Key Words: Drawing Development, Art Education, Representation, Expression
Key Words: Drawing Development, Art Education, Representation, Expression
ABSTRACT
Although different approaches to teaching children to draw have been advocated and
practiced, little is known about how these may influence children’s developing drawing
abilities. In this study the drawings of pupils receiving an art education which attempts to
teach representational and expressive skills concurrently (mainstream schools in England) are
compared to those of pupils who experience an alternative art education which emphasizes
One hundred and sixty 7- to 16-year-old pupils from the two school types completed
three expressive (happy, sad and angry) drawings, two representational drawings (an
observational drawing of a mannequin and a drawing of a house from memory) and one free
drawing. Two artists rated all the drawings for quality on 7-point scales, and stylistic features
(scene-based, size and color) of the free drawings were assessed. No consistent between-
school differences were found in the expressive drawings but Waldorf pupils produced
produced larger and more scene-based free drawings. Waldorf pupils also combined colors
more frequently, and the 7- and 10-year-olds tended to use more colors than their Mainstream
school counterparts. These results appear inconsistent with the difference in emphasis on
expression and representation in the two school types. However, observational research
investigating actual classroom practices within the two school types is required, as maybe
1
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
REVISITED
Drawing is one of the most basic forms of art as most works of art will either include
the representation of an image on the paper or canvas, or will work from sketches of the
artwork (Hickman, 2010). Furthermore, children generally report that they enjoy drawing and
benefits of drawing are recognized by teachers, parents and children (Burkitt, Jolley & Rose,
2010). Nevertheless, some teachers are uncertain about how children should be taught to
draw and to develop their creativity and aesthetic understanding (Burkitt et al., 2010). There
is also uncertainty about the impact of different school art education programs on children’s
has tended to focus on two types of drawing communication, representational and expressive.
Representational drawings refer to those in which the child aims to depict subject matter from
expressive drawing aims to communicate mood, feelings, and ideas. This paper will assess
the expressive, representational and free drawings of children taught by two school art
programs, the English National Curriculum and the Waldorf Curriculum. These programs
have been chosen as these have a different emphasis on the teaching of representation and
expression.
real-world referents (for reviews, see Cox, 2005; Golomb, 2002; Jolley, 2010). In particular,
with increasing age children produce subject matter with more detail, with elements that
display more appropriate spatial alignment and proportion, showing dimensionality with
2
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
depth and occlusion and with evidence of perspective systems. Furthermore, theoretical
accounts have been proposed to illustrate and explain developmentally the different types of
drawings can be characterized (fortuitous, failed, intellectual and visual). For Luquet, young
children become representational drawers by noticing that their scribbled marks show some
representational drawings despite these displaying a number of motor, cognitive and graphic
the child draws what he or she considers to reflect the essential or criterion elements of the
topic, where each element is drawn in its generic and characteristic shape (intellectual
realism). These are symbolic representations that reflect the child’s internal mental model of
the subject matter. Luquet explained that in these drawings children use a number of graphic
strategies including separation of details, transparency, air-view plan and folding out
techniques, often leading to multi-perspective drawings. Finally, Luquet comments that older
children notice that their representations are not accurate depictions of how the subject matter
is seen in reality. Consequently, children begin then to grapple with the graphic techniques of
visual realism with the aim of drawing subject matter or the scene from a single visual
perspective (visual realism). In a more recent theory, Willats (1997, 2005) presented five age-
related representational drawing systems in which he unpacked how children shift from a
typological system (rudimentary intellectual realism) through various projective systems that
show increasing visual likeness to subject matter in reality from a single viewpoint.
Both artists and researchers have noted that children’s drawings not only represent
real-world referents but also express emotional and conceptual messages. Artists from varied
artistic traditions have admired, studied and even collected children’s drawings to gain
3
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
inspiration from the natural creativity and inventiveness they have perceived in the drawings,
particularly those produced by young children (see Fineberg, 1997, 1998; Golomb, 2002).
Researchers have noted three broad types of expressive techniques used by children: literal,
content and abstract (Ives, 1984; Jolley, 2010; Jolley, Fenn & Jones, 2004; Morra, Caloni &
d’Amico, 1994; Picard, Brechet & Baldy, 2007). Literal expression is most commonly
communicated through facial expressions, while content expression uses subject matter from
our world for expressive effect such as a children’s birthday party scene. Both literal and
content expression are facilitated by abstract expressive properties such as color, line and
composition. For instance, content drawn with bright colors, uplifting lines and a balanced
composition is likely to contribute to the positive affect of the topic or scene, or if presented
without representational content is considered abstract expression. Studies have assessed the
developing frequency with which children use the three techniques in prescribed drawing
tasks (see Ives, 1984; Picard & Gauthier, 2012; Winston¸ Kenyon, Stewardson & Lepine,
1995). An alternative approach has been to use quantitative rating scales to assess the quality
of expression in children’s drawings (Davis, 1997; Jolley et al., 2004; Pariser & Van den
Berg, 1997, 2001) where both U-shape and age-incremental patterns have been reported. A
number of commentators have suggested that the U-shape curve is dependent upon rating
criteria and judges that favor a modernist perspective on art that emphasize expressive formal
properties above representation (Duncum, 1986, Jolley, 2010; Pariser, Kindler & van den
Berg, 2008). Indeed, in a recent empirical study seeking to explain these apparently
conflicting patterns, Jolley, Barlow, Rotenberg & Cox (in press) found that age incremental
patterns of expressive drawing measures were observed without statistically controlling for
but patterns tending towards a U shape curve were found with the statistical control.
Drawing Education
4
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
Consideration of the Western history of teaching drawing and art reveals different
Although the teaching of drawing to children has a long history (see Carline, 1968), it was
not until the 19th century that drawing lessons were received widely among the Western child
population (Ashwin, 1981; see also Jolley, 2010). The aim at that time was to facilitate
commonly followed. In one pedagogical practice children developed a repertoire of lines and
shapes through prescriptively set geometric exercises with the goal of producing accurate
representations of three-dimensional models (e.g., the teachings of Walter Smith and Henry
Pestalozzi as described in Ashwin, 1981). Alternatively, drawings were made from direct
observation of nature where children were expected to use problem solving skills to develop
accurate representational drawings (Ruskin, 1857). In contrast, the first half of the 20th
century saw some Western art educators, such as Cizek (see Carline, 1968), Lowenfeld
(1939) and Richardson (1948), emphasize the importance of creative freedom for children in
their drawing, favoring drawing activities that fostered imagination and expression. For
example, Richardson encouraged children to create imaginative drawings based on their own
visualizations and imagined detail of an event that she verbally described to them.
that place a different emphasis on representation and expression, and in the timing of their
delivery. The National Curriculum for England is taught in Mainstream schools. For drawing,
this curriculum aims to support children between the ages of 5 to 14 years to develop
Education & Employment, 1999). The art curriculum is delivered by a general teacher (who
typically does not have specialist art training) to pupils up to the age of 11 – after this the
curriculum is delivered by a teacher who has received specialist art training (Ofsted, 2009). In
5
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
addition to the dedicated art lesson drawing may also be included in subject lessons, however
this is hard to quantify (Downing, 2004). At age 14 children are given a choice about whether
to continue studying the arts, including drawing, or not. Those who continue to study art take
an examination at age 16 which focuses on their ability to create works of art from
These schools teach according to the philosophies of Rudolf Steiner, who advocated a
curriculum in which creativity and imagination are emphasized (Carlgren, 2008). Art
activities are central to the Waldorf curriculum and between the ages of 7 and 14 years
drawing activities are included in almost all subject lessons (Jünemann & Weitmann, 1977)
However, few directions about what or how to draw are given, instead children are
encouraged to express their own ‘creative artistic ability’ (Nobel, 1997, p. 149).
Representational drawing skills are not introduced until pupils are 12 years old (Jünemann &
Weitmann, 1977). Worldwide there over 1000 Waldorf schools and within England there are
Only two studies have considered the drawing abilities of pupils attending
Mainstream and Waldorf schools in England. Cox and Rowlands (2002) investigated the
drawing abilities of 5- to 7-year-old pupils and concluded that Waldorf pupils produced
‘better’ representational, scene and free drawings. However, limitations in this study
compromised the generalizability of the findings. The drawings were rated by undergraduate
psychology students with no art training who were given no guidelines on how rate the free
and scene drawings. Therefore, it is unclear on what basis the drawings were judged, and
without explicit instruction it is unlikely that the expressiveness of the drawings was
considered. Furthermore, the children studied were only at the beginning of their formal
education. A more recent study published in this journal by Rose, Jolley and Charman (2012)
6
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
went some way towards addressing these limitations. They investigated the representational
and expressive drawing abilities of pupils aged 5 to 9 years from the two school types. The
drawings in this study were assessed by trained artists using previously developed rating
scales. Among the younger pupils the findings lacked consistency for between-school
drawing skills compared to their Mainstream counterparts, although there was no evidence of
their representational drawing skills being more advanced at this age. Nevertheless,
unanswered questions still remain as Rose et al. did not consider the drawing abilities of older
pupils. These are particularly relevant as children within Waldorf schools are not taught
representational drawing skills until approximately 12 years old (Jünemann & Weitmann,
1977). Furthermore, drawings free from specific task instructions were not collected. Such
drawings may reflect more closely children’s self-directed drawings and therefore may
provide additional insight into their developing drawing ability. Finally, although Cox and
Rowlands and Rose et al. anecdotally commented that stylistically the drawings of Waldorf
pupils tended to reflect the art values emphasized within this pedagogy (larger, more colorful
Building upon this early ground-breaking work the aim of the current study was to
schools, as well as drawing ability and stylistic differences in the free drawings. To address
the limited age range of samples in the earlier work the current study included children aged
7 to 16 years. Therefore, the current study aimed to corroborate and extend findings from
previous studies. The need for replication has recently been argued for by Makel (2014) and
is especially relevant to emerging topics where the body of evidence is, as yet, small.
Accordingly, our procedure for the expressive and representational drawing tasks followed
7
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
that reported by Rose et al. (2012). In addition, artist raters assessed the free drawings for
overall artistic merit and stylistic features of use of color, size and the extent to which a
Based on the greater emphasis on expression in the Waldorf curricula, and the
superior expressive drawings of Waldorf pupils reported by Rose et al. (2012), it was
anticipated that the Waldorf School pupils throughout all age groups tested would show more
representational drawing features throughout the English National Curriculum but not until
around 12 years of age in the Waldorf curriculum, suggesting that Mainstream pupils might
have superior representational drawing ability at least up until 12 years of age. However,
previous research has indicated that younger Waldorf school children produce
representational drawings of at least the same quality as their Mainstream school counterparts
(Cox & Rowlands, 2002; Rose et al., 2012). Taking both curricular difference and empirical
research into account, we therefore tentatively predicted that there would be few differences
in representational drawing ability of the pupils younger than 12 years between the two
school types, but that adolescent Waldorf school pupils would display more advanced
representational drawing skills. For the free drawings it was predicted the Waldorf pupils
would produce drawings of higher artistic merit due to the greater emphasis on the arts in the
Waldorf curriculum. Furthermore, based on reports from Cox and Rowlands and Rose et al.,
it was expected that Waldorf school pupils would producing more scene-based drawings, use
a greater number of colors, combine colors more frequently and cover more of the page with
Method
Participants
8
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
Participants were 160 pupils from four age groups: Mean ages of the four groups were
7 years 3 months (SD = 3 months), 10 years 0 months (SD = 4 months), 13 years 11 months
(SD = 5 months) and 15years 11 months (SD = 5 months). In what follows, these age groups
are labelled as 7-, 10-, 14- and 16-year-olds, respectively. Forty children participated in each
age group, consisting of 20 children from each school type (Mainstream, Waldorf) with an
equal number of boys and girls. Teachers were instructed to select children who were
representative of their classes’ drawing ability rather than just those who were especially
good at drawing. For the youngest age group The British Picture Vocabulary Scale (Dunn,
Dunn, Whetton & Burley, 1997) was administered to check that all children could be
expected to understand the task instructions. All these children had average, or above average
vocabulary comprehension, so none were excluded. Five mainstream and four Waldorf
schools were involved in the research. To reduce sampling bias participants were recruited
for each age group and each school type from at least two schools. All the schools were from
areas predominantly classified as ‘urban prosperity’ and ‘comfortably off’ according to the
internet geodemographic tool ACORN (2010). This tool categorizes each postcode in the UK
into one of five main sociodemographic types based on data from the UK census data and
extensive lifestyle surveys. The current statutory inspection report for each school was
consulted, and although these make no reference to art teaching specifically, only schools
where the teaching was rated as satisfactory or good were chosen. Written consent was
gained from parents of each pupil and pupils gave verbal consent to participate. Ethical
approval was given by the university in which this project was conducted, and APA ethical
guidelines followed.
Materials
For each drawing every child was provided with a sheet of A4 paper, seven easy grip
colored pencils (red, green, blue, yellow, pink, brown and black) and an HB pencil. For the
9
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
observational drawing artists’ wooden mannequins with added facial features (eyes, mouth
and nose) were used. One was set up in a running position (facing the right) on the table in
Procedure
The BPVS was administered individually to all participants in the youngest age group prior to
the drawing tasks. All participants, in small groups, completed the six drawing tasks in three
sessions to avoid fatigue. The order of the sessions and tasks within each were
counterbalanced. For both expressive and representational drawing tasks the instructions
followed those reported by Rose et al. (2012). In the expressive drawing session the children
were asked to draw a happy, sad and angry picture, but with no further stipulation on the
content as ‘it can be of anything that you want’. In the representational drawing session the
towards the right. Children were asked to draw exactly what they saw and the researcher
emphasized the direction that the man was running in by saying ‘draw the man running that
way’ and pointing to the child’s right. Children were also asked to draw a real and life-like
house from memory. For the free drawing session, children were asked to draw anything,
‘whatever they wanted’. The instructions for each specific drawing task were followed by
general directions that they had 10 minutes to complete each drawing, that they should use
the paper and pencils in front of them, to try and make it their best drawing and that the
drawing should be their own and not to copy anyone else’s drawing. To reduce further the
risk of pupils copying or collaborating the children were seated apart from one another, and
talking was discouraged. In most schools these sessions took place over two consecutive
Some pupils also took part in a short semi-structured interview during the free
drawing session, these participants completed this session individually. They were provided
10
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
with the additional instruction that they should tell the researcher what they planned to draw,
before they started to draw. Furthermore, once they had declared the drawing finished, or the
ten minutes time allowed was up, they were asked to tell the researcher about their drawing.
They researcher used reflective comments (e.g. ‘what a lot of colors’, ‘that is big/small’,
‘really!’) and nudging prompts (e.g. ‘what about this part here’, ‘is there anything else that
you can tell me?’) to encourage as full a description as possible. This data is not reported
within this manuscript as it is a rich and full qualitative data set that addresses a somewhat
different research question, that of children’s creative intentions, to the focus of the present
study.
The expressive drawings were rated on the expressive use of color, composition, line,
and overall quality of expression on separate 7-point scales for each measure. These scales
had been developed previously by the two artists and used by Rose et al. (2012). Drawings
assigned to the lowest points on each of the scales were deemed to show an inappropriate
mood (pt. 1) or neutral mood (pt. 2) instead of the intended mood. Drawings displaying
minimal but nevertheless noticeable evidence of the appropriate mood were scored on the
next point (pt. 3) on the scale. Higher points on the scales were characterized by drawings
showing clearly the appropriate mood with improving consistency of expression throughout
the drawing, with increasing creative expressive ideas, and with a striking (intensifying)
expressive element in the drawing. A summary of the overall quality of expression scale can
For the present study the artists who had originally been involved in the construction
of these scales for rating expressive drawings were used to rate all the drawings. For the
expressive drawings they were re-familiarized in the use of the criteria for each of the four
expressive scales through joint discussion of a sample set of drawings from previous studies.
11
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
The artists then independently rated all 540 expressive drawings. The artists were blind to the
age and school of the child. Intraclass correlations (ICCs(a, k)) were used and were above 0.77
for all measures and for all moods, which is a level of interrater agreement that is at least
satisfactory (Nuendorf, 2002). For the statistical analysis the mean of the two raters scores for
The representational drawings of the mannequin running and the house were rated on
a 7-point Likert type scale for representational realism. The artist raters were provided with
guidelines, instructing them to use the whole scale and asking them to consider specific
elements of the drawing and the level of representational skill shown for each. The guidelines
for the mannequin drawings were based Cox, Perara and Xu’s (1999) rating scale and
included a photograph of the man set up in the running positon, from the view point seen by
the child. They were asked to rate each drawing taking into consideration the following
features: whether all parts of the man were depicted, whether limbs were depicted as single
lines or zone, the direction the man was facing, the overlap of limbs, the proportion in
comparison to the model, the detail of the facial features and the extent to which movement
was portrayed. For the house drawings raters were given guidelines based on Barrouillet,
Fayol & Chevrot (1994) rating scale, these asked raters to consider how visually realistic the
depictions of the house, roof, door and windows were, the extent to which perspective was
realistic, the amount of realistic detail included and whether the house was placed in realistic
surroundings. A copy of all the guidelines given to the raters is included in the online
supplementary materials. The house and mannequin drawings were rated by the same artist
raters as the expressive drawings. The interrater agreement between their ratings was high,
The free drawings were assessed on a 7-point scale for their overall artistic merit by
the same artist raters. These raters were instructed that a score of 7 should be given to those
12
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
drawings which they believed showed the greatest artistic merit and a score of 1 to those
which they felt showed the least. Intraclass correlations demonstrated high interrater
reliability, ICC(a, k) =.828. The more stylistic aspects of the drawings were assessed in four
ways. First, the extent to which a scene compared to a single object had been depicted (7-
point scale: 1 = single object to 7 = detailed and extensive scene). Second, the size of the
drawing (height x width) was measured. Third, the number of colors used was counted, and
fourth, the number of times colors were combined to create secondary or tertiary colors. For
scene/object, size and number of colors the interater reliability was very good, ICCs(a, k)
> .92. Furthermore, good interrater reliability was also found for the number of times colors
Results
Multi factorial ANOVAs were conducted to examine the between school differences
within the four age groups. To keep the reporting of these results brief higher order
interactions involving between school differences and main effects of school are focused on.
Higher order interactions were followed up with appropriate simple effects and post hoc
pairwise contrasts were carried out using Tukey’s tests to identify significant differences
between schools. For each level of tests done, on each data set, a Bonferroni adjustment to
alpha was made when interpreting whether the simple effects and post-hoc contrasts were
statistically significant. The alpha level was adjusted according to the number of tests done at
Example expressive drawings created by Mainstream and Waldorf school pupils from
the four age groups are shown in Figure 1. These example drawings all depict a happy mood
and were selected to represent the mean quality rating for each age group from each school
type. Additionally a graph showing the mean overall quality of expression scores by age
13
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
group and school type is presented in Figure 2. From this it appears that there are no large or
consistent between school differences in the quality of expression scores. Three-way mixed
ANOVAs were conducted to examine the effects of school (2), age (4) and mood (3) on
scores for the overall quality of expression and the use of line, color and composition to
express the prescribed mood. For overall quality of expression the main effect of school and
interactions involving school are summarized in Table 1, none of these were significant (ps
>.139).
For expressive use of line the results are summarized in Table 2. The main effect of
school was marginally significant as pupils from Mainstream schools (M = 3.89, SD = 0.84)
used line more expressively than Waldorf pupils (M = 3.64, SD = 1.00). However, the three-
and two way interactions were not significant (ps > .283).
For expressive use of color results are summarized in Table 3. There was no
statistically significant main effect of school, nor any significant two-way interactions (ps
> .172). However, a significant three-way interaction between mood, age and school was
detected. Investigation of this indicated that, when mood was held constant, significant
between-school differences were identifiable in drawings depicting a happy mood but not in
those expressing an angry or sad mood. Further analysis this indicated that at age 14, Waldorf
pupils (M = 4.82, SD = 1.28) used color more expressively in their happy drawings compared
to their Mainstream school counterparts (M = 3.72, SD = 1.37), F(1, 38) = 6.88, p = .013, η2
=.15. Furthermore, at age 7 there was some indication (not significant at the adjusted alpha
level but p<.05) that Waldorf pupils (M = 3.58, SD = 0.94) may use color more expressively
14
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
For expressive use of composition results are summarized in Table 4. For composition
there was no significant main effect of school or two-way interaction between mood and
school. However, there was a significant two-way interaction between age and school and
4.12, SD = 0.60) school pupils used composition more expressively than Waldorf pupils (M =
3.62, SD = 0.60), F(1, 38) = 6.67, p =.014, η2 =.15. Further insight into this was gained
through investigating the significant three-way interaction between mood, age and school.
This indicated that the between school difference at age 10 was only present in drawings
depicting an angry mood F(1, 38) = 18.28, p < .001, η2 =.33, with Mainstream pupils (M =
4.77, SD = 0.82) using composition more expressively in their angry drawings than their
Representational Drawing
from the four age groups are shown in Figure 3. These examples are the pupils’ drawings of
the artists mannequin set up in a running position, and each drawing was selected to represent
the mean rating for each age group from each school type. Additionally a graph showing the
mean representational drawing scores by age group and school type is presented in Figure 4.
It appears from this that the drawings of Waldorf school pupils are consistently scoring more
highly, especially in the older age groups. A two-way between-group ANOVA was
conducted to examine the effects of school (2) and age (4) on the representation scores (7-
point scale) for the mannequin drawings. The main effect for school was significant F(1, 152)
= 31.74, p <.001, η2 =.18, indicating that Waldorf pupils (M = 3.90, SD = 1.68) had drawn
(M = 3.01, SD = 1.57). The two-way interaction between age and school was not statistically
15
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
significant F(3, 152) = 1.24, p = .297, η2 =.02. A similar pattern of findings were found for
school and age effects on ratings (7-point scale) of the house drawings. The main effect of
school was significant and indicated that the houses drawn by Waldorf pupils (M = 4.02, SD
= 1.58) had been rated as more realistic than those drawn by their Mainstream school peers
(M = 3.52, SD = 1.06), F(1, 150) = 36.89, p = .003, η2 =.43. The two-way interaction between
school and age was not statistically significant F(3, 150) = 1.94, p = .125, η2 =.04.
The free drawings were analyzed on overall artistic merit, as well as stylistic measures
of number of colors, occurrences of color combing, size of drawing, and the extent to which a
scene was depicted. Example free drawings created by pupils from the two school types and
from the four age groups are shown in Figure 5. These example drawings were selected as
they represent the stylistic differences identified and the mean rating for artistic merit for
each age group from each school type. A two-way between group ANOVA was conducted to
examine the effects of school (2) and age (4) on the 7-point scale assessing artistic merit. The
main effect for school was very close to significant, F(1, 150) = 3.78, p = .054, η2 =.03,
suggesting that Steiner pupils’ (M = 3.72, SD = 1.40) free drawings were rated more highly
for artistic merit compared those of their National Curriculum counterparts (M = 3.31, SD =
1.34). The two-way interaction between age and school was also found to be very close to
statistical significance, F(3, 151) = 2.55, p = .058, η2 =.05 with follow up analysis indicating
that at age 7 Waldorf pupils (M = 3.63, SD = 1.55) produced drawings which were
significantly more highly rated than their Mainstream school peers (M = 2.44, SD = 0.97),
A two-way between group ANOVA investigating the association between school type
(2), age (4) and the number of colors used indicated that there was no significant main effect
16
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
of school , F(1, 150) = 2.24, p = .137, η2 = .02. However, the two-way interaction was very
close to significant, F(3, 150) = 2.63, p = .052, η2 = .05. Further analysis demonstrated that at
age 7, F(1, 38) = 4.08, p = .050, η2 = .10, and 10, F(1, 38) = 5.41, p = .025, η2 = .13, Waldorf
pupils (age 7: M = 4.70, SD = 1.63; age 10: M = 3.7, SD = 1.61), used more colors in their
drawings compared to their Mainstream school counter parts (age 7: M = 3.74, SD = 1.63;
age 10: M = 2.4, SD = 1.67). A Chi squared test indicated that across all age groups colors
were combined more frequently by those pupils attending Waldorf compared to Mainstream
A two-way between group ANOVA was carried to investigate the size (cm2) of the
free drawings produced by Mainstream and Waldorf pupils from the four different age
groups. The main effect of school was statistically significant indicating that Waldorf pupils’
drawings covered more of the page (M = 48.11, SD = 16.63) than those of their Mainstream
school counterparts (M = 37.09, SD = 19.57), F(1, 151) = 14.98, p < .001, η2 =.09. The two-
way interaction was not statistically significant, F(3, 151) = 2.25, p = .085, η2 =.04. The
findings for the extent to which drawings depicted a scene compared to a single object
followed a similar pattern to those found for the size of the drawing. There was a significant
main effect of school, F(1, 151) = 10.88, p = .001, η2 =.07, indicating that Waldorf pupils (M
their Mainstream peers (M = 2.77, SD = 1.58). The two-way interaction from the ANOVA
Discussion
The aim of this study was to compare drawing development in Mainstream and
Waldorf school pupils. In expressive drawings very few between school differences were
identified in overall quality of expression and the use of line, composition and color for
17
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
expressive purposes. This is reflected in the many non-significant findings in the analysis of
this data and also the small effect sizes found (generally <.01). In contrast, when the
representational drawing data was analyzed significant between school differences with effect
sizes exceeding large (>.18) were identified (see Cohen, 1988, for effect size guidelines).
These consistently indicated that Waldorf pupils, across all age groups, tended to produce
more visually realistic drawings than their Mainstream school counterparts. For the free
drawings, there was some indication that Waldorf School pupils produced drawings which
were rated more highly for artistic merit, but this was only significantly at age 7 with an
effect size approaching large (η2 =.17). Furthermore, wide-ranging and consistent stylistic
differences were found as Waldorf School pupils produced drawings which were more likely
to be scene based, cover more of the page, included more colors (at age 7 & 10) and with
That there were more similarities than differences in the expressive drawing measures
appear contrary to previous research by Rose et al. (2012) and also surprising given the
widespread emphasis on expression in the Waldorf Curriculum. It seems unlikely that the
inconsistency with Rose et al.’s findings are due to any differences in the administration of
tasks or their assessment as these were the same in the two studies. However, through
comprehension vocabulary of the youngest children the sampling strategy in the current study
was improved. Therefore the similar quality of expressive drawing may have been the result
of better matching of pupils across the two school types. Accordingly, contrary to previous
research and the differences in the curricula we are cautious of whether there is an expressive
advantage shown by Waldorf pupils. A potential explanation for this could be that classroom
practices in the two school types may actually be more similar than the curricula suggest.
Observational research of actual classroom practices in the two school types could provide
18
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
insight into this. A further factor that may contribute to the similarities found in expressive
drawing ability is the pupils’ wider shared culture, including picture books and images in the
media. Through these they will be exposed to similar repertoire of graphical symbols and
formal properties being used both literally and metaphorically to express emotion.
The superior representational drawing ability of Waldorf School pupils found in the
current study extend the findings of Cox and Rowlands (2000) to an older age group. In
addition they present a more stable picture than those of Rose et al. (2012) who suggested
that at age 5 years Mainstream pupils had superior skill, at age 7 Waldorf pupils had superior
skill and at age 9 there were no differences in their representational drawing skill. Therefore,
it seems that the most consistent finding, based on the available evidence, is that Waldorf
pupils appear to have representational drawing skills which are at least as good as, if not
indeed better, than those of their Mainstream school counterparts. Considering that
representational drawing is covered in the curriculum for even the youngest Mainstream
school pupils but not introduced until pre-adolescence in the Waldorf curriculum it is rather
surprising that the younger Waldorf school pupils seem to have better representational
drawing ability. It might be that other features of the Waldorf curriculum, such as the
considerable amount of time available for drawing, may support the development of
representational drawing skills from a young age. Furthermore, the differing approaches
advocated in the curricula may not be reflected in actual classroom practices. For example, in
Mainstream classroom pupils may not be receiving specific instruction on how to create
representational drawings and in Waldorf classrooms teachers may provide more support to
expressive drawing, observations of teaching practices in the two schools are needed.
The free drawings were analyzed for artistic merit as well as stylistic differences. The
analysis of the free drawings supports the previous findings of Cox and Rowlands (2000) as
19
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
they found that young Waldorf pupils produced free drawings which were rated as ‘better’
than those of their mainstream school counterparts. As the drawings in the current study were
rated by artists, rather than undergraduate students without specialist training in art,
confidence in the validity, as well as reliability, of this finding is increased. Furthermore, the
findings provide empirical evidence to support the anecdotal comments made by Cox and
Rowlands and Rose et al. about Waldorf pupils producing larger, more colorful scene-based
drawings in comparison to their Mainstream school counterparts. These findings also seem to
reflect differences in the two curricula. For instance, the larger size of the drawings and the
tendency to make more scene-based depictions appear to reflect art values expressed by
Wider characteristics of the school education received may also contribute to the
tendency of the Waldorf pupils to depict larger, colorful, scene based drawings. Rudolf
Steiner was influenced by Goethe (Woods, Ashley & Woods, 2005) and this is evident within
the Waldorf curriculum, particularly in the emphasis on color (based on Goethe’s Theory of
Colors) and also the emphasis on viewing things as a whole, within their context (Steiner,
1985). These characteristics of the Waldorf curriculum may account for the tendency for
Waldorf pupils to use a greater number of colors and also to depict scenes rather than single
objects in their free drawings. Consulting the Exemplar Schemes of Work (QCA, 2000)
provided to help Mainstream school teachers plan art lessons suggest that many of the lessons
might be topic based. If Mainstream pupils are often required to produce drawings of a
single topic this further supports our finding of fewer scene-based pictures in the Mainstream
school pupils’ free drawing. Furthermore, any pedagogic bias to topic-based drawings in the
that they would like the children to draw. Bremner and Moore (1984) reported that when
objects are named as part of the task instruction children tend to draw intellectually realistic,
20
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
rather than visually realistic forms. They argued that the verbal label cues the child’s
described, leading children to draw in an intellectual realistic style rather than depicting the
features of the object that they actually see in front of them. Other factors may also account
for the between school differences found, for example it has been found that children with
better perspective taking skills also have better spatial drawing ability (Ebersbach, Stiehler &
projective space around an object rather than the design features of the object (Lange-Kuttner
& Ebersbach, 2013) may also predispose them to draw more scene based drawings compared
to single objects. Furthermore, links between children’s developing concept of space and
their ability to draw visually realistically have been well documented (for a review see
Milbrath, McPherson, & Osborne, 2015). Consequently future research investigating these
perspective taking skills in pupils attending the two school types might provide evidence that
The focus of our paper was to examine art curricular effects on children’s drawings in
Waldorf and Mainstream schools. Nevertheless, there are other school art resources beyond
the art lesson that are likely to have an impact on children’s drawing development. The use of
drawing in other subject lessons, extracurricular art activities, and the amount of time and art
material resources are all likely to shape graphic skills and their development. The value
Waldorf schools place upon fostering children’s creativity and the pivotal role of art activities
throughout the curriculum (Carlgren, 2008) is in contrast to the Mainstream schools where
the arts have long since been the poor relation to the teaching of ‘academic’ subjects (e.g. see
Downing, 2004). This suggests that any Waldorf advantage in drawing needs to be seen in
this wider context of the respective art cultures in the schools. Nevertheless, considering the
21
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
that the school differences in drawing performance in our data were not more pronounced and
consistent. The broadly similar performance across the expressive drawing measures
between the school types is particularly interesting in this respect. It reminds us that
universally children appear motivated to draw across cultures (e.g., see Golomb, 2002;
Lindström, 2000) and that maybe drawing will develop irrespective of the specifics of art
curricula experienced.
In addition to considering school based differences and similarities that may influence
children’s drawing abilities wider influences must also be considered. This is particularly
students to experimental groups (school types) was not possible. Consequently it is possible
that some pre-existing differences between the pupils in these two school types might account
for the differences found in representational drawing and drawing style. For instance, parents
who value the arts more highly may choose to send their child to a Waldorf school as these
schools emphasize art and creativity throughout their curriculum. Indeed, both Rose et al.
(2012) and Cox and Rowlands (2000) speculated that Waldorf parents might give their
children additional support with drawing and that this might, at least in part, account for the
differences in drawing ability where they have been found between Waldorf and mainstream
school pupils. However, Rose (2014) reports from a survey of 80 parents of Mainstream and
Waldorf school pupils that the parents of Waldorf school pupils actually spend significantly
less time sitting with their child while they draw. Furthermore, when these parents were
asked about the type of help they offered to their children while they drew a third of Waldorf
parents expressed the view that you shouldn’t help or interfere while a child is drawing
who did offer help, 65% offered general encouragement, with many fewer offering ideas of
what to draw (3%) or advice about technical skills (5%). This is in contrast to the views
22
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
expressed by the Mainstream school pupils’ parents who reported giving ideas of what to
draw (12%) and advice on technical skills (24%) more frequently. Therefore it seems
unlikely that Waldorf pupils receive more support for drawing at home and consequently this
in expressive drawing abilities between pupils attending Mainstream and Waldorf schools do
not seem to reflect the differing emphasis on these drawing skills in the two curricula. To
gain further insight into the influence of teaching practices and school environment on
research would be particularly informative if drawings from the children of the observed
classes were also collected and compared between the two school types. This could provide
evidence of the direct effect that school differences and similarities might have on pupils’
23
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
References
Acorn (2010). Acorn: User guide. London: CACI.
Ashwin, C., (1981). Drawing and education in German speaking Europe 1800-1900. Ann
Barrouillet, P., Fayol, M., & Chevrot, C. (1994). Le dessin d'une maison. Construction d'une
Bremner, J. G., & Moore, S. (1984). Prior visual inspection and object naming: two factors
that enhance hidden feature inclusion in young children’s drawings. British Journal of
Bund der Freien Waldorfschulen, (2015). Waldorf World List. Retrieved from
https://www.freunde-waldorf.de/fileadmin/user_upload/images/Waldorf_World_List/
Waldorf_World_List.pdf
Burkitt, E., Jolley, R., & Rose, S. (2010). The attitudes and practices that shape children’s
drawing experience at home and at school. International Journal of Art and Design
Carlgren, F. (2008). Education towards freedom (3rd edition). Edinburgh, UK; Floris Books.
Carline, R. (1968). Draw they must: A history of the teaching and examining of art. London:
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale,
Cox, M. V. (2005). The pictorial world of the child. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
Press.
Cox, M. V., Perara, J., & Xu, F. (1998). Children’s drawing ability in the UK and China.
24
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
Cox, M. V. & Rowlands, A. (2000). The effect of three different educational approaches on
Davis, J, H (1997). The what and whether of the U: cultural implications of understanding
10.1159/000278717
Department for Education and Employment, (1999). Art and design: the National Curriculum
Downing, D. (2004). Saving a place for the arts? A survey of the arts in primary schools in
Duncum, P. (1986). Breaking down the alleged “U” curve of artistic development. Visual
Dunn, L.M., Dunn, L.M., Whetton, C., & Burley, J. (1997). The British picture vocabulary
Ebersbach, M., Stiehler, S., & Asmus, P. (2011). On the relationship between children’s
perspective taking in complex scenes and their spatial drawing ability. British Journal
Evans, J., Birkin, G., & Bowes, L. (2013). Arts subjects at key stage 4: A review of current
review-of-current-gcse-provision
Fineberg, J (1997). The innocent eye: Children’s art and the modern artist. Princeton, NJ:
Fineberg, J. (1998). Discovering child art: Essays on childhood, primitivism and modernism.
25
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
Hickman, R. (2010). Why We Make Art - and why it is taught (2nd Ed.). Bristol, UK:
Intellect.
Jolley, R. P. (2010). Children & pictures: drawing and understanding. Chichester, UK:
Wiley-Blackwell.
Jolley, R.P., Barlow, C.M., Cox, M.V., & Rotenberg, K.J. (in press). Linear and U-shape
trends in the development of expressive drawing from pre-schoolers to adult artists.
Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts.
Jolley, R.P., Fenn, K., & Jones, L. (2004). The development of children's expressive drawing.
British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 22, 545-567,
doi.org/10.1348/0261510042378236
Jünemann, M., & Weitmann, F., (1977). Drawing & painting in Rudolf Steiner schools.
Lange-Küttner, C., & Ebersbach, M. (2013). Girls in detail, boys in shape: gender differences
when drawing cubes in depth. British Journal of Psychology, 104, 413–37.
doi:10.1111/bjop.12010
Lindström, L. (2000). The cultural context: comparative studies of art education and
children's drawings. Stockholm: Stockholm Institute of Education Press.
Luquet, G. H. (1927). Le Dessin Enfantin [The drawings of a child]. Paris: Alcan.
Luquet, G. H. (2001). Children’s drawings. (A. Costell, Trans.) London: Free Association
Books (Original work published 1927).
Lowenfeld, M. (1939). The nature of creative activity. New York: Macmillan
26
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
Developmental Science Volume Two: Cognitive Processes (7th ed.). Hoboken, New
Morra, S., Caloni, B., & D’Amico, M. R. (1994). Working memory and the intentional
Nobel, A. (1997). Educating through art: The Steiner school approach. Edinburgh, UK:
Floris Books.
Nicol, J. & Taplin, J. (2012). Understanding the Steiner Waldorf approach: Early years
Ofstead (2009). Drawing together, art, craft and design in schools. Manchester, UK: Ofstead.
Pariser, D.A., Kindler, A.M. & van den Berg, A. (2008). Drawing and aesthetic judgments
H.M. (eds.), Children’s understanding and production of pictures, drawings and art:
Theoretical and empirical approaches (pp. 293-317). USA: Hogrefe & Huber.
Pariser, D.A., & Van Den Berg, A. (1997). The mind of the beholder: some provisional
doubts about the U-curve aesthetic development thesis. Studies in Art Education, 38,
158-178, doi.org/10.2307/1320291
Pariser, P. D., & Van Den Berg, A. (2001). Teaching art versus teaching taste, what art
teachers can learn from looking at cross-cultural evaluation of children’s art. Poetics,
29, 331-350, doi.org/10.1016/s0304-422x(01)00032-8
Picard, D., Brechet, C., & Baldy, R. (2007). Expressive strategies in drawing are related to
007-0035-5.
Richardson, M. (1948). Art and the child. London: London University Press.
Rose, S. E. (2014). Development of drawing ability and the attitudes and practices towards
27
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
Rose, S. E., Jolley, R. P., & Charman, A. (2012). An investigation of the expressive and
Montessori schools. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6, 83–95. doi.
10.1037/a0024460
Ruskin, J. (1857). The elements of drawing. London: Smith, Elder & Co.
Winston, A. S., Kenyon, B., Stewardson, J., & Lepine, T. (1995). Children’s sensitivity to
Woods, P., Ashley, M. & Woods, G. (2005). Steiner Schools in England. Bristol, UK:
University of the West of England, Centre for Research in Education and Democracy.
QCA (2000). Schemes of work: Art and design at key stages 1 and 2. Retrieved from
http://webarchive.nationalarchives.gov.uk/20090608182316/http://standards.dfes.gov.u
k/schemes2/art/?view=get
28
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
Figure Captions
Figure 1. Expressive drawings depicting a happy mood by children of all four age groups
Figure 2. Mean quality of expression scores (with Standard Errors shown) for each mood and
Figure 3. Representational drawings depicting the artist’s mannequin by children of all four
Figure 4. Mean representational drawing scores (with Standard Errors shown) for mannequin
and house drawings for each age group from Mainstream (M) and Waldorf (W)
schools.
Figure 5. Free drawings by children of all four age groups from Mainstream and Waldorf
schools.
29
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
Table 1
Main effect of school and interactions involving school for overall quality of expression
Table 2
Main effect of school and interactions involving school for use of line
Table 3
Main effect of school and interactions involving school for use of color
30
DRAWING DEVELOPMENT IN TWO EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
Table 4
Main effect of school and interactions involving school for use of composition
31