Piaget's theory of cognitive development
Piaget's theory of cognitive development
• table 7.1 the substages and intellectual accomplishments of the sensorimotor period
substage description
1. Reflex activity (birth to 1 month) Active exercise and refinement of inborn reflexes (e.g., accommodate sucking to fit the
shapes of different objects)
2. Primary circular reactions (1–4 months) Repetition of interesting acts centered on the child’s own body (e.g., repeatedly suck a
thumb, kick legs, or blow bubbles)
3. Secondary circular reactions (4–8 months) Repetition of interesting acts on objects (e.g., repeatedly shake a rattle to make an interest-
ing noise or bat a mobile to make it wiggle)
4. Coordination of secondary schemes Combination of actions to solve simple problems (e.g., bat aside a barrier to grasp an object,
(8–12 months) using the scheme as a means to an end); first evidence of intentionality
5. Tertiary circular reactions (12–18 months) Experimentation to find new ways to solve problems or produce interesting outcomes (e.g.,
explore bath-water by gently patting it then hitting it vigorously and watching the results, or
stroke, pinch, squeeze, and pat a cat to see how it responds to varied actions)
6. Beginning of thought (18–24 months) First evidence of insight; solve problems mentally, using symbols to stand for objects and
actions; visualize how a stick could be used (e.g., move an out-of-reach toy closer); no longer
limited to thinking by doing
190 CHAPTER 7
COGNITION 191
Laura Dwight/PhotoEdit
understands the properties of object permanence,
this should strike them as odd. At 21⁄2 months, in-
fants do not show signs that they detect a difference
between an object moving along a track under the
Until an infant masters the concept of object permanence, objects that are out- high-window and low-window conditions. But, just
side of his visual sight are “out of mind.” 2 weeks later, 3-month-olds look longer at the low-
window event compared with the high-window
event (Aguiar & Baillargeon, 2002). Thus, by 3
months or so, the infant is capable of mentally representing months, infants have gained an understanding that objects
such invisible moves and conceiving of the object in its final have qualities that should permit them to be visible when noth-
location. According to Piaget, the concept of object perma- ing is obstructing them.
nence is fully mastered at this point. In an unusual study of toddlers’ advanced understanding
Does research support Piaget? Recent studies suggest that of object permanence, researchers compared healthy 2-year-
infants may develop at least some understanding of object per- olds with 2-year-olds with spinal muscular atrophy (SMA),
manence far earlier than Piaget claimed (Baillargeon, 2002; which is characterized by normal IQ but severe muscle prob-
Ruffman, Slade, & Redman, 2005). For example, Renee Bail- lems limiting children’s movement (Riviere & Lecuyer, 2003).
largeon and her colleagues have used a method of testing for Based on Piaget’s original reaching task, infants watch as a
object concept that does not require reaching for a hidden ob- hand picks up a toy and then “visits” three separate cloths, de-
ject, only looking toward where it should be. In one study, in- positing the toy under the second location before moving to
fants as young as 21⁄2 months seemed surprised (as demon- the last cloth. Healthy toddlers incorrectly searched under the
strated by looking longer) when a toy that had disappeared third cloth for the toy, whereas the SMA toddlers correctly
behind one screen (left side of è Figure 7.2) reappeared from searched under the second cloth. In a second study, research-
ers made healthy toddlers wait before they were allowed to
search, and with this delay, they too responded correctly by
searching under the second cloth. What explains this? Healthy
toddlers may quickly and impulsively search at the location
where an object is likely to be hidden (where the hand was last
seen), but when given more time to think about it, they can go
beyond their first impulsive response to search successfully.
SMA toddlers have this extra time “built in.” Because of their
muscle problems, SMA toddlers are slower at searching and
have less experience with manual searches than other children.
They are less likely to make an impulsive reach in the wrong
direction because of the time and effort required to reach. This
research suggests that success at object permanence tasks may
depend on more than a cognitive awareness of the properties of
objects: success also could be influenced by task conditions
è figure 7.2 Test stimuli used by Aguiar and Baillargeon such as the time interval between seeing something hidden
(1999, 2002). The doll moves behind the screen on the left and being able to search for it.
and reappears on the right side of the second screen without In general, then, it seems that babies sometimes know a
appearing in the space between the screens. good deal more about object permanence than they reveal
SOURCE: Aguiar & Baillargeon (2002), p. 271, part of Figure 1. Reprinted from through their actions when they are given the kinds of search
Cognitive Psychology, 45, Developments of young infant’s reasoning about occluded
objects, Aguilar & Baillargeon, 267–336 © 2002 with permission from Elsevier. tasks originally devised by Piaget (Baillargeon, 2002). Such
findings, however, are not necessarily inconsistent with Piaget’s
192 CHAPTER 7
è figure 7.3 There is nothing to be surprised about in the high-window event, but in the low-window event, the doll should (but
does not) appear in middle space as it moves along the track.
SOURCE: Aguiar & Baillargeon (2002), p. 272, part of Figure 2.
findings (Haith & Benson, 1998). Indeed, Piaget contended inside and gropes to reach the chain, but fails completely. A
that looking behaviors were developmental precursors to the pause follows during which Lucienne manifests a very curious
reaching behaviors that he assessed. He did not believe, how- reaction. . . . She looks at the slit with great attention; then sev-
ever, that looking represented complete understanding of ob- eral times in succession, she opens and shuts her mouth, at first
slightly, then wider and wider! [Then]. . . . Lucienne unhesitat-
ject permanence (Fischer & Bidell, 1991; Haith & Benson,
ingly puts her finger in the slit, and instead of trying as before to
1998). An analysis of infants’ looking behaviors by Carolyn
reach the chain, she pulls so as to enlarge the opening. She suc-
Rovee-Collier (2001) suggests that Piaget was wise to distinguish ceeds and grasps the chain.
between infants’ looking and reaching. In some situations, look-
ing may developmentally precede reaching for an object, as Lucienne uses the symbol of opening and closing her
Piaget suggested. In other situations, however, infants’ actions mouth to “think” through the problem. In addition to permit-
may reveal a more sophisticated understanding of the world ting mental problem solving, the symbolic capacity will appear
than looking would indicate. Regardless of the specific measure in the language explosion and pretend play so evident in the
researchers use, infants gradually become more skilled at acting preschool years.
on their knowledge by searching in the right spot. They im- In all, children’s intellectual achievements during the six
prove their looking and reaching skills between 8 and 12 substages of the sensorimotor period are remarkable. By its
months, and by the end of the sensorimotor period, they are end, they have become deliberate thinkers with a symbolic ca-
masters of even very complex hide-and-seek games (Moore & pacity that allows them to solve some problems in their heads,
Meltzoff, 1999; Newman, Atkinson, & Braddick, 2001). and they have a grasp of object permanence and many other
concepts.
COGNITION 193
© Laura Dwight/Corbis
In this conservation-of-area-task, Rachel first determines that the yellow boards have the same amount of space covered by blocks. But after
the blocks are rearranged on one of the boards, she fails to conserve area, indicating that one board now has more open space.
194 CHAPTER 7
ever seen a reindeer fly?” and or not children believe in Santa Claus and for
“How does Santa get into how long.
houses without chimneys?”
What made sense to the
preoperational child no longer
How can young children be so easily fooled by their percep- would overflow the glass if it were poured back. Indeed, one
tions? According to Piaget, the preschooler is unable to engage young child tested by a college student shrieked, “Do it again!”
in decentration—the ability to focus on two or more dimensions as though pouring the water back without causing the glass to
of a problem at once. Consider the conservation task: the child overflow were some unparalleled feat of magic.
must focus on height and width simultaneously and recognize Finally, preoperational thinkers fail to demonstrate con-
that the increased width of the short, broad container compen- servation because of limitations in transformational thought—
sates for its lesser height. Preoperational thinkers engage in the ability to conceptualize transformations, or processes of
centration—the tendency to center attention on a single aspect change from one state to another, as when water is poured
of the problem. They focus on height alone and conclude that from one glass to another (see Figure 7.4). Preoperational
the taller glass has more liquid; or, alternatively, they focus on thinkers engage in static thought, or thought that is fixed on
width and conclude that the short, wide glass has more. In this end states rather than the changes that transform one state into
and other ways, preschoolers seem to have one-track minds. another, as when the water is sitting in the two glasses not being
A second contributor to success on conservation tasks is poured or manipulated.
reversibility—the process of mentally undoing or reversing an Preoperational children do not understand the concept of
action. Older children often display mastery of reversibility by conservation, then, because they engage in centration, irrevers-
suggesting that the water be poured back into its original con- ible thought, and static thought. The older child, in the stage
tainer to prove that it is still the same amount. The young child of concrete operations, has mastered decentration, reversibil-
shows irreversibility of thinking and may insist that the water ity, and transformational thought. The correct answer to the
COGNITION 195
Mass Two identical balls of playdough One ball is rolled into the Conserving child recognizes that
(continuous are presented. The child agrees shape of a sausage. each object contains the same
substance): that they have equal amounts of amount of dough (average age,
dough. 6–7).
Number: Child sees two rows of beads and One row of beads is Conserving child recognizes that
agrees that each row has the same increased in length. each row still contains the same
number. number of beads (average age, 6–7).
Area: The child sees two identical sheets, The blocks on one sheet Conserving child recognizes that
each covered by the same number are scattered. the amount of uncovered area
of blocks. The child agrees that remains the same for each sheet
each sheet has the same amount (average age, 9–10).
of uncovered area.
Volume Two identical balls of clay are One ball of clay is taken from Conserving child recognizes that
(water placed in two identical beakers the water, molded into a the water levels will be the same
displacement): that had been judged to have the different shape, and placed because nothing except the shape
same amount to drink. The child above the beaker. Child is of the clay has changed — that is,
sees the water level rise to the asked whether the water level the pieces of clay displace the
same point in each beaker. will be higher than, lower than, same amount of water (average
or the same as in the other age, 9–12).
beaker when the clay is
reinserted into the water.
80
60
40
20
0
5 6 7 8 9 10
Age
COGNITION 197
198 CHAPTER 7
Fail conservation tasks because they have: Solve conservation tasks because they have:
• Irreversible thought—Cannot mentally undo an action • Reversibility of thought—Can mentally reverse or undo an action
• Centration—Center on a single aspect of a problem rather than • Decentration—Can focus on two or more dimensions of a
two or more dimensions at once problem at once
• Static thought—Fail to understand transformations or processes • Transformational thought—Can understand the process of change
of change from one state to another from one state to another
Perceptual salience. Understanding is driven by how things look Logical reasoning. Children acquire a set of internal operations that
rather than derived from logical reasoning. can be applied to a variety of problems.
Transductive reasoning. Children combine unrelated facts, often Deductive reasoning. Children draw cause–effect conclusions logi-
leading them to draw faulty cause–effect conclusions simply cally, based on factual information presented to them.
because two events occur close together in time or space.
Egocentrism. Children have difficulty seeing things from other Less egocentrism. Children understand that other people may have
perspectives and assume that what is in their mind is also what thoughts different from their own.
others are thinking.
Single classification. Children classify objects by a single Multiple classification. Children can classify objects by multiple
dimension at one time. dimensions and can grasp class inclusion.
COGNITION 199
è figure 7.7 Where would you put a third eye? Tanya (age 9) did not show much inventiveness in drawing her “third eye.” But Ken
(age 11) said of his eye on top of a tuft of hair, “I could revolve the eye to look in all directions.” John (also 11) wanted a third eye in his
palm: “I could see around corners and see what kind of cookie I’d get out of the cookie jar.” Ken and John show early signs of formal-
operational thoughts.
200 CHAPTER 7
A
A
A pendulum is made by B
hanging a weight at the end 2 4 6
C
of a string fixed at the other
end. If released from A, it
swings at a regular rate. Length of string Weight Point of release Amount of impetus
Find out which of these factors makes the pendulum go faster or slower.
alone or in combination, determines how quickly the pendulum to formal operations as taking place gradually over years. Many
makes its arc. How would you go about solving this problem? researchers have found it useful to distinguish between early
The concrete operator is likely to jump right in without and late formal operations. For example, 11- to 13-year-olds just
much advanced planning, using a trial-and-error approach. entering the formal operations stage are able to consider simple
That is, the child may try a variety of things but fail to test differ- hypothetical propositions such as the three-eye problem. But
ent hypotheses systematically—for example, the hypothesis that most are not yet able to devise an overall game plan for solving
the shorter the string is, the faster the pendulum swings, all a problem or to systematically generate and test hypotheses.
other factors remaining constant. Concrete operators are there- These achievements are more likely later in adolescence.
fore unlikely to solve the problem. They can draw proper con- Consider the findings of Suzanne Martorano (1977), who
clusions from their observations—for example, from watching gave 80 girls in grades 6, 8, 10, and 12 a battery of 10 Piagetian
as someone else demonstrates what happens if a pendulum with tasks. Among them were the pendulum problem; a task requir-
a short string is compared with a pendulum with a long string. ing students to identify all the possible combinations of chemi-
What will the formal-operational individual do? In all like- cals that could produce a particular chemical reaction; and
lihood, the child will first sit and think, planning an overall analyzing how the behavior of a balance beam is affected by
strategy for solving the problem. All the possible hypotheses the heaviness of weights on the beam and their distances from
should be generated; after all, the one overlooked may be the the fulcrum, or center. The 6th- and 8th-graders (ages 11–12
right one. Then it must be determined how each hypothesis and 13–14) accomplished only 2 or 3 of the 10 tasks on the
can be tested. This is a matter of hypothetical-deductive rea- average; the 10th- and 12th-graders (ages 15–16 and 17–18)
soning, or reasoning from general ideas to their specific impli- accomplished an average of 5 or 6. Similarly, 10th- and 11th-
cations. In the pendulum problem, it means starting with a graders (ages 16–17) demonstrate more advanced scientific
hypothesis and tracing the specific implications of this idea in reasoning than 7th- and 8th-graders (ages 13–14) when asked
an if–then fashion: “If the length of the string matters, then I to consider evidence and evaluate theories regarding religion
should see a difference when I compare a long string with a and social class (Klaczynski, 2000). Still, the responses of older
short string while holding other factors constant.” The trick in adolescents contain biases similar to those shown by younger
hypothesis testing is to vary each factor (for example, the length adolescents. Both age groups more readily accept evidence
of the string) while holding all others constant (the weight, the consistent with their preexisting beliefs than evidence inconsis-
height from which the weight is dropped, and so on). (It is, by tent with these beliefs (Klaczynski & Gordon, 1996a, 1996b;
the way, the length of the string that matters; the shorter the Kuhn, 1993). Thus, although reasoning skills improve over the
string, the faster the swing.) adolescent years, adolescents do not consistently show formal
In sum, formal-operational thought involves being able to operations and logical scientific reasoning skills on all tasks
think systematically about hypothetical ideas and abstract con- (Klaczynski & Narasimham, 1998).
cepts. It also involves mastering the hypothetical-deductive ap- Contrary to Piaget’s claim that intuitive reasoning is re-
proach that scientists use—forming many hypotheses and sys- placed by scientific reasoning as children age, the two forms
tematically testing them through an experimental method. of reasoning—intuitive and scientific—seem to coexist in
older thinkers (Klaczynski, 2000, 2001). Being able to shift be-
tween intuitive and scientific reasoning provides flexibility in
Progress toward Mastery problem-solving situations as long as the thinker can effectively
Are 11- and 12-year-olds really capable of all these sophisticated select the appropriate strategy. However, like children (and
mental activities? Anyone who has had dealings with this age adults), adolescents often seem to adopt an intuitive strategy,
group will know that the answer to this question is usually not. leading them to conclusions inconsistent with scientific judg-
Piaget (1970) described the transition from concrete operations ment (Klaczynski, 2001). With age, however, adolescents are
COGNITION 201