Experiments in Attention
Experiments in Attention
At any given moment there are several stimuli in the environment competing for our attention.
However, our sense organs can respond to only a limited number of them at the same time. This
limit is known as span of attention. The span varies from individual to individual, from sense
organ to sense organ, and also according to the nature of the stimuli. The earliest psychologist to
be interested in the problem was Sir William Hamilton, who made a very crude experimental
attempt to study the problem.
An advance was made on Hamilton’s method by Jevons, the logician. However, real scientific
experimental work on the problem was started by J.M. Cattell, who used the tachistoscope for
this experiment. After Cattell, a number of experimenters have studied the span of attention
under different conditions. Later experimenters have distinguished between span of attention and
span of apprehension and also found that span of apprehension is greater than span of attention.
Problem:
To determine the span of attention for the following type of Visual stimuli:
1. Single dots
2. Grouped dots
ADVERTISEMENTS:
Now we are ready to determine the span. For experimental purposes the span can be defined as
the maximum number of dots to which at least 75 per cent of correct responses are made, viz. if a
subject responds 100 per cent correctly to three dots, 83.3 per cent to four dots and 66.67 per
cent to five dots, his span lies between 4 and 5. The span can now be determined by interpolation
between 4 and 5.
Procedure with the Other Sets of Cards:
The procedure for the other sets is essentially the same excepting for the instructions,
which are as following:
i. Instructions for Groups of Dots:
“This time instead of single dots you will see small separate groups of 3 dots each. After seeing
each card, tell me how many groups of dots are there in each card”.
ii. Instructions for Nonsense Syllables:
“In this series you will see some syllables instead of dots. After seeing each card, write down the
syllables as correctly as possible.”
iii. Instructions for Meaningful Words:
“Here on each card you will find a familiar and meaningful word. Try to write down the word
you see on each card.”
iv. Instructions for Numbers:
“In this set, instead of words or dots you will find numbers; as before you will have to write
down the number you see”.
After exposing all the sets determine the span in each case as illustrated in the case of dots. The
whole experiment can be done in two sessions. Otherwise the subject is likely to get bored and
fatigued.
(1) Study individual variations in the span for the different types.
(2) Compare the Spans:
There will be some interesting findings with respect to the differences in the attention spans
between single dots and groups of dots. The subject who has a span of 6 single dots may also
have a span of 6 for groups of dots though the latter actually includes 18 dots. This is because of
the factor of grouping. Each group of dots is responded to as a single stimulus, because of the
factor of organisation.
Similarly the span for meaningful words will be usually much higher than the span for nonsense
syllables, though both are made up of same number of letters of alphabet. This is because of the
factor of meaning and familiarity. In the case of meaningful words and numbers there is
apprehension or understanding in addition to mere attention. Furthermore, the factor of
familiarity is helpful.
Application:
This experiment has a number of practical applications. A very common illustration is the
registration numbers given to automobiles. Usually, automobile numbers do not exceed four
digits. This is because the traffic constable would be unable to note down the registration number
of automobiles violating traffic rules if the number exceeds four digits. However, the letters of
alphabet before the numbers are perceived because they are grouped separately.
When we are attending to some stimulus or work, any noise or other type of disturbance tends to
affect the efficiency of our attention. This phenomenon of irrelevant stimuli interfering with our
attentive process is called ‘distraction’. Not all stimuli can distract out attention, viz., the ticking
of a table clock on our study table does not ordinarily disturb us. Sometimes even strong stimuli
do not disturb us when we are prepared for it.
One experiment showed that students working on some problems could, to a large extent, resist
distractions of different types by putting in more effort. Baker employing dance music as
distractor found that in many instances, the subject did better when music was played. Morgan in
his classical experiments proved that subjects can soon get used to a distracting influence, and
that often efficiency is lost when distracting influence is removed.
Introspective reports, however, show that subjects feel a greater strain and have to put in greater
effort under distracting conditions to maintain the same level of efficiency of attention.
Experiments on distraction are usually carried out as group experiments.
Problem:
To determine the effect of extraneous and irrelevant stimuli on the work efficiency.
Material Required:
A long list of arithmetic problems of uniform difficulty, a sound proof room fitted with number
of buzzers, bells, bright lights, etc., to serve as visual and auditory distractions.
Procedure:
The experiment is done under four conditions:
1. Controlled condition.
2. Auditory distraction.
3. Visual distraction.
4. Combination of visual and auditory distraction.
The experiment can be conducted by adopting any one of the following experimental
designs:
Experimental Design 1:
Different groups of subjects are assigned to the four conditions.
Experimental Design 2:
The performance of all the subjects under controlled conditions, without any kind of deliberate
distraction, is assessed and on the basis of these scores, the subjects are grouped into three
matched groups. Each one of these groups is assigned to each one of the three conditions of
distraction.
Experimental Design 3:
The performance of each subject is assessed under all the four conditions.
In the first experimental design, the subjects are selected and assigned to the four conditions by
following the method of randomisation.
In the second experimental design, the subjects are categorised into three matched groups by
following any one of the techniques of matching the groups, and each one of these groups is
assigned to one experimental condition by following the method of randomisation.
In the third experimental design the subjects are categorised into four groups by following the
method of randomisation and the performance of each one of these groups under all the four
conditions is observed. However, the order of presentation of the four conditions should be
counter-balanced.
Instructions to the Subjects:
Give the selected arithmetic problems to the subjects and ask them to solve them.
1. Controlled Condition:
For five minutes allow them to solve the problems under normal conditions, and then ask them to
highlight the last problem they have solved.
2. Auditory Distraction:
Suddenly, at the end of 5 minutes, switch on the buzzers and the bells so that the room is filled
with loud noises. The subjects have to continue solving the problems. Ask the subjects to
indicate the last problem they have solved.
3. Visual Distraction:
At the end of five minutes switch off the buzzers but switch on the bright lights, flashing glaring
lights of different colours and ask the subjects to mark the last problem they have solved.
4. Combination of Visual and Auditory Distraction:
At the end of five minutes, switch on both the buzzers and the lights and ask the subjects to
highlight the last problem solved.
Results:
Now collect the answer sheets and correct them. Tabulate the number of problems attempted and
the number correctly solved for each of the five-minute periods. Take the introspective report of
the subject.
Tabulate the results as follows:
Compare the results under the four conditions. See whether work efficiency has, been affected.
Analyse the introspective reports to find out the subjects inner reactions to various distractions.
Also find out whether they had to put in greater effort to carry out the work under different
conditions of distraction.
Tabulate group results as follows:
1. Calculate the Mean & SD under all the conditions for problems attempted as well as problems
correctly solved.
2. Do all subjects show the same type of change under distraction?
3. Which condition is most distracting for the group and which the least?
4. Do all the subjects show the same trend of performance under all the four conditions?
It may be interesting to study the effect of preparedness of the subject for distraction.
Procedure:
Instruct the subjects and give them prior information about the occurrence of the distraction. This
can be done by giving the instructions for all the conditions at the beginning or specifically
before the start of each session studying the effect of a specified condition.
Applications:
Such experiments are useful in pinpointing factors that distract workers in factories, offices, etc.
where the efficiency of the workers can be improved by eliminating the distracting conditions.
Industrial psychologists have carried out several experiments on this subject. It has been found
that minimisation of noise in the work situation facilitates the employees to concentrate better on
their tasks resulting in better output. Further, excess of noise has also been found to lead to
stress.