A General Introduction To Psychoanalysis
A General Introduction To Psychoanalysis
The neurotic symptoms then have their meaning just like errors and the
dream, and like these they are related to the lives of the persons in whom
they appear. The importance of this insight into the nature of the
symptom can best be brought home to you by way of examples. That it is
borne out always and in all cases, I can only assert, not prove. He who
gathers his own experience will be convinced of it. For certain reasons,
216
however, I shall draw my instances not from hysteria, but from another
fundamentally related and very curious neurosis concerning which I
wish to say a few introductory words to you. This so-called compulsion
neurosis is not so popular as the widely known hysteria; it is, if I may use
the expression, not so noisily ostentatious, behaves more as a private
concern of the patient, renounces bodily manifestations almost entirely
and creates all its symptoms psychologically. Compulsion neurosis and
hysteria are those forms of neurotic disease by the study of which
psychoanalysis has been built up, and in whose treatment as well the
therapy celebrates its triumphs. Of these the compulsion neurosis, which
does not take that mysterious leap from the psychic to the physical, has
through psychoanalytic research become more intimately
comprehensible and transparent to us than hysteria, and we have come
to understand that it reveals far more vividly certain extreme
characteristics of the neuroses.
Surely this means violent suffering. I believe that the wildest psychiatric
phantasy could not have succeeded in deriving anything comparable,
and if one did not actually see it every day, one could hardly bring
oneself to believe it. Do not think, however, that you give the patient any
help when you coax him to divert himself, to put aside these stupid ideas
and to set himself to something useful in the place of his whimsical
occupations. This is just what he would like of his own accord, for he
possesses all his senses, shares your opinion of his compulsion
symptoms, in fact volunteers it quite readily. But he cannot do
otherwise; whatever activities actually are released under compulsion
neurosis are carried along by a driving energy, such as is probably never
met with in normal psychic life. He has only one remedy—to transfer and
change. In place of one stupid idea he can think of a somewhat milder
absurdity, he can proceed from one precaution and prohibition to
another, or carry through another ceremonial. He may shift, but he
cannot annul the compulsion. One of the chief characteristics of the
sickness is the instability of the symptoms; they can be shifted very far
from their original form. It is moreover striking that the contrasts
present in all psychological experience are so very sharply drawn in this
condition. In addition to the compulsion of positive and negative
content, an intellectual doubt makes itself felt that gradually attacks the
most ordinary and assured certainties. All these things merge into
steadily increasing uncertainty, lack of energy, curtailment of personal
liberty, despite the fact that the patient suffering from compulsion
neurosis is originally a most energetic character, often of extraordinary
obstinacy, as a rule intellectually gifted above the average. For the most
part he has attained a desirable stage of ethical development, is
overconscientious and more than usually correct. You can imagine that it
takes no inconsiderable piece of work to find one's way through this
maze of contradictory characteristics and symptoms. Indeed, for the
present our only object is to understand and to interpret some symptoms
of this disease.
218
A lady about thirty years old suffered from the most severe compulsions.
I might indeed have helped her if caprice of fortune had not destroyed
my work—perhaps I will yet have occasion to tell you about it. In the
course of each day the patient often executed, among others, the
following strange compulsive act. She ran from her room into an
adjoining one, placed herself in a definite spot beside a table which stood
in the middle of the room, rang for her maid, gave her a trivial errand to
do, or dismissed her without more ado, and then ran back again. This
was certainly not a severe symptom of disease, but it still deserved to
arouse curiosity. Its explanation was found, absolutely without any
assistance on the part of the physician, in the very simplest way, a way to
which no one can take exception. I hardly know how I alone could have
guessed the meaning of this compulsive act, or have found any
suggestion toward its interpretation. As often as I had asked the patient:
"Why do you do this? Of what use is it?" she had answered, "I don't
know." But one day after I had succeeded in surmounting a grave ethical
doubt of hers she suddenly saw the light and related the history of the
compulsive act. More than ten years prior she had married a man far
older than herself, who had proved impotent on the bridal night.
Countless times during the night he had run from his room to hers to
repeat the attempt, but each time without success. In the morning he
said angrily: "It is enough to make one ashamed before the maid who
does the beds," and took a bottle of red ink that happened to be in the
room, and poured its contents on the sheet, but not on the place where
such a stain would have been justifiable. At first I did not understand the
connection between this reminiscence and the compulsive act in
question, for the only agreement I could find between them was in the
running from one room into another,—possibly also in the appearance of
the maid. Then the patient led me to the table in the second room and let
me discover a large spot on the cover. She explained also that she placed
herself at the table in such a way that the maid could not miss seeing the
stain. Now it was no longer possible to doubt the intimate relation of the
scene after her bridal night and her present compulsive act, but there
were still a number of things to be learned about it.
In the first place, it is obvious that the patient identifies herself with her
husband, she is acting his part in her imitation of his running from one
room into the other. We must then admit—if she holds to this role—that
she replaces the bed and sheet by table and cover. This may seem
arbitrary, but we have not studied dream symbolism in vain. In dreams
also a table which must be interpreted as a bed, is frequently seen. "Bed
and board" together represent married life, one may therefore easily be
used to represent the other.
220
The evidence that the compulsive act carries meaning would thus be
plain; it appears as a representation, a repetition of the original
significant scene. However, we are not forced to stop at this semblance of
a solution; when we examine more closely the relation between these two
people, we shall probably be enlightened concerning something of wider
importance, namely, the purpose of the compulsive act. The nucleus of
this purpose is evidently the summoning of the maid; to her she wishes
to show the stain and refute her husband's remark: "It is enough to
shame one before the maid." He—whose part she is playing—therefore
feels no shame before the maid, hence the stain must be in the right
place. So we see that she has not merely repeated the scene, rather she
has amplified it, corrected it and "turned it to the good." Thereby,
however, she also corrects something else,—the thing which was so
embarrassing that night and necessitated the use of the red ink—
impotence. The compulsive act then says: "No, it is not true, he did not
have to be ashamed before the maid, he was not impotent." After the
manner of a dream she represents the fulfillment of this wish in an overt
action, she is ruled by the desire to help her husband over that
unfortunate incident.
Everything else that I could tell you about this case supports this clue
more specifically; all that we otherwise know about her tends to
strengthen this interpretation of a compulsive act incomprehensible in
itself. For years the woman has lived separated from her husband and is
struggling with the intention to obtain a legal divorce. But she is by no
means free from him; she forces herself to remain faithful to him, she
retires from the world to avoid temptation; in her imagination
she excuses and idealizes him. The deepest secret of her malady is that
by means of it she shields her husband from malicious gossip, justifies
her separation from him, and renders possible for him a comfortable
separate life. Thus the analysis of a harmless compulsive act leads to the
very heart of this case and at the same time reveals no inconsiderable
portion of the secret of the compulsion neurosis in general. I shall be
glad to have you dwell upon this instance, as it combines conditions that
one can scarcely demand in other cases. The interpretation of the
symptoms was discovered by the patient herself in one flash, without the
suggestion or interference of the analyst. It came about by the reference
to an experience, which did not, as is usually the case, belong to the half-
221
And one thing more! Have you not observed how this insignificant
compulsive act initiated us into the intimate life of the invalid? A woman
can scarcely relate anything more intimate than the story of her bridal
night, and is it without further significance that we just happened to
come on the intimacies of her sexual life? It might of course be the result
of the selection I have made in this instance. Let us not judge too quickly
and turn our attention to the second instance, one of an entirely different
kind, a sample of a frequently occurring variety, namely, the sleep ritual.
Allow me to pass over the other trivial incidents of this ritual; they would
teach us nothing new and cause too great digression from our purpose.
Do not overlook, however, the fact that all this does not run its course
quite smoothly. Everything is pervaded by the anxiety that things have
not been done properly; they must be examined, repeated. Her doubts
seize first on one, then on another precaution, and the result is that one
or two hours elapse during which the girl cannot and the intimidated
parents dare not sleep.
These torments were not so easily analyzed as the compulsive act of our
former patient. In the working out of the interpretations I had to hint
and suggest to the girl, and was met on her part either by positive denial
or mocking doubt. This first reaction of denial, however, was followed by
a time when she occupied herself of her own accord with the possibilities
223
that had been suggested, noted the associations they called out,
produced reminiscences, and established connections, until through her
own efforts she had reached and accepted all interpretations. In so far as
she did this, she desisted as well from the performance of her compulsive
rules, and even before the treatment had ended she had given up the
entire ritual. You must also know that the nature of present-day analysis
by no means enables us to follow out each individual symptom until its
meaning becomes clear. Rather it is necessary to abandon a given theme
again and again, yet with the certainty that we will be led back to it in
some other connection. The interpretation of the symptoms in this case,
which I am about to give you, is a synthesis of results, which, with the
interruptions of other work, needed weeks and months for their
compilation.
Our patient gradually learns to understand that she has banished clocks
and watches from her room during the night because the clock is the
symbol of the female genital. The clock, which we have learned to
interpret as a symbol for other things also, receives this role of the
genital organ through its relation to periodic occurrences at equal
intervals. A woman may for instance be found to boast that her
menstruation is as regular as clockwork. The special fear of our patient,
however, was that the ticking of the clock would disturb her in her sleep.
The ticking of the clock may be compared to the throbbing of the clitoris
during sexual excitement. Frequently she had actually been awakened by
this painful sensation and now this fear of an erection of the clitoris
caused her to remove all ticking clocks during the night. Flowerpots and
vases are, as are all vessels, also female symbols. The precaution,
therefore, that they should not fall and break at night, was not without
meaning. We know the widespread custom of breaking a plate or dish
when an engagement is celebrated. The fragment of which each guest
possesses himself symbolizes his renunciation of his claim to the bride, a
renunciation which we may assume as based on the monogamous
marriage law. Furthermore, to this part of her ceremonial our patient
adds a reminiscence and several associations. As a child she had slipped
once and fallen with a bowl of glass or clay, had cut her finger, and bled
violently. As she grew up and learned the facts of sexual intercourse, she
developed the fear that she might not bleed during her bridal night and
so not prove to be a virgin. Her precaution against the breaking of vases
224
One day she guessed the central idea of her ceremonial, when she
suddenly understood her rule not to let the pillow come in contact with
the bed. The pillows always had seemed a woman to her, the erect back
of the bed a man. By means of magic, we may say, she wished to keep
apart man and wife; it was her parents she wished to separate, so to
prevent their marital intercourse. She had sought to attain the same end
by more direct methods in earlier years, before the institution of her
ceremonial. She had simulated fear or exploited a genuine timidity in
order to keep open the door between the parents' bedroom and the
nursery. This demand had been retained in her present ceremonial. Thus
she had gained the opportunity of overhearing her parents, a proceeding
which at one time subjected her to months of sleeplessness. Not content
with this disturbance to her parents, she was at that time occasionally
able to gain her point and sleep between father and mother in their very
bed. Then "pillow" and "wooden wall" could really not come in contact.
Finally when she became so big that her presence between the parents
could not longer be borne comfortably, she consciously simulated fear
and actually succeeded in changing places with her mother and taking
her place at her father's side. This situation was undoubtedly the starting
point for the phantasies, whose after-effects made themselves felt in her
ritual.
played the part of the man, the father, and her head took the place of the
male organ. (Cf. the symbol of beheading to represent castration.)
Wild ideas, you will say, to run riot in the head of a virgin girl. I admit it,
but do not forget that I have not created these ideas but merely
interpreted them. A sleep ritual of this kind is itself very strange, and you
cannot deny the correspondence between the ritual and the phantasies
that yielded us the interpretation. For my part I am most anxious that
you observe in this connection that no single phantasy was projected in
the ceremonial, but a number of them had to be integrated,—they must
have their nodal points somewhere in space. Observe also that the
observance of the ritual reproduce the sexual desire now positively, now
negatively, and serve in part as their rejection, again as their
representation.
Whoever among you takes the trouble to look into the matter will
undoubtedly be deeply impressed by the wealth of evidential material.
But he will also encounter difficulties. We have learned that the meaning
of a symptom is found in its relation to the experience of the patient. The
more highly individualized the symptom is, the sooner we may hope to
establish these relations. Therefore the task resolves itself specifically
into the discovery for every nonsensical idea and useless action of a past
situation wherein the idea had been justified and the action purposeful.
A perfect example for this kind of symptom is the compulsive act of our
patient who ran to the table and rang for the maid. But there are
symptoms of a very different nature which are by no means rare. They
must be called typical symptoms of the disease, for they are
approximately alike in all cases, in which the individual differences
disappear or shrivel to such an extent that it is difficult to connect them
with the specific experiences of the patient and to relate them to the
particular situations of his past. Let us again direct our attention to the
compulsion neurosis. The sleep ritual of our second patient is already
quite typical, but bears enough individual features to render possible
what may be called an historic interpretation. But all compulsive
patients tend to repeat, to isolate their actions from others and to subject
them to a rhythmic sequence. Most of them wash too much. Agoraphobia
(topophobia, fear of spaces), a malady which is no longer grouped with
the compulsion neurosis, but is now called anxiety hysteria, invariably
shows the same pathological picture; it repeats with exhausting
monotony the same feature, the patient's fear of closed spaces, of large
open squares, of long stretched streets and parkways, and their feeling of
safety when acquaintances accompany them, when a carriage drives after
them, etc. On this identical groundwork, however, the individual
differences between the patients are superimposed—moods one might
almost call them, which are sharply contrasted in the various cases. The
one fears only narrow streets, the other only wide ones, the one can go
227
out walking only when there are few people abroad, the other when there
are many. Hysteria also, aside from its wealth of individual features, has
a superfluity of common typical symptoms that appear to resist any
facile historical methods of tracing them. But do not let us forget that it
is by these typical symptoms that we get our bearings in reaching a
diagnosis. When, in one case of hysteria we have finally traced back a
typical symptom to an experience or a series of similar experiences, for
instance followed back an hysterical vomiting to its origin in a succession
of disgust impressions, another case of vomiting will confuse us by
revealing an entirely different chain of experiences, seemingly just as
effective. It seems almost as though hysterical patients must vomit for
some reason as yet unknown, and that the historic factors, revealed by
analysis, are chance pretexts, seized on as opportunity best offered to
serve the purposes of a deeper need.
I SAID last time that we would not continue our work from the
standpoint of our doubts, but on the basis of our results. We have not
even touched upon two of the most interesting conclusions, derived
equally from the same two sample analyses.
Here again we must leave the road we have been traveling. For the time
being, it leads us no further and we have many other things to find out
before we can go on again. But before we leave this subject let us note
that the fixation on some particular phase of the past has bearings which
extend far beyond the neurosis. Every neurosis contains such a fixation,
but every fixation does not lead to a neurosis, nor fall into the same class
with neuroses, nor even set the conditions for the development of a
neurosis. Mourning is a type of emotional fixation on a theory of the
past, which also brings with it the most complete alienation from the
present and the future. But mourning is sharply distinguished from
neuroses that may be designated as pathological forms of mourning.
Now let us turn to the second conclusion of our analysis, which however
we will hardly need to limit subsequently. We have spoken of the
senseless compulsive activities of our first patient, and what intimate
memories she disclosed as belonging to them; later we also investigated
the connection between experience and symptom and thus discovered
the purpose hidden behind the compulsive activity. But we have entirely
omitted one factor that deserves our whole attention. As long as the
patient kept repeating the compulsive activity she did not know that it
was in any way related with the experience in question. The connection
between the two was hidden from her, she truthfully answered that she
did not know what compelled her to do this. Once, suddenly, under the
influence of the cure, she hit upon the connection and was able to tell it
to us. But still she did not know of the end in the service of which she
performed the compulsive activities, the purpose to correct a painful part
of the past and to place the husband, still loved by her, upon a higher
level. It took quite a long time and a great deal of trouble for her to grasp
and admit to me that such a motive alone could have been the motive
force of the compulsive activity.
The relation between the scene after the unhappy bridal night and the
tender motive of the patient yield what we have called the meaning of the
compulsive activity. But both the "whence" and the "why" remained
hidden from her as long as she continued to carry out the compulsive act.
Psychological processes had been going on within her for which the
compulsive act found an expression. She could, in a normal frame of
mind, observe their effect, but none of the psychological antecedents of
her action had come to the knowledge of her consciousness. She had
acted in just the same manner as a hypnotized person to whom
Bernheim had given the injunction that five minutes after his awakening
in the ward he was to open an umbrella, and he had carried out this
order on awakening, but could give no motive for his so doing. We have
exactly such facts in mind when we speak of the existence of unconscious
psychological processes. Let anyone in the world account for these facts
in a more correct scientific manner, and we will gladly withdraw
completely our assumption of unconscious psychological processes.
Until then, however, we shall continue to use this assumption, and when
anyone wants to bring forward the objection that the unconscious can
have no reality for science and is a mere makeshift, (une façon de
233
In our second patient we meet with fundamentally the same thing. She
had created a decree which she must follow: the pillow must not touch
the head of the bed; yet she does not know how it originated, what its
meaning is, nor to what motive it owes the source of its power. It is
immaterial whether she looks upon it with indifference or struggles
against it, storms against it, determines to overcome it. She must
nevertheless follow it and carry out its ordinance, though she asks
herself, in vain, why. One must admit that these symptoms of
compulsion neurosis offer the clearest evidence for a special sphere of
psychological activity, cut off from the rest. What else could be back of
these images and impulses, which appear from one knows not where,
which have such great resistance to all the influences of an otherwise
normal psychic life; which give the patient himself the impression that
here are super-powerful guests from another world, immortals mixing in
the affairs of mortals. Neurotic symptoms lead unmistakably to a
conviction of the existence of an unconscious psychology, and for that
very reason clinical psychiatry, which recognizes only a conscious
psychology, has no explanation other than that they are present as
indications of a particular kind of degeneration. To be sure, the
compulsive images and impulses are not themselves unconscious—no
more so than the carrying out of the compulsive-acts escapes
conscious observation. They would not have been symptoms had they
not penetrated through into consciousness. But their psychological
antecedents as disclosed by the analysis, the associations into which we
place them by our interpretations, are unconscious, at least until we have
made them known to the patient during the course of the analysis.
Consider now, in addition, that the facts established in our two cases are
confirmed in all the symptoms of all neurotic diseases, that always and
everywhere the meaning of the symptoms is unknown to the sufferer,
that analysis shows without fail that these symptoms are derivatives of
unconscious experiences which can, under various favorable conditions,
become conscious. You will understand then that in psychoanalysis we
cannot do without this unconscious psyche, and are accustomed to deal
with it as with something tangible. Perhaps you will also be able to
234
understand how those who know the unconscious only as an idea, who
have never analyzed, never interpreted dreams, or never translated
neurotic symptoms into meaning and purpose, are most ill-suited to pass
an opinion on this subject. Let us express our point of view once more.
Our ability to give meaning to neurotic symptoms by means of analytic
interpretation is an irrefutable indication of the existence of unconscious
psychological processes—or, if you prefer, an irrefutable proof of the
necessity for their assumption.
But that is not all. Thanks to a second discovery of Breuer's, for which he
alone deserves credit and which appears to me to be even more far-
reaching, we are able to learn still more concerning the relationship
between the unconscious and the neurotic symptom. Not alone is the
meaning of the symptoms invariably hidden in the unconscious; but the
very existence of the symptom is conditioned by its relation to this
unconscious. You will soon understand me. With Breuer I maintain the
following: Every time we hit upon a symptom we may conclude that the
patient cherishes definite unconscious experiences which withhold the
meaning of the symptoms. Vice versa, in order that the symptoms may
come into being, it is also essential that this meaning be unconscious.
Symptoms are not built up out of conscious experiences; as soon as the
unconscious processes in question become conscious, the symptom
disappears. You will at once recognize here the approach to our therapy,
a way to make symptoms disappear. It was by these means that Breuer
actually achieved the recovery of his patient, that is, freed her of her
symptoms; he found a technique for bringing into her consciousness the
unconscious experiences that carried the meaning of her symptoms, and
the symptoms disappeared.
Now we shall make a hasty digression so that you do not by any chance
imagine that this therapeutic work is too easy. From all we have learned
so far, the neurosis would appear as the result of a sort of ignorance, the
incognizance of psychological processes that we should know of. We
would thus very closely approximate the well-known Socratic teachings,
according to which evil itself is the result of ignorance. Now the
experienced physician will, as a rule, discover fairly readily what psychic
impulses in his several patients have remained unconscious. Accordingly
it would seem easy for him to cure the patient by imparting this
knowledge to him and freeing him of his ignorance. At least the part
played by the unconscious meaning of the symptoms could easily be
discovered in this manner, and it would only be in dealing with the
relationship of the symptoms to the experiences of the patient that the
physician would be handicapped. In the face of these experiences, of
course, he is the ignorant one of the two, for he did not go through these
experiences, and must wait until the patient remembers them and tells
them to him. But in many cases this difficulty could be readily overcome.
One can question the relatives of the patient concerning these
experiences, and they will often be in a position to point out those that
carry any traumatic significance; they may even be able to inform the
analyst of experiences of which the patient knows nothing because they
occurred in the very early years of his life. By a combination of such
236
means it would seem that the pathogenic ignorance of the patient could
be cleared up in a short time and without much trouble.
If only that were all! We have made discoveries for which we were at first
unprepared. Knowing and knowing is not always the same thing; there
are various kinds of knowing that are psychologically by no means
comparable. "Il y a fagots et fagots," 39 as Molière says. The knowledge
of the physician is not the same as that of the patient and cannot bring
about the same results. The physician can gain no results by transferring
his knowledge to the patient in so many words. This is perhaps putting it
incorrectly, for though the transference does not result in dissolving the
symptoms, it does set the analysis in motion, and calls out an energetic
denial, the first sign usually that this has taken place. The patient has
learned something that he did not know up to that time, the meaning of
his symptoms, and yet he knows it as little as before. So we discover
there is more than one kind of ignorance. It will require a deepening of
our psychological insight to make clear to us wherein the difference lies.
But our assertion nevertheless remains true that the symptoms
disappear with the knowledge of their meaning. For there is only one
limiting condition; the knowledge must be founded on an inner change
in the patient which can be attained only through psychic labors directed
toward a definite end. We have here been confronted by problems which
will soon lead us to the elaboration of a dynamics of symptom formation.
I must stop to ask you whether this is not all too vague and too
complicated? Do I not confuse you by so often retracting my words and
restricting them, spinning out trains of thought and then rejecting them?
I should be sorry if this were the case. However, I strongly dislike
simplification at the expense of truth, and am not averse to having you
receive the full impression of how many-sided and complicated the
subject is. I also think that there is no harm done if I say more on every
point than you can at the moment make use of. I know that every hearer
and reader arranges what is offered him in his own thoughts, shortens it,
simplifies it and extracts what he wishes to retain. Within a given
measure it is true that the more we begin with the more we have left. Let
me hope that, despite all the by-play, you have clearly grasped the
essential parts of my remarks, those about the meaning of symptoms,
about the unconscious, and the relation between the two. You probably
have also understood that our further efforts are to take two directions:
first, the clinical problem—to discover how persons become sick, how
they later on accomplish a neurotic adaptation toward life; secondly, a
problem of psychic dynamics, the evolution of the neurotic symptoms
themselves from the prerequisites of the neuroses. We will undoubtedly
somewhere come on a point of contact for these two problems.
I do not wish to go any further to-day, but since our time is not yet up I
intend to call your attention to another characteristic of our two
analyses, namely, the memory gaps or amnesias, whose full appreciation
will be possible later. You have heard that it is possible to express the
object of psychoanalytic treatment in a formula: all pathogenic
unconscious experience must be transposed into consciousness. You will
perhaps be surprised to learn that this formula can be replaced by
another: all the memory gaps of the patient must be filled out, his
amnesias must be abolished. Practically this amounts to the same thing.
Therefore an important role in the development of his symptoms must
be accredited to the amnesias of the neurotic. The analysis of our first
case, however, will hardly justify this valuation of the amnesia. The
patient has not forgotten the scene from which the compulsion act
derives—on the contrary, she remembers it vividly, nor is there any other
forgotten factor which comes into play in the development of these
symptoms. Less clear, but entirely analogous, is the situation in the case
of our second patient, the girl with the compulsive ritual. She, too, has
not really forgotten the behavior of her early years, the fact that she
insisted that the door between her bedroom and that of her parents be
kept open, and that she banished her mother out of her place in her
parents' bed. She recalls all this very clearly, although hesitatingly and
unwillingly. Only one factor stands out strikingly in our first case, that
though the patient carries out her compulsive act innumerable times, she
is not once reminded of its similarity with the experience after the bridal-
night; nor was this memory even suggested when by direct questions she
was asked to search for its motivation. The same is true of the girl, for in
her case not only her ritual, but the situation which provoked it, is
repeated identically night after night. In neither case is there any actual
amnesia, no lapse of memory, but an association is broken off which
should have called out a reproduction, a revival in the memory. Such a
238
has covered over the "whence" of the symptom, the experience upon
which it is based; for it is the "why," the tendency of the symptom, which
establishes its dependence on the unconscious, and indeed no less so in
the compulsion neuroses than in hysteria. In both cases the "why" may
have been unconscious from the very first.
Humanity, in the course of time, has had to endure from the hands of
science two great outrages against its naive self-love. The first was when
humanity discovered that our earth was not the center of the universe,
but only a tiny speck in a world-system hardly conceivable in its
magnitude.
We psychoanalysts were neither the first, nor the only ones to announce
this admonition to look within ourselves. It appears that we are fated to
represent it most insistently and to confirm it by means of empirical data
which are of importance to every single person. This is the reason for the
widespread revolt against our science, the omission of all considerations
of academic urbanity, and emancipation of the opposition from all
restraints of impartial logic. We were compelled to disturb the peace of
240
the world, in addition, in another manner, of which you will soon come
to know.