文献7
文献7
Technology Education
To cite this article: Nomfundo Radebe & Emmanuel Mushayikwa (2023) Bloom’s Taxonomy
and Classroom Talk: Exploring the Relationship Between the Nature of Small Group Discussion
Tasks and the Quality of Learners’ Talk, African Journal of Research in Mathematics, Science
and Technology Education, 27:1, 14-24, DOI: 10.1080/18117295.2023.2172037
The purpose of this study was to investigate the relationship between the nature of small group
discussion tasks and learners’ talk during Physical Science lessons. The study was conducted during
Physical Science lessons with three self-selected groups of Grade 11 learners from one school. A total
of three lessons were followed. During the lessons, learners were given group tasks to complete, and
their discussion was audio-recorded. The small group discussion tasks were coded using the revised
Bloom’s taxonomy. The audio-recordings were transcribed and coded using Mercer’s three types of
talk. The nature of tasks in the small group discussions was paired with the type of talk used as the
group members were responding to the tasks and the frequency of these pairs was analysed. Analysis
of the tasks vs. types of talk used indicated that learners predominately used cumulative talk to find
solutions to understanding tasks. Exploratory talk was predominately used to find solutions to
application tasks. The findings show that this relationship between the nature of small group
discussion tasks and learners’ type of talk is statistically significant at p = 0.05 level. No disputational
talk occurrences were observed. It is noted that the type of talk used to address each type of task is
related to the cognitive skills demanded by the task, group composition and the embracing of the spirit
of ubuntu by the group members.
Introduction
The South African school curriculum encourages an active and critical approach to learning (Depart-
ment of Basic Education, 2011). The approach aims to ensure that learners remain actively engaged
in the learning process by being involved in discussions with their teachers and peers. However, the
reality of South African classrooms is that learners take a passive role in learning processes while tea-
chers do most of the talking (Msimanga & Lelliott, 2014). A study by Sedova et al. (2019) shows that
learners who are given opportunities to talk during lessons, taking an active role in learning, achieve
better results than those who talk less. Resnick et al. (2018) suggest that such results are possible
because talk enables learners to actively engage with content. They add that talk also enables lear-
ners to develop a repertoire of reasoning skills where they ‘reason about content, rather than to mem-
orize facts or follow rules to solve a string of similar problems’ (Resnick et al., 2018, p. 331). This
entails that talk promotes the use of a high level of cognition and is therefore valuable in the teaching
and learning process. Hence, Khong et al. (2019) maintain that talk is important in learners’ under-
standing and intellectual development.
African Journal of Research in Mathematics, Science and Technology Education 15
In relation to the significant role that may be played by talk in the learning process, Msimanga and
Lelliott (2014) argue for the use of small group discussions to improve learners’ engagement in the
teaching and learning processes in South African schools. Such use of talk will be beneficial to
Science, Technology, Engineering and Mathematics (STEM) subjects as these place great value
on high order thinking skills (HOTS), which include critical thinking, problem solving and creative think-
ing. Howe and Mercer (2007) add that small group discussions must be guided by tasks with open-
ended questions that encourage collaborative engagement in order to achieve their intended learning
objective. Such an understanding is limited as open-ended questions may demand a variety of cog-
nitive skills which may influence the nature of small group discussions. There is paucity in research
about the relationship between the level of cognitive demand of tasks and learners’ talk during
small group discussions. Such research will provide guidance on aspects to consider in order to maxi-
mise learners’ engagement during small group discussions and to enable learning. As a result, this
study sought to investigate the relationship between the nature of small group discussion tasks
and learners’ talk during Physical Science lessons.
Small group discussions involve the use of talk to verbalise thoughts and internalise others’ point of
view. Littleton and Mercer (2013) add that talk does not only enable interactions to take place, it also
enables people to think together. Discussions amongst learners can cause a shift in their level of
understanding, thereby improving their actual development level within the zone of proximal develop-
ment (ZPD), as learners’ roles change from a more knowledgeable other to a less knowledgeable
other and vice versa during the small group discussion. ZPD refers to the ‘difference between the
actual development level as determined by independent problem solving and the level of potential
development as determined through problem solving under adult guidance or in collaboration with
a more capable peer’ (Vygotsky, 1978: 86). The use of small group discussions follows Vygotsky’s
(1978) notion of learning being mediated through social interactions amongst people.
Mercer (1995) identified three types of talk during social interactions namely, disputational, cumu-
lative and exploratory talk. According to Mercer (1995), disputational talk is the type of talk consisting
of short exchanges comprising assertions and counter-assertions, thereby creating a competitive
atmosphere amongst learners. In contrast, cumulative talk is a type of talk that lacks critical evaluation
of ideas where learners tend to agree with anything said by their peers without interrogating it (Mercer,
1995). Both cumulative and disputational talk impede improvement of learners’ understanding during
small group discussions as both types of talk limit the active engagement that is necessary in enabling
learning. Exploratory talk is a type of talk that involves critical evaluation of ideas and, therefore, is
considered valuable and effective in promoting conceptual learning (Mercer, 1995).
The levels of cognitive demand of various task types are well categorised by Bloom’s taxonomy
(Bloom et al., 1956). This taxonomy was revised by Anderson et al. (2001) to include the types of
knowledge engaged with when dealing with the various levels of cognitive demand. Figure 1
shows the revised version of Bloom’s taxonomy. The six levels of cognitive demand shown in
Figure 1 range from remembering, understanding, applying, analysing and evaluating to creating
new products. The level of cognitive demand increases as you move from remember towards
create. Those tasks requiring factual and conceptual knowledge are considered to demand low
order thinking skills (LOTS). Low order thinking skills refer to thinking processes that require
routine information and ‘mechanical application of previously acquired information such as listing
information previously memorised and inserting numbers into previously learned formulas’ (Lewis &
Smith, 1993: 193).
Tasks that require metacognitive knowledge, whereby learners analyse, evaluate and create as
shown in Figure 1, demand high order thinking skills (HOTS). High order thinking skills refer to thinking
16 Nomfundo Radebe and Emmanuel Mushayikwa
Figure 1. The revised Bloom’s taxonomy (adapted from Jensen et al. 2014)
processes that require one to make use of new and stored information, by interrelating, rearranging
and extending information to achieve an intended objective (Lewis & Smith, 1993). According to
Crowe et al. (2008), tasks that require learners to apply information in new ways demand thinking pro-
cesses that are between LOTS and HOTS, known as intermediate level. Such tasks involve the use of
procedural knowledge as shown in Figure 1, which is concerned with the method used to complete a
task although a great deal of conceptual understanding is required. However, this study considers
application tasks to demand HOTS as learners are required to use their conceptual knowledge as
they apply it in new ways and situations, therefore demanding some critical thinking, although this
is at a lower level than those tasks promoting metacognitive knowledge. Therefore, tasks that
require learners to apply, analyse, evaluate and create are considered to be HOTS, thus placed at
the top of the triangle in Figure 1.
Kutnick and Blatchford (2014) identified three approaches in which group work can be organised. The
first approach is called individuated work with talk and involves learners working individually on the
same task while allowed to talk to their fellow learners about the task. The second approach is
called peer interactive work and it involves learners working on the same task with a shared goal
and required to talk with fellow group members about the task. Peer interactive work is the small
group discussion approach this study will be focusing on. The last approach involves learners in a
group working with the teacher on the same or a different task. In either structure, group work provides
learners with an opportunity to learn to ‘share and listen to ideas, clarify misconceptions when they
arise and become more willing to provide extended explanations that facilitate deeper understanding
and continued engagement in the subject-matter’ (Chan, 2020: 4).
High-level tasks are essential for promoting effective and meaningful small group discussions, par-
ticularly in STEM subjects, as they can enhance learners’ critical analysis and evaluation skills. Smart
and Marshall (2013) conducted a study to understand factors affecting the interactions between tea-
chers and learners. They found a positive relationship between the level of tasks used by the teacher
and learners’ cognitive level. They noted that when teachers used high-level tasks that required lear-
ners to analyse and evaluate, the observed cognitive level in learners was higher and was low when
African Journal of Research in Mathematics, Science and Technology Education 17
the teacher used low-level tasks. Similar observations were made by Jensen et al. (2014), where they
found that using tasks that require a high level of cognitive demand throughout the teaching process
enhanced learners’ conceptual understanding. Such findings help us to understand the important role
played by tasks learners engage in during the teaching and learning process. In group work, such a
role can be played by the task given to learners to facilitate their engagement. However, the relation-
ship between the cognitive demand of tasks for small group discussions and learners’ talk in their
group has not been explored. Such knowledge will be valuable in preparing small group tasks for
the purpose of enhancing understanding and enabling learning.
Methodology
In order to explore the relationship between the cognitive demand of tasks for small group discus-
sions and learners’ talk in their groups, this qualitative case study was conducted in one of the
public schools in Soweto, in 2018. The study focused on 32 Grade 11 learners and their Physical
Science teacher, who voluntarily participated in the study. The selection of the study participants
was both purposive and convenient. It was purposive in that the researchers sought a Grade 11
physical Science teacher who was willing to contribute to the study. It was convenient because
the selected teacher then assisted in selecting learner participants of the study from among his
classes. Using this approach, 24 out of 32 Grade 11 Physical Science learners were selected as
they voluntarily consented to participate in this study. These learners did not receive any kind of
coaching on how to carry out discussions. This study focuses on the discussions of only 13 out
of 24 learners. The 13 learners were selected using the assistance of the teacher based on his
observations of seeing these learners working in groups even when given individual activities in
his lessons.
The learners formed three self-selected small groups. The groups were labelled groups A–C, with
four, five and four members, respectively. Learners were given tasks to complete by their teacher. The
teacher instructed learners to discuss the task with their group members. However, each learner was
expected to have their own individual write-up. The teacher was not involved in the discussions unless
called upon by the learners. As such, learners enjoyed the freedom of discussing with their group
members in their own preferred language. Audio-recorders were used to collect data by capturing
the talk between learners as they worked on tasks. The tasks were coded using the revised
Bloom’s taxonomy, as shown in Figure 1, where each task was classified as a remember, understand,
apply, analyse, evaluate or create task. Data were collected during three consecutive lessons. The
first two lessons focused on Ohm’s law: the relationship between current, potential difference and
resistance. The last lesson focused on solving conceptual problems using the mathematical
expression of Ohm’s law for series and parallel circuits.
The captured talk was transcribed and read several times to ensure that the data were captured
accurately. The transcripts were then analysed using Mercer’s (1995) types of talk to understand
the nature of talk learners employed as they were working on each task. Each type of talk was ident-
ified using characteristics of disputational, cumulative and exploratory talk as discussed above. The
two differently coded sets of data were brought together and re-coded as shown in Table 1.
Table 1 shows the codes that were used to categorise each type of talk against the task type. For
instance, if cumulative talk was used to answer an application task, that talk instance is coded as
ApCT while a cumulative talk instance used to answer an evaluation question is coded as ECT. To
ensure validity and reliability of these findings, two researchers categorised the raw data transcripts
independently using the agreed talk codes (see Table 1). Cohen’s kappa was used to check the level
of agreement between the coders. On comparison, if the Cohen kappa was between 0.6 and 0.8, the
coding had to be renegotiated. This was achieved through consensus building. The average Cohen’s
kappa for the whole data set was 0.9, which indicates that the coders had a nearly perfect agreement
as they coded the data. After having coded the data as illustrated in the two figures, the frequency of
each code shown in Table 1 was determined and represented as shown in Table 2. Thereafter, the
trends showing the relationship between the cognitive demand of tasks for small group discussions
and learners’ talk in their group was explored.
Findings
This study found that cumulative and exploratory talk were predominantly used by the groups of lear-
ners as they were finding solutions to the tasks which demanded the use of different thinking skills.
However, no occurrences of disputational talk were found during their interaction. Figure 2 shows
the discussion between Group A learners as they were finding solutions to Task 2 during the first
observed Physical Science lesson.
Figure 2 shows group A learners’ talk when they were engaging with an understanding task. In this
figure, Thato in line 1 is proposing a solution to the question. In lines 2–12, Neo is agreeing with what
Thato is saying without asking any questions that will enable exploration of ideas. Accepting ideas
without critically evaluating them is one of the characteristics of cumulative talk. Therefore, the discus-
sion in Figure 2 represents cumulative talk. Figure 3 represents learners’ talk as they were responding
to an application task.
Figure 3 is taken from group B’s discussion of the task shown at the top of the figure. In this Figure 3,
Tumelo does not understand how to work out the given application task (line 1). Thabang in line 2,
explains how Tumelo should work out the solution to the task. However, Thabo in lines 3–14 questions
the approach suggested by Thabang, compelling him to review the approach he was suggesting.
While Thabang is trying to figure out a way to address the task, Thabo in line 16 comes up with
another way to solve the problem while at the end seeking confirmation from his peers. In this
exchange, group members are asking questions and sharing relevant information that they think is
necessary to address the task. Sharing of relevant information and asking of questions are character-
istics of exploratory talk. Thus, this talk instance represents exploratory talk.
Table 2 summarises the findings from the analysis of the tasks given to learners as group work
during three consecutive lessons in a Grade 11 Physical Sciences classroom.
Table 2. Frequencies of the relationship between the task demand and the nature of learner talk
Table 2 shows that a vast majority of the tasks given to the learners demanded understanding and
application. Further, Table 2 shows that understanding tasks were primarily addressed through cumu-
lative talk (17 out of 28 instances) while application tasks were most frequently addressed through
exploratory talk (nine out of 12 instances). Table 2 also shows that exploratory talk was the only
type of talk used in addressing the few evaluate and create tasks. Moreover, Table 2 shows that
no talk occurrences were observed in analysing tasks while one occurrence of cumulative and
exploratory talk, respectively, was observed in remembering tasks. Overall, Table 2 shows that
there were more occurrences in the understanding tasks followed by application, remembering, evalu-
ate and create, respectively. Additionally, there were no occurrences of disputational talk in any of the
tasks discussed. However, there were 21 cumulative talk and 23 exploratory talk occurrences.
To further tease out whether there was any association between the type of task given by the
teacher and the type of talk learners engaged in to complete the task, a significance chi-square
test was conducted on the coded data in Table 2. The null hypothesis explored was that there is
no observed relationship between the task provided and the type of talk learners engaged in to com-
plete the task. Since none of the learner groups engaged in disputational talk, the analysis was limited
20 Nomfundo Radebe and Emmanuel Mushayikwa
to cumulative and exploratory talk. The task demand categories were also simplified into two groups:
remembering and understanding (LOTS), and application, analysis and creativity (HOTS). This
provided a two by two matrix (see Table 3), thus yielding one degree of freedom (d.f. = 1).
The computed value of chi-square based on the coded frequencies in Table 3 (n = 44) was found to
be 5.69, which is considerably higher than the standardised chi-square value obtained from statistical
tables at d.f. = 1 and significance level p = 0.05 (χ 2 = 3.84). This points to the rejection of the null
hypothesis, and implies that there was a significant relationship between the nature of the task pro-
vided and the type of talk that learners engaged in to complete the task. However, we need to be
careful to read too much into this statistic as the sample under study was rather small. A larger
sample would be required to provide a clearer picture of the nature of the association. Even with
this proviso, these findings do provide a tentative possibility that the type of talk used by learners
during small group discussions is related to the nature (cognitive level) of the tasks that they are pre-
sented with.
Table 2 also shows that group B had the most talk occurrences, 18, followed by group A with
17 occurrences, and group C with 9 occurrences, respectively. Table 2 indicates that group A does
not differentiate in their choice of talk according to the task demands as they choose cumulative
and exploratory talk seemingly randomly (or equally frequently) for each task demand. Instead,
group B chose largely cumulative talk for tasks demanding understanding and exploratory talk for
application tasks. The pattern of talk in group C may resemble the pattern from group B but the
number of observed incidents for this group is relatively low. Thus, the data show a relationship
between task demands and the type of talk used in small group discussions. In addition, the data
shows that group composition may also have an effect on the type of talk used by the learners.
Discussion
This study found that the group of learners predominately used exploratory talk to address application
tasks. The presence of exploratory talk in their small group discussions is related to the cognitive skills
demanded by the task. The productive and lengthy exchanges that learners had while thinking
together and discussing suitable procedures to follow in order to find solutions to the application
tasks, their conceptual understanding and reasoning skills required to advance their procedural
and conceptual knowledge were enhanced in accordance with the findings by Smart and Marshall
(2013) and Jensen et al. (2014). Improvement in the conceptual and procedural knowledge of
these learners will advance their overall performance in the topic, consequently improving their per-
formance in the subject as per Sedova et al. (2019).
The findings suggest that learners were using exploratory talk in response to the high cognitive
nature of application tasks, a relationship that Mercer et al. (2004) had also predicted. The learners
may have found the application tasks to be very challenging as they used exploratory talk mostly
to address them. The challenging nature of these tasks is related to their position in Figure 1, that
they demand HOTS unlike understanding tasks which demand LOTS. Application tasks require lear-
ners to make use of their acquired conceptual knowledge and apply information in new ways in order
to achieve an intended objective. Thus, suggesting that tasks demanding HOTS, such as application
tasks, elicit the use of exploratory talk.
While exploratory talk may have been predominately used in application tasks, cumulative talk was
primarily used in understanding tasks. Understanding tasks require and enhance conceptual knowl-
edge as indicated in Figure 1. However, the predominant use of cumulative talk in such tasks meant
that learners simply agreed with their fellow group members on proposed ideas without critical evalu-
ation of those ideas. While Mercer et al. (2004) attribute learners’ prevalent use of cumulative talk to
undeveloped reasoning skills, Scholtz et al. (2008) relate it to the strength of their knowledge base for
the content being engaged with. Mercer et al.’s (2004) attribution of learners’ prevalent use of cumu-
lative talk to undeveloped reasoning skills cannot be true in this study because the groups of learners
(1) predominately engaged in exploratory talk as compared to cumulative talk and (2) exploratory talk
was predominately used in application tasks which are at a higher cognitive level than understanding
tasks. Similar to Mercer et al. (2004), Scholtz et al.’s (2008) explanation does not apply in this study
although learners did not actively engage in critical evaluation of proposed ideas in order to address
the understanding tasks. In this study, the prevalent use of cumulative talk to address understanding
tasks is related to the cognitive skills demanded by the task. Understanding tasks require LOTS as
they are about ideas and concepts explanation. This is information that learners can remember
from the teaching that may have taken place and from reading, unlike tasks at higher cognitive
levels which demand the application of information in new ways, analysis, evaluation and the creation
of new products, ultimately developing learners’ procedural and metacognitive knowledge. Therefore
the nature of the understanding tasks requires more consensus building than critical exploration.
Learners were prone to relate the tasks to memory and build consensus (agree and offer more sup-
porting ideas) than to evaluate implications of ideas offered.
Since the learners involved in this study were untutored in group work, the absence of disputational
talk occurrences in their discussion may be related to their cultural background, which makes them
more inclined to the inclusive argumentation concept. Inclusive argumentation is a concept coined
by Scholtz et al. (2008) to describe ‘the ability to provide evidence and justifications for claims or
decisions taking cognisance of the views of others’ (p. 32). Inclusive argumentation acknowledges
the role ubuntu plays in discussions. Ubuntu diminishes the need for confrontational interactions
and promotes the togetherness atmosphere. In such an atmosphere, learners would support contri-
butions that are non-opposing and adopt neutral stances if the need to disagree emerges (Scholtz
et al., 2008). In this study, learners were working in harmony, without confrontational occurrences
which are disputational in nature. To ensure that the groups’ atmosphere remains harmonious, lear-
ners engaged in cumulative and exploratory talk only throughout their discussions as seen in Table 2.
This study acknowledges that although application tasks and understanding tasks were addressed
using predominately exploratory talk and cumulative talk, respectively, there are other factors at play
that must be considered in understanding the choices of talk used by learners to address the different
types of tasks in this study. The first factor is group characteristics. This study found that group com-
position may have an effect on the type of talk used by the learners as different groups used talk dif-
ferently. Group A was seemingly not differentiating in their choice of talk vs. task type as researchers
observed equal occurrences of cumulative talk and exploratory talk. Group B and C seemingly used
cumulative talk and exploratory talk to address understanding and application tasks, respectively.
This is of particular importance since learners grouped themselves using their own criteria
unknown to the researchers. Therefore, the characteristics of the groups seem to be a determinant
of the choice of talk used to address each type of task. More research is required to understand
this phenomenon.
The second factor that must be considered in understanding the choices of talk used by learners in
each type of task addressed is the language used by the learners to discuss the given tasks. Learners
in this study had the freedom to interact with their peers using a language they were comfortable with.
Therefore, the group interactions were not impeded by barriers in the language of teaching and learn-
ing, as Scholtz et al. (2008) proclaim that a limited capability in the language used to articulate ideas
hinders learners’ ability to engage in talk effectively. Taking Scholtz et al.’s (2008) observation, this
study acknowledges that the findings might change if learners had to strictly use the language of
African Journal of Research in Mathematics, Science and Technology Education 23
teaching and teaching of the school, which is English, as the learners were not English home
language speakers.
The last factor that must be considered in understanding the choices of talk used by learners in each
type of task addressed is the involvement of teachers in learners’ small group discussions. In this
study, the teacher did not interfere with learners’ interactions as learners were expected to report
their answers to the teacher during the whole class discussion following the group discussions.
Thus, learners were free to interact with their peers in the absence of their teacher’s involvement
the way they see fit. Active involvement of the teacher during the small group discussions may
alter the type of talk used to address each task.
Conclusion
This study gathers that there is an association between the cognitive nature of small group discussion
tasks and the type of talk used to address the tasks. This study found that understanding tasks,
demanding LOTS, prompted the use of cumulative talk while application tasks, demanding HOTS,
tended to elicit the use of exploratory talk during small group discussions. The absence of disputa-
tional talk occurrences in this study is related to learners embracing the spirit of ubuntu which
raises the need for learners to maintain a harmonious working atmosphere that promotes non-con-
frontational interactions amongst learners in their respective groups. This study also found that
group characteristics may have been a determinant of the choice of talk used by the learners for
each task type. However, this study does not have the required data to provide a detailed explanation
on this finding. Further research is required to understand the impact of group characteristics on the
choice of talk for different demands of small group discussion tasks.
Finally, though the chi-square test tentatively identifies a relationship between the nature of task and
the type of talk learners engaged in, there is insufficient evidence to enable an understanding of this
relationship owing to the limited sample size. There is a need for further explorations with larger
samples to establish the exact nature of such a relationship.
Acknowledgements
The publication of this paper was made possible by funding of the South African National Resource
Foundation (NRF).
Disclosure Statement
References
Anderson, L. W., Krathwohl, D. R., Airiasian, W., Cruikshank, K. A., Mayer, R. E., & Pintrich, P. R. (2001). A tax-
onomy for learning, teaching and assessing: A revision of Bloom’s Taxonomy of educational outcomes.
Longman.
Bloom, B. S., Krathwohl, D. R., & Masia, B. B. (1956). Taxonomy of educational objectives: The classification of
educational goals. D. McKay.
Chan, M. (2020). A multilevel SEM study of classroom talk on cooperative learning and academic achievement:
Does cooperative scaffolding matter? International Journal of Educational Research, 101, 101564. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.ijer.2020.101564
Crowe, A., Dirks, C., & Wendcroth, M. P. (2008). Biology in Bloom: Implementing Bloom’s taxonomy to enhance
student learning in biology. CBE—Life Sciences Education, 7, 368–381.
Department of Basic Education (2011). Curriculum and Assessment Policy Statement (CAPS). Government
Printer.
Howe, C., & Mercer, N. (2007). Children’s social development, peer interaction and classroom learning (primary
review research survey 2/1b). University of Cambridge, Faculty of Education.
24 Nomfundo Radebe and Emmanuel Mushayikwa
Jensen, J. L., McDaniel, M. A., Woodard, S. M., & Kummer, T. A. (2014). Teaching to the test … or testing to teach:
Exams requiring higher order thinking skills encourage greater conceptual understanding. Educational
Psychology Review, 26(2), 307–329. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-013-9248-9
Khong, T. D. H., Saito, E., & Gillies, R. M. (2019). Key issues in productive classroom talk and interventions.
Educational Review, 71(3), 334–349. https://doi.org/10.1080/00131911.2017.1410105
Kutnick, P., & Blatchford, P. (2014). Effective group work in primary school classrooms. Springer. https://doi.org/
10.1007/978-94-007-6991-5
Lewis, A., & Smith, D. (1993). Defining higher order thinking . Theory into Practice, 32(3), 131–137.
Littleton, K., & Mercer, N. (Eds.) (2013). Interthinking: Putting talk to work (1st ed.). Routledge. https://www.
taylorfrancis.com/chapters/mono/10.43249780203809433-8/understanding-interthinking-karen-littleton-neil-
mercer?context=ubx&refId=53f99d8c-1133-4859-a847-b5a45ff5c106
Mercer, N. (1995). The guided construction of knowledge: Talk amongst teachers and learners. Multilingual
Matters.
Mercer, N., Dawes, L., Wegerif, R. & Sams, C. (2004). Reasoning as a scientist: Ways of helping children to use
language to learn science. British Educational Research Journal, 30(3), 359–377.
Msimanga, A., & Lelliott, A. (2014). Talking science in multilingual contexts in South Africa: Possibilities and chal-
lenges for engagement in learners home languages in high school classrooms. International Journal of Science
Education, 36(7), 1159–1183. https://doi.org/10.1080/09500693.2013.851427
Resnick, L. B., Asterhan, C. S. C., Clarke, S. N., & Schantz, F. (2018). Next generation research in dialogic learn-
ing. In G. E. Hall, L. F. Quinn, & D. M. Gollnick (Eds.), The Wiley handbook of teaching and learning (pp. 323–
338). John Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118955901.ch13
Scholtz, Z., Braund, M., Hodges, M., Koopman, R., & Lubben, F. (2008). South African teachers’ ability to argue:
The emergence of inclusive argumentation. International Journal of Educational Development, 28(1), 21–34.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijedudev.2006.12.005
Sedova, K., Sedlacek, M., Svaricek, R., Majcik, M., Navratilova, J., Drexlerova, A., … Salamounova, Z. (2019). Do
those who talk more learn more? The relationship between student classroom talk and student achievement.
Learning and Instruction, 63, 101217.
Smart, J. B., & Marshall, J. C. (2013). Interactions between classroom discourse, teacher questioning, and student
cognitive engagement in middle school science. Journal of Science Teacher Education, 24(2), 249–267. https://
doi.org/10.1007/s10972-012-9297-9
Vygotsky, L. (1978). Interaction between learning and development . In L. Vygotsky, Mind in society: The devel-
opment of higher psychological processes (pp. 79–91). Harvard University Press.