Psychopathy EMA Complete
Psychopathy EMA Complete
DYNAMICS OF PSYCHOPATHY
Colin E. Vize
Lori N. Scott
Thomas W. Kamarck
Aidan G. C. Wright
University of Pittsburgh
Conflict of Interest Statement: All authors declare that they have no conflict of interest to
report.
Corresponding Author: Colin E. Vize, 4307 Sennott Square, Pittsburgh, PA 15260. Email:
[email protected]
2
DYNAMICS OF PSYCHOPATHY
Abstract
in day-to-day life, has not been used to study how psychopathy influences emotional experiences
and interpersonal behavior. This preregistered study examined how psychopathy relates to
dynamic socio-affective processes in daily life. Two samples enriched for traits related to
variety of interpersonal and affective experiences (Observation N=8,137 to 16,460). The samples
differed in socioeconomic, age, and ethnic diversity, which allowed us to examine the
replicability and generalizability of results. Results showed that while psychopathy was related
to distinct affective experiences in both samples (e.g., increased hostile affect), psychopathy was
affective processes. Future directions for research on the affective and interpersonal processes of
externalizing behaviors (e.g., aggression and antisocial behavior; Neumann et al., 2015).
planning, sensation seeking), a manipulative, grandiose interpersonal style, and affective deficits
including low fear and lack of remorse (Patrick, 2022). The affective and interpersonal
components of psychopathy are considered to be central (e.g., Verschuere et al., 2018), and are
thought to partially explain chronic antisocial behavior (Frick, 2009). These deficits have
primarily been studied experimentally, using various laboratory-based paradigms (e.g., startle-
response, Patrick et al., 1993; facial affect recognition, Marsh & Blair, 2008). Results from these
studies have motivated multiple models to account for the unique affective and interpersonal
psychopathy’s affective and interpersonal processes in daily life using ecological momentary
assessment (EMA), despite the promise of EMA in helping understand various socio-affective
dynamics relevant to psychopathy. The current preregistered study was conducted to address this
gap, focusing on the role of psychopathic traits in various socio-affective processes in day-to-day
life.
Theorists have emphasized the distinctive affective profile of psychopathy, and historical
guilt or remorse (Horley, 2014). In The Mask of Sanity, Cleckley (1941/1988) developed sixteen
functioning (general poverty in affective reactions, absence of nervousness, lack of guilt and
remorse, incapacity for deep affectional bonds). Cleckley (1941/1988) further clarified that the
4
DYNAMICS OF PSYCHOPATHY
general deficit in affective experiences referred to the richness and strength of affective
responses as opposed to a lack of any affective experiences at all. Lykken (1995) argued for the
stemming from his early research showing that individuals seen as prototypical Clecklian
psychopaths showed lower fear responses to a conditioned stimulus that had been paired with an
electric shock. The work of Cleckley and Lykken was particularly influential for later
assertiveness, and general emotional resilience, which collectively fall under the umbrella of
remorse and fear, there were positive relations between psychopathy and other forms of negative
affect—most notably anger and hostility (McCord & McCord, 1964). The consistent affective
deficit across all conceptualizations of psychopathy is a lack of remorse or empathy (i.e., high
callousness), which is thought to play a central role in explaining why psychopathy is related to
serious antisocial behavior (Blair, 2017). Despite the centrality of affective and interpersonal
psychopathy components are not equally represented across measures. Relatedly, traits within
psychopathy show divergent empirical relations with interpersonal and affective outcomes.
the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised [PCL-R]; Hare, 2003) are generally unrelated to broad
negative affect outcomes, but positively related to lack of remorse. Meanwhile, scales assessing
antisocial-lifestyle features (e.g., Factor 2 of the PCL-R) positively relate to various forms of
1
The psychopathy specifier for antisocial personality disorder within the alternative model of personality disorders in
the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013) is also
meant to capture these traits—the specifier is characterized by low levels of anxiousness and withdrawal, and high
levels of attention seeking.
5
DYNAMICS OF PSYCHOPATHY
negative affectivity. Thus, the multidimensionality of psychopathy and its various
affective relations.
their review of psychopathy and emotional functioning, Nentjes et al. (2022) highlight research
spanning perceptions and appraisals of stimuli, the experience, expression, and regulation of
affect, and physiological components of affect. Findings on subjective reports of affect, most
relevant to the present study, have highlighted the need to refine conceptualizations of affective
deficits in psychopathy.
the relations between psychopathy total scores and multiple affect-related outcomes, including
self-reported affect and threat detection and response. While evidence supported deficits in threat
of fear. Though data were limited, findings supported a significant positive link between
psychopathy and anger, a significant negative relation between psychopathy and happiness, and
Derefinko (2006) examined the relations between psychopathy and broad-based measures
of anxiety, including assessments of fear and constraint. Total psychopathy scores were unrelated
to anxiety and fear, but moderately negatively related to constraint. However, the interpersonal-
affective components of psychopathy showed small negative relations with anxiety, fear, and
constraint, and the impulsive-antisocial components were positively related to anxiety, unrelated
6
DYNAMICS OF PSYCHOPATHY
to fear, and strongly negatively related to constraint. Importantly, research has shown that the
divergent findings across psychopathy components generalize to other forms of negative affect
(anger-hostility, depression), and divergent effects are enhanced when controlling for overlap
Campos and colleagues (2022) examined the relations between psychopathy and specific
empathy dimensions (cognitive and affective), where cognitive empathy was defined as the
ability to infer mental states of others and take their perspective, and affective empathy was
defined as sensitivity to and ability to vicariously experience the emotions of others. Results
showed that total psychopathy scores were negatively related to both cognitive and affective
empathy, but, consistent with other research, there was important variability across psychopathy
to both cognitive and affective empathy, while these relations were smaller for antisocial-
lifestyle features. Additionally, boldness-related traits were unrelated to cognitive empathy but
Other work has further emphasized that the affective profile of psychopathy may be more
complex. Garofalo and colleagues (2019) found that psychopathy was strongly related to anger,
spitefulness, and contempt, the latter two being complex affective states comprised of more basic
affect components. However, consistent with other work (Hicks & Patrick, 2006), these
associations varied when controlling for scale overlap and also varied across specific subscales.
Boldness from the Triarchic Psychopathy Measure (TriPM; Patrick et al., 2009) was unrelated to
contempt and showed significant negative relations with various state and trait measures of anger
and negative emotionality. Yet, all scales from the Self-report Psychopathy Scale (SRP; Paulhus
being devoid of affect. Affective deficits in psychopathy are likely more circumscribed and
features are most strongly linked to negative affect while interpersonal boldness is negatively
related to negative affectivity and mostly related to positive affective experiences and adaptive
socio-affective functioning (e.g., Sleep et al., 2019). Thus, the relations between psychopathy
and socio-affective experiences are complex, but are further complicated by the mixed findings
Though Cleckley (1941/1988) included two women among his 15 case presentations,
research on psychopathy in women has lagged behind the research literature focused on
Rogstad and Rogers (2008) only identified two studies that had explicitly focused on emotional
processing deficits in women with psychopathic traits. Researchers have begun to address this
gap in the literature (Verona et al., 2013; Verona & Vitale, 2018), but results have been mixed
regarding whether affective deficits in psychopathy observed for men generalize to women.
Some research has found support for the generalizability of affective deficits showing that, like
men, incarcerated women high in Factor 1 psychopathy traits demonstrate diminished startle
response to conditioned stimuli. However, Verona and Vitale (2018) note that other research
using alternative paradigms to assess affective processes (e.g., emotion recognition deficits when
viewing pictures of faces), has been equivocal in its support for affective deficits in women with
psychopathic traits.
reviewed above is that this work has been conducted experimentally using a limited set of
measures. This is true of research focused on both men and women high in psychopathic traits.
To date, no research has examined the socio-affective dynamics of psychopathy in daily life.
This is an important gap to address—the problems and dysfunction associated with psychopathy
occur in the ebb and flow of day-to-day life and in interactions with others. Thus, using methods
that can assess interpersonal dynamics that unfold over time has significant promise for studying
the affective and interpersonal features of psychopathy. EMA methods can also serve to establish
The generalizability of socio-affective findings in psychopathy across sex has also received little
attention. Important exceptions (e.g., Verona et al., 2013) have provided evidence that the
affective deficits in men with psychopathy are also observed in women with psychopathy while
other work has not found evidence of generalizability (e.g., ). No research has attempted to
extend observations of these deficits or lack thereof (for men or women) to ecologically valid
contexts.
EMA studies involve the repeated collection of real-time data focused on participants’
experiences and behaviors in the natural environments where they spend their lives (Shiffman et
al., 2008). For those with personality pathology, EMA can provide important insights into the
contexts where personality pathology is most disruptive. The present study makes use of
different sampling time frames to gain insight into the socio-affective processes related to
psychopathy (three days of intensive pseudo-random sampling; three weeks of moderate pseudo-
random sampling). The EMA methods in the current study can also help to establish the
9
DYNAMICS OF PSYCHOPATHY
ecological validity and boundary conditions of laboratory results (Trull & Ebner-Priemer, 2013),
an important step since laboratory studies represent the bulk of empirical investigations on
ways that synchronize theory and method have been made recently (e.g., Wright &
Zimmermann, 2019), but have not been incorporated into psychopathy research. The current
psychopathy. Hypotheses are detailed at the end of the Method section after introducing study-
Method
All study procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University
of Pittsburgh (IRB Protocol #s: 000515 and 13050549). All data and code to reproduce our
results are available at the Open Science Foundation page for the project: https://osf.io/quhgm/?
includes a detailed overview of past publications using these data and what information was
known prior to the registration of our analytical plan. This study involved analyses of existing
data, thus sample size determination was not a part of the preregistration.
Sample 1
Sample 1 (S1) comprised 145 women drawn from the larger Pittsburgh Girls Study
(PGS). The PGS involves a community sample of 2,450 women who were initially recruited in
1999 and 2000 when they were ages 5 to 8 years old. An in-depth overview of the PGS is
described elsewhere (Keenan et al., 2010). Our secondary data analysis focused on a subsample
of the PGS, selected using a two-stage process. First, women from the PGS who reported verbal,
10
DYNAMICS OF PSYCHOPATHY
physical, or relational aggression or self-harm/suicidality within the past year were identified via
items administered annually in the PGS assessment battery over the course of three years (2014–
2017). These participants were then contacted by telephone for further screening to determine
whether they had engaged in externalized aggressive behavior in the past month (e.g.,
to beat someone up) or self-harming behaviors or suicidal ideation. Women who endorsed any of
these behaviors in the past month were included. The final sample size of N=142 are those
women who were selected for the study, completed the Psychopathic Personality Inventory-Short
Form at baseline, and completed at least 10 EMA assessments (protocol described below).
The sample was ethnically and socioeconomically diverse (69.7% African American,
28.1% Caucasian, 2.1% multi-racial; 2.1% identified as Hispanic; 52.1% reported receiving
public assistance). In terms of education attainment, 51.4% did not complete high school while
41.5% completed high school or GED only, and 34% were unemployed. The mean age of the
S1 Procedure
touch-screen smartphones to complete the EMA protocol. The protocol involved three weeks of
assessments, including a daily morning assessment and six daily pseudo-random prompts. For
pseudo-random prompts, participants provided their typical waking hours, which were split into
six bins within which prompts were delivered randomly. EMA items were rated using a Likert-
scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely). Pseudo-random prompts were the focus of the
present analyses, since they assessed specific interpersonal experiences and allowed for
11
DYNAMICS OF PSYCHOPATHY
examination of relevant socio-affective processes. For the pseudo-random prompts, participants
completed a mean of 115.9 out of a maximum of 126 (6 prompts x 21 days; 91.9% compliance).
Sample 2
Sample 2 (S2) included 159 middle-aged adults (M=40.67; SD=5.83; 46.3% male)
selected for high scores on standard self-report measures of trait hostility. Participants were
predominantly white (80.6% white; 12.5% African American; 1.9% Asian; 1.9% Hispanic) and
had higher educational attainment compared to S1 (12.8% of the sample completed high school
or GED only). Approximately 41.8% of the sample reported income greater than $50,000 per
year.
Full details of the study procedure are available in Kamarck et al. (2009). Briefly,
potential participants were screened over the telephone on two standard measures of hostility
(Buss-Durkee Motor Aggression subscale; Buss & Durkee, 1957; 10 items from the Cook-
Medley Hostility scale; Cook & Medley, 1954). Participants who passed the telephone screen
were invited to complete further in-person screening which included a more complete set of
hostility items from the Buss–Durkee Motor Aggression subscale (43 items) and the Cook–
Medley subscales (27 items). Individuals meeting inclusion criteria completed a larger battery of
self-report and laboratory assessments, before completing a four-day EMA protocol that was
S2 Procedure
repeated assessment of participants’ mood and social experiences and was administered using
palm pilots (Palm Pilot Professional, Palm, Santa Clara, California) provided to participants. An
auditory prompt was delivered every 45 minutes during waking hours, alerting participants to
12
DYNAMICS OF PSYCHOPATHY
complete the survey. Thus, participants could complete a total of 21 prompts per day. All EMA
items were rated using a 0-100 point sliding visual analogue scale, with “No” and “Yes”
displayed at either pole. Practice day data were not included in our analyses, consistent with past
use of these data (Vella et al., 2012). The mean number of completed entries per participant was
Measures
Descriptive information for the primary self-report scales and EMA variables are
presented in Table 1.
Self-reported Psychopathy
Sample 1
SF; Lilienfeld & Hess, 2001). The PPI-SF is a 56-item self-report measure of psychopathy
comprised of eight subscales. Empirical investigations of the parent measure, the Psychopathic
Personality Inventory-Revised (PPI-R; Lilienfeld & Widows, 1994), have shown that the
subscales form two psychopathy dimensions termed Impulsive Antisociality (made up of the
and Stress Immunity subscales) (Benning et al., 2003). The Coldheartedness scale does not load
on either factor and is considered a standalone subscale. In the current study, internal consistency
values were α=.82 for Impulsive Antisociality and α=.70 for Fearless-dominance. Internal
consistency for the PPI-SF-Total score was α=.80. Impulsive Antisociality and Fearless-
dominance were significantly positively correlated (r=.19; p=.02), consistent with past work
McCrae, 1992), a 240-item self-report measure based upon the Five-factor Model of personality.
Past work has shown that a reliable and valid proxy measure of the PPI-R can be derived using
items from the NEO-PI-R (Witt et al., 2009). Thus, we took the same approach used by Witt and
colleagues (2009) to construct scales for Impulsive Antisociality (17 items), Fearless-dominance
(17 items), and a total score (34 items) using a subset of NEO-PI-R items. Internal consistency
values were α=.69 for NEO-Impulsive Antisociality, α=.80 for NEO-Fearless-dominance, and α
were significantly negatively correlated (r= -.23; p <.01), consistent with other work using the
Ambulatory Assessment
See Table 1 for EMA variable descriptives, including ICCs and multilevel reliabilities.
Three affect composite scales were created in S1 and S2. Four positive affect items
(excited, happy, cheerful, joyful) were combined to create positive affect scales in S1 and S2.
Negative affect composites used in both S1 and S2 included a hostile affect composite of three
items (hostile, irritable, angry2) and a non-hostile negative affect composite of three items
(scared, sad, lonely). In S1, two additional affect items (guilty, regretful) were combined to
index remorse-related affect. In S2, an additional two affect items (nervous, jittery) were
Interpersonal Interactions
2
In S1, this item had slightly different wording: angry at others.
14
DYNAMICS OF PSYCHOPATHY
Negative Interpersonal Interactions. In S1, negative interpersonal interactions were
assessed with two EMA items. At each random prompt, participants were asked if they had 1)
felt insulted or criticized and 2) felt rejected, abandoned, excluded, or left out since they had
received the last prompt. The two items were combined to create a negative interpersonal
interaction scale.
In S2, negative interpersonal interactions were assessed with three EMA items. During
each EMA prompt, participants were asked when their most recent social interaction was. If
participants reported that the interaction was less than 45 minutes ago, they were asked whether
1) Someone treated you badly 2) Someone interfered with your efforts and 3) Someone was in
conflict with you. The three items were combined to create a negative interpersonal interaction
scale in S2.
Positive Interpersonal Interactions. In S1, participants were asked if they had felt
complimented or praised since they had received the last prompt. This single item was used to
examined a single EMA item asking participants whether Someone gave you positive feedback in
order to have a comparable positive interaction outcome in both S1 and S2. Other positive
interpersonal EMA items administered in S2 had participants rate whether the interaction was 1)
Pleasant, 2) Agreeable, 3) Friendly, and 4) Intimate/close, and to what extent participants 5) Felt
close to others and 6) Confided in others. Pleasant, agreeable, and friendly interaction items
were combined to create an agreeable interaction composite, and the intimate, felt close, and
Primary Hypotheses
15
DYNAMICS OF PSYCHOPATHY
We had several primary hypotheses. First, we expected divergent relations for Fearless-
positively related to all negative affect and negative interactions, but negatively related to
would significantly enhance the within-person links between negative interactions and different
forms of negative affect, with the exception of remorseful affect. Last, we expected Impulsive
Antisociality to have an enhancing effect on each path of our within-person mediation model
that Fearless-dominance would be negatively related to all forms of negative affect as well as
frequency of negative interactions, and positively related to positive affect. Second, we expected
FD-traits to have a dampening effect on within-person links between negative interactions and
negative affect. We did not have specific hypotheses for Fearless-dominance regarding its
enhancing or dampening effects on positive interactions and positive affect. Regarding emotional
psychopathy traits would generally be linked to lower emotional diversity (i.e., negatively
correlated).
Results
We examined the relations between psychopathy and average reported affect and
interpersonal outcomes over the course of the EMA protocols in S1 and S2 using multilevel
linear regression models with robust maximum likelihood due to the nesting of observations
within participants. All level-2 psychopathy predictors were grand-mean centered for analyses.
16
DYNAMICS OF PSYCHOPATHY
Time of day (assessed continuously within each day) and day of the week (weekday vs.
Results are displayed in Table 2. Overall, findings showed few replicable relations
between psychopathy traits and average affect in S1 and S2. The exception was for hostile affect.
In both S1 (β=.28; 95% CI=.14; .42) and S2 (β=.21; 95% CI=.02; .40), Impulsive Antisociality
was significantly positively related to hostile affect. Three other effects were statistically
and hostile affect (β=-.20; 95% CI=-.38; -.02). In S2, while NEO-Impulsive Antisociality was
positively related to sad affect (β=.20; 95% CI=.03; .37), NEO-Fearless-dominance showed a
For aggregated interpersonal outcomes, most results showed null relations for
psychopathy dimensions in both S1 and S2 (Table 3). The lone exception was a significant
positive relation between PPI-Impulsive Antisociality and the average number of negative
Emotional Diversity
Individual scores for emotional diversity were calculated using metrics that quantify the
emotions experienced (Simpson’s index) during the EMA protocol using R code adapted from
Benson et al. (2018).3 Individual differences in these indices were correlated with each
psychopathy scale (Fearless-dominance, Impulsive Antisociality, and total score). Scores were
calculated for global, positive, and negative emotional diversity. Results for the relation between
psychopathy features and emotional diversity are presented in Table S1 while Figure 1 displays
3
See the supplementary material for a detailed overview of how the two indices operationalize emotional diversity.
17
DYNAMICS OF PSYCHOPATHY
emotional diversity data for select individuals in S1 and S2 high in psychopathy.4 Overall, results
showed that null effects were most common (r range= -.17-.15; median r = -.01). PPI Total
scores in S1 showed a significant negative relation with negative Emodiversity (r = -.17, 95%
CI= -.32; .00), though this was confined to Simpson’s index of negative emotional diversity.
negative interactions impacted feelings of hostile affect and non-hostile negative affect and how
positive interactions impacted positive affect. Next, we tested whether level-2 psychopathy
scales enhanced or dampened the level-1 relation between negative interpersonal interactions and
the two negative affect scales (i.e., a cross-level interaction effect). Additionally, we extended
these basic dynamic models to test a dynamic mediational model involving hostile affect and
relation between lagged hostile affect (hostile affect at time t-1) and current hostile affect (hostile
affect at time t) was partly mediated by negative interpersonal interactions, and to what extent
We used multilevel structural equation models (MSEM; Sadikaj et al., 2021) to test all
dynamic models involving interpersonal interactions and affect. MSEM was carried out using
Bayesian model estimation. Diffuse, default priors were used for all model parameters.
Psychopathy scales were grand-mean centered for all models. Time of day and day of the week
served as within-person covariates. Last, lagged hostile affect was person-mean centered in
4
Radar plots for all high-psychopathy participants in S1 and S2 are available on the OSF page for the project:
https://osf.io/quhgm/?view_only=3d9cab1e3d5345caad7e278091dd0aba
18
DYNAMICS OF PSYCHOPATHY
models estimating the mediational effects of hostility. All MSEM and multilevel linear
regression models were implemented in MPlus (version 8.7; Muthén & Muthén, 2017).
Results from these models are displayed in Table 4. At the within-person level, there was
robust evidence that each of the assessed interpersonal situations gave rise to specific affective
responses in both S1 and S2. Specifically, relative to participants’ average levels, negative
interactions increased feelings of hostile affect (S1: β =.39; 95% CI=.36; .42, S2: β =.42; 95%
CI=.39; .45), sad affect (S1: β =.38; 95% CI=.34; .40, S2: β =.16; 95% CI=.13; .19), remorseful
affect (S1: β=.26; 95% CI=.23; .29), and anxious affect (S2: β=.18; 95% CI=.15; .21).
Additionally, receiving positive feedback increased positive affect (S1: β=.26; 95% CI=.22; .28,
S2: β =.28; 95% CI=.25; .30), as did agreeable interactions (S2: β =.38; 95% CI=.35; .40) and
intimate interactions (S2: β=.24; 95% CI=.22; .27). However, psychopathy features did not
affect and negative interpersonal interactions found support for negative interpersonal
interactions mediating the relation between past and future hostile affect in both S1 and S2.
Specifically, hostile affect at t-1 was related to future negative interactions (S1: β=.14; 95%
CI=.12; .16, S2: β =.11; 95% CI=.09; .13), and negative interactions increased hostile affect (S1:
β=.37; 95% CI=.32; .40, S2: β=.40; 95% CI=.36; .42). The mediational effects were significant
in both samples (S1: B=.05; 95% CI=.03; .07, S2: B=.06; 95% CI=.04; .07), but direct paths
between past hostile affect at t-1 and future hostile affect at time t remained significant (S1: β
=.21; 95% CI=.20; .22, S2: β =.26; 95% CI=.23; .27). Despite replicable within-person mediation
19
DYNAMICS OF PSYCHOPATHY
processes, tests for cross-level moderation of within-person effects showed mostly null results
(Table 5). The one exception was the impact of PPI-Fearless-dominance on hostility inertia in
S1, where PPI-Fearless-dominance had a dampening effect (β= -.20; 95% CI=-.35; -.04).
Secondary Analyses
discriminant information how other personality traits relate to and impact the socio-affective
processes in S1 and S2. Borderline personality disorder symptom count (S1) and NEO-PI-R
Neuroticism (S2) were used based on research showing that trait neuroticism is most commonly
associated with various socio-affective processes in daily life (e.g., Hisler et al., 2020). S1 lacked
a baseline neuroticism scale, so BPD was chosen as a substitute given that trait negative
affectivity is a prominent feature of BPD. Results are presented in the supplementary materials
(Tables S3-S7). BPD symptom count in S1 was significantly positively related to various forms
interactions. NEO-Neuroticism was significantly related to aggregated sad affect in S2. Neither
associations.
Discussion
We sought to address the current research gap on interpersonal and affective dynamics of
day-to-day life. Overall, we found limited support for our primary hypotheses. Results provided
robust evidence for various within-person socio-affective processes, but psychopathy traits
showed little evidence of enhancing or dampening these effects. Furthermore, replicable effects
were confined to specific dimensions of psychopathy and in many cases psychopathy dimensions
20
DYNAMICS OF PSYCHOPATHY
showed divergent results. Last, psychopathy was unrelated to diversity in emotional experiences.
Together, the findings highlight important avenues for future research on the socio-affective
dynamics of psychopathy.
While there was robust evidence for various within-person socio-affective processes,
psychopathy had little impact on these processes. Impulsive Antisociality did show replicable
relations with averaged hostile affect in S1 and S2, but this finding was the exception. In most
cases, significant findings were only observed in one sample, if at all. For averaged affect and
interpersonal outcomes, these findings highlight that psychopathic dimensions do not manifest
moderate or large relations with relevant affective or interpersonal experiences in daily life.
Regarding the lack of moderation of within-person effects, the findings highlight that specific
interaction) function similarly for individuals across a range of psychopathy scores. Collectively,
However, null findings are informative when statistical power is high to detect the
smallest effects of interest. Sensitivity analyses were conducted to examine the smallest effect
size detectable in S1 and S2.5 Results are reported in supplementary Table S2. Tests focused on
average reported affect, average reported interpersonal interactions, and emotional diversity
outcomes had low power (.22-.25) to detect small effects (r=.10) but were adequately powered
(.80) to detect effects of approximately r=.25 or larger (Table S). Power was very similar for
tests of cross-level moderation. Thus, our primary tests were underpowered to detect small
5
Sensitivity analyses were conducted in R (version 4.1.1; R Core Team, 2022) and MPlus (version 8.7; Muthén &
Muthén, 2017). The code needed to reproduce these analyses is available at https://osf.io/quhgm/?
view_only=3d9cab1e3d5345caad7e278091dd0aba. Sensitivity analyses are fully outlined in the supplementary
materials.
21
DYNAMICS OF PSYCHOPATHY
effects in socio-affective functioning, while they were well-powered to detect moderate to large
effects. Thus, the results suggest that if the affective-interpersonal components of psychopathy
serve as distinguishing features, they do not have a moderate or large impact on socio-affective
dynamics in daily life. Nonetheless, the results highlight potential avenues for future EMA
research on psychopathy.
Future work can enhance the assessment specificity of interpersonal situations. Although
exert their largest influence in specific interpersonal situations (e.g., significant dampening of
remorse- or guilt-related affect after harming another person). These effects may be difficult to
detect when collapsing across various negative interpersonal situations. These points are
underscored by past research using S1 data while focused on borderline personality disorder and
antisocial personality disorder traits. Scott and colleagues (2017) relied on theoretical
pathway between perceived rejection, negative affect, and aggressive urges and behavior) and
found evidence that BPD traits enhanced the link between perceived rejection and broad-based
negative affect, while this cross-level enhancement was not observed for antisocial personality
disorder traits, nor when perceived criticism was substituted for perceived rejection. Similarly,
Wright and colleagues (2017) found that narcissism amplified the link between perceptions of
other dominance and negative affect. Thus, the lack of empirical support for psychopathy traits
moderating the various within-person processes can be contrasted with past efforts that were
successful when focusing on BPD and narcissism, though the latter efforts were closely tied to
psychopathy dynamics may benefit from such enhanced designs, the present results also
22
DYNAMICS OF PSYCHOPATHY
highlight that common operationalizations of psychopathy pose more general statistical and
methodological hurdles.
opposing effects, consistent with past research on affective functioning in psychopathy (Nentjes
et al., 2022). For example, while Impulsive Antisociality was significantly positively related to
hostile affect in S1 and S2, Fearless-dominance was negatively related to hostility in both
samples. Though the PPI scales have not been used in EMA-based studies, the divergent findings
are in line past research showing that Impulsive Antisociality is related to a wide range of
negative outcomes while Fearless-dominance has been shown to largely overlap with traits
related to extraversion and low neuroticism, and in turn, is mostly unrelated to maladaptive
outcomes (Miller & Lynam, 2012). Researchers have argued that these opposing findings are to
be expected since Fearless-dominance and Boldness are meant to index the “mask” first
Yet, the opposing relations between psychopathy dimensions raise important practical
issues regarding the interpretation of psychopathy total scores for specific measures like the PPI-
come together to produce expected relations with relevant outcomes, whether it be curvilinear
relations (e.g., Blonigen, 2013) or trait-by-trait interactions (e.g., Lilienfeld et al., 2019).
However, evidence for these types of relations is limited (Benning & Smith, 2019; Crowe et al.,
6
Recent work has also suggested that in order to adequately test for trait-by-trait interactions in psychopathy, it will
require sample sizes much larger than typical sample sizes used in psychopathy research (Vize et al., 2022a).
23
DYNAMICS OF PSYCHOPATHY
psychopaths and examine how the types relate to external correlates (e.g., Hicks et al., 2004).
Together, these considerations suggest that future EMA-based studies on psychopathy and socio-
affective functioning may benefit from alternative measurement approaches. Relatedly, future
work could use more homogenous and narrow-band trait scales. Recent EMA research focused
on antagonism supports this approach, as variability was observed across different facets of
antagonism (e.g., callousness vs. manipulativeness) for affective and interpersonal outcomes
diversity in emotional experiences. However, results ran counter to our hypotheses and highlight
significant emotional heterogeneity worthy of further study. For example, for participants high in
psychopathy, positive affect was commonly reported and at various levels of intensity. In some
cases, individuals high in psychopathy reported very little negative affect while reporting high
levels of positive affect (Panel 2A, Figure 1). In others, the reported emotional experiences were
in line with typical descriptions of psychopathy (Panel 1A, Figure 1). Importantly, the
heterogeneity was not explained by factor profiles (i.e., higher standing on Fearless-dominance
relative to Impulsive Antisociality, and vice versa). It will be beneficial for future research to
psychopathy research has largely been focused on between-person data (i.e., nomothetic
research) while investigations of the individual variability within psychopathy is less common
(i.e., idiographic research). Insights from the large body of nomothetic research on socio-
strengths of nomothetic and idiographic traditions in future research (Wright & Woods, 2020).
24
DYNAMICS OF PSYCHOPATHY
Limitations
Our results should be interpreted with important limitations in mind. First, S1 was
composed of young, predominantly African American women of low socioeconomic status while
S2 was composed of middle-aged adults who were predominantly white and of medium to high
socioeconomic status. While these features of the samples allowed us to examine the
generalizability of the findings across various diversity dimensions, they differed from typical
samples used to study the socio-affective deficits involved in psychopathy (e.g., incarcerated
whereas measures like the PCL-R do not explicitly assess this content. Because Fearless-
certain that different results will be obtained if psychopathy measures that deemphasize
boldness-related content are used. Thus, future work will benefit from using alternative
operationalizations of psychopathy in EMA studies. Ideally, multiple measures can be used and
compared against one another to better understand the influence of psychopathy measurement on
Last, the EMA protocols in S1 and S2 were not explicitly designed to study socio-
affective processes tied to psychopathy. Future work would benefit from EMA protocols
example, as described above, interpersonal situations where callousness may be most relevant
can be explicitly assessed with EMA items written to capture those contextual features.
25
DYNAMICS OF PSYCHOPATHY
Relatedly, interpersonal situations where manipulation or dishonest behavior occurs would also
The present study offers important data regarding the role psychopathy plays in various
socio-affective processes that unfold in daily life. Though we observed primarily null findings,
the results highlight promising directions for future EMA-based psychopathy research, including
designing EMA studies focused on the interpersonal situations most relevant to psychopathy.
Thus far, psychopathy research has taken a nomothetic approach to understanding the disorder.
The present results point towards integrating the strengths of nomothetic and idiographic
echoing calls from other researchers (Verschuere et al., 2021), preregistration and other open
science practices have much to offer psychopathy research. Many of the factors that impact the
robustness of psychological findings are present in psychopathy research (e.g., small sample
sizes, flexible measurement and analytical frameworks, novel and eye-catching findings). These
methods are valuable tools for researchers working to further unravel the socio-affective
dynamics of psychopathy.
26
DYNAMICS OF PSYCHOPATHY
References
Benning, S. D., Patrick, C. J., Hicks, B. M., Blonigen, D. M., & Krueger, R. F. (2003). Factor
structure of the psychopathic personality inventory: validity and implications for clinical
Benning, S. D., & Smith, E. A. (2019). Forms, importance, and ineffability of factor interactions
Benning, S. D., & Smith, E. A. (2022). The Registration Continuum in Personality Disorder
Studies: Theory, Rationale, and Template. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and
Reconciling the dual roles of theory and clinical utility. Personality Disorders, 4(1), 87–88.
Buss, A.H., Durkee, A. (1957). An inventory for assessing different types of hostility. Journal of
Campos, C., Pasion, R., Azeredo, A., Ramião, E., Mazer, P., Macedo, I., & Barbosa, F. (2022).
Refining the link between psychopathy, antisocial behavior, and empathy: A meta-
94, 102145.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Professional manual: revised NEO personality inventory
Resources.
Crowe, M. L., Weiss, B. M., Sleep, C. E., Harris, A. M., Carter, N. T., Lynam, D. R., & Miller, J.
Garofalo, C., Neumann, C. S., Zeigler-hill, V., & Meloy, J. R. (2019). Spiteful and
Hicks, B. M., Markon, K. E., Patrick, C. J., Krueger, R. F., & Newman, J. P. (2004). Identifying
16(3), 276–288.
Hicks, B. M., & Patrick, C. J. (2006). Psychopathy and negative emotionality: Analyses of
suppressor effects reveal distinct relations with emotional distress, fearfulness, and anger-
103964.
Hoppenbrouwers, S. S., Bulten, B. H., & Brazil, I. A. (2016). Parsing fear: A reassessment of the
Horley, J. (2014). The emergence and development of psychopathy. History of the Human
Kamarck, T. W., Haskett, R. F., Muldoon, M., Flory, J. D., Anderson, B., Bies, R., Pollock, B.,
Keenan, K., Hipwell, A., Chung, T., Stepp, S., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., Loeber, R., & McTigue,
K. (2010). The Pittsburgh Girls Study: overview and initial findings. Journal of Clinical
Lilienfeld, S. O., & Hess, T. H. (2001). Psychopathic Personality Traits and Somatization: Sex
Lilienfeld, S. O., Patrick, C. J., Benning, S. D., Berg, J., Sellbom, M., & Edens, J. F. (2012). The
Lilienfeld, S. O., Watts, A. L., Murphy, B., Costello, T. H., Bowes, S. M., Smith, S. F., Latzman,
R. D., Haslam, N., & Tabb, K. (2019). Personality disorders as emergent interpersonal
33(5), 577–622.
29
DYNAMICS OF PSYCHOPATHY
Lykken, D. T. (1995). The antisocial personalities. Hillsdale.
Marcus, D. K., Fulton, J. J., & Edens, J. F. (2013). The two-factor model of psychopathic
4(1), 67–76.
Marsh, A. a., & Blair, R. J. R. (2008). Deficits in facial affect recognition among antisocial
McCord, W., & McCord, J. (1964). The psychopath: An essay on the criminal mind. Van
Nostrand.
Miller, J. D., & Lynam, D. R. (2015). Psychopathy and Personality: Advances and Debates.
Nentjes, L., Garofalo, C., & Kosson, D. S. (2022). Emotional functioning in psychopathy: A
critical review and integration with general emotion theories. In P. Marques, M. Paulino, &
L. Alho (Eds.), Psychopathy and Criminal Behavior: Current Trends and Challenges (pp.
Neumann, C. S., Hare, R. D., & Pardini, D. A. (2015). Antisociality and the Construct of
Psychopathy: Data From Across the Globe. Journal of Personality, 83(6), 678–692.
Patrick, C J, Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1993). Emotion in the criminal psychopath: startle
Patrick, Christopher J. (2022). Psychopathy: Current Knowledge and Future Directions. Annual
Patrick, Christopher J., Fowles, D. C., & Krueger, R. F. (2009). Triarchic conceptualization of
psychopathy and antisocial personality disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 28(8), 1472–
1484. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2008.09.004
Sadikaj, Wright, Dunkley, Zuroff, & Moskowitz. (2021). Multilevel Structural Equation
Modeling for Intensive Longitudinal Data: A Practical Guide for Personality Researchers.
In J. F. Rauthmann (Ed.), The Handbook of Personality Dynamics and Processes (pp. 855–
Scott, L. N., Wright, A. G. C., Beeney, J. E., Lazarus, S. A., Pilkonis, P. A., & Stepp, S. D.
Shiffman, S., Stone, A. A., & Hufford, M. R. (2008). Ecological momentary assessment. Annual
Sleep, C. E., Weiss, B. M., Lynam, D. R., & Miller, J. D. (2019). An Examination of the
Trull, T. J., & Ebner-Priemer, U. (2013). Ambulatory assessment. Annual Review of Clinical
Psychology, 9, 151–176.
Verona, E., Bresin, K., & Patrick, C. J. (2013). Revisiting psychopathy in women: Cleckley/hare
Verona, E., & Vitale, J. (2018). Psychopathy in women: Assessment, manifestations, and
Verschuere, B., Grothe, S. van G., Waldorp, L., Watts, A. L., Lilienfeld, S. O., Edens, J. F.,
Skeem, J. L., & Noordhof, A. (2018). What features of psychopathy might be central? A
Verschuere, B., Yasrebi-De Kom, F. M., van Zelm, I., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2021). A plea for
Vize, C. E., Baranger, D. A. A., Finsaas, M. C., Goldstein, B. L., Olino, T. M., & Lynam, D. R.
Vize, C. E., Lynam, D. R., Lamkin, J., Miller, J. D., & Pardini, D. (2016). Identifying essential
Vize, C. E., Ringwald, W. R., Edershile, E. A., & Wright, A. G. C. (2022). Antagonism in daily
Witt, E. A., Brent Donnellan, M., & Blonigen, D. M. (2009). Using existing self-report
Witt, E. A., Hopwood, C. J., Morey, L. C., Markowitz, J. C., McGlashan, T. H., Grilo, C. M.,
Sanislow, C. A., Shea, M. T., Skodol, A. E., Gunderson, J. G., & Donnellan, M. B. (2010).
32
DYNAMICS OF PSYCHOPATHY
Psychometric characteristics and clinical correlates of NEO-PI-R fearless dominance and
Wright, A.G.C., Stepp, S.D., Scott, L.N., Hallquist, M.N., Beeney, J.E., Lazarus, S., & Pilkonis,
P.A. (2017). The effect of pathological narcissism on interpersonal and affective processes
1467–1480.
33
DYNAMICS OF PSYCHOPATHY
Table 1
Table 2
Relations Between PPI Scales and Average Affect Ratings During EMA Protocols
Hostile Affect Sad Affect Positive Affect Remorseful Affect Anxious Affect
Estimate (SE) 95% CI Estimate (SE) 95% CI Estimate (SE) 95% CI Estimate (SE) 95% CI Estimate (SE) 95% CI
Sample 1
PPI-SF-IA .28** (.07) .14; .42 .14 (.08) -.02; .30 -.06 (.08) -.21; .09 .11 (.08) -.05; .27 -- --
†
PPI-SF-FD -.20* (.09) -.38; -.02 -.16 (.11) -.37; .05 .04 (.08) -.11; .19 -.20 (.10) -.41; .00 -- --
PPI-SF-Total .08 (.07) -.06; .22 -.03 (.09) -.21; .15 -.08 (.07) -.23; .06 -.09 (.08) -.24; .07 -- --
Sample 2
NEO-PPI-IA .21* (.10) .02; .40 .19* (.09) .02; .36 -.06 (.09) -.24; .11 -- -- .08 (.08) -.07; .24
NEO-PPI-FD -.10 (.08) -.26; .05 -.21* (.07) -.35; -.07 .13 (.08) -.03; .29 -- -- -.09 (.08) -.26; .07
NEO-PPI-Total .05 (.08) -.10; .21 -.05 (.08) -.20; .10 .07 (.09) -.09; .24 -- -- -.03 (.09) -.20; .15
Note: PPI-SF=Psychopathic Personality-Short Form; NEO-PPI=NEO-PI-R Proxy measure of the Psychopathic Personality Inventory; IA=Impulsive
Antisociality factor; FD= Fearless-dominance factor; All values are standardized estimates to facilitate comparisons across samples; †= p<.10; *=p<.05;
**=p<.01.
35
DYNAMICS OF PSYCHOPATHY
Table 3
Relations Between PPI Scales and Average Interpersonal Interaction Ratings During EMA Protocols
Negative Interaction Praise/Positive Feedback Agreeable Interaction Intimacy Interaction
Estimate (SE) 95% CI Estimate (SE) 95% CI Estimate (SE) 95% CI Estimate (SE) 95% CI
Sample 1
PPI-SF-IA .18** (.05) .09; .28 .08 (.08) -.07; .23 -- -- -- --
PPI-SF-FD -.07 (.08) -.22; .08 .15 (.10) -.04; .33 -- -- -- --
PPI-SF-Total .06 (.05) -.04; .17 .06 (.08) -.09; .22 -- -- -- --
Sample 2
NEO-PPI-IA .13 (.08) -.05; .30 .06 (.09) -.11; .23 -.16† (.09) -.33; .02 .09 (.09) -.07; .26
NEO-PPI-FD .04 (.08) -.12; .20 -.02 (.08) -.17; .14 .02 (.08) -.13; .16 -.05 (.08) -.21; .12
NEO-PPI-Total .13 (.08) -.02; .27 .02 (.08) -.13; .18 -.10 (.08) -.25; .06 .03 (.08) -.13; .18
Note: PPI-SF=Psychopathic Personality-Short Form; NEO-PPI=NEO-PI-R Proxy measure of the Psychopathic Personality
Inventory; IA=Impulsive Antisociality factor; FD= Fearless-dominance factor; All values are standardized estimates to facilitate
comparisons across samples; †= p<.10; **=p<.01.
36
DYNAMICS OF PSYCHOPATHY
Table 4
Between-person Heterogeneity in Affect Ratings for Select Participants High in Psychopathy in Samples 1 and 2
1A PPI Total=142; IA=70; FD=54 1B PPI Total=142; IA=61; FD=62 1C PPI Total=140; IA=54; FD=66
Neg. Emodiversity=.90 Neg. Emodiversity=.89 Neg. Emodiversity=.88
Pos. Emodiversity=.82 Pos. Emodiversity=.82 Pos. Emodiversity=.83
2A NEO−PPI Total=116; IA=52; FD=64 2B NEO−PPI Total=109; IA=67; FD=42 2C NEO−PPI Total=110; IA=68; FD=42
Neg. Emodiversity=.68 Neg. Emodiversity=.66 Neg. Emodiversity=.51
Pos. Emodiversity=.84 Pos. Emodiversity=.58 Pos. Emodiversity=.95