We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 39
Reading is Unnatural
WE’RE WIRED FOR LANGUAGE, BUT NOT FOR
READING • Speaking and understanding spoken language is a natural human ability, but reading is not. • By adolescence, learning a new language is the same as learning any other skill: it requires instruction and practice, and the learning and processing are handled by different brain areas from those that handled it in early childhood • In contrast, writing and reading did not exist until a few thousand years BCE and did not become common until only four or five centuries ago—long after the human brain had evolved into its modern state. Many people never learn to read well, or at all Learning to read = training our visual system • Learning to read involves training our visual system to recognize patterns—the patterns exhibited by text. • Actually, only part of our visual system is trained to recognize textual patterns involved in reading: the fovea and a small area immediately surrounding it (known as the perifovea), and the downstream neural networks running through the optic nerve to the visual cortex and into various parts of our brain. The neural networks starting elsewhere in our retinas do not get trained to read. • Learning to read also involves training the brain’s systems that control eye movement to move our eyes in a specific way over text. The main direction of eye movement depends on the direction in which the language we are reading is written. • European language scripts are read left to right, many middle Eastern language scripts are read right to left, and some language scripts are read top to bottom. • Beyond that, the precise eye movements differ depending on whether we are reading, skimming for overall meaning, or scanning for specific words. How we read • Assuming our visual system and brain have successfully been trained, reading becomes semi-automatic or fully automatic—both the eye movement and the processing. • The center of our visual field—the fovea and perifovea—is the only part of our visual field that is trained to read. • When we read, we may feel that our eyes scan smoothly across the lines of text, but that feeling is incorrect. In reality, our eyes continue with saccades during reading, but the movements generally follow the line of text. • They fix our fovea on a word, pause there for a fraction of a second to allow basic patterns to be captured and transmitted to the brain for further analysis, and then jump to the next important word (Larson, 2004) • Eye fixations while reading always land on words, usually near the center, never on word boundaries. • Very common small connector and function words like “a,” “and,” “the,” “or,” “is,” and “but” are usually skipped over, their presence either detected in perifoveal vision or simply assumed. • Most of the saccades during reading are in the text’s normal reading direction, but a few— about 10%—jump backwards to previous words. • At the end of each line of text, our eyes jump to where our brain guesses the next line begins. How much can we take in during each eye fixation during reading? • For reading European-language scripts at normal reading distances and text-font sizes, the fovea clearly sees 3–4 characters on either side of the fixation point. • The perifovea sees out about 15–20 characters from the fixation point, but not very clearly • Because our visual system has been trained to read, perception around the fixation point is asymmetrical: it is more sensitive to characters in the reading direction than in the other direction. IS READING FEATURE-DRIVEN OR CONTEXT- DRIVEN? • In feature-driven reading, the visual system starts by identifying simple features— line segments in a certain orientation or curves of a certain radius—on a page or display, and then combines them into more complex features, such as angles, multiple curves, shapes, and patterns. • Feature-driven reading is sometimes referred to as “bottom-up” or “context-free.” The brain’s ability to recognize basic features—lines, edges, angles, etc.—is built in and therefore automatic from birth. • In contrast, recognition of morphemes, words, and phrases has to be learned. It starts out as a nonautomatic, conscious process requiring conscious analysis of letters, morphemes, and words, but with enough practice it becomes automatic. • Obviously, the more common a morpheme, word, or phrase, the more likely that recognition of it will become automatic. • Context-driven or top-down reading operates in parallel with feature- driven reading but it works the opposite way: from whole sentences or the gist of a paragraph down to the words and characters. • The visual system starts by recognizing highlevel patterns like words, phrases, and sentences, or by knowing the text’s meaning in advance. • It then uses that knowledge to figure out—or guess—what the components of the high-level pattern must be (Boulton, 2009). • Context-driven reading is less likely to become fully automatic because most phrase-level and sentence-level patterns and contexts don’t occur frequently enough to allow their recognition to become burned into neural firing patterns. But there are exceptions, such as idiomatic expressions. • To experience context-driven reading, • Also, based on what we have already read and our knowledge of the world, our brains can sometimes predict text that the fovea has not yet read (or its meaning), allowing us to skip reading it • Feature-driven, bottom-up reading dominates; context assists • Similarly, educational researchers in the 1970s applied information theory to reading, and assumed that because of redundancies in written language, top-down, context-driven reading would be faster than bottom-up, feature-driven reading. • This assumption led them to hypothesize that reading for highly skilled (fast) readers would be dominated by context-driven (top- down) processing. • Skilled readers may resort to context-based reading when feature- based reading is disrupted by poor presentation of information (see examples later in this chapter). Also, in the race between context- based and feature-based reading to decipher the text we see, contextual cues sometimes win out over features. • As an example of context-based reading, Americans visiting England sometimes misread “to let” signs as “toilet,” because in the United States they see the word “toilet” often, but they almost never see the phrase “to let”—Americans use “for rent” instead. POOR INFORMATION DESIGN CAN DISRUPT READING • Careless writing or presentation of text can reduce skilled readers’ automatic, context-free reading to conscious, context-based reading, burdening working memory, thereby decreasing speed and comprehension. In unskilled readers, poor text presentation can block reading altogether Uncommon or unfamiliar vocabulary Difficult scripts and typefaces • Even when the vocabulary is familiar, reading can be disrupted by typefaces with unfamiliar or hard-to-distinguish shapes. Tiny fonts • Another way to make text hard to read in software applications, websites, and electronic appliances is to use fonts that are too small for their intended readers’ visual system to resolve Text on noisy background • Visual noise in and around text can disrupt recognition of features, characters, and words, and therefore drop reading out of automatic feature-based mode into a more conscious and context-based mode • In software user interfaces and websites, visual noise often results from designers’ placing text over a patterned background or displaying text in colors that contrast poorly with the background, The Federal Reserve Bank’s online mortgage calculator displayed text on a patterned background. Centered text • One aspect of reading that is highly automatic in most skilled readers is eye movement. In automatic (fast) reading, our eyes are trained to go back to the same horizontal position and down one line. If text is centered or right-aligned, each line of text starts in a different horizontal position. Automatic eye movements, therefore, take our eyes back to the wrong place, so we must consciously adjust our gaze to the actual start of each line. This drops us out of automatic mode and slows us down greatly. Design implications: Don’t disrupt reading; support it! • Obviously, a designer’s goal should be to support reading, not disrupt it. • Skilled (fast) reading is mostly automatic and mostly based on feature, character, and word recognition. The easier the recognition, the easier and faster the reading. Less skilled reading, by contrast, is greatly assisted by contextual cue Designers of interactive systems can support both reading methods by following these guidelines: • 1) Ensure that text in user interfaces allows the feature-based automatic processes to function effectively by avoiding the disruptive flaws described earlier: difficult or tiny fonts, patterned backgrounds, centering, etc. • 2) Use restricted, highly consistent vocabularies—sometimes referred to in the industry as plain language4 or simplified language (Redish, 2007). • 3) Format text to create a visual hierarchy (see Chapter 3) to facilitate easy scanning: use headings, bulleted lists, tables, and visually emphasized words. MUCH OF THE READING REQUIRED BY SOFTWARE IS UNNECESSARY • In addition to committing design mistakes that disrupt reading, many software user interfaces simply present too much text, requiring users to read more than is necessary. Consider how much unnecessary text there is in a dialog box for setting text entry properties in the SmartDraw application. • Software designers often justify lengthy instructions by arguing: “We need all that text to explain clearly to users what to do.” However, instructions can often be shortened with no loss of clarity. Design implications: Minimize the need for reading • Too much text in a user interface loses poor readers, who unfortunately are a significant percentage of the population. Too much text even alienates good readers: it turns using an interactive system into an intimidating amount of work. • Minimize the amount of prose text in a user interface; don’t present users with long blocks of prose text to read. • In instructions, use the least amount of text that gets most users to their intended goals. TEST ON REAL USERS • Finally, designers should test their designs on the intended user population to be confident that users can read all essential text quickly and effortlessly. • Some testing can be done early, using prototypes and partial implementations, but it should also be done just before release. • Fortunately, last-minute changes to text font sizes and formats are usually easy to make.
SPEED READING TECHNIQUES: Unlocking the Power of Speed Reading Techniques for Rapid Information Processing and Enhanced Comprehension (2023 Guide for Beginners)