Tan 2016
Tan 2016
Research Paper
art ic l e i nf o a b s t r a c t
Article history: The presence or absence of differences in the perceptions of previous visitors and non-visitors toward a
Received 8 July 2015 destination is important to tourist theorists and practitioners. Destination familiarity, destination image,
Received in revised form and future visit intention are suitable marketing variables for investigations into these two groups.
18 December 2015
Destination familiarity has been defined in many different ways. Adding to the complexity, related
Accepted 19 December 2015
concepts, such as awareness, knowledge, experience, and expertise, have, in one way or another, been
used in combination with familiarity. There is also inconsistency in whether familiarity is a unidimen-
Keywords: sional or multidimensional construct. Considering previous studies, Prentice (2004) provided an inter-
Destination familiarity related seven-dimension familiarity construct. Taking into account previous familiarity and familiarity-
Experiential familiarity
related studies and setting experiential familiarity as a moderator, this study aims to achieve a deeper
Destination image
understanding of familiarity by examining how Prentice's familiarity dimensions are interrelated, as well
Future visit intention
Destination marketing organizations as to gain insights into the structural relationships among familiarity, destination image, and future visit
intention via the comparison of previous visitors and non-visitors. Hong Kong was selected as the tourist
destination, and the partial least squares method was applied to analyze 493 surveys collected from
residents in Taiwan. The findings provide a range of academic and practical implications. In this light,
certain previous findings may have to be reconsidered. The present work indicates the importance of
experience as a criterion for segmenting consumers, and it demonstrates the usefulness of a critical
examination of non-visitors. This study offers numerous suggestions regarding how destination mar-
keting organizations can formulate effective strategies for both previous visitors and non-visitors.
& 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jdmm.2015.12.008
2212-571X/& 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Please cite this article as: Tan, W.-K., & Wu, C.-E. An investigation of the relationships among destination familiarity, destination image
and future visit intention. Journal of Destination Marketing & Management (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jdmm.2015.12.008i
2 W.-K. Tan, C.-E. Wu / Journal of Destination Marketing & Management ∎ (∎∎∎∎) ∎∎∎–∎∎∎
Familiarity
Educational H1b Self-described (Part 1)
Familiarity Familiarity
H1c
H3a
H1a H1d H3c
H1e
H2a
Expected Proximate
Familiarity H5 Familiarity
Familiarity (Part 2)
H13-H16
Familiarity
(Part 1)
H7-H10
Destination
Image
H19-
Cognitive H6 Affective H20 Future Visit
Destination Destination
Intention
Image Image
H11-H12
H17-H18
Familiarity
(Part 2)
Familiarity
(b) Relationships among familiarity, destination image, and future visit intention
Fig. 1. Research model: (a) relationships among familiarity dimensions, (b) relationships among familiarity, destination image, and future visit intention.
important stakeholders of a destination, i.e. visitors, and consider feel connected to a destination), educational (the extent of formal
the destination familiarity of previous visitors and non-visitors. and informal education), self‐assured (own judgments and feel-
Destination familiarity is also worthy of further examination. ings of safety), and expected (the extent of coziness and attrac-
This important concept has been defined in many different ways tions expected) familiarity. However, further research regarding
(Baloglu, 2001). Related concepts, such as awareness, knowledge, the composition of familiarity remains necessary because these
experience, expertise, and prior knowledge, have, in one way or seven dimensions are interrelated (Prentice, 2004).
another, been used in combination with familiarity, i.e. as a sy- The holistic overview by Prentice (2004) serves as a useful
nonym, a component, or an umbrella term with familiarity as a launch pad for further examination of the destination familiarity of
component (Lee, Floyd, & Shinew, 2002; Sharifpour, Walters, previous visitors and non-visitors, and its subsequent impact on
Ritchie, & Winter, 2014). There is also inconsistency in whether the destination image and future behavioral intention to visit the
familiarity should represent a unidimensional or multidimensional destination. Experiential familiarity can be used to segment in-
construct: the answer depends on how familiarity is defined. Fa- dividuals into non-visitors and previous visitors. Although some
miliarity has traditionally been treated as a unidimensional con- studies have examined a limited number of the components of
struct, such as the number of previous trips (Snepenger, Meged, familiarity, to the best of our knowledge, few researchers have
Snelling, & Worrall, 1990). However, current researchers have in- demonstrated how all dimensions of Prentice's familiarity tax-
creasingly recognized the multidimensionality of familiarity. For onomy are interrelated and how this interrelationship affects fa-
example, Baloglu (2001) operationalized familiarity as a multi- miliarity as an antecedent of destination image and visit intention
dimensional construct that consists of previous experiences (ex- within a single study. The question that arises is, when viewed
periential familiarity), the extent of information used (informa- from Prentice's familiarity taxonomy, does the familiarity of non-
tional familiarity), and how familiar with a place individuals be- visitors significantly differ from previous visitors to cause them to
lieve themselves to be (self-rated familiarity). Based on previous behave differently in terms of future visit intention?
studies, Prentice (2004) gathered and expanded the familiarity Through a comparison of previous visitors and non-visitors,
taxonomy into seven dimensions: experiential, informational, self‐ with experiential familiarity as a moderator, the present study
described (self-rated), proximate (the extent to which individuals aims to achieve a deeper theoretical understanding of familiarity
Please cite this article as: Tan, W.-K., & Wu, C.-E. An investigation of the relationships among destination familiarity, destination image
and future visit intention. Journal of Destination Marketing & Management (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jdmm.2015.12.008i
W.-K. Tan, C.-E. Wu / Journal of Destination Marketing & Management ∎ (∎∎∎∎) ∎∎∎–∎∎∎ 3
by examining how Prentice's various familiarity dimensions are et al., 2014). Prior knowledge has also been measured via famil-
interrelated. Furthermore, the study aims to use Prentice's famil- iarity or expertise (Khosrowjerdi & Iranshahi, 2011). However,
iarity taxonomy to gain deeper insights into the structural re- some researchers do not equate expertise with familiarity. For
lationships among familiarity, destination image, and future visit example, Gursoy (2003) treated prior knowledge as two separate
intention. In addition to its theoretical contribution, under- dimensions: expertise and familiarity. Kerstetter and Cho (2004)
standing familiarity and its implications for destination image and postulated that prior knowledge is accumulated via familiarity,
visit intention will facilitate a better design of strategies by DMOs expertise, and past experience. However, an empirical analysis
to address the concerns of these two tourist groups. The city demonstrated that the dimensions of familiarity and expertise are
destination chosen for this study is Hong Kong, which represents a loaded together. Their study further suggested that ‘given the ease
popular and nearby (flight duration of approximately 1 h) tourist with which individuals can find and gain in-depth knowledge
destination for Taiwanese residents. about destinations today, the original contention that they are
distinctly separate constructs in tourism may be misleading’
(Kerstetter & Cho, 2004, p. 974).
2. Literature review and hypothesis development Familiarity may be equated with subjective knowledge. Sub-
jective knowledge indicates how much individuals think they
This study uses experiential familiarity as a moderator for know and often reflects consumer self-confidence (Brucks, 1985).
segmenting individuals into two groups: previous visitors and According to Brucks (1985), familiarity is referred to as subjective
non-visitors. The research model subsequently discussed is pro- knowledge and, in addition to objective knowledge and previous
posed for investigating the relationship between the dimensions experience, constitutes consumer knowledge.
of Prentice's familiarity taxonomy (Fig. 1a) and understanding how By considering familiarity as affect-as-information (Prentice,
familiarity, destination image, and future visit intention are related 2006), researchers, such as Baloglu (2001) and Prentice (2004),
(Fig. 1b) in previous visitors and non-visitors. interpreted familiarity as a multidimensional construct and oper-
ationalized it as a combination of experiential, self-described (self-
2.1. Destination familiarity rated), and informational familiarity. Experiential familiarity is the
extent of previous experience and may be operationalized as first-
2.1.1. Types of destination familiarity timer or repeater (Chen & Lin, 2012) and number of visits (Pre-
The concept of familiarity often appears in combination with ntice, 2004). As a subjective measure of familiarity, self-described
other related concepts, such as awareness, knowledge, experience, familiarity represents how familiar with a place individuals be-
and expertise (Sharifpour et al., 2014). Furthermore, the different lieve themselves to be without the necessity of previous visitation
definitions of familiarity result in inconsistencies in the oper- (Prentice, 2004). Some researchers have operationalized self-de-
ationalization of the concept. Through a review of the literature scribed familiarity as a single-item construct (Elliot & Papado-
regarding familiarity and its related concepts, this study attempts poulos, 2015; Lu, Chi, & Lu, 2014; Sun et al., 2013). Informational
to build a model for investigating the interdependency between familiarity refers to the extent of exposure to destination-related
different types of familiarity. information and is operationalized as a single or multiple sources
Alba and Hutchinson (1987) linked familiarity with experience (Baloglu, 2001). Prentice (2004) made a distinction between per-
and defined it as ‘the number of product-related experiences that sonal education and destination-related information sources and
have been accumulated by the consumer’ (Alba & Hutchinson, categorized destination familiarity gained through the former as
1987, p. 411). As a result, familiarity has often been conceptualized educational familiarity. Personal education may be obtained via
as a single dimension of previous visitation (Milman & Pizam, formal mediated learning (such as learning through educational
1995) or the number of previous visits (Sun, Chi, & Xu, 2013; Tasci, institutions) or informal mediated learning (such as learning
Gartner, & Cavusgil, 2007), or it has been operationalized as the through watching television and movies or reading novels). Pre-
contrasts between repeaters and newcomers (Prentice, 2006). Si- ntice (2004) narrowly operationalized educational familiarity as
milar to familiarity, experience has also been conceptualized and having read the works of novelists and poets but also argued for
operationalized in various ways (Sharifpour et al., 2014), including the inclusion of having watched movies and TV programs as
general international travel experience (Kozak, Crotts, & Law, components of educational familiarity. Educational familiarity is
2007) or previous visitation to a particular destination (Fuchs & therefore related to movie-induced tourism (Connell, 2012).
Reichel, 2011). Considering other familiarity-related studies, Prentice (2004)
Other researchers have argued that familiarity does not need to added three additional familiarity dimensions: self-assured, ex-
originate from actual experience (Srull, 1983). Non-visitors may pected, and proximate. Safety and security are a priority for a
have a certain level of destination familiarity created by education, prosperous tourism industry in any destination (Tasci & Boylu,
mass media, travel guides and personal contact with other in- 2010). The related concept of self-assured familiarity refers to the
dividuals (Gursoy, 2011; Prentice & Andersen, 2003). The Internet extent of assurance regarding traveling in the destination (Pre-
is also widely used for travel planning across all customer seg- ntice, 2004), and some researchers have considered self-assured
ments (Xiang, Magnini, & Fesenmaier, 2015). Thus, familiarity may familiarity as judgments and feelings regarding safety (Jansen,
be related to the information search and is associated with the 2011). Expected familiarity is the extent of coziness, comfort, and
amount of time spent processing information (Baker, Hutchinson, attractions expected. This definition is consistent with Cohen
Moore, & Nedungadi, 1986). (1972), who considered familiarity as a preference for the tourist
For some researchers, the previously described experience and bubble, i.e., comfort. Proximate familiarity is the extent to which
familiarity gained via information sources are components of prior individuals feel connected to the tourist destination and whether
knowledge. Webb (2000) distinguished between familiarity and they can identify with the destination (Jansen, 2011). Prentice
experience by defining the former as ‘knowledge gained through (2004) operationalized proximate familiarity as nationality. How-
exposure to various information concerning the service provider’ ever, in a subsequent study of visits to Scandinavian countries,
(Webb, 2000, p. 6) and the latter as ‘knowledge gained through Prentice (2006) included Scandinavian language proficiency and
direct involvement with a service provider’ (Webb, 2000, p. 6). membership in organizations that promote cultural ties when
This prior destination knowledge is thus viewed as a multi- investigating visits to Scandinavian countries. Jansen (2011) con-
dimensional construct (Huang, Gursoy, & Xu, 2014; Sharifpour sidered proximate familiarity as the presence of an emotional
Please cite this article as: Tan, W.-K., & Wu, C.-E. An investigation of the relationships among destination familiarity, destination image
and future visit intention. Journal of Destination Marketing & Management (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jdmm.2015.12.008i
4 W.-K. Tan, C.-E. Wu / Journal of Destination Marketing & Management ∎ (∎∎∎∎) ∎∎∎–∎∎∎
connection with a place and/or having friends or relatives who live ‘a set of beliefs, ideas, and impressions that people have of a place
in the destination. It is thus related to the concept of place at- or destination’ (Baloglu & McCleary, 1999, p. 871). As a subjective
tachment and connection, which refers to the affective bond and concept (Martin & Bosque, 2008), it is a complex amalgam of
connection between individuals and specific places (Hammitt, products and attributes woven into a complete impression (Gart-
Backlund, & Bixler, 2006; Yuksel, Yuksel, & Bilim, 2010). ner, 1993; Kim, 2014). Destination image attributes can be classi-
fied as cognitive or affective, and ‘the coexistence of both com-
2.1.2. Inter-dependency between types of destination familiarity ponents (cognitive and affective) may explain in a better way the
Prentice (2004) suggested that the seven types of destination image a tourist has of a place’ (Martin & Bosque, 2008, p. 264).
familiarity are interdependent. However, there is a lack of re- Cognitive image refers to an individual's perceptions, beliefs and
search, at least within a single study, regarding the extent of in- attitudes regarding the destination (Martin & Bosque, 2008; Qu,
terdependency. In the present work, the research model shown in Kim, & Im, 2011). Cognitive attributes may be functional/tangible
Fig. 1a is used to determine the relationships between these fa- (e.g. landscape and cultural attractions) or psychological/abstract
miliarity dimensions. (e.g. feelings of hospitality and atmosphere) (Martin & Bosque,
Studies regarding familiarity and its related concepts (such as 2008). Affective image is related to the feelings and emotions that
knowledge, experience and expertise) on information search be- a tourist destination evokes (Baloglu, 1999; Martin & Bosque,
havior have provided contradictory results (Lee et al., 2002). Some 2008). Given that previous studies have demonstrated that the
studies have demonstrated that familiarity has a negative influ- cognitive destination image contributes to the affective destina-
ence on information searches; other studies have suggested a tion image (Chen & Phou, 2013; Kim & Richardson, 2003; Papa-
positive or an inverted-U relationship (Kerstetter & Cho, 2004). dimitriou, Kaplanidou, & Apostolopoulou, 2015; Sonmez & Sir-
Sharifpour et al. (2014) determined that tourists with a higher akaya, 2002). Thus it is proposed that (Fig. 1b):
level of subjective knowledge prefer to depend on their personal
knowledge. One potential reason for these contradicting results is H6. Cognitive destination image influences affective destination
that researchers have different ideas regarding these familiarity- image.
related concepts, which results in the non-uniform application of
the concepts to their studies. Movies, a type of information source 2.2.2. Relationship between destination image and familiarity
and a component of educational familiarity, have been demon- While imagery is related to the production side of tourism,
strated to help increase familiarity with a destination. However, familiarity is distinct from imagery and is related to the production
Kim and Richardson (2003) demonstrated that a movie on Vienna side (Prentice, 2004). Despite their distinctiveness, previous stu-
did not increase the viewers' level of familiarity with the city. dies have demonstrated that, in general, increased familiarity
Familiarity can provide individuals with feelings of safety and provides a more favorable destination image (Beerli & Martín,
security perceptions regarding a destination (Tasci & Boylu, 2010). 2004; Kerstetter & Cho, 2004; Milman & Pizam, 1995). One's
Self-assured familiarity is an indicator of consumer confidence. subjective familiarity with a destination can influence the desti-
Information from secondary sources (Frias, Rodriguez, & Castane- nation image (Sharifpour et al., 2014; Sun et al., 2013).
da, 2008) and subjective knowledge (Sharifpour et al., 2014) may Similar to familiarity, individuals may have an image of a des-
minimize tourist risk perception. In turn, feeling safe in the des- tination without visiting it (Echtner & Ritchie, 2003) by consulting
tination may lead tourists to have a feeling of coziness (Jansen, various information sources (Greaves & Skinner, 2010). Movies and
2011). TV programs are useful sources of image formation (Kim & Ri-
Information from secondary sources and familiarity provides chardson, 2003). For example, Kim and Richardson (2003) de-
individuals with a sense of comfort (Tasci & Knutson, 2004), helps monstrated that the effects of a movie were significant for some
individuals to create a greater connection, and provides a frame of components of the image of Vienna. However, it is interesting to
reference for individuals to imagine themselves as part of an en- note that movies may also create negative images (McCartney,
vironment and to develop a person-place image (Hammitt et al., Butler, & Bennett, 2008; Sonmez & Sirakaya, 2002). Some re-
2006). Thus, increased familiarity can contribute to the emotional searchers, such as Baloglu and McCleary (1999), and Gartner
connection to a destination (Yang, Yuan, & Hu, 2009). (1993), have further proposed that information sources affect the
By extrapolating the results of previous studies, this study cognitive evaluation of a destination. External information is less
proposes the following hypotheses (Fig. 1a): capable of changing the affective aspect of an image (Li, Pan,
Zhang, & Smith, 2009).
H1. Educational familiarity influences informational familiarity When making travel decisions, individuals consider factors that
(H1a), self-described familiarity (H1b), self-assured familiarity will reduce risk as much as possible (Chang, 2011). Self-assured
(H1c), expected familiarity (H1d), and proximate familiarity (H1e). familiarity refers to the extent of feeling assured and safe (Pre-
H2. Informational familiarity influences self-described familiarity ntice, 2004). Thus, it measures the perception that mechanisms
(H2a), self-assured familiarity (H2b), expected familiarity (H2c), have been put in place to mitigate and address perceived dangers
and proximate familiarity (H2d). to an acceptable level. The safety and security of a destination have
been considered from two approaches. The first approach in-
H3. Self-described familiarity influences self-assured familiarity vestigates the safety and security of a destination as part of the
(H3a), expected familiarity (H3b), and proximate familiarity (H3c). destination image and forms one of the many cognitive attributes.
Another approach is to investigate risk perception in isolation
H4. Self-assured familiarity influences expected familiarity (H4a),
from the destination image (Fuchs & Reichel, 2011). Chew and
and proximate familiarity (H4b).
Jahari (2014) treated the perceived risk and destination image as
H5. Expected familiarity influences proximate familiarity. distinctive constructs and determined that certain types of risk
influence the destination image.
2.2. Destination familiarity and image From the cognitive perspective, familiarity with a destination
may provide a feeling of comfort (Yang et al., 2009). Studies have
2.2.1. Destination image indicated that the destination image may represent an antecedent
Destination image remains one of the most popular research of destination attachment (Prayag & Ryan, 2012; Veasna, Wu, &
topics in tourism (King, Chen, & Funk, 2015). Destination image is Huang, 2013). Thus, it is postulated that (Fig. 1b):
Please cite this article as: Tan, W.-K., & Wu, C.-E. An investigation of the relationships among destination familiarity, destination image
and future visit intention. Journal of Destination Marketing & Management (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jdmm.2015.12.008i
W.-K. Tan, C.-E. Wu / Journal of Destination Marketing & Management ∎ (∎∎∎∎) ∎∎∎–∎∎∎ 5
H7. Educational familiarity influences cognitive destination image on their own experience as their major source of information
(H7a) and affective destination image (H7b). (Assael, 2004), other studies have indicated that the information
search efforts of repeat visitors are not necessarily reduced (Lehto,
H8. Informational familiarity influences cognitive destination
O'Leary, & Morrison, 2004). Choi et al. (2011) also determined that
image (H8a) and affective destination image (H8b).
visitors tend to be more active than non-visitors in searching for
H9. Self-described familiarity influences cognitive destination information regarding Korea. Furthermore, past experience miti-
image (H9a) and affective destination image (H9b). gates consumer risk (Lehto et al., 2004; Sharifpour et al., 2014) and
generates a connection with the destination (Ednie, Daigle, &
H10. Self-assured familiarity influences cognitive destination im- Leahy, 2010).
age (H10a) and affective destination image (H10b). Experience may also affect an individual's perception of the
H11. Cognitive destination image influences expected familiarity image of a destination (Beerli & Martin, 2004; Martin & Bosque,
(H11a) and proximate familiarity (H11b). 2008). Chew and Jahari (2014, p. 383) have suggested that ‘repeat
tourists have different cognitive processes in image formation and
H12. Affective destination image influences expected familiarity travel behavior than those of first-time visitors’; thus, the desti-
(H12a) and proximate familiarity (H12b). nation image held by visitors who have made previous visits tends
to be different from the image held by first-time visitors (Frias-
2.3. Destination familiarity, image, and future visit intention Jamilena et al., 2012). Milman and Pizam (1995) empirically de-
monstrated that individuals who had previously visited Central
Studies have often demonstrated that familiarity positively in- Florida had a more positive image of the destination compared
fluences visit intention (Chen & Lin, 2012; Jeong, Holland, Jun, & with non-visitors. Phillips and Jang (2010) also identified differ-
Gibson, 2012; Tsai, 2012). For example, educational familiarity, in ences in how visitors and non-visitors construct their destination
the forms of place-specific films and TV programs, affect visit in- image of New York. However, there are also results that point in a
tention (Connell & Meyer, 2009; Hao & Ryan, 2013). Baloglu (2001) different direction. Hughes and Allen (2008) used semi-structured
also demonstrated the positive effect of informational familiarity interviews and determined there were few significant differences
on travel intention. In addition to external information sources, between the views of visitors and non-visitors regarding Central
one's subjective knowledge of a destination is an important in- and Eastern Europe.
dicator of decision-making (Sharifpour et al., 2014). Previous stu- Finally, experiential familiarity explains why tourists choose to
dies, such as Chen and Phou (2013), Loureiro (2014), Morais and visit a destination (Kerstetter and Cho, 2004). Baloglu (2001) de-
Lin (2010), and Yuksel et al. (2010), have also indicated that an termined that previous visitors who are familiar with a destination
individual's emotional connection to a destination influences are more likely to revisit the destination. Individuals who had
tourist behavioral intentions, such as future visit intention. How- previously visited Central Florida were also more interested in
ever, an individual's perceived risks may undermine the will- revisiting it compared with individuals who were merely aware of
ingness to visit a destination (Chew & Jahari, 2014). It is thus the destination (Milman & Pizam, 1995). Motivational differences
reasonable to assume that visit intention may increase if the between first-time and repeaters affect their activities while in
tourists perceive that the risks have been addressed by specific Hong Kong (Lau & McKercher, 2004).
mechanisms (i.e. assured familiarity). Thus, we suggest that Thus, past experience, such as whether an individual has vis-
(Fig. 1b): ited a destination, has often emerged as an important factor that
influences tourism decision-making. This study uses experiential
H13. Educational familiarity influences future visit intention.
familiarity as a variable for segmenting travelers into two groups:
H14. Informational familiarity influences future visit intention. non-visitors and previous visitors. The previously described hy-
potheses are subsequently tested and compared across these two
H15. Self-described familiarity influences future visit intention.
visitor groups.
H16. Self-assured familiarity influences future visit intention.
Please cite this article as: Tan, W.-K., & Wu, C.-E. An investigation of the relationships among destination familiarity, destination image
and future visit intention. Journal of Destination Marketing & Management (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jdmm.2015.12.008i
6 W.-K. Tan, C.-E. Wu / Journal of Destination Marketing & Management ∎ (∎∎∎∎) ∎∎∎–∎∎∎
Table 1 Table 2
Destination image of Hong Kong. Constructs and items.
Please cite this article as: Tan, W.-K., & Wu, C.-E. An investigation of the relationships among destination familiarity, destination image
and future visit intention. Journal of Destination Marketing & Management (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jdmm.2015.12.008i
W.-K. Tan, C.-E. Wu / Journal of Destination Marketing & Management ∎ (∎∎∎∎) ∎∎∎–∎∎∎ 7
the measurement model, test the hypotheses and conduct a multi- Table 4
group analysis. PLS was applied because few researchers have Descriptive statistics, AVE, CR and Cronbach's alpha of the constructs.
empirically examined, within a single study, the relationships
Previous visitors Non-visitors
among the different types of familiarity and their effects on des-
tination image and future visit intention. Many existing studies Mean SD AVE CR Alpha Mean SD AVE CR Alpha
have considered only a limited number of the relationships ex-
F(Edn) 3.74 0.82 0.63 0.87 0.81 3.33 0.79 0.59 0.85 0.77
amined in the present work. PLS is also suitable for complex
F(Info) 3.54 0.65 0.51 0.88 0.83 3.27 0.75 0.58 0.91 0.88
models with a substantial number of constructs, which is the case F(SD) 2.68 0.84 0.81 0.90 0.77 2.26 0.86 0.83 0.91 0.80
in this study (Urbach & Ahlemann, 2010). Furthermore, the F(SA) 3.17 0.72 0.64 0.90 0.86 2.83 0.72 0.62 0.89 0.85
method has the added advantage of placing minimal demands on F(Exp) 2.86 0.85 0.85 0.92 0.83 2.84 0.80 0.85 0.92 0.83
the measurement scales, sample size, and residual distributions F(Prox) 3.07 0.80 0.85 0.92 0.83 2.82 0.78 0.80 0.89 0.75
DI(Fame) 3.82 0.56 0.56 0.86 0.80 3.55 0.67 0.64 0.90 0.86
(Chin, 1998). The bootstrap procedure was used to generate the t-
DI(Infra) 3.37 0.68 0.60 0.88 0.83 3.18 0.64 0.61 0.86 0.84
values. DI(Aff) 3.86 0.71 0.71 0.91 0.86 3.48 0.72 0.73 0.92 0.88
INT 3.52 0.80 0.75 0.90 0.84 3.31 0.85 0.81 0.93 0.88
Please cite this article as: Tan, W.-K., & Wu, C.-E. An investigation of the relationships among destination familiarity, destination image
and future visit intention. Journal of Destination Marketing & Management (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jdmm.2015.12.008i
8 W.-K. Tan, C.-E. Wu / Journal of Destination Marketing & Management ∎ (∎∎∎∎) ∎∎∎–∎∎∎
Table 5
Discriminant validity assessment.
Previous visitors
DI(Fame) 0.745
DI(Aff) 0.243 0.843
DI(Infra) 0.448 0.526 0.774
F(SA) 0.208 0.379 0.398 0.801
F(Edn) 0.182 0.332 0.504 0.418 0.796
F(Exp) 0.076 0.434 0.373 0.394 0.306 0.924
F(Info) 0.382 0.389 0.535 0.172 0.415 0.279 0.711
F(Prox) 0.197 0.090 0.104 0.237 -0.021 0.150 0.123 0.924
F(SD) 0.339 0.017 -0.005 0.215 0.009 0.119 0.073 0.316 0.901
INT 0.483 0.502 0.544 0.277 0.335 0.288 0.471 0.168 0.094 0.867
Non-visitors
DI(Fame) 0.802
DI(Aff) 0.469 0.856
DI(Infra) 0.660 0.549 0.780
F(SA) 0.480 0.292 0.551 0.789
F(Edn) 0.610 0.340 0.521 0.524 0.770
F(Exp) 0.398 0.373 0.525 0.510 0.377 0.924
F(Info) 0.452 0.298 0.421 0.358 0.493 0.222 0.763
F(Prox) 0.326 0.399 0.440 0.396 0.425 0.547 0.336 0.894
F(SD) 0.163 0.218 0.304 0.391 0.323 0.506 0.309 0.499 0.913
INT 0.393 0.531 0.521 0.341 0.430 0.330 0.390 0.447 0.196 0.898
Note:
DI(Fame), cognitive destination image (fame and activities); DI(Aff), affective destination image; DI(Infra), cognitive destination image (basic infrastructure); F(SA), self-
assured familiarity; F(Edn), educational familiarity; F(Exp), expected familiarity; F(Info), informational familiarity; F(Prox), proximate familiarity; F(SD), self-described fa-
miliarity; INT, future visit intention.
DI1, cognitive destination image (fame and activities); DI2, affective destination image; DI3, cognitive destination image (basic infrastructure); F1, self-assured familiarity; F2,
educational familiarity; F3, expected familiarity; F4, informational familiarity; F5, proximate familiarity; F6, self-described familiarity; INT, future visit intention.
The diagonal elements (bold) are the square roots of AVE. The off-diagonal elements denote the correlations between the constructs.
4.1. Relationships between types of destination familiarity not of non-visitors (H4b); however, the difference is not significant
across the two groups. Expected familiarity only affects the prox-
The empirical analysis indicates that the extent of inter- imate familiarity of non-visitors (H5; path coefficient ¼0.288);
dependency among the different types of destination familiarity. however, the latter relationship is significantly different across the
Overall, educational familiarity is more influential than informa- two visitor groups.
tional familiarity: both educational familiarity and informational
familiarity are felt more intensely by non-visitors compared with 4.2. Destination familiarity and image
previous visitors. Educational familiarity influences informational
(H1a; previous visitor path coefficient ¼0.415, non-visitor path Not all destination familiarity types as suggested by hypotheses
coefficient ¼0.493) and self-assured (H1c; previous visitor path 7–10 influence the attributes of destination image. The cognitive
coefficient ¼0.424, non-visitor path coefficient ¼0.407) familiarity; destination image (fame and activities) of non-visitors is positively
however, it has no impact on expected familiarity (H1d). An in- influenced by educational (H7a(1); path coefficient ¼0.433), self-
crease in educational familiarity leads to an increase in proximate assured (H10a(1); path coefficient ¼ 0.233) and informational (H8a
familiarity for non-visitors (path coefficient ¼0.162), whereas the (1); path coefficient ¼0.195) familiarity; however, it is negatively
effect is reversed for previous visitors (path coefficient ¼ 0.188) affected by self-described familiarity (H9a(1); path
(H1e). Its influence on self-described familiarity applies only to coefficient ¼ 0.128). The cognitive destination image (fame and
non-visitors (path coefficient ¼0.225), but not to previous visitors, activities) of previous visitors is influenced by fewer types of fa-
which causes a significant difference across the two visitor groups miliarity, i.e., informational (H8a(1); path coefficient ¼ 0.346) and
(H1b). On the other hand, the influence of informational famil- self-described (H9a(1); path coefficient ¼ 0.296) familiarity.
iarity is more restrictive. Informational familiarity positively in- Furthermore, the influence of the various types of familiarity on
fluences the self-described familiarity (H2a; path the cognitive destination image (basic infrastructure) is more
coefficient ¼0.198) and negatively affects the expected familiarity uniform across the two visitor groups. The educational (H7a(2);
(H2c; path coefficient ¼ 0.138) of non-visitors. previous visitor path coefficient ¼0.234, non-visitor path
Self-described familiarity has a positive influence on the self- coefficient ¼0.245), informational (H8a(2); previous visitor path
assured (H3a; previous visitor path coefficient ¼0.212, non-visitor coefficient ¼0.401, non-visitor path coefficient ¼ 0.164), and self-
path coefficient ¼0.233) and proximate (H3c; previous visitor path assured (H10a(2); previous visitor path coefficient ¼0.250, non-
coefficient ¼0.238, non-visitor path coefficient ¼0.234) familiarity visitor path coefficient ¼0.349) familiarity types positively influ-
of both visitor groups; however, it only positively affects the ex- ence the cognitive destination image (basic infrastructure) of both
pected familiarity of non-visitors (H3b; path coefficient ¼0.354). visitor groups.
The relationship between self-described and expected familiarity However, the influence of destination familiarity types on af-
is significantly different across the two visitor groups. fective destination image, as compared to cognitive destination
Self-assured familiarity adds to the expected familiarity of both image, is weaker. Only the self-assured (H10b; path coefficient ¼
visitor groups (H4a; previous visitor path coefficient ¼ 0.208, non- 0.223) and informational (H8b; path coefficient ¼ 0.172) familiarity
visitor path coefficient ¼0.213). It only contributes to the prox- types influence the affective destination image; furthermore, this
imate familiarity of previous visitors (path coefficient ¼ 0.208), but influence only applies to previous visitors.
Please cite this article as: Tan, W.-K., & Wu, C.-E. An investigation of the relationships among destination familiarity, destination image
and future visit intention. Journal of Destination Marketing & Management (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jdmm.2015.12.008i
W.-K. Tan, C.-E. Wu / Journal of Destination Marketing & Management ∎ (∎∎∎∎) ∎∎∎–∎∎∎ 9
Please cite this article as: Tan, W.-K., & Wu, C.-E. An investigation of the relationships among destination familiarity, destination image
and future visit intention. Journal of Destination Marketing & Management (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jdmm.2015.12.008i
10 W.-K. Tan, C.-E. Wu / Journal of Destination Marketing & Management ∎ (∎∎∎∎) ∎∎∎–∎∎∎
has focused on the subjective aspects of familiarity. In contrast to differently. The findings suggest that compared with non-visitors,
the educational and informational familiarity types, which tend to individuals with experience with a destination (previous visitors)
be objective measures of familiarity and are related to the in- are more active and are in a better position to use external sources
formation-processing ability (Sharifpour et al., 2014), self-de- of informational familiarity to build a cognitive image of the
scribed familiarity is a subjective measure. Previous visitors de- destination.
pend on their experiences and do not need educational and in- Previous studies have also proposed that information sources
formational familiarity to form their subjective familiarity. How- affect the cognitive evaluation of a destination and are less capable
ever, non-visitors do not have this experience: thus they must of changing the affective aspect (Gartner, 1993; Li et al., 2009).
depend on educational and informational familiarity. The present Although this finding is generally correct, the statement requires
study also identified a potential influence of subjective familiarity modification given that when experiential familiarity is the mod-
on the feeling that individuals have toward the destination, such erator, informational familiarity affects the cognitive image, and it
as assurance, coziness, or connection. also influences the affective image of previous visitors, but not
As expected, the feeling that there are mechanisms in place to non-visitors. Thus, experience further enhances the usefulness of
mitigate and address risks (self-assured familiarity) may make an external information sources by assisting the affective evaluation
individual feel comfortable about a place. Self-assured familiarity of the destination.
contributes to the proximate familiarity of previous visitors, but Familiarity has been frequently demonstrated to lead to a
not non-visitors: however, the difference between the two groups better destination image (Kerstetter & Cho, 2004): however, the
is not significant. Feeling cozy (expected familiarity) toward a present study indicates that this statement is too general. When
destination does not necessarily indicate a connection with the considered from the perspective of subjective familiarity (self-
place because expected familiarity only affects the proximate fa- described familiarity), and taking into account experiential famil-
miliarity of non-visitors, but not previous visitors. iarity, a higher subjective familiarity may lead to a lower opinion
of the cognitive destination image (fame and activities) among
5.2. Destination familiarity and image non-visitors and a better opinion of the cognitive destination
image (fame and activities) among previous visitors.
The empirical results indicate the usefulness of classifying in- Past experience, along with self-assured familiarity, also exerts
formation sources as educational or informational familiarity. an influence on the cognitive destination image. Non-visitors,
Whereas information shapes the destination image (Greaves & without a personal past experience of observing the risks, are less
Skinner, 2010), this study further demonstrates that the educa- confident and factor the self-assured element into both types of
tional and informational familiarity types differ in their influences cognitive images. Previous visitors, who have a past experience
on the cognitive image, i.e. the factual aspects of the destination with the destination, are in a better position to allocate the self-
(Yang et al., 2009). These findings also indicate the strengths and assured element to the cognitive destination image (basic infra-
limitations of educational familiarity. structure), which consists of the necessary conditions for a tourist
In general, among non-visitors, educational familiarity has a destination and to leave the cognitive destination image (fame and
wider sway of influence on cognitive image compared with in- activities) alone.
formational familiarity. The opposite is true for previous visitors, The coziness of a destination is best felt when the destination
with informational familiarity exerting a more substantial influ- image is coupled with past experience. Cognitive destination im-
ence on cognitive image compared with educational familiarity. age (fame and activities), cognitive destination image (basic in-
Thus, the usefulness of educational familiarity decreases when frastructure), and affective destination image influence the ex-
past experience is available. This diminishing influence is best pected familiarity of previous visitors: however, only cognitive
demonstrated in the relationship between educational familiarity destination image (basic infrastructure) affects the expected fa-
and cognitive destination image (fame and activities), in which miliarity of non-visitors. The expected familiarity of both groups is
educational familiarity plays no role among previous visitors but also influenced by self-assured familiarity. This finding is not
has the highest path coefficient among non-visitors. surprising because if an individual feels assured in a place, and
The characteristics of the information sources can be used to Hong Kong has the necessary elements of a general tourist desti-
explain the previously described results. Educational familiarity nation, the individual should likely feel comfortable with Hong
includes information obtained from educational institutions, mo- Kong. Only affective destination image influences the proximate
vies, and TV programs. Destination-specific information received familiarity of non-visitors. Furthermore, it requires more than
from educational institutions is often part of the academic curri- being a famous tourist destination to generate a connection to a
culum. Movies and TV programs primarily provide entertainment place, and substantially more is needed for previous visitors to
and relaxation, and destination-specific information is often pas- connect with a place after a visit compared with non-visitors.
sively received by viewers. Furthermore, this information flow Affective destination image is related to how an individual feels
tends to be consistent given that movies and TV programs with about the destination. Surprisingly, cognitive destination image
Hong Kong as the backdrop are common in Taiwan. Thus, medi- (basic infrastructure), but not cognitive destination image (fame
ated learning is particularly useful in providing individuals with- and activities), influences the affective destination image. Whereas
out Hong Kong travel experience with the information necessary informational familiarity is unable to touch the heart of non-visi-
to construct a cognitive image. Past experience with Hong Kong tors when they think about the destination (affective destination
leads to a substantially more diversified and detailed demand for image), informational familiarity, combined with the past experi-
information (Morais & Lin, 2010). Individuals also need to be more ences of previous visitors, is capable of influencing the affective
proactive when seeking information covered by informational fa- destination image. Cognitive destination image (fame and activ-
miliarity. Previous visitors, using their experience as a guide, ities) is related to the reputation of and activities in Hong Kong.
gather additional information via external sources of informational Non-visitors may not be too concerned with and attracted by the
familiarity to update themselves on the destination and fill the reputation of Hong Kong, or they may not be able to judge the
gaps in their experience. activities of Hong Kong without first experiencing them. Thus,
Thus, the general statement that information helps to shape the cognitive destination image (fame and activities) plays no part in
destination image has to be modified because non-visitors and influencing the affective destination image of non-visitors. How-
previous visitors use informational and educational familiarity ever, the importance of cognitive destination image (fame and
Please cite this article as: Tan, W.-K., & Wu, C.-E. An investigation of the relationships among destination familiarity, destination image
and future visit intention. Journal of Destination Marketing & Management (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jdmm.2015.12.008i
W.-K. Tan, C.-E. Wu / Journal of Destination Marketing & Management ∎ (∎∎∎∎) ∎∎∎–∎∎∎ 11
activities) as viewed by previous visitors cannot be ignored be- study thus supports the argument that the effectiveness of TV
cause cognitive destination image (fame and activities) directly travel programs depends on whether the destination is attracting
influences visit intention rather than through the affective desti- an emerging or a mature market, and fresh new angles are needed
nation image. for mature destinations (McCartney et al., 2008).
5.3. Destination familiarity, image, and future visit intention 5.4. Practical implications
Destination image studies are in agreement that a positive Several practical implications have emerged from this study.
image is needed to attract tourists (Greaves & Skinner, 2010). This When the path coefficients of the significant positive relationship
study confirms this observation, with a slight modification. The between the various constructs and future visit intention were
future visit intention of previous visitors is most influenced by the ranked, informational familiarity often ranked relatively low.
reputation of Hong Kong and its activities, followed by the visitors DMOs should note that merely pushing destination information to
feeling toward the destination, the general features of the tourist potential customers is not sufficient: there should be other stra-
destination, and informational familiarity. Cognitive destination tegies to generate interest to visit the destination. The findings
image (fame and activities) plays no role in encouraging non- clearly indicate that previous visitors and non-visitors differ in
visitors to visit the destination, and the difference with previous many ways. Thus, there is a need for DMOs to devise different
visitors is sufficiently significant. In contrast, the future visit in- marketing strategies for these two groups. For example, whereas it
tention of non-visitors is most positively affected by the affective is useful to encourage individuals to revisit Hong Kong by high-
destination image, cognitive destination image (basic infra- lighting the activities individuals can do there, it is more difficult
structure), proximate familiarity, and informational familiarity; to persuade non-visitors via this mean.
however, it is negatively influenced by self-described familiarity. DMOs need to be aware of the importance of affective desti-
Similar to other studies (Baloglu, 2001), the present work nation image to both groups, i.e. the affective destination image
provides empirical evidence that indicates the importance of ex- affects future visit intention. However, the levers for influencing
ternal information given that informational familiarity directly the affective destination image of non-visitors are substantially
influences future visit intention in both visitor groups and in- more limited compared with previous visitors. Thus, there is a
directly affects this intention via other constructs. However, pre- need to increase the number of levers available. DMOs also need to
vious studies have not often highlighted or adequately considered carefully consider the issue that cognitive destination image (fame
some of the other results of the current research. and activities) is not a direct antecedent of affective destination
First, emotional connection and attachment to a place are often image. Hong Kong DMOs have been actively creating the image
investigated from the perspective of individuals who have visited a that Hong Kong is a place to eat, shop, and have fun. However, this
destination or have participated in an activity. In contrast to some approach is not convincing for non-visitors.
studies, such as Morais and Lin (2010), few studies have in- Practitioners should also further investigate the potential in-
vestigated the place connection among non-visitors. The present fluence of self-described familiarity on self-assured, expected, and
study has demonstrated that proximate familiarity (which is re- proximate familiarity. Self-described familiarity may provide sur-
lated to the connection to a place) may also exist in the minds of prises for DMOs. Whereas familiarity leads to a better destination
non-visitors (although at a lower level than in previous visitors) image, this study indicates that this statement is too general. As
and influence visit intention. Place connection is formed through previously discussed, a high self-described familiarity among non-
educational, self-described, and expected familiarity, as well as visitors may lead to a lower opinion of the cognitive destination
affective destination image. image (fame and activities) and adversely affect future visit in-
Second, familiarity is often assumed to lead to visit intention tention. This finding implies that non-visitors may be more diffi-
(Chen & Lin, 2012). However, this is not always true for self-de- cult to please and may have preconceived, unfavorable ideas re-
scribed familiarity. This study has demonstrated that the more garding Hong Kong. In addition to addressing their concerns,
non-visitors think they know about Hong Kong, the less likely they DMOs should also constantly generate ‘surprises’ so that non-
will visit the place. This finding may imply that non-visitors are visitors never feel too familiar with Hong Kong. However, self-
more difficult to please and may already have a preconceived, described familiarity can add to self-assured and proximate fa-
unfavorable idea about Hong Kong. These outcomes should also miliarity, as well as positively and indirectly influence future visit
take into account the specifics of a destination. Hong Kong is intention via other routes. Thus, DMOs need to make trade-offs
geographically and culturally close to Taiwan: thus, a substantial when dealing with self-described familiarity.
portion of individuals who are attracted by its reputation should Feeling coziness toward a destination helps non-visitors to
have already visited Hong Kong, which leaves the individuals who generate proximate familiarity and, subsequently, future visit in-
have not visited Hong Kong (the non-visitor group) as being less tention. However, the negative relationship between informational
moved by this reason. Instead, non-visitors are more concerned and expected familiarity among non-visitors may imply that the
regarding what they feel about the destination (i.e. affective des- content of the information gathered does not provide non-visitors,
tination image and proximate familiarity). who have no firsthand experience to rely on, with the perception
Third, educational familiarity does not directly influence visit that Hong Kong is a comfortable place. Because of their past ex-
intention, and its effect is only indirectly felt through other con- perience, previous visitors have less need for expected and prox-
structs, given that many studies have demonstrated that movie- imate familiarity to influence their future visit intention.
induced tourism plays a role in drawing visitors (Connell & Meyer, In the previous visitor group, expected familiarity had the
2009). One potential reason is that Hong Kong is a relatively ma- lowest mean among the different types of familiarity, and cogni-
ture market in Taiwan, and movies and TV programs with Hong tive destination image (fame and activities) exhibited a negative
Kong as the backdrop are a common feature in the Taiwan en- impact on expected familiarity. Thus, Hong Kong DMOs need to
tertainment scene. Hong Kong is also frequently mentioned in ask previous visitors whether their trip to Hong Kong provided
formal education media. As a result, although educational famil- them with the impression that the place was crowded, disorderly,
iarity had the highest mean in both groups, short of special or fast-paced, among other characteristics, which could have un-
themes, new storylines, or ‘shocks’, it would be difficult for this dermined the feeling of coziness. Whereas Hong Kong is definitely
familiarity type to induce a direct impact on visit intention. This a bustling city, DMOs may want to compare it with other
Please cite this article as: Tan, W.-K., & Wu, C.-E. An investigation of the relationships among destination familiarity, destination image
and future visit intention. Journal of Destination Marketing & Management (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jdmm.2015.12.008i
12 W.-K. Tan, C.-E. Wu / Journal of Destination Marketing & Management ∎ (∎∎∎∎) ∎∎∎–∎∎∎
Please cite this article as: Tan, W.-K., & Wu, C.-E. An investigation of the relationships among destination familiarity, destination image
and future visit intention. Journal of Destination Marketing & Management (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jdmm.2015.12.008i
W.-K. Tan, C.-E. Wu / Journal of Destination Marketing & Management ∎ (∎∎∎∎) ∎∎∎–∎∎∎ 13
Gursoy, D. (2011). Modeling tourist information search behavior: A structural mod- Ozdemir, B., Aksu, A., Ehtiyar, R., Cizel, B., Cizel, R. B., & Icigen, E. T. (2012). Re-
eling approach. Saarbrucken: Lambert Academic Publishing. lationships among tourist profile, satisfaction and destination loyalty: Ex-
Hair, J. F., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L., & Black, W. C. (1998). Multivariate data amining empirical evidence in Antalya region in Turkey. Journal of Hospitality
analysis ((5th ed.). New Jersey: Prentice Hall. Marketing Management, 21(5), 506–540.
Hammitt, W. E., Backlund, E. A., & Bixler, R. D. (2006). Place bonding for recreation Papadimitriou, D., Kaplanidou, K. K., & Apostolopoulou, A. (2015). Destination im-
places: Conceptual and empirical development. Leisure Studies, 25, 17–41. age components and word-of-mouth intentions in urban tourism: A multi-
Hao, X., & Ryan, C. (2013). Interpretation, film language and tourist destinations: A group approach. Journal of Hospitality Tourism Research.
case study of Hibiscus Town, China. Annals of Tourism Research, 42, 334–358. Papathanassis, A., & Knolle, F. (2011). Exploring the adoption and processing of
Horng, J. S., Liu, C. H., Chou, H. Y., & Tsai, C. Y. (2012). Understanding the impact of online holiday reviews: A grounded theory approach. Tourism Management, 32,
culinary brand equity and destination familiarity on travel intentions. Tourism 215–224.
Management, 33, 815–824. Phillips, W. M. J., & Jang, S. C. (2010). Destination image differences between visitors
Huang, L., Gursoy, D., & Xu, H. (2014). Impact of personality traits and involvement and non-visitors: a case of New York City. International Journal of Tourism Re-
on prior knowledge. Annals of Tourism Research, 48, 42–57. search, 12, 642–645.
Hughes, H. L., & Allen, D. (2008). Visitor and non-visitor images of Central and Prayag, G., & Ryan, C. (2012). Antecedents of tourists' loyalty to Mauritius: The role
Eastern Europe: A qualitative analysis. International Journal of Tourism Research, and influence of destination image, place attachment, involvement, and sa-
10, 27–40. tisfaction. Journal of Travel Research, 51(3), 342–356.
Jansen, H. J. (2011). Tourist familiarity in Amsterdam: Route choice behaviour of (un) Prentice, R. (2004). Tourism familiarity and imagery. Annals of Tourism Research, 31
familiar domestic tourists within Amsterdam's inner city (Master's dissertation). (4), 923–945.
The Netherlands: Universiteit Utrecht. Prentice, R. (2006). Evocation and experiential seduction: Updating choice-sets
Jeong, C., Holland, S., Jun, S. H., & Gibson, H. (2012). Enhancing destination image modeling. Tourism Management, 27, 1153–1170.
through travel website information. International Journal of Tourism Research, 14 Prentice, R., & Andersen, V. (2003). Festival as creative destination. Annals of
(1), 16–27. Tourism Research, 30, 7–30.
Kastenholz, E. (2010). ‘Cultural proximity’ as a determinant of destination image. Qu, H. L., Kim, L. H., & Im, H. H. (2011). A model of destination branding: Integrating
Journal of Vacation Marketing, 16, 313–322. the concepts of the branding and destination image. Tourism Management, 32,
Kastenholz, E., Eusebio, C., & Carneiro, M. J. (2013). Studying factors influencing 465–476.
repeat visitation of cultural tourists. Journal of Vacation Marketing, 19(4), Ringle, C.M., Wende, S., and Will, A. (2008). SmartPLS 2.0 (Beta). Available at 〈http://
343–358. www.smartpls.de〉.
Kerstetter, D., & Cho, M. (2004). Prior knowledge, credibility and information Riscinto-Kozub, K., & Childs, N. (2012). Conversion of local winery awareness. In-
search. Annals of Tourism Research, 31(4), 961–985. ternational Journal of Wine Business Research, 24(4), 287–301.
Khosrowjerdi, M., & Iranshahi, M. (2011). Prior knowledge and information-seeking Sharifpour, M., Walters, G., Ritchie, B., & Winter, C. (2014). Investigating the role of
behavior of PhD and MA students. Library Information Science Research, 33, prior knowledge in tourist decision making: A structural equation model of risk
331–335. perceptions and information search. Journal of Travel Research, 53(3), 307–322.
Kim, J. H. (2014). The antecedents of memorable tourism experiences: The devel- Snepenger, D., Meged, K., Snelling, M., & Worrall, K. (1990). Information search
opment of a scale to measure the destination attributes associated with strategies by destination-native tourists. Journal of Travel Research, 29(1), 13–16.
memorable experiences. Tourism Management, 44, 34–45. Sonmez, S. F., & Sirakaya, E. (2002). A distorted destination image? The case of
Kim, H., & Richardson, S. (2003). Motion picture impacts on destination images. Turkey. Journal of Travel Research, 41(2), 185–196.
Annals of Tourism Research, 30(1), 216–237. Srull, T. (1983). The role of prior knowledge in the acquisition, retention, and use of
King, C., Chen, N., & Funk, D. C. (2015). Exploring destination image decay: A study new information In: R. Bagozzi, & A. Tybout (Eds.), Advances in consumer re-
of sport tourists' destination image change after event participation. Journal of search, 10 (pp. 572–576). Ann Arbor, MI: Association for Consumer Research.
Hospitality Tourism Research, 39(1), 3–31. Sun, X., Chi, C. G. Q., & Xu, H. (2013). Developing destination loyalty: The case of
Kozak, M., Crotts, J. C., & Law, R. (2007). The impact of the perception of risk on Hainan Island. Annals of Tourism Research, 43, 547–577.
international travelers. International Journal of Tourism Research, 9(4), 233–242. Tasci, A. (2009). Social distance: The missing link in the loop of movies, destination
Lau, A., & McKercher, B. (2004). Exploration versus acquisition: A comparison of image, and tourist behavior? Journal of Travel Research, 47(4), 494–507.
first-time and repeat visitors. Journal of Travel Research, 42, 279–285. Tasci, A., & Boylu, Y. (2010). Cultural comparison of tourists' safety perception in
Lee, I., Floyd, M. F., & Shinew, K. J. (2002). The relationship between information use relation to trip satisfaction. International Journal of Tourism Research, 12,
and park awareness: A study of urban park users. Journal of Park and Recreation 179–192.
Administration, 20(1), 22–41. Tasci, A., Gartner, W., & Cavusgil, S. (2007). Conceptualization and operationaliza-
Lehto, X., O'Leary, J. T., & Morrison, A. (2004). The effect of prior experience on tion of destination image. Journal of Hospitality Tourism Research, 31(2),
vacation behavior. Annals of Tourism Research, 31(4), 801–818. 194–223.
Li, X., Pan, B., Zhang, L., & Smith, W. (2009). The effect of online information search Tasci, A., & Knutson, B. (2004). An argument for providing authenticity and famil-
on image development: Insights from a mixed-methods study. Journal of Travel iarity in tourism destinations. Journal of Hospitality Leisure Marketing, 11(1),
Research, 48(1), 45–57. 85–109.
Lim, Y., & Weaver, P. A. (2014). Customer-based brand equity for a destination: The Tsai (2012). Place attachment and tourism marketing: Investigating international
effect of destination image on preference for products associated with a des- tourists in Singapore. International Journal of Tourism Research, 14, 139–152.
tination brand. International Journal of Tourism Research, 16, 223–231. Urbach, N., & Ahlemann, F. (2010). Structural equation modeling in information
Loureiro, S. M. C. (2014). The role of the rural tourism experience economy in place systems research using partial least squares. Journal of Information Technology
attachment and behavioral intentions. International Journal of Hospitality Theory and Application, 11(2), 5–40.
Management, 40, 1–9. Veasna, S., Wu, W., & Huang, C. (2013). The impact of destination source credibility
Lu, A. C. C., Chi, C. G. Q., & Lu, C. Y. (2014). Sensation seeking, message sensation on destination satisfaction: The mediating effects of destination attachment
value, and destinations: A cross-cultural comparison. Journal of Hospitality
and destination image. Tourism Management, 36, 511–526.
Tourism Research.
Webb, D. (2000). Understanding customer role and its importance in the formation
Martin, H. S., & Bosque, I. A. R. (2008). Exploring the cognitive-affective nature of
of service quality expectations. The Service Industries Journal, 20(1), 1–21.
destination image and the role of psychological factors in its formation. Tourism
Wong, C., & Liu, F. (2011). A study of pre-trip use of travel guidebooks by leisure
Management, 29, 263–277.
travelers. Tourism Management, 32, 616–628.
McCartney, G., Butler, R., & Bennett, M. (2008). A strategic use of the commu-
Xiang, Z., Magnini, V. P., & Fesenmaier, D. R. (2015). Information technology and
nication mix in the destination image-formation process. Journal of Travel Re-
consumer behavior in travel and tourism: Insights from travel planning using
search, 47(2), 183–196.
the internet. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 22, 244–249.
McGregor, A. (2000). Dynamic texts and tourist gaze: Death, bones and buffalo.
Yang, J., Yuan, B., & Hu, P. (2009). Tourism destination image and visit intention:
Annals of Tourism Research, 27, 27–50.
Examining the role of familiarity. Journal of China Tourism Research, 5, 174–187.
McIntosh, R. W. (1972). Tourism principles, practices, and philosophies. Columbus,
Yuksel, A., Yuksel, F., & Bilim, Y. (2010). Destination attachment: Effects on customer
OH: Grid.
satisfaction and cognitive, affective and conative loyalty. Tourism Management,
Milman, A., & Pizam, A. (1995). The role of awareness and familiarity with a des-
31(2), 274–284.
tination: The central Florida case. Journal of Travel Research, 33, 21–27.
Zhang, H., Fu, X., Cai, L. A., & Lu, L. (2014). Destination image and tourist loyalty: A
Morais, D., & Lin, C. (2010). Why do first-time and repeat visitors patronize a
meta-analysis. Tourism Management, 40, 213–223.
destination? Journal of Travel Tourism Marketing, 27, 193–210.
Osti, L., Turner, L., & King, B. (2009). Cultural differences in travel guidebooks in-
formation search. Journal of Vacation Marketing, 15(1), 63–78.
Please cite this article as: Tan, W.-K., & Wu, C.-E. An investigation of the relationships among destination familiarity, destination image
and future visit intention. Journal of Destination Marketing & Management (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jdmm.2015.12.008i