. For longer papers, such as those
required in graduate courses, her descriptions sound much like those of Bill, Pam, and Karen. All of
these writers, though, share the unifying characteristic of beginning to write before the task is well
defined in their minds, unlike the one-drafters who do not write at that stage.
Another consistent and clearly related difference between one-and multi-drafters is the differ-
ence in the quantity of options they will generate, from words and sentences to whole sections
of a paper, and the way in which they will evaluate those options. As they wrote, all four of the
one-drafters limited their options by generating several choices and then making a decision fairly
quickly. There were numerous occasions in the think-aloud protocols of three of the four one-
drafters in which they would stop, try another word, question a phrase, raise the possibility of
another idea to include, and then make a quick decision. When Ted re-read one of his paragraphs,
he saw a different direction that he might have taken that would perhaps be better, but he accepted
what he had. (“That’ll do here, OK … OK” he said to himself and moved on.) Nina, another one-
drafter, generated no alternate options aloud as she wrote.
As is evident in this description of one-drafters, they exhibited none of the agonizing over pos-
sibilities that other writers experience, and they appear to be able to accept their choices quickly
and move on. While observers may question whether limiting options in this manner cuts off further
discovery and possibly better solutions or whether the internal debate goes on prior to transcrib-
ing, one-drafters are obviously efficient writers. They generate fewer choices, reach decisions more
quickly, and do most or all of the decision-making before transcribing on paper. Thus, three of the
four one-drafters finished the paper in the time allotted, and the fourth writer was almost finished.
They can pace themselves fairly accurately too, giving me their estimates of how long it takes them
to write papers of particular lengths. All four one-drafters describe themselves as incurable pro-
crastinators who begin even long papers the night before they are due, allowing themselves about
the right number of hours in which to transcribe their mental constructs onto paper. Nina explained
that she makes choices quickly because she is always writing at the last minute under pressure and
doesn’t have time to consider more options. Another one-drafter offered a vivid description of the
tension and stress that can be involved in these last minute, all-night sessions.
While they worry about whether they will finish on time, these one-drafters generally do. Con-
tributing to their efficiency are two time-saving procedures involved as they get words on paper.
Because most decisions are made before they commit words to paper, they do little or no scratch-
ing out and re-writing; and they do a minimum of re-reading both as they proceed and also when
they are finished. The few changes I observed being made were either single words or a few short
phrases, unlike the multi-drafters who rejected or scratched out whole sentences and paragraphs.
As Nina wrote, she never re-read her developing text, though she reported that she does a little
re-reading when she is finished with longer papers. The tinkering with words that she might do
then, she says, is counterproductive because she rarely feels that she is improving the text with
these changes. (Nina and the other one-drafters would probably be quite successful at the kind of
“invisible writing” that has been investigated, that is, writing done under conditions in which writ-
ers cannot see what they are writing or scan as they progress. See Blau.)
In contrast to the one-drafters’ limited options, quick decisions, few changes on paper and
little or no re-reading, the multi-drafters were frequently observed generating and exploring many
options, spending a long time in making their choices, and making frequent and large-scale changes
on paper. Bill said that he produces large quantities of text because he needs to see it in order to
see if he wants to retain it, unlike the one-drafters who exhibit little or no need to examine their
developing text. Moreover, as Bill noted, the text he generates is also on occasion a heuristic for
more text. As he writes, Bill engages in numerous revising tactics. He writes a sentence, stops
to examine it by switching it around, going back to add clauses, or combining it with other text
COMPOSING BEHAVIORS OF ONE- AND MULTI-DRAFT WRITERS 37
on the same page or a different sheet of paper. For the assigned writing task, he began with one
sheet of paper, moved to another, tore off some of it and discarded it, and added part back to a
previous sheet. At home when writing a longer paper, he will similarly engage in extensive cutting
and pasting. In a somewhat different manner. Pam did not generate as many options on paper for
this study. Instead, her protocol recorded various alternative plans and pre-texts that she would
stop to explore verbally for five or ten minutes before transcribing anything. What she did write,
though, was often heavily edited so that at the end of the hour, she, like Bill, had only progressed
somewhat through an introductory paragraph of several sentences. Thus, while Bill had produced
large amounts of text on paper that were later rejected after having been written, Pam spent more
of her time generating and rejecting plans and pre-texts than crossing out transcriptions.
Writing is a more time-consuming task for these multi-drafters because they expect to produce
many options and a large amount of text that will be discarded. Both Bill and Pam described general
writing procedures in which they begin by freewriting, and, as they proceed, distilling from earlier
drafts what will be used in later drafts. Both proceed incrementally, that is, by starting in and then
starting again before finishing a whole draft. Both writers are used to re-reading frequently, partly
to locate what Pam called “key elements” that will be retained for later drafts and partly, as Bill
explained, because the act of generating more options and exploring them causes him to lose track
of where he is.
Because both Bill and Pam seem to be comfortable when working within an as-yet only partially
focused text, it would be interesting to explore what has been termed their “tolerance for ambigu-
ity,” a trait defined as a person’s ability to function calmly in a situation in which interpretation of
all stimuli is not completely clear. (See Budner, and Frenkel-Brunswick.) People who have little or
no tolerance for ambiguity perceive ambiguous situations as sources of psychological discomfort,
and they may try to reach conclusions quickly rather than to take the time to consider all of the
essential elements of an unclear situation. People with more tolerance for ambiguity enjoy being in
ambiguous situations and tend to seek them out. The relevance here, of course, is the question of
whether one-drafters will not begin until they have structured the task and will also move quickly
to conclusions in part, at least, because of having some degree of intolerance for ambiguity. This
might be a fruitful area for further research.
For those interested in the mental processes which accompany behaviors, another dimension to
explore is the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI), a measure of expressed preferences (i.e., not
performance tasks) in four bi-polar dimensions of personality. The work of Geroge H. Jensen and
John K. DiTiberio has indicated some relationships between the personality types identified by the
MBTI and writing processes. Of particular interest here is that Bill, who had independently taken
the MBTI for other reasons, reported that he scored highly in the dimensions of “extraversion” and
“perceiving.” Extraverts, say Jensen and DiTiberio, “often leap into tasks with little planning, then
rely on trial and error to complete the task” (288), and they “often find freewriting a good method
for developing ideas, for they think better when writing quickly, impulsively, and uncritically” (289).
Perceivers, another type described by Jensen and DiTiberio, appear to share tendencies similar to
those with a tolerance for ambiguity, for perceivers “are willing to leave the outer world unstruc-
tured. … Quickly made decisions narrow their field of vision” (295). Perceiving types tend to select
broad topics for writing, like a wide range of alternatives, and always want to read one more book
on the subject. Their revisions thus often need to be refocused (296). The similarities here to Bill’s
writing behaviors show us that while the MBTI is somewhat circular in that the scoring is a reflec-
tion of people’s self-description, it can confirm (and perhaps clarify) the relationship of writing
behaviors to more general human behaviors.
From these descriptions of one- and multi-drafters it is readily apparent that they differ in their
need for closure. The one-drafters move quickly to decisions while composing, and they report
38 MURIEL HARRIS
that once they are done with a paper, they prefer not to look back at it, either immediately to
re-read it or at some future time, to think about revising it. Ted explained that he generally is will-
ing to do one re-reading at the time of completing a paper and sometimes to make a few wording
changes, but that is all. He shrugged off the possibility of doing even a second re-reading of any of
his writing once it is done because he says he can’t stand to look at it again. All of the one-drafters
reported that they hardly, if ever, rewrite a paper. This distaste for returning to a completed text
can be the source of problems for these one-drafters. Forced by a teacher in a graduate course
who wanted first drafts one week and revisions the next week, Nina explained that she deliber-
ately resorted to “writing a bad paper” for the first submission in order to submit her “real” draft
as the “revised” paper. Writing a series of drafts is clearly harder for one-drafters such as Nina
than we have yet acknowledged.
These one-drafters are as reluctant to start as they are impatient to finish. Although they tend
to delay the drafting process, this does not apply to their preparation which often starts well in
advance and is the “interesting” or “enjoyable” part for them. With writing that produces few sur-
prises or discoveries for any of them because the generative process precedes transcription, draft-
ing on paper is more “tedious” (a word they frequently used during their interviews) than for other
writers. Said Ted, “Writing is something I have to do, not something I want to do.” Even Jackie, who
allows for some revising while drafting in order to develop the details of her plan, reported that
she has a hard time going back to revise a paper once it is completed. She, like the others, reported
a sense of feeling the paper is over and done with. “Done, dead and done, done, finished, done,”
concluded another of these one-drafters.
On the other hand, the multi-drafters observed in this study explained that they are never done
with a paper. They can easily and willingly go back to it or to keep writing indefinitely. Asked when
they know they are finished, Bill and Pam explained that they never feel they are “done” with a piece
of discourse, merely that they have to stop in order to meet a deadline. As Pam said, she never gets
to a last draft and doesn’t care about producing “neat packages.” Understandably, she has trouble
with conclusions and with “wrapping up” at the end of a piece of discourse. Asked how pervasive
her redrafting is for all of her writing, Pam commented that she writes informal letters to parents
and friends every day and is getting to the point that she doesn’t rewrite these letters as much. Bill
too noted that he fights against products and hates to finish. As a result, both Bill and Pam often
fail to meet their deadlines. Cindy, bored by her “journalistic one-draft writing,” expressed a strong
desire to return to some of her previously completed papers in order to rewrite them.
One way of distinguishing the early drafts produced by the multi-drafters for this study from the
drafts produced by the one-drafters is to draw upon Linda Flower’s distinction between Writer-
Based and Reader-Based prose. Writer-Based prose, explains Flower, is “verbal expression written
by a writer to himself and for himself. It is the working of his own verbal thought. In its structure,
Writer-Based prose reflects the associative, narrative path of the writer’s own confrontation with
her subject” (19–20). Reader-Based prose, on the other hand, is “a deliberate attempt to commu-
nicate something to a reader. To do that it creates a shared language and shared context between
writer and reader. It also offers the reader an issue-oriented rhetorical structure rather than a
replay of the writer’s discovery process” (20). Although Flower acknowledges that Writer-Based
prose is a “problem” that composition courses are designed to correct, she also affirms its useful-
ness as a search tool, a strategy for handling the difficulty of attending to multiple complex tasks
simultaneously. Writer-Based prose needs to be revised into Reader-Based prose, but it can be
effective as a “medium for thinking.” And for the multi-drafters observed in this study, character-
izing the initial drafts of two of the multi-drafters as Writer-Based helps to see how their early
drafts differ from those of the one-drafters.
COMPOSING BEHAVIORS OF ONE- AND MULTI-DRAFT WRITERS 39
One feature of Writer-Based prose, as offered by Flower, is that it reflects the writer’s method
of searching by means of surveying what she knows, often in a narrative manner. Information tends
to be structured as a narrative of the discovery process or as a survey of the data in the writer’s
mind. Reader-Based prose, on the other hand, restructures the information so that it is accessible
to the reader. Both the protocols and the written drafts produced by the two confirmed multi-
drafters, Bill and Pam, reveal this Writer-Based orientation as their initial way into writing. Bill very
clearly began with a memory search through his own experience, made some brief notes, and then
wrote a narrative as his first sentence in response to the request that he describe to an academic
counselor the skills needed for his field: “I went through what must have been a million different
majors before I wound up in English and it was actually my first choice.” Pam spent the hour explor-
ing the appropriateness of the term “skills.”
In distinct contrast, all four of the one-drafters began by constructing a conceptual framework
for the response they would write, most typically by defining a few categories or headings which
would be the focus or main point of the paper. With a few words in mind that indicated his major
points, Ted then moved on to ask himself who would be reading his response, what the context
would be, and what format the writing would use. He moved quickly from a search for a point
to considerations of how his audience would use his information. Similarly, Amy rather promptly
chose a few terms, decided to herself that “that’ll be the focus,” and then said, “OK, I’m trying to
get into a role here. I’m responding to someone who … This is not something they are going to
give out to people. But they’re going to read it and compile responses, put something together
for themselves.” She then began writing her draft and completed it within the hour. Asked what
constraints and concerns she is most aware of when actually writing, Amy said that she is generally
concerned with clarity for the reader. The point of contrast here is that the search process was
both different in kind and longer for the multi-drafters. Initially, their time was spent discovering
what they think about the subject, whereas the one-drafters chose a framework within a few min-
utes and moved on to orient their writing to their readers. Because the transformation or rework-
ing of text comes later for the multi-drafters, rewriting is a necessary component of their writing.
The standard bit of advice, about writing the introductory paragraph later, would be a necessary
step for them but would not be a productive or appropriate strategy for one-drafters to try. For
the one-drafters, the introductory paragraph is the appropriate starting point. In fact, given what
they said about the necessity of knowing their focus beforehand, the introductory paragraph is not
merely appropriate but necessary.
Because the early stages of a piece of writing are, for multi-drafters, so intricately bound up
with mental searching, surveying, and discovering, the writing that is produced is not oriented to
the reader. For their early drafts, Bill and Pam both acknowledged that their writing is not yet
understandable to others. When Pam commented that in her early drafts, “the reader can’t yet see
where I’m going,” she sighed over the difficulties this had caused in trying to work with her Master’s
thesis committee. If some writers’ early drafts are so personal and so unlikely to be accessible to
readers, it is worth speculating about how effective peer editing sessions could be for such multi-
drafters who appear in classrooms with “rough drafts” as instructed.
Conclusions
One way to summarize the characteristics of one- and multi-drafters is to consider what they
gain by being one-drafters and at what cost they gain these advantages. Clearly, one-drafters are
efficient writers. This efficiency is achieved by mentally revising beforehand, by generating options
verbally rather than on paper, by generating only a limited number of options before settling on one
and getting on with the task, and by doing little or no re-reading. They are able to pace themselves
and can probably perform comfortably in situations such as the workplace or in in-class writing
where it is advantageous to produce first-draft, final-draft pieces of discourse. Their drafts are
40 MURIEL HARRIS
readily accessible to readers, and they can expend effort early on in polishing the text for greater
clarity. But at what cost? One-drafters are obviously in danger of cutting themselves off from fur-
ther exploration, from a richer field of discovery than is possible during the time in which they gen-
erate options. When they exhibit a willingness to settle on one of their options, they may thereby
have eliminated the possibility of searching for a better one. In their impatience to move on, they
may even settle on options they know could be improved on. Their impulse to write dwindles as
these writers experience little or none of the excitement of discovery or exploration during writ-
ing. The interesting portion of a writing task, the struggle with text and sense of exploration, is
largely completed when they begin to commit themselves to paper (or computer screen). Because
they are less likely to enjoy writing, the task of starting is more likely to be put off to the last minute
and to become a stressful situation, thus reinforcing their inclination not to re-read and their desire
to be done and to put the paper behind them forever once they have finished. And it appears that it
is as hard for true one-drafters to suspend the need for closure as it is for multi-drafters to reach
quick decisions and push themselves rapidly toward closure.
Multi-drafters appear to be the flip side of the same coin. Their relative inefficiency causes them
to miss deadlines, to create Writer-Based first drafts, to produce large quantities of text that is dis-
carded, and to get lost in their own writing. They need to re-read and re-draft, and they probably
appear at first glance to be poorer writers than one-drafters. But they are more likely to be writers
who will plunge in eagerly, will write and re-write, and will use writing to explore widely and richly.
They also are more likely to affirm the value of writing as a heuristic, the merits of freewriting, and
the need for cutting and pasting of text. They may, if statistics are gathered, be the writers who
benefit most from collaborative discussions such as those in writing labs with tutors. Their drafts
are truly amenable to change and available for re-working.
Implications
Acknowledging the reality of one- and multi-drafting involves enlarging both our perspectives on
revision and our instructional practices with students. In terms of what the reality of one-drafting
and multi-drafting tells us about revision, it is apparent that we need to account for this diversity
of revision behaviors as we construct a more detailed picture of revision. As Stephen Witte notes,
“revising research that limits itself to examining changes in written text or drafts espouses a reduc-
tionist view of revising as a stage in a linear sequence of stages” (“Revising” 266). Revision can and
does occur when writers set goals, create plans, and compose pre-text, as well as when they tran-
scribe and re-draft after transcription. Revision can be triggered by cognitive activity alone and/or
by interaction with text; and attitudes, preferences, and cognitive make-up play a role in when and
how much a writer revises—or is willing to revise—a text.
Yet, while recognizing the many dimensions to be explored in understanding revision, we can
also use this diversity as a source for helping students with different types of problems and con-
cerns. For students who are one-drafters or have tendencies toward single drafting, we need to
provide help in several areas. They’ll have to learn to do more reviewing of written text both as
they write and afterwards, in order to evaluate and revise. They will also need to be aware that
they should have strategies that provide for more exploration and invention than they may pres-
ently allow themselves. While acknowledging their distaste for returning to a draft to open it up
again, we also need to help them see how and when this can be productive. Moreover, we can pro-
vide assistance in helping one-drafters and other writers who cluster near that end of the spectrum
recognize that sometimes they have a preference for choosing an option even after they recognize
that it may not be the best one. When Tim, one of the one-drafters I observed, noted at one point
in his protocol that he should take a different direction for one of his paragraphs but won’t, he
shows similarities to another writer, David, observed by Witte (“Pre-Text and Composing” 406),
who is reluctant to spend more than fifteen seconds reworking a sentence in pre-text, even though
COMPOSING BEHAVIORS OF ONE- AND MULTI-DRAFT WRITERS 41
he demonstrates the ability to evoke criteria that could lead to better formulations if he chose to
stop and revise mentally (David typically does little revision of written text). This impatience, this
need to keep moving along, that does not always allow for the production of good text, can obvi-
ously work against producing good text, and it is unlikely that such writers will either recognize or
conquer the problem on their own. They may have snared themselves in their own vicious circles
if their tendency to procrastinate puts them in a deadline crunch, which, in turn, does not afford
them the luxury of time to consider new options. Such behaviors can become a composing habit
so entrenched that it is no longer noticed.
As we work with one-drafters, we will also have to learn ourselves how to distinguish them
from writers who see themselves as one-drafters because they are not inclined, for one reason or
another, to expend more energy on drafting. Inertia, lack of motivation, lack of information about
what multiple drafts can do, higher priorities for other tasks, and so on are not characteristic
of true one-drafters, and we must be able to identify the writer who might take refuge behind a
label of “one-drafter” from the writer who exhibits some or many of the characteristics of one-
draft composing and who wants to become a better writer. For example, in our writing lab I have
worked with students who think they are one-drafters because of assorted fears, anxieties, and
misinformation. “But I have to get it right the first time.” “My teachers never liked to see scratch-
ing out on the paper, even when we wrote in class,” or “I hate making choices, so I go with what I
have” are not the comments of true one-drafters.
With multiple-drafters we have other work to do. To become more efficient writers, they will
need to become more proficient planners and creaters of pre-text, though given their heavy depen-
dence on seeing what they have written, they will probably still rely a great deal on reading and work-
ing with their transcribed text. They will also need to become more proficient at times at focusing
on a topic quickly, recognizing the difficulties involved in agonizing endlessly over possibilities. In the
words of a reviewer of this paper, they will have to learn when and how “to get on with it.”
Besides assisting with these strategies, we can help students become more aware of their com-
posing behaviors. We can assist multi-drafters in recognizing that they are not slow or inept writ-
ers but writers who may linger too long over making choices. For writers who have difficulty
returning to a completed text in order to revise, we can relate the problem to the larger picture, an
impatience with returning to any completed task. Granted, this is not a giant leap forward, but too
many students are willing to throw in the towel with writing skills in particular without recognizing
the link to their more general orientations to life. Similarly, the impatient writer who, like Ted, pro-
claims to have a virulent case of the “I-hate-to-write” syndrome may be a competent one-drafter
(or have a preference for fewer drafts) who needs to see that it is the transcribing stage of writing
that is the source of the impatience, procrastination, and irritation. On the other hand, writers
more inclined to be multi-drafters need to recognize that their frustration, self-criticism, and/or
low grades may be due to having readers intervene at too early a stage in the drafting. What I am
suggesting here is that some writers unknowingly get themselves caught in linguistic traps. They
think they are making generalizations about the whole act of “writing,” that blanket term for all the
processes involved, when they may well be voicing problems or attitudes about one or another of
the processes. What is needed here is some assistance in helping students define their problems
more precisely. To do this, classroom teachers can open conferences like a writing lab tutorial, by
asking questions about the student’s writing processes and difficulties.
In addition to individualizing our work with students, we can also look at our own teaching prac-
tices. When we offer classroom strategies and heuristics, we need to remind our students that it
is likely that some will be very inappropriate for different students. Being unable to freewrite is not
necessarily a sign of an inept writer. One writer’s written text may be just as effective a heuristic
for that writer as the planning sheets are for another writer. Beyond these strategies and acknowl-
edgments, we have to examine how we talk about or teach composing processes. There is a very
real danger in imposing a single, “ideal” composing style on students, as Jack Selzer found teachers
42 MURIEL HARRIS
attempting to do in his survey of the literature. Similarly, as Susan McLeod notes, teachers tend
to teach their own composing behaviors in the classroom and are thus in danger either of impos-
ing their redrafting approaches on students whose preference for revising prior to transcribing
serves them well or of touting their one- or few-draft strategies to students who fare better when
interacting with their transcribed text. Imposing personal preferences, observes McLeod, would
put us in the peculiar position of trying to fix something that isn’t broken. And there’s enough of
that going around as is.
Works Cited
Axelrod, Rise B., and Charles R. Cooper. The St. Martin’s Guide to Writing. New York: St. Martin’s, 1985.
Bartholomae, David. “Against the Grain.” Waldrep 1:19–29.
Beach, Richard. “Self-Evaluation Strategies of Extensive Revisers and Non-revisers.” College Composition and
Communication 27 (1976): 160–64.
Blau, Sheridan. “Invisible Writing: Investigating Cognitive Processes in Composition.” College Composition and
Communication 34 (1983): 297–312.
Bloom, Lynn Z. “How I Write.” Waldrep 1:31–37.
Bridwell, Lillian S. “Revising Strategies in Twelfth Grade Students’ Transactional Writing.” Research in the
Teaching of English 14 (1980): 197–222.
Budner, S. “Intolerance of Ambiguity as a Personality Variable.” Journal of Personality 30 (1962): 29–50.
Faigley, Lester, and Stephen Witte. “Analyzing Revision.” College Composition and Communication 32 (1981):
400–14.
Flower, Linda. “Writer-Based Prose: A Cognitive Basis for Problems in Writing.” College English 41 (1979):
19–37.
Flower, Linda, John R. Hayes, Linda Carey, Karen Shriver, and James Stratman. “Detection, Diagnosis, and the
Strategies of Revision.” College Composition and Communication 37 (1986): 16–55.
Freedman, Sarah Warshauer, Anne Haas Dyson, Linda Flower, and Wallace Chafe. Research in Writing: Past,
Present, and Future. Technical Report No. 1. Center for the Study of Writing. Berkeley: University of
California, 1987.
Frenkel-Brunswick, Else. “Intolerance of Ambiguity as an Emotional and Perceptual Personality Variable.”
Journal of Personality 18 (1949): 108–43.
Gere, Ann Ruggles. “Insights from the Blind: Composing Without Revising.” Revising: New Essays for Teachers
of Writing. Ed. Ronald Sudol. Urbana, IL: ERIC/NCTE, 1982, 52–70.
Hairston, Maxine. “Different Products, Different Processes: A Theory about Writing.” College Composition
and Communication 37 (1986): 442–52.
Henley, Joan. “A Revisionist View of Revision.” Washington English Journal 8.2 (1986): 5–7.
Jensen, George, and John DiTiberio.”Personality and Individual Writing Processes.” College Composition and
Communication 35 (1984): 285–300.
Lutz, William. “How I Write.” Waldrep 1:183–88.
McLeod, Susan. “The New Orthodoxy: Rethinking the Process Approach.” Freshman English News 14.3
(1986): 16–21.
Murray, Patricia Y. “Doing Writing.” Waldrep 1:225–39.
Reid, Joy. “The Radical Outliner and the Radical Brainstormer: A Perspective on Composing Processes.”
TESOL Quarterly 18 (1985): 529–34.
Selzer, Jack. “Exploring Options in Composing.” College Composition and Communication 35 (1984): 276–84.
Sommers, Nancy. “Revision Strategies of Student Writers and Experienced Adult Writers.” College Composi-
tion and Communication 31 (1980): 378–88.
Waldrep, Tom, ed. Writers on Writing. Vol. 1, New York: Random House, 1985. 2 vols.
Witte, Stephen P. “Pre-Text and Composing.” College Composition and Communication 38 (1987): 397–425.
—— “Revising, Composing Theory, and Research Design.” The Acquisition of Written Language: Response and
Revision. Ed. Sarah Warshauer Freedman. Norwood, NJ: Ablex, 1985, 250–84.
Reprinted from Muriel Harris. “Composing Behaviors of One-And-Multi-draft Writers.” College English, Vol.
51, No. 2, February 1989, 174–190. Copyright 1989 by the National Council of Teachers of English. Reprinted
with permission.
MOVING WRITERS, SHAPING MOTIVES, MOTIVATING
CRITIQUE AND CHANGE: A GENRE
APPROACH TO TEACHING WRITING
Mary Jo Reiff
University of Tennessee
What prompted me to write this essay for this anthology? My “assignment” was to write about the
genre of the Pedagogical Insight essay. I immediately called on my genre knowledge—my past experi-
ence with reading and writing similar texts in similar situations—to orient me to the expectations
of this genre. While I am familiar with the genre of “the essay”—and my awareness alerted me to
the fact that my piece could be less formal than an article, a piece based more in experience than in
research—I was not as familiar with the expectations of this particular genre, a Pedagogical Insight
essay. I began, then, with these questions: What are the actions that this genre performs? How will
I position myself within this genre—what identity and relations will I assume? In what ways will the
genre define and sustain the field’s discussions of pedagogy or pedagogical approaches? What are
the potential sites of resistance and transformation?
I looked for clues about how the assignment located me within a situation and provided me with
the rhetorical means for acting within that situation. From the authors’ invitation, I constructed
the rhetorical situation that helped motivate and shape my response. The audience was described
as “new teachers,” and the purpose was “to ground abstract composition theory, as presented by
the anthologized articles, in the immediacy of a real classroom context and a real teacher’s lived
experience.” There was a specified length (1,500 words), but within the constraints of audience,
purpose, and format, there was also a great deal of choice within this genre, with the authors not-
ing that they “hope for a great diversity in tone, stance, and focus.” A sample Pedagogical Insight
essay was included in the materials sent—an example of one writer’s “appropriate” response.
Writing an effective response would mean conceiving of my role as a writer not only in rela-
tion to readers and other writers and their purposes, but also in relation to the social and cultural
formations in which they interact. As a result, the larger cultural context beyond the immediate
situation of this Pedagogical Insight essay also helped to generate and organize my response. In
the materials passed along to help contextualize my response, I was given the proposal (shaped in
response to the editors at the National Council of Teachers of English as well as to a secondary
audience of contributors and reviewers) that positioned the imagined readers of this anthology,
positioned the book intertextually (among “competing texts”), and provided an overview of how
the book responds to material conditions and functions epistemologically. My response, then, is
situated very purposefully and mediated by various contextual factors, not the least of which is a
response to the multiple and related voices included in this section on relations.
Our students are similarly positioned within and by genres. When confronted with a writing assign-
ment, students are suspended within a complex web of relations—from the institutional, disciplin-
ary, and/or course objectives that frame the assignment to the defined roles for writers, their
purposes, their subjects, and their conventions for writing. More important, a genre approach
allows students to see a writing assignment itself as a social action—a response to the whole dis-
ciplinary and institutional context for the assignment, not just a response to the teacher. Students
can access and participate effectively in academic situations by identifying the assumptions and
expectations regarding subject, their roles as writers (as critics, knowledgeable professionals in the
field), the roles of readers (teacher-readers, specialist audiences, implied audiences), and purposes
for writing (to describe, analyze, argue, evaluate, etc.) that are embedded in the assignment.
44 MARY JO REIFF
Approaching writing through a contextual genre theory consists of using genre as a lens for
accessing, understanding, and writing in various situations and contexts. A genre approach to
teaching writing is careful not to treat genres as static forms or systems of classification. Rather,
students learn how to recognize genres as rhetorical responses to and reflections of the situations
in which they are used; furthermore, students learn how to use genres to intervene in situations.
Students begin by (1) collecting samples of a wide range of responses within a particular genre;
(2) identifying and describing the larger cultural scene and rhetorical situation from which genre
emerges (setting, subject, participants, purposes); (3) identifying and describing the patterns of the
genre, including content, structure, format, sentences, and diction; and (4) analyzing genres for
what they tell us about situation and making an argumentative or critical claim about what these
patterns reveal about the attitudes, values, and actions embedded in the genre.
For example, a prelaw student in my advanced composition class explored the genre activities
of the law community by first examining the genre system—the textualized sites such as opinions,
wills, deeds, contracts, and briefs—that defines and sustains the legal community. After choosing
to focus her study on the genre of case briefs, the student began by collecting samples of constitu-
tional law briefs, discovering that the shared purposes and functions “illustrated the legal commu-
nity’s shared value of commitment to tradition, as well as the need for a standard and convenient
form of communicating important and complex legal concepts.” Through her study of the repeated
rhetorical patterns and social actions of legal briefs, the student gained access to the habits, beliefs,
and values of the law community. She not only learned about the genre features of case briefs—
such as the technical terminology, rigid format, and formal style—but she also become more aware
of how these formal patternings reflected and reinforced the goals of the community. Recogniz-
ing that all the briefs follow the same organizational strategy of presenting sections labeled “case
information,” “facts of the case,” “procedural history,” “issue,” “holding,” and “court reasoning,”
she surmised that “Even the rigid structure of the format [suggests] the community’s emphasis on
logic and order, which are two esteemed values of the profession.” The genre not only reflects the
legal community’s valuing of logic and order but, as the student discovered, also reinscribes these
values by “maintaining a system of communication that relates the scientific and the complex world
of law,” establishing a relationship, in effect, between scientific, technical precision and the less
precise interpretation of law and, as a result, reinforcing the belief that legal cases are unambiguous
and clearcut. Furthermore, reflecting on the legal jargon, such as writ of mandamus, or the formal
language of verbs like sayeth and witnesseth or words like hereunder and wherewith, the student
makes the following connections among text, contexts, and the ideological effects of genres:
Legal language is part of a lawyer’s professional training, so the habit of “talking like a lawyer” is
deeply rooted in the practices of the community. This tells us that lawyers feel compelled to use
established jargon to maintain a legitimate status in the eyes of other community members. In
addition, formal language is needed to surround legal proceedings with an air of solemnity, and to
send the message that any legal proceeding is a significant matter with important consequences.
The writer also notes how this use of legal language reinscribes a power relationship of sorts, sepa-
rating “insiders” (members of the profession) from “outsiders” (the public) by cultivating a language
“that reads like a foreign language to those outside the profession.” For students like this one, using
genre as a lens for inquiry cultivates a consciousness of the rhetorical strategies used to carry out
the social actions of a group or disciplinary community, thus making the complex, multitextured
relations of the legal community more tangible and accessible.
MOVING WRITERS, SHAPING MOTIVES 45
Students’ critical awareness of how genres work—their understanding of how rhetorical features
are connected to social actions—enables them to more effectively critique and resist genres by
creating alternatives. For example, a student’s critique of the wedding invitation as a genre allowed
the discovery of a particular cultural view of women or gender bias in its rhetorical patterns and
language (particularly in the references to the bride’s parents who “request the honor of your pres-
ence”). Embedded in the invitation are cultural assumptions of women as objects or property to
be “handed over” from parents to spouse. The textual patternings of the genre, such as references
to the parents and the bride as “their daughter” and the omission of the bride’s name (while nam-
ing the bride’s parents and the groom), reinforce what the student describes as a cultural attitude
toward marriage that involves a loss of identity for women. Wedding invitations, then, in the stu-
dent’s final analysis, are cultural artifacts that through their repeated use in similar situations—the
repeated cultural event of formally announcing marriages—not only reflect but reinscribe gender
inequality and unequal dis t r ibutions of power in relationships (Devitt, Reiff, and Bawarshi).
Classroom genres, too, reflect and enact the social relations of classrooms, and because of the
recurring forms of language use of genres, the institutionally sanctioned academic genres might be
more easily perpetuated, thus excluding students for whom these genres are less accessible. Brad
Peters, in “Genre, Antigenre, and Reinventing the Forms of Conceptualization,” describes a college
composition course in which students read about the United States invasion of Panama in a book
that takes a Panamanian perspective. The students were then told to write an essay exam that fol-
lowed a particular format moving from a summary of the argument, to the three most compelling
points for a Latin American reader, to the three most fallacious points for a Latin American reader,
and finally to the student’s reaction compared to that of the Latin American reader. One student,
Rita, wrote the essay exam from the fictional perspective of her close friend Maria, a native Latin
American, and after completing the rhetorical analysis part of the exam, dropped the persona and
took up her own in the form of a letter to Maria. Peters identifies this as an “antigenre” but points
out that Rita’s response satisfies the social purpose of the genre while reconstituting voice and
varying the format of the genre. This demonstrates that even when the writing assignment is fairly
prescriptive and students are asked to write a fairly traditional genre, there is room for them to
maneuver within (and because of) the constraints of the genre.
One criticism leveled against a genre approach to literacy teaching is that it focuses on analysis and
critique of genres, stopping short of having writers use genres to enact change. Genres—as they
function to define, critique, and bring about change—can provide rich pedagogical sites, sites for
intervention. Bruce McComiskey, for example, pairs academic and public genres—having students
write a critical analysis of education followed by a brochure for high school students, or following
an analysis of the cultural values of advertisements with letters to advertisers arguing the negative
effect on consumers. Genre analysis encourages students to critique sites of intervention, analyz-
ing how such genres enable participation in the process while also limiting intervention. Students
identify linguistic and rhetorical patterns and analyze their significance, while simultaneously cri-
tiquing the cultural and social values encoded in the genre (what the genre allows users to do and
what it does not allow them to do, whose needs are most/least served, how it enables or limits the
way its users do their work).
But the final step would be to ask students to produce new genres or genres that encode alter-
native values for the purpose of intervening. Students could create their own genres that respond
to those they analyzed. Or, after interrogating the sites at which change happens, students can
46 MARY JO REIFF
more directly intervene in these sites by writing their own alternative genres or “antigenres” in
response. I often have my students follow their analysis of genres by inventing and formulating their
own generic response or by writing a manual for others on how to write that genre. The idea is
that as students critique genres as sites of rhetorical action and cultural production and reproduc-
tion, they also see how genres function as motivated social actions, enabling them to enter into the
production of alternatives.
Works Cited
Devitt, Amy, Mary Jo Reiff, and Anis Bawarshi. Scenes of Writing: Strategies for Composing with Genres. New
York: Longman, 2004.
McComiskey, Bruce. Teaching Composition as a Social Process. Logan, UT: Utah State UP, 2000.
Peters, Brad. “Genre, Antigenre, and Reinventing the Forms of Conceptualization.” Genre and Writing: Issues,
Arguments, Alternatives. Portsmouth: Boynton/Cook, 1997.
Smith, Summer. “The Genre of the End Comment: Conventions in Teacher Responses to Student Writing.”
College Composition and Communication 48.2 (1997): 249–68.
Reprinted from Mary Jo Reiff. “Moving Writers, Shaping Motives, Motivating Critique and Change: A Genre
Approach to Teaching Writing.” In Relations, Locations, Positions: Composition Theory for Writing Teachers. Eds.
Peter Vandenberg, Sue Hum, Jennifer Clary-Lemon. NCTE: Urbana, IL, 2006, 157–206. Copyright 2006 by
the National Council of Teachers of English. Reprinted with permission.
References
Britton, J., Burgess, T., Martin, N., McLeod, A., & Rosen,
H. (1975). Th e development of writing abilities 11–18.
London: Macmillan Education Ltd.
the writer re-envision the essay and could very well lead
to substantial rewriting” (Axelrod and
for whatever rolls out on paper the fi rst time around. The
novice view of a fi rst draft as written
and that these changes will improve the text. Given this
widely entrenched notion of redrafting as being
advantageous, it would be comforting
and short pre-texts. The point here is that Witte has shown
us that revision can and does occur
(see Witte, “Pre-Text” 422). But Witte notes that his data
do not support a “vote for pre-text”
plan extensively before their pens hit paper (or before the
cursor blips on their screens). Their
ers I mean those writers who construct their plans and the
pre-texts that carry out those plans
student writer who does not redraft but writes “fi rst
draft/fi nal draft” papers, fi nished products
not changed over time. That is, they all stated that their
writing habits have not altered as they
two very defi nite multi-drafters, Bill and Pam; and two
writers, Karen and Cindy, who described
notes on paper or, as Amy and Nina did, sit for awhile and
mentally plan, but all expressed a clearly
topic while Pam said that she looks for something “broad or
ambiguous” or “something small that
ize her arguments until she writes them out and generally
writes and rewrites as she proceeds, but
task for this study. Since she had previously written the
same kind of letter assigned in this study,
that once they are done with a paper, they prefer not to
look back at it, either immediately to
who wanted fi rst drafts one week and revisions the next
week, Nina explained that she deliber
they know they are fi nished, Bill and Pam explained that
they never feel they are “done” with a piece
and friends every day and is getting to the point that she
doesn’t rewrite these letters as much. Bill
would be, and what format the writing would use. He moved
quickly from a search for a point
the reader. For their early drafts, Bill and Pam both
acknowledged that their writing is not yet
Conclusions
again, we also need to help them see how and when this can
be productive. Moreover, we can pro
that it may not be the best one. When Tim, one of the
one-drafters I observed, noted at one point
need to keep moving along, that does not always allow for
the production of good text, can obvi
ers but writers who may linger too long over making
choices. For writers who have diffi culty
Works Cited
and values of the law community. She not only learned about
the genre features of case briefs—
ing the bride’s parents and the groom), reinforce what the
student describes as a cultural attitude
way its users do their work). But the fi nal step would be
to ask students to produce new genres or genres that encode
alter
production of alternatives.
enact, and carry out our teaching lives. Just as genres may
provide a framework for facilitating both
Works Cited
“No. You can’t say that. You’ll bore them to death.” Ruth
is one of ten UCLA undergraduates with whom I discussed
writer’s block, that frustrating,
blasé attitudes toward school. They were set off from the
community by the twin facts that all
Rules
Plans
than the earlier research, they are still too neat, too
rigidly sequenced to approximate the stun
Flower and Hayes’ students who need more rules and plans,
blockers may well be stymied by
In high school, Ruth was told and told again that a good
essay always grabs a reader’s atten
This keeps Ruth from toying with ideas on paper, from the
kind of linguistic play that often frees
not at all synchronous with the plans and rules she uses to
discuss her exploration. It is interesting
Dale, Ellen, Debbie, Susan, and Miles all write with the
aid of rules. But their rules differ from block
that some blockers adhere to: e.g., “Rules like ‘write only
what you know about’ just aren’t true. I
rules and plans (Ellen: “I can throw things out”) that are
sometimes expressed with a vagueness
the fl uid way the goals and plans are conceived, and part
of it arises from the effective impact of
In most cases, the rules our blockers use are not “wrong”
or “incorrect”—it is good practice, for
Set
lates are not true plans at all but, rather, infl exible
and static cognitive blueprints. 15 Put another way,
cognition.
Feedback
The above diffi culties are made all the more problematic
by the fact that they seem resistant to or
the ease with which they seek out, interpret, and apply
feedback on their rules, plans, and produc
tions. They “operate” and then they “test,” and the testing
is not only against some internalized
that will aid her in rejecting some rules and balancing and
sequencing others.
A Note on Treatment
rigid rule or the infl exible plan that may lie at the base
of the student’s writing problem. That was
were in confl ict, and perhaps that confl ict was not
exclusively cognitive. Her case keeps analyses
Notes
Liu, J., & Sadler, R. W. (2003). Th e eff ect and aff ect
of peer review in electronic versus traditional modes on L2
writing. Journal of English for Academic Purposes, 2 (3),
193–227.
Macrorie, K. (1968). Writing to be read. New York: Hayden
Book Company.
Methodology
fi rst draft.
They are governed, like the linear model itself, by the Law
of Occam’s razor that prohibits logi
they feel that if they know what they want to say, then
there is little reason for making revisions. The only
modifi cation of ideas in the students’ essays occurred
when they tried out two or
“something larger” that they sensed was wrong and work from
there. The students do not have
and are bound to the rules which they have been taught.
that they have only one objective for each cycle and that
each cycle can be defi ned by a different
Notes
Acknowledgment
in.
Keen, A. (2010). Can the Internet save the book? Barnes and
Noble Review. www.salon.com/books/feature/2010/07/09/
clay_shirky/index.html.
the hell with ____; I’m going to write to who I damn well
want to; otherwise I can hardly
write at all.
mind from the start. We’ve been putting off writing that
letter to that person who intimidates us.
we need to write.
their writing. And think how much richer our writing would
be if we defi ned ourselves as inexpert
unawareness of readers.
are thinking too much about how their readers will receive
their words. They are acting too much
admires in Melville:
an awe. (158)
fact the power—of the voice. They would value this odd but
resonant voice if they found it in a pub
and readers, I was going to suggest four terms for the four
conditions listed above, but I gradually
— Are the readers’ needs being met? (The text may meet the
needs of readers whether the writer was thinking about
them or not, and whether the text is oriented toward them
or not.)
But on the other hand, we need the contrary model that affi
rms what is also obvious once we
education.
Power 50–77).
the writer has immaturely taken too much for granted and
unthinkingly assumed that her limited
process. How can she engage readers more till she has
engaged herself more?
they write! (And while we’re at it, let’s hook them up with
a better class of discourse community.)”
as Poesis or Play
(Cicotello 176)
And Chomsky:
I can be using language in the strictest sense with no
intention of communicating. … As a
of uses.
audiences.
tom reader.
about it.
its own sake and let it function a bit on its own, without
much intention and without much need
Contraries in Teaching
because they don’t try hard enough to keep the pen moving
and forget about readers. They must
persist and doggedly push aside those feelings of, “My head
is empty, I have run out of anything
then they turn around and let others overhear. Notice how
poets tend to argue for the importance
private and solitary and tune out others—to write only for
oneself and not give a damn about
to be a ham.
and read every word they write. Yet on the other hand,
schools characteristically offer little or no
Note
Works Cited
Elbow, Peter. Writing with Power. New York: Oxford UP, 1981.
under the system, then, that praise to the face was open
disgrace” (Hemingway 1964, 150).
man essays that had been graded and marked by four randomly
chosen and traditionally trained
errors to mark that not enough time remains for them to use
whole words or complete sentences
praise.
1977; Daly and Miller 1975a; Daly and Shamo 1978; Holland
1980). The problem for highly appre
and how often one writes, but even how others evaluate that
writing (Daly 1977). What may be
I cannot but justly hate him for the same” (1947, 617).
Following relative standards, we are in no
sense dishonest or condescending in praising one writer for
what we might ignore or criticize in
ber two in trade for the big curtain where Carol Marroll is
standing. The contestant, with
A. Conceptual level.
1. “Your thesis—that the new American ideal is ‘something
for nothing’—is strong and clear.”
6. “Your ideas are brilliant and the way you have argued
your point is convincing. Keep up with original and
thought-provoking ways of looking at life around you.”
B. Structural level.
C. Sentential level.
D. Lexical level.
3. “Nice title.”
It’s a good bet that genuine praise can lift the hearts, as
well as the pens, of the writers who sit in
students will take with them as they move from our class to
the next, from one paper assignment
not just in one course or from one teacher, but over four
years and across the disciplines. 2 To see
the check marks and squiggles are a good or bad thing will
not surprise us. That students might fi nd
also not surprise us. What did surprise us, though, is the
role feedback plays in the complex story
Other students stall and become stuck writing the same kind
of formulaic paper, again and again,
clear that it will be diffi cult for him to apply even the
best comments to future writing assignments
comments that ask her to slow down, read texts closely and
carefully, and, in a word, change. The
are grateful for the insights they have given us. And in
encouraging our students to imagine other
As our students teach us, their papers don’t end when they
turn them in for a grade, nor do
Acknowledgments
undergraduate year.
harvard.edu.
Works Cited
permission.
6 6. Genre: Irene L. Clark
tion, the man (sometimes a woman) with the money has pet
projects, such as Carnegie
and his libraries, and as long as the philanthropist is
alive, giving his money away is a
on heavy stock, and may include wood pulp mixed with cotton
or linen. Generally, let
| |
“the instrument with which the rhetor thinks and the realm
in and about which he thinks” (65; my
its own topoi within which teachers and students assume and
enact a complex set of desires,
and the various other genres that have already located them
and their teachers in an ideological
mann’s and Connors and Glenn’s (see also Murray and James
Williams), the writing prompt remains
they begin to write. The writing prompt not only moves the
student writer to action; it also cues
dents read their way into the position of writer via our
prompts. Given this, it is perhaps more than
herself as reader for the student text that the prompt will
eventually make possible. The challenge
work here is how the teacher creates the illusion that the
writer addressing them is not the same
Here, the student not only slips out of his assigned role
as a “cultural anthropologist” by acknowl
Summary
the FYW course does not just begin with them by virtue of
their being (enrolled) in this setting;
they do. … .
Notes
Works Cited
Freedman, Aviva, and Peter Medway, eds. Genre and the New
Rhetoric. Bristol: Taylor and Francis, 1994.
one can write with power. Even though it may take some
people a long time before they can write
stops you from writing right now, today, words that people
will want to read and even want to
about how to get real voice into your writing, but I will
present it in terms of an analysis of why
as we write.
up for all the writing you haven’t done. Use writing for as
many different tasks as you can. Keep a
ing the way we felt; thus most little children speak and
write with real voice.) Therefore I will use
other will make you more powerful in what you write. Then
read your rough writing to each other.
Because you often don’t even know what your power or your
inner self sounds like, you have to
try many different tones and voices. Fool around, jump from
one mood or voice to another, mimic,
you never use. And if, as sometimes happens, you know you
are angry but somehow cannot really
ence. Or at least not the wrong one: you may well have to
play it safe. But make sure you also work
for you. Perhaps you must write an essay for a teacher who
never seems to understand you;
it, yet they will turn out to have the kind of responses
they have to angry writing. That is, they will
condescendingly.
To the degree that you keep your anger hidden, you are
likely to write words especially lack
you are angry and tell your reader all your feelings in
whatever voices come. Then get back to the
whatever style comes out. Put all your effort into fi nding
the best ideas and arguments you can,
and don’t worry about your tone. After you express the
feelings and voices swirling around in you,
and after you get all the insights you can while not having
to worry about the audience and the
for that reader. That is, you can get past the anger and
confusion, but keep the good ideas and the
anger you have. It may be that you have three hundred pages
of angry words you need to say to
someone, but if you can get one page that really opens the
door all the way, that can be enough.
more economical.)
A long and messy path is common and benefi cial, but you
can get some of the benefi ts quicker if
but very short. And it’s for the wastepaper basket. Having
done this, I can turn to my offi cial evalu
they would rather not have. When you write in your real
voice, it often brings tears or shaking—
if it takes time.
Most children have real voice but then lose it. It is often
just plain loud: like screeching or bang
Children can usually feel when things are unfair, but they
are often persuaded to go along
Another reason people don’t use real voice is that they run
away from their power. There’s
people will say “You did this to me” and try to make you
feel responsible for some of their actions.
fi rst. You cause explosions when you thought you were just
asking for the salt or saying hello. In
effect I’m saying, “Why don’t you shoot that gun you have?
Oh yes, by the way, I can’t tell you how
it well. But how can you learn to aim well till you start
pulling the trigger? If you start letting your
people you know and trust deeply. Find people who are
willing to be in the same room with you
while you pull the trigger. Try using the power in ways
where the results don’t matter. Write letters
and corporate voice. You can avoid “I” and its fl avor, and
talk entirely in terms of “we” and “they”
meaning.
get them to read a piece and then ask them a week later
what they remember. Passages they dislike
and try again till at last the words began to ring true,
all of a sudden the writing got powerful and
If you seek real voice you should realize that you probably
face a dilemma. You probably have
But you are stuck. You can either use voices you like or
you can be heard. For a while, you can’t
have it both ways.
But if you do have the courage to use and inhabit that real
voice, you will get the knack of reso
not ultimately stuck with just one voice forever. But also
because it highlights the mystery. Real
way and not necessarily the best. Such writing can lead to
gushy or analytical words about how
hate mail, and the only work I’ve ever done that was
attacked at a national meeting by a colleague
or primal” (536).
Amen.
mission, its agenda for social reform, and its desire for
professional status. The early process move
critical pedagogy.
Ten years later, Gary Tate, Amy Ruppier, and Kurt Schick
edited a series of essays entitled A
Guide to Composition Pedagogies, in which the only mention
of style comes in William Covino’s essay
era.
the only sounds heard in the white walled room distant and
unreal: (Abs)
Here, the student sets the scene for the reader: a hospital
at night. One by one, the writer adds
ent passages into a single day’s work. “You must have time
to absorb what you have been observing
(2000, 295).
were. 3
does not want to keep his students away from learning and
understanding the dimensions of Gram
among ideas where they may not have looked before. His
double-voice technique encourages
have ignored his work. Wendy Bishop notes that his “work
didn’t seem to be half as infl uential
That was twenty years ago, and it seems safe to say that
Weathers’s reputation has not changed
much. My sense is that Weathers has been lumped into a
group of compositionists—including
Conclusion
During the early process years of the late 1960s and 1970s,
the teaching of style, via Christensen’s
Notes
(1991, 36).
rights reserved.
8 8. Teaching Grammar in the Context of
Writing: James D. Williams
labels that will enable them to think about and talk about
their language” (p. 150). Certainly our
An Instructive Example
It is worth noting at the outset that both sides in this
dispute—the grammarians and the antigram
correctness.
able” (p. 88). Yet Janice Neuleib in her essay found the
same conclusions to be “startling” and ques
issue.
of composition.
to Composition,” p. 250)
future discussions.
sound [s], differ from the spoken plurals of sea and gnu,
the sound [z], only in that the sounds of the
word endings, they would reject a) and b), then apply c),
producing the plural as /baxz/, with word
ers follow the second rule, they would have to analyze the
sound [x] as [non-labial, noncoronal,
Grammar 2
a stable entity, for its form changes with each new issue
of each linguistics journal, as new “rules of
offering a demonstration.
of grammatical choice.
down, and it soon hits ___ ground. Now ___ velocity with
which each succeeding cannonball
language.
structure rules:
S � VX
X � MX
(p. 92)
(P. 93)
agrees, but some people think they are, and for these
people, assuming that they have internalized
the rules, even inadequate rules are of heuristic value,
for they allow them to access the internal
Recognizing verbs.
Redefining Error
Vai tended to answer “yes” when asked (in Vai), “Can you
call the sun the moon and the moon
matical rules. 38
is square.
Notes
with permission.
9 9. Non-Native Speakers of English: John
R. Edlund and Olga Griswold
University Press.
Erlbaum.
guage Centre.
Development.
tion, 6, 186–214.
enterprise.
limitations. 3
A Pattern of Neglect
negligible.
to use with those ESL writers who are new to this context.
That is, many of these readers assume
composition studies.
it is variable.
written forms.
forms.
the target culture admire each other and see each other as
equals; both the learner’s culture and
to adapt their writing for sixth graders but not for adult
newspaper readers (Lusignan & Fortier,
tion research.
want to achieve and not when they achieve what the academy
wants them to. Profi ciency might be
many errors are not treated and that the more often an
error is made, the less likely a teacher
group input.
language. They not only need to access the memory but also,
in effect, translate the experience from
writer has in her or his mind but that does not appear in
the text. Mature writers are able to move
cross-cultural contexts.)
that power and wealth; those without, often could not. And
those who could not afford English
lessons that would allow them to obtain a high enough score
on the TOEFL to be admitted to, for
Conclusion
language research.
Notes
theorists.
and mathematics.
the persistency as, for example, the notion that the earth
is fl at: it conforms to our common-sense
much money as Sam” with “John has less money than Sam” is
paralleled by the synonymity of “John
does not have as little money as Sam” and “John has more
money than Sam.” All of the students
for example, or nearly any word from among those that Orr
fi nds her students having diffi culty with.
Orr’s Problem
Science Education
Racism
the former, then its speakers were protected under the body
of law supporting bilingual educa
Conclusion
Works Cited
I am not trying to imply that there has not been any effort
to address second language issues in
they are going, what they already know, what they need to
know, and how best to teach them. It
does surface from time to time in the work of those who are
otherwise knowledgeable about
Japan also attended Yale and Amherst College (King 11). The
fi rst infl ux of international students
suit in 1931 (Allen 307; Darian 77). While there were some
exceptions—such as the program at
sity grew over the last two centuries. Yet, the growing
presence of international students did
Works Cited
color, font, and placement of the text but also with the
creation of meaning(s) from juxtaposition
between modes. They saw, I hope, that the modes should not
simply repeat each other. For exam
between the written mode and the visual mode is richer and
more productive than in the other
Student Work
Gabe Mann
points the fi nger at Bush and the White House. The collage
interprets the word “control” with the
Trade Center are among the many images that, the collage
suggests, are controlled by Bush and
Stacy Johnson
Kelley Kaufmann
Behzad Khorsand
tion. MTV has “caught” the world and holds the world on a
fi shhook. For the fi rst part of the
(starrynite.swf)
cent Van Gogh, and the jazz piece “In a Sentimental Mood”
by Duke Ellington and John Coltrane.
word, image, and sound all come from very well known
sources. Casey’s project takes these ele
dots converge to form the words of the title, and then they
dissolve again into the small yellow dots
instance, the words “but still not sleeping” throb, and the
words “my weariness amazes me” gradu
(http://www.poemsthatgo.com/gallery/summer2002/sky/launch.htm)
and images each add their own part to the meaning, drawing
of course on the resources of each
mode.
(http://www.poemsthatgo.com/gallery/fall2001/murmuring/launch.htm)
Citations
Text
Image
with permission.
Audio
script.org.
Thanks