0% found this document useful (0 votes)
4 views

Dialnet-LinearityInLanguage-2729078

Uploaded by

dangtriet0305
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
4 views

Dialnet-LinearityInLanguage-2729078

Uploaded by

dangtriet0305
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 17

International Journal

of
English Studies
IJES
UNIVERSITY OF MURCIA www.um.es/ijes

Linearity in Language. Rhetorical-discursive Preferences in


English and Spanish in the Light of Kaplan’s Model

RAFAEL MONROY-CASAS *
Universidad de Murcia

ABSTRACT
In the present work the author tries to analyse one of the fundamental concepts that underlie
Kaplan's theory: his idea of “linearity”. Rather surprisingly, despite its importance, it is a
construct that usually goes undefined in the literature. Different parameters of rhetorical
organisation will be considered in this paper in order to clarify the essence of linearity. We shall
check then Kaplan’s contention that English is a “linear” language whereas Spanish, a member
of the Romance family, is characterised by a broken or non-linear structure. We shall also verify
if there exist differences between English and Spanish in the discursive organisation of an
expository text. Finally, we shall discuss which parameters appear to be more coincidental and
more divergent within the rhetorical organisation of each language.

KEYWORDS: linear language, contrastive rhetoric, expository prose, rhetorical devices,


Spanish rhetorical conventions, formal parameters.

________________
*Address for correspondence: Rafael Monroy-Casas. Departamento de Filología Inglesa, Campus de La Merced
30071, Murcia, Spain. Tel: 00 34 968363174; Fax: 00 34 968363185; e-mail: [email protected]

© Servicio de Publicaciones. Universidad de Murcia. All rights reserved. IJES, vol. 8 (2), 2008, pp. 173-189
174 Rafael Monroy

I. INTRODUCTION
Over the past forty years there has been an increasing concern with the written text in all its
manifestations: from being one of the least studied linguistic skills up to the end of the sixties it
has become one of the most prolific areas of current research (Kaplan, 1987; Purves, 1988;
Martin, 1992; Kachru B, 1992; Rubin, 1995; Connor, 1995, 1996; Davison, 1998; Grabe &
Kaplan, 1996; Kaplan, 1987, 2000; Kaplan & Grabe, 2002, etc. The behaviour of Spanish
learners of English as a second or foreign language has been studied by Santana-Seda, 1975;
Montaño-Harmón, 1991; Lux & Grabe, 1991; Ostler, 1992; Reppen & Grabe, 1993; Monroy &
Scheu, 1997; Moreno, 1997; Trujillo, 2002, etc.).This “discursive linguistics” in Enkvist’s
words (1987) embraces text linguistics, stylistics, genre studies, speech analysis but also
contrastive rhetoric (CR for short), a theory first formulated by Kaplan in 1966. The study of
paragraph organisation in different languages (five basic types were established) was approached
by Kaplan as the starting point to assess writing as product, one of the four fundamental skills
required to master a foreign language within the behaviourist paradigm. The theory was also a
reflection on certain schemes of classical rhetoric with a view to developing those skills needed
to write properly in a foreign language context, mainly English1. This implied in fact giving up
the rhetorical conventions of the first language that might cause interference with the ones
favoured by the target language. The focus nowadays has broadened up considerably
encompassing “differences and similarities in writing across languages” (Connor, 2001: 28)
including academic and professional writing (Swales, 1990; Mauranen, 1993; Tirkonnen-Condit,
1996, etc. ).
Kaplan’s initial theory derived from an ontological stand very much like the one that
underlines British contextualism as initiated by Malinowsky and Firth and continued by Halliday
and followers of context linguistics: that logic is not a universal, but the product of a specific
culture; consequently, every single culture has its own rhetorical schemes. In his own words,
“Logic… is evolved out of a culture; it is not a universal. Rhetoric…is not universal either, but
varies from culture to culture and even from time to time within a given culture’ (1966: 2). And a
few years later, “My original conception was that…rhetoric constituted a linguistic area
influenced by the Whorf-Sapir hypothesis…I would still maintain… that rhetoric is a
phenomenon tied to the linguistic system of a particular language” (1972: Preface).This idea,
very much like the Vosslerian concept of ‘idiomatology’, would explain why the student of a

© Servicio de Publicaciones. Universidad de Murcia. All rights reserved. IJES, vol. 8 (2), 2008, pp. 173-189
Linearity in Language. Rhetorical-discursive Preferences in English and Spanish 175

foreign language violates the expectations of the native reader. Both content and form would be
surface manifestations observable, according to Scribner and Cole (1981), at three levels: the
functional discursive (a given culture can favour say a much more expressive way of writing
than another), the level of cognitive exigency (the way of structuring and organising
information), and the pragmatic level (a given community’s writing expectations). These three
levels are mutually interrelated and highly conventional in each culture. Kaplan’s model is
based therefore on the empirical fact that linguistic systems differ not only at the phonological
or lexico-structural level, but also in their rhetorical preferences. This determinist view, contrary
to the notion of a universal grammar, does not establish significant differences at the cognitive
level; it simply emphasises the idea that each language organises reality in a specific way. From
a contrastive perspective, as envisaged by Kaplan’s theory, it is obvious that the rhetorical option
of each linguistic system implies an ontological limitation that is necessary to overcome within a
second language learning context. In the case of English as a foreign language, the Spanish
learner would have to leave aside the “broken” structure of his/her language, typical of the
Romance languages, and move towards the “linear” structure of a language like English.

II. LINEARITY
Kaplan went farther however. He not only dared to present a typology of rhetorical preferences, but
relying heavily on style manuals defined English as a ‘predominantly linear’ language unlike the
‘broken’ or indirect structure that, in his opinion, characterises Romance, Slavonic and Semitic
languages (1966: 15). This self-indulgent view of the discursive reality has rightly been criticised as
being ethnocentric, ill-defined and vague, lacking empirical support and portraying a stereotyped
reality (Enkvist, 1997 –Connor (1996: 16) summarises further criticisms). Kaplan acknowledges
this in his contribution to Sarangi and Coulthard (2000), and although he has modified his initial
position in the sense that he no longer holds the view that rhetorical patterns reflect a particular way
of thinking, but they are rather the result of different writing conventions that are learned, he adds
that this does not alter the essential empirical fact that “there are differences between languages in
rhetorical preference” (2000: 84). From this fundamental premise, some corollaries follow such as
a) languages present 'gaps' not just at the lexical or structural level, but also at the rhetorical level;
b) every speaker perceives these differences in comparing his/her language with other linguistic
systems; c) there is a tendency to transfer unconsciously to the second language the resources and

© Servicio de Publicaciones. Universidad de Murcia. All rights reserved. IJES, vol. 8 (2), 2008, pp. 173-189
176 Rafael Monroy

rhetorical devices of the first language, and d) there are languages (there is no mention of English
any more) whose rhetorical discourse is more linear than that of other languages. Kaplan simply
acknowledges that “every speaker perceives his/her language as linear and all others as non-linear”
(2000: 84).
This change in the perception of the dominant rhetorical trend (linearity /non-linearity) derives
from the different perception that various authors, whose mother tongue is not English, have of their
own language. Kaplan (2000) observed that whenever he presented to speakers of other languages
his model in which English stood out as more linear than the rest, they considered theirs to be more
linear than English. The concept of linearity has, on the other hand, a clear cognitive significance:
despite Kaplan’s unambiguous statement (2000: 85) that Aristotle linear rhetoric is in no way
cognitively superior to non-linear rhetoric, it is obvious that linearity is psychologically interpreted
in a more positive key than non-linearity, as clearly reflected in the desire expressed by all to have
a linear language. On the other hand, it is unquestionable that English occupies a hegemonic
position in certain academic as well as non-academic circles. This leads to the construction of a
rhetorical model of such a kind that whoever fails to imitate it is deemed to be at a disadvantage
(Connor & Kaplan, 1987 – see, however, Kachru Y, 1997) either lacking in discourse
sophistication or, even worse, in rhetorical coherence (Mauranen, 1993: 1-2).
A first step prior to any contrastive endeavour is therefore to try to define the concept of
‘linearity’. Only in this way will we be in a position to establish the linear /non-linear character of a
text in a given language and draw conclusions across languages. We are fully aware that there is
not such a thing as a homogeneous norm in academic writing: not all English writers use a linear
style consistently. As early as 1974, Braddock pointed out that linearity was a simplified picture
of English writing conventions as many professional native-speaker writers did not always write
following the linearity principle. More recently Connor (2001: 39) expressed identical view with
regards to article introductions. And Kachru, Y. (1997) on her part considers “problematic” to set
up specific writing norms for English. More specifically, there are authors who voice their doubts
that expository prose in English is a well-defined text type (Grabe, 1987; Biber, 1987, 1988).
Surprisingly, linearity is usually taken to be a self-evident, straightforward label that refers
to formal discoursal progression free from digressions without any further qualification. Such a
generic definition needs, however, to be operationally defined for the construct to have a certain
validity. This is what we have done by considering a number of formal parameters of rhetorical

© Servicio de Publicaciones. Universidad de Murcia. All rights reserved. IJES, vol. 8 (2), 2008, pp. 173-189
Linearity in Language. Rhetorical-discursive Preferences in English and Spanish 177

organisation that are based on general Western rhetorical conventions2. In Monroy & Scheu (1997)
the following guidelines are established for a straight linear rhetorical pattern:
1. Thematic unit (TU). We consider a text to display this category (also referred to as ‘discourse
topic’, Lautamatti, 1987) whenever there is a single thesis binding together the whole text; the
presence of more than one thesis would be interpreted as an absence of the above-mentioned
feature.
2. Thematic progression (TP). We refer here to the mechanism by means of which the writer
establishes a direct relationship between all the different thematic sentences that link every
paragraph with the central thesis. A weak or null relationship between the thematic sentences and
the central thesis is understood to be deficient in this feature.
3. Paragraph unity (PU). This is achieved whenever a paragraph displays a monothematic structure
(Smith & Leidlich (1980). It coincides with Morenberg and Sommers ‘direct paragraph’ (1999),
where sentences develop the controlling idea by expanding, qualifying and illustrating it. The
polythematic trend is considered , on the other hand, a feature of a non-linear language.

4. Personal tone (PT). This feature is revealed by the tendency to make use of pronominals that refer
to the subject (consistent point of view –see Hinds’ (1987) ‘writer responsible vs. ‘reader
responsible’ languages). The use of different points of view or of no human agents in thematic
position would reflect the opposite, non-linear trend.
5. Inter-paragraph cohesion (CO). Cohesion between paragraphs is achieved by the presence in
the text of elements linking paragraphs with one another in a co-referential, co-classification or
co-extensive way (Halliday, & Hasan, 1976). Non-linearity is manifested in the tendency to
avoid paragraph linkage.

6. Concreteness (CON). This feature refers to the tendency to use concrete words as they
supposedly contribute to the global effect of linearity in the text. Greater reliance on abstract words
would reflect the opposite, non-linear tendency. It must be pointed out that this parameter is
somehow language-bound in the sense that, typologically speaking, there are languages who favour
concreteness –English being a case in point (Vinay and Darbelnet, 1995) – as against other more
abstract languages such as French or Spanish.

© Servicio de Publicaciones. Universidad de Murcia. All rights reserved. IJES, vol. 8 (2), 2008, pp. 173-189
178 Rafael Monroy

7. Sentence simplicity (SS). This basically involves the presence in the text of simple or
coordinated sentences; the overuse of complex or subordinated sentences would reveal a non-
linear characteristic3.

III. AIMS
Taking as our starting point Kaplan’s premise that there exists a logical principle underpinning the
discursive organisation of every language and that such a principle is rooted in Aristotle’s logic and
Galileo's systematisation, which the idea of linearity underlies (1980: 402), we try to prove the
following null hypotheses: 1. There are no significant differences between English and Spanish
university students in the discursive organisation of an expository text. 2. The rhetorical behaviour
of the Spanish informants does not substantiate the idea of a non-linear logic (i.e. broken structure).
3. Finally, there is no parametric correspondence between English and Spanish with regard to the
profile of rhetorical organisation for each of the two languages involved.

IV. METHODOLOGY
IV.1. Participants
Thirty four subjects were used as informants, seventeen fourth-year Spanish students of English and
seventeen English students who were spending the year at Murcia University. The Spanish group
was randomly selected from the sixty eight who regularly attend classes to match the seventeen
Erasmus students who comprised the British group. None of them knew the purpose of the writing
assignments nor had they taken part in any prior activity directly related to the aim of the
experiment. Finally, all papers had a maximum length of eight hundred words.

IV.2. Instruments
Thirty four papers were written, seventeen for each group, bearing in mind the following principles
put forward by the International Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement: Study
of Written Composition (IEA): we chose, firstly, the expository mode as Kaplan did initially (1966:
4); it seems the mode most commonly used to study intercultural differences. Secondly, an almost
identical subject matter was used in both cases in order to ensure the register variable and the same
type of text. True that within the category of expository writing several sub-genres can be
identified (Grabe, 1987). In our case, we have used a humanities text type assuming that its
frequency of occurrence can be similar in both languages. This is particularly the case with the

© Servicio de Publicaciones. Universidad de Murcia. All rights reserved. IJES, vol. 8 (2), 2008, pp. 173-189
Linearity in Language. Rhetorical-discursive Preferences in English and Spanish 179

Spanish group. Despite sharing identical roots, the Hispanic tradition in written composition
favours a more generic and literary-biased approach to writing conventions than the Anglo-
American tradition, more concerned with the orderly arrangement of the parts of a written text.
Literary authors are models to imitate, but more in vocabulary and critical reading skills than in
the rhetorical organisation of the information. Although writing is explicitly taught at Spanish
schools, it is only recently that linguistic analyses of non-literary written texts has been
systematically undertaken (Onieva Morales, 1995). This penchant for literature is in no way
exclusive to Spaniards. Kaplan himself complaints that “Writing through composing….is the
rarest of the writing types practised by literate individuals. It includes the creation of novels and
short stories, of poems and plays, of theoretical and philosophical treatises by scholars, and –
curiously– of the kinds of essays school children are most commonly asked to write”. (1988:
283).

IV.3. Procedure
The Spanish group was asked to write on “positive aspects of the English character” while the
English group wrote about “positive aspects of the Spanish character”. Following Krashen’s
suggestion made in his Monitor Model (1982) as to the possible relationship between time variable
and product quality, it was decided not to confine the task to strict chronological limits so that each
student was free to write at will although he/she was to hand in his/her assignment a week later. The
task, on the other hand, was carried out at the beginning of the academic year on the basis that this
was deemed the best period for gathering unbiased information from the students as to the purpose
of the exercise. Although both groups obviously had some writing practice experience, they were
not fully conscious of the rhetorical traditions existing in their respective cultures.
The pooling of the samples was carried out by three members of the Department of
English Philology at Murcia University who gave a scoring to the writing assignments. Linearity
was measured using the seven parameters of rhetorical organisation mentioned above. These
categories were applied on a binary basis despite the fact that most of them, particularly the last
one, are not easily amenable to a yes / no answer. We took the presence of each of them as a sign
of ‘linearity’, and their absence as a characteristic of non-linear or broken structure.

© Servicio de Publicaciones. Universidad de Murcia. All rights reserved. IJES, vol. 8 (2), 2008, pp. 173-189
180 Rafael Monroy

V. DATA ANALYSIS AND RESULTS


In order to see the amount or presence or absence of the above-mentioned parameters for each of
the informants, we codified each subject’s answers assigning 1 to an affirmative response and 2 to a
negative answer in all the variables. Once all the texts were collated, t Student was applied to see if
there were significant differences between these parameters in English as compared with Spanish.

PARAMETERS LANG. N MEAN SD t

1. Thematic unit (TU) Engl 17 1.23 0.43


Span. 17 1.17 0.39 0.68

2. Thematic progression (TP) Engl. 17 1.23 0.43


Span. 17 1.41 0.50 0.28
3. Paragraph unity (PU) Engl. 17 1.17 0.39
Span. 17 1.17 0.39 1.00
4. Personal tone (PT) Engl. 17 1.29 0.47
Span. 17 1.58 0.50 0.09

5. Inter-paragraph cohesion Engl. 17 1.35 0.49


(CO)
Span. 17 1.58 0.50 0.18
6. Concreteness (CON) Engl. 17 1.52 0.51
Span. 17 1.58 0.50 0.73
7. Sentence simplicity (SS) Engl. 17 1.82 0.39
Span. 17 1.94 0.24 0.30
p< 0.05

Table 1. Parameters of rhetorical organization. Means and SDs

As Table I shows, there are no significant differences in any of the indexes analysed.
However it is worth commenting on the tendencies which emerge in each of the seven parameters.
In the case of Thematic Unit (TU), the means both of the English as well as the Spanish group show
a positive tendency towards the presence of this feature. The two mean values (1.23 and 1.17) are
closer to 1 (linearity) than to 2 (non-linearity), the Spanish value being slightly higher than the

© Servicio de Publicaciones. Universidad de Murcia. All rights reserved. IJES, vol. 8 (2), 2008, pp. 173-189
Linearity in Language. Rhetorical-discursive Preferences in English and Spanish 181

English one. This tendency is further confirmed in Table II were 82% of the Spaniards and 76% of
the British favour a linear tendency. t value, however, is non-significant (0.68) between groups at
p<.05.
Unlike the Spanish group who scored higher (Table I), the English students shed a score in
Thematic Progression (TP) identical with the one they achieved in the Thematic Unit parameter.
The percentage of essays showing Thematic Progression (Table II) is high in both groups,
Spaniards scoring slightly lower than the British (59% vs. 76% respectively). Despite the averages
being overall more non-linear, they clearly show a non-significant preference for linearity (t value
of 0.28).
Paragraph Unity (PU) characterises by a marked tendency towards linearity in both groups
(Table I). Interestingly, the percentage of students favouring linearity is identical (82%) in the two
(Table II). Although beyond a significance t value level, the data reveal a careful paragraph structure
in both groups.

PARAMETERS LANG. 1 (LINEARITY) 2 (NON-LINEAR)


%/N %/N
1. Thematic unit (TU) Engl. 76 (13) 24 (4)
Span. 82 (14) 18 (3)
2. Thematic progression (TP) Engl. 76 (13) 24 (4)
Span. 59 (10) 41 (7)
3. Paragraph unity(PU) Engl. 82 (14) 18 (3)
Span. 82 (14) 18 (3)
4. Personal tone (PT) Engl. 71 (12) 29 (5)
Span. 41 (7) 59 (10)
5. Inter-paragraph cohesion Engl. 65 (11) 35 (6)
(CO)
Span. 41 (7) 59 (10)
6. Concreteness (CON) Engl. 47 (8) 53 (9)
Span. 41 (7) 59 (10)
7. Sentence simplicidad (SS) Engl. 18 (3) 82 14)
Span. 6 (1) 94 (16)

Table 2. Percentages and frequencies

© Servicio de Publicaciones. Universidad de Murcia. All rights reserved. IJES, vol. 8 (2), 2008, pp. 173-189
182 Rafael Monroy

As far as Personal Tone (PT) is concerned, there are clear differences between the two groups
of informants. The mean value of the British seems to favour linearity (1.29) whereas the Spanish
group mean leans towards non linearity (1.58). This is further reflected in Table II where a total of
71% of the British students adhered to linearity as opposed to 41% of the Spaniards. The difference
is non-significant at p<.05, but a t value of 0.09 reveals that some significance is present. This
supports Reid’s study (1992) in which she found that native English speakers used more pronouns
than Spanish speakers, and seems to contradict Monroy & Scheu’s (1997) where the Spanish group
scored higher in personal tone than the British group. This apparent discrepancy is due to a
difference in the methodology used: in this experiment two different groups are involved, whereas
in the 1997 study the informants were all Spaniards writing first in Spanish and then in English.
In Inter-paragraph Cohesion (CO) the British informants show a trend similar to the one
observed in PT. Again, without being significant (t 0.18), they lean more towards linearity than the
Spanish group (mean values 1.35 vs. 1.58 respectively). The percentage shed by the two groups
(65% vs. 41 %) -Table II) further confirms this tendency.
In Concreteness (CON), on the other hand, the British group shows no preference for either
linearity or non-linearity, the mean value being almost equidistant between 1 and 2 (1.52). The
Spaniards scored a mean (1.58) identical to the one found in the two previous parameters (CO and
PT), reflecting therefore a non-linear tendency. Data from Table II provide some evidence to the
effect that, even by a small percentage, the English are more inclined towards linearity, which is not
surprising if we accept Vinay & Dalbernet’s claim (1995) that English, unlike French or Spanish, is
a concrete, reality language. t-values, however, proved to be highly non-significant (0.73).
Sentence Simplicity (SS), is the parameter in which both groups deviate most sharply from
the linearity features ( 1.82 and 1.94 mean scores). In the Spanish sample, there was only one
instance in which simple and coordinated sentences surpassed subordinated structures. In spite of
being non-significant (t 0.30), sentence simplicity was a feature neither of the British nor of the
Spanish group: only 18% and 6% achieved it respectively. These results do not corroborate findings
by Reid (1988), Montaño-Harmón (1991) or Reppen and Grabe (1993) who found that Spanish
students tend to use an elaborate, ornate style with few simple sentences. The academic background
of the informants (university level in our case vs. elementary (Reppen and Grabe) or secondary
level (Montaño-Harmón) could provide an explanation for such a behaviour. Also, the type of
writing task (expository vs non-expository; and within the former a further subdivision between

© Servicio de Publicaciones. Universidad de Murcia. All rights reserved. IJES, vol. 8 (2), 2008, pp. 173-189
Linearity in Language. Rhetorical-discursive Preferences in English and Spanish 183

C/C (Comparison /contrast) and G topic (description of a graph or chart) (Reid, 1990) can have a
bearing, among further reasons, on the outcome.

6. DISCUSSION
Several remarks are fitting in connection with our aims. As stated at the beginning, we wanted to
know, firstly, whether Kaplan’s claim concerning the different rhetorical organisations of a
discursive text was confirmed for English and Spanish. Our sample, although not very large,
provides evidence to the effect that there is no such difference. None of the values turned out to be
significant to a level p < .05, consequently one cannot talk of a relation between 1, which
corresponds to the positive pole (i.e. linearity) of each parameter, and 2 , reflecting the absence (or
non-linearity) of the parameters in question. Only in the case of personal tone (PT) was a level of
significance of p<.09 . This, on the other hand, is understandable given the higher deictic usage of
English as compared to Spanish.
Our second aim, which consisted of checking whether the rhetorical behaviour of the English
informants justified the idea of linearity as against the non-linear or broken structure of the Spanish
informants as postulated by Kaplan, was not borne out by our data. In the first parameter (TU), both
groups show a clear preference for feature 1 linked to linearity. In fact, the percentage of the
Spanish group is narrowly higher than that of the British group (82 % vs.76 % respectively). As to
thematic progression (TP), the English group does better than the Spanish. Nonetheless, the
Spaniards incline lightly more towards feature 1 than towards feature 2. Mention has been made
above of the fact that both groups yield an identical percentage in the third parameter (PU) linked to
linearity. Admittedly, there are no significant differences between the English and the Spaniards
regarding these three parameters. All one can state is that both groups show a slight trend towards
linearity, but by no means is the British group more conspicuous for linearity than the Spanish one.
In the three following parameters (PT, CO and CON) , the English group yields higher
percentages in feature 1 than in 2. The Spaniards, on the other hand, obtained an identical result in
the three (41 %). Thus, by a narrow margin, the Spanish group favours non-linearity in indexes four
(PT), five (CO) and six (CON), though, again, the trend is statistically non-significant. Only
personal tone proved significant at a p< .09. And the two groups favoured non-linearity in SS.
Our last aim consisted of seeing which parameters were more coincidental and more divergent
in the rhetorical organization of the two languages under analysis. As Table II shows, it is

© Servicio de Publicaciones. Universidad de Murcia. All rights reserved. IJES, vol. 8 (2), 2008, pp. 173-189
184 Rafael Monroy

Paragraph Unity (PU) followed by Thematic Unity (TU) and Thematic Progression (TP) where
there is greatest coincidence between the two groups, As most divergent parameter we find Personal
Tone (PT), Inter-paragraph Cohesion (CO) and, not far behind, Sentence Simplicity (SS) and
Concreteness (CO). A ranking of the parameters in terms of decreasing linearity shows that the only
clear correspondences in both groups take place in the PU and the SS indexes. The former stands
out as the most linear whereas the latter is the least linear feature displayed by all the informants.
However, although the preference for linearity is identical in PU in both groups, this does not apply
in the case of SS. Correspondences between the remaining parameters are not clear. True that TP
and CON can arguably rank equally second and fifth in the two groups, but the percentage shed in
either group reveals a different preference towards linearity. This is particularly the case with PT
which ranks third in both groups and yet the British, unlike the Spaniards, lean more towards
linearity. All the remaining parameters (TU, CO and TP) rank either differently or show opposite
trends in linearity.

7. CONCLUSION
In general terms then, one cannot talk of linearity in English as compared to Spanish. In various
parameters the tendencies of each group converge non-significantly towards either linearity
(indexes 1, 2, 3) or non-linearity (index 7 and possibly 6). In the remaining parameters, the English
group showed a non-significant tendency towards linearity for a t Student analysis with a p<.05. It is
worth noticing that with the exception of PT , the rest of the parameters display an important degree
of non-significance. Only in index 4 did the British group show significance at 9%, while the
Spanish group remained equidistant between both poles, favouring neither linearity nor non-
linearity. In general, one can say that linearity was adhered to by neither of the groups. It remains to
be seen to what extent a larger sample and a more refined linearity scale would confirm or disprove
these results.

© Servicio de Publicaciones. Universidad de Murcia. All rights reserved. IJES, vol. 8 (2), 2008, pp. 173-189
Linearity in Language. Rhetorical-discursive Preferences in English and Spanish 185

NOTES

1
The following titles are but a few of the many books devoted to academic writing in English: The Literary Thesis:
A Guide to Research by G. Watson (1977); How to write reports by J. Mitchell (Fontana, 1974); How to write
essays by R. Lewis (Heinemann, 1976); Scientists must write by R. Barras (Chapman & Hall, 1978); Writing the
research paper. A handbook, by A.C. Winkler & J. R. McCuen (Harcourt Brace J., 1979); Students must write by R.
Barras (Methuen, 1982); Approaches to Academic Reading and Writing by M.A. Arnaudet & M. E. Barret (Prentice-
Hall, 1984); Writing a Thesis. A Guide to Long Essays and Dissertations by G. Watson (Longman, 1987); The
student’s writing guide for the arts and social sciences by Gordon Taylor (C.U.P., 1989); Teaching creative writing
ed. by M. Monteith & R. Miles (Open University, 1992); Academic Writing for Graduate Students. A Course for
Nonnative Speakers of English by J. Swales & Ch. B. Feak (The University of Michigan Press, 1994), etc.

2
Books presenting analytical techniques do in fact include some of these parameters. See, for instance, Connor, U.
and M.A. Johns. (1990, Eds.) Coherence in writing: Research and pedagogical perspectives. Alexandria, VA:
TESOL. Also Purves, A. C. (1988). Writing across languages and cultures: Issues in contrastive rhetoric. Newbury
Park, CA: Sage.

3
This division is no doubt very rough. It finds its justification only on the binary principle we have established to
elucidate the concept of lineariry. A sounder analysis would have to take into account the two basic discoursal
controlling mechanisms: topic and focus. Of special relevance is Kaplan’s idea that focus is ‘specifically language-
bound’ and that ‘some languages have formulaic devices for topic establishment and syntactic manipulations for
focus establishment’ (1983: 150) .

REFERENCES
Bjarkman, P. C., & Hammond, R. H. (Eds.1990). American Spanish Pronunciation: Theoretical
and Applied Perspectives. Washington, D.C.: Georgetown University Press.

Biber, D. (1987). A typology of English texts. Linguistics, 27, 3-43.

Biber, D. (1988). Variations across Speech and Writing. Cambridge: C.U.P.

Braddock, R.; Lloyd-Jones, R. & Schoer, L. (1963). Research in written compostion. Campaign,
Ill.: National Council of Teachers of English.

Braddock, R. (1974). The frequency of placement of topic sentences in expository prose. Research
in the teaching of English, 8, 287-302.

Clayann G. P. (2001).Contrastive Rhetoric Revisited and Redefined. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence


Erlbaum Associates.

Connor, U. (1995). Examining syntactic variation across three English-speaking nationalities


through a multifeatured/ multidimensional approach. In Rubin, D.L. (Ed.). Composing
Social Identity in Written Language Hilsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp.75-87.

© Servicio de Publicaciones. Universidad de Murcia. All rights reserved. IJES, vol. 8 (2), 2008, pp. 173-189
186 Rafael Monroy

Connor, U. (1996). Contrastive Rhetoric: Cross-cultural Aspects of Second-language Writing. New


York: C.U.P.

Connor, U. (2001). Changing currents in contrastive rhetoric: New paradigms. In A. Moreno & V.
Colwell (Eds.), Perspectivas recientes sobre el discurso/ Recent Perspectives on Discourse.
León: Secretariado de Publicaciones, Universidad de León, pp. 27-56.

Connor, U. & Kaplan, R. B. (Eds., 1987). Writing across Languages: analysis of L2 texts. Reading,
MA: Addison-Wesley.

Corbett, J. (2001). Contrastive Rhetoric and Resistance to Writing. In Ed. C. G. Panetta (Ed.),
Contrastive Rhetoric Revisited and Redefined. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates, pp. 105-119.

Enkvist, N. E. (1987). Text linguistics for the applier: An orientation. In U. Connor & R. B. Kaplan
(Eds.), Writing across languages: Analysis of L2 text. Reading MA: Addison-Wesley,
pp.23-43.

Enkvist, N.E. ( 1997). Why we need contrastive rhetoric. Alternation, 4:1, 188-206.

Grabe, W. (1987). Contrastive rhetoric and text type research. In U. Connor & R. B. Kaplan
(Eds.). Writing across Languages: analysis of L2 texts. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley,
pp.115-137.

Grabe, W. & Kaplan, R.B. (1996). Theory and Practice of Writing: An Applied Linguistic
Perspective. London: Longman.

Halliday, M.A.K. & Hasan, R. (1976). Cohesion in English. London: Longman.

Hinds, J. (1987). Reader vs. writer responsibility: A new typology. In U. Connor & R. B. Kaplan
(Eds.), Writing across Languages: analysis of L2 text. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley,
pp.141-152.

Hoey, M. (1991). Patterns of lexis in text. Oxford and New York: O.U.P.

Jones, S. & Tetroe, J. (1987). Composing in a second language. In A. Matsuhashi (Ed.), Writing in
real time: Modelling production processes. Norwood, N.J.: Ablex, pp. 127-142.

Kachru, Braj B. (Ed., 1992). The Other Tongue: English across Cultures. Urbana, Il: University of
Illinois Press.

Kachru, Y. (1997). Cultural meaning and contrastive rhetoric in English education. World
Englishes, 16:3, 337-350.

© Servicio de Publicaciones. Universidad de Murcia. All rights reserved. IJES, vol. 8 (2), 2008, pp. 173-189
Linearity in Language. Rhetorical-discursive Preferences in English and Spanish 187

Kaplan, R.B. (1966). Cultural thought patterns in intercultural education. Language Learning. 16, 1-
20.
Kaplan, R.B. (1972). The Anatomy of Rhetoric: Prolegomena to a Functional Theory of Rhetoric.
Philadelphia: The Center for Curriculum Development.

Kaplan R. B. (1984). Annual Review of Applied Linguistics III. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

Kaplan, R.B. (1983). Contrastive rhetorics: some implications for the writing process. In
Freedman, A., Pringle, I. & Yalden, J. (Eds.), Learning to Write: First Language/ Second
Language. London: Longman, pp. 139-161.

Kaplan, R.B. (1987). Cultural thought patterns revisited. In U. Connor & R. B. Kaplan (Eds.),
Writing across languages: Analysis of L2 text. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley, pp. 9-23.

Kaplan R.B. (1988). Contrastive Rhetoric and Second Language Learning: Notes toward a
theory of contrastive Rhetoric. In A. C. Purves (Ed.), Writing across Languages and
Cultures: Issues in Contrastive Rhetoric. London: Sage Publications, pp.275-304.

Kaplan, R.B. (2000). Contrastive rhetoric and discourse analysis: Who writes what to whom?
When? In what circumstances? In S. Sarangi & M. Coulthard (Eds.) Discourse and Social
Life. Edinburgh: Pearson Education (Longman), pp. 82-101.

Kaplan, R.B. & Grabe, W. ( 2002). A modern history of written discourse analysis. Journal of
Second Language Writing, 11:3, 191-223.

Kellerman, E. (1995). Crosslinguistic influence:Transfer to nowhere. In W. Grabe et al. (Eds.),


Annual review of applied linguistics, 15. New York: C.U.P., 125-150.

Krashen, S. D. (1982). Principles and practice in second language acquisition. Oxford: Pergamon
Press.

Kroll, B. (Ed.) Second Language Writing . Research insights for the classroom. Cambridge:
CUP.

Lautamatti, L. (1987). Observations on the Development of the Topic of Simplified Discourse.


In U. Connor & R. B. Kaplan (Eds.), Writing across Languages: analysis of L2 texts.
Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley, pp. 87-114.

Leki, I. (1991). Twenty-five years of contrastive rhetoric. The state of the Art. TESOL Quarterly,
25:1, 123-143.

Lux, P. & Grabe, W. ( 1991) Multivariate approaches to contrastive rhetoric. Lenguas Moderna,
18, 133-160.

© Servicio de Publicaciones. Universidad de Murcia. All rights reserved. IJES, vol. 8 (2), 2008, pp. 173-189
188 Rafael Monroy

MacDonald, S. P. (1991). The Literary Argument and Its Discoursive Conventions. In, W. Nash,
(Ed.), The Writing Scholar. Studies in Academic Discourse. London: Sage Publications,
pp. 31-62.

Martin, J. E. (1992). Towards a Theory of Texts for Contrastive Rhetoric: An Introduction to


Issues of Texts for Students and Practitioners of Contrastive Rhetoric. New York, NY:
Peter Lang.

Matsuda, P. (1997). Contrastive rhetoric in context: A dynamic of L2 writing. Journal of Second


Language Writing, 6:1, 45-60.

Mauranen, A. (1993). Cultural Differences in Academic Rhetoric. Frankfurt and Main: Peter
Lang.

Monroy, R. & Scheu, D. (1997). Reflejo cultural en los estilos de hispanohablantes estudiantes de
inglés como LE. Consideraciones pedagógicas. In Estudios de Lingüística Aplicada y
Literatura. Homenaje Póstumo al Prof. Juan Conesa. Departamento de Filología Inglesa.
Universidad de Murcia, pp. 201-221.

Morenberg, M. & Sommers, J. (1999). The writer’s opinion. New York: Longman.

Moreno, A.I. (1997). Genre constraints across languages. Casual metatext in Spanish and English
RAs. English for Specific Purposes, 16:3, 161-179.

Montaño-Harmon, M. (1991). Discourse features of written Mexican Spanish: Current research in


contrastive rhetoric and its implications. Hispania, 74, 417-425.

Odlin, T. (2002). Language transfer and cross-linguistic studies: Relativism, universalism and th
native language. In R.B. Kaplan. (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Applied Linguistics. New
York: O.U.P., pp. 253-261.

Ostler, S.E. (1992). Cultural Sensitiveness: Teaching Spanish Speakers to Write in English.
Bowling Green, OH: Bowling Green University.

Purves, A. (Ed.,1988). Writing Across Languages and Cultures. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Reppen , R. & Grabe, W. ( 1993) Spanish transfer effects in the English writing of elementary
school students. Lenguas Modernas, 20, 113-128.

Reid, J. ( 1988). Quantitative differences in English prose written by Arabir, Chinese, Spanish, and
English students. Ph D. Dissertation. CO: Colorado State University.

Reid, J. (1990). Responding to different topic types: a quantitative analysis from a contrastive
rhetoric perspective. In B. Kroll (Ed.), Second Language Writing. Research insights for the
classroom . Cambridge: C.U.P., pp.191-210.

© Servicio de Publicaciones. Universidad de Murcia. All rights reserved. IJES, vol. 8 (2), 2008, pp. 173-189
Linearity in Language. Rhetorical-discursive Preferences in English and Spanish 189

Reid, J. (1992). A Computer Text Analysis of Four Cohesion Devices in English Discourse by
Native and Nonnative Writers. Journal of Second Language Writing, 1:2, 79-108.

Rubin, D.L. (Ed., 1995). Composing Social Identity in Written Language. Hilsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.

Santana-Seda, O. (1975). A contrastive study in rhetoric: An analysis of the organization of English


and Spanish paragraphs written by native speakers of each language. Dissertation Abstracts
International, 35: 3862A.

Sarangi, S. & Coulthard, M. (Eds., 2000). Discourse and Social Life. London: Longman.

Scribner, S. & Cole, M. (1981). The psychology of literacy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard U.P.

Smith, W. F. & Leidlich, D. (1980). Rhetoric for Today. New York: Harcourt, Brace
Jovanovich.

Swales, J. (1990). Genre analysis: English in academic and research settings. Cambridge: CUP.

Tirkkonen-Condit, S. (1996). Explicitness vs. implicitness in argumentation: An intercultural


comparison. Multilingua, 15:3, 274-275.

Trujillo S. F. (2002). Retórica Contrastiva y Expresión Escrita: Evaluación y Estudio de Textos


en Inglés y Español. Granada: Editorial Universidad de Granada.

Valero Garcés, C. & Mª C. Calle Martínez (1997). Constrastive rhetoric in ESP: A cross-linguistic
análisis of finite verb profiles in English and Spanish medical abstracts. Unesco Alsed-LSP
Newsletter, 20, 2:44, 22-35.
Vande K.W. (1990). Themes, thematic progressions, and some implications for understanding
discourse. Written Communication 8, 311-347.

Vinay, J. P. & J. Darbelnet (1995). Comparative Stylistics of French and English. A methodology
for translation. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. English translation of the original French
edition (1958) Stylistique comparée du Français et de l’anglais. Paris: Didier.

© Servicio de Publicaciones. Universidad de Murcia. All rights reserved. IJES, vol. 8 (2), 2008, pp. 173-189

You might also like