What Does It Take To Learn A Word
What Does It Take To Learn A Word
Author manuscript
Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
Author Manuscript
Abstract
Vocabulary learning is deceptively hard, but toddlers often make it look easy. Prior theories
proposed that children's rapid acquisition of words is based on language-specific knowledge and
constraints. In contrast, more recent work converges on the view that word learning proceeds via
domain-general processes that are tuned to richly structured—not impoverished—input. We argue
that new theoretical insights, coupled with methodological tools, have pushed the field toward an
appreciation of simple, content-free processes working together as a system to support the
acquisition of words. We illustrate this by considering three central phenomena of early language
development: referential ambiguity, fast-mapping, and the vocabulary spurt.
Keywords
Author Manuscript
Introduction
Words are deceptively simple, but profoundly important to language. The spoken form of a
word is a complex sequence of articulations and acoustic cues. In the lexicon (our mental
storehouse of words), these sequences must be linked to a rich set of semantic features, to
syntactic properties like part of speech, and to other representations like orthography (the
word's spelling). How is this complex set of information learned?
This question has engendered an enormous amount of research over the last 40 years. This
research illustrates a core issue in the cognitive sciences: Is human language acquired via
Author Manuscript
Virtually everyone agrees that children's ability for language is amazing. To reach an
average-sized vocabulary by kindergarten, children have been argued to learn up to nine new
words a day. In 1960, Quine1. illustrated the difficulty of this feat, which we paraphrase
here:
Imagine you are a field linguist studying a community whose language you do not
know. You go hunting with a group of tribesmen and see a rabbit hop past. One of
Samuelson and McMurray Page 2
the tribesmen shouts “gavagai.” How to you determine what this new word means?
Author Manuscript
It could be “rabbit” but it could also be “hopping,” “fluffy,” “dinner,” “get it!” or a
host of other things.
This problem is further complicated by the relative cognitive immaturity of the very young
learner: toddlers have a limited understanding of abstract concepts; they can't do math; they
can't hop on one foot; and they are still learning to feed themselves. When the challenging
problem of inferring a new word's meaning meets the poor cognitive skills of typical
children, this creates a mystery. How can children learn so many words so quickly?
For many years, the most widely-accepted answer to this question was that young learners
are imbued with specialized abilities and/or innate knowledge that guide them to the correct
word meanings. For example, children may come to the word learning table with the
assumption that most new words refer to whole objects (the rabbit) not parts (ears) or
Author Manuscript
features (fluffy); or they may assume that words refer to the more common or “basic level”
of description (e.g., gavagai means “rabbit”), rather than a subordinate level of description
(e.g., “eastern cottontail rabbit” or “Peter rabbit”) or a superordinate level of description
(e.g., “rodents”, “mammals”). Such knowledge is captured as constraints2, principles3, or
more recently, prior expectations4, and there is considerable evidence that children at some
ages behave in ways that appear consistent with these kind of language specific abilities.
Now things are changing. The field of word learning is in the middle of a shift in viewpoint.
There are new theoretical developments like a radical new understanding of learning (see
Aslin article in this collection) as well as a richer understanding of how toddlers' own bodies
play a role in cognition (see Oudeyer article in this volume and 5). These advances are being
supported by data from new technologies like eye-tracking and wearable cameras6. Finally,
sophisticated new computational tools are giving us an ever-clearer picture of the subtle
Author Manuscript
In many ways these innovations confirm prior findings regarding regularities in children's
behaviors, thereby supporting the prior work that is a powerful and important basis for our
understanding of word learning. However, these new advances offer critical insight into
where these principles and biases come from, and raise the possibility that they are not the
product of innate specialization. As a result, the field is shifting from a focus on identifying
and characterizing specialized abilities to an examination of the structure and richness of the
linguistic input, and to the often unexpected—or emergent—consequences of very simple
Author Manuscript
learning mechanisms. This shift has, in turn, increased our appreciation of how children
shape the input they receive and learn how to learn words as they go along.
Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
Samuelson and McMurray Page 3
fun toy with moveable beads, and so on. A new word could refer to any of these items; it
Author Manuscript
could also refer to a property of any of these objects—one table is blue, the other has red
legs, the tree is big; it could also relate the speaker's feelings or intentions with respect to the
objects.
In such a context, if the teacher were to say “wow, blicket!” how could the child possibly
figure out what the teacher was intending to communicate? Although complicated, children
appear able to solve this problem with very little effort. By 16 months of age, they have
learned that “table” can refer to the bright blue object in the foreground and they can
demonstrate their understanding of that word by pointing to it. Further, this concept is
already starting to become more complex—the same child may also understand that “table”
can refer to the other (red) table.
Early research10–14 suggested children were able to identify referents and create novel word-
Author Manuscript
Of course, this view may be optimistic. These kinds of estimates usually tap only the surface
of learning—probing, for instance, word usage in the simplest tasks. In this sense,
vocabulary estimates fail to capture the considerable changes that can happen in how words
Author Manuscript
are used and understood from childhood to adulthood. Nevertheless, given the scope of the
word-learning problem and children's relative cognitive immaturity, it is easy to be
impressed with their language-learning prowess. The hard part is to figure out how they do
this.
Before then, accounts of early cognition were often grounded in Piagetian theory, which
Author Manuscript
suggested that infants' and toddlers' conception of the world was bound to their sensory and
motor experiences, and grew more abstract as they constructed knowledge about the world
around them. According to Piagetian theory, for example, it was not until 7 years of age that
children were fully capable of logical, abstract thought.
In the last part of the 20th century, research started to suggest that Piaget's theoretical
account and empirical methods may have underestimated young children's abilities. New
Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
Samuelson and McMurray Page 4
techniques were developed that used measures of infants' looking rather than overt behavior
Author Manuscript
such as reaching or verbal responses. These suggested that even very young infants
understood basic principles of physics. For example, infants at some level seemed to
understand the fact that two solid objects cannot occupy the same physical space15, and
could distinguish causal and non-causal motion events16. This work seemed to suggest that
infants are endowed with a primitive understanding of objects and their mechanical
interactions, agents and their goal-directed interactions, number systems, places, and spatial
layouts, as well as the thinking of social partners17,18.
her novel container full of coffee as a “mug,” the child could use the whole object constraint
to map the word to the drinking container. And later, when mom said “can you grab it by the
handle?” because the child already knows “mug” she would use the mutual exclusivity
constraint to assume the new novel word (handle) must be mapped to something else.
Thus, children's systematic behaviors when learning new words were explained by a wide
range of different mechanisms, often with competing proposals to explain the same
behavior. Take for example, the systematic way that children behave when they hear a novel
name in the presence of both known and novel objects. The mutual-exclusivity constraint
describes this as a sort of reasoning based on an assumption about how words work21.
However, children's quick identification of novel referents in this situation could also derive
from children understanding that novel names tend go with novel categories11. Or children
Author Manuscript
may follow a principal that no two words mean exactly the same thing but that all word
meanings contrast in some way22. Alternatively, it may be based on children's knowledge
about others' behaviors – for example, children may assume that adults tend to name the
most novel thing in a context23,24. All of these lead to similar patterns of behaviors, though
from ostensibly different reasoning principles. However, they also pin this behavior on the
idea that the child comes to the table with some useful knowledge or assumptions about how
to interpret new words.
In a similar way, the set of explanations proposed for the vocabulary spurt included many
different language-specific processes. For example, there might be a shift from learning
based on association to a conceptual understanding that words are not just associated to
objects, but that they serve to refer to objects as part of a communicative system, that is, they
act as symbols25. Alternatively, children might achieve the sudden insight about the nature
Author Manuscript
of language itself, for example realizing that most objects tend to have names (the naming
insight)12, or that that most words refer to categories of objects, not individual items26.
These accounts offered detailed descriptions of children's behaviors when confronted with
novel objects and complex learning scenarios.
Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
Samuelson and McMurray Page 5
they rely on knowledge and processes that are tailored to the specific problems of learning
Author Manuscript
words, and often to specific situations or specific sub-parts of the more general problem. A
second notable feature is that they are, for the most part, static: these accounts do not
suggest a mechanism for how these word learning behaviors develop. This question of
developmental process—the causal events that give rise to the behaviors that support word
learning—is driving a shift in the field.
that appear in many other domains of cognition, may underlie word learning, and sometimes
even conspire to make children look and act as if they have knowledge that is highly
specialized for the problem of learning words. This work opens the door to examining how
word-learning behaviors are shaped by non-linguistic aspects of the child's environment and
the child's interaction with that environment. It suggests that children may be amazing word
learners not because they are endowed with amazing innate abilities, but because they
flexibly assemble a set of simple processes to rapidly learn many, many words.
One impetus for the emphasis on domain general processes comes from a novel view of the
problem faced by children. The dominant framing of the problem of referential ambiguity
largely derives from an adult-centric perspective: Adults know there are many possible ways
to talk about a scene and see many possible referents for a novel word. Consequently from
Author Manuscript
the adult perspective, the problem of referential ambiguity looms large and may even be
insurmountable – there are just too many possible meanings for a new word in a new scene.
However, this may not accurately reflect the child's perspective. Recent work has examined
the referent selection problem from the child's view using head-mounted cameras and eye-
tracking systems27. It turns out that young word-learners do not typically have large
numbers of objects in view. Rather, their short arms and smaller stature mean that often there
are only one or two objects in view when names are provided (Figure 2)28. Thus, children
confront a much narrower version of referential ambiguity than Quine assumed (contrast the
top and bottom panels of Figure 2). While this may not rule in or out more abstract
interpretations of a novel word (feelings and intentions, for example), it certainly gets the
child to the right object, and raises the possibility that previously unexplored, more general,
factors like the size of the visual field, or the physical abilities of the child play an important
Author Manuscript
role.
Similarly, children's selection of an unnamed object as the referent for a novel word might
not be the outcome of a sophisticated deductive reasoning process but instead the result of
their attraction to the most novel object in a context29,30. That is, even with no linguistic
input, children tend to pay more attention to objects that are new31. Similarly, children tend
pay attention to things that are in their mother's hands – this simple attentional bias can often
Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
Samuelson and McMurray Page 6
lead them to choose items that have recently been manipulated, offered or touched,
Author Manuscript
mimicking a process of social inference where children appear to know what mothers are
intending to refer to32,33. Finally, parents often label whatever children happen to be
attending to34, basically solving the referential ambiguity problem for the child. All of these
situations are ones that we used to think of as driven by knowledge (like mutual exclusivity)
or skills (like social inference) that were geared to learning words. However, as we describe
below, it quickly becomes apparent that they could also be the result of many general
processes—like attentional biases–that work together to support infants' selection of a
referent in the moment. That doesn't of course rule out that such knowledge or skills play a
role – particularly later in development as children become cognitively and socially more
advanced. However, it again highlights how rather unexpected domain general factors could
be doing much of the work, particularly early in infancy.
Author Manuscript
because many prior studies did not test the children after a delay or failed to test their
memory, instead re-testing their ability to solve the mapping problem.
At the same time, though, retention is not divorced from the process of word learning. When
children explore the to-be-named novel objects prior to the naming event, retention
increases36. Retention abilities also improve over the course of early vocabulary
development, such that by 2.5 years children do reliably demonstrate retention of word-
referent mappings formed after brief exposure37. It thus appears that rather than
instantaneously learning novel word-object mappings from the very earliest ages, children's
word learning abilities grow as they acquire vocabulary38,39 and knowledge about things in
the world, how they are named, how people talk about them28 and how people interact40.
But, this is bigger than just fast-mapping – learning a word is really a slow process of
Author Manuscript
gradually determining what kinds of things a word refers to 36,37,15,38, see, e.g., 39,40. In fact,
recent studies have shown that children and adults can learn new word/object mappings even
if there is no information to solve the referential ambiguity problem in a given encounter –
when all the objects in an encounter are equally novel44,45. In this situation, it appears that
what learners do is gradually accumulate how likely a word is to be heard with many
different objects, and choose the most likely object for a word42, but see 43. This again
Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
Samuelson and McMurray Page 7
suggests that the process of retention is distinct from the process of referent selection since
Author Manuscript
people appear to also be able to retain words even without successful referent selection.
Furthermore, longer term learning is not quite the same as the processes children use to
solve the referential ambiguity problem14. Rather, recent experiments with fast-mapping,
suggest it is not necessarily a logical inference problem. When children encounter a novel
word, there are multiple possible interpretations. These compete during the short time
between when the word is heard and when the child responds, and this competition is biased
by a variety of domain-general processes like attention, selection, and the history of learning
about the words and the objects; it may also be biased in older children by things like their
understanding of others' intentions or their knowledge of the language48. At the end of this
competition, the link between the word and the interpretation of a word that wins (e.g., the
referent selected in that moment) is strengthened while any links between that word and
other possible referents are weakened8.
Author Manuscript
over many encounters, work on fast-mapping has started to focus on longer term processes
that unfold over development. However, a sizeable body of research has examined even
longer-time scales, asking how the child's vocabulary (typically the number of words known)
changes over the course of months or even years. Do we see a similar move toward domain
general processes here? As it turns out, the answer is ‘yes’.
One of the most important phenomena in this domain is the so-called vocabulary spurt. The
vocabulary spurt is defined by a rapid acceleration of the pace at which toddlers add new
words to their productive vocabulary. As can be seen in Figure 3, in the first few months
after children produce their first word, new words are added to the vocabulary slowly—one
or two a week. Around the time that children have 50 words in their productive vocabulary,
typically near 18 months of age, they start adding words much more quickly. Thus, there
Author Manuscript
This phenomenon had previously been understood to indicate an underlying shift in the
mechanisms supporting word learning. These kind of explanations included things like the
sudden onset of constraints or principles (like mutual exclusivity), the acquisition of skills
for inferring other people's intentions (e.g., which object did they intend to name), or a
sudden insight about language like the naming insight. However, McMurray (2007)
Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
Samuelson and McMurray Page 8
demonstrated that the accelerating trajectory of the word spurt is actually the necessary
Author Manuscript
consequence of two basic facts about word learning: 1) children learn multiple words at
once, and 2) those words vary in difficulty (with most words being moderately difficult).
These are both fairly non controversial.
That's not to say that children's social skills may not also be improving at this time, or that
they are not developing new strategies that can assist in referent selection and/or retention.
Indeed, developmental studies suggest there are big changes around this age in a number of
abilities including children's use of eye-gaze52, their general pragmatic competence53, and
categorization (Gopnik & Meltzoff, 1987). Likewise, exciting recent work demonstrates that
as children's vocabularies grow, they become better able to use what they know about
words54, how words go together55–57, and how people talk to each other58 to learn even
more words. However, such changes are not required to explain the spurt—it is an emergent
consequence of a very simple property of learning.
Author Manuscript
More importantly, however, this new view suggests that all of these general processes are at
the heart of early word learning and that they work together with developing social
competencies – that also extend beyond the realm of word learning - to support and
bootstrap both the child's initial lexical development and their growing representations of
syntax and more complex linguistic mappings. Furthermore, these processes unfold
Author Manuscript
dynamically over time. As a result, objects that draw the attention of a 15-month-old with a
small vocabulary will present themselves differently when she is 30 months of age and
knows more words and can engage in more complex linguistic interactions with others. In
this way, domain-general processes that support word learning change over development to
enable smart word learning to emerge from the joint action of multiple simple processes —
none of which by itself is particularly smart. Thus, this perspective suggests that word
learning is amazing not for being supported by domain-specific and special-purpose
Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
Samuelson and McMurray Page 9
processes, but for the way simple, domain-general processes work together as a system to
Author Manuscript
We are also starting to understand how these influences cascade over development. This is
critical when we consider that word learning is not conducted in a vacuum – children must
Author Manuscript
learn which words go with which meanings at the same time as they are learning how to
produce and perceive speech. For example, Jana Iverson and colleagues have examined the
fact that children at risk for autism show a later onset of complex babbling (for related
results, see Iverson essay in this volume). They investigated the developmental precursors of
this, finding that these children also show less mature visual-manual exploration which, in
turn, leads them to less oral exploration of objects (e.g., mouthing objects), which impairs
their articulatory development 65–67. It is these kinds of developmental cascades – from
primarily real-time behaviors like manual exploration and mouthing, to longer term
developmental changes like the stability and precision of speech articulation – that create the
articulation and auditory perception abilities that are the foundation for the word learning
skills we have discussed here.
Author Manuscript
Furthermore, recent work suggests that these complex problems of development that cross
multiple levels from perception, to action, to social interaction, to cognition, and timescales
from in-the-moment behavior to learning, may actually be easier to solve simultaneously
rather than in isolation. For, example recent computational modeling68 suggests as children
acquire mappings between words and object mappings (as we've discussed here), this may
actually help early auditory organization, by teaching them which sounds are meaningfully
different.
This systems view may also pave the way for smarter interventions. It is well known that
children's word knowledge can vary greatly across factors such as socio-economic status,
gender, and reading level. For example, children who struggle with language and hearing
impairments know fewer words and know less about them69–71 But an overemphasis on the
role of endowed knowledge and/or constraints offers little leverage when learning goes awry.
Author Manuscript
For instance, if we believe the primary deficit in autism is an innate inability to understand
the intentions of others, intervention must focus on changes to that endowment. In contrast,
Iverson's work suggests interventions for children at risk for autism should focus on
supporting early motor development—boosting infants' abilities to manipulate and explore
objects which may cascade forward to increase exploration in oral articulation, setting the
stage for early communication. Similarly, recent research suggests children with Specific
Language Impairment have a deficit in real-time processing such that competition between
Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
Samuelson and McMurray Page 10
representations of words is not strong enough to resolve ambiguity during recognition. This
Author Manuscript
could cascade forward to hurt future leaning because an inability to determine the correct
word in the moment means representations cannot be updated with new information. This
raises the possibility that early interventions aimed at boosting competitive recognition
processes may change the later course of word learning and language development in these
children.
Of course, much work is needed to specify the relations between real-time behaviors,
learning, and development. But recent changes in multiple aspects of the field—from
experimental, observational, and statistical methods, to the theoretical view of where
knowledge originates—open the door to a much richer understanding of a child's developing
language system and may also offer multiple avenues for changing it72.
This manuscript is part of a forthcoming special issue and does not have a graphical abstract
Author Manuscript
at this time.
Acknowledgments
Preparation of this article was supported by NIH grants to LKS (HD045713) and BM (DC008089). The content is
solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National
Institutes of Health.
References
1. Quine, WVO. Word and object. (Cambridge University Press; 1960.
2. Markman EM. Constraints children place on word meanings. Cogn Sci. 1990; 14:57–77.
3. Golinkoff RM, Mervis CB, Hirsh-Pasek K. Early object labels: the case for a developmental lexical
principles framework. J Child Lang. 1994; 21:125–155. [PubMed: 8006089]
Author Manuscript
4. Xu F, Tenenbaum JB. Word learning as Bayesian inference. Psychol Rev. 2007; 114:245–272.
[PubMed: 17500627]
5. Yu C, Smith LB. Embodied attention and word learning by toddlers. Cognition. 2012; 125:244–262.
[PubMed: 22878116]
6. Roy BC, Frank MC, Decamp P, Miller M, Roy D. Predicting the birth of a spoken word. Proc Natl
Acad Sci U S A. 2015; 112:12663–12668. [PubMed: 26392523]
7. Brent MR, Siskind JM. The role of exposure to isolated words in early vocabulary development.
Cognition. 2001; 81:B33–44. [PubMed: 11376642]
8. McMurray B, Horst JS, Samuelson LK. Word learning emerges from the interaction of online
referent selection and slow associative learning. Psychol Rev. 2012; 119:831–877. [PubMed:
23088341]
9. Frank MC, Goodman ND, Tenenbaum JB. Using Speakers ' Referential Intentions to Model Early
Cross-Situational Word Learning. Psychol Sci. 2009; 20:578–585. [PubMed: 19389131]
10. Carey S, Bartlett E. Acquiring a Single New Word. Pap reports child Lang Dev. 1978; 15:17–29.
Author Manuscript
11. Golinkoff RM, Hirsh-Pasek K, Bailey LM, Wenger NR. Young children and adults use lexical
principles to learn new nouns. Dev Psychol. 1992; 28:99–108.
12. Mervis CB, Bertrand J. Acquisition of the Novel Name — Nameless Category ( N3C ) Principle
Mervis and Bertrand. 1994:1646–1662.
13. Dollaghan C. Child meets word Fast mapping in preschool children. J Speech, Lang Hear Res.
1985; 28:449–454.
14. Kucker SC, McMurray B, Samuelson LK. Slowing Down Fast Mapping: Redefining the Dynamics
of Word Learning. Child Dev Perspect. 2015; 9 n/a–n/a.
Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
Samuelson and McMurray Page 11
15. Baillargeon R. Object permanence in 3 1/2- and 4 1/2-month-old infants. Dev Psychol. 1987;
23:655–664.
Author Manuscript
16. Leslie AM, Keeble S. Do six-month-old infants percieve causality? Cognition. 1987; 25:265–288.
[PubMed: 3581732]
17. Spelke ES, Kinzler KD. Core knowledge. Dev Sci. 2007; 10:89–96. [PubMed: 17181705]
18. Spelke, ES.; Newport, EL. Handbook of child psychology. In: Damon, W.; Lerner, RM., editors.
Theoretical models of human development. 6th. Vol. 1. John Wiley & Sons Inc; 1998. p. 275-340.
19. Golinkoff RM, Hirsh-Pasek K. Baby wordsmith from associationist to social sophisticate. Curr Dir
Psychol Sci. 2006; 15:30–33.
20. Woodward, AL.; Markman, EM. Handbook of child psychology: Volume 2: Cognition, Perception,
and Language. W, D.; Series, E.; Kuhn, D.; Siegler, RS., editors. Wiley; 1998. p. 371-420.
21. Markman EM, Wachtel GF. Children's use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of
words. Cogn Psychol. 1988; 20:121–157. [PubMed: 3365937]
22. Clark EV. Pragmatics in acquisition. J Child Lang. 2014; 41:105–116. [PubMed: 25023500]
23. Akhtar N, Carpenter M, Tomasello M. The Role of Discourse Novelty in Early Word Learning.
Child Dev. 1996; 67:635.
Author Manuscript
24. Diesendruck G, Markson L. Children's avoidance of lexical overlap: a pragmatic account. Dev
Psychol. 2001; 37:630–641. [PubMed: 11552759]
25. Nazzi T, Bertoncini J. Before and after the vocabulary spurt: Two modes of word acquisition? Dev
Sci. 2003; 6:136–142.
26. Gopnik A, Meltzoff AN. The Development of Categorization in the 2nd Year and Its Relation to
Other Cognitive and Linguistic Developments. Child Dev. 1987; 58:1523–1531.
27. Yoshida H, Smith LB. What's in View for Toddlers? Using a Head Camera to Study Visual
Experience Infancy. 2008; 13:229–248. [PubMed: 20585411]
28. Pereira AF, Smith LB, Yu C. A bottom-up view of toddler word learning. Psychon Bull Rev. 2014;
21:178–185. [PubMed: 23813190]
29. Samuelson LK, Smith LB. Memory and Attention Make Smart Word Learning: An Alternative
Account of Akhtar, Carpenter, and Tomasello. Child Dev. 1998; 69:94–104. [PubMed: 9499560]
30. Kucker SC, McMurray B, Samuelson LK. Too much of a good thing: How novelty biases and
vocabulary influence familiar and novel referent selection in 18-month-old children and
Author Manuscript
37. Bion, RaH; Borovsky, A.; Fernald, A. Fast mapping, slow learning: disambiguation of novel word-
object mappings in relation to vocabulary learning at 18, 24, and 30months. Cognition. 2013;
126:39–53. [PubMed: 23063233]
38. Grassmann S, Schulze C, Tomasello M. Children's level of word knowledge predicts their
exclusion of familiar objects as referents of novel words. Front Psychol. 2015; 6:1200. [PubMed:
26322005]
39. Samuelson LK, Kucker SC, Spencer JP. Moving word learning to a novel space: A dynamic
systems view of referent selection and retention. Cogn Sci.
40. Clark EV. On the pragmatics of contrast. J Child Lang. 1990; 17:417–432. [PubMed: 2199470]
Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
Samuelson and McMurray Page 12
41. Carey, S. Linguistic Theory and Psychological Reality. Bresnan, G.; Miller, G.; Halle, M., editors.
MIT Press; 1978. p. 264-293.
Author Manuscript
42. Swingley D. Fast mapping and slow mapping in children's word learning. Lang Learn Dev. 2010;
6:179–183.
43. McGregor KK, Capone N. The Effect of Semantic Word Retrieval. J Speech, Lang Hear Res. 2005;
48:1468–1481. [PubMed: 16478384]
44. Smith LB, Yu C. Infants rapidly learn word-referent mappings via cross-situational statistics.
Cognition. 2008; 106:1558–68. [PubMed: 17692305]
45. Yurovsky D, Fricker DC, Yu C, Smith LB. The role of partial knowledge in statistical word
learning. Psychon Bull Rev. 2014; 21:1–22. [PubMed: 23702980]
46. Roembke T, McMurray B. Beyond Propose-But_Verify and Associative Bean Counting. J Mem
Lang. 2016; 87:105–127. [PubMed: 26858510]
47. Trueswell JC, Medina TN, Hafri A, Gleitman LR. Propose but verify: fast mapping meets cross-
situational word learning. Cogn Psychol. 2013; 66:126–56. [PubMed: 23142693]
48. Tomasello, M.; Bates, E. Language Development: The Essential Readings Essential readings in
developmental psychology. Malden: Blackwell Publishing; 2001. p. 111-128.
Author Manuscript
58. Sullivan J, Barner D. Discourse bootstrapping: Preschoolers use linguistic discourse to learn new
words. Dev Sci. 2016; 19:63–75. [PubMed: 25702754]
59. Gottlieb G, Lickliter R. Probabilistic epigenesis. Dev Sci. 2007; 10:1–11. [PubMed: 17181692]
60. Oyama, S. The ontogeny of information: Developmental systems and evolution. Duke University
Press; 2000.
61. Spencer JP, et al. Short arms and talking eggs: Why we should no longer abide the nativist-
empiricist debate. Child Dev Perspect. 2009; 3:79–87. [PubMed: 19784383]
62. Yeung HH, Chen LM, Werker JF. Referential Labeling Can Facilitate Phonetic Learning in Infancy.
Child Dev. 2013; 00:1–14.
63. Robinson CW, Sloutsky VM. Visual processing speed: Effects of auditory input on visual
processing. Dev Sci. 2007; 10:734–740. [PubMed: 17973789]
64. Samuelson LK, Smith LB, Perry LK, Spencer JP. Grounding word learning in space. PLoS One.
2011; 6:e28095. [PubMed: 22194807]
Author Manuscript
65. Leezenbaum NB, Campbell SB, Butler D, Iverson JM. Maternal verbal responses to
communication of infants at low and heightened risk of autism. Autism. 2014; 18:694–703.
[PubMed: 24113343]
66. Iverson JM, Wozniak RH. Variation in Vocal-Motor Development in Infant Siblings of Children
with Autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007; 37:158–170. [PubMed: 17191097]
67. Koterba, Ea; Leezenbaum, NB.; Iverson, JM. Object exploration at 6 and 9 months in infants with
and without risk for autism. Autism. 2014; 18:97–105. [PubMed: 23175749]
68. Rasanen O, Rasilo H. Joint model of word segmentation and meaning acquisition through cross
situatinal learning. Psychol Rev.
Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
Samuelson and McMurray Page 13
69. McGregor KK, Newman RM, Reilly RM, Capone NC. Semantic representation and naming in
children with specific language impairment. J Speech, Lang Hear Res. 2002; 45:998–1014.
Author Manuscript
[PubMed: 12381056]
70. McGregor KK, Oleson J, Bahnsen A, Duff D. Children with developmental language impairment
have vocabulary deficits characterized by limited breadth and depth. Int J Lang Commun Disord.
2013; 48:307–319. [PubMed: 23650887]
71. Tomblin JB, Harrison M, Ambrose SE, Walker EA, MP M. Languae outcomes in young children
with mild to severe hearing loss. Ear Hear.
72. Karmiloff Smith A. Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders.
Trends Cogn Sci. 1998; 2:389–398. [PubMed: 21227254]
73. Plunkett K. Lexical segmentation and vocabulary growth in early language acquisition. J Child
Lang. 1993; 20:43–60. [PubMed: 8454686]
Further Reading
74. Kucker SC, McMurray B, Samuelson LK. Slowing Down Fast Mapping: Redefining the Dynamics
Author Manuscript
of Word Learning. Child Development Perspectives. 2015; 9(2) n/a–n/a. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12110
75. Roy BC, Frank MC, Decamp P, Miller M, Roy D. Predicting the birth of a spoken word. 2015;
2015(15):1–6. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1419773112
76. Smith LB. It's all connected: Pathways in visual object recognition and early noun learning. The
American Psychologist. 2013; 68(8):618–29. DOI: 10.1037/a0034185 [PubMed: 24320634]
77. Spencer JP, Blumberg MS, McMurray B, Robinson SR, Samuelson LK, Tomblin JB. Short arms
and talking eggs: Why we should no longer abide the nativist-empiricist debate. Child
Development Perspectives. 2009; 3(2):79–87. DOI: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2009.00081.x [PubMed:
19784383]
78. Yu C. Linking words to world: an embodiment perspective. The Routledge Handbook of Embodied
Cognition. 2014:139–149.
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
Samuelson and McMurray Page 14
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Figure 1. A typical preschool classroom features many potential referents for a new word
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
Samuelson and McMurray Page 15
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Figure 2. Differences in the number of namable objects in view from the child's (top panel) and
parent's (bottom panel) perspective
Author Manuscript
Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
Samuelson and McMurray Page 16
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Figure 3. Number of words known as a function of time for individual children. From Plunkett73
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.