The Gestalt of The Self in Gestalt Thera
The Gestalt of The Self in Gestalt Thera
abstract | The concept of the “self” in Gestalt therapy has not, to the
author’s mind, been fully developed, making it a “heterogenous introject,” an
amalgam of theoretical concepts from different scientific sources. This may
explain why some therapists of the first generation have not even attempted
to define “self” at all, while others have tried to develop this concept in vari-
ous directions. As a consequence, to date no concise gestalt of the “self” has
emerged. The author explores some current philosophical approaches to the
self as well as recent scientific research to have a more precise idea of how
this concept can be understood. By taking into account the central concept of
“gestalt,” we can gain more clarity regarding the fundamental characteristics of
Gestalt therapy. The author concludes with a basic structure for a new config-
uration of theory.
Starting Point
physics, psychology, philosophy, and the arts. Thus, Gestalt therapy can-
not be reduced to its “postmodern” aspects. Frederick Perls’s (b)
reminder that “there is no end to integration” () must be first applied
to the competing styles of today’s Gestalt therapy. Indeed, integration
should neither be confused with a pure addition of parts, nor with an
introjection of new elements. We may need to consult or develop con-
cepts on a level of higher complexity in Wertheimer’s () sense.
The Gestalt therapy concepts of the self are an amalgam of terminolo-
gies from at least three sources: psychoanalysis, Gestalt psychology/field
theory, and functionalism and pragmatism. There is little coherence. In
fact, we find a wide range of views expressed in a collection of articles
edited by Robine (), in which leading Gestalt therapists reveal that
we are currently witnessing the flourishing of a still greater range of con-
ceptual variations. A coherent conceptualization of subjectivity is there-
fore not yet in sight. Consequently, we still have not reached a “holistic
foundational theory of human nature,” as Kroschel () has pointed
out. The splitting of the human being into mind and body underlies this
theoretical deficit and is characteristic of Western culture. Descartes is
erroneously considered to be its originator, though what he implicitly
meant with his “cogito, ergo sum” was “I think, and that is why I know
that I exist.” His goal was to reassure himself; thus, as a philosophical
topic, he established self-awareness as the ultimate and indisputable
certainty (Gabriel ). He was trying to capture the prevailing reli-
gious view of the earthly Leib [body] and the transcendent soul or mind
by defining them as two different substances, matter and mind, which
interact. Many thinkers have since been troubled by this dualism.
The central theoretical question is: “How does the interaction between
mind and body work?” Regarded in a historical context, one finds the
European reflection on the “I” and finally on the “self ” embedded in a
struggle—between Greek philosophy, Christian dogmatics, philosophy
since the Enlightenment and, finally, scientific explanations—to develop
a valid view of human nature. The “self ” denotes a phenomenon that
mediates between the mental/psychological and the physical.
Freud () argues:
science, not even the most exact, begins with such definitions. The
true beginning of scientific activity consists rather in describing
phenomena and then in proceeding to group, classify, and cor-
relate them. Even at the stage of description it is not possible to
avoid applying certain abstract ideas to the material in hand, ideas
derived from somewhere or other but certainly not from the new
observations alone. ()
Overall Assessment
Corollary
Sources
Freud’s concepts of ego, id, and superego are, like the “self,”
reifications and, as such, confusing. Their history is confusing, too.
Nitzschke (), referring to Freud (), researched the history
of the concept of the “id”; what follows is a summary of what he
discovered. Psychoanalysts claim as a fact that Freud introduced
the concept of the “id” in , along with the concepts of “ego” and
“superego,” and they attribute the evolution of the “id” to Nietzsche.
Inspired by Schopenhauer, Nietzsche () speaks of the “self ” in
Zarathustra: “Behind thy thoughts and feelings, my brother, there is
a mighty lord, an unknown sage—it is called Self; it dwelleth in thy
body, it is thy body” (). Groddeck (/) attributed the “It”
in the title of his book, The Book of the It, to the body, describing
it in a psychophysical sense as something wonderful that regulates
everything the body does and what happens to it. He sent that book
to Freud, who never cited him but published his own book, The
Ego and the Id, in the same year (Freud /), describing the
“id” as a continuation of the ego into the instinctual. I agree with
Nitzschke’s (, ) argument that Freud took his concept from
the philosopher Hartmann (), who reformulated Nietzsche and
radically abbreviated Schopenhauer’s notion of “It is thinking in
us,” calling it the “id,” which he thought lay in the unconscious. This
concept is far from Nietzsche’s concept in Zarathustra of “mighty
lord, an unknown sage,” referred to above. This is confirmed by Jeron
(, ff.), for example, who describes the Freudian concept of
“the id” as “blind (!) instinct,” with drive energy and physiochemical
processes as its basis; and “the ego” as an “adaptive apparatus” (my
translation). This view of the nature of the human being dates back
to the nineteenth century; the sharpest criticism of such concepts
has come from within psychoanalysis’s own ranks. Pohlen and
42 | Gestalt Review
Damasio () states: “Last, naturalizing the mind may solve one mys-
tery but only to raise the curtain on other mysteries quietly awaiting
their turn. Placing the construction of conscious minds in the history
of biology and culture opens the way to reconciling traditional human-
ism and modern science” (). The philosopher Nagel () considers
it urgently necessary to solve the body–mind problem by adopting a
new worldview. Many approaches have already been developed since
Ehrenfels (), a precursor of Gestalt psychology, Einstein, and James.
Nagel repeats one of Kant’s theses, namely that the intelligibility of this
world (as a basis for all sciences) is not an accident. The characteris-
tics of nature lead to the development of beings who are endowed with
reason and can therefore understand (see the “anthropic principle” in
the section on philosophy, above). Descriptions of the world as a whole
must contain a description of ourselves and must also be examined by
the natural sciences. During the “postmodern revolution,” there was the
risk that this connection to the sciences might be lost. As Milich ()
has shown, early postmodernism emerged from a European-American
cultural conflict in New York in the s. The exaggerated opposing
of culture and technological progress later led to the development of a
front between cultural studies and the natural sciences. This obscured
the possibility of an integrative concept.
The Gestalt of the “Self” in Gestalt Therapy | 47
The philosopher Metzinger (, ) argues that the concept of subjec-
tive experience has replaced the concept of the self. He notes that con-
sciousness became a recognized goal of research only in the last decade
of the twentieth century, and that the natural sciences and philosophy
are trying to work together on this issue. He finds this remarkable,
because there is a fear of reductionism in the humanities, and a fear of
insoluble mysteries in the natural sciences. In brain research, conscious-
ness is defined as the cooperation between the brain and the whole body.
Damasio () addresses feelings, so often broadly neglected in
theories: “[T]he first and most elementary product of the proto-self
is primordial feelings, which occur spontaneously and continuously
whenever one is awake. They provide a direct experience of one’s own
living body, wordless, unadorned, and connected to nothing but sheer
48 | Gestalt Review
existence. . . . [T]hey originate at the level of the brain stem rather than
the cerebral cortex” (). Furthermore: “These feelings accomplish a
distinction between self and non-self. They are, in a nutshell, feelings
of knowing. We shall see that the construction of a conscious mind
depends, at several stages, on the generation of such feelings” (; empha-
sis in original). Damasio also emphasizes the importance of subjectivity:
“A knower, by whatever name one may want to call it—self, experiencer,
protagonist—needs to be generated in the brain if the mind is to become
conscious. When the brain manages to introduce a knower in the mind,
subjectivity follows” (). These points lead directly back to the results
described above, in the section on philosophy.
Awareness means that we observe ourselves as we observe. We
observe and reflect on what we feel physically and emotionally, and what
we think in the same moment. The prefrontal cortex strives to find fixed
“patterns”—here again we are talking about a “gestalt”—and to develop
plans. The prefrontal cortex reconciles motivations and is involved in the
learned control of congenital behaviors. It subordinates limbic behav-
ioral programs to specific overarching goals, for example, adaptive social
behavior. This is a clear indication of the top-down effect of wholes on
their parts (see the section below, on “Levels of Complexity”). The activ-
ities of the brain are, according to Damasio, shaped by the brain itself,
the body, the natural world, and human culture. When they change, new
neural structures are created.
Perhaps it is time to formulate a clear idea of what “psyche” can mean,
namely the stream of individual consciousness, the control of all the
life processes of the entire organism, and the creative adaptation to the
physical and social conditions of the surrounding environment. At any
given point in time, this environment must be understood as a hierarchy
of levels as illustrated in Uexküll’s () integrative model (see below).
Even in regard to plants, the environment cannot be viewed as purely
“biologistic,” that is, physiological. Even at this basic level, information
plays an important role. For example, certain plants use scents to warn
conspecifics about their natural enemies. This potential is even more
evident in animals that live in communities, and in humans who are
highly cooperative and have developed complex social structures. The
complex laws of society, which regulate cooperation and define priori-
ties for the collective, apply not only at the human level but also at the
level of living beings.
The Gestalt of the “Self” in Gestalt Therapy | 49
T. Uexküll and Wesiack () state that hardly anything in the history
of science has proved to be as tenacious as mechanistic or machine-
oriented thinking (see Höll ). In psychology, that way of thinking
seemed to have been overcome by Gestalt theory, but it returned all the
more powerfully in the form of computer metaphors. It still plays an
important role in this kind of science. Uexküll and Wesiack advocate a
holistic perspective for psychosomatics, considering that only this per-
spective can render it possible to understand such phenomena as order
and regularity in nature. Incidentally, it was a concern of Fritz Perls that
Gestalt therapy should be understood as the basis of a holistic world-
view. He therefore considered that the humanities and the natural sci-
ences should be integrated. This view was partly motivated by Smuts’s
() volume, Holism and Evolution.
If there were only matter and energy, these phenomena could only
have arisen by chance. This, however, stands in contradiction to all prob-
ability. Building on the work of J. Uexküll (a Baltic German biologist who
worked in the fields of muscular physiology, animal behavior studies,
and the cybernetics of life), T. Uexküll and Wesiack () worked to
overcome the division of the Western worldview into the arts and lit-
erature on the one hand, and technology and the natural sciences on
the other. They suggest that this fruitless dualism be replaced by levels
of complexity, that is, the complexity of the specific laws of a certain
level. Soma, psyche, group, society, and culture are seen as a hierarchy
of levels in which each level is the influencing and integrating environ-
ment for the wholes of the less complex level. Thus, there are top-down
and bottom-up influences. The levels of human complexity according to
Uexküll and Wesiack are as follows:
culture
society
group
psyche
soma
from them. From the theory of data transmission, J. Uexküll, who is seen
as the “father of psychosomatics,” concluded together with the German
physician, V. Weizsaecker (–), that information theory prom-
ised in “a subtle way” to reinterpret the life processes which J. Uexküll
captured in his “situation circle” model. The meaning of a message as
its intrinsic quality cannot be reduced to its material base. Once one
has followed this line of thought, one can only wonder not to have seen
it before (under the influence of the materialistic worldview). In our
thoughts, we are always moving in this immaterial sphere. Ehrenfels
() demonstrated the same phenomenon in the transposition of
melodies, which he then used to introduce the concept of the gestalt.
Thinking means building structures and creating connections to reduce
redundancy and thus also the amount of information. This is dealt with
by information theory. The more disordered a message, the more bits
one needs to represent it.
• Psychoanalysis: sexuality, talking cure, defense mechanisms;
Reich: muscular resistance; Gindler/Selver: sensory
awareness; Rogers: empathy; Moreno: role-playing through
chair work.
• Figure–ground-formation; Self-organization; Contact
cycle; Organism–environment field; Awareness; Here and
now; Theory of feelings; Polarity conscious/unconscious;
Polarity of motivations; Dialogical therapeutic relationship;
Pathology as creative adjustment; Understanding through
identification; Artistic media as a holistic mode of
expression.
/
• Theory of levels of complexity; Psyche and soma obey
overarching laws.
/
• Subjectivity of the cells; Hierarchy of the levels; Goldstein:
organism–environment field, self-actualization.
• Laws of Newtonian physics.
()
• As a possible complement? The long sought for foundation
of a theory of consciousness?
Thesis : If we combine the above with the concept of the Gestalt, the
result is as follows: “Gestalt” is synonymous with information that is
sorted and thus reduced. Because of the necessity to bring order into
complexity, understood here as a disordered profusion of information,
our brains make a lot of processes work. They do so by using innate
information processing structures, which are continually further devel-
oped over the course of the lifespan, to create order and connections. In
other words, they create gestalts. In this context, we can give the gestalt
concept a new, fundamental meaning, since “gestalt” is synonymous
with sorted information. “Gestalt” denotes a perceived arrangement of
material elements or waves in the form of sounds, colors, or any other
elements of perceiving, feeling, and thinking, to which the brain auton-
omously tries to give meaning.
At present, to give this concept public recognition requires a major
revolution in thinking. Perhaps without the digital revolution, it would
hardly have a chance even today. Floods of information, however, are
now being sent around the globe, with the respective media playing
only a subordinate role. From there it is only a small step to abandon
the fiction of the smallest particles as the basic principle of the cosmos.
Nowadays, we can even read on the web that information is the raw
material of the future.
acknowledgments
An alternate version of this article appeared in German as “Die Gestalt des ‘Selbst’ in
der Gestalttherapie” (Gestalttherapie: Forum für Gestaltperspektiven : –), .
The English translation is by the author, who is grateful to Deirdre Winter and Nancy
Amendt-Lyon for their support. The author is also obliged to Susan L. Fischer for her
careful attention to details of clarity and reference attribution in the present article.
references
Blankertz, Stefan. . Kritischer Pragmatismus. Zur Soziologie Paul Goodmans
[Critical Pragmatism. Paul Goodman’s Sociology]. Wetzlar: Büchse der Pandora.
Bocian, Bernd. . “Von der Revision der Freudschen Theorie und Methode zum
Entwurf der Gestalttherapie: Grundlegendes zu einem Figur-Hintergrund-
Verhältnis” [“From the Revision of Freud’s Theory and Method to the Design
of Gestalt Therapy: Understanding a Figure-Background Relationship”]. In
Gestalttherapie und Psychoanalyse, edited by Bernd Bocian and Frank-M.
Staemmler, –. Göttingen: Vandenhoeck & Rupprecht.
Damasio, Antonio R. . Self Comes to Mind. Constructing the Conscious Brain. New
York: Pantheon Books.
Dewey, John. . “Art as Experience.” In John Dewey: The Later Works, –. Vol.
, edited by Jo Ann Boydston. Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press,
original work published .
———. . “The Reflex Arc Concept in Psychology.” Psychological Review (): –.
The Gestalt of the “Self” in Gestalt Therapy | 57
Ehrenfels, Christian von. . “Über Gestaltqualitäten” [“On the Qualities of Form”].
In Christian von Ehrenfels: Leben und Werk, edited by Reinhard Fabian, –.
Amsterdam and Atlanta: Rodopi, original work published .
Foucault, Michel. . The Hermeneutics of the Subject: Lectures at the College de
France –. London: Picador-Macmillan.
Francesetti, Gianni, Micela Gecele, and Jan Roubal, eds. . Gestalt Therapy in
Clinical Practice. From Psychopathology to the Aesthetics of Contact. Milan:
FrancoAngeli.
Freud, Sigmund. . Instincts and their Vicissitudes (The Standard Edition of the
Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud). Vol. (–): On the
History of the Psycho-Analytic Moment, Papers on Metapsychology and Other
Works. New York: W.W. Norton, –.
———. . The Ego and the Id (The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological
Works of Sigmund Freud), edited by James Strachey. New York: W.W. Norton,
original work published .
Friedlaender, Salomo. . Schöpferische Indifferenz [Creative Indifference]. Munich:
Müller.
Gabriel, Markus. . Ich ist nicht Gehirn. Philosophie des Geistes für das .
Jahrhundert [“I” Is Not Synonymous with Brain. Philosophy of the Mind for the
st Century]. Berlin: Ullstein.
Ginger, Serge. . Gestalt Therapy: The Art of Contact. London: Carnac.
Goernitz, Brigitte, and Thomas Goernitz. . Von der Quantenphysik zum
Bewusstsein. Kosmos, Geist und Materie [From Quantum Physics to
Consciousness: Cosmos, Spirit, and Matter]. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer.
Goldstein, Kurt. . The Organism. New York: Zone Books.
Groddeck, Georg. . The Book of the It. Cambridge: Ravenio Books, original work
published .
Gutjahr, Lothar. . “Corporal Concernedness or Contact? Gestalt Therapy and the ‘New
Phenomenology.’” Gestalt Journal of Australia and New Zealand (): –.
Hartmann, Eduard von. . Philosophie des Unbewussten [Philosophy of the
Unconscious]. Berlin: Carl Dunker’s Verlag.
Heil, Joachim. . “Philosophie und Psychologie im Denken von William James”
[“We Live Forward, We Understand Backward: Philosophy and Psychology in
the Thinking of William James”]. Internationale Zeitschrift für Philosophie und
Psychosomatik : –.
Heisenberg, Werner. . Physics and Beyond; Encounters and Conversations. New
York: Harper & Row.
Höll, Kathleen. . “Philosophical Anarchism as a Source of Gestalt Therapy: Its
Importance for Therapeutic Practice and for Orientation in the Political Field.”
International Gestalt Journal (): –.
———. . “The Socio-Political Dimension of Gestalt: Springboard into a Complex
Theory of Human Health and Diseases.” Keynote lecture at the European
Association of Gestalt Therapy, Prague, September –, .
58 | Gestalt Review