nckh đề project 1
nckh đề project 1
To cite this article: Amin Rasti-Behbahani & Maryam Shahbazi (2022) Investigating the
effectiveness of a digital game-based task on the acquisition of word knowledge, Computer
Assisted Language Learning, 35:8, 1920-1945, DOI: 10.1080/09588221.2020.1846567
ABSTRACT KEYWORDS
This study investigates the probable effect of a digital Digital game-based
game-based vocabulary learning (DGBVL) task on the acqui- learning; vocabulary
sition of some components of a word knowledge frame- acquisition; word
knowledge framework
work. In so doing, 124 Persian speakers (56 males and 68
females) were randomly assigned to either a control or an
experimental group. The experimental group participants
completed a DGBVL task for acquiring ten low-frequent
inanimate object names, or lexical nouns, by playing a
commercial adventure game. The control group partici-
pants practiced the same words in a fill-in-the-blank
vocabulary acquisition exercise. In brief, first, all participants
sat for a word-checklist and a proficiency test; next, they
completed their tasks, and three weeks later, all participants
sat for eight achievement tests. In the achievement test
booklet, participants’ knowledge of receptive, productive,
recognition, and recall dimensions and scopes of meaning,
orthography, and association were evaluated. The results
revealed 1) the efficiency of the DGBVL task in enhancing
the acquisition of these components, 2) the precedence of
productive knowledge acquisition by the experimental
group participants, 3) strong associations among the com-
ponents acquired through DGBVL task assistance, and 4)
gains in the components that were not associated with
others due to the efficiency of DGBVL.
1. Introduction
Acquiring vocabulary is essential for language learners because “all other
things being equal, learners with big vocabularies are more proficient in
a wide range of language skills than learners with smaller vocabularies”
2. Literature review
2.1. Components of word knowledge
Aspects, dimensions, and scopes of word knowledge are outcomes of the
researchers’ attempts to answer what does it mean to know a word? For
instance, Richards (1976) answered that knowing a word was the acquisi-
tion of pieces of knowledge such as frequency, degree of exposure,
1922 A. RASTI-BEHBAHANI AND M. SHAHBAZI
Table 1. Aspects Involved in Knowing a Word (adapted from Nation, 2001, p. 27).
Form Spoken R What does the word sound link?
P How is the word pronounced?
Written R What does the word look like?
P How is the word written and spelled?
Word parts R What parts are recognizable in this word?
P What word parts are needed to express the meanings?
Meaning Form and meaning R What meaning does this word form signals?
P What word form can be used to express this meaning?
Concept and referents R What is included in this concept?
P What items can the concept refer to?
Associations R What other words does this make us think of?
P What other words could we use instead of this one?
Use Grammatical functions R In what patterns does the word occur?
P In what pattern must we use this word?
Collocations R What words or type of words with this one?
P What words or type of words must we use with this?
Constrains on use (register, R Where, when, and how often would we expect to meet
frequency, ect.) this word
P Where, when, and how often can we use this word?
R ¼ Receptive Knowledge P ¼ Productive Knowledge.
Table 2. Degrees of form-meaning knowledge (adapted from Laufer & Goldstein, 2004,
p. 407).
Recall Recognition
Productive (retrieval of form) Supply the L2 word Select the L2 word
Receptive (retrieval of meaning) Supply the L1 word Select the L1 word
3. Methodology
The design of the study was modeled on the study by Webb (2007). The
independent factor was a DGBVL task, and the dependent factor was the
acquisition of some components of the word knowledge framework.
1926 A. RASTI-BEHBAHANI AND M. SHAHBAZI
3.1. Participants
In this study, based on a convenience sampling method, we recruited
124 Persian speakers from two private English language learning insti-
tutes in Behbahan, Iran. The Oxford placement test (2004) revealed that
the volunteers’ English proficiency was at the lower-intermediate level.
The participants, both male (N ¼ 56) and female (N ¼ 68) teenagers
(11 13 years old), were randomly assigned to either control (N ¼ 62) or
experimental groups (N ¼ 62). Moreover, both participants and their
families were asked to fill in a consent form after being acquainted with
their rights and the ethics of the research.
3.2. Materials
We used a digital game, a game-guide, or walkthrough, and a regular
vocabulary learning task (RVLT). They are explained in detail below.
Figure 1. A gamer is trying to use an object (a rock) to interact with the environment
(breaking the glass).
Figure 2. A gamer is trying to combine and fix the found pieces of an object (a door knob)
to solve a problem (opening a door) in the game.
3.2.2. Walkthrough
A walkthrough is an instruction manual for the game. It helps a gamer
to complete the game by following its instructions. The walkthrough was
downloaded from the official website of the game. The first chapter of
the walkthrough was extracted and simplified by numbering the senten-
ces, putting them in numerical order, adding picture guides for the
mini-games that were not relevant to study but had to be played, and
rewriting complex sentences to make it both user-friendly and easy to
follow (Table 3).
Participants were given the walkthrough to prevent the negative effects
of extensive interactivity, which can result in negative cognitive load and
probably lead to difficulty in recalling vocabulary items (deHaan et al.,
2010). Moreover, it could reduce time-on-task and prevent demotivation.
1928 A. RASTI-BEHBAHANI AND M. SHAHBAZI
3.2.3. RVLT
The RVLT was a fill-in-the-blanks task that was designed by the researchers.
In this task, the control group participants had to complete ten sentences by
choosing appropriate words from the wordlist at the top of the page
(Appendix A). The wordlist contained 12 words, and the sentences were
extracted from the British National Corpus. The control group participants did
not need to modify the grammatical form of the words to fit their sentences.
3.3. Instruments
We used the Oxford placement test (2004), a vocabulary checklist, and a
test booklet for eliciting data from the participants in this study. The
COMPUTER ASSISTED LANGUAGE LEARNING 1929
3.4. Procedure
First, to comply with the research ethics, we explained participants’
rights in this study. Also, we asked them to fill in a letter of consent that
had to be signed by them and by their parents. The day after, they sat
the Oxford proficiency test, and also the vocabulary checklist test in a
COMPUTER ASSISTED LANGUAGE LEARNING 1931
4. Results
As the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test revealed that the test scores were not
normally distributed (Table 5), the nonparametric Mann Whitney U test,
1932 A. RASTI-BEHBAHANI AND M. SHAHBAZI
Friedman Test, Wilcoxon and Bonferroni post hoc tests, and Spearman
rho correlation (Hall, 2015) tests were used to analyze the data.
Table 8. Wilcoxon and Bonferroni correction post hoc tests results (Experimental Group).
Meaning Meaning Meaning Meaning
Orthography Orthography (Productive (Productive (Receptive (Receptive Association Association Productive Receptive
(Receptive) (Productive) Recognition) Recall) Recognition) Recall) (Receptive) (productive) (total) (Total)
Orthography (Receptive) Z 2.531 6.453 1.327 2.957 -.107 5.715 -.075 9.666 9.674
Sig. 0.511 0.000 8.297 0.140 41.176 0.005 42.298 0.000 0.000
Orthography (Productive) Z 2.531 3.679 4.822 -.631 3.107 3.530 3.845 9.669 9.670
Sig. 0.511 0.010 0.000 23.752 0.085 0.019 0.005 0.000 0.000
Meaning (Productive Recognition) Z 6.453 3.679 7.163 5.550 5.709 2.250 5.881 9.671 9.653
Sig. 0.000 0.010 0.000 0.012 0.005 1.099 0.000 0.000 0.000
A. RASTI-BEHBAHANI AND M. SHAHBAZI
Meaning (Productive Recall) Z 1.327 4.822 7.163 5.734 2.169 7.555 2.150 9.668 9.670
Sig. 8.297 0.000 0.000 0.000 1.353 0.000 1.420 0.000 0.000
Meaning (Receptive Recognition) Z 2.957 -.631 5.550 5.734 3.086 4.536 3.243 9.667 9.669
Sig. 0.140 23.752 0.012 0.000 0.091 0.000 0.053 0.000 0.000
Meaning (Receptive Recall) Z -.107 3.107 5.709 2.169 3.086 6.417 -.315 9.667 9.673
Sig. 41.176 0.085 0.005 1.353 0.091 0.000 33.889 0.000 0.000
Association (Receptive) Z 5.715 3.530 2.250 7.555 4.536 6.417 6.592 9.667 9.670
Sig. 0.005 0.019 1.099 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000
Association (productive) Z -.075 3.845 5.881 2.150 3.243 -.315 6.592 9.669 9.669
Sig. 42.298 0.005 0.000 1.420 0.053 33.889 0.000 0.000 0.000
Productive (total) Z 9.666 9.669 9.671 9.668 9.667 9.667 9.667 9.669 1.204
Sig. 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 10.292
Receptive (total) Z 9.674 9.670 9.653 9.670 9.669 9.673 9.670 9.669 1.204
Sig. 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 10.292
COMPUTER ASSISTED LANGUAGE LEARNING 1935
For ensuring the efficiency of the DGBVL task in enhancing the acqui-
sition of the productive dimension of form-meaning recognition, the
control group’s performances in each achievement test had to be com-
pared (Tables 9 and 10).
The results show that, in general, the control group participants’ per-
formances in both receptive (total) and productive (total) tests are not
significantly different. However, in particular, their performance in the
receptive knowledge of association, at p 0.05, is significantly different
from their performances in the productive knowledge of association and
most of the other tests. In other words, the RVLT enhanced the acquisi-
tion of receptive knowledge of association effectively.
Overall, it seems that the type of vocabulary learning task can play a
significant role in the acquisition of dimensions, aspects, and compo-
nents of word knowledge. Moreover, depending on the type of task, the
weight of the effect can vary on the different aspects and scopes of the
word knowledge framework.
Table 10. Wilcoxon and Bonferroni correction post hoc tests results (Control Group).
Meaning Meaning Meaning Meaning
Orthography Orthography (Productive (Productive (Receptive (Receptive Association Association Receptive Productive
(Receptive) (Productive) Recognition) Recall) Recognition) Recall) (Receptive) (productive) (Total) (total)
Orthography (Receptive) Z 3.322 2.237 2.514 -.477 -.700 4.041 -.328 6.863 6.850
Sig. 0.040 1.138 0.536 28.496 21.763 0.002 33.436 0.000 0.000
Orthography (Productive) Z 3.322 1.562 4.822 3.281 2.851 -.794 3.655 6.865 6.856
Sig. 0.040 5.326 0.000 0.046 0.196 19.218 0.011 0.000 0.000
Meaning (Productive Recognition) Z 2.237 1.562 4.550 2.887 1.302 2.564 1.945 6.855 6.855
Sig. 1.138 5.326 0.000 0.174 8.674 0.465 2.332 0.000 0.000
A. RASTI-BEHBAHANI AND M. SHAHBAZI
Meaning (Productive Recall) Z 2.514 4.822 4.550 2.985 3.900 5.914 2.525 6.861 6.852
Sig. 0.536 0.000 0.000 0.127 0.004 0.000 0.521 0.000 0.000
Meaning (Receptive Recognition) Z -.477 3.281 2.887 2.985 1.701 4.627 -.441 6.860 6.852
Sig. 28.496 0.046 0.174 0.127 3.999 0.000 29.656 0.000 0.000
Meaning (Receptive Recall) Z -.700 2.851 1.302 3.900 1.701 3.938 -.985 6.860 6.852
Sig. 21.763 0.196 8.674 0.004 3.999 0.003 14.606 0.000 0.000
Association (Receptive) Z 4.041 -.794 2.564 5.914 4.627 3.938 4.501 6.862 6.851
Sig. 0.002 19.218 0.465 0.000 0.000 0.003 0.000 0.000 0.000
Association (productive) Z -.328 3.655 1.945 2.525 -.441 -.985 4.501 6.860 6.860
Sig. 33.436 0.011 2.332 0.521 29.656 14.606 0.000 0.000 0.000
Receptive (total) Z 6.863 6.865 6.855 6.861 6.860 6.860 6.862 6.860 -.623
Sig. 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 24.002
Productive (total) Z 6.850 6.856 6.855 6.852 6.852 6.852 6.851 6.860 -.623
Sig. 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 24.002
Table 11. Results of Spearman correlation (experimental group).
Meaning Meaning Meaning
Orthography Orthography (Productive Meaning (Receptive (Receptive Association Association
(Receptive) (Productive) Recognition) (Productive Recall) Recognition) Recall) (Receptive) (productive)
Orthography (Receptive) Correlation Coefficient .532 .074 .532 .505 .545 .526 .565
Sig. .000 .567 .000 .000 .000 .000 .000
Orthography (Productive) Correlation Coefficient .532 .426 0.993 .908 .947 .893 .983
Sig. .000 .001 .000 .000 .000 .000 .000
Meaning (Productive Recognition) Correlation Coefficient .074 .426 .426 .458 .432 .442 .397
Sig. .567 .001 .001 .000 .000 .000 .001
Meaning (Productive Recall) Correlation Coefficient .532 0.993 .426 .908 .947 .893 .983
Sig. .000 .000 .001 .000 .000 .000 .000
Meaning (Receptive Recognition) Correlation Coefficient .505 .908 .458 .908 .877 .991 .897
Sig. .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 .000
Meaning (Receptive Recall) Correlation Coefficient .545 .947 .432 .947 .877 .865 .933
Sig. .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 .000
Association (Receptive) Correlation Coefficient .526 .893 .442 .893 .991 .865 .907
Sig. .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 .000
Association (productive) Correlation Coefficient .565 .983 .397 .983 .897 .933 .907
Sig. .000 .000 .001 .000 .000 .000 .000
¼ significant; the sample size (N ¼ 62).
COMPUTER ASSISTED LANGUAGE LEARNING
1937
1938
5. Discussion
This study investigated the effect of a DGBVL task on the acquisition of
word knowledge. The outcomes supported the previous findings in the
DGBVL literature that had reported on the positive effect of DGBVL
tasks (Janebi Enayat & Haghighatpasand, 2019; Sundqvist, 2019).
Further, this study adds that a DGBVL task may enhance the acquisition
of aspects, dimensions, and scopes of the word knowledge such as recep-
tive, productive, recognition, and recall knowledge of orthography,
meaning, and association. A good explanation for this positive effect can
be the role of context. According to Webb (2007), contextualized
vocabulary items can be acquired effectively due to surrounding contexts.
Digital games provide a rich multimedia context, in which textual, pic-
torial, and auditory information are easily accessible for gamers (Janebi
Enayat & Haghighatpasand, 2019). In such contexts the richness of input
can support effective word knowledge acquisition. This idea is tenable by
referring to the dual coding theory, which suggests that a simultaneous
provision of visual and textual inputs leads to their long-term retention
(Nation, 2001). Hence the experimental group participants during the
game experienced the target words multidimensionally. In other words,
they saw the textual forms and pictures and heard the pronunciations of
the target words simultaneously. Thus, they would have stored combina-
tions of rich information about every target word containing knowledge
of the aspects, dimensions, and scopes and their associations, in
their lexicon.
In the second question, we tried to find which aspects, dimensions,
and scopes of the word knowledge framework are acquired more effect-
ively. The results showed that the experimental group participants
acquired the productive-recognition of form-meaning more effectively;
however, the control group participants acquired the receptive knowledge
of association more effectively. Considering the experimental group par-
ticipants’ performance, the precedence of form-meaning and recognition
knowledge over other aspects and scopes is not a peculiar and
1940 A. RASTI-BEHBAHANI AND M. SHAHBAZI
sentence. Therefore, they were always comparing the nouns and their
definitions to each other. Duly, they outperformed in the test of receptive
knowledge of association.
Eventually, it should be noted that although we tried to avoid the
effect of earlier tests on answering the later tests strictly, testing 10 target
words eight times might have helped the participants to learn some
words from the earlier tests and affected the answers. Hence, it can be
speculated that, for example, if the DGBVL task encouraged the partici-
pants in investing most of their attentional sources in the forms, the par-
ticipants might have also focused on the forms during their tests and
picked some clues about the forms from earlier tests and used them to
answer the forthcoming tests.
Finally, we found that although components of the word knowledge
framework can be associated, and gains in one component can elevate
gains in other associated components (Schmitt, 1998; Schmitt & Meara,
1997), these associations are highly susceptible to the type and the struc-
ture of vocabulary acquisition tasks. In this study, unlike with the RVLT,
the DGBVL task helped the participants generate strong and positive
associations among most components of the word knowledge framework.
Schmitt and Meara (1997) and Schmitt (1998) have found that there are
particular and strong associations between meaning and other compo-
nents of the word knowledge framework. Hence, the DGBVL task might
have enhanced the acquisition of meaning senses and developed the
form-meaning links efficiently. The acquisition of other associated com-
ponents was duly enhanced efficiently. Moreover, by comparing the
amount and strength of the associations formed among the components
by the DGBVL task with those formed by the RVLT, it can be realized
that the nature of DGBVL task inputs is different from the regular
paper-based vocabulary learning tasks. Also, it can be inferred that the
richness and the distinctive nature of the DGBVL task inputs might have
led to the efficient acquisition of the components and the strong links
among the associated components.
6. Conclusion
This study has three major limitations. Firstly, the target words were
selected among concrete nouns only. Therefore, the findings of this study
may not extend to adjectives, verbs, and adverbs. Second, although tests
sequencing and administration were done carefully, participants might
have learned some words from the earlier tests and that could have
affected the answers. Third, having only concrete nouns did not allow
the effect of the DGBVL task on the other aspects such as grammar,
1942 A. RASTI-BEHBAHANI AND M. SHAHBAZI
Notes
1. The information about the frequency profiles and the range of the nouns were
obtained from https://www.lextutor.ca/vp/comp/.
2. https://www.oxfordlearnersdictionaries.com/
acknowledgements
I would like to acknowledge Mr. Ismael Farrokhian for his precious comments on
this paper.
Disclosure statement
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).
Notes on contributors
Amin Rasti-Behbahani is a postdoctoral researcher at University of Jyvaskyla. His
research interests are digital-game-based learning, and vocabulary acquisition.
Maryam Shahbazi is a master graduate from Jyv€askyl€a University of Applied Sciences
and her research interest is digital-game-based learning.
ORCID
Amin Rasti-Behbahani http://orcid.org/0000-0002-1507-6986
References
Bahari, A. (2020). Game-based collaborative vocabulary learning in blended and dis-
tance L2 learning. Open Learning: The Journal of Open, Distance and e-Learning,
1–22. doi:10.1080/02680513.2020.1814229
Chen, H. J. H., & Hsu, H. L. (2019). The impact of a serious game on vocabulary and
content learning. Computer Assisted Language Learning, 1–22. doi:10.1080/09588221.
2019.1593197
COMPUTER ASSISTED LANGUAGE LEARNING 1943
Chen, M. H., Tseng, W. T., & Hsiao, T. Y. (2018). The effectiveness of digital game-
based vocabulary learning: A framework-based view of meta-analysis. British Journal
of Educational Technology, 49(1), 69–77. doi:10.1111/bjet.12526
Cohen, A. D. (1987). Using verbal reports in research on language learning. In C.
Faerch & G. Kasper (Eds.), Multilingual matters, No. 30. Introspection in second lan-
guage research (pp. 82–95). Clevedon, England: Multilingual Matters.
De La Fuente, M. J. (2002). Negotiation and oral acquisition of L2 vocabulary: The roles
of input and output in the receptive and productive acquisition of words. Studies in
Second Language Acquisition, 24(1), 81–112. doi:10.1017/S0272263102001043
deHaan, J., Reed, W. M., & Kuwada, K. (2010). The effect of interactivity with a music
video game on second language vocabulary recall. Language Learning & Technology,
14(2), 74–94.
Ebrahimzadeh, M., & Alavi, S. (2017). Readers, players, and watchers: Short and long-
term vocabulary retention through digital video games. International Journal of Applied
Linguistics and English Literature, 6(4), 52–62. doi:10.7575/aiac.ijalel.v.6n.4p.52
Geranpayeh, A. (2003). A quick review of the English Quick Placement Test. Research
Notes, 12, 8–10.
Hall, J. L. (2015). A guide to doing statistics in second language research using SPSS and
R. New York: Routledge.
Janebi Enayat, M., & Haghighatpasand, M. (2019). Exploiting adventure video games for
second language vocabulary recall: A mixed-methods study. Innovation in Language
Learning and Teaching, 13(1), 61–75.
Jasso, P. R. (2012). A non-academic computer video game: Its effect on vocabulary
acquisition in the EFL classroom (Published master’s thesis). New York: Syracuse
University.
Kirriemuir, J. (2002). The relevance of video games and gaming consoles to the higher
and further education learning experience. Retrieved June 20, 2009, from http://www.
jisc.ac.uk/index.cfm?name=techwatch_report_0201.
Krashen, S. (1989). We acquire vocabulary and spelling by reading: Additional evidence for
the input hypothesis. The Modern Language Journal, 73(4), 440–464. doi:10.2307/326879
Laufer, B., & Goldstein, Z. (2004). Testing vocabulary knowledge: Size, strength, and
computer adaptiveness. Language Learning, 54(3), 399–436. doi:10.1111/j.0023-8333.
2004.00260.x
Lee, J. S. (2019). Informal digital learning of English and second language vocabulary
outcomes: Can quantity conquer quality?. British Journal of Educational Technology,
50(2), 767–778. doi:10.1111/bjet.12599
Meara, P. (1997). Towards a new approach to modelling vocabulary acquisition. In N.
Schmitt & M. McCarthy (Eds.), Vocabulary: Description, acquisition and pedagogy
(pp. 109–121). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Mondria, J. A., & Wiersma, B. (2004). Receptive, productive, and receptive þ productive
L2 vocabulary learning: What difference does it make. Vocabulary in a Second
Language: Selection, Acquisition, and Testing, 15(1), 79–100.
Morris, C. D., Bransford, J. D., & Franks, J. J. (1977). Levels of processing versus trans-
fer appropriate processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 16(5), 519–533.
Nation, I. S. P. (2001). Learning vocabulary in another language. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Pivec, M., Dziabenko, O., & Schinnerl, I. (2003, July). Aspects of game-based learning. In
3rd International Conference on Knowledge Management, Graz, Austria (pp.
216–225).
1944 A. RASTI-BEHBAHANI AND M. SHAHBAZI
Rabu, S. N. A., & Talib, Z. (2017). The effects of digital game-based learning on primary
school students’ English vocabulary achievement and acceptance. Innovative Teaching
and Learning Journal (ITLJ), 1(1), 61–74.
Rasti-Behbahani, A. (2017, March). The effect of video games on learning lexical word
classes. Presented at Nordic Educational Research Association Conference, Aalborg
University, Denmark.
Richards, J. C. (1976). The role of vocabulary teaching. TESOL Quarterly, 10(1), 77–89.
doi:10.2307/3585941
Ringbom, H. (1987). The role of the first language in foreign language learning (Vol. 34).
Clevedon: Multilingual Matters Ltd.
Schmitt, N. (1998). Tracking the incremental acquisition of second language vocabulary: A
longitudinal study. Language Learning, 48(2), 281–317. doi:10.1111/1467-9922.00042
Schmitt, N. (2010). Researching vocabulary: A vocabulary research manual. Hampshire:
Palgrave Macmilan.
Schmitt, N., & Meara, P. (1997). Researching vocabulary through a word knowledge
framework: Word associations and verbal suffixes. Studies in Second Language
Acquisition, 19(1), 17–36. doi:10.1017/S0272263197001022
Sundqvist, P. (2019). Commercial-off-the-shelf games in the digital wild and L2 learner
vocabulary. Language Learning & Technology, 23(1), 87–113. https://doi.org/10125/44674.
Sundqvist, P., & Wikstr€om, P. (2015). Out-of-school digital gameplay and in-school L2
english vocabulary outcomes. System, 51, 65–76.
Vahdat, S., & Rasti-Behbahani, A. (2013). The effect of video games on Iranian EFL
learners’ vocabulary learning. The Reading Matrix, 13(1), 61–71.
Webb, S. (2005). Receptive and productive vocabulary learning: The effects of reading
and writing on word knowledge. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27(01),
33–52. doi:10.1017/S0272263105050023
Webb, S. (2007). Learning word pairs and glossed sentences: The effects of a single con-
text on vocabulary knowledge. Language Teaching Research, 11(1), 63–81. doi:10.1177/
1362168806072463
Zou, D., Huang, Y., & Xie, H. (2019). Digital game-based vocabulary learning: Where
are we and where are we going? Computer Assisted Language Learning, 1–27. doi:10.
1080/09588221.2019.1640745
There’s a slide … … … … … … . at the back of the hall which I can control from
the front.
When she heard or saw nothing more, she dropped the net … … … . back into
place, to disappear once more into the darkness of her house.
When you wash dishes, wear … … … . To protect your skin.