Fragments of Memory The Case of H.M and the Medial Temporal Lobes’ Role in Memory Formation - Rohan Pandit
Fragments of Memory The Case of H.M and the Medial Temporal Lobes’ Role in Memory Formation - Rohan Pandit
Formation
Rohan Pandit
PSYCH 250
Carolyn A Cohen
11/18/2024
Fragments of Memory: The Case of H.M. and the Medial Temporal Lobes’ Role in
Memory Formation
Memory defines the essence of human experience, shaping our understanding of the
world and anchoring our sense of identity. Try to imagine a life where new memories cannot be
formed. Where every moment was lived in isolation from the past and anticipation for the future.
This was the unfortunate reality for Henry Molaison, widely recognized as H.M. His case
1953, H.M. underwent a radical surgical procedure to alleviate intractable epilepsy by removing
portions of his medial temporal lobes, including the hippocampus. While the operation achieved
its goal of reducing seizures, it resulted in severe anterograde amnesia, leaving H.M. unable to
form new declarative memories (Scoville & Milner, 1957). This case became a landmark in
cognitive neuroscience, transforming our understanding of how memories are encoded, stored,
and retrieved.
Before H. M’s case, most scholars believed that memory was diffusely distributed across
the brain, as theorized by Karl Lashley (Lashley, 1950). However, the problems that H.M had
with his memory challenged this idea, showing that the medial temporal lobes play an important
role in forming declarative memories, especially episodic and semantic ones (Milner, Corkin, &
Teuber, 1968). Even with the inability to form long-term memories, H.M still had his procedural
memory, such as learning to trace shapes in a mirror (Corkin, 1968). This dissociation
highlighted the specialized functions of brain regions and laid the foundation for the distinction
This paper will explore the medial temporal lobes’ role in memory formation and
retrieval, using H.M’s case as the focal point. Through an analysis of primary research studies,
exploration of neurological mechanisms, and a home-based experiment which I conducted, I aim
to demonstrate how the medial temporal lobes enabled the encoding of episodic and semantic
memories while leaving other types of memory intact. Furthermore, this paper will also talk
Prior the H.M’s case, prevailing theories about memory showed that it was distributed
across the brain in a diffuse manner. Karl Lashley’s studies on lesioning cortical region in rats in
search of the “engram” (the physical trace of memory) concluded that memory was not localized
to any specific area in the brain (Lashley, 1950). Lashley suggested the principles of mass action
and equipotentiality, claiming that memory impairment depended more on the total amount of
While these ideas guided early memory research, H.M’s specific memory deficits directly
contradicted these claims. The discovery that damage to the medial temporal lobes disrupted
declarative memory formation but spared procedural memory challenged Lashley’s conclusions
and marked a paradigm shift in memory research (Scoville & Milner, 1957).
H.M’s case was essentially direct evidence for the functional specialization of the medial
temporal lobes, particularly the hippocampus and surrounding structures, in memory formation.
Scoville and Milner (1957) first reported that bilateral removal of the medial temporal lobes
would result in severe anterograde amnesia. H.M could not form new declarative memories, such
as remembering people he had met or events that happened after his operation. However, he was
still able to perform tasks like drawing shapes in a mirror, which were all procedural memories
(Corkin, 1968). This differentiation created the foundation for distinguishing between declarative
Milner et al. (1968) found that while H.M retained his personality and short-term memory his
hippocampus’s role in consolidation. H.M’s preserved semantic memory for knowledge acquired
before his surgery also indicated that the medial temporal lobes are not the storage area for long-
The transition from fleeting experiences to lasting memories is a complex process called
memory consolidation. H.M’s case brought this subtle mechanism into focus, revealing how
vulnerable new memories are before being stabilized into long-term storage. Research using
animal models like the electroconvulsive shock experiments in rodents showed that disrupting
the medial temporal lobes shortly after learning caused significant memory loss. On the other
hand, allowing more time for consolidation preserved these memories (Duncan, 1949). These
findings support the consolidation period model, which emphasizes the importance of this
Learning curves for the 4 groups of animals shocked through the legs after each trial.
humans. Functional MRI scans show heightened hippocampal activity during episodic memory
tasks, showing its important role in encoding and stabilizing autobiographical information
(Squire, Stark, & Clark, 2004). My extrapolation from these findings is that H.M’s memory
problems were not caused by an inability to store memories permanently but by the removal of
The distinction between declarative and nondeclarative memory stemmed from H.M’s
dissociation of abilities. Declarative memory refers to semantic and episodic memories that can
conditioned behaviors that operate outside conscious awareness (Squire, 1987). H.M’s ability to
acquire procedural skills, like learning to trace shapes in a mirror, despite being unable to recall
the training itself, served as one of the first clear demonstrations of this distinction (Gabrieli,
Similar patterns have been observed in other individuals with medial temporal lobe
damage. For example, patients with amnesia due to encephalitis often show significant
impairments in declarative memory while retaining the ability to learn and perform procedural
tasks (Squire & Zola, 1996). These examples show that the medial temporal lobes are necessary
for forming declarative memories, whereas nondeclarative memory relies in other neural
Although H.M’s case was honestly groundbreaking, subsequent research has revealed
nuances in the medial temporal lobes’ role. For instance, studies on patients with unilateral
hippocampal damage suggest that the degree of impairment depends on the extent of the lesion
and the affected hemisphere. Right hippocampal damage tends to impair spatial memory, while
left hippocampal damage affects verbal memory (Smith & Milner, 1981). Also, debates about the
relationship between episodic and semantic memory have emerged. In 2002, Tulving (2002)
argued that episodic memory builds upon semantic memory, while others contend that episodic
Ethical Implications
H.M’s case definitely raise a couple red flags when it comes to experimental surgeries.
While his family did consent to the procedure in hopes of alleviating his suffering, the
unanticipated consequences show the need for rigorous ethical standards in medical research.
The lack of alternative treatments for epilepsy at the time might have justified the risk, but
modern protocols prioritize the non-invasive and comprehensive patient counselling first.
The case of H.M. presents an intriguing complexity that extends beyond a simple
narrative of medial temporal lobe dysfunction and anterograde amnesia. A careful analysis
reveals sophisticated interactions among memory systems and their corresponding neural
substrates. Although his ability to form declarative memories was severely compromised, H.M.
retained his capacity for procedural learning, indicating that distinct neural pathways can
function with relative autonomy. This observation prompts us to consider fundamental questions
about the nature of these systems' interactions and their implications for understanding memory
Consider the possibility that procedural and declarative memory systems, despite their
anatomical separation, participate in shared encoding processes within larger cortical networks.
Research utilizing fMRI with neurologically intact subjects has demonstrated that procedural
learning tasks, including motor sequence acquisition, temporarily activate medial temporal lobe
structures during initial learning phases (Albouy et al., 2008). This finding suggests that H.M.'s
preserved procedural memory capabilities may not indicate a complete absence of medial
temporal lobe involvement, but rather illustrate a neural adaptation wherein subcortical
structures, particularly the basal ganglia and cerebellum, assume primary responsibility for
learning. His experience demonstrates how the brain's inherent redundancy enables alternative
Another layer of depth lies in the medial temporal lobes’ role in contextualizing
memories. Aside from merely encoding events, it connects various elements of experiences such
as time, place, and emotions to create complete episodic memories (Eichenbaum, 2004). In
H.M., this ability was entirely lost, though he could still recall individual pieces of semantic
knowledge he had learned before his surgery. For example, he could remember facts about the
world but could not place those facts into the context of his personal experiences.
This separation between factual knowledge and the ability to give it context raises
important questions about how memories are stored over time. There is some research that shows
that episodic memory relies on the hippocampus at first to organize and connect contextual
details, but over time, these memories are transferred to cortical regions for long-term storage
(Winocur & Moscovitch, 2011). H.M’s inability to form new episodic memories is probably
because of a failure in this initial linking process, while his preserved semantic memory shows
the stability of the cortical networks that store knowledge already learned.
Memory Consolidation as a Dynamic Process
from a fragile to a stable state. However, more recent studies are showing that memory retrieval
itself can trigger reconsolidation, a process by which previously stable memories become
temporarily malleable and subject to modifications (Dudai, 2004). In H.M., this process was
likely impaired, as the medial temporal lobes are necessary for reconsolidating episodic
This blockage may explain some of H.M.’s behavioral patterns like his repeated inability
to update his understanding of recurring events (e.g., the death of his uncle). Each time he found
out about the death, the memory failed to stabilize due to the loss of reconsolidation
mechanisms. This phenomenon not only sheds light on his personal experience but also
emphasizes the vulnerability of memory systems when key neural structures are compromised.
Even though H.M’s medial temporal lobes were removed, he retained his ability to learn
new procedural tasks and showed improvements with practice, such as the rotary pursuit task
where he had to keep a stylus on a rotating disk through repeated practice. This task
demonstrated his capacity to acquire and refine motor skills over time, despite having no
conscious recollection of the training sessions (Corkin, 1968). I extrapolated that procedural
memory systems can adapt to compensate for declarative memory loss, albeit within certain
limitations.
Interestingly, neuroplasticity may not have been confined to procedural learning alone.
There are other studies on individuals with similar lesions which provided some evidence of
partial declarative memory recovery through recruitment of adjacent cortical areas (Steinvorth,
Levine, & Corkin, 2005). Although H.M.’s damage was quite rough, his preserved intelligence
and problem-solving abilities suggest that his remaining brain structures adapted to support his
overall cognition. I believe that this highlights a potential avenue for therapeutic strategies,
where residual neural networks could be trained to compensate for damaged regions.
The experimental nature of his surgery reflects a time when the long-term consequences
of such procedures were poorly understood. Today, his case serves as a reminder of the ethical
obligations inherent in experimental treatments. The removal of his medial temporal lobes
achieved the primary goal of reducing his seizures but came at the cost of a profound personal
interventions like deep brain stimulation or optogenetics. H.M.’s experience underscores the
importance of rigorous preclinical research, thorough patient counseling, and long-term follow-
Conclusion
H.M.’s story is one of the most fascinating I have encountered in neuroscience. After
H.M’s surgery to treat epilepsy, whatever I thought I knew about memory was completely
redefined. Watching him persevere and get better at new skills while not being able to form new,
lasting memories had to be the most shocking thing yet. Tasks like mirror tracing and rotary
pursuit showed that parts of his brain were still working in ways we did not fully understand.
This raised a question for me. How does the brain decide which abilities to preserve and which to
lose after an injury like his? I hope that in the future there will be more studies and experiments
The more research I did for this paper, I realized just how complex memory systems
really are. The medial temporal lobes do more than store memories. They link details like time,
place, and emotions, creating full experiences we can recall later. H.M.’s case revealed how
much we rely on these processes. Yet, his ability to improve at physical tasks without
remembering to practice them surprised me even more. It showed me how adaptable the brain
can be. Other areas, like the basal ganglia and cerebellum, seemed to take over in ways I had not
considered before. This left me wondering if the brain can be trained to compensate even more
I also found myself thinking about the ethics of his surgery. Sure, it might have reduced
his seizures, but it literally took away his independence and changed his life forever. My goal is
to become a cardiologist, and the ethics of it all scares me more than any other aspect of being a
doctor. I could not help but question the decisions made at that time. How do we ensure that
advancements in science do not come at such a great personal cost? H.M’s case really reinforced
the idea that behind every scientific breakthrough is a person who lives with the consequences of
H.M.’s life taught me so much about the fragility and resilience of the human brain. It
helped me understand how memory works and how much we still have to learn. It also left me
with questions about where we go from here. What other systems in the brain might help us
recover lost functions? How can we ensure that ethical decisions guide future discoveries? For
me, H.M.’s legacy is not just about memory. It is about understanding what makes us human and
Albouy, G., King, B. R., Maquet, P., & Doyon, J. (2008). Consolidation of motor
sequence memories: The role of the striatum and motor cortex. Current Opinion in Behavioral
Dudai, Y. (2004). The neurobiology of consolidations, or, how stable is the engram?
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142050
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.028
Gabrieli, J. D. E., Corkin, S., Mickel, S. F., & Crowden, A. M. (1993). The impaired
learning of semantic knowledge following bilateral medial temporal-lobe resection. Brain and
Lashley, K. S. (1950). In search of the engram. Symposia of the Society for Experimental
Biology, 4, 454-482.
251. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.287.5451.248
Milner, B., Corkin, S., & Teuber, H. L. (1968). Further analysis of the hippocampal
https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(68)90021-3
Preston, A. R., & Eichenbaum, H. (2013). Interplay of hippocampus and prefrontal cortex
Scoville, W. B., & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal
https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.20.1.11
Smith, M. L., & Milner, B. (1981). The role of the right hippocampus in the recall of
3932(81)90090-7
Squire, L. R., Stark, C. E. L., & Clark, R. E. (2004). The medial temporal lobe. Annual
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144130
Squire, L. R., & Zola, S. M. (1996). Structure and function of declarative and
13515-13522. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.93.24.13515
Steinvorth, S., Levine, B., & Corkin, S. (2005). Medial temporal lobe structures are
https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617711000683