Publishedversion
Publishedversion
The Structural Organization of Human Values – Evidence from Three Rounds of the
University of Muenster
Shalom H. Schwartz
Corresponding author:
Prof. Dr. Wolfgang Bilsky, University of Muenster, FB 07/Psych. Inst. IV, Fliednerstrasse 21,
48149 Muenster, Germany, Tel.: 0049 251 83 34198, Fax: 0049 251 83 31343
Email: [email protected]
2
Abstract
Since 1987, a multitude of studies referring to the Schwartz (1992) structural model of human
values have been published. Although most studies support this conceptual approach, few
were based on representative samples. The implementation of the biennial European Social
Survey (ESS) in 2002, made responses from 71 representative national samples from 32
countries to a 21-item version of the Portrait Values Questionnaire available for assessing this
model of human values. We present structural analyses of these data using a theory-based
approach to multidimensional scaling (Bilsky, Gollan & Döring, 2008) that can be applied to
optimally assess the fit of data to diverse theories. The analyses support the circular structure
of basic values across countries and within countries across time. They also replicate two
findings based on other samples, surveys, and methods of analysis (Fontaine et al., 2008):
Deviations from the structure are fewer and the contrast between protection and growth values
Introduction
Universals in the content of human values and their structural organization have been
the focus of research for more than two decades (Schwartz & Bilsky, 1987, 1990; Schwartz,
1992, 2006). In this context, ordinal multidimensional scaling (MDS) has been a central
approach for analyzing value structures. Starting from regional hypotheses, MDS displays the
Studies using the Schwartz Value Survey (SVS) measured values in teacher, university
student, and other adult samples in 67 countries from 1988 to 2007 (Schwartz, 1992, 2007). In
1994, Schwartz developed an alternative instrument, the Portrait Values Questionnaire (PVQ),
that is cognitively less demanding than the SVS and can be applied to younger and less
educated samples (Schwartz, 2006; Schwartz, et al., 2001). Since then the scope of studies has
expanded considerably, including samples of adolescents and children (Bubeck & Bilsky,
2004; Bilsky, Niemann, Schmitz & Rose, 2005; Boehnke & Welzel, 2006; Hofmann–
Towfigh, 2007). Most of these studies corroborated Schwartz (1992) assumptions about ten
basic values (Table 1, columns 1 and 2) and their structural relations (Figure 1) as outlined in
his revised values model (Schwartz, 1992, p. 45). However, few strictly representative
With the inclusion of a short form of the PVQ in the European Social Survey (ESS; see
items in Table 1, column 3), data from many representative national samples are now
available to assess the Schwartz theory of basic values. The ESS is a biennial multi–country
survey in which 32 countries have participated at least once, thus far. We analyze the data on
value preferences in 71 samples from ESS rounds 1-3, completed between 2002 and 2007.
Two sources have reported analyses of the structure of values in the first ESS round.
Schwartz (2007) reported complete support for the theorized structure in 15 countries and
4
substantial support in the remaining 5 countries. In contrast, Mohler, Rammstedt and Wohn
(2006) claimed that the “German ESS data showed, like those of many other cultures, quite
some deviations from the ideal value circle ...” (p. 257). Neither of these sources provided
evaluation. We therefore undertook to analyze the PVQ data available from all three ESS
rounds, documenting our statistical approach to multidimensional scaling in a way that allows
easy reproduction by others for future ESS rounds and other uses of the short form of the
PVQ. Data from the first three ESS rounds allow not only an extensive assessment of the fit
of the theorized circular structure of ten basic values in representative national samples, but
also examination of the stability of the structure within countries over time and comparisons
equivalence of the structure of values, measured with the SVS, in samples of teachers and
students from 38 countries. They found that the average value structure across samples
corresponded very well with the circular structure of values posited by the Schwartz value
theory. Sampling fluctuations accounted for most of the observed deviations from the average
structure. However, there were also systematic patterns of variation across countries. We
Specifically, Fontaine et al. (2008) reported that the higher the level of societal
development of a country, the less the structure of values in the sample deviated from the
average value structure and the stronger the contrast between two sets of values, ‘growth’
versus ‘protection’. These sets differ based on their relations to anxiety (Schwartz, 2005b,
1
Anxiety relations provide a supplementary explanation for the organization of the basic value structure by
contrasting self-transcendence and openness to change values (growth) with self-enhancement and conservation
values (protection).
5
conformity, and tradition) express anxiety-based self-protection. Fontaine et al. (2008) linked
these patterns to the differences in value forming experiences tied to societal development.
The current study examines whether similar systematic patterns of variation are found when
values are measured with a different instrument, in representative national samples, and in a
Method
Data from the first three rounds were downloaded from the data archive of the European
Social Survey2 (ESS13, ESS24, and ESS35). Because missing data and failure to discriminate
among value items might distort results, we first cleaned the data following the procedure
described by Schwartz (2005a)6. We excluded persons with more than 5 missing responses
and those who gave the same answer to more than 16 value items.
responses to the 21 PVQ–items, separately for each sample. We applied listwise deletion for
missing cases. These correlation matrices provided the basis for our MDS analyses. Table 2
lists the countries studied in each ESS round and, in columns 2-6, it presents the sample sizes
2
http://www.europeansocialsurvey.org/; data archive and distributor of the ESS data: Norwegian Social Science
Data Services (NSD), retrieved 31.10.08.
3
ESS1: http://ess.nsd.uib.no/index.jsp?year=2003&country=&module=download, retrieved 11.01.08.; R Jowell
and the Central Co-ordinating Team, European Social Survey 2002/2003: Technical Report, London: Centre for
Comparative Social Surveys, City University (2003).
4
ESS2: http://ess.nsd.uib.no/index.jsp?year=2005&country=&module=download, retrieved 17.09.08; data for
Italy were added from ESS2IT: http://ess.nsd.uib.no/index.jsp?year=2005&country=&module=download,
retrieved: 29.10.08; R Jowell and the Central Co-ordinating Team, European Social Survey 2004/2005:
Technical Report, London: Centre for Comparative Social Surveys, City University (2005).
5
ESS3: http://ess.nsd.uib.no/index.jsp?year=2007&country=&module=download, retrieved 07.07.08; data for
Latvia and Romania were added from ESS3LVRO: http://ess.nsd.uib.no/index.jsp?
year=2007&country=&module=download, retrieved 07.07.08; R Jowell and the Central Co-ordinating Team,
European Social Survey 2006/2007: Technical Report, London: Centre for Comparative Social Surveys, City
University (2007)
6
SPSS-Syntax used for data cleaning, see: http://essedunet.nsd.uib.no/cms/topics/1/4/all.html; retrieved
31.10.08.
6
value structure and to the strength of the contrast between protection and growth values, we
level of education, life expectancy, level of democracy, and democratic participation. A single
factor accounted for 74.52% of the total variance; the remaining factors had eigen values
below .80. Countries with higher scores on this factor were higher on each of the indicators.
Norway scored highest on this social development index (SDI) in our data set and Ukraine
scored lowest. We report relations of this index to the patterns of variation because it captures
Numerous factors may affect the results of multidimensional scaling (Borg & Groenen,
2005; Borg & Staufenbiel, 2007; Shye, 1985). The measures of proximity used, the loss
function, and the respective starting configuration may all make a difference. Many statistical
PROXSCAL, and SYSTAT–MDS (see the detailed overview in Borg & Groenen, 2005), that
have different default options and choices. Hence, failure to report the many statistical
decisions and computational steps taken prevents exact replication and may result in
We chose a theory-based MDS approach for our analyses. Central to this approach is a
starting configuration which assigns every variable (i.e., every value item) its place within the
hypothesized structure of values (this is also called a “weak confirmatory” MDS by Borg &
Staufenbiel, 2007; cf. Bilsky, 2008). Of course, a strict confirmatory MDS with regional
handled by standard software packages (for special applications, see Groenen & van der Lans,
our purposes because Schwartz (1992) offers an explicit, theoretically grounded hypothesis
about the structure of the values. More general methodological considerations in Borg and
Groenen (2005) also support this choice which can be used to optimally assess diverse
theories:
The MDS program optimizes Stress, which is substantively blind: that is, it is not
tailored to the particular questions that are being asked. ... Minimizing Stress gives a
solution that is locally optimal. Yet, other local minimum solutions may exist with a
similar Stress, or possibly even with lower Stress ... The question is which solution
should be preferred. If a hypothesis for the data is available, then, of course, we would
be particularly interested in the solution that most directly speaks to this hypothesis.
This is obviously the solution that most closely satisfies the hypothesis…. (p. 228;
The value items represent the 10 more general basic values in the values theory. The
hypothesized overall structure of relations among the 10 values determines the hypothesized
locations of the 21 items relative to one another. We therefore deduced a design matrix of
values from the Schwartz model (Figure 1) as a first step (Bilsky, Gollan & Döring, 2008).
Design matrix. In this model (Schwartz, 1992, p. 45), the 10 values are represented by
nine sectors (Figure 1). One sector is further divided into an inner and an outer segment, each
of which represents a different value (conformity and tradition). Equal spacing of the nine
sectors at 40° angles is not a defining feature of the Schwartz model. Nonetheless, such a
simple and regular structure is functional. In the absence of evidence for a more specific
The nine sectors serve as the basis for specifying the prototypical location of each value
by corresponding coordinates (Bilsky, Gollan & Döring, 2008). We determine the coordinates
trigonometrically by referring to the unit circle and summarize them in the design matrix.
Nine of the 10 values are represented by points on the periphery of this circle; their
coordinates derive from the centre of that circular arc which is marked by the respective
(value) sector. The coordinates of the tenth value (conformity) are determined in the same
way, though with a radius of 0.5 instead of 1.0. Table 3 shows the design matrix and Figure 2
Starting configuration. As a second step, the starting configuration for all the value
items is defined. In the PVQ21, two items operationalize each of nine values and three items
operationalize the tenth (universalism). The starting configuration, like the design matrix,
should represent the prototypical structure of values. Therefore, all items that index the same
value receive the coordinates specified for that value in the design matrix.
Data analysis
Our structural analyses of the ESS values data were accomplished with PROXSCAL, an
MDS program in SPSS. This program offers many options for multidimensional scaling. The
common space, and initial (starting) configurations. We analyzed the matrices of Pearson
correlation coefficients between the 21 PVQ–items with ordinal MDS (defaults for ties and
Results
Given the many samples in our study, we first introduce the general form of the two–
dimensional graphical MDS–splits of value items by referring to the three German ESS
samples. Table 4 summarizes findings of the analyses in each sample and ESS round.8
8
A comprehensive research report including all MDS plots per country and ESS round is available from the first
author ([email protected]).
9
Figure 3a, 3b, and 3c plot results of the MDS–analyses in the three German samples. To
provide a simple, clear overview, the value items have been replaced by the numbers of the
values they represent as shown in Figure 2. The sequence of the numbers corresponds to the
circular structure of Schwartz original model (1992, p. 14) in which all ten values were
represented by separate sectors. The plots present two alternative splits for tradition (3) and
conformity (4), one referring to the original model (dashed line) and the other to the revised
Figure 3 reveals that the MDS–splits of all German samples correspond perfectly to the
Schwartz (1992) structural theory. This holds for both the original (p. 14) and the revised
model (p. 45) of the value structure. Note that in this context bent lines pose no problems with
respect to interpretation as long as a particular value region does not include value items of a
different basic value (Borg & Shye, 1995; Shye, Elizur & Hoffman, 1994).
about all observed deviations from the hypothesized circular structure. As can be seen, in
42/71 samples, the theorized circular order of values was perfectly reproduced. Moreover,
every one of the deviations, whether reversals of the order around the circle or mixing of two
One may ask whether providing a theory-based starting configuration biases the
resulting MDS structure in favor of the theory at the expense of the adequacy of the fit with
the data. To address this concern we also performed an MDS in each sample using the three
other options for starting configurations in the PROXSCAL program, Simplex (the default),
Torgerson, and Random (we ran 1000 random starts). Columns 7-10 of Table 2 report the
stress-1 values obtained with each type of starting configuration. As can be seen, these stress
values are virtually the same. The mean stress values across all samples are: Simplex=0.121,
10
=0.118 (additional analyses with stress convergence = .000001 and maximum number of
iterations = 1000 did not result in reduced stress values or different MDS structures). Thus, by
using a theory-based starting configuration one can obtain an optimal assessment of the match
between the data and the theory at no loss in accuracy of representing the data. In contrast,
using any other starting configuration with the same data may reach an alternative local
Figure 4 illustrates the dependence of the MDS solution on the starting configuration.
It compares the solutions obtained in the Czech values data of ESS round 2 with the theory-
based starting configuration and the default starting configuration (Simplex). With the theory-
based starting configuration, this sample showed two deviations from the hypothesized
circular structure (UN peripheral to BE, HE and ST mixed). With the Simplex starting
of deviations from the theorized circular structure, we counted the number of deviations per
sample within each country. For example there was one deviation in the three samples in
Austria, yielding a count of .33, and 3 deviations in the one sample in Cyprus, yielding a
count of 3.00. The correlation of the number of deviations with SDI across the 32 countries
was -.649 (p<.001). Thus, the higher the societal development in a country, the less the
observed value structure deviated from the theorized circular structure. SDI accounted for a
9
We assessed deviations from the theory-based structure whereas Fontaine et al. (2008) focused on deviations
from the overall average structure. This reflects our primary interest in the fit of the theory to the data in all
countries in contrast to their interest in the stability of the structure across countries. Our approach also sheds
light on the stability of the hypothesized structure.
11
Contrast between Protection Values and Growth Values. The method we used to assess
the strength of the contrast between protection and growth values followed the approach
Fontaine et al. (2008) used with their raw SVS data.10 They computed two scales, one
including the items that shifted toward the protection pole of the protection-growth value
dimension with increased SDI and one including the items that shifted toward the growth
pole. The 21 PVQ value items parallel many of the items in the SVS. We identified seven
PVQ items (items 2,4,5,7,13,14 and 17) whose contents parallel those of the Fontaine et al.
(2008) SVS protection-shift items and five PVQ items (1,6,8,18,19) whose contents parallel
those of their SVS growth-shift items. We then computed a scale for each based on each
individual’s centered responses. Individuals from all ESS rounds constituted the sample for
each country.
For the 32 countries, the within-country correlation between the two scales ranged from
-.669 to -.418. The more negative this correlation, the stronger the contrast between protection
and growth values in the country. To examine relations of the strength of the protection-
growth contrast with societal development, we correlated the SDI with the correlation
between the two scales (Fisher’s z transformed) across the 32 countries. The resulting
correlation of -.852 (p<.001) indicates that the higher the level of societal development of a
country, the stronger the contrast between protection and growth values.
Discussion
When evaluating the empirical structures, it is important to consider the form of any
deviations. Deviations from the Schwartz model may take several forms. A first type of
deviation is failure to find a distinct region for each value. Table 4 indicates such deviations
by [x+y], with x and y standing for the values that mix together (e.g., Finland, round 2, TR +
CO). If these values are adjacent in the hypothesized circle (see Figure 1), the deviation is of
minor importance. Schwartz and Sagiv (1995) demonstrated that such deviations are likely to
10
Because the strength of this contrast depends on correlations among raw responses, our approach to assessing
it does not use the MDS solutions we obtained.
12
be chance variations. They may well be ignored, especially if the mixed values form distinct
A second form of deviation is a peripheral rather than side-by-side split between values.
The values form distinct regions but one emerges behind the other rather than in wedge–like
regions around the circle. The value theory hypothesizes such a split for tradition (3) and
conformity values (4) but not for other values. Table 4 indicates such deviations by [x/y], with
x standing for the peripheral and y for the central value (e.g., Austria, round 3, CO and SE). If
the two values are adjacent in the hypothesized value structure, this type of deviation too is
A third form of deviation is a reversal of the order of values. Table 4 indicates such
deviations by a reversal in the order of the numbers that stand for the values (e.g., Turkey,
round 2, CO and SE). Reversals of adjacent values are less important than other reversals,
especially if they do not recur across ESS rounds. If reversals recur, it is worth considering
Finally, a fourth form of deviation is the mislocation of single items to the spatial region
of another value. Table 4 indicates such deviations in the ‘deviations’ column by listing the
item and its location (e.g., CO_7 in Hungary, round 3, between SE and PO). Such
mislocations are more serious if they entail location in a region distant from the hypothesized
region of the item and if they recur across ESS rounds. The only two such mislocations in the
71 samples analyzed here were neither distant from their hypothesized region nor did they
recur across ESS rounds. Should more serious deviations appear in a sample, however, it
would suggest either problems of translation or a difference between the meaning of the value
item in the sample and its intended meaning that is confirmed in most other samples.
Five countries exhibited recurrent deviations from the hypothesized value structure. In
Estonia, hedonism and stimulation values were mixed together rather than adjacent in both
13
all three rounds. In Poland as in Hungary, universalism was peripheral to benevolence in all
three rounds. In Portugal, security reversed with tradition and conformity in all three rounds.
In Sweden, security and conformity mixed in two of three rounds. Each of these deviations is
minor, but their recurrence suggests that they merit special attention. They may reflect
possible artifacts (e.g., problematic translation of one or two items) or, more interestingly,
culture-specific organizations of values. The fact that all of these deviations were due to
mixing or shifts in location between values adjacent in the theorized structure suggests that
whatever culture-specific variation there is in the significance of the values is not great.
The fact that these country deviations replicated across samples from the different
rounds points to the stability of the value structure over time when using the same instrument
to measure values. In nine countries that participated in all three ESS rounds, the theorized
structure replicated across all three rounds, further supporting the stability of the structure
across time.
In six of the 10 countries that participated in only one ESS round, minor deviations of
the sort Schwartz and Sagiv (1995) demonstrated as likely to be due to chance were found
(Bulgaria, Cyprus, Italy, Latvia, Romania, and Turkey). It is probably too early to attribute
One specific deviation occurred many more times than any other. This was the location
of universalism peripheral to benevolence (15 times). According to the values theory, this pair
of values shares the motivation to transcend self-interest and to seek to benefit others.
Analyses of values data with earlier questionnaires also revealed that universalism values
The peripheral location was attributed to the greater abstractness of universalism compared
with benevolence. Universalism values concern the welfare of those in the wider society and
world outside the ingroup rather than the welfare of close others. Similar reasoning explained
14
the location of tradition values peripheral to conformity values (Schwartz, 1992, 2006). Both
values express the motivation to submit to external expectations and avoid conflict. Tradition
values refer to submission to more abstract sources of expectation such as religion and
accepted customs, conformity refers to submission to the expectations of those with whom
In sum, the vast majority of our analyses corroborate the Schwartz model (see Table 4,
and the individual plots available from the authors for all samples). This finding suggests that
this theoretical approach is a conceptually and empirically appropriate and sound basis for
cross–cultural research. Regarding the contradiction between the Schwartz (2007) and Mohler
et al. (2006) findings cited at the outset, the current analyses clearly support the former. By
explicitly specifying the detailed procedures in carrying out the structural analyses, such
controversies can be avoided in the future. The current article demonstrates that one can avoid
Our findings about the circular structure of human values are in line with other research
using confirmatory factor analysis (e.g., Schwartz & Boehnke, 2004) and support the general
stability of the value model. Nonetheless, we too observed two systematic patterns of
variation in the findings that parallel variations reported by Fontaine et al. (2008). We
measured values with a different instrument, in representative national samples rather than in
teacher and student samples, in a set of countries that overlapped only partly with the set they
studied, and we used somewhat different measures of deviation, of contrast in values, and of
societal development. Despite these differences, both patterns of variation replicated in this
study. Specifically, the higher the level of societal development, the fewer the deviations from
the theoretical circular structure of ten basic values and the stronger the contrast between
Why might societal development reduce deviations from the theorized structure of
values? Fontaine et al. (2008) offered three explanations that imply that the observed
association is a methodological artifact. First, they noted that high SDI countries were
overrepresented in their dataset. This influenced the overall average structural configuration
to which they compared each country configuration. The greater influence of high SDI
countries on the target of comparison could create a spurious impression of more structural
deviation in low SDI countries. This explanation cannot apply to our findings, however. We
Second, Fontaine et al. (2008) noted that shifts in respondents’ understandings of items
due to translation or context of administration might be greater in the less developed societies
whose languages were more distant from the languages of the majority of samples that
contributed to the overall average configuration. In the current study, the context of
administration was quite similar in all samples; respondents completed the PVQ during an
individual face-to-face interview. Moreover, despite use of 27 languages in the ESS samples,
the rate of deviation from the theory-based configuration (which privileges no particular
language) differed little across the major language groups as well as the non-Indo-European
languages (Turkish and Hebrew). Hence translation and context differences apparently do not
explain the association between SDI and the extent of deviation from the hypothesized value
structure.
Third, Fontaine et al. (2008) noted that people in less developed countries may have less
experience using numerical scales such as those in the SVS. This could lead to less accurate
communication of their value priorities by people from low SDI countries and hence to more
deviant value structures. Although a lack of ‘test wiseness’ might also apply in the current
study, it is less likely to have been a problem for three reasons: The PVQ does not use a
numerical response scale, its items are more concrete than those of the SVS, and a response
16
format that involves comparisons between others and self draws upon people’s everyday
It therefore seems likely that the observed differences in the rate of deviation from the
hypothesized value structure may reflect real differences in how well-articulated people’s
value systems are in societies at different levels of development. Fontaine et al. (2008) cited
the findings of Schwartz (2004) indicating that cultures in more developed countries
encourage individual autonomy more and cultivate people’s unique personal ideas and
preferences. The stronger cultural autonomy and weaker embeddedness in developed societies
mean that prevailing norms and customs impose fewer constraints on individual decision
making and behavior. This confronts people with more opportunities and obligations to make
choices, the main vehicle for developing articulated value systems (Schwartz, 2006). This, in
turn, would lead to fewer deviations from the theory-based value structure in higher SDI
countries.
The explanations Fontaine et al. (2008, p. 362) offer for the greater contrast between
protection and growth values in more developed societies also pertain to our replication of
Increasing SDI in the set of ESS countries largely reflects a shift in their labor markets
from jobs in agriculture, construction, heavy manufacturing and other primary and secondary
occupations to jobs in service and information industries. The latter demand more innovation,
creativity, critical thinking, complex interpersonal skills, and less routine activity. The nature
of jobs, socialization in smaller families, exposure to higher education, and prevailing cultural
autonomy in more developed societies all encourage an interest in what is unique, novel,
challenging, and different. In other words, they encourage people to pursue growth values.
But such pursuit entails forgoing to some extent the security of the status quo, of conformity
to norms and traditions. That is, it entails sacrificing protection values, the primary default
option of people according to Maslow (1959) and Higgins (1997). As a result, people in more
17
developed countries are likely to experience conflict between protection and growth values
more frequently than are people in less developed countries. This gives rise to the stronger
In summary, the vast majority of our analyses corroborate the circular model of the
structure of human values. These analyses also provide detailed guidance on how best to
assess the fit of new data to this model by using MDS with a theory-based starting
configuration. This approach can also be applied to other instruments used to measure basic
values such as the SVS and to instruments that operationalize other theories.
Even when this optimal starting configuration is used with the PVQ, however, there are at
least two systematic variations in the pattern of value relations. On the one hand, the circular
model fits somewhat less well in less developed societies, perhaps, as we have argued,
because the culture and organization of experience in these societies confronts people with
fewer opportunities and demands to make choices independently among alternatives, the
vehicle through which value systems take shape. In addition, the contrast between protection
and growth values is sharper in more developed societies. This may also reflect greater
opportunities and demands to make choices in more developed societies, specifically between
maintaining the status quo and innovating. Future studies may uncover other variations within
culture and social structure and consequently in the everyday experiences and choices through
References
Bilsky, W. (2008). Value structure – On the use of weakly constrained confirmatory MDS.
Paper presented at the 19th International Congress of the International Association for
Cross–Cultural Psychology July 27–31, 2008, Bremen, Germany. (Berichte aus dem
Bilsky, W., Gollan, T., & Döring, A. (2008). Análise confirmátoria de escalonamento
Teixeira (Ed.), Valores humanos e gestão (pp. 213–221). São Paolo: Senac.
Bilsky, W., Niemann, F., Schmitz, J., & Rose, I. (2005). Value structure at an early age:
Conference in Rome, 10–13 July 2005 (pp. 241–248). Prague: Agentura Action M.
(http://miami.uni–muenster.de/servlets/DerivateServlet/Derivate–4360/
bilsky_elizur_2005.pdf)
Boehnke, K., & Welzel, C. (2006). Wertetransmission und Wertwandel: Eine explorative
26, 341–360.
Bubeck, M., & Bilsky, W. (2004). Value structures at an early age. Swiss Journal of Psychology,
63, 31–41.
Borg, I., & Groenen, P. (2005). Modern Multidimensional Scaling. Berlin: Springer.
Borg, I., Groenen, P., Jehn, K.A., & Bilsky, W. (2009). Embedding the organizational culture
profile into Schwartz`s universal value theory using multidimensional scaling with
Borg, I., & Shye, S. (1995). Facet theory: form and content. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Borg, I., & Staufenbiel, T. (2007). Theorien und Methoden der Skalierung. Huber: Bern.
19
Fischer, R., Milfont, T.L., & Gouveia, V.V. (2009). Does social context affect value
Fontaine, J.R.J., Poortinga, Y.H., Delbeke, L., & Schwartz, S.H. (2008). Structural
Groenen, P. & van der Lans, I. (2006). Multidimensional scaling with regional restrictions for
Facet Theory: An application to Levy’s political protest data. In M. Braun & P.Ph.
Higgins, E.T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52, 1280-1300.
Maslow, A. H. (Ed.) (1959). New knowledge in human values. New York: Harper.
Mohler, P., Rammstedt, B., & Wohn, K. (2006). The great value divide – testing Hofstede’s
experimental social psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 1–65). New York: Academic Press.
Schwartz, S. H. (2004). Mapping and interpreting cultural differences around the world. In H.
Schwartz, S. H. (2005a). Human values. European Social Survey Education Net. Retrieved
Schwartz, S. H. (2005b, July). The structure and implications of individuals’ value systems.
Keynote address at the 1st European Association for Survey Research Conference,
Barcelona, Spain.
cross-nationally - lessons from the European Social Survey (pp. 161-193). London:
Sage.
Schwartz, S. H., & Bilsky, W. (1987). Toward a universal psychological structure of human
Schwartz, S. H., & Bilsky, W. (1990). Toward a theory of the universal content and structure
Schwartz, S.H., & Boehnke, K. (2004). Evaluating the structure of human values with
Schwartz, S.H., Melech, G., Lehmann, A., Burgess, S., Harris, M. & Owens, V. (2001).
Extending the cross–cultural validity of the theory of basic human values with a
Schwartz, S.H., & Sagiv, L. (1995). Identifying culture–specifics in the content and structure
Shye, S., Elizur, D., & Hoffman, M. (1994). Facet theory. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
21
Table 1
Basic Values, Core Goals, and PVQ–Items in the European Social Survey (ESS)
Basic Value Core Motivational Goal PVQ–Items as numbered & labeled in the ESS
3. Ipeqopt: He thinks it is important that every person
UN Understanding, appreciation, in the world should be treated equally. He believes everyone
should have equal opportunities in life.
Universalism tolerance and protection for the 8. Ipudrst: It is important to him to listen to people
welfare of all people and for who are different from him. Even when he disagrees with them,
nature. he still wants to understand them.
19. Impenv: He strongly believes that people should
care for nature. Looking after the environment is important to
him.
BE Preservation and enhancement 12. Iphlppl: It is very important to him to help the
people around him. He wants to care for their well–being.
Benevolence of the welfare of people with 18. Iplylfr: It is important to him to be loyal to his friends. He
whom one is in frequent wants to devote himself to people close to him.
personal contact.
Table 2
Participating countries, ESS round, sample size (N), and Stress 1 for different starting configurations
Table 2 (continued)
Table 3
Prototypical specification of value structure: Design matrix based on the revised Schwartz model
(1992, p. 45)
Table 4
Table 4 (continued)
Table 4 (continued)
Growth
Protection
Figure 1. Structural Relations among Basic Values (cf. Schwartz, 1992, 2006).
29
Figure 3a. Round 1 (N=2,685); Stress 1=.10 Figure 3b. Round 2 (N=2,640); Stress 1=.11
31
0.75
0 1
2 1
0.25 9
8 0 2
DIM_2
1 3
9 8
7 4
7 4
-0.25 6
5
6
5 3
-0.75
-1.1 -0.6 0.0 0.6 1.1
DIM_1
Figure 4a. Theory Based Custom
Starting Configuration, Stress 1=.10
0.75
3
1
0.25 8 9
2
0
9 4
DIM_2
8 4
6 7 1
7 2 3
0
-0.25 6 5
5
1
-0.75
-1.1 -0.6 0.0 0.6 1.1
DIM_1
Figure 4b. Simplex (default) Starting
Configuration; Stress 1=.11
Author #1
Westfälische Wilhelms-Universität
Fliednerstr. 21
E-mail: [email protected]
Biographical Sketch:
Author #2
Westfälische Wilhelms-Universität
Fliednerstr. 21
E-mail: [email protected]
Biographical Sketch:
Author #3
E-mail: [email protected]
Biographical Sketch:
Shalom H. Schwartz is the Leon and Clara Sznajderman Emeritus Professor of Psychology at
the Hebrew University in Jerusalem. He received his Ph.D. in social psychology from the
University of Michigan-Ann Arbor in 1967 and taught at the University of Wisconsin before
moving to Israel. He coordinates an international project applying his individual and culture
level values theories and methods in over 75 countries. His recent work concerns two broad
topics: the nature and sources of basic human values and their role as bases of attitudes and
behavior; the nature and sources of cultural value orientations as expressions of and
influences on the institutional structures, policies, and prevailing norms and practices in
different societies.