Sentics
Sentics
k
Sentics
ii
Sentics
iii
Clynes
iv
Sentics
SENTICS
THE TOUCH OF THE
EMOTIONS
DR. MANFRED CLYNES
i
Clynes
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
I thank all those who have contributed to make this book possible:
men and women who came to me with their troubles; musicians
who gave of themselves; those who helped in scientific work at the
Rockland State Hospital in Orangeburg, New York, and at the Uni-
versity of California, San Diego and the Biocybernetic Institute; and
above all the thousands who participated in sentic cycles and
reported their experience.
I cannot mention them all-but certainly without Pablo Casals,
Rudolf Serkin, Darius Clynes, Warren McCullough, Otto Schmitt,
Michael Kohn, and Steven Bunnell, this book would not have been
born.
ii
Sentics
Dedication
To the source of essentic form and to those who get drunk on it
'The memory of my father of Pablo Casals and of Artur Schnabel and
to three beautiful women who have lived in me
iii
Clynes
iv
Sentics
Contents
Foreword vii
Foreword 2 VIII
Preface i
Introduction vii
The Qualities of Experience 3
Emotions 15
Acton Theory 22
Measuring Essentic Form, the Biologic Basis of Emotion
Communication 28
Are Essentic Forms Culturally Conditioned? 44
The Communicating Power and Mathematical Equation of
Essentic Form 54
Producing and Recognizing Essentic Form, Empathy, and
Sympathy 61
Music and Sentics: Music as a Sentic Mirror 78
PART TWO 105
Sentic Circles and Their Capacity to Transform 107
The Nature of Sentic Experience: Further Elaborations of
Sentic Theory 162
Sentography and Personal Relationship Profiles 183
Essentic Form and the Transformation of Sources of Energy
193
Sentics and the Sources of Ethical Being 207
Sentics and Space Travel 217
Sentics and Natural Order: the Evolution of Sentic States and
the Generation of New Ideas 224
v
Clynes
vi
Sentics
Foreword
Dr. Clynes and I met the first time at the reception desk of the
newsroom of the New York Times where I was working as a reporter
in the newspaper's science department. It was around the time of the
congressional hearings on the Nuclear Test Ban Treaty of 1961, and
I had been sent to the reception area by the City Desk to interview a
person who claimed to have solved the single-seemingly in-
surmountable, at the time-technical problem standing in the way of
a total ban on all tests – in the air, space, water, and underground.
That problem was how to distinguish between natural earthquakes
and earth tremors touched off by underground nuclear test deto-
nations. The man who it seemed had found the solution was Dr.
Manfred Clynes.
I no longer recall the details of Clynes's scheme except that it
seemed at the time to be ingeniously simple and quite possibly work-
able. However, the process of the government's working out the de
tails to the treaty had already progressed too far for any substantial
modification: tests would be banned only in space, the atmosphere
and under water; underground testing was to be excluded from the
ban.
What stayed with me about Dr. Clynes's visit, however, and his
(characteristically) mumbled explanation of how to differentiate
earthquakes from underground bomb tests was his ingenious and
strikingly original style of thought. It was of a quality for which
vii
Clynes
viii
Sentics
I
Clynes
Simply stated the URS law holds that, first, information about
several events in the world outside and within an organism is per-
ceived more acutely (if not actually differently) under changing
conditions than under static ones. Thus, we are more sensitive to
increasing or to decreasing temperature and lighting than to con-
stant ones—which may even become unnoticeable in time. Sec-
ondly, the law states that each of these two kinds of information
(increasing or decreasing change) requires its own one-way channel,
based on the fact that molecules can arrive only in positive numbers.
This channel many be a series of nerve fibers, a selective membrane,
a sequence of chemical reactions or hormones, or some other
dynamic biological system. For example, information about heating
travels in one channel and that about cooling in another. And, as
shown in the experiment with the eye's response to changing il-
lumination, two channels that carry information about a "similar"
(but actually, opposite) quality -e.g., increasing and decreasing
temperature, illumination—may respond at different rates. But other
systems have only developed one channel, such as the sensation of
smell, which is only sensitive to rate of increase.
From this work, Dr. Clynes began thinking more and more about
the relation of our experience of various "standard" qualities of the
perceived world. This sort of thought led him to experiments in
which he was able, for the first time, to associate specific electrical
brain wave patterns with particular events involving certain qualities
of the perceived world. Thus, as will be explained in this book, Man-
fred was able, first, to associate a particular recorded brain wave
shape with the perception of redness and later to expand the list to
more than one hundred other different visual stimuli (colors, pat-
terns) that were associated with specific shapes of brain waves
generated by subjects who perceived the stimuli. It was after he had
demonstrated this remarkable feat to his own and others' satisfaction
that he made a gigantic-elegant-conceptual leap that resulted in the
birth of a whole new field of science-sentics- -which this book is
about.
II
Sentics
III
Clynes
IV
Sentics
V
Clynes
VI
Sentics
VII
Clynes
Foreword 2
Dr. Manfred Clynes' great achievement is to have made emotions
and their communication respectable, recognizably rendered into
graphs, analysable and measurable. These most intimate, basic and
powerful of human drives are no longer imprisoned within the
realm of conjecture and blind groping with more or less working hy-
potheses, but have been released for study, respect and compassion.
It is obvious that this signal breakthrough could only have been
achieved by a musician: Dr. Clynes is a very distinguished pianist; a
scientist who remains a musician at heart. In the same way that
painting and drawing furthered our understanding of the human
body and of our anatomy, so is music the true and only revelation of
emotion, whether as pure music or as the music of language or
dance. When, as in Manfred Clynes' case, the music is allied to a
penetrating intellect and scientific insight, miracles may happen.
Dr. Clynes' contribution to the interrelationship of emotions, to
our crying need for personal expression, and for the cleansing of our
minds and souls, illuminates the good sense of our tribal ancestors
with their games and gestures and oral traditions, for they knew the
supreme importance of sound and contact. Perhaps his gift to
humanity will serve to lead us to a more balanced existence, to a co-
ordinated and reciprocal strengthening of mind and heart. Perhaps
it will lead to greater harmony and fewer wars. I welcome this sentic
science, for the god of love has finally put on the guise of science to
lead us into the truly humane - for we, having lost our intuition, and
often employing our intellect merely to enhance our brutality, and
having lost our faith in feeling and conscience, may rediscover our-
selves through the only avenue we acknowledge, that of science. I
pray, however, that just as every thing may be used to both good and
evil ends, this new science may be pursued to good, compassionate
and wholesome purposes only.
Yehudi Menuhin New York
VIII
Sentics
Preface
This book is about emotion communication. We are discovering
rather to our surprise—that nature has made the communication of
emotions elegantly simple. To find out how nature's dynamic
communicative forms function in man with precision and power,
this is the branch of science called sentics.
Western man has long lived as an uneasy guest within nature. In
trying to discover natural order, he put himself outside the order he
was discovering-as an observer, if not always a dispassionate ob-
server. He could not fit his passions and emotions into the order, but
had to regard them as epiphenomena. The existential attributes of
emotions could not compare with those of atoms and molecules they
did not seem to belong to the world at all as entities. Many psy-
chologists even doubted their existence, since there was nowhere an
emotion could be shown to be located.
Spinoza had long ago regarded emotions as part of nature to be
contemplated with the same interest and enjoyment as other natural
phenomena, but later centuries progressively divided Western man's
attitude into the objective and subjective in an attempt to rid
mankind of superstition and prejudice. Having accomplished this
task in good measure, man found himself paying the price of having
driven a painful wedge into his being, dividing him from himself as
well as from his fellow beings and natural environment.
i
Clynes
The work that is presented in this book may help to reunite man's
divided nature. Research in the developing science of sentics-the
study of genetically programmed dynamic forms of emotional ex-
pression—is leading us to discover the simple elegance of nature's
plan within us. Finding experimentally how emotions are grounded
precisely in natural order, and how the keys to the communication
of emotion are designed according to specific laws, we can discover
a new sense of our belonging to nature and recognize our common
brotherhood. Sentic studies are uncovering a natural basis of
emotion communication universal among humans.
In the chapters that follow we shall describe the precise dynamic
forms that are characteristic of each emotion. We call them essentic
forms. These forms operate like keys in the locks of our nervous
system. People touch one another emotionally through these ex-
pressive space-time forms. Without them we would live in
emotional isolation. Nature has provided for emotion
communication and contagion between individuals-the are of
communication—through precise spatio-temporal forms, ge-
netically programmed as specifically as the form of a kidney is
programmed. This book describes how these elementary biologic
forms were found.
To measure essentic form, new instruments – sentographs – had
to be developed. These instruments are used for discovering the
precise nature of essentic form and for diagnosing aspects of person-
ality. Chapter Four of this book will describe the methods by which
the sentograph can be used to measure emotional expression and
Chapter Eleven will relate how it can be helpful in discovering a sub-
ject's emotional relationships with key figures in his life and with his
environment.
Essentic forms are not idiosyncratic to each individual—although
when and how he may want to use them may well be. They represent
the natural "words" of emotion communication, "words" that were
developed before speech arose. Some of these we share with animals-
a dog often understands our tone of voice as well as a child.
Throughout this book we have tried to look at these "words," care-
ii
Sentics
fully and lovingly, to see a glimpse of their exquisite nature and of the
laws of their power.
The power of communication of each "word" of essentic form, as
expressed in any expressive action (a laugh, a pat on the back, an
angry scowl, etc.), depends on its faithfulness to the characteristic
shape that it expresses. This biologic solution to the design problem
of how individuals may communicate qualities to one another has
also made it necessary to develop a new mathematics for dealing
with the power of emotion communication. For thousands of vears
artists. have used the remarkable potentiation of realizing precise
form to communicate and generate emotion and qualities. The bio-
logic basis for this power is now being discovered through the ex-
perimental isolation of pure essentic forms.
The neurophysiologic function of these forms in communicating
and generating emotions has largely remained outside the field of
psychoanalysis as well as that of traditional psychology. Neither
these nor psychiatry has systematically investigated the biologic
dynamic functions of essentic forms. Yet, the study of their function
leads to methods for using them for therapeutic purposes. The
individual's experience of generating specific emotions through re-
peatedly producing essentic form in accordance with their biologic
timing, and being able to be in touch with virtually the entire
spectrum of emotions in a controlled manner which these methods
provide give the individual a powerful preventive and normative
tool. In Part Two of this book, we shall describe the discovery and
development of "sentic cycles," which involves the experience of
seven basic emotion states in a specific order over the period of a half
hour. Practicing this method of "active meditation" has led many
subjects to a feeling of peace, increased emotional fluidity and re-
lease of blocked emotions, and other beneficial effects. It provides a
feeling of grounding in one's inner self. The Buddhist practice of
contemplation of feelings presages this experience. The therapist
may use it as a tool that produces insight into the personality struc-
ture, permits the experience of emotion, and can lead to peace for
the person seeking therapy.
iii
Clynes
How does the practice of sentic cycles and sentic training relate to
other forms of therapy? The theory of psychoanalysis presently
seems too self-contained to engender a systematic extension linking
it with sentic theory. One may hope that a way will be found to pro-
vide an integration of the two systems of thought. The Jungian con-
cept of archetypes may constitute a partial bridge. The findings and
theories of sentics are also in accord with much of the Gestalt school
of psychotherapy and psychology. Some of the phenomena de-
scribed and measured in sentics may be recognized by those experi-
enced in Gestalt therapy. The importance of closing the Gestalt, the
experience of wholes, clearly relates to the experience of essentic
form.
In recent years, biofeedback methods have been developed to
teach a person to control bodily functions that are not normally sub-
ject to conscious control. To achieve this, instruments are used that
inform him of the effectiveness of his control over these processes.
But in discovering essentic forms within himself, a person does not
have to use instrumental biofeedback techniques—the feeling itself
is an inherent biologic feedback. A person knows through his own
feeling when he has expressed the true essentic form. Practice of es-
sentic form is self-centering.
Since sentics is very much involved in the study of fantasy
emotions, it also strikes a responsive chord in all disciplines where
fantasy emotion is of importance. This, of course, includes the arts
and music-sentics has much to say concerning the communication
of qualities by artists and musicians. The biologic elements of
emotion communication are nowhere better employed than in the
arts.
But perhaps the most important application of sentics should be
in education. Its methods can allow children, or adults, to learn to be
in touch with their emotions, to discriminate between them, and to
enjoy their controlled use. For people of all ages, a greater awareness
of the emotional processes they share (and specifically the shared ex-
perience of doing sentic cycles) may provide a direct way for people
of different races and backgrounds to experience their common
iv
Sentics
v
Clynes
vi
Sentics
Introduction
It is man's predicament that he feels the necessity to create a self-
conscious vision of himself in the universe in order to feel at home
in it. It is not enough that such a world view be intellectual; it is nec-
essary that he develop a feeling of belonging. His role is not defined
for him; he feels he needs to define it and discover it for himself. In
part, his unique position and problem is posed to him by his gift of
language. To re-create his experience in word images is al most too
difficult a challenge for him. His resulting social institutions do not
clearly reflect his nature. And so he moves like a spiral growing be-
tween the poles of verbalization and differentiated direct experi-
ence-trying to grasp the transitory and to discover the timeless that
reveals itself forever anew in each moment.
"Trying to grasp": in that phrase man's predicament is summed up.
That phrase, with its double physiological and psychological
meaning, points to man's condition and to the contribution of
language to his dilemma. That he may "grasp" more, man's reach
should indeed exceed his "grasp." And so, embarked on his quest, he
discovers how to ask questions.
Only man can ask a question. Discovery of questions was man's
first chain reaction: every good question generates others. (We are
now experiencing a Malthusian question explosion perhaps even
greater than the population explosion.) A new question leads-it does
not follow. Where does it arise then? If it comes by chance, what is
vii
Clynes
even consider how the form of living movement relates to the invis-
ible order of the genetic code. Yet most of life is concerned with
movement and only through movement do we communicate in the
present moment.
We can try to distinguish those aspects of man's possible time pat-
terns of behavior and communication which are programmed
directly as a consequence of DNA relationships, from those which
man acquired or devised arbitrarily. In doing so, as we shall see, it is
possible to discover in oneself time forms of expression which are
"solid" and "true" in the sense that they are faithful to our biologic
programming. And strangely, when these true forms are discovered
they provide the means for the most powerful immediate
communication with others in the present moment. This seems to
be so because the production and the perception of these forms is
governed by a coordinated biologic design, much as are the separate
coordinated functions of speaking and hearing.
Indeed, one of the strangest and yet commonplace natural phe-
nomena is the way in which we communicate qualities and shades
of emotion to each other through vision, sound, and touch. With
methods to be described, we can now measure and identify some of
the extraordinary precision with which these dynamic
communications are created and sensed. By producing and
measuring the precise time profile, or trajectory of the expressive
act, in a new way, we are beginning to be able to analyze the remark-
able communicative power that is inherent in the trajectories of
these forms and to determine which aspects of these processes ap-
pear to be biologically de signed and genetically preserved.
It is found, in fact, that the nervous system has design features that
allow it both to generate precise elements of communication faithful
to specific qualities, and also to recognize these elements when
communicated by others. It can do this, moreover, through a large
variety of modes of sensory communication. But behind the forms
of each particular sensory communication, it appears there lies a
true generating form specific for that emotional quality. We call
these essentic forms.
ix
Clynes
xi
Clynes
1
But note: even the direction of time, clear in our experience, is not given
by the translation into physics of this sensorium.
xiii
Clynes
xv
Clynes
xvi
Sentics
SENTICS
xvii
Clynes
xviii
Sentics
PART ONE
Sentic States: A Common Basis of Human Being
1
Clynes
2
Sentics
3
Clynes
the physiologic code, but also it has left a great many areas of experi-
ence untranslated and has made mistakes in the naming of others.1
In this chapter we shall look at what underlies various aspects of the
concept of quality.
1
Other processes of the brain may be involved in the recognition of the pat-
terns which we have not observed and it is possible that considerable
dissimilarities could arise in such processes. The probability of this, however,
must be small since in all those processes that have been available for
measurement, the brain functions operate similarly with respect to the
quality of red. If there were processes which have escaped our attention
operating in the recognition of red these would necessarily have to be on a
much more limited scale electrically than the ones we have observed.
5
Clynes
6
Sentics
7
Clynes
8
Sentics
1
If we look at the origins of the word "quality" we see that it comes from the
Latin root quale meaning "such." Thus the origin of the word "quality" re ally
means "suchness," such as it is—an entirely appropriate formulation as we
may agree, as it cannot be defined in any other terms.
11
Clynes
12
Sentics
13
Clynes
14
Sentics
Emotions
Belonging to a class of qualities inherently linked to the motor
system, each emotion, as a unique quality, completes its identity only
if the motor dynamic program, part of which we usually call "expres-
sion," is included. Emotion and its expression form an existential
unit, a system. In order to characterize and understand its function,
we need to consider its system properties.
Unfortunately, as psychology as a science developed, emotion and
its expression were mostly studied separately. To study emotion
without its expression (and vice versa) is like cutting off the hand to
study its function. This is one reason why many functions of
emotions remained obscure and also why quantitative methods were
not found for their study. Indeed, unless our concepts correspond to
functioning processes we will not discover their lawful behavior.
(Galileo, for example, had to conceive of the concept of acceleration
before measurements could correspond to theories.) In studying
emotion as a system identity, including the phenomena of expres-
sion as part of the system, in a new scientific approach, it becomes
possible to evolve new concepts, subject to precise measurements.
This approach is neither exclusively psychological nor exclusively
physiological, but considers emotion and its expression as one
functional system.
But before we go on to discuss the theories and findings developed
from this approach, let us briefly review what we know of the char-
acteristic properties of emotions.
15
Clynes
Emotions reflect and affect the quality of life, yet because of our
uncertain understanding of the phenomena of emotion we have
been far from clear as to how. Thus, some thinkers were even in favor
of avoiding or eliminating emotion entirely. They regard emotion as
restricting the free use of the mind and blinding man to what is "in
his best interests." Others equally strongly assert that life without
emotions is not worth living. To navigate between these extremes, a
key question seems to be: how to experience emotions con-
structively rather than being used by them.
The following are some characteristics of the state of emotion, its
expression, and its generation:
Emotion states
1. Each basic emotion is a unique experience.
2. To each basic emotion there corresponds a characteristic
brain pattern.
3. Each emotional state has a characteristic inertia, in terms of
brain and bodily processes. Once it has been established, it
will persist for some duration of time.
4. An emotion will tend to confine specific action patterns
according to its nature over a period of time.
5. Hormonal and cardiovascular changes occur together with an
emotional state. The extent and type of these changes will
depend on the nature of the specific emotion and also on the
attitude of control by the individual. Hormonal changes, in
turn, may predispose a person to experience motion.
6. Memory, unconscious, and autonomic processes influence the
control functions relating to emotional states.
7. The experience of emotions is influenced by age, sex, genetic
inheritance, and diurnal, seasonal, and other biologic
rhythms.
Emotional expression
1. The expression of emotion is an essential aspect of its nature,
Emotion needs to be expressed, much as a control system
needs to respond to its input until the desired output is
16
Sentics
obtained.
2. Expression has an effect on the intensity of the state of
emotion. It can both charge (increase) and discharge
(decrease) the intensity.
Generation of emotion
1. The generation of emotion occurs through perception of and
changes in our existential circumstances: our relationships
with others, our environment, our losses and our gains, our
freedom to pursue our needs, and our self-image.
2. Emotion can also be generated through perceiving emotion in
others.
3. Emotion can be generated purely through the imagination by
imagining and remembering persons, forms, qualities, and
situations.
4. All these processes can be variously affected by drugs and by
specific electrical stimulation of the brain.
Emotions and environment interaction
1. Emotions play a crucial part in our interactions with our
social and natural environment.
2. Our drives and mental energy are affected specifically by our
emotions.
3. The degree of crowding—and its effect on privacy and
intimacy-affects the experience of emotional states.
4. Processes of habituation and adaptation also affect the
experience of emotion in a specific environment.
Obviously, emotions have an all-pervasive significance to our life
potential. Yet we have great difficulty in studying them system-
atically. One problem is in the use of vaguely defined words. Thus,
"emotion" and "feeling" are used almost interchangeably in psycho-
logic literature as well as in common parlance. In fact, there is no
accepted definition that distinguishes the appropriate use of these
two concepts. At best, a consensus might be that "feelings" are low-
intensity "emotions." Although an emotion may be overpowering
and beyond control, or it may be sensed and enjoyed without one's
17
Clynes
18
Sentics
in more specific ways. The latter two principles introduced here for
formal reasons will be elucidated in the following chapter.
19
Clynes
1
One day when visiting the 200, I saw a bear yawn. Before I knew what I
was doing, I found myself yawning, too. But how did I know the bear was
yawning, and was not hungry or angry? Clearly, it was through perceiving
the precise way that he opened his mouth. (And as I dictate this anecdote, I
also yawn. The imagined yawn generated a yawn in me also!) A yawn has a
clear beginning and end, and a specific timecourse, like other expressive
actions.
20
Sentics
21
Clynes
Acton Theory
If we consider a simple, single voluntary movement, such as an eye
movement or a finger movement, we see readily that it consists of a
decision or command, and its execution. The decision embodies the
idea of the movement. We have named the idea of a quality, together
with its specific brain function concomitants, an "idiolog." We may
say that there are sensory idiologs (the idea of a sound or smell),
affective idiologs (the idea of joy or anger), and motor idiologs (the
idea of particular movement). A motor idiolog calls for the exe-
cution of a single voluntary motor action. For example, if we have an
idea to throw a ball to hit a certain object, we may have a precise
motor idiolog. Its execution, in turn, may correspond to this idiolog
more or less precisely. Our success with the throw will depend upon
the precision of the idiolog and the precision of its execution.
Simple movements initiated by single voluntary decisions (such as
the single movement of a finger, or of an arm, or of the eyes) are pre-
programmed by the brain before they begin. Every such voluntary
movement is carried out cooperatively by two sets of muscles, one
set to accelerate the motion and the other to decelerate it. For each
movement, a set of "agonist" and "antagonist" muscles work together
to create the precise character of the intended movement. The two
muscle systems clearly work together with the most remarkable
precision in order to start, accelerate, and stop movement as
required. This "coordination" is programmed by the brain in the mo-
ment it makes the decision.
22
Sentics
23
Clynes
24
Sentics
The Acton
Within the duration of the present moment no further decision
can be made. A single movement, once begun, must run its course
as preprogrammed by the idiolog of the motor action. Such a single
movement, consisting of the decision and its execution, has a clear
beginning and end. We need to recognize that such a voluntary
movement constitutes an existential entity. We have named this
entity an acton. An acton is rooted both in the psychologic and phys-
iologic. To state it as it was defined in 1969:
A single movement and its decision constitute an existential
unit integrally combining the physiologic and conscious aspects.
We call such a preprogrammed voluntary movement having a
clear beginning and end an "acton." An acton is the combination
of the acton's idiolog and its execution. (Clynes 1968; 1969.)
By recognizing such unity where it exists, we may develop theories
that correspond more closely to actual processes.
An expressive movement which has a clear beginning and end is
also an acton, but a special kind of acton. Expressive actons are
actons whose dynamic forms (i.e., space-time forms) are modulated
by the sentic states seeking expression. The sentic state, through the
cerebellum and other brain structures,1 influences the pre-pro
grammed course of the acton so as to alter it from the unmodulated,
undifferentiated course of the non-expressive acton. For example, a
playful toss of a stone differs much from a throw in anger. Moreover,
each sentic state has its own characteristic way of modulating the
acton. In this way, essentic form is expressed.
The modulation of the acton affects its shape and its total duration
as well. Such sentically modulated actons are called E-actons, for
"expressive actons." There are specific E-actons for each character-
istic sentic state. A smile is an example of such an E-acton. So is a
1
Many of the brain's automatic computations that shape the acton are
performed by the cerebellum, as has become clear through the work of J. C.
Eccles, whose findings have been significant in clarifying the role of the
cerebellum in governing specific motor programs.
25
Clynes
1
Most of us may have observed, for example, that repeated sobbing or
laughing or "angering" may intensify the sentic state over an initial period of
time.
26
Sentics
27
Clynes
28
Sentics
29
Clynes
30
Sentics
Figure 6. Typical response shapes illustrate the essentic form of grief, joy,
hate, and reverence, respectively. Note the pronounced horizontal compo-
nent for hate (indicating an orientation away from the body as seen with
Anger in Figure 5), the late muscle acceleration in muscle 2 indicating a
secondary thrust characteristic of passion. The response to grief is similar
to that of love but is flatter and slightly outward. Muscular action for grief
displays a lassitude which prevents the subject from actively lifting and re-
leasing pressure-the opposite of that for joy, which exhibits a rebound over-
shoot after the initial downward deflection of the finger. Reverence shows
some similarity to love but on a longer time scale—the programming of the
acton is extended in time.
activity are called "electromyograms." (In order to ob serve the
changes of activity in these more clearly, the electromyogram is
rectified and integrated with a time constant of less than o1 second.)
In addition to such electromyograms, we may simultaneously
measure the electric activity of the brain in a manner similar to that
described in Chapter One for observing the brain's responses to vis-
ual stimuli. (See Figure 7.) The patterns of change which we observe
33
Clynes
on various levels all tend to confirm the stability of the essentic form.
The clearest measure of essentic form is provided by the recordings
of transient finger pressure. The arm is in fact a biologic filter
through which activity of the brain not connected with expression of
a particular essentic form is eliminated. The brain's electric activity,
in addition to aspects related to essentic form, contains all other
activity related to the innumerable conscious, unconscious, and au-
tonomic functions. Thus the expression of essentic form can at
present be revealed in the brain's electric activity measured from the
scalp only through averaging. We also measured processes that
reflect changes related to particular sentic states, rather than to each
separate expression. We observed and compared the changes in
34
Sentics
Figure 8A. Comparison of forms recorded from three different subjects ex-
pressing the sentic states of anger and love. Note the general similari. ties
of corresponding essentic forms and also features of individual differences.
Average of fifty actons.
35
Clynes
Figure 8B. Sentograms compared (vertical traces only) from different sub-
jects expressing the states of anger, hate, and joy as illustrative examples.
istic ones of well-defined forms.† Let us now look at the sentograms of the
specific essentic forms in some detail, in a sequence as given on pages 33-
35.
same sentic state when obtaining measurements in a different cul-
ture. Fortunately, however, we are aided here by the very nature of
the existence we wish to demonstrate-since it turns out that the
qualities of the spectrum of emotions are more precise by far than the
words used to describe them. We find that, regardless of the impreci-
sion of the language, sentic states tend to sort themselves out into
characteristic ones of well-defined forms.1 Let us now look at the
1
By being in the right "ball park," the subject himself focuses in on the true
existence, the form of which is inwardly given to him. Being attentive to that
inward form, he soon finds the expression that corresponds to it. The process
36
Sentics
Figure 9. This bar graph illustrates how the observed sentograms for var-
ious sentic states (measured from a number of subjects) can be readily
classified and identified according to parameters of the observed form.
Measuring the interval of time from the beginning of the subject's ex. pres-
sion to its peak pressure and multiplying it by a weighted tangent of the
angle of pressure (calculated from the ratio of the size of the vertical and
horizontal traces), one thus obtains an index which clearly separates the
individual forms statistically into distinct categories. It is significant that
each sentic state illustrated is clearly distinct for different individuals.
is similar to finding a way to hit an object with a ball, once having a clear
idea of what you want to hit. Because each expression completed is experi-
enced as an entity by the subject, the subject knows the feeling of the correct
expression. One can even say that the correct expression provides a minor
satisfaction to the subject, in a way not dissimilar to having hit the target.
The satisfaction is different, however, from that of hitting a target since it
partakes of the quality of the sentic state concerned. An interesting point,
however, is that this satisfaction is felt only to the degree that the correct
form is actually produced. Because of the feedback that an E-acton has on
the sentic state, the subject can be aware of the nature of his expressive act.
The feeling of sincerity thus gains on operational definition.
37
Clynes
Anger
The essentic form of anger consists of a sharp movement in a
direction markedly away from the body (a large outward horizontal
component, downward on graph). The acton starts rapidly and ends
after a relatively short time (less than one second). In terms of effort
it is most intense at its beginning. It tends to occur together with a
sharp, short expiration of breath and corresponding diaphragm con-
traction. (In generating these fantasy states, the subject is in the sit-
ting position with the body quiet, as described.) The head tends to
be in a slightly downward tilted position. The gaze is directed slightly
downward. (Even the raising of the eyes or of the head is mostly felt
as contrary to the natural expression of anger.) A firm jaw position
with slight clenching of the teeth also tends to occur during anger,
breathing tends to be rapid, and oxygen consumption and heart rate
increased. (We shall not here describe indications of anger in the fa-
cial expression as they are well known. The facial expression during
sentic states does not change transiently with each Eacton, in our
method, but tends to have a steady and cumulative character for
each state.)
Hate
The essentic form of hate is markedly different from anger, but
also shows similarities. At first, some subjects find it difficult to dis-
tinguish between expressions of these two. There is a passionate
character in hate that finds its expression in a late-developing
muscular activity within each acton. The late muscular tension is ob-
served in specific muscle groups and is essential to its passionate na-
ture. The essentic form of hate starts more slowly than anger and is
also strongly away from the body. The peak of intensity in terms of
tension occurs considerably later than in anger. Through the specific
secondary, late muscle activity (see Figure 6), the essentic form is
also abruptly terminated with a rapid diminution of pressure.1 This
1
In using the tone of voice to express hate, one may observe this late active
muscle action in the rapid, tense termination of the voice, often associated
with a tense, final closure of the mouth, and clenching of teeth.
38
Sentics
deceleration is far more rapid than the initial acceleration. The form
ends abruptly with an exceedingly high rate of change of motion.
The experience of hate is also accompanied by expiration of the
breath to some extent paralleling the dynamic course of the finger
pressure. In addition to tension of the diaphragm, more extensive
abdominal tension also accompanies the acton.
Respiration rate, heart rate, and oxygen consumption are
increased ‡ during the sentic state of hate. Termination pressure of
the acton generally does not coincide with the initial base level, so
that a remnant of steady tension may continue to exist for some time.
Grief
The essentic form of grief1 is principally a letting go, an almost
passive letting go or collapse. The acton is neither away from the
body nor toward it. It appears to be governed by a special lassitude.
The initial downward exertion is followed by a period of apparent
weakness and relative immobility. The finger pressure remains rel-
atively passive and constant. Respiration tends to stop at the bottom
of the expiratory phase, like the end of a sigh. Before the next acton
can be executed, one must "pick oneself up" and be poised for the
next expression of grief which again is an almost passive dropping.
The position of the head tends to be slightly inclined to the right, for
right-handed people. The gaze is somewhat downward; the head
may also be tilted slightly forward. The abdominal musculature is
relaxed, as are muscle systems in general. There is no late muscular
activity. Oxygen consumption, heart rate, and respiration all tend to
be lower. Respiration particularly is slowed down considerably. The
duration of the grief acton is greatly prolonged, perhaps three or four
seconds, or longer.
1
Grief is distinguished from sadness in that it contains an irretrievable ele-
ment; with regard to that aspect, no hope is possible. For example, the death
of a loved one is often imagined by subjects; in music, the slow movement of
Beethoven's Third Symphony provides an example.
39
Clynes
Love
The subject is told that we are not dealing with sexual love in this
phase but rather with "motherly" or "brotherly" love. Again, words
are less precise than the sentic state and this state appears to be
generally well understood by the subjects.
The essentic form of love is a prolonged, smooth curve. The
direction of pressure tends to be slightly toward the body rather than
away from it. The muscles participating display a smooth continuous
action with greatest intensity near the beginning and toward the
middle. The acton starts with a much lower acceleration than for
anger or hate. Respiration rate and oxygen consumption are slower.
During an acton there is slight tension in the diaphragm with
abdominal relaxation. The breath does not rest at the end of ex-
piration as it does during grief, but is smooth. Love actons are often
begun near the end of inspiration and continue during the be-
ginning of expiration. But they can be executed during various res-
piratory phases. Love actons are terminated gradually with a smooth
diminution of pressure. There is a smooth return to the base level
from which the next expressive acton can be begun. Position of the
head tends to be neither raised nor lowered.
Sex
'The essentic form of sex also displays a passionate element of
"late" muscular activity (see Figure 5). The late active component
arises from special muscle groups. There is a marked horizontal
pressure component with some continuing steady state tension after
completion of the acton. The dynamic form is highly characteristic
of the manner in which the muscular activity develops, providing
specific modes of pressure, acceleration, and deceleration with a
marked horizontal component. The acton is somewhat prolonged
but shorter than the love acton. There is some abdominal tension,
but of a very different, more sustained character than for hate.
Breathing tends to be rapid, and at times temporarily blocked during
the expiration phase, so that the breathing contains abrupt panting-
like short puffs that start and end abruptly. Similar abrupt phases of
40
Sentics
Joy
The essentic form of joy is expressed predominantly vertically. It
tends to be neither away from nor toward the body and has a
bouncelike character. A relatively small initial downward pressure is
followed by the longer tail of a rebound ending in a floating kind of
return to the base line. The common phrase "jumping for joy" ap-
pears to be reflected in this form. The floating sensation of the over-
shoot phase is an essential part of the acton experience. This
sensation may well be related to aspects of the experience of weight-
lessness encountered by astronauts, or the frequently encountered
dream phenomenon of floating in the air as if flying.1 But the acton
also includes an initial rapid though moderate push, with
moderately high acceleration. The acton is characterized by a "free"
manner of exertion, without late muscular activity or maintained
tensions. The absence of a horizontal component also appears to
contribute to the freeness of the implied motion. The duration of the
joy acton is relatively short. The position of the head tends to be
slightly raised, the gaze slightly upward, and the head inclined nei-
ther to the right nor to the left. Respiration rate tends to be slightly
increased. The joy E-actons occur with inspiration and rarely with
expiration. The inspiration tends to begin with a rapid phase, as if
displaying an initial eagerness. Expiration is relaxed. Of all the sentic
states considered, joy is most consistently associated with the in-
1
This aspect will be further discussed in Chapter Fourteen on sentics and
space travel.
41
Clynes
Reverence
The essentic form of reverence resembles that of love but is also
different in a number of aspects. The horizontal component of
pressure is not toward the body but slightly outward, or nearly
vertical. Its course is even more prolonged than that of love and be-
gins with very low acceleration. It is accompanied by a minimum of
tension of any kind. There is no abdominal tension and very little
tension of the diaphragm. Respiration is slowed down to a very slow
rate, slower than in any of the other sentic forms considered. There
tends to be a respiratory pause at the end of inspiration, the opposite
condition as found in grief (an anti-sigh!). Heart rate and oxygen
consumption tend to be lowered considerably. Head position and
gaze are often slightly upward.
No significant differences between essentic forms produced by
male and female subjects have been observed so far. Subjects are all
able to experience at least some of the emotions on first trial. After
three trial sessions the effectiveness, as reported by the subject in
terms of which emotions they could feel, is well over go per cent.
Subjects who may have had difficulties with one or two of the states
at first find that this difficulty diminishes with subsequent trials.
Self-scoring indices, in which a subject scores himself one half hour
after the completion of the test concerning the intensity of the sepa-
rate emotions experienced on a scale from o to 5, give an average
intensity of emotions from four hundred subjects as 3.8. Subjects on
the average tend to have least problems with anger, rarely with grief
and sex, more with hate, love, and joy in that order. Difficulties are
most often experienced with reverence, an emotion perhaps that half
1
Sometimes, subjects do not imagine the idiolog of the E-acton described
here: instead they imagine themselves joyfully hugging another person, and
express the implied dynamics of the hugging which gives a very different
form; such differences interestingly shed light on the idiolog processes in-
volved.
42
Sentics
the subjects are "out of touch" with. It is usual in these subjects, how-
ever, to find scores for this rising from o to 1 to 3 or 4 after three
trials. If instructions are misunderstood and subjects do not
modulate their touch, they also report that they do not experience
the states, but feel boredom instead. Near optimal mean time be-
tween initiating signals was 4.8 sec. for anger, 5.3 for hate, 8.2 for
grief, 7.4 for love, 4-9 for sex, 5.2 for joy, 9.8 for reverence. Standard
deviation of successive times of initiating ranges from o.6 to o.9 sec.
and is proportionately somewhat less for the longer periods.
The sentic states we have described are those we have measured
most.1 There are of course other states which may be considered as
basic, that is, states that are not mixtures of several sentic states. But
an informed guess might be that there are not more than twenty
such basic sentic states. As these studies proceed it should become
possible to identify progressively the basic states, as has been done
with colors and other systems of qualities.
Many states, on the other hand, are clearly mixtures (envy, for
example). These too may have essentic forms. We are gradually find-
ing out how the essentic forms of mixed states are synthesized from
the essentic forms of the states making up the mixture. The study of
this aspect is helped by analyzing sentographic personal relationship
in profiles which we shall discuss in Chapter Eleven. Certainly, the
essentic form of a compound state is not a simple sum of the essentic
forms of the component states. One would not expect this to be so;
as is known to every cook, the taste of a dish is not the sum of the
taste of the ingredients. The elegant way in which the biologic design
does seem to have solved this problem is also discussed in Chapter
Eleven.
Thus, the essentic forms illustrated in this chapter can be regarded
as a general frame for the emotional spectrum. We may hope to fill
the gaps gradually as research progresses further.
1
Others studied include hope, courage, guilt, and shame.
43
Clynes
become the essentic form of love and vice versa. If the forms are arbi-
trarily learned, it should be possible to retrain or condition a subject
to express sentic states with different or switched essentic forms.
One can try to train subjects to do this, offering suitable induce-
ments and rewards, that is to say, positive or negative reinforce-
ments.
It is found, however, that even after prolonged periods of training,
subjects are unable to express or to generate the specific sentic state
through a different dynamic form. Such different forms will not act
to generate the original state in accordance with the fifth sentic
principle.1 Nor can the subject, if he feels the sentic state of love, ex-
press that state through the form of anger or vice versa. A caress
cannot replace a jabbing movement in expressing anger, and vice
versa. Thus (and fortunately so) it does not appear possible to sub-
stitute artificial sentic forms for the natural ones. Moreover, the de-
liberate introduction and use of such artificial forms appears to
cause continuing "blockage," an inability to experience the satis-
faction of expression (a process equivalent to an aspect of repres-
sion).
The stubborn resistance of this natural process to be receptive to
artifice is a further indication of the innate precision of the recog-
nition process.2 Even though it proves not to be possible to retrain an
individual to alter the function of essentic form, we needed to
question further whether inheritance could result in different es-
1
A schematic listing of the sentic principles discussed throughout this book
can be found in the appendix.
2
It is fortunate that this is so, otherwise our understanding of the qualities
of love, anger, or grief would be as unstable as the changing tastes in fashions
of the day. Instead, we find that the essentic forms of love, anger, and grief
have as great a stability as the quality of red. Because of this we are able to
communicate with and through the expressive achievements of the distant
past, recognizing elements of humanity present in Greek sculpture, in the
paintings of Rembrandt, in Bach or Beethoven as well as in Picasso or
Stravinsky, and hearing cadences in the poetry of Homer and Shakespeare,
whose mental time forms - idiologs - communicate unchanging qualities to
us.
47
Clynes
sentic forms among various human groups. To see whether the spe-
cific essentic forms we discovered were universal to human nature
we carried out studies in several different cultures.
In a pilot study we sought to measure essentic form in a remote
Mexican village, hoping to learn how to conduct a larger-scale study.
We carried with us all the equipment including the averaging com-
puter. This made the expedition perhaps more cumbersome than
necessary. The Mexican people proved to be exceedingly friendly to
us and everywhere we were gladly given all the help we needed. We
found a young university student eager to help us as an interpreter,
and after his simple explanations, Mexicans in even the smallest
villages were readily willing to go through the procedures. Most of
them seemed to enjoy the sessions.
Our attempts were futile, of course, in finding places of Mexican
culture uninfluenced by North American life. Even the most remote
villages prided themselves on their transistor radios and pin-up girls
clipped from American magazines.
We did learn from that pilot project, though, the importance of
adequately translating the key words denoting the sentic states. If
these words are imperfectly translated, then observed differences are
ambiguous; they might be ascribed to either different word conno-
tation, or to a different essentic form. Perfect translations are, of
course, never possible even with these so widely used words; each
language has its own subtlety of meaning and connotation that pre-
vents a completely accurate translation. Yet slight differences of word
meaning should not really matter, since we had previously noted that
a subject tends to discover the precise essentic form within himself
and the commonality underlying the corresponding words should
overshadow small differences in conventional meaning.
Sometimes, however, such differences are sufficiently marked to
cause difficulties. For example, the word "joy" turns out to be difficult
to translate into Spanish. Neither the word alegria nor contento is
sufficiently equivalent to the Blakean or Mozartean concept of joy.1
1
On the other hand, there is a word in the Balinese language for the intense
48
Sentics
49
Clynes
50
Sentics
51
Clynes
52
Sentics
Figure 11. A comparison of the essentic forms of love and anger in four
cultures: Mexican, Japanese, American, and Balinese (vertical component
shown). Similarities of the sentograms are apparent. Differences be. tween
individuals are typically of about the same order as variation between cul-
tures.
sentic states in this manner. For the Balinese, the quasi-magical
aspect of this was prominent.1
Results of measurements of the Japanese and Balinese essentic
forms confirmed the similarity of different cultural groups and sup-
ported the view that specific essentic forms are characteristic of
human nature, regardless of race and culture. (See Figure 11.) This
is, of course, of inestimable value for the communication of
emotions and qualities among all people of the earth. It is a docu-
mentation of our brotherhood, in terms of our common inheritance
of unchanging, pure qualities of emotions and their expressive
forms, which are potentially programmed, so it seems, into every
man.
1
In our studies of the Balinese we were fortunate to have the assistance of
Dr. Denny Thong, a remarkable young doctor who had pioneered medical
services for the villages of Bali, where modern medical and surgical instru-
ments are almost totally lacking. He also has pioneered in providing psy-
chiatric services to these islanders, especially for those few cases of
schizophrenia among the three million population that do occur. Through his
enthusiastic help we were also able to obtain adequate translations of all the
words denoting sentic states except for hate.
53
Clynes
56
Sentics
1
A different, but related confirmation of the essentic form that we observed
has also come from the work of Bruce Brown at Brigham Young University,
who constructed his own sentograph and found similar essentic forms in the
sento graphic responses of fifty-four subjects to a set of emotional speech
sample stimuli. (He in fact found a clearer differentiation of affect through
sentographic responses than through verbal responses, given by the Osgood
semantic differential measures.)
57
Clynes
58
Sentics
59
Clynes
merit of the theory that it can be posed at all. And we have at least a
few clues on how to proceed in our search toward that goal. If there
is an answer to this question (and there could be reasons why an
answer could not be formulated), it may be that within some decades
it could be within our grasp.
60
Sentics
61
Clynes
tend to feel love for him.) This empathy arises when, in the manifest
openness of the person expressing, we sense that the expression
arises from the center of his being. We see no "artifice" in it and
nothing intrudes into the pattern of communication to act as a
warning sign of another level of communication, hiding behind
conventional, learned clichés or other "superficial" influences. We
intuitively distinguish a genuine, deeply felt communication from
superficial expressions.
Superficial expressions may have sentic significance in revealing a
person's psychological condition, particularly in the way he may be
blocking deeper, more meaningful communication. But we rec-
ognize the sharp differences between such expressions and authentic
ones precisely through the perception of essentic form. In order to
do this, only one's natural sensibilities are required; to pay attention
to these, however, is an attitude we may have thoroughly forgotten.
It involves an intense quietness, a true listening. One of the most
encouraging applications of sentics is that it provides a means to
sharpen the sensibilities and reawaken forgotten capacities in this
regard.
tinuous experience of the coherence between the sentic state and its
experienced expression. This sensation of coherence is not easily ex-
plained in words. In part, it is associated with an aspect of satis-
faction felt when the essentic form expressed truly corresponds to
the sentic state. We experience a sense of being true to ourselves at
that moment. But another aspect of the experience of coherence lies
in the savoring of the quality itself, both as it is expressed and as the
experience of the state.
Thus, one can have various degrees of awareness of the effect of the
essentic form one is expressing. But how necessary is such
awareness? Can essentic forms be expressed with complete
unawareness of what they are or of the state they are expressing? And
if so, what would result from such unawareness?
Consider, for example, that in writing a poem one may be in a state
where the lines come to awareness one after the other, with a
wholeness that could be impaired by any degree of self-con-
sciousness or a disturbance of the state from which they spring. In
the creative act it is important to allow the essentic form to be born
as the issue of that state—to allow the state to be intensified and
focused through the act of expression—but to avoid contamination
of the state through perception of incongruous forms. In short, we
have a paradigm very similar to that of the generation of sentic states
by the method of repeated E-acton expression.
But in personal interactions awareness of the quality and point of
view of expression can be not only helpful; it can transform the
relationship. In creative interpersonal relationships the self-steering
interplay of perception and production of essentic form is central.
And the processes of dream formation also partake of these
functions in ways we shall discuss in later chapters. Indeed,
production and perception of essentic form are biologic bridges for
the dynamic interchange between fantasy and reality, and in a differ-
ent sense, between art and life.
65
Clynes
68
Sentics
1
Debussy's contribution to the language of Western music was in fact to re-
verse this priority: his eloquence is not achieved directly through essentic
form; in his work, it is merely "quoted" and alluded to. Communication is
not achieved through production and recognition of essentic form but rather
through sounds that directly suggest sensory environments. It is thus both
more direct and more indirect, in different ways, than expressionist music.
This tradition has continued to much electronic music of today.
70
Sentics
1
In considering our specific distinction between the words "empathy" and
"sympathy," we are struck by the difficulty in finding appropriately
corresponding words in other languages. Thus, in French, there is sympathie
and sympathique, but no word at all for "empathy." The German word Ein-
fühlung seems to have given rise through translation to the English usage of
"empathy (according to Webster's Dictionary), while Sympathie is also used
in German.
73
Clynes
75
Clynes
76
Sentics
1
A simple but revealing experiment is to produce an after-image on the
retina by looking at a bright image, and then observe how the afterimage
moves as the eyes are rotated with lids closed. Although the afterimage rep-
resents a particular fixed spot on the retina, it will be seen to move as the eyes
are turned.
77
Clynes
1
Sentic training, including the practice of sentic cycles, provides a new
method for musical training and makes it possible for music students and
musicians to discover individually how to be naturally expressive in music,
while eschewing exaggeration. Developing musical talent and ability
through sentic training is the subject of a book presently in preparation by
the author. If one considers how much time musicians spent practicing
technical aspects and how little, if at all, emotional qualities are generally
practiced (or are even known how to be practiced), it is apparent that a
method that permits systematic emotional practice is long overdue!
79
Clynes
sentic state and its orthoessentic form-and following that form with
greatest faithfulness or inner precision. In addition, the inner pulse
must be there to provide the personal presence.
85
Clynes
86
Sentics
87
Clynes
88
Sentics
music, the intensity and specific effects of separate essentic forms depend on
the context from which they spring. The context of the sentic state needs to be
established. In that established state, specific musical phrases gain added
significance.
91
Clynes
performances, recorded in the same NBC studio with the same or-
chestra (although the personnel of the orchestra varied slightly).
Among the works available, we chose to analyze three
performances of the Brahms Haydn Variations Op. 56B (performed
in 1935, 1938, and 1948) because of the number of variations that
permit convenient comparison of both the total performance time of
the work and of the times of the individual sections, the variations,
within it. We were amazed to discover that the total timing of two of
these performances differed only by o.3 second in 16 minutes 50 sec-
onds (about 1 part in 2,000) although recorded ten years apart (See
Figure 13.) Comparisons of individual variations show most of them
to be within one second of each other, illustrating that the similarity
in over-all timing was not just a coincidence. Major portions of the
1935 performance also coincide with the 1938 performance, further
confirming the stability. Where deviations appear, they are relatively
substantial. The timing data illustrate that if the idea and concept
remained the same there was a great stability, but if the musician
changed his mind about a variation, the change would amount to at
least an order of magnitude greater than the variability. Other
performances show similar results.1
That the idea and concept could remain the same over so many
years—during which time there occurred a major world upheaval
and thirteen years of aging of the conductor, as well as members of
the orchestra—is astonishing. The stability of the idea/concept that
Toscanini's execution demonstrated here is a remarkable proof of
the precision of feelings and idiologs. Such precision is probably
present in all great art, whether it is a line in a Raphael painting or
an eloquent phrase from the cello of Pablo Casals. (See Plates 4-8 in
photo insert.) The source of this precision is the human qualities and
corresponding idiologs and not a stability of repetitiousness due to
rote learning. There can be no question of Toscanini trying to
remember how he did it ten years previously: a good artist always
1
Toscanini indicated his own metronome marks in his score: however, he
was in practice an order of magnitude more accurate than the increments of
the metronome steps!
92
Sentics
93
Clynes
1
In fact, what Beethoven sensed is in accordance with measurements of how
the dynamics of the tone of voice in expressive exclamations such as "ah" or
are produced. These measurements suggest that the expressive character of
the sensed qualities follows the essentic forms when observations are taken
of the dynamic muscular changes that produce them rather than by an-
alyzing the sound alone.
2
Since the information and significance lie in the transient form itself, fre
quency analysis is at a disadvantage, since it must take separate, finite time
segments for analysis.
94
Sentics
cutent initiation of action, the inner pulse, is not, however, the same
as meter, rhythm, or beating time. Two pieces of music with the
same meter or notated rhythm may have entirely different essentic
pulse forms.
In fact, we can measure the inner musical pulse without producing
any sound at all. (See Figures 14-21.) As in the measurement of es-
sentic form, a subject sits in a chair and presses the middle finger of
his right hand (if he is right-handed) on the pressure transducer ex-
pressing his experience of the inner pulse as he thinks the specific
piece of music, without producing any sound. In a way, this is a kind
of conducting in which the movement of the arm is replaced by
transient pressure of the finger. As the musical piece is thought
through (one thinks the music, not about the music) in real time, the
inner pulse is expressed repeatedly. To standardize the measurement
of the form of the pulse we normalize the tempo to sixty per minute,
or one second per pulse (in order to have comparable pulse forms-
the active and resting phases of the inner pulse "telescope" differently
at different speeds). This in fact is not far removed from the real
tempo of the pieces we are investigating. (It turns out that the
deviations in the tempo from that otherwise desired by the sub ject
do not affect the specific nature of the inner pulse.)
A counter is used as a visual synchronizing agent, but no auditory
synchronization is used.1 Fifty pulse shapes (of fifty seconds total
duration) are averaged with a CAT computer for each music piece.
The shapes generated seemed to be related to the personality of the
composer in a highly intimate manner. It was found that with some
care we could reliably observe pulse shapes characteristic of
individual composers, regardless of the particular piece chosen. But
only musicians capable of an intimate understanding of the
composers could produce their characteristic pulse shape in this
way.2
1
An auditory synchronizing signal (as opposed to a visual one) causes one
to tend to synchronize not with the initiation of the downbeat, but with its
lowest point, thus phase-shifting the observed pulse form.
2
In his book Theme and Variations, Yehudi Menuhin speaks of meeting Bela
95
Clynes
Bartók for the first time. "I shall never forget my first meeting with him: it
was in November 1943. . . . I was anxious to play for Bartôk-to receive his
criticism before performing his music in public.
"Though I had had no preconceived idea of his manner or appearance, his
music had already revealed to me his innermost secrets. A composer is
unable to hide anything_ by his music you shall know him.
"Immediately, with the first notes, there burst forth between us, like an
electric current, an intimate bond, which was to remain fast and firm. It was
as if we had known each other for years. I believe that between a composer
and his interpreter there can exist a stronger, more intimate bond, even with
the exchange of words, than between the composer and a friend he may have
known all his life. For the composer reserves the core of his personality, the
essence of his self for his works."
96
Sentics
Figure 14. Essentic form of the inner pulse of slow movements of Bee.
thoven (vertical component). Different movements are compared, as well
as the same movement for different interpreters. The lowest trace is of a
first movement in triple meter whose pulse is considered comparable in an
appropriate time scale (one pulse per bar in this case). The inner pulse
shape continues into periods of rest. Considered as a second-order
dynamic system, it has a damping factor equivalent to approximately 0.2,
indicating about three afterbeats before cessation, and comparatively high
inertia. The high inertia tends to give both an inherent propulsion and a
comparatively late down (negative) peak. There is a prolonged initial acton
preprogramming as compared with the low-inertia pulse of Mozart (see
Figure 15). The relative symmetry of the pulse precludes introduction of
late sexual elements of longing as observed in the second phases of the
inner pulses associated with romantic composers, and gives rise to an im-
pression of "ethical constraint." (Average of fifty pulses.)
97
Clynes
Figure 15. The essentic form of the inner pulse of Mozart shows consid.
erably lower inertia than that of Beethoven. The down peak occurs much
earlier, and there is a small overshoot with damping of about 0.7. The
Mozart pulse has no more than one afterbeat compared with several for
Beethoven. Its relatively light and buoyant character is related to the low-
inertia term coupled with slight underdamping and a response time
corresponding to a normally preprogrammed free acton. In that sense the
Mozart pulse is freer than the Beethoven pulse and we may see how it could
well be associated with such descriptive terms as a "cosmic pulse" as com-
1
A few examples are given in Appendix IV, where certain phrases by Bee
thoven, Mozart, and Chopin are compared to show how essentic form pro-
vides meaning and expression.
98
Sentics
Figure 16. Essentic form of the Schubert pulse illustrated shows a genet.
ally very early down peak, and low inertia, but also a fast rise leading to
overshoot. There is a characteristic upward deflection related to elements
of hope and longing. But this characteristic Schubert rise is very different
from the romantic sexual rise, accompanied by higher inertia and tension.
The Schubert pulse is relatively highly damped. (Lyric Brahms has a high
inertia and low damping.)
has not yet been analyzed, so that one still depends on individuals at
this time to have the "sensitivity" to perceive these implications.
The stability, however, shows that, like red, the personality
Beethoven revealed in his music has a precise existence that con-
tinues. This stability is not a result of "tradition" or of "style." Mozart
and Haydn have very different inner musical pulse shapes! So have
Debussy and Ravel. However, the study of changes in pulse shape
with the history of music is interesting as an indication of the change
in the sentic matrix (the inclusion of sexual longing, of disdain, of
anger, of despair or hope, of enthusiasm, etc. as important elements
99
Clynes
Figure 17. Baroque music as conceived in the customary baroque style has
a pulse which is primarily time beating and not charged with essentic form.
(Though this is not so for Casals, whose interpretation of Bach rather has
aspects of a super Beethoven pulse.) It is interesting that differences in
interpretive approach are so clearly brought out by the measure of essentic
form of the inner pulse.
100
Sentics
101
Clynes
102
Sentics
103
Clynes
Figure 21. Comparison of the inner musical pulse forms of Haydn and
Mozart. These forms were measured for various slow movements (K.491,
467, 570 for Mozart; Sonatas in C Major E Flat Major, and D Major for
Haydn). Note the interesting and consistent differences in in the horizontal
trace also. The sense of awe, wonder, and "natural piety" characteristic of
the Haydn pulse is reflected by the upward striving, rounded top portion of
the pulse compared with the more sober pulse shape of Mozart. Note
particularly that, though the composers share a style, the context of their
individual personal character results in different pulse forms.
104
Sentics
PART TWO
Sentic Cycles, Personal Applications and
Extensions of Sentic Theory
105
Clynes
106
Sentics
107
Clynes
1
* In the study of music, the practice and development of sentic discrim-
ination is clearly different from the development of skills in mastering instru-
mental technique. The monolithic drive to develop technical skills frequently
leads- except in those exceptionally "gifted"-to the neglect of practice of sentic
discrimination and development. Finger exercises have been foisted on mu-
sic students for generations, but no one until now has devised systematic
"'feeling exercises" although it is certainly obvious how much they are
needed. As we shall see, sentic training afforded by sentic cycles ac-
complished this also.
108
Sentics
on the seat, with his feet firmly placed on the floor (shoes may be
removed); legs should not be crossed. The finger rest is positioned on
a coffee table, or on a second, hard-seat chair. The person starts the
tape, puts the third finger on the finger rest, and closes his eyes.
Whenever he hears a click, he expresses with a single expressive
action of pressure the quality of the state called for, as precisely as he
can. States to be generated are announced by the tape: no emotion,
anger, hate, grief, love, sex, joy, and reverence. Each state lasts several
minutes and contains a sequence of timing clicks initiating ex-
pressive actions. The participant cannot predict when the next click
will occur. The person is ready to express the quality called for and
awaits the next click to carry out the expression. One expression for
each click is to be done.
At the end of the cycle (or several such cycles) he sits quietly for a
minute or two before getting up and resuming his activities. He may
not want to talk for some minutes afterward.
1
I even started to look forward to coming to work-to spend hours doing this
and to get paid for it as well!
109
Clynes
tween actions were gradually discovered for each sentic state. These
turned out to be quite different for different states.
As the design of the sequences of intervals was undertaken, every
interval was sculptured in relationship to those before and after.
Every change produced a change in the effect of the experience on
the subject. To try to perfect the sequences was at times like training
a dog to choose between an ellipse and a circle: as the differences are
made smaller and smaller, the dog tends to go insane! It was nec-
essary to sense the differences of each change, but also to compare
the new effect with how it was before the change was made. In spite
of the benefits of sentics, this seemed sometimes enough to drive one
crazy. But, persevering over several years, programs and tapes were
developed for initiating each action and for the sequence of sentic
states that seemed close to optimal. The real payoff, remarkably, was
that the subtleties of timing being nearly optimal for one individual
were also nearly optimal for others. Considering sentic theory, this
is perhaps really not so surprising. In fact if it were not so, music
would not function either. Still, it was gratifying to observe this
result.
110
Sentics
1
"Clyning" is the term introduced by Dr. A. French to describe carrying out
sentic states.
111
Clynes
Figure 22. Variations of heart rate during a sentic cycle. Many subjects
show marked increases in heart rate especially during anger and sex.
he can and may act freely, spontaneously, without worry that it will
have a punitive effect.
Subjects are often afraid to express emotions in real life for various
reasons, but they are willing and even glad-to experience similar
emotions while doing sentic cycles. The fear of losing control which
they experience in a real situation does not seem to play an
inhibiting effect during sentic cycles. 1
The state produced by sentic cycles is very different from hypnosis.
One is very alert and in full control at all times. Physiologic
responses include changes in heart rate and respiration. These vary
systematically with each sentic state, as does oxygen consumption.
(See Figures 22-25.) Huenergardt reported that heart rate increased
on the average from 75 to 115 beats per minute during the experi-
1
Even a musical performance does not have such freedom - an audience is
more or less critical, and there also is self-criticism. But when one plays to
oneself, perhaps a fantasy, without thought of success or failure, listening and
"speaking" at the same time, one comes closest to some aspect of the experi-
ence that sentic cycles, without the need for hours and years of dulling
practice, can give.
112
Sentics
Figure 23. Changes in respiration and heart rate during a sentic cycle.
Respiration accelerates during anger and hate. During grief the respiration
has a gasping character with rest periods at the expiratory end of the cycle.
Respiration slows during love, and speeds up markedly for sex. (In-
spiration is downward in the figure.) During reverence there is a marked
113
Clynes
114
Sentics
Figure 24. In the upper left graph oxygen consumption for four sentic
cycles of the same subject shows consistent change for the various sentic
states. Changes in all four cycles are in the same direction for
corresponding states, except for one step between grief and love in the
fourth cycle. Passionate states tend to show greater oxygen consumption.
Note the relation between oxen consumption and respiration rates
indicated in Figure 23. The lower left graph shows the average oxygen up-
take for the four cycles.
115
Clynes
inspiration. Also, though both grief and love induce slowed res-
piration, they differ in that there is a marked pause after the ex-
piratory phase for grief before the next inspiration.
After sentic cycles, subjects' faces are slightly flushed, their eyes
sparkle, and they comment often on "having had an experience" or
having been "turned on." Even agitated patients are generally able to
express their feelings with this method and at these times their over-
all bodily agitation ceases.
Practice of sentic cycles teaches one discrimination in the ex-
pressive language of touch. As this language becomes known more
precisely, communication with others through touch becomes more
effective and powerful. It becomes easier to know what one is
communicating through touch—as well as in voice inflection and
other modes.
However, depending on the "point of view" as discussed earlier,
the communication of touch forms may occur without a deeply felt
origin or cause for the touch. The social danger of lightheaded touch
communication and manipulation must be pointed out. As
discussed earlier, in a form of touch there may be a full commitment
of the entire individual or merely the production of a known form-
one of human relationship, the other a manipulation or a quasi-
artistic expression. On the other hand, in playful use of essentic
form, pleasant feelings may be enhanced and the communication of
unpleasant feelings can become modified through the pleasure of
the purity of expression, as in music.
For persons who are in a sentic "rut." "sentic cycles improve the
fluidity of their mental state, abolishing sentic rigidity, a symptom of
depression and anxiety. French and Tupin have reported, "Some sub-
jects found immediate and dramatic relief of symptoms of depres-
sion, anxiety, and sleep disturbance after the first forty-five minute
session following only a few minutes of instruction." However, one
does not necessarily have to have been depressed to feel increasingly
alive through the ability to control one's sentic state. We may
perhaps say that the sentic cycle is a form of discipline in which a
116
Sentics
Modifying Aggression
One characteristic effect of sentic cycles is frequently observed—a
special form of contentment. After completion of sentic cycles, one's
face may portray a characteristic "contented smile" that persists for
some time. Immediately afterward one often wishes not to talk but
to allow the experience to "sink in." Yet at those times one does not
feel isolated; on the contrary, there is a sense of sharing an inner ex-
istence one feels is common to men. This feeling communicates it-
self, even in the absence of verbal expression.1
Removal of anxiety or anger is not invariably desirable, in fact. A
subject may feel that the experience can, so to speak, cheat him out
of his maintained rightful anger. A person may not wish to give up
the emotional set deriving from a specific individual situation. In
preventing this, a person can always exercise his choice to use sentic
cycles judiciously. The absence of anxiety and anger may require
1
* The teacher or therapist, too, is often moved to a spontaneous flow of love
toward the subject at these times. (This kind of experience has also been
reported by Dr. French.) Frequently, the condition of the subject immediately
after sentic cycle is so free from hostility and aggression, and yet full of such
a special vitality, that it seems as if a natural flow of love, which is present in
the teacher or therapist but has been previously inhibited by the sentic
messages of the people around him, is allowed to flow toward the subject
without hindrance.
Such an empathic experience is furthered by the knowledge that the subject
has just experienced all the emotions of the cycles. Like a shared musical ex-
perience of the late works of Beethoven, a bond appears between therapist
and subject, even if the therapist or teacher has not participated in the cycle
himself. The two individuals are no longer strangers, but have "shared" a
particular, rather beautiful intimacy, like a sunset witnessed together.
118
Sentics
120
Sentics
121
Clynes
could not find an appropriate feeling. All the time I was pressing the
button very hard as the click sounded. There was warmth going to it,
but not feeling. Perhaps I should say heat, not warmth. After a while,
as I realized that feelings were not going to accompany, I con-
centrated on the effort. I pressed down passionately, and abstract
thoughts began to form. Revulsion of the Vietnam conflict, friends
who had been severely injured, injustices in general. Whatever
hatred I could summon was injury to my beliefs. By now my finger
was pressing very hard, so hard that I found a severe cramp was
forming in the first knuckle, and I wondered if the finger was
breaking or broken. I kept on pressing as the clicks sounded, but my
feeling wasn't with it, really, and I thought it would never end.
On "grief," an entirely new world happened. Sadness enveloped
me quickly. Perhaps because of my overwhelming feeling for injured
friends. I sank down into that. My finger motion must have been
gentler, more caressing, more toward myself but at the same time
reaching out toward the other person first. For a while I had been
hearing faint voices calling and hadn't thought much about it: it
must have been down the street. Voices began calling my name. I
was not sure it was Peter calling my name from some hospital. But at
the same time I saw the vision I'd had of him since then, standing in
a corner of our kitchen, looking as he did before he went away,
smiling, coming forward, calling my name. Seemingly simultane-
ously, I had been hearing my father's voice, calling my name in a very
familiar way; it made me laugh, or at least smile. Then I felt the grief
at his death. Then a hallucination I'd had of him at home, lying in his
coffin in the fireplace as he was at the mortuary, but he turned to me
and said, "Death is O.K.," just turning his head a little. I then pro-
jected myself into my mother's death and felt enormous pain and
guilt. Then visited the funerals of both Charles's parents and felt
similar emotions, but more concerned with what Charles was feel-
ing. As the clicks continued, I tried to enter myself into grief, it was
not difficult for me to do. I can feel sad. Each situation was fairly easy
to get into.
123
Clynes
124
Sentics
125
Clynes
126
Sentics
127
Clynes
129
Clynes
tween a simply sexual experience and one where you are feeling
much love too and how different orgasms are under the two
conditions. Combination produced a total body shudder, like having
the chills, that was surprising and pleasant.
JOY-Light feeling, headiness, smile; image of a new baby. Feeling
of things floating upalmost expansion of inner space in head. Think
of feeling close after sex; and of people I love. Main feeling though
that strikes me is the actual physical sensations of lightness and how
particular they are and how they are different from love, and sex. The
uniqueness of each emotion.
REVERENCE-Think of my grandfather, of a teacher I revere, im-
age of the temple at Miyajima in Japan, built on the water. Very
serious quality to this feeling and somehow more intellectual than
the others, not quite as kinesthetic. Not as expansive-almost con-
straining. Aware of posture and breathing throughout. By second
cycle, tired and resenting having to do it over. Throughout seemed
arbitrary to start a feeling and have to hold it back between clicks.
Second Session (1/15/73)
Finger wanting to persist once an emotion started-seems unnat-
ural to stop and start, finger getting tired and stiff, much tension,
mouth very expressive.
Started exhausted and with severe headache, after felt very relaxed
and pleasantly tired. No desire to do second cycle though. Enjoyed
getting into the awareness of posture and breathing and the
quietness of sitting in dark room and getting into self in this way.
Kind of feeling I often get from writing. Feeling of getting into self
and exploring inner space.
130
Sentics
During second cycle felt flow of emotion from one to next, on first
cycle some overlap of emotions. Anger seemed longer than others,
and hate and reverence were also long. Felt grief most because of re-
cent personal experience. Felt mental picture of each emotion, and
felt apprehension and tenseness drain from body. Voice appeared
softer and speech slower than before session. Found experience
totally absorbing. Every businessman should dyne every day to pre-
vent ulcers. Felt build-up of body heat through face and chest.
131
Clynes
132
Sentics
133
Clynes
Plate 4. The essentic forms of love and sex and their associated
muscle activity are here compared with drawings by Picasso of
Mother and Child, and Pan, respectively. There appears to be an
analogy that may well not be altogether accidental between the
uniquely rounded forms of the Mother and Child drawing (the
enfolding arms) and the measured essentic form of love. An
embrace illustrated visually and its dynamic tactile
representation in essentic form show a resemblance. On the
right, the accentuation and particular angularity of the horns,
arms, and shoulder with strong dark accents and implied thrusts
compare well with the measured essentic form of the sexual
quality with its secondary accents of muscle activity. It would
seem as if the dynamic visual impressions, communicated by a
134
Sentics
135
Clynes
136
Sentics
137
Clynes
138
Sentics
139
Clynes
140
Sentics
141
Clynes
142
Sentics
143
Clynes
144
Sentics
145
Clynes
146
Sentics
147
Clynes
148
Sentics
149
Clynes
feelings, rather than to let them control me, or to, at least, let me live
them.
One of the physical experiences, the tension of my jaw, at the
present time I feel remarkably relaxed, as much as at any time in my
life, though there is the sense of the grief inside and the question as
to what I can do about it, about reversing everything that I under-
stand that makes for this grief. I think I feel calmer than I've ever felt
before in my life.
150
Sentics
long time looking out the living room window into the back yard.
The yard seemed especially beautiful. The only fear I had was that
this feeling would go away. I thought about doing the sentic cycles
and what the real purpose was for doing them. I felt for the first time
I had a glimpse of what was really happening. I felt like my soul had
been "tapped" and wondered how incredible it was to have this feel-
ing. Also wished I could feel like this always. I had visions of people
walking across my yard and me going out to greet them with warmth
and love. I particularly enjoyed the morning playing with the chil-
dren, listening to music, reading to them, which was unusual
because most of the time I feel annoyed by them Several hours after
dong the cycles, I still feel good but not as I felt earlier. It is hard to
describe how I feel now-almost fearful that I won't feel the same
again and a little fearful that I felt so good and peaceful.
In a way, I feel a little silly about my feelings. Also, I am trying to
judge them—whether they were good or bad, right or wrong, and
whether or not they are appropriate feelings for someone like me (at
my age). I started to get the feeling of the tremendous responsibility
of these feelings—normally I don't feel responsible for my feelings or
I am not aware of my responsibility.
Sixth Session (12/8/73 )
ANGER-Not much at first but then started to get angry at helpless
feelings and anxiety. HATE-Both in anger and hate I was caught up
with my feelings of inadequacy, helplessness, and how I hated to feel
that way.
GRIEF-Can't remember much about grief except the general feel-
ing of grief. Felt very sad and almost like crying but I couldn't.
LOVE-I don't remember at all what I felt other than feeling
frustrated during this cycle. I try to concentrate just on the feeling of
love and I can generally get it when I think of the children. I feel
rather empty during this cycle-not that I feel unloved, but I feel like
my own love for others is thwarted. I realize more and more the
difficulty I have in showing, feeling, and expressing my love, but I
151
Clynes
152
Sentics
time was too long was in joy. I wanted to give up, wanted to stop and
cry. I couldn't conjure up any joy. I didn't want to.
It wasn't difficult to express angeL Didn't have any specific per-
son's face there. When hate came in I thought about my mother, but
anger itself (was not associated with) imagery. Just came out.
It's dangerous to express anger. I'll be hit. I was hyperventilating.
HATE-My first feeling hate was one of every part of me going into
some middle part of me, and seething. Almost as though image I had
was of looking at someone. I hate, and every part of me is feeling the
hate, but not expressing it. Almost like an absence of feeling for per-
son. That's what true hate is. Then anger came in. To me it connected
up with a person, my mother. It was just anger afterward.
GRIEF-Right before grief I started feeling very like the anger was
spent. It started waning and I started to feel very sad. When grief
came on I wanted to fight the feeling of grief and fought it the whole
way. I could feel myself trying and I didn't want to try.
LOVE During love ... I just got feeling about people I like, have
feelings of love for, one after another, different people. D., he was just
holding me and stroking me, just being cradled, and I rocked back
and forth. The image of the sun-a lot of warmth- being bathed in
warmth. It went in the direction of abandoning myself to the sun, to
a person, letting it come in. More receiving than giving.
SEX-I was questioning that love was connected with sex. I resisted.
Got in a block. My image was of a man kissing my breasts and caress-
ing me. Then I got the image of a penis coming into me, and I
resisted, and then there was a whole image of being forced against
my will in a sexual situation of a man trying to make love to me. I
think I was trying to get away from my own sexual feeling. I might
really get turned on by rape fantasy. Resisted it. Then I started feeling
very sad.
When I heard the word joy, I thought, My God, there is no joy. I
started thinking of ice cream. Picture of me and my mother I'm
holding on to ice cream cone. Made me very sad that no joy
connected with sex. Felt very barren I was starting to cry. At one
153
Clynes
point I tried to get joy through crying, but wasn't feeling it partic-
ularly REVERENCE-My first image was of a church and God and
altar. Gothic image of everything being much much bigger than I
was.
Then of infinite space-coming down and enveloping me. How
peaceful death must be. Then image of drowning in water and very
warm liquid, just as though death was going to rescue me Then I felt
very very calm.
Second Cycle (Done separately, on same day)
Feel really good. It was a different experience, this time, totally.
Much more loving and warm. It was just a transition of feelings that
carried along. Last time I tried to compartmentalize. This time I just
stayed and went along, and experienced an organic kind of tran-
sition from one feeling state to another.
NO EMOTION-Just thought of something like "Star Trek."
Someplace way in future where I was sitting in a big chair and
looking all around me passively. My fantasy was that I had been
programmed not to feel any emotion and I was playing that part.
ANGER-I thought of it as my mother trying to slap me or hurt me
in some way. It got to be a real struggle. At one point I felt I couldn't
get away from being hurt, but I tried to assert myself. Click, she was
trying to hurt me and I reacted against that.
My anger was to stave off an attack, to scare person away with my
anger. That session seemed very long to me.
HATE-_I didn't want to hate. I pictured my mother and I pictured
R. I didn't want to hate them because I felt it was a loss on my part,
in some way I was the loser. I went into the imagery of when I was
with R. the last time and I had been aggressive sexually and he had
not responded. I felt I had made a total fool of myself.
Extreme anger and hatred for being made a fool of.
Tremendous effort, in the beginning, to feel hate. Element of my
hatred not having an effect, a feeling of helplessness.
154
Sentics
155
Clynes
Many of these reports are of the very first sentic cycle session ever
done by the subject, following only a few minutes' instruction. The
depth of experience and satisfactions made possible by the simple,
vet profoundly affecting meditative rite of sentic cycles continues to
astonish users well after their initial experience. With their finger
rests and sentic cycle tape they can do sentic cycles at home, when-
ever they feel the need, whenever they wish to affirm their sense of
belonging. For many it is an opening of a new and better world, a
door to security for which they have received the key.
1
We consider some aspects of this characteristic experience a "virtual body
image" and describe the phenomenon in the following chapter 19
156
Sentics
157
Clynes
158
Sentics
159
Clynes
160
Sentics
around her. To quite an extent, Mrs. C.N. became aware that al-
though her body was not functioning, her mind and her person were
sound; her disease could not really touch that part of her that was
most herself.1
1
In some ways it may have helped her to discover these aspects of her person
more than if she had been healthy. She had the advantage of being able to
stick to a schedule that allowed for a daily sentic cycle experience. Healthy
individuals seldom would be willing to submit themselves to such a stringent
schedule
161
Clynes
beauty of storm, or the joy of color to the majesty of the starry night?
Each has its time and place, and, outside of this, its effective quality
changes too. Each has its time and place also in the context of man's
life. The very concept of quality gives pleasures an order that
depends on other dimensions than that of intensity. What are these
dimensionalities?
Among the many words and concepts of natural language giving
rise to confusion in regard to natural brain processes, few are more
enigmatic and unresearched than the notion of "satisfaction." The
concepts of beauty, goodness, and justice have given rise to whole
fields of inquiry, but the concept of satisfaction has not received
similar attention from philosophers. Perhaps for this reason, the
unclarified concept is acting as a rampant weed in our culture. Psy-
chologists, as well as Freud and his followers, have not given it a large
measure of consideration, nor have the physiologists. Perhaps the
worst thing that one can say about a concept is that everybody
assumes he knows what it means. The study of sentics and of the
effect of sentic cycles, however, may help clarify some of the dynamic
principles of satisfaction.
The word itself has the Latin roots satis and facere, "to make sated."
If we inquire further as to the meaning of "sated" the dictionary
refers us to "the 'satisfaction' of hunger or of desire," or "to quieten"-a
circuitous route! But if we consider examples of the experience of
satisfaction, we notice that when hunger is satisfied there exists a
feeling that is not merely the absence of hunger, but a distinct feeling
of its own. And when we complete defecation there exists a feeling
that is not merely the absence of the urge to defecate but also has a
quality of its own. Indeed, after we have emptied the bladder,
sneezed, or scratched in response to an itch, there is in each case a
feeling of satisfaction that has a quality of its own and lasts for a
period of time, fading into nothing before a new build-up phase be-
gins. At the end of orgasm, too, there are a number of sensations that
have a quality of their own.
"To make quiet" is an insufficient description of what these
processes have in common. They do share some dynamic properties
163
Clynes
of what are called relaxation "oscillators," and the fact that such dis-
tinctive qualities of sensation of "satisfaction" occur at the end of the
discharge period. That these phenomena do occur at those times in
the cycle implies a bimodal neuronal organization, since the "satis-
faction" sensation is not coincidental with the discharge phase but
follows it.
On the other hand, there is another type of satisfaction. Many sen-
sations and qualities can give satisfaction without a build-up period
and discharge phase-for example, lying in the sun or savoring the
greenness of grass. The sight of the ocean or of a snowy mountain
peak or of the starry skies—not replaceable by looking at re-
productions—these qualities appear to be integral functions of our
nervous system programming that require the dimension of space
itself.
In earlier chapters we have talked about the satisfaction of express-
ing essentic form. If we now consider the organization of them into
sentic cycles, we observe that the specific new satisfactions provided
by sentic cycles indicate the presence of elements of both types. The
experience and expression of emotion provide one aspect. Its own
quality of peace, if it affords it, is itself a satisfaction, lasting for some
time. And the memory of that peace is another kind of satisfaction,
that tells one that it is there "for the asking."
165
Clynes
1
Expressing an emotion is a form of giving. the concept of "giving" and its
ramifications in sentic theory as related to satisfaction are very significant.
A kiss, for example, has potentially a "giving component that transcends the
mutual sensory excitation involved. Sentic "giving" is also contained in a
blessing and in a curse. "Giving" in relation to sentic experience shall be
treated more fully in another book.
166
Sentics
whose view leaves the initiative to the other fellow, but we may feel
strongly that, in a different context, they are quite wrong.
Perhaps if Freud really heard Beethoven's last works, for example,
he would have valued the mutual reality of generalized emotions
(and the function of pre-sentic control) as dimensions additional to
the personal and social realms of which he was a master. He could
not readily tap that intrinsic source of joy, belonging, and happiness,
nor could those millions who followed him exclusively. If he could
have done so, the concepts of being "worthy of love" and "love val
wed as a privilege would have seemed meaningless to him in that
context. Sentic studies demonstrate how human beings, regardless of
race, sex, and, to a large extent, age, can experience sentic states and
their relationship as a part of human heritage.
Sentic Afterimages
We regard sentic states as a special class of qualities. One of the
known characteristics of sensory qualities is that they have afterim-
ages. (For example, after looking at a bright light, we can see an im-
age of it if we close our eyes.) We might expect, therefore, that the
experienced qualities of sentic states could also display analogous
phenomena—that there should be sentic afterimages, or, to use a
seemingly more appropriate term, sentic "after-experience" as dis-
tinguished from satisfaction as such. (The term "afterimage" how-
ever does refer to all forms of sensory stimulation, not just the
visual.) Afterimages must not be confused with the memory of the
experience itself. Some forms of afterimages appear to persist in the
same quality as the original stimulus; other afterimages sometimes
indicate the opposite quality of the originating stimulus. (Thus, the
afterimage of a bright light is a bright image, but the afterimage of
purple is green, and vice versa.) During afterimages, we are generally
less sensitive to the same stimulus—an aspect of refractoriness.1
(For example, a loud sound raises the threshold of hearing.) Some
sensory modalities such as temperature sensors include after-sensa-
tions that may not be easy to separate from the other bodily sensa-
tions that accompany them (e.g., the glow felt after a warm bath).
The nature of sentic afterimages is not yet fully understood. It is
clear, however, that the order of sentic states experienced in sentic
cycles has a considerable effect on the specific experience of each
state. For example, the relationship between Grief and Love is such
that Love is facilitated after Grief. In the reverse order, however, the
specific experience of Grief is also affected—but not facilitated-by
the immediately preceding state of Love. Each state appears to cast
its shadow on the following ones in a way that needs more study.
Hormonal and other bodily changes accompanying each specific
state produce a distinct cumulative patter for every sentic state
sequence. This happens because hormones secreted into the blood-
1
A refractory period is that period of insensitivity to a stimulus that follows
excitation.
169
Clynes
171
Clynes
173
Clynes
Figure 26. Average changes in electric brain potentials during the expres-
sion of love and also during the expression of no emotion. form of love is
reflected in the top trace, representing the left frontal lead, while other oc-
cipital and frontal leads do not show this form. Early activity especially no-
table in the occipital leads includes auditory response to the click. Note that
the form bears a resemblance to the essentic form of love as measured
through the finger pressure transducer. The response on the right is shown
for comparison, as a control, with the absence of the love-modulated E-
acton. (Leads are left frontal, left occipital, right occipital, and right
frontal; all leads are referred to a common vertex lead. Average of one
hundred actons.)
174
Sentics
175
Clynes
Figure 28. Very large slow waves observed in the second trace (left occip-
ital lead) during the phase of reverence. Such slow waves are not normally
encountered in the observed electric behavior recorded from the scalp.
These remarkable waves persisted for over one minute, and have a period
of 4-5.5 seconds per cycle and an amplitude of over 100 microvolts. The
pulse on the fourth trace indicates the soft clicks initiating the E-actons.
(Leads from top to bottom: left occipital, right occipital, left frontal, right
frontal.)
Figure 29. Selective presence of large slow waves on the right occipital lead
primarily (third trace from the top) during the love phase.
176
Sentics
Figure 30. Shift bursts as shown here are often observed during anger, in
single leads and sometimes in two or more leads at a time. These shifts in
the base DC level are of considerable magnitude and persist for some time
after the burst associated with it. (The second trace of the top example
demonstrates such a shift most dramatically.) This type of behavior is ob-
served quite frequently and sometimes at a regular periodicity. 'The bursts
do not seem to be phase-related to the individual actons. (Leads as in
Figure 29.)
177
Clynes
178
Sentics
179
Clynes
181
Clynes
182
Sentics
183
Clynes
184
Sentics
185
Clynes
example, but his sentogram for "wife" would show not the irritation
but an expression of the relationship at a deeper level. These
sentograms acquire special significance as we relate them to the es-
sentic forms for specific emotions. Such a comparison often is re-
vealing and significant in a way that verbal expression of feelings
cannot be.
The figure shown (see Figure 32) comprises the collection of
sentographic forms of the PRP for one subject. The sentogram can
be compared with the established essentic forms for specific states
previously measured. For example, the sentogram for father may
closely resemble the essentic form for love as in the figure; and, the
essentic forms of anger, hate, or sex may be observed in particular
sentograms produced by the subject as an expression of his relation-
ship with specific individuals.
Other stable sentograms may clearly differ from any of the estab-
lished essentic forms. These are especially interesting in a different
way. They may express "mixed" states, i.e., combinations of basic
states. Or, they could represent basic states we have not yet inves-
tigated.
187
Clynes
188
Sentics
189
Clynes
190
Sentics
PART THREE
Sentics, the Individual, and Society
191
Clynes
A surface of grace
Bounds fluid motion
Myriad nerve-channels, like parachute chords
Connect surface movement to the moving thought-
Essentic form is born
192
Sentics
194
Sentics
195
Clynes
this. Too much exposure may cause too great a dissipation of the in-
centive to act.)
197
Clynes
198
Sentics
stress, we may lose appetite and starve ourselves, or, under re pressed
emotion, overeat.
It is interesting and remarkable that during sentic cycles hunger is
sharply attenuated or disappears (this also happens during sleep).
The fantasy experience provided by sentic states appears to fill the
sentic domain, and displaces the sensation of hunger.
If we love the other person, and there is desire to merge with the
other individual, then how we integrate that image (or idiolog of the
person) with the sexual impulse is very important. The inability to
integrate these may become a source of sexual problems.1 In fact, ex-
perience with experimenting with the order of the phases of sentic
cycles shows that the phase of sex when coupled between love and
joy has an entirely different function, than when it is positioned be
tween other sentic states. The kind of satisfaction obtained from it
may vary greatly according to the sentic states associated with it (an
example of the "loose coupling" of qualities referred to in Chapter
Nine). The sexual drive shows itself to include more than so-called
"physiologic gratification." It, too, comprises virtual body images
and cognitive aspects (in the sense of Chapter Eleven. Combining or
divorcing ardor, joy, and love in relation to sexual experience, we al-
ter its dynamic function both as to unconscious and conscious expe-
rience.
Orgasm—as well as a tickle or a yawn-includes a wide range of ex-
perience (which should have better verbal description) that includes
the image of relationship. Not only does the intensity of the experi-
ence vary greatly with these relationship images, but certainly also
the qualities and consequent satisfactions.
The sentic function of the relationship image is definite and real,
but adequate words do not exist yet to describe its range and depth.
This makes it most difficult for us to be clear about the sentic
meaning of relationship. (Words such as "brother" mix up relation-
ship concepts with genealogy.) In terms of sexual experiencing, the
sentic meaning and function of relationship cannot be constrained
to fit the established word categories such as "lover," "mother," "fa-
ther," "friend," and so on. The extent to which the person or person
idiolog encompasses a range of conscious and unconscious
possibilities—as potentialities of relationship—controls the degree
of satisfaction and quality. Although we have no words to denote
these potentialities we can clearly sense their sentic function. Dis-
1
The effect on the dynamics of the unconscious processes is very different,
depending on the idiolog consciously experienced.
200
Sentics
201
Clynes
neous enjoyment that the experience has to offer. Anxiety, guilt, fears
of impotence and of homosexual fantasies, phobic feelings, and
anger – these are examples of some of the processes that may occur
against the will of the individual. Sentic cycles can be used to redi-
rect these fantasy processes by offering new associations, and by pro-
viding enjoyable experience of the emotional spectrum.
This method, acting in part somewhat like Wolpe's desensitization
method (a technique of behavior modification which works by re-
peatedly and gradually associating the quality of an unpleasant expe-
rience with pleasant contexts), consists of deliberately and briefly
bringing to the subject's mind the negative associations when he is
established in a positive generalized sentic state—for example, love
or joy. In this way, habitual fears of sexual inadequacies may also be
replaced by repeatedly establishing positive associations during the
sex phase of sentic cycles. As the specific stimulus giving rise to fear
is thought of in a positive sentic state, the fear-provoking stimulus
becomes less potent after sufficient repeated association, and
assumes some of the character of the state in which it is presented.
In this way, it can be possible to abolish the fear of snakes, for in-
stance, and perhaps even induce a love of them. One should be care-
ful, however, that the positive state is well established first, before the
negative fantasy is presented. (Otherwise the subject might end up
with a fear of love, instead of a love of snakes! In practice this is not
difficult to avoid.
Not surprisingly, the reduction of non-specific anxiety produced
by sentic cycles, can be helpful in problems involving impotence and
frigidity. (Where possible, it is desirable that both members of a cou-
ple partake in sentic training.)
202
Sentics
203
Clynes
The hostile behavior that one observes arising from the frustration
of drives can be clearly differentiated in animals, as well as in
humans, from the constructive forms of aggressiveness. The latter
involves not attacking another individual, but rather a problem, and
energies are spent not fighting others but on the solution of a prob-
lem or the execution of a plan.
On the whole we may admit with Freud that in humans, creativity
is more like excretion than like ingestion. But in creativity there is
also an element of ingestion. Ideas and expressions are received.1
The concepts of aggressiveness and of psychic energy overlap, like
many other concepts of natural language. The problem is to under-
stand distinctions between emotions, drives and their relation to
psychic energy. But the concept of psychic energy itself is diffuse. Do
we equate the energy of anger to that of joyous excitement? For
many, anxiety is the driving force behind aggressive action and psy-
chic energy. In more disturbed individuals there is no longer an
awareness of the goal which would remove anxiety; that is to say,
anxiety becomes non-specific. Eventually such anxiety may no
longer produce activity. A drive based on the urge to remove anxiety
is also a drive to remove the blocking which anxiety produces. There
comes a point, however, when an individual no longer recognizes
the blocks and restrictions, and is ready to forget that he indeed does
have a drive which is being so blocked. The person increasingly re-
presses both the manifestations of the drive, as well as its blocking,
until gradually he becomes resigned, gives up, and becomes one of
Eliot's "hollow" men.
A crucial difference between energy based on anxiety and creative
energy seems to be that in the latter instead of trying to get some-
1
At times, one experiences the dynamics of being a mere "vessel" for re-
ceiving ideas and forms that appear to come to one. The processes of being
obsessed by a drive seeking to purge one, or to excrete, or literally "express,"
is joined by another, conjugate experience of being a vessel for receiving that
which comes. This requires not suction, but a clarity of emptiness, like a
clearly focused tele scope erected onto the black sky, open to receive the light
of an unsought star.
204
Sentics
205
Clynes
1
The combination of the experience of calm or quietness with
increased psychic energy, an apparent paradox, is paralleled by the
experience associated with meditation. In fact, sentic cycles may be
regarded as a unique "active-quiet" form of meditation, which
includes considered experience of emotion.
206
Sentics
207
Clynes
208
Sentics
form, we may prefer to cultivate a set of sentic states that are ac-
cepting, inclusive, incorporating, and that engender energy without
destructiveness or hostility. An ethical educational approach derived
from the biologic basis of sentics could lead us to educate the young
at an early age in the elements of sentics, teaching them in practical
terms how every individual has the ability to experience and
communicate these states. Their relationships could gradually be
transformed to include many individuals who previously might have
been rejected and disliked. Through an enlightened cultivation of es-
sentic forms for communication, and the enjoyment of sentic
practice, many of them would no longer be quite as "ignorant of
what they are most assured." Through sentic education we may hope
for a greater sense of brotherhood, and in turn for less deprivation
and isolation. The practice of sentic cycles tends to diminish prej-
udice by increasing our range of empathy. By discarding the
timeworn notion that emotional qualities are vague and only matters
of personal opinion, we make room for the natural roots of
emotional communication that bind people together.
Our dislike of individuals is often based on our dislike of their
sentie habits, which have developed in them as a result of their edu-
cation and experience. (Some of these are "defense mechanisms" in
Freudian thought.) An object of sentic training is to minimize the
need for rigidified defense mechanisms, and to develop a sense of
openness and trust. We shall always have much sorrow and pain, and
also injustice; but the former could be bore better with sentic train-
ing, and constructive psychic energy could be increasingly provided
to reduce the latter. We have seen in the past how hate breeds hate,
contempt breeds contempt, and anger breeds anger. Allowing the ex-
pression of these in non-destructive sentic cycles, we may free our-
selves from their chain of necessity.
210
Sentics
1
This is another instance of the cognitive aspects of qualities of emotions.
211
Clynes
1
The physiological phenomenon of adaptation based on rate sensitivity
holds also for sentic experience. The range of adaptation time constants in-
volved needs to be further experimentally determined and certainly will vary
for different sentic aspects, over a range of hours, days, to months.
213
Clynes
1
While each individual may be different, those differences are more like the
"spice than the "meat one relates primarily to their essential humanity and
secondarily to their specific differences.
214
Sentics
215
Clynes
loss and be a compensation for the loss, but would not allow the
individual literally to forget the loss of the first entity. A loss followed
by a gain, even of the identical entity, does not cancel to no event
(this is an application of unidirectional rate sensitivity and rein con-
trol to living systems). The situation is different from that of a mole-
cule in which one atom has been replaced by another, equivalent
atom. The state of such a molecule in no way reflects that a sub-
stitution has taken place.
The knowledge of twoness acts as a branch point that provides for the
separate images in the data processing scheme of the nervous system.
Without that knowledge, the processes of empathy could not
function adequately: the result is that there are two elogized images,
not one. This constitutes a sentic ethical principle of nonequivalence.
We may state it formally thus:
Individuals are elogized as unique entities regardless of how
similar they may be to one another. They cannot be substituted
and are empathy non-equivalent, ie., ergodic substitution is
impossible.
The process of elogizing individuals results in a one-to-one
correspondence between an individual and his clogized existence in
the person with whom he has a relationship. Accordingly a sub-
stituted individual is a contradiction in itself. Love permits no sub-
stitution. This is in fact so, no matter how closely similar the two
individuals may be. The similarity may make it quite difficult to form
coherent distinct images but this is an unessential difficulty, giving
rise to some confusion at worst.
Generalized love and love for a particular individual differ in that
for the latter a specific individual is elogized and forms a living im-
age in the person who loves. Such an "elogy" of an individual, like a
thought, cannot be "unthought." It (and its recall) can only fade -for
good or bad, as it may be!
216
Sentics
217
Clynes
218
Sentics
220
Sentics
223
Clynes
224
Sentics
1
This view can lead to attempts at synthesis such as the book by E. O. Wil-
son, Sociobiology, in which the qualities of emotion and experience appear
as arbitrary byproducts of the DNA's program of producing more DNA (Wil-
son "an egg's way of making more eggs"); paradoxically, in this "synthesis"
the elements of experience are not considered fundamental natural entities.
225
Clynes
that the hydrogen atom does not arise from nothing). This would
mean that a series of entities potentially pre-exist waiting to be
realized through chance interaction. Clearly the qualities so far
realized in our own development would represent only a fraction,
and perhaps a very small fraction of entities existing in potentiality.
A chance process of evolution would determine whether one or an-
other preexisting quality would become realized first. The order in
which qualities come into existence would be subject to chance
(though it may be that certain qualities have to exist first before oth-
ers can come into being), as well as the time at which they come into
existence. But the number and possible nature of the potential
entities would be a predetermined function of natural design,
implicit in the genetic code.
There is growing evidence of various kinds for this second al-
ternative. Studies of enzyme structure have discovered that enzymes
which have evolved separately, within totally different organic
functional systems, nevertheless turn out to have similar structures
– evidence for the "pinball machine" theory. Further, similar specific,
highly ingenious solutions to environmental problems in totally
different parts of the world are found in both the plant and animal
world.1 Language, too, may have arisen independently among sepa-
rate human groups. Thus, on various levels we see nature displaying
"preferred" solutions. We may try to account for these through
purely statistical considerations, but there is growing difficulty to
avoid concluding that some design choice is implied in the relative
stabilities of some of the DNA molecule configurations in the ge-
netic apparatus. We shy away from the concept of "design" if we
regard it anthropomorphically, as intelligence having to make
certain new choices. But we are well satisfied that there is design in
the universe with regard to atoms and molecules; physicists admire
the "elegant design" implicit in natural law—that in fact appears to
be a source of its beauty, as well as of the awe and wonder which
1
For example, certain algae plants have adapted themselves to live at geyser
temperatures of 170 degrees F. by similar "unique" means in Yellowstone
Park and at Russian geyser locations.
227
Clynes
229
Clynes
230
Sentics
1
Some of the concepts that form the basis of this book presented themselves
to the author in such a way and in dreams, and he was forced to follow their
consequences whether he wanted to or not. In the state of being apreene,
ideas may present themselves with "demonic" power--the result of an agency
outside the willful direction of the individual.
2
We should be very careful to differentiate between the apreene state which
is focused on the receiving of new ideas, and the state of empathic involve-
ment that encompasses the drama of personal existence and its emotions.
Being recep tive to ideas is not the same as being sensitive or "listening" to
people.
231
Clynes
232
Sentics
1
There have been devices available from time to time that produce sounds
of laughter. These can make one laugh, in sympathetic resonance, without
other "reason."
233
Clynes
235
Clynes
237
Clynes
expression for new sentic states beyond the range of our present
potential for experience.
238
Sentics
Epilogue
As I write this, I seem to hear a kind of cosmic laughter, laughter
at our feeble efforts to understand-laughter provoked by the
incompatible idiologs of our efforts and the enormity of the task -the
seemingly deliberate need for comprehension beyond human
capacity inherently required.
Beyond this, I feel the need to express gratitude, a sense of respon-
sibility, and awe for having been allowed to explore a little of that ex-
panding island of the unknown in the ocean of the unknowable – an
island that is expanding more and more as the answer to every good
question raises several others in chain reaction.
Every such question partakes of both fantasy and reality, as we
have seen. And as we endeavor through sentics to see more reality in
fantasy, and more fantasy in reality, we may also progress in clar-
ifying thought-as we become more aware of the dynamic anatomy of
emotion in ourselves, and in the world around us.
Man, the breeder of questions, may he find his Blessing-answer to
no question.
239
Clynes
and he said
with a smile,
"The Kingdom of heaven is also
Where fantasy and reality meet."
240
Sentics
APPENDICES
I. Biologic Design Principles For Sentic Communication
II. A Glossary of Sentic Terms
III. Mathematical Appendix
IV. Music Appendix
V. References and Selected Bibliography
241
Clynes
243
Clynes
244
Sentics
245
Clynes
247
Clynes
248
Sentics
249
Clynes
250
Sentics
251
Clynes
252
Sentics
253
Clynes
EXAMPLE I
Bach, Prelude in F minor, Bk. II, Wohltemperierte Klavier.
This example has characteristic two-note phrases often used by
Bach (as well as by Mozart and other composers in their own way).
Such a two-note phrase (in slow tempo) lends itself to a particular
kind of ambiguity. It can be related to either the essentic form of grief
or of love. As we have seen, these two forms have a degree of similar-
ity, although they are quite distinct. One may suggest a sigh, the
other a caress. Bach often uses such phrases in context of grief and
sadness, as in the St. Matthew Passion, where the text makes the
intent clear. However, the phrase may also be interpreted as a love
acton. In fact, a proper combination of both modes, successively,
may often be most meaningful.
In the sadness or grief interpretation, the grief acton starts on the
first beat of the bar. The upbeat coincides with the "pick up phase"
after the collapse (see Figure 37) which is characteristic of the grief
acton. Consequently in such an interpretation the upbeat note is
played in a slightly detached way (portamento). The last note of the
1
A subject, the pianist Steven Manes, commented on how he experienced
this: "It feels just like playing.
254
Sentics
Figure 37
Figure 38
Figure 39
255
Clynes
This interpretation lends itself here also in that the bass notes
during the rest can be considered as coinciding with the preparatory
upbeat for the love acton (inspiration). The breathing pattern
implied here is more conjointed than in the grief mode (see Figure
40).
In each case expiration occurs on the first beat of the bar but in the
grief interpretation the breathing rests at the end of expiration and is
resumed with a kind of gasp, to be expelled again as a sigh. In the
love mode, the breath alternates smoothly between expiration and
inspiration. (For breathing patterns associated with love and grief
see Chapter Nine.) Thus in either case the particular E-actons act as
a kind of template in which the musical parameters find their place.
The detailed shape of the essentic form is inwardly implied by mu-
sical material but not completely filled. The imagination completes
the form. Thus we tend to hear subtleties of inner forms, not all
actually physically realized in the sound, but rather outlined. This is
so even for realizations on bowed stringed instruments or the voice.
With continuous sounding instruments, there may even be greater
danger that the musician will provide actual sounds that conflict
with the appropriate essentic form—for the listener such contrary
produced sounds are difficult to think away.
In the example shown (see Plate 11 in insert) a particular
interpretive solution there are grief actons for the first two phrases,
the third one passing into more intense anguish, and the fourth one
love. This progression is accorded appropriate meaning by the
harmonic progression, and especially the B fat/B natural/C progres-
256
Sentics
sion of the bass, the pull and harmonic resolution of which reinforce
and guide the change in affect.
The nature of the musical meaning may be compared with the
feeling tone of the Pietà (see Plate 5 in insert) on a much less grand
scale.
EXAMPLE II
Chopin, Ballade No. 3 A Flat, Opus 47.
The main theme of this composition, given in the first four bars,
has a unique magic of its own. (The ballade is composed after a poem
"Ondine," the story of a fisherman enticed by a mermaid.) The first
half of this theme can be felt to embody longing, yeaming, and ec-
stasy. It begins with a sense of being attracted, a pull, expanding, and
at the second bar, there is a continuing sense of floating upward after
the C is struck. This apparently limitless "being carried upward" is
accompanied by a sense of ecstasy, lasting for a moment, till we are
gently carried back to the ground by the following F-E flat phrase.
The inner form of this feeling and virtual body image may be il
lustrated graphically as follows (see Figure 41). There are several out-
standing points to be made in connection with the expression of
this.
First, this feeling is produced only if there is slight acceleration in
the second half of the bar, and a corresponding lengthening of the
first part of the second bar (rubato). The acceleration relates to the
feeling of being pulled, the attraction or surrender, the giving of
one's self. The first part of the second bar is lengthened in turn and
gives us that one extra moment (0.2 second) experienced as floating
257
Clynes
258
Sentics
EXAMPLE III
Mozart, Excerpt from Sonata in C Minor, K. 457,
Second Mvt. (Bars 24-25), Adagio.
This theme of Mozart is note for note the same for the first five or
six seconds as the theme of the slow movement of Beethoven's
Pathétique Sonata. Op. 13, shown in Example IV. It is also in the
same key and uses similar harmony. Yet the inner pulse is quite
different. (If one were to substitute one for the other, one would nec-
essarily stumble when conducting because of the different pulse
forms and points of view implied; see pages 77-81).
The theme, repeated in the second bar, can be experienced to have
a character akin to hope. The second part of the essentic form trace
corresponds to a gentle, benign, Mozartean angelic smile which be-
gins at the end of the "hope" phase. Note particularly that the hope
form continues past the beginning of the third note of the melody-
there is in fact a crescendo in intensity implied during the first half
of the note-which of course the piano cannot provide. A bowed
string instrument (or a singer) could supply what the "ear" demands
here. Such a crescendo in intensity does not particularly mean an
increase in total volume. Its tension can be expressed by a change in
timbre, a greater and specific emphasis on the higher overtones as
required in this example. Such refinements of tone production
knowingly carried to the furthermost limits mark the power of the
art of a Casals.
259
Clynes
EXAMPLE IV
Beethoven, Sonata Op. 13 (Pathétique), Slow Movement.
This opening theme is note for note the same for the first two bars
as the Mozart theme in the previous example. The pulse forms are
different, however.
The world of this theme is noble, serene, and exalted. We find in it
fulfillment, satisfaction, and at the end, especially, love and grati-
tude. The qualities of this theme on the whole are exalted so that it is
difficult to find corresponding words to denote their character.
Beethoven seems to be stretching the very fabric of our experience
to let an unearthly, yet immediate light illumine and touch us. It
would not be far wrong to say it has a holy quality.
The essentic forms are in part correspondingly unfamiliar, except
where they suggest, as at the beginning, a particular kind of rever-
ence, and at the end, love. Two different sentographic performances
by the same artist are compared here. (See Plates 14 and 15 in insert;
260
Sentics
numbers on plates refer to the bars of the score.) The bottom trace is
recorded from Sentograph I (right hand). The upper two traces are
recorded from Sentograph II (left hand), representing essentic
forms. (The horizontal trace contains a slight muscular tremor,
invisible in the actual expression.)
EXAMPLE V
Schubert F minor Fantasy for Piano, 4 Hands
This example is chosen to illustrate transition from sadness to
hope (bars 13-24).
The phrase of bar a3 illustrates how Schubert, by tying the three
notes together (and by the harmonic content), implies the essentic
form of hope. However, it is important to realize that it ends with the
third tied note. (The essentic form of hope has a duration of about
3.3. seconds.) The following dotted rhythm is light, Schubertian, not
a heavier march as if it were Beethoven. It is a good example of how
the Schubertian point of view, implicit in the inner pulse form, con-
strains the particular phrasing. Through it, we are momentarily
transported in thought to the future and view the fulfillment of hope
with a particular joy. Variations of this juxtaposition of character—
261
Clynes
262
Sentics
263
Clynes
264
Sentics
265
Clynes
266
Sentics
267
Clynes
268
Sentics
269
Clynes
270
Sentics
271
Clynes
272
Sentics
273
Clynes
274
Sentics
275
Clynes
276
Sentics
277
Clynes
Sentic Cycles
A. STANDARD SENTIC CYCLE: set containing Sentic Cycle cas-
sette audiotape with the sequence No Emotion, Anger, Hate, Grief,
Love, Sex, Joy, Reverence (27 min.); a Finger Rest; and an illustrated
Instruction Booklet.
B. ADVANCED SENTIC CYCLE: containing an Advance I Sentic
Cycle audiotape, with only positive emotions, No Emotion, Love,
Reverence, Bliss, Compassion, and Give Blessing (32 min.) with an
illustrated Instruction Booklet (no finger rest required).
C. VIDEO OF ADVANCED SENTIC CYCLE: As B, but with this
you can do the Advanced Cycle together with Dr. Manfred Clynes,
and by yourself; contains visual information especially helpful for
the Advanced Cycle (VHS, 70 min.).
Live Performances by Manfred Clynes Cassette audiotapes of
concerts by Manfred Clynes, recorded live, piano, 90 minutes each.
D. J.S. BACH Goldberg Variations E. BEETHOVEN Diabelli Vari-
ations Op. 120, and MOZART Adagio in B minor, and Sonata in B
flat, K570.
These recordings have been regarded by some experts as arguably
unsurpassed performances of these works.
To order:
A B C D or E
United States § 32.00 28.50 49.50 12.00
Australia SA 32.50 28.50 49.50 12.00
Great Britain § 17.50 15.00 27.50 7.00
Please send cheque or money order to:
American Sentic Association, Box 2716, La Jofla, California,
92038 Australian Sentic Association, c/o Unity Press, 6a Ortona
Road, Lindfield, N.S.W. 2070 European Sentic Association, c/o
Prism Press, 2 South Street, Bridport, Dorset, DT6 3NQ, England.
You are cordially invited to become a Member of the American,
European or Australian Sentic Association. Annual Membership fee
278
Sentics
is $25.00. Students and Senior Citizens, $10.00 (£14.00 and £6.00 for
the European Association).
279
Clynes
280