Narcisism Scale
Narcisism Scale
Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice (2003), 76, 87–102
© 2003 The British Psychological Society
www.bps.org.uk
The purpose of this study was to ascertain the level of narcissistic injury among trainee
counselling psychologists using the Narcissistic Injury Scale (Slyter, 1991). This 38-item
Likert scale is based on Miller’s (1981) de nition of narcissistic injury; a speci c type of
psychological damage which focuses on feelings about the self and past relationships
related to self-development. Theorists suggest that if untreated, narcissistic issues can
interfere with client work and lead to a number of problems for trainees, in uencing
drop-out rates and increasing burnout. The results of the study indicate that a high
degree of narcissistic injury may be prevalent among trainee counselling psychologists
and furthermore, that narcissistic injury does seem to be related to the quality of the
perceived parent–child relationship. Consequently, the study suggests that therapeutic
work could be affected in those trainees who fail to address their own narcissism. The
study provides some tentative support for the utility of the Narcissistic Injury Scale.
* Requests for reprints should be addressed to Dr Rachel Tribe, Department of Psychology, University of East London, Romford
Road, London E15 4L2, UK (e-mail: [email protected]).
88 Andrea Halewood and Rachel Tribe
narcissistic disturbance and viewed narcissim as a loss of a sense of self. This loss and the
extent to which it is prevalent in trainee counselling psychologists are the research
questions with which this study is concerned.
Similarly Lichtenstein (1977) suggests that when the mother recognizes and
encourages only certain aspects of the infant’s emerging personality she imparts an
‘identity theme’ quoted in Mollon (1993, p. 64). This view emphasizes the mother’s
role in the development of the sense of self. Johnson (1987) suggests that much of
the pathology of the narcissist is caused by the individual’s rejection of the parts of
him/herself that were not supported and mirrored. Consequently, the narcissist will
try hard to conceal these parts and will work towards presenting a protective false
self instead.
Narcisstic injury among trainee psychologists 89
Theoretical models of narcissism
Since Freud, several psychological theories have attempted to explain the narcissistic
phenomenon; the most comprehensive of these argue that developmental problems in
childhood lead to the development of narcissistic disorders.
The psychodynamic model of narcissism is dominated by two overlapping schools
of thought; the object-relations school and the self-psychology school represented by
Kohut.
Kohut suggests that we all have a component of narcissism in our psyche, that we are
all born as narcissists but gradually our infantile narcissism matures into a healthy adult
version. Anarcissistic disorder results when this process is disrupted. Kohut argued that
the narcissistic experience begins when the infant’s relationship with his/her mother is
undermined by her inevitable failure to respond to the infant’s every whim. The infant
attempts to compensate for this disappointment by attempting to imbue an outside
‘other’ with absolute power and perfection instead, an image which Kohut refers to as
the ‘idealised parental imago’. Attachment to this other is the infant’s way of restoring
the early sense of wholeness. Alternatively, the infant might attempt to create instead a
perfect ‘self’, which contains everything good and excludes everything bad. Kohut
(1977) refers to this as the conguration of the ‘narcissistic self’. These congurations
make up Kohut’s concept of the ‘bipolar self ’ with narcissism on the one side and
grandiosity on the other.
(the) ideal person and my ideal image of that person and my real self are all one, and better
than the ideal person whom I wanted to love me, so that I do not need anybody else
anymore. (Quoted in Mollon, 1993, p. 78)
Kernberg (1975) argues that the narcissist is one who failed to successfully master the
rapprochement subphase, the stage between the ages of 14 and 24 months, at which
children discover that there are limits to what they can do, that they are not omnipotent.
His view of narcissistic psychopathology is that of a turning away from relatedness to
others. In this sense, it is more representative of broader trends in psychoanalytic
theorizing, whereas Kohut’s formulation represented a more radical theoretical
innovation.
Johnson (1987) suggests that although the culture reinforces pathological narcis-
sism, it originates within the family, and argues that the key to understanding narcissism
is through an understanding of the rapprochement subphase and narcissistic injury.
Personal therapy
Guy and Liaboe (1986) recommend that more attention be drawn to the benets of
personal therapy, not just as a means to enhance therapeutic skills, but also as a way to
minimize any harmful consequences of clinical practice on therapists.
The British Psychological Society (BPS) course requirement for personal therapy for
part one of the Diploma in Counselling Psychology is as follows
Candidates must undertake at least 40 hours of personal counselling psychology/therapy,
normally on an individual basis. Personal therapy should ideally be provided by an
experienced counselling psychologist . . . Under normal circumstances it is expected
that the candidate remains with the same therapist throughout. (British Psychological
Society, 1999, p. 8).
For part 2 the BPS states that ‘Candidates are encouraged to pursue a course of
personal therapy although they are not required to do so’ (British Psychological Society,
1999, p. 11).
Personal therapy is thought to be important for a number of reasons: to relieve the
stress of practising therapy, to improve the therapist’s awareness of their own problems
and areas of conict, and to help recognize and deal with countertransference issues.
The results of a study by MacDevitt (1987) indicate that awareness of counter-
transference issues is highly signicantly and positively related to the amount of
personal therapy a therapist has received. Many theorists have argued that therapists’
awareness of countertransference reactions is viewed as essential for successful therapy
outcomes (Singer & Luborsky, 1977; Strupp, 1980; Winnicott, 1949 all as quoted in
MacDevitt 1987).
Freud noted in 1910 that each therapist’s complexes and resistances will limit his or
her achievements (Freud 1910/1916). Or, as Corey (1991) argues, therapists cannot take
their clients any further than they have been prepared to go.
The debate continues to rage around the statutory requirement that trainee
counsellors and counselling psychologists engage in personal therapy. The effects of
personal therapy on client or therapist outcomes are not clearly established, and the
evidence is not unequivocal for or against the benets. (For a more in depth discussion
of this issue the reader is referred to Feltham, 1999; Grimmer & Tribe, 2001; Macran &
Shapiro, 1998; Macran, Stiles, & Smith, 1999.)
However, a study of narcissistic problems in analysts suggested that narcissistic
issues should be an area of concern (Schumacher Finell, 1985). Schumacher Finell
questions whether the narcissism of therapists is something of a blind spot, too sensitive
to be dealt with. Certainly this is a subject largely neglected in the research literature,
although there is a huge amount of research on narcissism in everybody else.
However, personal therapy may not resolve the problem if the narcissistic individual
has split off his or her aggression. They may idealize the therapist so much so that the
negative transference needs to be split off and projected into others. Glickauf-Hughes
and Mehlman (1995) suggest that personal therapy for the narcissistically injured
counsellor will be difcult for a number of reasons. Clients in this group tend to be
94 Andrea Halewood and Rachel Tribe
highly responsive to their therapist’s feelings and will be constantly monitoring
themselves and the therapist to ensure that they are not too burdensome. They may
be unaware of their own needs and therefore unlikely to ask that these needs be met,
and may also have great difculty in acknowledging any form of vulnerability. These
authors suggest that it is essential that the therapist’s therapist has adequately resolved
his or her own narcissistic and dependency issues before working with this client group.
Narcissism in the therapist may not, therefore, always be a reaction to the client’s
narcissism. It may exist independently and quite separately. As Shumacher Finell points
out, if the therapist fails to work through their own grandiosity, exhibitionism,
aggression, and power, the potential for countertransferenc e over and above a specic
reaction to a narcissistic transference in clients is enormous. Furthermore, an over-
emphasis on using a particular label such as narcissistic personality disorder (NPD) may
reect projected narcissistic tendencies that can be defensively disowned by nding
them in one’s clients rather than in oneself. As Hinshelwood (1985) points out, trainees
may disown unwanted parts of themselves by projecting them into patients.
Given the dangers outlined above of unresolved narcissism, this study looks at the
degree of narcissism in trainee counselling psychologists to ascertain whether this
should be an area of concern.
Research hypotheses
The research hypotheses were as follows:
1. There will be a higher degree of narcissistic injury among trainee counselling
psychologists than among controls.
2. There will be a relationship between the parent–child relationship and narcissistic
injury.
Method
Design
The study had a between-groups quasi-experimental design. The experimental group
was composed of trainee postgraduate counselling psychology students (N = 36) and
Narcisstic injury among trainee psychologists 95
the control group was composed of mature postgraduate students (N = 34). The groups
were matched for gender, age, and nationality.
Participants
The participants for this study were 36 counselling psychology students enrolled on the
second year of the BPS diploma all of whom had completed the 40 h of personal therapy
required by the BPS with a mean of 42 sessions. Participants were assessed using The
Narcissistic Injury Scale (Slyter, 1991). Forty-two percent of the counselling psychology
students were aged 26–35, 50%were aged 36–45, and 8%were aged 46–55. The sample
was predominantly female (60%).
The control group was composed of 34 postgraduate mature students. Six percent
were aged 19–25, 41%were aged 26–35, 41%were aged 36–45, and 12%were aged
46–55. None of the control group had undertaken any form of personal therapy. All
participants were recruited using the non-random selection technique of snowball
sampling among postgraduate students at three London universities. Students were told
that the study was concerned with the ‘developmental factors which may inuence the
choice of counselling psychology as a profession’. The questionnaire was administered
between March and April 2000.
Instruments
Narcissistic injury was measured by the Narcissistic Injury Scale (© Slyter, 1991). The
items for the instrument are scored by summing the ratings, which yields a possible
range of 38–228.
The total scale score for the NIS is the sum of 38 individual Likert ratings. Twelve items
are ller items and are not used to compute the total but are included in the scale to
reduce the response set. The minimum total for the NIS is 38, and the maximum is 228.
Rios and Hill (1993) presented evidence for the construct validity of the NIS. These
authors found that the NIS correlated positively with depression, grandiosity, and
difculties in coping, and negatively with self-esteem and parental bonding.
Results
Hypothesis 1
The mean narcissistic injury scores are given in Table 1. A linear regression was used in
order to predict which of the independent variables (age, sex, and grouping) could
predict high scores on the NIS (dependent variable). This demonstrated that age and sex
were not signicant variables, but that grouping was signicant at the p > .001 level. A
t test was then used to test whether the difference between the two sets of scores was
statistically signicant. This demonstrated a signicant difference between the narcis-
sistic injury scores of the experimental group and the control group, with the
experimental group gaining signicantly higher scores (t = 8.1; df = 68; p > .001,
one-tailed test).
Hypothesis 2
An additional area of interest was the relationship between perceived parent–child
relationship and score on the NIS (Table 2). Three of the ‘ller’ items (‘my parents
96 Andrea Halewood and Rachel Tribe
Table 1. Mean of narcissistic injury scores on narcissistic injury scale (standard deviations in
parentheses)
Counselling psychologist
trainees Control group All
Note. A t test for unrelated samples showed that the experimental group obtained signi cantly higher
scores than the control group (t = 8.1; df = 68; p > .001, one-tailed test).
understood me’, ‘my parents respected my feelings’, and ‘my parents saw me as the
person I really was’) were used to assess this. Pearson’s product– moment correlation
coefcient was used to explore this relationship, and a negative correlation was found to
be signicant at the .05 level (r = ± .258; df = 68; p > 0.05; two-tailed test).
Table 2. Correlation matrix showing relationship between parent–child relationship and narcissistic
injury score (N = 70)
Discussion
Evaluation of results
Hypothesis 1
A highly signicant difference was found between the mean NIS scores of the trainee
counselling psychologists and that of the control group (t = 8.1; df = 68; p > .001; one-
tailed test), indicating a higher degree of narcissistic injury among trainee counselling
psychologists than among controls, as predicted.
The 38 items of the Narcissistic Injury Scale were then divided into subscales in order
to bring the variables together for analysis. Only those items that quite clearly pertained
to one of the subscales were included. Mean scores on all subscales of the NIS between
groups were highly signicant, with trainee counselling psychologists scoring higher
than controls on the restriction of emotional affect (t = 6.1; p < .001), lack of under-
standing of the self and the presence of a false self (t = 4.1; p < .001), the need for
mirroring and understanding (t = 4.3; p < .001), problems with setting boundaries
(t = 4.3; p < .001), grandiosity (t = 3.9; p < .001), depression (t = 5.5; p < .001),
perceptions of the parent–child relationship (t = 7.0; p < .001), and perfectionism
(t = 4.5; p < .001).
Narcisstic injury among trainee psychologists 97
These ndings, suggesting the possible presence of narcissistic injury among trainee
counselling psychologists, may therefore support the contention that those with a high
degree of narcissistic injury are attracted to the therapeutic eld. If this is so, trainees
may well struggle with the narcissistic issues outlined above, and may need to consider
how to resolve these. Kernberg (1975) commented on the difculties faced by
therapists in training whose narcissistic resistances were not resolved, and these results
may indicate cause for concern, certainly for further research, given that the sample was
taken from trainees who have already completed the requisite 40 h of personal therapy
demanded by the BPS.
As Schumacher Finell (1985) argues, narcissim is notoriously difcult to treat.
Narcissistic defences involve splitting, denial, and projection and therefore make insight
very difcult. She suggests that teachers and supervisors should attempt to identify the
narcissistic problems that are not picked up and worked through during the trainee’s
personal therapy. Having done so, they should then suggest that these issues be taken up
in personal therapy. She goes on to say that if the trainee counsellor cannot work
through these issues, only partial results will be achieved, and the student’s ability to
deal with these issues with clients will be limited.
Hypothesis 2
The second research hypothesis was that there would be a relationship between the
perceived parent–child relationship and the degree of narcissistic injury. This was
demonstrated in two ways. First, by computing the three ‘ller’ items on the NIS which
related to a positive parent–child relationship. Slyter (1991) added these items to the
scale in order to reduce the response set, and consequently they were reverse scored
and so not included in the overall NIS score. However, they provided useful information
about perceptions of the parent–child relationship and so were retained for the purpose
of this study.
The items were as follows
1. My parents understood me.
2. My parents respected my feelings.
3. My parents saw me as the person I really was.
A negative correlation was found to be signicant (r = ± .258; df = 68; p < .05;
two-tailed test). High scores on these questions therefore correlated with low scores
on the NIS, and therefore a lower degree of narcissistic injury. This could indicate that
those who enjoyed a parental relationship characterized by feeling understood,
respected, and mirrored, seem to have a far lower degree of narcissistic injury than
those for whom these elements were lacking. However, this nding would need to be
interpreted with some caution: narcissistically injured therapists may be reluctant to
criticize parents towards whom they may still adopt a protective role. As Ford (1963)
found in his study, his trainees entertained fantasies of their mothers as being
understanding and caring, an idea which persisted despite later awareness that this
may not have been the case.
Perceptions of the parent–child relationship were then explored in terms of the
items on the NIS which pertained to perceived parent–child relationship. The 38 items
of the NIS were divided into subscales in order to bring all the variables together for
analysis, and also to clarify the relative contributions of each dimension of narcissistic
98 Andrea Halewood and Rachel Tribe
injury to the total score. One of the subscales contained the following questions which
pertained to less positive perceptions of the parent–child relationship;
1. I must not show any dissatisfaction or disappointment with my parents, since this
would lead to their withdrawal and loss of affection.
2. I always had to be strong for my parents.
3. My parents were critical whenever anyone displayed weakness.
4. My parents reacted negatively to any expression of anger on my part.
5. As a child I had ‘been good’, suffering quietly and without crying.
A highly signicant difference was found between the scores of the counselling
psychologists and the control group on this dimension (t = 7; df = 68; p < .001; two-
tailed test), indicating that the counselling psychologists perceptions of the parent–
child relationship were more negative than those of the control group; they perceived
their parents as less supportive. These relationships were characterized by a with-
holding of the self, a focus on good behaviour, and a need for the child to support the
parents.
Motivation
Counselling psychologists may benet from an understanding of why they were drawn
to the work in the rst place. Racusin et al. (1981) suggest that therapists’ awareness of
Narcisstic injury among trainee psychologists 99
their covert motivations for career choice should facilitate professional growth and
maximize effective therapeutic functioning.
Countertransference awareness
Identifying and understanding his or her own need to receive narcissistic supplies
through working with clients can increase the counselling psychologist’s counter-
transference awareness which could prevent the counselling psychologist from enter-
ing into a narcissistic collusion with clients. Glickauf-Hughes and Mehlman (1995) warn
that in order to improve the quality of treatment provided to others with similar
difculties, the therapist should be aware of how these issues may manifest themselves
in their personal lives and, furthermore, that it is essential that the therapist is able to
differentiate between narcissistic clients and those who have been victims of some form
of narcissistic abuse.
Parenti cation
An understanding of their own role within the family and of how they may have been
‘parentied’ may increase the counselling psychologist’s own understanding of their
family dynamics. Learning how their boundaries were not respected when they were a
child, how they were not able to set limits, may enable the therapist to improve these
abilities both with their clients and in their interpersonal relationships.
Ethical issues
Failure to develop self-awareness in counselling psychologists raises a number of
important ethical issues. Corey (1991) suggests that unless counsellors develop self-
awareness, they are in danger of obstructing the client’s progress or, worse still, they
may use their clients in various ways to satisfy their own needs. The British Association
for Counselling (1993) Code of Ethics and Practice for Counsellors stresses that
counsellors are expected to monitor their own personal functioning, and to seek
help if their personal resources are sufciently depleted to require this.
Burnout
Without an adequate examination of their own narcissistic issues, counselling psychol-
ogists are failing to observe these guidelines, with potential costs to their clients and to
themselves in terms of burnout. Nichols (1988) suggests that there may come a time
when, if psychologists fail to practise good self-care and use support systems, they will
be seen as guilty of irresponsibility in their professional behaviour.
100 Andrea Halewood and Rachel Tribe
It is therefore important that counselling psychologists are self-aware, honest, and
able to be self-reective and self-critical. As Shillito-Clarke (1996) points out, one of the
hallmarks of counselling psychology is the emphasis on self-awareness.
The current study has a number of implications for counselling psychology trainees
and trainers. If, as these data suggest, trainee counselling psychologists are more likely
to suffer from some form of narcissistic injury, then this should be an area of some
concern. The operationalization of narcissistic injury presented in this investigation
(NIS; Slyter, 1991) might be of value in identifying individuals who are at risk of various
psychological difculties resulting in burnout or dropping out of training altogether.
One problem in the therapy of these individuals is that they often separate themselves
from these experiences through defences such as denial, repression, and splitting.
Furthermore, if anger, hostility, and rage are central to the life of the narcissist as
Raskin, Novacek, and Hogon (1991) suggest, then this should also be an area of concern
for both the trainee counselling psychologist and the client.
As the research hypotheses were conrmed, this study adds some tentative support
to the research literature on the causes and manifestations of narcissistic injury.
However, the design of the study would be improved with a far larger sample size,
and would benet from combining the NIS with other measures of narcissism and a
measure of global functioning and/or a depression inventory. This is an area that would
benet from further research.
References
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manua l of mental disorders
(4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
British Association for Counselling. (1993). Code of ethics and practice for counsellors. Rugby,
UK: British Association for Counselling.
British Psychological Society. (1999). Regulations and syllabus for the diploma in counselling
psychology April 1999 to March 2000. Leicester, UK: British Psychological Society.
Buss, D. M., & Chiodo, L. M. (1991). Narcissistic arts in everyday life. Journal of Personality, 59(2),
179–215.
Corey, G. (1991). Theory and practice of counselling and psychotherapy (4th ed.). Pacic Grove,
CA: Brooks/Cole.
Cushway, D. (1992). Stress in clinical psychology trainees. British Journal of Clinical Psychology,
31, 169–179.
Feltham, C. (1999). Controversies in psychotherapy and counselling. London: Sage.
Ford, E. S. C. (1963). Being and becoming a psychotherapist: The search for identity. American
Journal of Psychotherapy, 17, 472–482.
Freud, S. (1914). On narcissism: An introduction on metapsychology: The theory of
psychoana lysis, The Penguin Freud Library Vol. II. London: Penguin Books.
Freud, S. (1986). The future prospects of psychoanalytic psychotherapy (standard ed.). In
J. Klauber (Ed.), Difculties in the ana lytic encounter. London: Free Association Books
Mareseld Library. (Original published 1910).
Gelder, X., Gath, D., & Mayou, R. (1983). Oxford textbook of psychiatry (3rd ed.). Oxford, UK:
Oxford University Press.
Glickauf-Hughes, C., & Mehlman, E. (1995). Narcissistic issues in therapists: Diagnostic and
treatment considerations. Psychotherapy, 32(2), 213–221.
Grimmer, A., & Tribe, R. (2001). Counselling psychologists perceptions of the impact of
mandatory personal therapy on professional development—An exploratory study.
Counselling Psychology Quarterly, 14(4), 287–301.
Narcisstic injury among trainee psychologists 101
Guy, J., & Liaboe, G. (1986). Personal therapy for the experienced psychotherapist: Adiscussion of
its usefulness and utilisation. The Clinical Psychologist, Winter, 20–23.
Guy, J. D. (1987). The personal life of the psychotherapist. New York: Wiley.
Hinshelwood, R. D. (1985). Questions of training. Free Associations: Psychoanalysis, Groups,
Politics, Culture, 2, 7–18.
Johnson, S. M. (1987). Humanizing the narcissistic style. New York: Norton.
Kernberg, O. (1975). Borderline conditions and pathological narcissism. Northvale, NJ:
Aronson.
Kohut, H. (1971). The analysis of the self. New York: International University Press.
Kohut, H. (1977). The restoration of the self. New York: International University Press.
Kohut, H. (1978). Forms and transformations of narcissism. In P. Ornstein (Ed.), The search for the
self (Vol. 1, pp. 427–460). New York: International Universities Press. (Original published
1966)
Laliotis, D. A., & Grayson, J. H. (1985). Psychologist heal thyself: What is available for the impaired
psychologist? American Psychologist, 40(1), 84–96.
Langford, J., & Clance, P. (1993). The impostor phenomena: Recent research ndings regarding
dynamics, personality and familypatterns and their implications for treatment. Psychotherapy,
113(3), 495–501.
Lichtenstein, H. (1977). Identity conguration and developmental alternatives. The dilemma of
hum an identity. New York: Aronson.
MacDevitt, J. (1987). Therapists’ personal therapy and professional self-awareness. Psycho-
therapy, 24(4), 693–703.
Macran, S., & Shapiro, D. A. (1998). The role of personal therapy for therapists: A review. British
Journal of Medical Psychology, 71, 13 –25.
Macran, S., Stiles, W. B., & Smith, J. A. (1999). How does personal therapy affect therapists’
practice? Journal of Counselling Psychology, 46(4), 419–431.
Menninger, K. (1957). Psychological factors in the choice of medicine as a profession. Bulletin of
the Menninger Clinic, 21, 51–58, 99–106.
Miller, A. (1981). The drama of being a child. London: Virago.
Mollon, P. (1989). Anxiety, supervision and a space for thinking: Some narcissistic perils for clinical
psychologists in learning psychotherapy. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 62, 113–122.
Mollon, P. (1993). The fragile self—The structure of narcissistic disturbance. London: Whurr.
Nichols, K. A. (1988). Practising what we preach. The Psychologist, February, 50–51.
Ornstein, P. (1991). From narcissism to ego psychology to self psychology. In J. Sandler (Ed.),
Freud’s on narcissism: An introduction (pp. xx –xx). London: Yale University Press.
Phillips, A. (1988). In F. Kermode (Ed.), Winnicott (pp. xx –xx). Location: Fontana Modern
Masters.
Racusin, G. R., Abramowitz, S. I., & Winter, W. D. (1981). Becoming a therapist: Family dynamics
and career choice. Professional Psychology, 12(2), 271–279.
Raskin, R., Novacek, J., & Hogan, R. (1991). Narcissism, self-esteem and defensive self-
enhancement. Journal of Personality, 59(1), 19 –38.
Rios, X., & Hill, X. (1993). The correlates of narcissistic injury: Parental bonding, depression,
grandiosity and the impact of divorce. Master’s thesis research, University of Maryland.
Rowan, J. (1989). A late developer. In W. Dryden & L. Spurling (Eds.), On becoming a
psychotherapist (pp. xx –xx). London: Tavistock/Routledge.
Russell, G. A. (1985). Narcissism and the narcissistic personality disorder: A comparison of the
theories of Kernberg and Kohut. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 58, 137–148.
Schumacher Finell, J. (1985). Narcissistic problems in analysts. International Journal of
Psycho-Analysis, 66, 433–445.
Shillito-Clarke, C. (1996). Ethical issues in counselling psychology. In The handbook of
counselling psychology (pp. xx –xx). London: Sage.
Slyter, S. (1991). The na rcissistic injury scale © (Rev. version). Unpublished instrument.
Storr, A. (1979). The art of psychotherapy. Oxford, UK: Butterworth Heinemann.
102 Andrea Halewood and Rachel Tribe
Watson, P. J., & Biderman, M. D. (1994). Narcissistic Traits Scale: Validity evidence and sex
differences in narcissism. Persona lity and Individua l Differences, 16(3), 501–504.
Winnicott, D. W. (1976). Ego distortion in terms of true and false self. In The maturational
processes and the facilitating environment (pp. xx –xx). London: Hogarth. (Original pub-
lished 1960)
Woolfe, R. (1996). The nature of counselling psychology. In X. Woolfe & X. Dryden (Eds.), The
handbook of counselling psychology (pp. xx –xx). London: Sage.
Zamostny, K. P., Slyter, S. L., & Rios, P. (1993). Narcissistic injury and its relationship to early
trauma, early resources, and adjustment to college. Journal of Counselling Psychology, 40(4),
501–510.