Language Diversity
Language Diversity
Explanation
Luyện tập đề IELTS Reading Practice với passage Language Diversity được lấy từ
cuốn sách IELTS Practice Test Plus 3 - Test 6 - Passage 3 với trải nghiệm thi IELTS
trên máy và giải thích đáp án chi tiết bằng Linearthinking, kèm list từ vựng IELTS cần
học trong bài đọc.
Làm Bài
Từ vựng
Bài đọc (reading passage )
Language diversity
One of the most influential ideas in the study of languages is that of universal grammar (UG). Put forward
by Noam Chomsky in the 1960s , it is widely interpreted as meaning that all languages are basically the
same, and that the human brain is born language-ready, with an in-built programme that is able to
interpret the common rules underlying any mother tongue. For five decades this idea prevailed, and
influenced work in linguistics, psychology and cognitive science. To understand language, it implied, you
must sweep aside the huge diversity of languages, and find their common human core.
Since the theory of UG was proposed, linguists have identified many universal language rules. However,
there are almost always exceptions. It was once believed, for example, that if a language had syllables[1 ]
that begin with a vowel and end with a consonant (VC), it would also have syllables that begin with a
consonant and end with a vowel (CV). This universal lasted until 1999 , when linguists showed that
Arrernte, spoken by Indigenous Australians from the area around Alice Springs in the Northern Territory,
has VC syllables but no CV syllables.
Other non-universal universals describe the basic rules of putting words together. Take the rule that every
language contains four basic word classes: nouns, verbs, adjectives and adverbs. Work in the past two
decades has shown that several languages lack an open adverb class, which means that new adverbs
cannot be readily formed, unlike in English where you can turn any adjective into an adverb, for example
‘soft’ into ‘softly’. Others, such as Lao, spoken in Laos, have no adjectives at all. More controversially,
some linguists argue that a few languages, such as Straits Salish, spoken by indigenous people from
north-western regions of North America, do not even have distinct nouns or verbs. Instead, they have a
single class of words to include events, objects and qualities.
Even apparently indisputable universals have been found lacking. This includes recursion, or the ability to
infinitely place one grammatical unit inside a similar unit, such as ‘Jack thinks that Mary thinks that ... the
bus will be on time’. It is widely considered to be the most essential characteristic of human language, one
that sets it apart from the communications of all other animals. Yet Dan Everett at Illinois State University
recently published controversial work showing that Amazonian Piraha does not have this quality.
But what if the very diversity of languages is the key to understanding human communication? Linguists
Nicholas Evans of the Australian National University in Canberra, and Stephen Levinson of the Max Planck
Institute for Psycholinguistics in Nijmegen, the Netherlands, believe that languages do not share a
common set of rules. Instead, they say, their sheer variety is a defining feature of human communication -
something not seen in other animals. While there is no doubt that human thinking influences the form that
language takes, if Evans and Levinson are correct, language in turn shapes our brains. This suggests that
humans are more diverse than we thought, with our brains having differences depending on the language
environment in which we grew up. And that leads to a disturbing conclusion: every time a language
becomes extinct, humanity loses an important piece of diversity.
If languages do not obey a single set of shared rules, then how are they created? ‘Instead of universals.
you get standard engineering solutions that languages adopt again and again, and then you get outliers.'
says Evans. He and Levinson argue that this is because any given language is a complex system shaped
by many factors, including culture, genetics and history. There- are no absolutely universal traits of
language, they say, only tendencies. And it is a mix of strong and weak tendencies that characterizes the
‘bio-cultural’ mix that we call language.
According to the two linguists, the strong tendencies explain why many languages display common
patterns. A variety of factors tend to push language in a similar direction, such as the structure of the
brain, the biology of speech, and the efficiencies of communication. Widely shared linguistic elements
may also be ones that build on a particularly human kind of reasoning. For example, the fact that before
we learn to speak we perceive the world as a place full of things causing actions (agents) and things
having actions done to them (patients) explains why most languages deploy these grammatical
categories.
Weak tendencies, in contrast, are explained by the idiosyncrasies of different languages. Evans and
Levinson argue that many aspects of the particular natural history of a population may affect its language.
For instance, Andy Butcher at Flinders University in Adelaide, South Australia, has observed that
indigenous Australian children have by far the highest incidence of chronic middle-ear infection of any
population on the planet, and that most indigenous Australian languages lack many sounds that are
common in other languages, but which are hard to hear with a middle-ear infection. Whether this
condition has shaped the sound systems of these languages is unknown, says Evans, but it is important to
consider the idea.
Levinson and Evans are not the first to question the theory of universal grammar, but no one has
summarized these ideas quite as persuasively, and given them as much reach. As a result, their
arguments have generated widespread enthusiasm, particularly among those linguists who are tired of
trying to squeeze their findings into the straitjacket of ‘absolute universals’. To some, it is the final nail in
UG’s coffin. Michael Tomasello, co-director of the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology in
Leipzig, Germany, has been a long-standing critic of the idea that all languages conform to a set of rules.
‘Universal grammar is dead,’ he says.
[1 ] a unit of sound
Câu hỏi (questions )
Question 1 - 6
Do the following statements agree with the information given in the Reading Passage?
NOT GIVEN if it is impossible to say what the writer thinks about this
1 In the final decades of the twentieth century, a single theory of language learning was dominant.
4 The search for new universal language rules has largely ended.
5 If Evans and Levinson are right, people develop in the same way no matter what language they speak.
6 The loss of any single language might have implications for the human race.
Question 7 - 11
Choose appropriate options A, B, C or D.
7 Which of the following views about language are held by Evans and Levinson?
8 According to Evans and Levinson, apparent similarities between languages could be due to
B faulty analysis.
9 In the eighth paragraph, what does the reference to a middle-ear infection serve as?
10 What does the writer suggest about Evans’ and Levinson’s theory of language development?
Question 12 - 14
Complete each sentences with correct ending
List of Endings
It has now been suggested that Amazonia Piraha does not have 14
Answer key (đáp án và giải thích )
Hãy cùng DOL khám phá kho đề thi IELTS Reading Practice với lời giải thích
chi tiết ở đây ->