Provided by UWE Bristol Research Repository
Provided by UWE Bristol Research Repository
Brief Report: Testing a Dissonance Body Image Intervention among Young Girls
Full citation:
Halliwell, E., & Diedrichs, P.C. (2014). Brief report: Testing a dissonance body image
intervention among young girls. Health Psychology, 33, 201-204. doi: 10.1037/a0032585
Author Note
Emma Halliwell, Centre for Appearance Research, University of the West of England, UK;
Phillipa C. Diedrichs, Centre for Appearance Research, University of the West of England,
UK.
Psychology, University of the West of England, Frenchay, Coldharbour Lane, Bristol, BS16
The authors wish to acknowledge the assistance of Chantelle Bailey and Rebecca Bellow in
1
Running head: Brief Report: Testing a Dissonance Body Image Intervention
Abstract
Objective: Body image and eating disorder interventions based on cognitive dissonance have
been shown to be effective among girls and women aged 14 and above. This paper reports a
delivered in a school setting to 12 and 13 year old girls in the UK. Methods: Girls (N = 106,
mean age= 12.07 years, SD = .27) were allocated to the intervention condition or a waitlist
control. Results: In contrast to the control group, girls in the intervention condition reported
significant reductions in body dissatisfaction and internalization of a thin body ideal post-
intervention. There was no significant change in self-reported dietary restraint for either
condition. In addition, compared to the control group, girls in the intervention condition
Conclusions: Results suggests that dissonance based programs can reduce body
dissatisfaction, internalization and negative media effects among a younger group of girls
2
Running head: Brief Report: Testing a Dissonance Body Image Intervention
Brief Report: Testing a Dissonance Body Image Intervention among Young Girls
(Wertheim & Paxton, 2011) and has numerous negative consequences including increased
negative affect (Stice & Bearman, 2001) and reduced levels of physical activity (Neumark-
Sztainer, Paxton, Hannan, Haines, & Story, 2006). Body dissatisfaction is also a risk factor
for the onset of eating disorders (Stice, Marti, & Durant, 2011).
effective eating disorder and body dissatisfaction prevention programs. Meta-analytic review
reveals that they are the most effective targeted interventions to date for girls aged 14 and
above (Stice, Shaw, & Marti, 2007). For girls with pre-existing body image issues, CD
affect, psychosocial impairment and risk for onset of eating disorders (Stice, et al., 2007) with
effects maintained up to three years post-intervention (e.g. Stice, Rohde, Shaw & Gau, 2011).
In addition, there is evidence that CD interventions are also effective as universal prevention
programs, although they have smaller effects in universal than in targeted trials (e.g. Becker,
media literacy and behavioral components. However, a critical component of the intervention
is engaging participants in counter-attitudinal activities that require them to speak out against
the thin ideal of beauty (Stice & Presnell, 2007). This creates dissonance because individuals
who internalize the thin ideal are acting in a manner inconsistent with their beliefs. As
dissonance is uncomfortable, individuals change their beliefs to correspond with their actions.
during the sessions. Comparison of CD activities and the full intervention indicates that both
are effective at reducing eating pathology (Roehrig, Thompson, Brannick, & van den Berg,
3
Running head: Brief Report: Testing a Dissonance Body Image Intervention
2006) and supports the central role of dissonance. The most widely used CD programs
involve four hours of intervention. However, two hours of intervention has been shown to be
To date, CD interventions have only been examined among girls aged 14 or older, and
there is currently no intervention with a similar evidence-base for early adolescent girls. This
is an important gap in the literature. Body dissatisfaction is often established before 14 and
internalization is evident among 7-11 year old girls (Evans, Tové, Boothroyd & Drewett, in
press). The need for early prevention efforts has been increasingly recognized by researchers
and clinicians (Holt & Ricciardelli, 2008). Given the efficacy of CD interventions with older
Exposure to media portraying the thin ideal of beauty is a variable risk factor for the
development of body dissatisfaction and disordered eating among girls (Levine & Murnen,
2009). Meta-analysis reveals that exposure to thin-ideal media has an immediate negative
effect on adolescent girls’ body image (e.g. Grabe, et al., 2008). To date, the ability of CD
interventions to increase resilience to media exposure effects has not been investigated.
brief, CD intervention among 12 and 13 year old girls in the UK. It examines whether a CD
intervention has the capacity to increase girls’ resilience to thin ideal media, in addition to
Method
Participants
Participants were 104 girls aged 12 and 13 years (Mage=12.07, SDage = .27; MBMI=
19.92, SDBMI = 4.56), from a single-sex secondary school. Seventy percent of the girls were
White, 7% were Black, 12% were Asian, and 11% were Mixed Race. School staff assigned
4
Running head: Brief Report: Testing a Dissonance Body Image Intervention
two classes (n = 52) to the intervention condition and two classes (n = 52) to the control
condition. Ninety-six girls completed a follow-up media exposure study and were randomly
Materials
Six activities from the Body Project (Stice & Presnell, 2007) were selected for this
intervention. Participants also completed two additional psycho educational and media
literacy activities which focused on critiquing the thin-ideal. The content of each session is
reported in table 1.
Advertisements were selected from magazines aimed at adolescent girls. In the model
exposure condition three featured thin models and one featured the product only. In the
control condition the four advertisements featured hair or perfume products and no models.
Measures
Internalization of the thin ideal. The Thin Ideal Internalization Scale (Stice & Agras,
1998) was administered. Pre-testing revealed that 12 year old girls did not understand the
term ‘shapely’ and one item was deleted for this study. Items were rated on a 5-point Likert-
scale (1= strongly disagree and 5= strongly agree). The Cronbach’s alpha was 0.77 at
stomach, buttocks, and thighs) from the Body Parts Scale (Berscheid, Walster, & Bohrnstedt,
1973) on a 5-point Likert scale (1= very satisfied and 5= very dissatisfied). The Cronbach’s
5
Running head: Brief Report: Testing a Dissonance Body Image Intervention
Dietary restraint. The Dutch Restrained Eating Scale (DRES; Van Strien, Frijiters,
Bergers, & Defares, 1986) was used with items rated on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = never and
5 = always). The Cronbach’s alpha was 0.87 at baseline and 0.88 post-intervention.
These trait measures are valid for adolescent girls (Stice et al., 2006).
State body satisfaction. Participants rated how satisfied they felt right now with their
‘body size and shape’ and their ‘weight’ on a 7-point scale (1 = extremely dissatisfied and 7 =
extremely satisfied). The items were highly correlated, r(94) = .85, p<.001.
Procedure
The study was approved by the university ethics committee. Passive parental consent
was obtained for 98% of girls. A week before the intervention girls were informed about the
study and invited to participate. All eligible girls consented and completed the baseline
questionnaire supervised by the first author. Girls in the control condition attended their
regular lessons which did not include content related to body image. Girls in the intervention
condition attended weekly 20 minute sessions for four weeks. They worked in groups of 6 to
8 facilitated by one of the authors or a doctoral student. The week after the last session all
Four weeks later participants took part in a media exposure study. This was presented
control images. They were told that current mood can influence our preferences and were
asked to rate how they were feeling right now about various aspects of their lives including
two items assessing state body satisfaction. Participants were debriefed. After the study was
completed the control group of girls we also invited to take part in the intervention.
Results
6
Running head: Brief Report: Testing a Dissonance Body Image Intervention
Fourteen participants did not complete the follow up questionnaire; they did not differ
significantly from the participants who completed follow up on any variables. Maximum-
likelihood estimation was used to impute missing data for these participants as it is preferable
to case deletion (Schafer & Graham, 2002). At baseline there were no significant differences
between the girls assigned to each condition on BMI, internalization, or restraint. However,
girls in the intervention condition reported higher body dissatisfaction (M = 3.09, SD = .93)
than girls in the control condition (M = 2.70, SD = .86), t(102) = -2.32, p = .02, d = .44.
interaction effect, Lambda = .88, F(3, 100) = 4.49, p = .01, partial η2 = .12. Significant
univariate interaction effects between time and condition were evident for internalization,
F(1, 102) = 7.10, p = .009, partial η2 = .07 and body dissatisfaction, F(1, 102) = 10.02, p =
.002, partial η2 = .09, but not for restraint, F(1, 102) = .77, p = .38, partial η2 = .01.
In the control group there were no statistically significant differences across time for
.69) or body dissatisfaction, t(51) = -1.20, p = .24, d = -.10 (T1 M = 2.68, SD = .86, T2 M =
internalization, t(51) = 2.44, p = .02, d = .36, (T1 M = 3.20, SD = .83 T2 M = 2.91, SD = .80)
and body dissatisfaction, t(51) = 3.18, p = .003, d = .29 (T1 M = 3.09, SD = .93 T2 M = 2.82,
SD = .96).
revealed non-significant main effects for intervention F(1, 80) = 2.16, ns, partial η2 = .03, and
7
Running head: Brief Report: Testing a Dissonance Body Image Intervention
media exposure, F(1, 80) = 1.32, ns, partial η2 = .02. However, there was a significant
interaction between the intervention and exposure, F(1, 73) = 6.61, p < .05, partial η2 = .08.
The control participants reported less body satisfaction after exposure to thin models
(M = 3.63, SD = 1.75) than after control images (M = 4.78, SD = 1.63), F(1,37) = 4.46, p <
.05, partial η2 = .11. In the intervention group there was no significant difference between
state body satisfaction reported after viewing thin models (M = 4.88, SD = 1.71) or control
Discussion
Girls aged 12 and 13 reported significantly lower levels of trait body dissatisfaction
and internalization after participating in a relatively short CD intervention. These findings are
promising as they suggest that CD interventions may be effective among a younger group of
girls than has been previously studied and that the intervention is effective in the UK, as well
as the USA. Another novel finding of this study is that the girls who took part in the
intervention were protected from negative media exposure effects five weeks post-
intervention. This suggests that CD interventions may also provide resilience to thin ideal
media imagery, a variable risk factor for the development of eating disorders (Levine &
Murnen, 2009). The media exposure results also have the advantage of being free from
As expected, the effect sizes in this study (d = .36 for internalization and d = .29 for
body dissatisfaction) are lower than effect sizes from pre to immediately post treatment in
targeted trials (internalization, d = 1.09 and body dissatisfaction, d = .74, Stice et al, 2006).
However, our effect sizes are also smaller than in other universal trials (internalization, d =
.53 and body dissatisfaction, d = .33, Becker et al., 2008). The shorter intervention may have
lower efficacy or dissonance may be less powerful for 12 and 13 year old girls than for older
8
Running head: Brief Report: Testing a Dissonance Body Image Intervention
restraint do not correlate with actual calorie restriction (Stice et al., 2007) and moderate levels
of dieting can be related to positive health behaviors, such as increased consumption of fruit
and vegetables and decreased fat consumption (Daee et al., 2002). However, an eight year
prospective study with adolescent girls indicates that self-reported dietary restraint assessed
via the DRES is a risk factor for the onset of eating disorders (Stice et al., 2011). Although
this was a non-targeted sample, the initial levels of restraint (intervention M = 2.19, SD = .84,
control M = 2.37, SD = .86) were similar to those in a targeted intervention with girls aged14
to 19 (Stice et al, 2009; intervention M = 2.35, SD = .86, control M = 2.31, SD = .86). It may
be that the restraint items are interpreted differently by early and middle/late adolescent girls.
The intervention is multifaceted and it is not possible to tease apart the influence of
dissonance and media literacy components in this study. It would be informative to examine
the impact of this intervention on a more comprehensive measure of body satisfaction and on
a measure of restraint that taps into actual reductions in food intake, like the Eating Inventory
(Stunkard & Messer, 1988). Another limitation of this study is that allocation to condition
was not randomized. Constraints on condition allocation are typical of applied research. In
this case, concerns about lack of randomization are reduced by the known efficacy of the
intervention and because the analysis accounts for baseline differences in the study variables.
The choice of a non-targeted, shortened intervention was driven by the need to have
tools that can be easily integrated into the curriculum. The results of this initial investigation
of a CD intervention with a younger age group are promising. They findings clearly need
replication and future research should evaluate the longer term impact of the CD intervention.
However, these results add to the evidence that relatively short CD interventions are effective
9
Running head: Brief Report: Testing a Dissonance Body Image Intervention
References
Becker, C. B., Smith, L. M., & Ciao, A. C. (2006). Peer-facilitated eating disorder prevention: A
Berschied, E., Walster, E., & Bohrnstedt, G. (1973, November). The happy American body: A
Daee, A., Robinson, P., Lawson, M., Turpin, J.S., Gregory, B., & Tobias, J.D. (2002). Psychologic
and physiologic effects of dieting in adolescents. Southern Medical Journal, 95, 1032-1041.
Grabe, S., Ward, L.M., & Hyde, J.S. (2008). The role of the media in body image concerns among
134, 460-476.
Holt, K. E., & Ricciardelli, L. A. (2008). Weight concerns among elementary school children: A
Levine, M.P., & Murnen, S.K. (2009). “Everybody knows that mass media are/are not [pick one] a
cause of eating disorders”. A critical review of evidence for a causal link between media,
negative body image, and disordered eating in females. Journal of Social and Clinical
Matusek, J.A., Wendt, S.J., & Wiseman, C.V. (2004). Dissonance thin-ideal and didactic healthy
behavior eating disorder prevention programs: Results from a controlled trial. International
Neumark-Sztainer, D., Paxton, S.J., Hannan, P.J., Haines, J. & Story, M. (2006). Does body
Roehrig, M., Thompson, J.K., Brannick, M., & van den Berg, P. (2006). Dissonance-based eating
10
Running head: Brief Report: Testing a Dissonance Body Image Intervention
Schafer, J.L., & Graham, J.W. (2002). Missing data: Our view of the state of the art. Psychological
Methods, 7, 147-177.
Evans, E.H., Tovée, M.J., Boothroyd, L. G., & Drewett, R.F. (in press). Body dissatisfaction and
Stice, E., & Bearman, K. (2001). Body-image and eating disturbances prospectively predict increases
Stice, E., Shaw, H., Burton, E., & Wade, E. (2006). Dissonance and healthy weight eating disorder
Stice, E., & Presnell, K. (2007). The body project: Promoting body acceptance and preventing
Stice, E., Cooper, J, Al, Schoeller, D. A., Tappe, K., & Lowe, M.R. (2007). Are dietary restraint
scales valid measures of moderate- to long-term dietary restriction? Objective biological and
Stice, E., Rohde, P., Shaw, H., & Gau, J. (2011). An effectiveness trial of a selected dissonance-
based eating disorder prevention program for female high school students: Long-term effects.
Stice, E., Shaw, H., & Marti, C. N. (2007). A meta-analytic review of eating disorder prevention
Stunkard, A.J., & Messick, S. (1988). The Eating Inventory. San Antonio: Psychological Corp.
Van Strien, T., Frijiters, J.E., Bergers, G.P.A., & Defares, P.B. (1996). The Dutch Eating Behavior
Wertheim, E & Paxton, S.J. ( 2011). Body image development in adolescent girls. p. 76-84, In Cash,
T., & Smolak, L. (eds.). Body Image: A handbook of science, practices and prevention.
11
Running head: Brief Report: Testing a Dissonance Body Image Intervention
2. Cost of pursuing the thin ideal. Due to the age of the participants this discussion
was limited to the costs of the thin ideal to themselves and other girls, and the
struggling with her body image to persuade the girl not to pursue the thin-ideal.
2. The session focused on critiquing beauty ideals and beauty myths. Participants
demonstrate airbrushing.*
dominant beauty myths, (e.g., ‘Most models and actresses are a healthy weight’,
‘Attractive people are happier than less attractive people’), and one focused on the
media (‘Looking at fashion magazines can make girls and women feel bad about
themselves’). They were then given evidence challenging each beauty myth and
supporting the media effect and asked to reassess their original answers.*
4 1. Role-play to resist pressure to pursue the thin ideal. The facilitator took the role of
*Additional activities
12