Effects of Learning Skills Intervention
Effects of Learning Skills Intervention
Research http://rer.aera.net
Published on behalf of
and
http://www.sagepublications.com
Additional services and information for Review of Educational Research can be found at:
Subscriptions: http://rer.aera.net/subscriptions
Reprints: http://www.aera.net/reprints
Permissions: http://www.aera.net/permissions
Citations: http://rer.sagepub.com/content/66/2/99.refs.html
What is This?
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net by Sheraz Ahmad on July 19, 2014
Review of Educational Research
Summer 1996, Vol. 66, No. 2, pp. 99-136
John Hattie
University of North Carolina at Greensboro
John Biggs
University of Hong Kong
Nola Purđie
Queensland University of Technology
The aim of this review is to identify features of study skills interventions that
are likely to lead to success. Via a meta-analysis we examine 51 studies in
which interventions aimed to enhance student learning by improving student
use of either one or a combination of learning or study skills. Such interven-
tions typically focused on task-related skills, self-management of learning, or
affective components such as motivation and self-concept. Using the SOLO
model (Biggs & Collis, 1982), we categorized the interventions (a) into four
hierarchical levels of structural complexity and (b) as either near or far in
terms oftransfer. The results support the notion ofsituated cognition, whereby
it is recommended that training other than for simple mnemonic performance
should be in context, use tasks within the same domain as the target content,
and promote a high degree of learner activity and metacognitive awareness.
The research was facilitated by a grant from the Australian Research Grants.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to the first author.
99
itself; creating positive attitudes toward both content and context; and minimizing
learning pathologies. Some of these variables are both mediating and true depen
dent variables; for example, study skill enhancement may be an end in itself or a
subgoal whereby enhanced performance is the ultimate criterion by which the
success of the intervention is to be judged and the enhancement of study skills is
the means by which performance itself is enhanced. A general explanation for
these programs is that they are interventions for enhancing learning.
Interventions may broadly be classified as cognitive, metacognitive, and affec
tive in nature. Cognitive interventions are those that focus on developing or
enhancing particular task-related skills, such as underlining, note taking, and
summarizing. Specific skills taught directively are seen as tactics, which can be
grouped and used purposefully as a strategy (Snowman, 1984). Deny and Murphy
(1986) described these strategies as "the collection of mental tactics employed by
an individual in a particular learning situation to facilitate acquisition of knowl
edge or skill" (p. 2). Metacognitive interventions are those that focus on the self-
management of learning, that is, on planning, implementing, and monitoring one's
learning efforts, and on the conditional knowledge of when, where, why, and how
to use particular tactics and strategies in their appropriate contexts. Affective
interventions are those that focus on such noncognitive aspects of learning as
motivation and self-concept. Attributions for success and failure were regarded
here as affective.
Intervention programs may comprise any one or more of these kinds of targets.
In fact, whereas in earlier interventions the thrust was in teaching cognitive skills
and strategies directly, in recent years the emphasis has shifted to embedding the
application of such skills in specific contexts, as is explained in the review below.
The aim of this review is to identify features of study skills interventions that are
likely to lead to success. Via a meta-analysis, we survey the more recent interven
tion studies so as to assess the relative effect sizes of different kinds and conditions
of intervention and, more generally, to see the extent to which the various
theoretical stances may be supported.
Literature Review
There has been an enormous amount of research on study skills. In the ERIC
database we located 1,415 separate journal articles, published between 1982 and
1992, reporting research on various aspects of study skills (although only a
fraction of that number appear in the present study, for reasons expressed below).
There have been reviews of some of this literature (e.g., Hartley, 1986; Pintrich &
de Groot, 1990; Tabberer, 1984) and six meta-analyses of particular kinds of
interventions. We could find no meta-analyses, however, which attempted to
identify the features of a study skills intervention that are likely to lead to its
success.
The Nature of the Intervention
The direct teaching of detached study skills has a long history; yet, as relatively
late as 1968, Haslam and Brown reported that "published research on the produc
tivity of study skills instruction for high school students appears to be almost
nonexistent" (p. 223). In any event, this early work did not consider theory-driven
questions like the following: Why should some interventions appear to work while
100
Pintrich & de Groot, 1990) and as a continuing support to the complex of learning-
related beliefs and procedures (McCombs, 1984). Third, the teaching context
should evoke, support, and maintain the components being targeted by interven
tion (Biggs, 1993; Derry & Murphy, 1986; Garner, 1990).
Collectively, then, there are several suggested conditions for successful strategy
training: (a) high and appropriate motivation, including self-efficacy and appro
priate attributions (such as attributing failures to a lack of effort, and setting
realistic and attainable goals); (b) the strategic and contextual knowledge for
doing the task; and (c) a teaching-learning context that supports and reinforces the
strategies being taught.
At the present time, however, attempts at modeling intervention programs for
enhanced learning lack broadly based supportive data. Not to put too fine a point
on it, theory may have leapt ahead of the evidence. But even within the consensus
referred to here, the relative effectiveness of a variety of programs and thrusts
needs evaluating for both theoretical and practical reasons.
Classifying the Interventions
A typical way of classifying interventions is on the basis of their supporting
theories, but too often such theories are either ambiguous or not mutually exclu
sive. For example, as already noted, there are many variations on the metacognitive
theme, and most theories of intervention now refer to a metacognitive basis. These
variations involve self-regulation in some form or another, although some inter
ventions in the recent literature are eclectic or atheoretical.
An examination of the thrust or purpose of an intervention is a fruitful way to
identify what parameters that particular intervention aims to change: performance,
attributions, self-concept, motivation, attitudes, study skills, and so on. These are
examined in the present study. Of course, like supporting theories, they are not
mutually exclusive, as many interventions are aimed at changing several depen
dent variables simultaneously.
It would be desirable to classify interventions in mutually exclusive terms that
relate to the nature of each such intervention. In other words, we would like to
classify interventions in terms of their independent variables rather than in terms
of their effects on dependent variables. Such a classification might refer to the
structural complexity of interventions and whether they are intended to achieve
near or far transfer. The so-called SOLO taxonomy (Biggs & Collis, 1982),
described in the following section, has been used to order the structure of re
sponses. Because this taxonomy is based on structural complexity, it may readily
be adapted to suit the present case.
The SOLO Taxonomy: A Hierarchical Model of Learning Outcomes
Biggs and Collis (1982) started from a study of learning outcomes in (mainly)
high school content domains and found that students learn quite diverse material
in stages of ascending structural complexity that display a similar sequence across
tasks. This led to the formulation of the SOLO taxonomy, where "SOLO" is an
acronym for "structure of the observed learning outcome." This taxonomy makes
it possible, in the course of a student's learning a subject, to identify in broad terms
the stage at which the student is currently operating.
The following stages occur.
103
• The student engages in preliminary preparation, but the task itself is not
attacked in an appropriate way (prestructuraΐ).
• One {unistructural) and then several {multistructural) aspects of the task are
picked up serially, but are not interrelated.
• Several aspects are integrated into a coherent whole {relational).
• That coherent whole is generalized to a higher level of abstraction {extended
abstract).
The SOLO model is readily generalizable, and we use it here to provide a
convenient and exclusive system for classifying interventions intended to enhance
learning, as is explained and illustrated below.
The present classification begins with the unistructural and not the prestructural
stage, as the latter by definition refers to an intervention already expected to be
unsatisfactory. An example might be an intervention based on an unacceptable
and undeveloped theory base, such as learning in the presence of "good luck"
tokens (which may be an interesting question but is not one in which we are
interested here).
(1) Unistructural. A unistructural intervention is based on one relevant feature
or dimension. An example might be an intervention focused on a single point of
change, such as coaching on one algorithm, training in underlining, using a
mnemonic, or anxiety reduction. The target parameter may be an individual
characteristic or a skill or technique. The essential feature is that it alone is the
focus, independently of the context or its adaptation to or modification by content.
A typical example of a unistructural intervention is that reported by Scruggs
and Mastropieri (1986a), in which a trained experimenter taught students to use a
mnemonic strategy for learning information that is not immediately meaningful
and which has an abstract, numerical component. In this instance, the material to
be learned was the hardness index, from 1 to 10, for each of eight minerals. The
content to be learned, however, could just as easily have been drawn from any
subject area. There were three experimental conditions involving use of mediating
keywords based on imagining pictures linking the numbers indicating hardness
(e.g., "one is a bun") with codings of the minerals (e.g., "actor" for the mineral
actinolite) in high-, medium-, and low-structure conditions (ranging from supplied
to self-generated keywords and pictures), and a control condition. Here the
experimental conditions were procedurally simple and direct, involving essen
tially one technique (mnemonic) aimed at accurate recall.
(2) Multistructural. A multistructural intervention involves a range of indepen
dent strategies or procedures, but without any integration or orchestration as to
individual differences or demands of content or context. Examples include typical
study skills packages taught directively, without a metacognitive or conditional
framework. An example is provided by Haslam and Brown (1968), who taught the
Brown-Holzman Effective Study Skills Course: High School Level to high school
sophomores in twenty 55-minute class periods. The course involved better time
utilization, reading and writing techniques, techniques for preparing for and
taking examinations, realistic goal setting, student-to-student tips, and the like. In
short, it was a typical study skills course. A basic assumption is that all the "study
habits" are detachable, teachable, and usable across the board in many school
subjects, resulting in greater increases in grade point average than would be found
in a control group. Instrumentation included manuals and workbooks developed
104
by Brown and Holzman over several years prior to the study in question. It is
considered multistructural because it comprises a range of skills taught directively.
(3) Relational. All the components in a relational intervention are integrated to
suit the individual's self-assessment, are orchestrated to the demands of the
particular task and context, and are self-regulated with discretion. Metacognitive
interventions, emphasizing self-monitoring and self-regulation, would fit into this
category, as would many attribution retraining studies. For example, in Relich,
Debus, and Walker's (1986) study, a group of sixth graders identified as "learned
helpless" and deficient in arithmetic skills were given attribution retraining fol
lowed by manipulated success rates in division exercises, so that the beliefs about
success and failure set up by the retraining were directly reinforced by the
manipulated performances.
(4) Extended abstract. In an extended abstract intervention, the integration
achieved in the previous category is generalized to a new domain. Interventions
with this thrust would be those aiming for far transfer. In theory, Feuerstein's
(1969) Instrumental Enrichment program is an example and was the only one we
could find in this category.
Instrumental Enrichment was initially developed to cater to the learning needs
of culturally and economically deprived adolescents who were failing at school.
Its emphasis is on active student participation, with much independent work and
discussion, concentrating on basic cognitive processes, problem solving tactics,
and motivational factors. Curriculum content is deliberately excluded; instead,
there is an emphasis on teaching thinking about thinking, learning about learning,
and cognitive and metacognitive processes. There is a battery of curriculum
material with titles such as "organization of dots," "analytic perception," "orien
tation in space," "family relations," "comparisons," "classification," "numerical
progressions," "stencil design," "temporal relations," "transitive relations," and
"syllogisms." These exercises are aimed at nurturing learning sets and systematic
data-gathering behavior, developing skills in comparative analysis to improve
relational insights, and removing attitudinal inhibitions that often operate in low-
achieving adolescents. It is claimed that none of the Instrumental Enrichment
tasks are designed to "teach to the test."
The Feuerstein packages are classified as extended abstract on the grounds that
the intervention aims to produce structural changes in an individual's cognitive
functioning to the point where autonomous or independent learning can occur.
The Instrumental Enrichment exercises are designed to develop specific cognitive
and metacognitive skills necessary not only for success in tests of general ability
but also in everyday classroom tasks that require the student to apply abstract
principles such as those relating to perception, reasoning, planning, communica
tion, efficiency, elaboration, organization, and relationships.
The interaction of transfer and the SOLO taxonomy. A program may aim to
enhance performances that are either closely related or distantly related to the
training tasks. The former kind of transfer is called near, and the latter kind of
transfer is called/ør. Whether a program aims at near or far transfer is independent
of its structure in SOLO terms, although the question of near and far transfer
interacts with this taxonomic system, Unistructural models may, in theory, aim at
near or far transfer, but direct training in a single skill is generally in the context
of near transfer. Multistructural and relational models can readily be applied to
105
situations testing near and far transfer. Multistructural models are frequently
constructed on the assumption that providing students with a wide range of study
procedures would enable them to operate effectively in a wide range of situations,
in the typical study skills training format. Relational models are most frequently
focused on the context in which they are used, but if the individual acquires
strategies and the conditional knowledge of when and where they might work,
some degree of far transfer might be expected. Extended abstract models, in being
involved with learning how to learn, for example, are essentially concerned with
far transfer.
Method
Sample of Studies
We first searched various computer-based information sources using the key
words study skills, learning strategies, learning processes, cognitive style, study
habits, cognitive strategies, cognitive processes, learning style, metacognitive
skills, and thinking skills. These keywords were searched for in Psychological
Abstracts (1983 to 1992) and the database of the Educational Resources Informa
tion Center (ERIC) (1983 to 1992). After locating various articles, we searched the
references cited in them for further studies. Criteria for including a study in the
sample were that (a) it was concerned with learning or study s k i l l s . ( b ) it was
possible to calculate an effect s i z e . ( c ) there was some type of intervention, and (d)
the outcome was either performance, study skills, or affect. This yielded the
present sample of 51 studies (denoted by asterisks in the reference list). There
were some studies with more than one sample, and most had multiple indicators
of the variables of interest. As a consequence, there were 270 effect sizes that
could be coded. Table 1 presents the summary of information from each study.
Variables Coded From Each Study
The following general information was coded from each study: publication
year, publication form (journal article, book chapter, or thesis), and sample size.
A number of characteristics of each program were also coded. The thrust of the
intervention referred to the major intention of the program. There were six levels
of thrust: attribution, to change the attributions students made for success and/or
failure; motivation, to change the student's motivation for learning; study skills, to
diminish use of ineffective study behaviors and train students to use one or a
package of targeted skills; structural aids, which help the learner interact with
content to define structural and high-level meaning (these include concept map
ping, certain kinds of note taking and summarizing, and organizers); Feuerstein
programs, comprising more general ways in which the student can adopt task-
appropriate strategies, such as using analogy and relating ideas, elaborative pro
cessing, and a "meaning orientation"; and memory, where interventions were
aimed at improving accuracy of recall for quite specific factual material.
The nature of each intervention was also classified according to the type of
outcome for which it aimed, as either reproductive or transformational A training
program that aimed to develop study skills that are used mainly for the reproduc
tion of content (e.g., memory programs) was classified as reproductive. A pro
gram that aimed to help students deal with content at a high cognitive level—that
106
Amato, Bernard, 1989 University High Teacher Structural Content Performance Reproductive Relational Far 131 2 -0.57
DΆmico, & aids
DeBellefeuille
Andre & 1979 Upper sec Medium Self Structural Content Performance Reproductive Multistructural Near 71 1 0.61
Anderson aids
Armbruster, 1987 Primary Mixed Teacher Structural Content Performance Transfor Relational Far 82 1 1.75
Anderson, aids mational
& Ostertag
Atkinson & 1975 University High Self Memori Other Performance Reproductive Unistructural Near 52 1 1.11
Raugh zation
Barnes, Ginther, 1989 Lower sec Mixed Teacher Structural Content Performance Reproductive Relational Near 83 2 0.07
&Cochran aids
Bean, Singer, 1986 Upper sec High Teacher Structural Content Attitude/ Reproductive Multistructural Far 72 4 0.60
Sorter, & Frazee aids performance
Billingsley & 1988 Upper sec Und-ach Teacher Structural Content Performance Reproductive Relational Near 54 4 0.85
Wildman aids
Bretzing, 1987 Lower sec Mixed Teacher Structural Content Performance Reproductive Multistructural Near 42 1 1.68
Kulhavy, & aids
Caterino
Brown & 1976 Primary Low Teacher Memori Other Performance Reproductive Unistructural Near 66 1 0.33
Barclay zation
Brown, 1979 Primary Low Teacher Memori Other Performance Reproductive Unistructural Near 58 2 0.35
Campione, & zation
Barclay
Casey 1990 Primary Mixed Self Study skills Individual Performance Transfor Relational Far 68 2 0.79
mational
Danner & Taylor 1973 Primary Medium Teacher Memori Other Performance Reproductive Unistructural Near 120 2 0.90
zation
Dansereau et al. 1979 University High Teacher Study skills Individual Attitude/ Reproductive Relational Far 87 8 0.62
study skills/
performance
Dendato & 1986 University High Teacher Study skills Individual Attitude/ Transfor Relational/ Near/far 43 4 0.96
Diener performance mational multistructural
Dwyer 1986 University High Teacher Structural Content Near 136 6 0.38
aids Performance Reproductive Unistructural
Englert, Raphael, 1991 Primary Mixed Teacher Structural Content Near 174 1 0.88
Anderson, aids Study skills Transformational Relational
Anthony, &
Stevens
Feuerstein, Miller, 1981 Adult Low Teacher Feuerstein Other Performance Transformational Ext abstract Far 184 1 0.79
Hoffman, Rand,
Mintzker, & Jensen
Feuerstein, Rand, 1979 Lower sec Low Teacher Feuerstein Other Performance Transformational Ext abstract Far 114 3 0.51
Hoffman, Hoffman,
& Miller
Gadzella, 1977 University High Teacher Study skills Individual All Other Relational Far 160 3 0.13
Goldston,
& Zimmerman
Greiner & Karoly 1976 University High Teacher Study skills Content Performance/ Transformational Multistructural Far 96 12 0.33
study
Haslam & Brown 1968 Upper sec Mixed Teacher Study skills Individual All Other Multistructural Far 118 4 1.26
Ĵudd et al. 1979 Adult High Self Study skills Individual Attitude/ Other Multistructural Near 160 12 0.23
performance
Kiewra & Benton 1987 University High Self Structural Content Performance Reproductive Unistructural Near 43 0.36
aids
Klein & Freitag 1992 University Medium Self Motivation Individual Study skills Transformational Multistructural Near 64 2 0.97
Kratzing 1992 University High Study skills Individual Study skills Other Multistructural Near 140 23 -0.01
Lange], Guttentag, 1990 Primary Mixed Teacher Memori Other Performance Reproductive Unistructural Near 48 0.95
&Nida zation
Lodico, Ghatala, 1983 Primary Mixed Teacher Structural Content Performance Reproductive Relational Near 72 2 0.47
Levin, Pressley, aids
ÄBell
Martin 1984 Upper sec Mixed Teacher Feuerstein Other Performance Transformational Ext abstract Far 28 2 1.09
McBride & Dwyer 1985 University High Self Structural Content Performance Reproductive Multistructural Near 112 0.25
aids
McKeachie, 1985 University High Teacher Study skills Individual Performance Transformational Relational Far 419 4 0.29
Pintrich, & Lin
Morgan 1985 University High Teacher Study skills Individual Performance Other Unistructural Near 226 2 0.17
Narrol, Silverman, 1982 Upper sec Low Teacher Feuerstein Other All Transformational Ext abstract Far 102 25 0.45
& Waksman
Nist & Simpson 1989 University Medium Self Study skills Individual Performance Transformational Relational Far 73 1 0.23
Nist, Mealey, 1990 University Under ach Teacher Study skills Individual All Other Relational Far 239 21 0.35
Simpson, & Kroc
Okebukola 1988 University High Teacher Structural Content Performance Reproductive Relational Near 145 1 0.07
& Jegede aids
Purdie 1989 Upper sec Mixed Teacher Motivation Individual Study skills Other Relational Far 132 18 0.02
Rand, Mintzker, 1981 Adult Low Teacher Feuerstein Other All Transformational Ext abstract Far 203 1 -0.48
Miller, Hoffman,
& Friedlender
Rand, 1979 Lower sec Low Teacher Feuerstein Other All Transformational Ext abstract Far 114 25 0.18
Tannenbaum,
& Feuerstein
Relich, Debus, 1986 Primary Mixed Self Attribution Individual All Other Relational Near 14 8 1.42
& Walker
Schunk & Cox 1986 Lower sec Medium Teacher Structural Content Attitude/ Reproductive Unistructural Near 90 2 0.57
aids performance
Schunk & Gunn 1986 Primary Mixed Teacher Structural Content Reproductive Multistructural Near 50 2 2.14
aids Performance
Scruggs & 1986 Primary Medium/ Teacher Memori Other Reproductive Unistructural Near 96 6 1.64
Mastropieri high zation Memory
Scruggs & 1986 Primary Teacher Study skills Individual Reproductive Multistructural Far 76 2 0.22
Mastropieri Low Performance
Scruggs, 1985 Primary Teacher Memori Other Reproductive Unistructural Near 36 4 0.76
Mastropieri, Levin, Under ach zation Performance
McLoone, Gaffney,
& Prater
Shayer & Beasley 1987 Lower sec Low Teacher Feuerstein Other Performance Transformational Ext abstract Far 12 7 1.03
Simbo 1988 Upper sec Mixed Teacher Structural Content Performance Reproductive Unistructural Near 180 1.11
aids 1
Swing & Peterson 1988 Primary Mixed Teacher Structural Content Performance Reproductive Relational Near 121 0.12
aids 8
VanOverwalle 1990 University High Teacher Study skills/ Individual Performance Transformational/ Multistructural Near/far 240 0.02
& De Metsenaere attribution reproductive 10
Weinstein et al. 1979 University High Teacher Memori Other Performance Unistructural Near 100 1.88
zation Reproductive
Weisberg 1990 Lower sec Under ach Teacher Structural Content Performance Relational Near 50 10 0.90
& Balajthy aids Reproductive
Wilson 1986 Lower sec Medium Teacher Study skills Individual Performance Multistructural Far 47 2 0.47
Transformational
Note. Pub = year of publication; N = sample size.
is, to take the content and in some way transform it for a variety of purposes and
in different contexts—was classified as transformational.
The nature of the intervention was categorized according to the SOLO tax
onomy described above. Interventions were classified as unistructural,
multistructural, relational, or extended abstract and as near ox far in terms of the
degree of transfer between training task and outcome measure.
The outcome measure used to assess the effectiveness of an intervention was
also classified. Academic performance measures such as subject-based tests and
examinations, grade point averages, and tests of general ability were categorized
as performance. Where the outcome measured change in either one or a range of
study behaviors, the category study skills was assigned. Affect was used when the
outcome measure was related to self-efficacy, self-concept, or attitude.
The tests used were coded, and their quality was approximated by coding those
that were published and normed as high, those for which there was some evidence
of psychometric investigation of the instrument as medium, and those created for
the study with no attempts at psychometric rigor as low.
A number of characteristics of the research design were coded. The studies
were graded according to quality (coded independently and agreed to by all three
authors and classified as low, medium, and high; those that were of patently low
quality were not included in the analysis), subject selection (voluntary, intact class
groups), theoretical orientation of study (theory based, atheoretical), purpose of
study (specifically related to study or learning skills, or study skills was second
ary), design of study (control-experimental, pretest-posttest), and direction of
program (self-directed or teacher-directed).
Characteristics of the participants in each study were coded according to several
categories—for example, age (primary-elementary, junior secondary, secondary,
college-university, adults), ability level (low, medium, high, mixed, underachiev
ing), and socioeconomic status (low, middle, upper, mixed).
Computation and Analysis of Effect Sizes
The effect size calculated is g, the difference between the means of the interven
tion group and the control group, or the difference between the pretest and posttest
group means, divided by the pooled standard deviation. The sign of the difference
was positive when a treatment had a positive effect (thus, those that reduced
learning pathologies such as anxiety, surface approaches, and negative attitudes
were coded as positive effects). The gs were converted to ds by correcting them
for bias (as the gs overestimate the population effect size, particularly in small
samples; see Hedges & Olkin, 1985). To determine whether each set of ds shared
a common effect size (i.e., was consistent across the studies), we calculated a
homogeneity statistic Qw, which has an approximate chi-square distribution with
k - 1 degrees of freedom, where k is the number of effect sizes (Hedges & Olkin,
1985). Given the large number of effect sizes that are combined into the various
categories, and the sensitivity of the chi-square statistic to this number, it is not
surprising that nearly all homogeneity statistics are significant. As the most
critical comparisons are presented in interaction tables between at least two
variables, we are more confident that these means are sufficiently homogeneous
to use the means as reasonable estimates of the typical value.
We then used categorical models to determine the relation between the study
111
TABLE 5 (continued)
Weighted Standard
Variable and class effect error ßw
Age of subjects 126.24
Primary 40 0.91 .061 1,014.13
Lower secondary 54 0.51 .061 316.49
Upper secondary 59 0.45 .101 729.61
University 103 0.28 .027 936.50
Adults 14 0.22 .024 124.02
Purpose of article 38.13
Specifically study 133 0.38 .025 1,999.70
Study secondary 137 0.53 .084 1,209.16
Selection of subjects 130.09
Self-selected 18 0.63 .030 150.83
Intact classes 122 0.33 .032 1,249.28
Other 130 0.54 .081 1,716.79
Theoretical orientation 110.31
Theoretical 234 0.45 .060 2,660.63
Atheoretical 36 0.47 .023 476.05
to improve the recall of the factual material that was actually used in the training
stage of the intervention. For this meta-analysis, we were not able to locate usable
(i.e., those with sufficient detail of intervention procedures or data) studies that
investigated whether students were able to transfer such memory strategies to
situations where they were required to learn material unrelated to that used in the
initial intervention. It seems that it is easier to use unistructural programs to
improve memory outcomes where students are not required to use more demand
ing cognitive procedures such as the unprompted generation of strategy use in
different learning contexts and with unfamiliar content.
A study by Atkinson and Raugh (1975) provides a typical illustration of a
program classified as unistructural near. In this study, a group of Stanford Univer
sity students were taught to use the mnemonic keyword method for learning
Russian vocabulary. This method divides the study of a vocabulary item into two
TABLE 6
Structural complexity of interventions by outcome measure
Performance Study skills Affect Total
Nature of Testing
the program conditions n M n M n M n M
Unistructural Near 29 0.84 1 0.56 30 0.83
Multistructural Near 16 0.45 24 0.03 8 0.53 48 0.25
Multistructural Far 21 0.25 3 1.13 3 0.81 27 0.41
Relational Near 29 0.62 1 0.88 7 1.40 37 0.77
Relational Far 22 0.33 34 0.22 8 0.49 64 0.29
Extended abstract 40 0.69 7 -0.16 17 0.02 64 0.42
Total/mean 157 0.57 69 0.16 44 0.48 270 0.45
119
stages, one requiring a sound association between the spoken foreign word and an
English keyword and the other requiring the student to construct a mental image
of a physical interaction between the keyword and the English translation. It is not
surprising that an effect size of 1.13 was obtained for the experimental group.
Where students are taught a single and very specific strategy to help them
remember a list of words and are then tested on that same list, a reasonable degree
of success is to be expected. Although there was a follow-up test some weeks later
on which experimental students outperformed control students, again the test list
contained the same words that students had learned previously. To demonstrate
far transfer of the mnemonic strategy, a different list of words would have had to
be used. Similar results were found in two previous meta-analyses of vocabulary
instruction (Klesius & Searls, 1990; Stahl & Fairbanks, 1986), in which the
mnemonic keyword method was found to produce positive effect sizes on imme
diate posttests. Klesius and Searls, however, also found dramatic declines in
performance on delayed posttests.
Multistructural interventions are moderately successful in producing near trans
fer on performance (M = 0.45) and positive attitudes to study (M = 0.53), but they
have no effect on reported use of study skills (M = 0.03). When far transfer is
tested, effects on performance drop to 0.25 but are massive on study skills (M =
1.13) and strong on affect (M = .81). However, it must be noted that these means
for affect and study skills are each based on only three effect sizes and that all
three effect sizes come from one study, Haslam and Brown (1968). They used the
Brown-Holzman Effective Study Skills Course and assessed the effects of this
course using the Brown-Holzman Survey of Study Habits and Attitudes (Brown &
Holzman, 1967). Although the intent was far transfer, what one is seeing here may
be something close to "teaching to the test"; that is, the program addressed content
that was keyed to the self-report items in the survey itself. It must also be said,
however, that this study, along with others in the same category, did achieve small
gains in grade point average.
A demonstration of a successful application of a multistructural intervention
with near transfer is that of Schunk and Gunn (1986). One objective of this study
was to train children to use task strategies to solve division problems, with a view
to improving both performance and self-efficacy. These task strategies were
specific to the solving of problems with one to three digits in the divisor and two
to five digits in the dividend. A comparison of pretest and posttest scores on tests
that required students to solve the same sorts of division problems that were used
during the training sessions indicated a substantial improvement. Schunk and
Gunn also used path analysis to explore the theoretical relationships between task
strategies, attributions (ability, effort, task ease, and luck), self-efficacy, and
performance. The largest direct influence on changes in division skills was due to
the use of effective task strategies.
Relational near programs were systematically effective over all outcomes. In
relational near programs, a range of metacognitive interventions was aimed at
teaching students to change their attributional perspectives (Relich, Debus, &
Walker, 1986) and to use such strategies as self-questioning (Billingsley &
Wildman, 1988), identifying main ideas (Weisberg & Balajthy, 1990), concept
mapping (Okebukola & Jegede, 1988), and/or strategy monitoring (Lodico, Ghatala,
Levin, Pressley, & Bell, 1983). The degree of success on these and similar
120
type are those focused on near transfer. The lowest effects are for multistructural
programs, both near and far. This finding is contrary to that of Kirschenbaum and
Perri (1982), who found that multiple-component (i.e., multistructural) interven
tions were generally more successful than single-component interventions.
Overall, the effects of these interventions are greatest on performance, some
what lower on affect, and comparatively much lower on study skills. Across all
three types of outcome measure, if only the testing conditions are considered, the
mean for the near conditions is 0.57 (n = 115), which is greater than the mean of
0.33 (n = 91) for the far conditions.
Except for the extended abstract programs, interventions for enhanced learning
also had very positive effects on affect. It is useful to examine more closely the
exact nature of this affective outcome as it was measured in the interventions
themselves. In general, the positive outcome was derived from measures of
attitude change. For example, students reported greater liking for teachers and
increased agreement with the goals of education (Gadzella, Goldston, &
Zimmerman, 1977) or more positive attitudes towards study and specific subjects
(Bean, Singer, Sorter, & Frazee, 1986; Dansereau et al., 1979; Haslam & Brown,
1968; Nist, Mealey, Simpson, & Kroc, 1990). A more positive attitude also was
reflected in reduced anxiety (Dansereau et al., 1979; Nist et al., 1990) and
increased task persistence (Relich et al., 1986).
The effects on study skills methods appear to be very small for programs aiming
at near and far transfer (where there is sufficient sample size). It is very difficult
to change the study skills that students have acquired, usually over many years of
study, and as will be later shown, older students are more resistant to change.The
improvement of student learning via the manipulation of study skills often fails to
take account of the interaction between students' intentions and the context of
learning. A learning skills intervention with first-year university students not only
failed to achieve increases in deep approaches to learning but also led to an
increase in surface approaches—the opposite of what was intended (Ramsden,
Beswick, & Bowden, 1986). It was suggested that the reason for this failure was
linked to the incorrect assumption that the observed behaviors of effective stu
dents can be taught to less successful students independently of the teaching and
assessment context. Indeed, the less successful students perceived assessment
122
tasks as requiring rote learning, and as long as their perception existed, incorrect
though it may have been, students would ignore deep approaches and use self-
management skills to rote learn more effectively. Even when students do learn to
be more flexible in their use of learning strategies, rigid teaching contexts often
prevent the use of some strategies.
Reproductive versus transformational outcomes. If the various outcomes are
classified either as requiring students to simply reproduce the content of material
learned or as requiring them to express knowledge in different or creative ways,
then, as expected, the effect sizes are (a) greatest for the reproductive outcomes
when the intentions of study skills programs are towards the lower end of the
SOLO taxonomy but (b) reasonably consistent across most levels (Table 6). The
transformational effects are greater when the study skills programs are towards the
upper end of the SOLO taxonomy. When these were further subdivided into
different outcomes, then the effects on the performance outcomes were consis
tently high. The effects on the study skills and affect outcomes are high only on
the reproductive programs (M = 0.71, n = 7) and much lower on the transforma
tional programs (M = 0.20, n = 32).
Thrust of the program. Table 8 examines the effects of interventions with
different thrusts: attribution, motivation, study skills, structural aids, Feuerstein,
and memory. An intervention for enhanced learning is more likely to have a large
effect when its thrust is attribution, memory, or structural aids. The lowest effects
are associated with motivation and study skills.
When the thrust of the program is to change memory or attribution, the effects
on all outcomes are most positive (Table 8). The effects of motivation programs
are the lowest, although this result was derived from the findings of one study
(Purdie, 1989) in which an achievement motivation training program was found
to have mixed effects on the participants' approaches to studying. Although there
were positive effects of the program on students' study motives (a reduction in
surface motive and an increase in deep motive), these effects were not matched by
changes in surface and deep strategies.
There were only 6 effect sizes where attribution retraining was involved, but
overall effects were strong on performance (M = 0.60) and especially, as would be
expected, on affect (M = 1.32). Motivation training involved 20 effect sizes, all to
do with study skills, but was relatively ineffective (M = 0.11). Study skills
TABLE 8
Outcome measures by the thrust of the program
Performance Study skills Affect Total
n M n M n M n M
Attribution 6 0.60 6 1.32 12 0.96
Motivation 20 0.11 20 0.11
Study skills 47 0.26 41 0.23 18 0.63 106 0.31
Structural aids 46 0.58 1 0.88 3 0.53 50 0.58
Feuerstein 40 0.69 7 -0.16 17 0.02 64 0.42
Memory 18 1.09 18 1.09
Total/mean 157 0.57 69 0.16 44 0.48 270 0.45
123
training, oddly enough, had much stronger effects on affect (M = 0.63) than on
reported use of study skills (M = 0.23). This would suggest that study skills
training is more valuable as an anxiety reducer than as an enhancer of learning.
Programs using study skills as the major thrust were more effective with reproduc
tive intentions (M = 0.54, n = 10) than with transformational intentions (M = 0.37,
n 3l).
Structural aids—such as advance organizers, summarizing (Armbruster, Ander
son, & Ostertag, 1987); rehearsal (Dwyer, 1986), the selection and use of effective
task strategies (Schunk & Gunn, 1986), the construction of graphic organizers,
summary writing (Weisberg & Balajthy, 1990) and writing strategies like plan
ning, organizing, writing, editing, and revising (Englert, Raphael, Anderson,
Anthony & Stevens, 1991)—appear to be uniformly effective, with an effect size
on performance of 0.58. Memory, when separated from other unistructural inter
ventions, is even more powerful in its effect on performance, with an effect size
of 1.09.
When classifying by both thrust and SOLO categories, study skills programs
are more effective at the more complex end of the SOLO taxonomy (Table 9);
training in the use of structural aids is effective over most levels of the taxonomy
and for both near and far testing conditions, with the exception of three effect sizes
for relational far. Not surprisingly, the memory programs tended to be more
unistructural, which highlights the value of such programs in enhancing immedi
ate and lower-order cognitive processes.
When the thrust of a program is to modify students' causal attributions for
academic achievement, effects are greatest in relational near contexts. This result
is based on eight effect sizes, all from one study (Relich et al., 1986). The other
four effect sizes were smaller and came from another study (Van Overwalle & De
Metsenaere, 1990) classified as multistructural. Despite the small number of
effect sizes, a comparison of the two programs provides some insight into reasons
for the greater effectiveness of the intervention reported by Relich et al. Students
who had previously been identified as learned helpless and deficient in particular
mathematics skills received attributional training in the context of mathematics
instruction. They were provided with opportunity over eight training sessions to
practice making attributions for success and failure on sets of mathematics prob
lems, and they were given attribution feedback by the instructor. On the other
hand, in the intervention reported by Van Overwalle and De Metsenaere, the
attributional manipulation lasted only 50 minutes and consisted of a video presen
tation showing real-life experiences of several students who discussed reasons for
their successes and failures in university examinations. Experimental students in
this study were provided neither with feedback about their attributions nor with
specific opportunity to practice new patterns of attribution. The effects were
trivially different from zero.
Attributes of the Students
Ability. The effect sizes are greatest for those in the middle of the academic
distribution and those classified as underachieving (see Table 10). It is probable
that these are the cohort most likely to seek study skills assistance, and they are
most likely to benefit, given their abilities to learn both from the content matter
taught and from the study skills applied. It seems that low-ability students are
124
TABLE 10
Structural complexity of the intervention by academic ability
Under-
Low Medium Upper achieving Mixed
Nature of the program
and testing conditions n M n M n M n M n M
Unistructural—near 3 0.34 7 0.95 14 0.86 2 1.03 4 0.76
Multistructural—near 3 0.85 42 0.08 3 1.99
Multistructural—far 2 0.22 2 0.47 19 0.25 4 1.24
Relational—near 2 0.96 21 0.68 14 0.88
Relational—far 1 0.23 32 0.42 21 0.12 10 0.24
Extended abstract—far 62 0.40 2 1.09
Total/mean 67 0.39 13 0.80 109 0.33 53 0.61 28 0.64
unable to benefit from interventions of most kinds, with the Feuerstein programs
being the exception.
The medium-ability students benefit most from unistructural and multistructural
programs, whereas the underachieving students benefit from all programs (Table
10). It is the underachievers and higher-ability students who benefit more from the
programs at the relational level, whereas it is the medium-ability and underachiev
ing students who benefit more at the multistructural levels. The higher-ability
students benefit from memory (M = 1.82, n = 5) and affect (M = 0.63, n = 17) but
otherwise not at all.
Age. Interventions may be effective across all age groups, but it is the youngest
students who accrue the greatest benefits across all outcomes. University students
and adults show much lower effects on their performance outcomes but stronger
effects on affect (Table 11). Study skills training is more effective with young
students and becomes relatively ineffective at the upper secondary and tertiary
levels, which adds further support to the point already made that students' actual
study behaviors are developed and maintained to cope with a little-changing
teaching context.
Although most programs in which the thrust is study skills use university
students, the effects on study skills are minimal. Most affective outcomes for
university students related either to improved attitudes towards learning or to the
TABLE 11
Outcome measure moderated by age of students
Performance Study skills Affect
Age of students n M n M n M
Preprimary 2 0.79
Primary 31 0.84 1 0.88 6 1.32
Lower secondary 44 0.58 2 0.44 8 0.09
Upper secondary 231 1.05 25 0.07 13 0.21
University 51 0.27 41 0.19 11 0.68
Adults 6 0.06 6 0.43
126
TABLE 12
Outcome measure moderated by purpose of the study
Performance Study skills Affect
Purpose n M n M n M
Specifically study 56 0.52 57 0.15 20 0.62
Secondarily study 101 0.61 69 0.17 44 0.48
performance, 0.16 for study skills, and 0.48 for affect. The greatest effect is thus
on performance, which yields a figure as high as that for typical innovations in
education; ironically, the smallest effect relates to the reported use of study skills
on the various outcomes. The performance figure itself breaks down to 1.09 for
memory only, 0.69 on reproductive performance in general (i.e., a low cognitive
performance level), and 0.53 for transformational performance, which is a high
level of cognitive processing. When we look at the nature of the enhanced learning
intervention programs that produce these effects, and with whom these interven
tions are most successful, a clear and comprehensible picture emerges.
Unistructural programs, involving direct teaching of mostly mnemonic devices,
are highly effective with virtually all students. Effect sizes greater than 1.0 are
typically found for reproductive performances. Much the same is the case for
multistructural programs, such as conventional study skills training, being used
for near transfer on low-cognitive-level tasks. When multistructural programs are
used for high cognitive levels or for far transfer, they are not effective, except in
one study (Haslam & Brown, 1968), which found that reported use of study skills
and effects were enhanced. Multistructural approaches were most effective, when
they worked at all, with younger rather than older students.
When relational programs, which integrate the informed use of strategies to suit
particular content, were used for near transfer, they were highly effective in all
domains (performance, study skills, and affect) over all ages and ability levels, but
were particularly useful with high-ability and older students. This is very reason
able in view of the high cognitive demands metacognitive interventions, based on
conditional knowledge, are likely to make.
Given that unistructural methods were highly effective with everybody, for
their own limited purposes, groups of different ability levels otherwise showed
different degrees of receptiveness to intervention. Mixed-ability students, under-
achievers, and younger (primary and early secondary) students were relatively
receptive to interventions, particularly unistructural and multistructural. Low-
ability children were least amenable to intervention, possibly because they were
least able to comprehend instructions, but more likely because the programs
tended to be too demanding on subjects. At all events, there seems to be a problem
in achieving change in the low-ability group that needs addressing in future
research.
What might we then conclude from this meta-analysis? Despite, perhaps, the
conventional wisdom, most intervention does work most of the time. After all, the
effect size over all studies was 0.45; and a very respectable 0.57 for performance.
Even when we allow for the very clear success of mnemonic-type programs, this
128
is not our present concern, which is with interventions over and above the context
of teaching itself.
When the aim is to change students' attributions for success and failure,
teachers should emphasize the importance of systematically using strategies ap
propriate to the task in hand. The way in which teachers give feedback to students
about their use of strategies will probably influence their attributions for success
or failure more than will feedback regarding either ability or effort. Two studies
in this meta-analysis demonstrated the desirability of providing feedback that
explicitly links improved performance with strategy use (Schunk & Cox, 1986;
Schunk & Gunn, 1986). Schunk and Cox suggest that the teacher who tells a
student, "That's good, you're really working hard" (effort attributional feedback),
may not be as effective as the one who links success with appropriate strategy use,
as in "That's correct. You got it right because you applied the steps in the right
order."
In general, then, the thrust of these findings is quite compatible with the thrust
of situated cognition and its implications (J. S. Brown, Collins, & Duguid, 1989;
Marton, 1988) and with systems theory (Biggs, 1993). That is, improving learning
is less likely to be achieved by targeting the individual in terms of a deficit model,
which presupposes that the individual is lacking the right strategies and needs to
be taught them or is using the wrong strategies and needs to have them removed.
The results of this meta-analysis support the notion of situated cognition, whereby
it is recommended that training other than for simple mnemonic performance
should (a) be in context. ( b ) use tasks within the same domain as the target content.
Strategy training should be seen as a balanced system in which the individual's
abilities, insights, and sense of responsibility are brought into use, so that the
strategies that are appropriate to the task at hand can be used. The student will
need to know what those strategies are, of course, and also the conditional
knowledge that empowers them: the how, when, where, and why of their use. In
other words, effective strategy training becomes embedded in the teaching context
itself, a conclusion that has profound implications for future research, develop
ment, and application in strategy training.
This analysis, then, returns to the issue of the teaching context itself and points
to the central importance of the interface between interventions involving strategy
and attribution training and the teaching context. We have not been able in the
space available to address that issue adequately, but that is a matter of one thing
at a time. We wished in the present article to determine which interventions appear
to work and under what conditions different kinds of intervention work best. We
have addressed that issue, and the results are clear. To fully explore the relation
ship between the present results and teaching is quite a different exercise and, at
this stage, probably not a meta-analytic one.
References
References marked with asterisks indicate studies included in the meta-analysis.
*Amato, J. L., Bernard, R. M, D'Amico, M, & DeBellefeuille, B. (1989). Can
instructional variables be combined effectively to enhance learning achievement?
Canadian Journal of Educational Communication, 78(2), 85-109.
131
132
133
134
135
Authors
JOHN HATTIE is Professor and Chair, Department of Educational Research and
Methodology, 209 Curry, University of North Carolina, Greensboro, NC 27412-
5001; [email protected]. He specializes in measurement, self-concept, and
teaching and learning.
JOHN BIGGS is Professor, Department of Education, University of Hong Kong. He
specializes in learning and cognition, study processes, and cross-cultural learning
theories.
NOLA PURDIE is Lecturer, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane, Austra
lia. She specializes in cross-cultural learning and study skills.
136