Hippocampus and MEMORY
Hippocampus and MEMORY
Author manuscript
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Author Manuscript
2Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Center, Toronto, Ontario, M6A 2E1 Canada
3Department of Psychology, Baycrest Center, Toronto, Ontario M6A 2E1, Canada
4Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina 27708
5Department of Psychology, Trent University, Peterborough, Ontario K9J 7B8, Canada
6Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario M5T 1R8, Canada
7Department of Psychology and Cognitive Science Program, University of Arizona, Tucson,
Arizona 85721
Abstract
The last decade has seen dramatic technological and conceptual changes in research on episodic
Author Manuscript
memory and the brain. New technologies, and increased use of more naturalistic observations,
have enabled investigators to delve deeply into the structures that mediate episodic memory,
particularly the hippocampus, and to track functional and structural interactions among brain
regions that support it. Conceptually, episodic memory is increasingly being viewed as subject to
lifelong transformations that are reflected in the neural substrates that mediate it. In keeping with
this dynamic perspective, research on episodic memory (and the hippocampus) has infiltrated
domains, from perception to language and from empathy to problem solving, that were once
considered outside its boundaries. Using the component process model as a framework, and
focusing on the hippocampus, its subfields, and specialization along its longitudinal axis, along
with its interaction with other brain regions, we consider these new developments and their
implications for the organization of episodic memory and its contribution to functions in other
domains.
Author Manuscript
Keywords
episodic memory; frontal cortex; hippocampus; parietal cortex; schema; transformation
DISCLOSURE STATEMENT
The authors are not aware of any affiliations, memberships, funding, or financial holdings that might be perceived as affecting the
objectivity of this review.
Moscovitch et al. Page 2
INTRODUCTION
Author Manuscript
The concept of episodic memory, according to Tulving (1983, 2002), refers to a declarative
memory that contains information specific to the time and place of acquisition (what
laypeople may call an autobiographical episode), as distinguished from semantic memory,
which is concerned with knowledge not tied to its context of acquisition. Recollection, a
process that enables one to relive episodes, is associated with autonoetic consciousness, a
subjective sense of time and of the self as the one who experienced the episode and
possesses the memory. By contrast, the process of knowing enables one to recognize an
event or stimulus as familiar without locating it in time and place. Familiarity-based memory
is associated with noetic consciousness, or at least with a much reduced autonoetic
consciousness. Because these events and processes are what researchers studying episodic
memory mean to capture in a laboratory setting, Tulving referred to the single, unrelated
trials that are the typical memoranda of laboratory experiments as events (or mini-events)
Author Manuscript
embedded within the larger episode of the laboratory experiment. For an event to qualify as
an episodic memory, details of the event and of the place in which it occurred must be
present at retrieval, accompanied by autonoetic consciousness that enables reexperiencing
[James 1950 (1890), p. 658; Tulving 1983, 2002; see Rubin & Umanath 2015 for a critique
and alternative].
Tulving’s formulation still governs much of the research on episodic memory and the brain,
but the introduction of new topics, modes of inquiry, more naturalistic approaches and
technologies has added great variety to the landscape. It is this altered landscape that we
wish to capture in our review. Because it is considered central to our understanding of the
brain basis of episodic memory, the hippocampus (HPC) serves as the hub of our article,
much as it is considered the hub of regions dealing with episodic memory. The article
Author Manuscript
focuses on four parts: (a) the role the HPC plays in representing the various aspects and
attributes of episodic memory; (b) other structures that are implicated in different aspects of
episodic memory and its transformation to gist and semantics; (c) the contribution of the
HPC and episodic memory to nonepisodic memory functions such as priming, short-term
memory (STM), and semantics and to nonmnemonic functions such as perception, problem
solving, decision making, and language; and finally, and briefly, (d ) the functional and
structural connectivity between the HPC and other regions and networks implicated in
episodic memory.
In the interest of space and timeliness, we favored the most recent references and reviews
over early empirical papers. All figures accompanying the review can be downloaded by
following the Supplemental Material link in the online version of this article or at the
Author Manuscript
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 3
We use a component process model as our framework. Building on Tulving’s ideas, and the
acknowledged role of the HPC in memory, Moscovitch & Winocur (1992; also Moscovitch
1992) proposed that at encoding the HPC obligatorily binds together into a memory trace or
engram (Dudai 2012, Josselyn et al. 2015, Tonegawa et al. 2015) those neural elements in
the medial temporal lobe (MTL) and neocortex that give rise to the multimodal,
multidomain representations that constitute the content of a conscious experience. The
experience includes the accompanying phenomenological awareness and reflects a process
involving the network interactions that make up the experience itself (Moscovitch 1995). In
this view, consciousness, or the phenomenology of experience, is inextricably linked to
episodic memory. The episodic memory trace or engram consists of a bound ensemble of
HPC-neocortical neurons with a sparsely coded HPC component. This is envisioned as a
spatial scaffold or matrix (Nadel 2008, O’Keefe & Nadel 1978) that acts as a pointer or
Author Manuscript
index (Teyler & Rudy 2007) to neocortical components that together represent the totality of
the experience, including not only the perceptual, emotional, and conceptual details that
form the content of the experience, but also the processes that imbue it with a sense of
autonoetic consciousness. Because HPC binding is obligatory, storage is random, and only
close temporal contiguity, or close contiguity with a reinstated context, determines the
elements that are bound to each other.
the conscious experience of the episode (Moscovitch 2008). Because the HPC-mediated
operations, once initiated, are obligatory, control processes at encoding and retrieval,
mediated by prefrontal cortex (PFC) and related structures, operate on the information
delivered to the HPC and on the output from it to make memory intelligent and goal
directed.
www.annualreviews.org/)].
Receiving its input from the entorhinal cortex, which in turn receives its input from the
perirhinal cortex (PRC) and the parahippocampal cortex (PHC), the HPC integrates
information about complex object representations from the PRC and view-specific scene
representations from the PHC into a view-invariant representation that “frames the spatial
relations among the various parts of the environment ” (Nadel & Peterson 2013, p. 1248)
and locates those parts and their features within that spatial frame (Bird & Burgess 2008,
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 4
Hassabis & Maguire 2009, Nadel 2008). HPC-mediated memories are said to reflect
Author Manuscript
relational associations (Eichenbaum et al. 2007, Olsen et al. 2012), in that both the separate
elements of some event and their relation retain their distinctiveness.
This framework focuses primarily on representational inputs to the HPC from the posterior
neocortex, and it says nothing about HPC connections to anterior structures such as the
amygdala (emotion), anterior temporal cortex (semantics), and PFC [schemas and working
memory (WM)]. These structures play different, but important, roles in episodic memory
through their interaction with the HPC. We will pay some attention below to the relation of
the HPC to the ventromedial PFC (vmPFC). Readers interested in the interactions between
HPC and lateral frontal cortex during encoding and retrieval should refer to Simons & Spiers
(2003) and Anderson & Huddleston (2012), and to Talmi (2013) on amygdala and emotion.
The HPC creates the same kind of representation throughout its length, but at different
Author Manuscript
scales. At the posterior end, its representations capture detailed information about local
spatiotemporal aspects of an experienced event based on input from the posterior neocortex.
At the anterior end, its representations capture global aspects of an event (i.e., the general
context and the emotion and meaning attached to it) based also on interactions with the
anterior neocortex. Thus, these two types of HPC representation arise primarily from
differences in input-output connections along the long axis of the HPC (Poppenk et al. 2013,
Strange et al. 2014) [Supplemental Figure 2 (follow the Supplemental Material link in the
online version of this article or at http://www.annualreviews.org/)]. This framework sets the
stage for investigating the contribution of various HPC subfields to episodic memory (see
below) and for understanding how memories are transformed from detailed representations
to schematic ones at both functional and neurological levels (Penfield & Mathieson 1974).
Author Manuscript
Memory Transformation
As Bartlett (1932) noted, memory representations are not static entities but change
throughout an individual’s life with time and experience. As we discuss in the sections that
follow, some memories retain their highly detailed specificity and, as the data suggest,
continue to rely on the HPC and remain recollective; others are transformed through
forgetting or schematization, so that they lose details and retain only the gist or familiarity;
and others become incorporated into a semantic network and acquire its properties. The
latter cases rely more on neocortical structures, with the vmPFC and anterior temporal lobe
prominent among them. In some cases, specific and gist-like representations can coexist,
leading to dynamic interactions and to the dominance of one memory over the other or to the
conversion of one type to the other, depending on circumstances. The neural instantiation of
these memories corresponds to their functional representation, in accordance with what we
Author Manuscript
believe is a general principle in cognitive neuroscience: Representations that differ from one
another must necessarily be mediated by different structures (collections of neurons), and
representations mediated by different structures must necessarily differ in some fundamental
way from one another. This principle, which we call functional-neural isomorphism (F-NI),
helps account for patterns of HPC-neocortical involvement during memory consolidation.
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 5
Process-Specific Alliances
Author Manuscript
Not all components of the HPC nor of the neocortical (and other) structures with which they
interact are activated at the same time or in all tasks. Instead, we conceive of subsets of
components forming temporary process-specific alliances (PSAs) whose composition is
determined by the moment-to-moment demands of a task. Whereas the posterior neocortical
components, in conjunction with the posterior hippocampus (pHPC), determine the local,
spatio-perceptual aspects of the experience, anterior components of the HPC, in conjunction
with the anterior temporal lobe, PFC, and amygdala, represent conceptual and emotional
aspects. The PSAs also include control structures that regulate encoding and retrieval. All
these memory components can interact with components earlier and later in the hierarchy,
and in other domains, such as language, planning, and decision making, leading to the
involvement of the HPC, and by inference episodic memory, in nonmnemonic functions (see
below).
Author Manuscript
2. The same regions remain active for a while due to top-down modulation
from the PFC, which allows the persistence of object and context
representations, as well as unified event representations, within WM.
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 6
To measure these aspects of autobiographical memory retrieval, Levine et al. (2002) created
the Autobiographical Interview (AI), whose scoring distinguishes between different internal
details that are unique to the event (perception, emotions, locations) and capture episodic
Author Manuscript
aspects of it, and external details, which are not unique to the event and capture more
semantic aspects. Administering the AI to patients with unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy or
lobectomy that included the HPC, St-Laurent et al. (2014) found that memory for perceptual
details was most affected; memory for more global details, such as the story elements that
comprise the series of events within an episode, was also impaired, but not as severely; and
memory for external details was preserved. Using the AI, or a variety of similar tests such as
the Autobiographical Memory Inventory (Kopelman et al. 1989) and the Test Episodique de
Mémoire du Passé autobiographique (TEMPau) task (Piolino et al. 2009), investigators
found the same pattern of impaired episodic but relatively spared semantic aspects of
autobiographical memory in patients with MTL lesions or dysfunction, regardless of
etiology (transient epilepsy, ischemic attacks, degenerative disorders such Alzheimer’s
disease, amnesic variant of minimal cognitive impairment, later stages of frontotemporal
Author Manuscript
dementia, and psychiatric and mood disorders; reviewed in Piolino et al. 2009, Viard et al.
2012, Winocur & Moscovitch 2011).
Film clips provide more experimental control than tests of autobiographical memory, yet
they retain a naturalistic quality. The pattern of results obtained with clips in patients with
MTL lesions (St-Laurent et al. 2014), compared to HPC activation in healthy controls in
fMRI studies, was similar to that obtained on recall of autobiographical episodes (Ben-
Yakov & Dudai 2011, Furman et al. 2012, Maguire & Mullally 2013) [Supplemental Figure
3 (follow the Supplemental Material link in the online version of this article or at http://
www.annualreviews.org/)].
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 7
Although the use of more naturalistic stimuli has grown, by far the largest majority of
Author Manuscript
studies use unrelated stimuli as the method of choice in studying episodic memory,
sometimes presented in arbitrary contexts and tested almost exclusively by recognition. It is
reassuring, therefore, that similar regions are activated when laboratory memory studies
emphasize recollection (but not familiarity), whether measured subjectively, by asking
individuals to rate the extent to which the item evoked the experience at study (recollection
or know response), or objectively, by asking individuals to recognize elements of the context
in which the stimulus appeared (source memory) (Skinner & Fernandes 2007). Known as the
recollective network, these regions overlap substantially with those activated during recall of
vivid, autobiographical memories (Cabeza & St. Jacques 2007, Rugg & Vilberg 2013,
Svoboda et al. 2006) and include HPC, PHC, medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), VPC
(angular gyrus), and retrosplenial cortex/posterior cingulate [Supplemental Figure 4 (follow
the Supplemental Material link in the online version of this article or at http://
Author Manuscript
With respect to performance in patients with MTL lesions that include the HPC, Montaldi &
Mayes (2011) and Yonelinas (2013) concluded that even when overall accuracy, an index of
strength, is equated, memory under conditions associated with recollection is selectively
impaired compared to memory based on familiarity (for counterarguments, see Dede et al.
2013, Squire & Wixted 2011). By contrast, familiarity-based memory is impaired and
recollection is preserved following PRC lesions that spare the HPC (Bowles et al. 2010).
Author Manuscript
As we noted earlier, recollection, mediated by the HPC, depends on relational yet flexible
associations among disparate (random) and distinct (separable) elements (for reviews, see
Eichenbaum et al. 2007, Olsen et al. 2012, Yonelinas 2013). When associations lose their
relational nature and become unitized (e.g., combining the words fire and dog into firedog),
HPC involvement as inferred from lesion (Quamme et al. 2007) and fMRI (Haskins et al.
2008) is diminished or lost.
that provide the scaffold or framework for supporting memory for events (Hassabis &
Maguire 2009). Studies of patients with HPC lesions and functional neuroimaging studies in
healthy people show that constructing coherent scenes is dependent on the HPC (Maguire &
Mullally 2013). Moreover, there is great overlap among brain regions and networks activated
on spatial tasks and on episodic memory tasks, even when the latter do not have an obvious
spatial component (Spreng et al. 2009). Robin et al. (2015), however, found that even when
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 8
narratives lack information about spatial location, participants spontaneously add them at
Author Manuscript
There is evidence that memory for events is facilitated by familiar spatial information (e.g.,
Robin & Moscovitch 2014), and that regions sensitive to memory for events interact with
regions sensitive to spatial memory in the HPC even at the single-cell level (Miller et al.
2013). In a study that suggests the primacy of spatial over event memory in the HPC,
Chadwick et al. (2010) had participants recall in detail film clips in which two different
events occurred in two distinct spatial locations. Using multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA)
to differentiate neurally the retrieved memories from one another, they found that
classification accuracy for distinct episodes was significantly better than chance only in the
HPC. When required to classify the clips with regard to differences in location as compared
to event content, only locations could be classified accurately, and then only in the HPC.
Author Manuscript
There are at least three aspects of temporal processing that have been addressed in the
literature (Schacter et al. 2012): (a) the temporal tag or signal associated with different
moments in the unfolding of an event, which can also code for duration; (b) the coding for
the temporal order of elements within and across episodes; and (c) the subjective sense of
Author Manuscript
time, so that we can identify our experience of the memory as occurring in the near or
distant past or future.
To account for the first two aspects, based on evidence from time cells (Eichenbaum 2014),
investigators have proposed a hippocampal mechanism that could construct a scale-invariant
representation of time over hundreds of seconds; this could serve as the contextual/neuronal
background in which events are embedded via association (Howard & Eichenbaum 2013).
This model can account both for temporal order effects in memory and for the reduction in
temporal precision with temporal distance. Davachi & Dubrow (2015) noted that contiguity
as a determinant of temporal order is better for elements within an event or segment than
across them, pointing to an important role for event segmentation. They also note that
learning a sequence through repeated presentations may rely on pattern completion, with one
Author Manuscript
item serving as a cue for eliciting replay of the associated elements. Although both proposals
are broadly consistent with the temporal contiguity aspects of the component process model,
it remains to be explained why, in general, memory for temporal information is very poor in
comparison to memory for spatial information.
Memory for duration and temporal order across very short intervals such as milliseconds,
and across long intervals such as minutes, hours, or days, seems to depend on information
and strategies mediated by extrahippocampal structures (Davachi & Dubrow 2015,
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 9
Moscovitch 1992), among them the cerebellum, PFC, posterior parietal cortex (PPC)
Author Manuscript
(Danckert et al. 2007), and basal ganglia. The cognitive processes and neural mechanisms
that underlie our subjective sense of time as past, present, or future are even less well
understood, though some have proposed that the HPC plays a crucial role. The growing
interest in the temporal aspects of memory will hopefully stimulate research that will add to
our meager knowledge of them and resolve some of the controversies.
INTRAHIPPOCAMPAL SPECIALIZATION
Memory Precision, Pattern Separation, and Hippocampus Subfields
Although there is ample evidence that relational associations account for our ability to
recollect the details of an episode, a number of studies suggest that the precision, or high
resolution, of those memories is a critical component even for recognition of single items
such as faces, as long as the items are complex and the targets are distinguished from similar
Author Manuscript
lures after a long delay (Craik 1986, Smith et al. 2014a, Yonelinas 2013). To account for this
sensitivity to complexity and precision, investigators have pointed to a fundamental
computational mechanism, called orthogonalization, instantiated in the organization and
operation of the projections between two hippocampal subfields, the dentate gyrus (DG) and
CA3. This orthogonalization, or pattern separation that is dependent on DG, yields distinct
representations that enable memory precision between items (or collections of items) with
overlapping features (Hunsaker & Kesner 2013, Norman 2010) [Supplemental Figure 5
(follow the Supplemental Material link in the online version of this article or at http://
www.annualreviews.org/)]. The efficiency of pattern separation should determine the
precision of the memory and the number of separable details held in memory (Poppenk et al.
2013).
Author Manuscript
Recent advances in high-resolution fMRI have allowed investigators to test these ideas more
directly. Bakker et al. (2008) found that DG/CA3 (which were considered together because
they could not be delineated separately) showed the greatest sensitivity to tests of memory
discrimination between highly similar items (which they presumed depended on pattern
separation). CA1, on the other hand, showed a linear response and tracked the similarity
matrix, indicating that the more similar the items were, the more likely they were to be
coded as the same. Pattern separation was related to recollection (Kim & Yassa 2013) in
younger adults. Older adults showed a reduction in DG/CA3 responsivity to pattern
separation but normal CA1 responses, consistent with their poorer ability to distinguish
targets from similar foils at recognition (Yassa et al. 2011). Importantly, the extent of HPC
involvement in distinguishing between target and lures may depend on whether the
differences between them are relational or based on single features (Aly et al. 2013,
Author Manuscript
Yonelinas 2013).
The same involvement of HPC subfields is evident in distinguishing among more naturalistic
stimuli. Precision of episodic recall of film clips, as measured by the extent to which
participants could rate the clips as different, was related to the size and activity of the CA3
region (Chadwick et al. 2014) [Supplemental Figure 6 (follow the Supplemental Material
link in the online version of this article or at http://www.annualreviews.org/)]. Comparable
findings were reported by Bonnici et al. (2013) for autobiographical memories in the pHPC,
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 10
where remote, but not recent, memories could be distinguished from one another only in DG
Author Manuscript
and CA3.
(Malykhin et al. 2010). DG/CA3 ratios are higher in the pHPC than in the aHPC, with
greater CA1 proliferation in the anterior portion. This would suggest more powerful pattern
separation capabilities in the pHPC than in the aHPC, a notion that fits well with recent
work demonstrating greater precision and detail in the grain of HPC representations and
reduced receptive field size of place cells as one moves from anterior to posterior regions
(Strange et al. 2014). Neuroanatomical (Aggleton 2012) and connectivity analysis during
resting state (Ranganath & Ritchey 2012) showed that the pHPC is preferentially connected
to perceptual regions in the posterior neocortex, whereas the aHPC is preferentially
connected to anterior regions, such as the vmPFC and the lateral temporal cortex extending
into the temporal pole and the amygdala, which are associated with the processing of
schemas, semantic information, and social and emotional cues, respectively. These
connectivity patterns could conspire to create unique aHPC and pHPC encoding and
Author Manuscript
representational environments, with the aHPC coding information in terms of the general or
global relations among entities, and the pHPC coding information in terms of precise
positions within some continuous dimension (Poppenk et al. 2013) (Supplemental Figure 2).
The evidence generally supports the model (see Poppenk et al. 2013 for review). Whether on
tests of autobiographical memory (Nadel et al. 2013) or on laboratory tests of spatial
memory (Evensmoen et al. 2015), aHPC activity is associated with the general locations
where events occurred or coarse map-like representations of those locations, whereas
activity in the pHPC is associated with local details or finer-grained maps (for comparable
results in rodents see review in Preston & Eichenbaum 2013). Similar effects are found in
studies of real-world navigation in a virtual environment. Greater aHPC activation is
associated with increased distance between sequentially presented landmarks (Morgan et al.
Author Manuscript
2011), greater scale of the environment (Baumann & Mattingley 2013), and direction to
goal, whereas pHPC activations are inversely correlated with distance to goal (Howard et al.
2014). Consistent with this model, and with the hypothesis that recollection is associated
with memory for local details, Poppenk & Moscovitch (2011) found that individual
differences in recollection were correlated with the size of the pHPC (and the pHPC/aHPC
ratio), which in turn was related to the functional connectivity of the pHPC with posterior
neocortical regions at rest.
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 11
With respect to autobiographical memory, our framework has much in common with
Author Manuscript
Such differences are also observed when considering global and local aspects of a single
object. The neural suppression that occurs when an object is repeated is seen in the DG/CA3
subfields of the aHPC, but when fine discrimination between targets and similar lures is
Author Manuscript
Similarly, encoding novel events, or forming relational associations between random items,
is associated with aHPC activation, possibly because such activities typically involve higher-
order conceptual processing, whereas encoding familiar or repeated events relies less on
such conceptual processes and more on attending to perceptual aspects of the event
(Poppenk et al. 2013). Since repeated events also have a retrieval component, this
formulation is consistent with evidence of differential aHPC and pHPC activation at
encoding and retrieval (Kim 2015).
with respect to episodic memory. Another virtue of this conception of aHPC function is that
it serves as a bridge between detailed episodic representations that are dependent on the
HPC and more schematic, gist-like representations that emerge as memories are transformed
with time and experience (Preston & Eichenbaum 2013, Robin et al. 2015, Winocur &
Moscovitch 2011) and become reliant on extrahippocampal structures, such as the vmPFC
and anterior temporal lobe, that have strong aHPC connections.
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 12
associative memory study trials (Tambini et al. 2010). Memory can be reduced by
Author Manuscript
interference from other stimuli (Ben-Yakov et al. 2013) or enhanced by emotional stimuli
(Dunsmoor et al. 2015) presented during this postencoding period.
Postencoding processes during sleep have complex effects on memories acquired during the
previous day, and recent work suggests that their impact may be related to the type of
memory examined as well as to the stages of sleep under study (Diekelman & Born 2010,
Stickgold 2013). With respect to episodic memory, there is evidence that recollection can be
preserved or degraded and transformed to familiarity, but the conditions that lead to one or
the other outcome, and the neural mechanisms involved, have yet to be elucidated (Lewis &
Durrant 2011).
Remote Memory
The issue of memory transformation lies at the heart of the debate on the neural
Author Manuscript
representation of remote memory, in which changes can occur across weeks, years, and even
decades (Nadel & Moscovitch 1997, Winocur & Moscovitch 2011). The observation that
remote, compared to recent, memories appear to be relatively spared following damage to
the MTL, and the HPC in particular, formed the foundation of the standard consolidation
theory (SCT). This theory states that the HPC serves as a temporary memory structure
needed only until memories are consolidated elsewhere, presumably in the neocortex, where
they are retained and from where they can be retrieved. Although Penfield & Milner (1958)
and Penfield & Mathieson (1974) noted that extra-HPC memories are generalized (gist-like)
and fundamentally different from the detailed, specific memories mediated by the HPC,
some versions of SCT did not make any distinction between the two types of memory. In
other versions, all remote memories were assumed to be incorporated into a neocortical
semantic or schematic network, leaving no HPC-mediated specific episodic memories
Author Manuscript
behind (Frankland & Bontempi 2005, McClelland et al. 1995, Wang & Morris 2010).
Drawing on the work of previous investigators, Nadel & Moscovitch (1997) took issue with
each of these interpretations and with evidence that all remote memories are spared
following HPC damage. They noted that individuals can retain detailed remote episodic
memories and that the HPC and related structures are needed for their retention and retrieval,
no matter how old the memories are. Reactivation of old memories leads to the re-encoding
and formation of a rich distributed network of multiple traces in the HPC, which provides
some protection from small, but not large, HPC lesions. Gist-like (semanticized) memories
of episodes as well as semantic memories, mediated as they are by other structures, would
survive HPC damage under any circumstances. The multiple trace theory proposed by Nadel
& Moscovitch (1997) links the type of memory representation (detailed versus gist-like/
Author Manuscript
schematic/semantic) with the structure that mediates it (HPC versus neocortex), and it helps
account for the different patterns of retrograde amnesia observed after HPC damage.
Building on ideas from multiple trace theory, Winocur & Moscovitch (2011; see also
Winocur et al. 2010) proposed that some autobiographical and spatial memories in humans,
as well as context-dependent memories in rodents, are transformed with time and experience
from highly detailed, context-specific memories to less detailed, schematic memories that
retain the gist of an experience or event or schematic representation of an environment, but
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 13
not many particulars. These transformed memories are based on representations in extra-
Author Manuscript
HPC structures and do not depend on the HPC. Insofar as memories remain detailed and
retain their contextual specificity, they will continue to depend on representations encoded in
the HPC, regardless of their age. Both types of representations can coexist and dynamically
interact, so that a memory that has previously been manifested in a schematic rather than
detailed form can regain its specificity with appropriate reminders and once again engage the
HPC (Winocur et al. 2010, Winocur & Moscovitch 2011).
Appealing to the principle of F-NI, Winocur & Moscovitch (2011) reasoned that a memory
that was once mediated by the HPC and is now mediated by the cortex must differ in accord
with the processing and representational capabilities of these two structures. If the intricate
architecture of the HPC is needed to perform the computations that are central to the
retention and retrieval of precise, detailed, and relational representations of recent memories,
it is difficult to see how comparable representations, only older, could be mediated solely by
Author Manuscript
The debate between adherents of each camp has continued for 20 years and has been
reviewed extensively, so it will only be summarized here. Performance on tests of
autobiographical memory that are especially sensitive to attributes that capture the essence
of episodic memory—such as perceptual details, viewpoint specificity, vividness, judgments
of recollection, and relational associations—suffers after HPC damage or dysfunction,
regardless of etiology and of the amount of time passed since the memory was acquired.
Unless the damage extends to extra-HPC regions, memories for gist or semantic aspects of
personal experiences, public events, and well-known narratives such as bible stories and
fairy tales are relatively well preserved (Winocur & Moscovitch 2011), though memory for
details, even for well-known stories, may be compromised (Kwan et al. 2013, Verfaellie et
Author Manuscript
al. 2014). The same is true of spatial memory: Memory for perceptual details needed to
reexperience the environment (scene construction) is lost, whereas schematic memories
sufficient for navigation are retained (Winocur & Moscovitch 2011).
Evidence from fMRI is consistent with this observation. Autobiographical memories that are
detailed and vivid engage the HPC no matter how long ago they were acquired (Winocur &
Moscovitch 2011). Importantly, the pattern of activation along the long axis of the HPC
resembles that observed for recently acquired novel and familiar memories: More recent
memories engage the aHPC and more remote ones engage the pHPC, and the distinction
between recent and remote memories, and among the memories themselves, is mediated by
the posterior DG/CA3 subfield (Bonnici et al. 2013). Likewise, connectivity of the HPC to
other structures in the autobiographical memory network varies with vividness and
Author Manuscript
The same pattern of decreasing HPC and increasing neocortical activation with remoteness
and accompanying loss of specificity or recollection is found in traditional laboratory tests
of episodic memory with delays of one (Ritchey et al. 2015) to six weeks (Viskontas et al.
2009a; but see Nieuwenhuis & Takashima 2011 for different types of associative memories
that may be schema dependent). Likewise, for remote spatial memory, studies suggest that
what is preserved after HPC damage is a gist-like, schematic representation of the
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 14
environment (essentially a coarse-grained map) that is adequate for navigation but not for
Author Manuscript
conjuring up the kind of details that allow for reexperiencing the environment (Rosenbaum
et al. 2000, Winocur & Moscovitch 2011). Consistent with this interpretation, HPC
activation is found during mental navigation in a recently encountered environment but is
diminished or absent when navigating in a familiar environment (Hirshhorn et al. 2012).
Proponents of the SCT argue that extensive retrograde amnesia for autobiographical events
occurs only when lesions of the MTL extend beyond the HPC to include the temporal
neocortex (Squire & Wixted 2011). Although this explanation may account for deficits in
remote memory in some patients, it cannot account for deficits in many others whose lesions
are restricted to the HPC or its projections via the fornix (see review in Winocur &
Moscovitch 2011), or to disruption restricted to CA1 fields in transient global amnesia
(Bartsch et al. 2011). To account for fMRI results, proponents of the SCT have argued that
HPC activity observed while recalling remote memories is related to the re-encoding of
Author Manuscript
autobiographical memories retrieved from the neocortex rather than to the memories
themselves (Squire & Wixted 2011). This critique is difficult to refute, though recent
evidence of the different patterns of activation for recent and remote memories in the HPC
and midline structures would argue against a simple re-encoding interpretation (Bonnici et
al. 2013).
(Winocur et al. 2010). This loss of context specificity is accompanied by a lesser dependence
on the HPC and a higher dependence on the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) or prelimbic
cortex, the rodent homologue of the vmPFC in humans, as determined by both the effects of
lesions and early gene expression (Frankland & Bontempi 2005, Winocur et al. 2010).
Importantly, providing reminder cues in the long-delay condition a day prior to the test
(Winocur et al. 2010) or periodically reactivating the context (de Oliveira Alvares et al.
2012) restore or maintain contextual specificity, rendering the memory once again HPC
dependent and vulnerable to the effects of HPC lesions.
once again to the disruptive effects of HPC lesions, protein synthesis inhibitors, or synaptic
interference (Dudai 2012, Nader & Hardt 2009, Wang & Morris 2010).
The fact that specificity and hippocampal dependence could be restored by reminders not
only speaks to the dynamic nature of memory and against the SCT, but also suggests that
some vestige of the original specific memory must be retained by the HPC. Evidence in
support of this hypothesis comes from optogenetic studies in which temporary disruption of
those CA1 cells that encoded the information leads to loss of contextual fear memories even
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 15
at long delays (Goshen et al. 2011, Tanaka et al. 2014), just as CA1 disruption causes remote
Author Manuscript
memory loss in humans (Bartsch et al. 2011). Recent studies in which cells that constitute
the memory (engram) are labeled (tagged), reactivated, or suppressed support our hypothesis
that long-term retention of context-dependent memories is mediated by neural ensembles in
the HPC (Josselyn et al. 2015, Tonegawa et al. 2015).
Memory Transformation and the Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex: Remote Memory and
Schemas
The vmPFC seems to play a special role in mediating remote memories (Frankland &
Bontempi 2005), but its precise nature is not clear. One promising clue to vmPFC function
comes from the work of Tse, Morris, and collaborators (Tse et al. 2007; see Wang & Morris
2010) on schemas, which can be defined as “adaptable associative networks of knowledge
extracted over multiple similar experiences” (Ghosh et al. 2014, p. 12057). Tse et al. (2007)
Author Manuscript
showed that the vmPFC is crucial for assimilating new olfactory-location associations into a
spatial schema, which is acquired over many exposures. As a result, the new associations
become independent of the HPC more quickly, relying instead on the ACC/vmPFC.
Such results are interpreted according to a schema modification model (Dudai 2012, Wang
& Morris 2010), which posits that schemas provide the organizing structures that influence
memory formation and retrieval (Bartlett 1932) as well as interpretation of ongoing events.
We propose that the vmPFC, with its reciprocal connections to the aHPC, could act as a hub
binding the global context of events represented in the aHPC and general knowledge,
including that about the self, into a schema that captures what is common to all such events
(see also Benoit et al. 2014). A fundamental function of any schema is to make predictions
about what one should expect to experience in a given context/situation/setting, to aid in
interpreting events that occur there, and to enable one to notice new details that do not fit the
Author Manuscript
schema. For example, one may have schemas of what kitchens are like and what one does in
them. Walking into a strange kitchen may produce a novel episodic memory for the kitchen
and for the local events that transpire there; schemas ensure that the experience of being in
the kitchen is not in itself strange but relatively predictable.
Some investigators have suggested that the vmPFC is the consolidated (remote) memory
homologue of the HPC, taking over its function. This notion runs counter to the F-NI
principle. Our proposal is that the vmPFC and HPC may both deal with context, but of
fundamentally different sorts. The HPC binds together local and global contextual
information about a specific event or environment, whereas the vmPFC binds together what
is common across similar events and possibly helps “decide” which among them is relevant
for a particular task (Preston & Eichenbaum 2013).
Author Manuscript
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 16
Tse et al.’s (2007) studies and Takashima et al.’s (2009) human analogue
of them, but not when events are isolated and random as in many
traditional laboratory experiments.
Interim Summary
Memories for recent events draw on interactions between schemas, semantics, and
perceptual aspects of an experience, mediated in part by different regions in the anterior and
posterior neo-cortex. These are bound together in relational representations mediated by the
pHPC for local details and the aHPC for global context, accounting for the specificity
characteristic of episodic memory (Craik 1986). Over time, detailed information about local
aspects of the event become degraded or lost, and our memory reports come to rely on more
global aspects mediated by the aHPC in conjunction with schema-related information
mediated by the vmPFC. The latter helps determine performance when the HPC is entirely
Author Manuscript
lost. Damage to the vmPFC leads to deficient and distorted use of schemas in memory,
perception, and reasoning.
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 17
The two-stage model of recollection predicts that priming for relational associations should
be preserved insofar as it draws on information underlying the first rapid, nonconscious
Author Manuscript
stage of recollection mediated by the HPC. Recent studies show that priming of relational
associations, measured by manual reaction times (Schacter et al. 2004) or eye movements
(Hannula & Ranganath 2009), is accompanied by HPC activation, is impaired in people with
HPC damage (Olsen et al. 2012), and is more robust for items that are subsequently judged
to be recollected than for familiar ones (Sheldon & Moscovitch 2010). Going one step
further, Henke and collaborators showed that the HPC encodes information presented
subliminally as long as it is relational (Henke 2010).
The Ventral Posterior Parietal Cortex and Subjective Aspects of Memory (Autonoetic
Consciousness)
Consistent with our component process model, studies on nonconscious memory suggest
that engagement of the HPC is not sufficient for explicit recollection or episodic memory,
Author Manuscript
and that other structures also need to be recruited. Structures in the frontoparietal network
are among the possible candidates. Here we focus on the PPC, since it has been the object of
much debate concerning its role in episodic memory.
Activation of the PPC, particularly on the left, often accompanies memory retrieval on
laboratory tests of recognition and autobiographical memory (Cabeza & St. Jacques 2007,
Rugg & Vilberg 2013, Svoboda et al. 2006). Of the two major divisions of the PPC, dorsal
parietal cortex (DPC) and VPC, the VPC is associated with successful recollection, source
monitoring, and high-confidence responses, whereas the DPC is associated with familiarity
and low-confidence responses (for other differences between the two divisions, see Cabeza
et al. 2012) [Supplemental Figure 8 (follow the Supplemental Material link in the online
version of this article or at http://www.annualreviews.org/)].
Author Manuscript
A number of hypotheses have been proposed to account for VPC involvement in episodic
memory, and recent reviews have discussed extensively their merits and deficiencies
(Berryhill 2012, Cabeza et al. 2012, Rugg & Vilberg 2013). Here, we focus on the
hypothesis that the VPC is implicated in the apprehension of the subjective qualities of
memory (Simons et al. 2010). This hypothesis accounts not only for the reduced and less
detailed recollections of patients with VPC lesions that can be ameliorated by cuing
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 18
(Berryhill 2012, Davidson et al. 2008), but also for the low confidence the patients have in
Author Manuscript
their memory even when source accuracy is high (Hower et al. 2014, Simons et al. 2010), an
outcome that can be produced in normal people by deactivating the VPC with theta burst
stimulation (Yazar et al. 2014). These findings raise the possibility that the VPC, perhaps via
its interactions with the frontal cortex, is an essential component for enabling autonoetic
consciousness. It is interesting in this regard that VPC activation accompanies memories that
come to mind involuntarily, either when they are elicited by a cue (Hall et al. 2014) or when
they overcome active suppression (Benoit et al. 2014). It is also noteworthy that the VPC has
been associated with functions that are the hallmarks of autonoetic consciousness: WM and
attention, time perception, feelings of intentionality, and a sense of self associated with
theory of mind (ToM) (see Berryhill 2012, Cabeza et al. 2012). As noted, the VPC is also
linked with the PFC (Prebble et al. 2013), another region that is associated with ToM
(Mitchell 2009) and whose damage, or disconnection, can lead to reduced recollection and
Author Manuscript
These findings suggest a dissociation between successful retrieval of the content of episodic
memories and the phenomenological experience that accompanies successful retrieval. It
remains to be determined whether the diminished autonoetic consciousness associated with
VPC and PFC damage or disconnection results from an impaired postretrieval process that,
as part of the second stage of recollection, attributes phenomenological experience to a
detailed memory, or whether it results from loss of the “consciousness feature” of the HPC-
neocortical engram, which VPC codes much as inferior temporal structures code for
perceptual features.
Following the introduction of the concept of episodic memory in 1972, much research has
focused on how to distinguish episodic memory from other cognitive functions in general,
and in particular from other forms of memory, at both the functional and neural levels. Given
the widespread connections of the HPC (Aggleton 2012, Ranganath & Ritchey 2012) and its
position in the component process model, the isolation and encapsulation of episodic
memory could not be sustained. The last decade has seen an imperialist encroachment of
episodic memory and the HPC into various domains, from perception to imagination, and
from decision making to food regulation (Robinson et al. 2013, Rozin et al. 1998). We
highlight four of these domains and consider how they relate to our model (see also Rubin et
al. 2014).
The idea that the HPC sits at the end of a perceptual hierarchy, receiving input from lower
levels and projecting back to them, suggests that HPC influence should be felt in perception
and WM, thereby eroding the hard and fast distinction between long-term memory (LTM),
perception, and WM. According to the hierarchical representation model (HRM; Lee et al.
2012), the HPC’s role at the top of the hierarchy and the role of the structures below it are
defined strictly not in terms of memory or perception, but rather (or also) in terms of the
representations they support, consistent with the component process model. Insofar as the
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 19
HPC represents relational information among separable entities, or even among features in
Author Manuscript
single complex stimuli, perceptual discrimination based on such features activates the HPC
and is impaired in people with HPC damage (Lee et al. 2012, Yonelinas 2013).
Drawing on their theory stating that the HPC is implicated in scene construction, Mullally et
al. (2012) showed that boundary extension—the normal tendency to reconstruct a scene with
a larger background than actually was presented—was markedly diminished in patients with
HPC lesions compared to controls, even when stimuli were presented as briefly as 200 ms
[Supplemental Figure 9 (follow the Supplemental Material link in the online version of this
article or at http://www.annualreviews.org/)]. In subsequent neuroimaging studies, Chadwick
et al. (2013) showed that boundary extension is accompanied by hippocampal activation
that, in turn, projects down the hierarchy to levels in the visual cortex.
HPC has also been shown to be implicated in STM and WM, especially if precision and
Author Manuscript
relational binding are required (Olsen et al. 2012, Yonelinas 2013). Binding errors in
reporting information in STM in people with HPC damage (Pertzov et al. 2013) and
matching a target array of items on a screen with a test array presented shortly afterward was
shown to be associated with HPC activation and to be reduced in people with medial
temporal/HPC damage (Nichols et al. 2006, Olson et al. 2006; but see Talmi et al. 2005) or
atrophy; the reduction was correlated with the extent of posterior hippocampal atrophy for
location and with perirhinal atrophy for objects (Das et al. 2015). Performance on all the
tests was correlated with estimates of recollection, whereas only performance on the object
test was correlated with familiarity.
Though such developments blur the boundaries between LTM, WM, and perception,
obliterating them would obscure the striking differences one sees clinically: Patients with
Author Manuscript
HPC lesions rarely complain of perceptual or WM deficits but always complain of episodic
memory deficits, whereas the reverse is true of people with damage to levels that are lower
in the hierarchy. Capitalizing on this observation, Squire and collaborators (e.g., Kim et al.
2015, Squire & Wixted 2011) presented evidence showing that if the lesions were confined
to the HPC and the tests did not implicate LTM, then perception and STM/WM was normal
in patients with HPC lesions (see also Baddeley et al. 2010 and rebuttal in Clark & Maguire
2016; Maguire et al. 2015).
To counter this negative evidence, and the arguments based on it, Yonelinas (2013) and Lee
et al. (2012) have argued, in accord with the HRM, that the complexity of the items and the
precision of the discrimination determine HPC involvement on tests of perception and
STM/WM. Moreover, it is likely that susceptibility to interference increases as one moves up
the hierarchy, and hence the impact on memory is more keenly felt when higher levels are
Author Manuscript
damaged. The component process model and the F-NI principle are consistent with either
account, and the resolution of the debate rests on the question of whether HPC-mediated
relational processing applies only to memory or also to perception (see also Clark &
Maguire 2016, Maguire et al. 2015).
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 20
One of the most striking findings in recent years points to the large overlap in processes and
structures involved in retrieving episodic memories, imagining future personal events, and
constructing scenes, thereby propelling the HPC into domains it had not occupied previously
(Addis & Schacter 2011, Buckner 2010, Maguire & Mullally 2013, Schacter et al. 2012,
Viard et al. 2012). Because HPC-based memories are relational, their elements could be
recombined to form new associations, which together with constructive processes dependent
on the pHPC (Gaesser et al. 2013) would create new scenarios (Romero & Moscovitch
2012) and construct new scenes (Hassabis & Maguire 2009) in the service of current needs.
Addis, Schacter, and colleagues referred to this as episodic simulation, a cognitive operation
that draws on processes similar to episodic memory to create imagined events [see Maguire
& Mullally (2013), who argue that scene construction is the fundamental process].
Author Manuscript
Recent studies have shown that episodic simulation can lead to better social problem solving
(Sheldon et al. 2011), greater empathy (Ciaramelli et al. 2013), and more willingness to help
people in distress (Gaesser & Schacter 2014); in all cases, performance was related to the
vividness or level of detail of the simulation. HPC damage or deterioration with aging leads
to a drop in problem-solving performance (Sheldon et al. 2011) and reduces empathy in
general (Beadle et al. 2013, Davidson et al. 2012), whereas training on recovery of episodic
details improves problem solving (Madore & Schacter 2014). On the other side of the coin,
vivid negative images associated with past events and conjured spontaneously in social
situations contribute to social anxiety (Moscovitch et al. 2013).
spend the money in the future as compared to merely thinking conceptually about its
usefulness; the extent of the reduction is related to vmPFC activation and its functional
connectivity with the HPC. Consistent with the fMRI results, Palombo et al. (2015) showed
that episodic simulation is ineffective in reducing the normal tendency for temporal
discounting in people with amnesia associated with MTL damage (Kwan et al. 2013),
because their capacity for simulation is compromised.
without the participant’s awareness, such that when BC is presented, the common element,
B, reactivates the previously learned association, AB, and integrates both with C, a process
they termed integrative encoding. This accords with the principle of obligatory binding of
elements occurring in close temporal contiguity. Similar processes influence reward-based
decisions in which a stimulus (A) acquires value through its secondary association (B) with
monetary reward (C) (Wimmer & Shohamy 2012). The degree to which A comes to be
associated with C is determined also by its interaction with schema-related processes in the
vmPFC and its intrinsic resting-state connectivity with the HPC (Gerraty et al. 2014).
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 21
(Gilboa et al. 2014) and transitive inference in rats and humans who are presented with
overlapping stimulus pairs in which one member of the pair is rewarded (e.g., A > B, B > C,
C > D) and participants must infer how it is related to the other items (Preston &
Eichenbaum 2013). If the latter task is solved by forming relational associations, then the
HPC is necessary, but if it is solved by unitization, then it can be performed without recourse
to the HPC but may still be dependent on the vmPFC and PRC (Ryan et al. 2013).
Such effects cannot be said to be related to full-blown episodic memory, which is defined as
being associated with autonoetic consciousness, but should rather be related to operations of
pattern separation, pattern completion, and associative binding that operate outside of
conscious awareness but presumably underlie both episodic memory and these types of
associative learning, in accord with the two-stage model of recollection and HPC activation.
Consistent with this observation, Reber & Henke (2012) showed that associative inference
Author Manuscript
based on integrative encoding can be attained even when AB and BC items are presented
subliminally.
the flow of speech, or created rapidly in the present and incorporated effortlessly into the
context that forms the common ground of social discourse” (Duff & Brown-Schmidt 2012).
Discourse analysis reveals that such functions are aberrant or impoverished in amnesic
patients (Race et al. 2015). The paucity of the experiential aspects of episodic memory in
patients with HPC damage is revealed in their greater than normal selection of the definite
over indefinite article and the diminished use of the historical present, a figure of speech in
which a past event is referred in the present tense (e.g., Yesterday, I saw a fire; the fireman
goes into the house, battles the flames…) and whose frequency of usage is correlated with
the number of internal details in their narrative (Park et al. 2011).
episodic memory representations mediated by the HPC. Evidence from studies of patients
with amnesia and with semantic dementia suggests that people, places, public events, and
even common objects and their names can be imbued with autobiographical significance
(AS) in the sense that they automatically (either with or without conscious awareness)
conjure up recollective information that facilitates naming, recognition, and semantic
judgments. Consistent with the component process model, this AS advantage is lost in
people with MTL damage (for review, see Renoult et al. 2012).
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 22
Renoult et al. (2015) used event-related potentials to explore this phenomenon further. When
Author Manuscript
These sets of findings suggest a novel interpretation of reports by Quiroga and collaborators
(see review in Quiroga 2012), who found that single cells in the human HPC, which they
named concept cells, respond selectively and invariantly to representations of famous people
across modalities. Viskontas et al. (2009b), however, noted that the majority of the cells
respond to people who are personally familiar to the patient. As in studies on AS, the
pictures or names presented to the patients may automatically have evoked a recollective
process, and therefore referring to the cells as AS or episodic cells would be equally
Author Manuscript
appropriate.
episodic memories, whereas in patients with MTL lesions, the responses are fewer and less
idiosyncratic because they rely more on semantic memory (Sheldon et al. 2013). Consistent
with this finding is a report by Addis et al. (2015) showing that performance on tests of
divergent thinking is related to one’s ability to construct episodic simulations and can be
enhanced by inducing episodic specificity (Madore et al. 2015).
2011). This fast mapping process may depend on rapid assimilation of the item to pre-
existing schemas or semantic memory networks mediated by neocortex (Coutanche &
Thompson-Schill 2015, Sharon et al. 2011). Item-name associations learned through fast
mapping, however, are prone to catastrophic interference (Merhav et al. 2014). The reverse
pattern, of better acquisition through the explicit route and poor fast mapping, is seen in
patients with anterior temporal lesions that include the perirhinal cortex (Merhav et al. 2015,
Sharon et al. 2011), suggesting that this structure may help mediate fast mapping. Not all
attempts to replicate these fast-mapping results, however, have been successful (e.g., Smith
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 23
et al. 2014b). The reasons for the discrepancies will need to be elucidated if we are to gain a
Author Manuscript
better understanding of the neural mechanisms mediating fast mapping (Coutanche &
Thompson-Schill 2015).
The evidence we reviewed indicates that episodic memory, as mediated by the HPC,
contributes more to language and semantics than classical views on dissociable multiple
memory systems would lead us to believe. As with perception and WM, we do not wish to
end by obliterating distinctions that we believe are still useful; rather, based on our
component process model, we propose that the systems are much more interactive than we
once thought, enabling episodic memory and the HPC to influence functions outside their
traditional domain. This interactive approach is increasingly evident in neuroimaging
research, with its current emphasis on networks and on structural and functional connectivity
among nodes in those networks.
Author Manuscript
each other in general and during the process of interest. There are many examples of PSAs
that contribute to episodic memory and fulfill these criteria; here, we mention only two.
The first example is the PSA between the pHPC and VPC during recollective retrieval.
Consistent with the first criterion, meta-analyses of event-related fMRI studies have strongly
linked these regions to retrieval success and recollection (Cabeza et al. 2012). During
retrieval, the pHPC and VPC show a recollection-related activity pattern, whereas a very
different pattern emerges in the aHPC (e.g., Daselaar et al. 2006) [Supplemental Figure 10
(follow the Supplemental Material link in the online version of this article or at http://
www.annualreviews.org/)]. In keeping with the second criterion, the pHPC has been linked
to the recovery of episodic memory details and the VPC to the complementary process of
operating on the recovered information (Cabeza et al. 2012). Finally, fulfilling the third
criterion, the pHPC and VPC have direct white-matter connections and interact very closely
Author Manuscript
Another example is the PSA between the HPC and domain-specific posterior cortices during
encoding and retrieval. Consistent with the first criterion, activity in domain-specific cortices
has been associated with successful encoding of relevant stimuli and with the reactivation of
this information during retrieval (Danker & Anderson 2010). Second, HPC lesions yield
global memory deficits, whereas cortical damage yields domain-specific memory deficits.
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 24
Finally, the HPC and domain-specific cortices are anatomically linked, and there is evidence
Author Manuscript
that they are functionally connected during rest (Schlichting & Preston 2014) and during
successful encoding and retrieval processes (e.g., Ritchey et al. 2013) [Supplemental Figure
11 (follow the Supplemental Material link in the online version of this article or at http://
www.annualreviews.org/)].
Given that PSAs are rapidly assembled and disassembled, they require a mechanism that can
quickly control communication between distant brain regions, and the most likely candidate
is neuronal oscillations. There is abundant evidence that oscillations, particularly those in
gamma (40–100 Hz) and theta (4–8 Hz) frequencies, control PSAs during episodic encoding
and retrieval (Nyhus & Curran 2010). During encoding, gamma phase synchronization helps
bind perceptual features into objects in posterior cortices (Engel et al. 1991), and gamma-
theta coupling helps integrate objects into events in the HPC and posterior cortices ( Jensen
& Lisman 2005). In animal studies, HPC theta has been linked to successful spatial learning
Author Manuscript
(Ekstrom et al. 2001) and to long-term potentiation (Hyman et al. 2003). In human studies,
successful encoding has been associated with both gamma (Osipova et al. 2006) and theta
(Long et al. 2014) frequencies in the HPC and posterior cortices. The coupling is assumed to
be controlled by the PFC, and as WM load increases, theta power increases in frontal-
midline electroencephalography (EEG) electrodes (frontal-midline theta) ( Jensen & Tesche
2002). Moreover, an intracranial EEG study found phase-amplitude coupling between theta
and beta-gamma when faces were maintained in WM (Axmacher et al. 2010).
During retrieval, access to a fraction of the HPC representation of the original event is
assumed to cause reinstatement of the associated theta cycle, which triggers the
reinstatement of the nested gamma cycles and their associated cortical representations
( Jensen & Lisman 2005). In keeping with this hypothesis, human studies have found greater
Author Manuscript
gamma and theta (Osipova et al. 2006) power in the HPC and posterior cortices for correct
old responses than for new ones. There is evidence of greater gamma power for remember
than for know responses in several scalp locations (Burgess & Ali 2002). One of these
locations is the parietal cortex, consistent with the aforementioned PSA between the pHPC
and the VPC.
In sum, the HPC can only support episodic memory if it interacts very closely with other
regions. Each episodic process is mediated by a transient collaboration between regions
mediating complementary operations, or PSA, consistent with our component process
model. PSAs require a mechanism that can rapidly turn on and off interactions among
distant regions, such as neural oscillations in gamma and theta frequencies ( Johnson &
Knight 2015), and our understanding of these PSAs may help distinguish between recurrent
Author Manuscript
CONCLUSION
Since its inception, the scientific study of memory has been influenced by the research
program of its pioneers. Ebbinghaus [1964 (1885)] wished to study memory’s essence,
isolated from the influence of any other mental function. Bartlett (1932), on the other hand,
saw memory as inextricably tied to other mental functions, and often operating in their
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 25
whereas Bartlett used complex verbal and pictorial stimuli. Given Ebbinghaus’s memoranda,
memory could only be seen as strengthened or lost (forgotten). Memory for Bartlett’s
material, on the other hand, could also be transformed with time and experience to reflect
the cultural and personal schemas of the person. Memory is the only topic covered in
Ebbinghaus’s classical book, whereas Bartlett’s reads like a book in social psychology.
Indeed, Bartlett eschewed the word memory since it implied something static, preferring
instead to title his book Remembering to reflect an active, adaptive, and changing process
akin to other actions we perform. In this regard, his view was closer to that of James [1950
(1890)], who was concerned with the phenomenology of memory and had a functionalist
approach to psychology. Although this review has discussed elements of all three
perspectives, it has focused especially on Bartlett’s and James’s. At this time, it would be
surprising if it were otherwise, given how interactive brain regions are with one another and
Author Manuscript
how new technological advances drive this point home. We should take care, however, not to
lose sight of the trees while our attention is directed toward the interactive forest.
Supplementary Material
Refer to Web version on PubMed Central for supplementary material.
Acknowledgments
The writing of this article and some of the research reported herein were supported by grants to M.M. and G.W.
from the Canadian Institutes for Health Research (grant MGP 6694) and from the Natural Sciences and Engineering
Research Council of Canada; to L.N. from NIH, NSF, and the LuMind Foundation; and to R.C. from NIH-NIA
(AG19731). The authors gratefully acknowledge the support of the following people: Nick Hoang in preparing the
manuscript; Yadin Dudai, Jessica Robin, and Talya Sadeh for their helpful comments; and Dan Schacter for his
suggestions, encouragement, and patience.
Author Manuscript
LITERATURE CITED
Addis DR, Moscovitch M, Crawley AP, McAndrews MP. Recollective qualities modulate hippocampal
activation during autobiographical memory retrieval. Hippocampus. 2004; 14:752–62. [PubMed:
15318333]
Addis DR, Pan L, Musicaro R, Schacter DL. Divergent thinking and constructing episodic simulations.
Memory. 2015 In press.
Addis DR, Schacter DL. The hippocampus and imagining the future: Where do we stand? Front Hum
Neurosci. 2011; 5:173. [PubMed: 22291625]
Aggleton JP. Multiple anatomical systems embedded within the primate medial temporal lobe:
implications for hippocampal function. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012; 36:1579–96. [PubMed:
21964564]
Aly M, Ranganath C, Yonelinas AP. Detecting changes in scenes: The hippocampus is critical for
strength-based perception. Neuron. 2013; 78:1127–37. [PubMed: 23791201]
Author Manuscript
Anderson MC, Huddleston E. Towards a cognitive and neurobiological model of motivated forgetting.
Neb Symp Motiv. 2012; 58:53–120.
Andrews-Hanna JR. The brain’s default network and its adaptive role in internal mentation.
Neuroscientist. 2012; 18:251–70. [PubMed: 21677128]
Axmacher N, Henseler MM, Jensen O, Weinreich I, Elger CE, Fell J. Cross-frequency coupling
supports multi-item working memory in the human hippocampus. PNAS. 2010; 107:3228–33.
[PubMed: 20133762]
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 26
Baddeley A, Allen R, Vargha-Khadem F. Is the hippocampus necessary for visual and verbal binding
in working memory? Neuropsychologia. 2010; 48:1089–95. [PubMed: 20006631]
Author Manuscript
Bakker A, Kirwan CB, Miller M, Stark CE. Pattern separation in the human hippocampal CA3 and
dentate gyrus. Science. 2008; 319:1640–42. [PubMed: 18356518]
Bartlett, FC. Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge Univ. Press; 1932.
Bartsch T, Dohring J, Rohr A, Jansen O, Deuschl G. CA1 neurons in the human hippocampus are
critical for autobiographical memory, mental time travel, and autonoetic consciousness. PNAS.
2011; 108:17562–67. [PubMed: 21987814]
Baumann O, Mattingley JB. Dissociable representations of environmental size and complexity in the :
uman hippocampus. J Neurosci. 2013; 33:10526–33. [PubMed: 23785164]
Beadle JN, Tranel D, Cohen NJ, Duff MC. Empathy in hippocampal amnesia. Front Psychol. 2013;
4:69. [PubMed: 23526601]
Ben-Yakov A, Dudai Y. Constructing realistic engrams: Poststimulus activity of hippocampus and
dorsal striatum predicts subsequent episodic memory. J Neurosci. 2011; 31:9032–42. [PubMed:
21677186]
Author Manuscript
Bonnici HM, Chadwick MJ, Maguire EA. Representations of recent and remote autobiographical
memories in hippocampal subfields. Hippocampus. 2013; 23:849–54. [PubMed: 23749406]
Bowles B, Crupi C, Pigott S, Parrent A, Wiebe S, et al. Double dissociation of selective recollection
and familiarity impairments following two different surgical treatments for temporal-lobe epilepsy.
Neuropsychologia. 2010; 48:2640–47. [PubMed: 20466009]
Buckner RL. The role of the hippocampus in prediction and imagination. Annu Rev Psychol. 2010;
61:27–48. [PubMed: 19958178]
Burgess AP, Ali L. Functional connectivity of gamma EEG activity is modulated at low frequency
during conscious recollection. Intl J Psychophysiol. 2002; 46:91–100.
Buzsaki G, Moser EI. Memory, navigation and theta rhythm in the hippocampal-entorhinal system. Nat
Neurosci. 2013; 16:130–38. [PubMed: 23354386]
Cabeza R, Ciaramelli E, Moscovitch M. Cognitive contributions of the ventral parietal cortex: an
integrative theoretical account. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012; 16:338–52. [PubMed: 22609315]
Cabeza R, Moscovitch M. Memory systems, processing modes, and components: functional
Author Manuscript
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 27
Clark IA, Maguire EA. Remembering preservation in hippocampal amnesia. Annu Rev Psychol. 2016;
67:51–82. [PubMed: 26361051]
Cohn M, Moscovitch M, Lahat A, McAndrews MP. Recollection versus strength as the primary
determinant of hippocampal engagement at retrieval. PNAS. 2009; 106:22451–55. [PubMed:
20007783]
Conway MA. Episodic memories. Neuropsychologia. 2009; 47:2305–13. [PubMed: 19524094]
Corkin, S. Permanent Present Tense: The Unforgettable Life of the Amnesic Patient, H.M. New York:
Basic Books; 2013.
Coutanche MN, Thompson-Schill SL. Rapid consolidation of new knowledge in adulthood via fast
mapping. Trends Cogn Sci. 2015; 19:486–88. [PubMed: 26139618]
Craik, FIM. A functional account of age differences in memory. In: Klix, F.; Hagendorf, H., editors.
Human Memory and Cognitive Capabilities. Amsterdam: North-Holland; 1986. p. 409-22.
Dalla Barba G, La Corte V. The hippocampus, a time machine that makes errors. Trends Cogn Sci.
2013; 17:102–4. [PubMed: 23415077]
Author Manuscript
Danckert J, Ferber S, Pun C, Broderick C, Striemer C, et al. Neglected time: impaired temporal
perception of multisecond intervals in unilateral neglect. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007; 19:1706–20.
[PubMed: 17854283]
Danker JF, Anderson JR. The ghosts of brain states past: Remembering reactivates the brain regions
engaged during encoding. Psychol Bull. 2010; 136:87–102. [PubMed: 20063927]
Das SR, Mancuso L, Olson IR, Arnold SE, Wolk DA. Short-term memory depends on dissociable
medial temporal lobe regions in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Cereb Cortex. 2015 In press.
Daselaar SM, Fleck M, Dobbins IG, Madden DJ, Cabeza R. Effects of healthy aging on hippocampal
and rhinal memory functions: an event-related fMRI study. Cereb Cortex. 2006; 16:1771–82.
[PubMed: 16421332]
Davachi L, Dubrow S. How the hippocampus preserves order: the role of prediction and context.
Trends Cogn Sci. 2015; 19:92–99. [PubMed: 25600586]
Davidson PS, Anaki D, Ciaramelli E, Cohn M, Kim ASN, et al. Does parietal cortex support episodic
memory? Evidence from focal lesion patients. Neuropsychologia. 2008; 46:1743–55. [PubMed:
Author Manuscript
18313699]
Davidson PS, Drouin H, Kwan D, Moscovitch M, Rosenbaum RS. Memory as social glue: close
interpersonal relationships in amnesic patients. Front Psychol. 2012; 3:531. [PubMed: 23316176]
de Oliveira Alvares L, Einarsson EO, Santana F, Crestani AP, Haubrich J, et al. Periodically reactivated
context memory retains its precision and dependence on the hippocampus. Hippocampus. 2012;
22:1092–95. [PubMed: 22120981]
Dede AJ, Wixted JT, Hopkins RO, Squire LR. Hippocampal damage impairs recognition memory
broadly, affecting both parameters in two prominent models of memory. PNAS. 2013; 110:6577–
82. [PubMed: 23576763]
Diekelmann S, Born J. The memory function of sleep. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010; 11:114–26. [PubMed:
20046194]
Dudai Y. The restless engram: Consolidations never end. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2012; 35:227–47.
[PubMed: 22443508]
Duff MC, Brown-Schmidt S. The hippocampus and the flexible use and processing of language. Front
Author Manuscript
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 28
Ekstrom AD, Meltzer J, McNaughton BL, Barnes CA. NMDA receptor antagonism blocks experience-
dependent expansion of hippocampal “place fields. Neuron. 2001; 31:631–38. [PubMed:
Author Manuscript
11545721]
Engel AK, Kreiter AK, Konig P, Singer W. Synchronization of oscillatory neuronal responses between
striate and extrastriate visual cortical areas of the cat. PNAS. 1991; 88:6048–52. [PubMed:
2068083]
Evensmoen HR, Ladstein J, Hansen TI, Moller JA, Witter MP, et al. From details to large scale: The
representation of environmental positions follows a granularity gradient along the human
hippocampal and entorhinal anterior-posterior axis. Hippocampus. 2015; 25:119–35. [PubMed:
25155295]
Frankland PW, Bontempi B. The organization of recent and remote memories. Nat Rev Neurosci.
2005; 6:119–30. [PubMed: 15685217]
Furman O, Mendelsohn A, Dudai Y. The episodic engram transformed: Time reduces retrieval-related
brain activity but correlates it with memory accuracy. Learn Mem. 2012; 19:575–87. [PubMed:
23154929]
Gaesser B, Schacter DL. Episodic simulation and episodic memory can increase intentions to help
Author Manuscript
mental navigation for a recently learned, but not a highly familiar environment: a longitudinal
fMRI study. Hippocampus. 2012; 22:842–52. [PubMed: 21584904]
Howard MW, Eichenbaum H. The hippocampus, time, and memory across scales. J Exp Psychol: Gen.
2013; 142:1211–30. [PubMed: 23915126]
Howard LR, Javadi AH, Yu Y, Mill RD, Morrison LC, et al. The hippocampus and entorhinal cortex
encode the path and Euclidean distances to goals during navigation. Curr Biol. 2014; 24:1331–40.
[PubMed: 24909328]
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 29
Hower KH, Wixted J, Berryhill ME, Olson IR. Impaired perception of mnemonic oldness, but not
mnemonic newness, after parietal lobe damage. Neuropsychologia. 2014; 56:409–17. [PubMed:
Author Manuscript
24565734]
Hunsaker MR, Kesner RP. The operation of pattern separation and pattern completion processes
associated with different attributes or domains of memory. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013; 37:36–
58. [PubMed: 23043857]
Hyman JM, Wyble BP, Goyal V, Rossi CA, Hasselmo ME. Stimulation in hippocampal region CA1 in
behaving rats yields long-term potentiation when delivered to the peak of theta and long-term
depression when delivered to the trough. J Neurosci. 2003; 23:11725–31. [PubMed: 14684874]
James, W. Principles of Psychology. Vol. 1. New York: Dover; 1950 (1890).
Jensen O, Lisman JE. Hippocampal sequence-encoding driven by a cortical multi-item working
memory buffer. Trends Neurosci. 2005; 28:67–72. [PubMed: 15667928]
Jensen O, Tesche CD. Frontal theta activity in humans increases with memory load in a working
memory task. Eur J Neurosci. 2002; 15:1395–99. [PubMed: 11994134]
Johnson EL, Knight RT. Intracranial recordings and human memory. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2015;
31:18–25. [PubMed: 25113154]
Author Manuscript
Josselyn SA, Kohler S, Frankland PW. Finding the engram. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015; 16:521–34.
[PubMed: 26289572]
Kandel ER, Dudai Y, Mayford MR. The molecular and systems biology of memory. Cell. 2014;
157:163–86. [PubMed: 24679534]
Kim H. Encoding and retrieval along the long axis of the hippocampus and their relationships with
dorsal attention and default mode networks: the HERNET model. Hippocampus. 2015; 25(4):500–
10. [PubMed: 25367784]
Kim J, Yassa MA. Assessing recollection and familiarity of similar lures in a behavioral pattern
separation task. Hippocampus. 2013; 23:287–94. [PubMed: 23401187]
Kim S, Dede AJO, Hopkins RO, Squire LR. Memory, scene construction, and the human
hippocampus. PNAS. 2015; 112:4767–72. [PubMed: 25825712]
Kopelman MD, Wilson BA, Baddeley AD. The autobiographical memory interview: a new assessment
of autobiographical and personal semantic memory in amnesic patients. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol.
1989; 11:724–44. [PubMed: 2808661]
Author Manuscript
Kwan D, Craver CF, Green L, Myerson J, Rosenbaum RS. Dissociations in future thinking following
hippocampal damage: evidence from discounting and time perspective in episodic amnesia. J Exp
Psychol: Gen. 2013; 142:1355–69. [PubMed: 23978187]
Lee AC, Yeung LK, Barense MD. The hippocampus and visual perception. Front Hum Neurosci. 2012;
6:91. [PubMed: 22529794]
Levine B, Svoboda E, Hay JF, Winocur G, Moscovitch M. Aging and autobiographical memory:
dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval. Psychol Aging. 2002; 17:677–89. [PubMed:
12507363]
Levine B, Svoboda E, Turner GR, Mandic M, Mackey A. Behavioral and functional neuroanatomical
correlates of anterograde autobiographical memory in isolated retrograde amnesic patient M.L.
Neuropsychologia. 2009; 47:2188–96. [PubMed: 19154750]
Lewis PA, Durrant SJ. Overlapping memory replay during sleep builds cognitive schemata. Trends
Cogn Sci. 2011; 15:343–51. [PubMed: 21764357]
Long NM, Burke JF, Kahana MJ. Subsequent memory effect in intracranial and scalp EEG.
NeuroImage. 2014; 84:488–94. [PubMed: 24012858]
Author Manuscript
Madore KP, Addis DR, Schacter DL. Creativity and memory: effects of an episodic-specificity
induction on divergent thinking. Psychol Sci. 2015; 26:1461–68. [PubMed: 26205963]
Madore KP, Schacter DL. An episodic specificity induction enhances means-end problem solving in
young and older adults. Psychol Aging. 2014; 29:913–24. [PubMed: 25365688]
Maguire EA, Intraub H, Mullally SL. Scenes, spaces, and memory traces: What does the hippocampus
do? Neuroscientist. 2015 In press.
Maguire EA, Mullally SL. The hippocampus: a manifesto for change. J Exp Psychol: Gen. 2013;
142:1180–89. [PubMed: 23855494]
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 30
Malykhin NV, Lebel RM, Coupland NJ, Wilman AH, Carter R. In vivo quantification of hippocampal
subfields using 4.7 T fast spin echo imaging. NeuroImage. 2010; 49:1224–30. [PubMed:
Author Manuscript
19786104]
McClelland JL, McNaughton BL, O’Reilly RC. Why there are complementary learning systems in the
hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of
learning and memory. Psychol Rev. 1995; 102:419–57. [PubMed: 7624455]
McCormick C, St-Laurent M, Ty A, Valiante TA, McAndrews MP. Functional and effective
hippocampal-neocortical connectivity during construction and elaboration of autobiographical
memory retrieval. Cereb Cortex. 2013; 25:1297–305. [PubMed: 24275829]
Merhav M, Karni A, Gilboa A. Neocortical catastrophic interference in healthy and amnesic adults: a
paradoxical matter of time. Hippocampus. 2014; 24(12):1653–62. [PubMed: 25154723]
Merhav M, Karni A, Gilboa A. Not all declarative memories are created equal: fast mapping as a direct
route to cortical declarative representations. NeuroImage. 2015; 117:80–92. [PubMed:
25988227]
Migo EM, Mayes AR, Montaldi D. Measuring recollection and familiarity: improving the remember/
know procedure. Conscious Cogn. 2012; 21:1435–55. [PubMed: 22846231]
Author Manuscript
Miller JF, Neufang M, Solway A, Brandt A, Trippel M, et al. Neural activity in human hippocampal
formation reveals the spatial context of retrieved memories. Science. 2013; 342:1111–14.
[PubMed: 24288336]
Mitchell JP. Inferences about mental states. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B. 2009; 364:1309–16. [PubMed:
19528012]
Montaldi D, Mayes AR. Familiarity, recollection and medial temporal lobe function: an unresolved
issue. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011; 15:339–40. [PubMed: 21752696]
Morgan LK, Macevoy SP, Aguirre GK, Epstein RA. Distances between real-world locations are
represented in the human hippocampus. J Neurosci. 2011; 31:1238–45. [PubMed: 21273408]
Moscovitch DA, Chiupka CA, Gavric DL. Within the mind’s eye: Negative mental imagery activates
different emotion regulation strategies in high versus low socially anxious individuals. J Behav
Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2013; 44:426–32. [PubMed: 23787175]
Moscovitch M. Memory and working-with-memory: a component process model based on modules
and central systems. J Cogn Neurosci. 1992; 4:257–67. [PubMed: 23964882]
Author Manuscript
Relevance to Learning and Memory. Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press; 2008. p. 3-15.
Nadel L, Hoscheidt S, Ryan LR. Spatial cognition and the hippocampus: the anterior-posterior axis. J
Cogn Neurosci. 2013; 25:22–28. [PubMed: 23198887]
Nadel L, Moscovitch M. Memory consolidation, retrograde amnesia and the hippocampal complex.
Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1997; 7:217–27. [PubMed: 9142752]
Nadel L, Peterson MA. The hippocampus: part of an interactive posterior representational system
spanning perceptual and memorial systems. J Exp Psychol: Gen. 2013; 142:1242–54. [PubMed:
23895347]
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 31
Nader K, Hardt O. A single standard for memory: the case for reconsolidation. Nat Rev Neurosci.
2009; 10:224–34. [PubMed: 19229241]
Author Manuscript
Nichols EA, Kao YC, Verfaellie M, Gabrieli JD. Working memory and long-term memory for faces:
evidence from fMRI and global amnesia for involvement of the medial temporal lobes.
Hippocampus. 2006; 16:604–16. [PubMed: 16770797]
Nieuwenhuis IL, Takashima A. The role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in memory
consolidation. Behav Brain Res. 2011; 218:325–34. [PubMed: 21147169]
Norman KA. How hippocampus and cortex contribute to recognition memory: revisiting the
complementary learning systems model. Hippocampus. 2010; 20:1217–27. [PubMed: 20857486]
Nyhus E, Curran T. Functional role of gamma and theta oscillations in episodic memory. Neurosci
Biobehav Rev. 2010; 34:1023–35. [PubMed: 20060015]
O’Keefe, J.; Nadel, L. The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press; 1978.
Olsen RK, Moses SN, Riggs L, Ryan JD. The hippocampus supports multiple cognitive processes
through relational binding and comparison. Front Hum Neurosci. 2012; 6:146. [PubMed:
22661938]
Olson IR, Page K, Moore KS, Chatterjee A, Verfaellie M. Working memory for conjunctions relies on
Author Manuscript
Pertzov Y, Miller TD, Gorgoraptis N, Caine D, Schott JM, et al. Binding deficits in memory following
medial temporal lobe damage in patients with voltage-gated potassium channel complex
antibody-associated limbic encephalitis. Brain. 2013; 136:2474–85. [PubMed: 23757763]
Piolino P, Desgranges B, Eustache F. Episodic autobiographical memories over the course of time:
cognitive, neuropsychological and neuroimaging findings. Neuropsychologia. 2009; 47:2314–29.
[PubMed: 19524095]
Poppenk J, Evensmoen HR, Moscovitch M, Nadel L. Long-axis specialization of the human
hippocampus. Trends Cogn Sci. 2013; 17:230–40. [PubMed: 23597720]
Poppenk J, Moscovitch M. A hippocampal marker of recollection memory ability among healthy
young adults: contributions of posterior and anterior segments. Neuron. 2011; 72:931–37.
[PubMed: 22196329]
Prebble SC, Addis DR, Tippett LJ. Autobiographical memory and sense of self. Psychol Bull. 2013;
139:815–40. [PubMed: 23025923]
Preston AR, Eichenbaum H. Interplay of hippocampus and prefrontal cortex in memory. Curr Biol.
2013; 23:R764–73. [PubMed: 24028960]
Author Manuscript
Quamme JR, Yonelinas AP, Norman KA. Effect of unitization on associative recognition in amnesia.
Hippocampus. 2007; 17:192–200. [PubMed: 17203466]
Quiroga RQ. Concept cells: the building blocks of declarative memory functions. Nat Rev Neurosci.
2012; 13:587–97. [PubMed: 22760181]
Race E, Keane MM, Verfaellie M. Sharing mental simulations and stories: hippocampal contributions
to discourse integration. Cortex. 2015; 63:271–81. [PubMed: 25303274]
Ranganath C, Ritchey M. Two cortical systems for memory-guided behaviour. Nat Rev Neurosci.
2012; 13:713–26. [PubMed: 22992647]
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 32
Reagh ZM, Watabe J, Ly M, Murray E, Yassa MA. Dissociated signals in human dentate gyrus and
CA3 predict different facets of recognition memory. J Neurosci. 2014; 34:13301–13. [PubMed:
Author Manuscript
25274810]
Reber TP, Henke K. Integrating unseen events over time. Conscious Cogn. 2012; 21:953–60.
[PubMed: 22425537]
Renoult L, Davidson PS, Palombo DJ, Moscovitch M, Levine B. Personal semantics: at the crossroads
of semantic and episodic memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012; 16:550–58. [PubMed: 23040159]
Renoult L, Davidson PS, Schmitz E, Park L, Campbell K, et al. Autobiographically significant
concepts: more episodic than semantic in nature? An electrophysiological investigation of
overlapping types of memory. J Cogn Neurosci. 2015; 27:57–72. [PubMed: 25061931]
Ritchey M, Montchal ME, Yonelinas AP, Ranganath C. Delay-dependent contributions of medial
temporal lobe regions to episodic memory retrieval. eLife. 2015; 4:e05025.
Ritchey M, Wing EA, LaBar KS, Cabeza R. Neural similarity between encoding and retrieval is related
to memory via hippocampal interactions. Cereb Cortex. 2013; 23:2818–28. [PubMed: 22967731]
Robin J, Hirshhorn M, Rosenbaum RS, Winocur G, Moscovitch M, Grady CL. Functional connectivity
of hippocampal and prefrontal networks during episodic and spatial memory based on real-world
Author Manuscript
Rosenbaum RS, Priselac S, Köhler S, Black SE, Gao F, et al. Remote spatial memory in an amnesic
person with extensive bilateral hippocampal lesions. Nat Neurosci. 2000; 3:1044–48. [PubMed:
11017178]
Rozin P, Dow S, Moscovitch M, Rajaram S. What causes humans to begin and end a meal? A role for
memory for what has been eaten, as evidenced by a study of multiple meal eating in amnesic
patients. Psychol Sci. 1998; 9:392–96.
Rubin DC, Umanath S. Event memory: a theory of memory for laboratory, autobiographical, and
fictional events. Psychol Rev. 2015; 122:1–23. [PubMed: 25330330]
Rubin RD, Watson PD, Duff MC, Cohen NJ. The role of the hippocampus in flexible cognition and
social behavior. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014; 8:742. [PubMed: 25324753]
Rugg MD, Vilberg KL. Brain networks underlying episodic memory retrieval. Curr Opin Neurobiol.
2013; 23:255–60. [PubMed: 23206590]
Ryan JD, Moses SN, Barense M, Rosenbaum RS. Intact learning of new relations in amnesia as
achieved through unitization. J Neurosci. 2013; 33:9601–13. [PubMed: 23739957]
Sadeh T, Maril A, Goshen-Gottstein Y. Encoding-related brain activity dissociates between the
Author Manuscript
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 33
Schnider, A. The Confabulating Mind: How the Brain Creates Reality. Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ.
Press; 2008.
Author Manuscript
Shallice T, Cooper R. The organisation of mind. Cortex. 2012; 48:1366–70. [PubMed: 23040241]
Sharon T, Moscovitch M, Gilboa A. Rapid neocortical acquisition of long-term arbitrary associations
independent of the hippocampus. PNAS. 2011; 108:1146–51. [PubMed: 21199935]
Sheldon S, Levine B. Same as it ever was: Vividness modulates the similarities and differences
between the neural networks that support retrieving remote and recent autobiographical
memories. NeuroImage. 2013; 83:880–91. [PubMed: 23845428]
Sheldon S, McAndrews MP, Moscovitch M. Episodic memory processes mediated by the medial
temporal lobes contribute to open-ended problem solving. Neuropsychologia. 2011; 49:2439–47.
[PubMed: 21550352]
Sheldon S, Moscovitch M. The nature and time-course of medial temporal lobe contributions to
semantic retrieval: an fMRI study on verbal fluency. Hippocampus. 2012; 22:1451–66. [PubMed:
22102548]
Sheldon S, Romero K, Moscovitch M. Medial temporal lobe amnesia impairs performance on a free
association task. Hippocampus. 2013; 23:405–12. [PubMed: 23436387]
Author Manuscript
Sheldon SA, Moscovitch M. Recollective performance advantages for implicit memory tasks. Memory.
2010; 18:681–97. [PubMed: 20721805]
Shohamy D, Wagner AD. Integrating memories in the human brain: hippocampal-midbrain encoding
of overlapping events. Neuron. 2008; 60:378–89. [PubMed: 18957228]
Simons J, Peers P, Mazuz Y, Berryhill M, Olson I. Dissociation between memory accuracy and
memory confidence following bilateral parietal lesions. Cereb Cortex. 2010; 20:479–85.
[PubMed: 19542474]
Simons JS, Spiers HJ. Prefrontal and medial temporal lobe interactions in long-term memory. Nat Rev
Neurosci. 2003; 4:637–48. [PubMed: 12894239]
Skinner EI, Fernandes MA. Neural correlates of recollection and familiarity: a review of neuroimaging
and patient data. Neuropsychologia. 2007; 45:2163–79. [PubMed: 17445844]
Smith CN, Jeneson, Frascino JC, Kirwan CB, Hopkins RO, Squire LR. When recognition memory is
independent of hippocampal function. PNAS. 2014a; 111:9935–40. [PubMed: 24958865]
Smith CN, Urgolites ZJ, Hopkins RO, Squire LR. Comparison of explicit and incidental learning
Author Manuscript
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.
Moscovitch et al. Page 34
Talmi D, Grady CL, Goshen-Gottstein Y, Moscovitch M. Neuroimaging the serial position curve: a test
of single-store versus dual-store models. Psychol Sci. 2005; 16:716–23. [PubMed: 16137258]
Author Manuscript
Tambini A, Ketz N, Davachi L. Enhanced brain correlations during rest are related to memory for
recent experiences. Neuron. 2010; 65:280–90. [PubMed: 20152133]
Tanaka KZ, Pevzner A, Hamidi AB, Nakazawa Y, Graham J, Wiltgen BJ. Cortical representations are
reinstated by the hippocampus during memory retrieval. Neuron. 2014; 84(2):347–54. [PubMed:
25308331]
Teyler TJ, Rudy JW. The hippocampal indexing theory and episodic memory: updating the index.
Hippocampus. 2007; 17:1158–69. [PubMed: 17696170]
Tonegawa S, Pignatelli M, Roy DS, Ryan TJ. Memory engram storage and retrieval. Curr Opin
Neurobiol. 2015; 35:101–9. [PubMed: 26280931]
Tse D, Langston RF, Kakeyama M, Bethus I, Spooner PA, et al. Schemas and memory consolidation.
Science. 2007; 316:76–82. [PubMed: 17412951]
Tulving, E. Elements of Episodic Memory. Oxford, UK: Clarendon; 1983.
Tulving E. Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002; 53:1–25. [PubMed:
11752477]
Author Manuscript
van Kesteren MT, Ruiter DJ, Fernandez G, Henson RN. How schema and novelty augment memory
formation. Trends Neurosci. 2012; 35:211–19. [PubMed: 22398180]
Verfaellie M, Bousquet K, Keane MM. Medial temporal and neocortical contributions to remote
memory for semantic narratives: evidence from amnesia. Neuropsychologia. 2014; 61:105–12.
[PubMed: 24953960]
Viard A, Desgranges B, Eustache F, Piolino P. Factors affecting medial temporal lobe engagement for
past and future episodic events: an ALE meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Brain Cogn.
2012; 80:111–25. [PubMed: 22705648]
Viskontas IV, Carr VA, Engel SA, Knowlton BJ. The neural correlates of recollection: Hippocampal
activation declines as episodic memory fades. Hippocampus. 2009a; 19:265–72. [PubMed:
18830998]
Viskontas IV, Quiroga RQ, Fried I. Human medial temporal lobe neurons respond preferentially to
personally relevant images. PNAS. 2009b; 106:21329–34. [PubMed: 19955441]
Wang SH, Morris RG. Hippocampal-neocortical interactions in memory formation, consolidation, and
Author Manuscript
memory and long-term memory. Behav Brain Res. 2013; 254:34–44. [PubMed: 23721964]
Zola-Morgan S, Squire LR, Amaral DG. Human amnesia and the medial temporal region: enduring
memory impairment following a bilateral lesion limited to field CA1 of the hippocampus. J
Neurosci. 1986; 6:2950–67. [PubMed: 3760943]
Annu Rev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.